Skip to main content
Nature Portfolio logoLink to Nature Portfolio
. 2022 Jun 29;92(5):1443–1449. doi: 10.1038/s41390-022-02176-8

Social epidemiology of early adolescent problematic screen use in the United States

Jason M Nagata 1,, Gurbinder Singh 1, Omar M Sajjad 2, Kyle T Ganson 3, Alexander Testa 4, Dylan B Jackson 5, Shervin Assari 6,7,8, Stuart B Murray 9, Kirsten Bibbins-Domingo 10, Fiona C Baker 11,12
PMCID: PMC9243697  PMID: 35768491

Abstract

Objective

To determine sociodemographic correlates of problematic screen use (social media, video games, mobile phones) among a racially/ethnically and socioeconomically diverse population-based sample of 10–14-year-old early adolescents.

Study design

We analyzed cross-sectional data from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development Study (Year 2, 2018–2020; N = 8753). Multiple linear regression analyses were used to estimate associations between sociodemographic factors (age, sex, race/ethnicity, primary language, household income, parental education) and adolescent-reported problematic video game (Video Game Addiction Questionnaire), social media (Social Media Addiction Questionnaire), and mobile phone use (Mobile Phone Involvement Questionnaire).

Results

Boys reported higher problematic video game use while girls reported higher problematic social media and mobile phone use. Native American, black, and Latinx adolescents reported higher scores across all problematic screen measures compared to non-Latinx white adolescents. Having unmarried/unpartnered parents was associated with higher problematic social media use. Although higher household income was generally protective against problematic video game use, these associations were weaker for black than white adolescents (p for interaction <0.05).

Conclusions

Given the sociodemographic differences in problematic screen use, digital literacy education strategies can focus on at-risk populations, encourage targeted counseling by pediatricians, and adapt family media use plans for diverse backgrounds.

Impact

  • While sociodemographic differences in screen time are documented, we examined sociodemographic differences in problematic screen use in a large, diverse sample of early adolescents in the US.

  • Boys reported higher problematic video game use while girls reported higher problematic social media and mobile phone use.

  • Native American, black, and Latinx adolescents reported higher scores across all problematic screen measures compared to non-Latinx white adolescents.

  • Although higher household income was generally protective against problematic video game use, these associations were weaker for black than white adolescents.

  • Beyond time spent on screens, pediatricians, parents, and educators should be aware of sociodemographic differences in problematic screen use.

Introduction

Children and adolescents are increasingly interacting with the world through tablets, smartphones, televisions, and gaming consoles;1 95% of US adolescents have access to a smartphone, and 45% report being online “almost constantly.”2 Studies have demonstrated associations between more screen time and higher caloric intake, sedentary behavior, depressive and anxiety symptoms, eating disorders, and a poorer quality of life,35 although relationships may be nuanced. Beyond time spent on screens, it is important to consider the extent of control over usage and interference with other activities, which could reflect problematic use including addiction and impairments in functioning. In some populations, deleterious outcomes may be the result of problematic screen use,6,7 which can span various modalities, including video games, social media, and mobile phones.8 Problematic video game use is characterized by an inability to regulate control over gaming, resulting in negative consequences in personal, social, occupational, familial, and other relevant areas of functioning.9 Problematic social media use is characterized by a perceived need to be online and constantly connected to technology, as typified by colloquialized phrases such as being “alone together.”10,11 While problematic mobile phone use shares some similarities with problematic social media use, it includes a broader range of applications that may be used on a mobile phone (e.g., texting, apps, video chat).12,13 Regardless of modality, problematic screen use can lead to negative psychological effects and functioning.913 Recognizing the prevalence of problematic screen use behaviors and sociodemographic elements associated with children’s interaction with video games, social media, and smartphones is crucial in implementing measures to prevent downstream persistent psychological distress, reduction of physical activity, and increased risk of obesity among groups at higher risk.14

Prior literature has demonstrated important sociodemographic disparities in general screen time; however, there is a paucity of literature documenting sociodemographic correlates of problematic screen use. Children who belong to minority groups or groups from lower socioeconomic backgrounds report higher levels of screen time than their white peers or peers from higher socioeconomic backgrounds, respectively.15,16 Among a sample of US adolescents, black eighth graders reported higher daily use of video games compared to their white counterparts, even after controlling for socioeconomic status.17 Social media use tends to be higher in girls than boys.16 One Turkish study investigating social media addiction found associations with age and income.8 A study of vocational students in Switzerland demonstrated that problematic mobile phone use was more prevalent in children aged 15–16 compared to 19 years or older.12 However, data on sociodemographic associations with problematic screen use in the US has been limited, especially among early adolescents who may be at the greatest risk.

With these apparent gaps in the literature, the purpose of our study was to explore problematic screen use behaviors across a population-based and racially/ethnically and economically diverse sample of US children aged 10–14 years old, considering three different domains: video games, social media, and mobile phones.

Methods

We conducted a secondary cross-sectional analysis of data from the 2-year follow-up of the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study (4.0 release). The ABCD Study is a longitudinal study (baseline 2016–2018) of health and cognitive development in 11,875 children from 21 recruitment sites across the US. The ABCD Study participants, recruitment, protocol, and measures have previously been described in detail.18 Participants were 10–14 years old during the 2-year follow-up, which was conducted between 2018 and 2020. After omitting study participants who were not asked problematic screen use questions due to not reporting video game, social media, or mobile phone use (n = 3122), 8753 children remained in the analytic sample (Appendix A). Institutional review board (IRB) approval was received from the University of California, San Diego and the respective IRBs of each study site. Written assent was obtained from participants, and written informed consent was obtained from their caregivers.

Measures and study variables

Dependent variables

Video Game Addiction Questionnaire (VGAQ)

The six-question VGAQ was used to assess problematic video game use as reported by adolescents. The questions were modeled after the Bergen Facebook Addiction Scale.19 The Bergen Facebook Addiction Scale consists of a unidimensional factor structure questionnaire assessing Facebook addiction, but prior authors have extrapolated its application to broader video game and social media addiction among high school and college students.20,21 Example questions include “I play video games so much that it has had a bad effect on my schoolwork or job” and “I feel the need to play video games more and more.” Likert-type scale responses ranged from 1 (never) to 6 (very often). Participants who reported any video game use on weekdays or weekends were asked these items.

Social Media Addiction Questionnaire (SMAQ)

The six-question SMAQ was used to assess problematic social media use as reported by adolescents. The questions were also modeled after the Bergen Facebook Addiction Scale.19 Examples include “I’ve tried to use my social media apps less but I can’t” and “I’ve become stressed or upset if I am not allowed to use my social media apps.” Likert-type scale responses ranged from 1 (never) to 6 (very often). Participants who reported having at least one social media account were asked these items.

Mobile Phone Involvement Questionnaire (MPIQ)

The eight-question MPIQ was designed to assess problematic mobile phone use as reported by adolescents.22 Examples include “I interrupt whatever else I am doing when I am contacted on my phone” and “I lose track of how much I am using my phone.” Likert-type scale responses ranged from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). A prior study has utilized this questionnaire to assess smartphone dependence with respect to digital multitasking during homework among US high school students.23 Participants who reported having mobile phones were asked these items.

Screen use

Screen use for the following modalities was determined using adolescents’ self-reported hours of use on a typical weekday and weekend: multi-player gaming, single-player gaming, texting, social media, video chatting, browsing the internet, and watching/streaming movies, videos, or TV.24 Total typical daily screen use was calculated as the weighted sum ([weekday average × 5] + [weekend average × 2])/7.

Independent variables

Parents reported participants’ sex (male or female), country of birth (US or outside US), primary language (English or non-English), and race/ethnicity (non-Latinx/Hispanic white, non-Latinx/Hispanic black, Native American, Latinx/Hispanic, Asian, or Other) at baseline.

In addition, parents reported highest parent education and household income at Year 2. In order to assess highest parent education, the questionnaire asked, “What is the highest grade or level of school you have completed or the highest degree you have received?” and “What is the highest grade or level of school your partner completed or highest degree they received?” Highest parent education was classified as high school or lower versus college or higher. To assess household income, the questionnaire asked, “What is your total combined family income for the past 12 months? This should include income (before taxes and deductions) from all sources, wages, rent from properties, social security, disability and/or veteran’s benefits, unemployment benefits, workman’s compensation, help from relatives (include child payments and alimony), and so on.” Household income was grouped into two categories reflecting the US median household income: less than $75,000 and $75,000 or more.25

Statistical analyses

Data analyses were performed in 2021 using Stata 15.1 (StataCorp). Multiple linear regression analyses were conducted to estimate cross-sectional associations between sociodemographic factors (age, sex, race/ethnicity, primary language of the child, household income, parents’ highest education, parent marital status) and three forms of problematic screen use (video game, social media, mobile phone), adjusting for the site. We expected variation in the association between income and our outcomes by race/ethnicity based on previous literature demonstrating minorities’ diminished returns (MDRs). These were tested by statistical interaction terms of household income multiplied by race/ethnicity (non-Hispanic white = 0). The coefficients for these interactions could be interpreted as differences in the change in the outcome due to a change in household income if the youth was a racial/ethnic minority compared to white (a positive and significant interaction would indicate MDRs). Some adolescents within the sample were twins or siblings. Sensitivity analyses were conducted including only one sibling per family, and findings did not substantially differ; therefore, we present results from the full sample. Propensity weights were applied to match key sociodemographic variables in the ABCD Study to the American Community Survey from the US Census.26

Results

Table 1 describes sociodemographic characteristics of the 8753 participants included. The analytic sample was approximately matched by sex (47.8% female) and racially and ethnically diverse (44.7% racial/ethnic minority). Table 2 shows responses to the problematic screen use questions. All problematic screen use and screen time measures were significantly correlated (Appendix B). Problematic social media and mobile phone use had the strongest correlation (r = 0.59, p < 0.001).

Table 1.

Sociodemographic and screen time characteristics of Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study participants (N = 8753).

Mean (SD)/%
Sociodemographic characteristics
Age (years) 12.0 (0.7)
Sex (%)
 Female 47.8
 Male 52.2
Race/ethnicity (%)
 White 55.3
 Latinx/Hispanic 19.4
 Black 15.7
 Asian 5.1
 Native American 3.2
 Other 1.3
Primary language of adolescent (%)
 English 88.9
 Non-English 11.1
Household income (%)
 Less than $75,000 48.0
 $75,000 and greater 52.0
Parents’ highest education (%)
 High school education or less 14.9
 College education or more 85.1
Parent marital status (%)
 Married/partnered 70.5
 Unmarried/unpartnered 29.5
Screen time
 Total recreational screen time 7.34 (5.81)
 Television 1.68 (1.83)
 Videos 1.48 (1.93)
 Single-player video games 1.07 (1.69)
 Multi-player video games 1.20 (1.84)
 Texting 0.75 (1.59)
 Social media 0.76 (1.68)
 Video chat 0.51 (1.37)
 Browsing the internet 0.37 (0.80)
Problematic screen use measures
 Video Game Addiction Questionnaire Scorea 2.09 (1.08)
 Social Media Addiction Questionnaire Scoreb 1.85 (0.90)
 Mobile Phone Involvement Questionnaire Scorec 3.10 (1.12)

ABCD propensity weights were applied based on the American Community Survey from the US Census.

SD standard deviation.

aAsked among a subset who reported video game use (n = 7595).

bAsked among a subset who reported social media use (n = 5652).

cAsked among a subset who reported mobile use (n = 7361).

Table 2.

Problematic video game, social media, and mobile phone use in the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study.

Video Game Addiction Questionnairea Never Very rarely Rarely Sometimes Often Very often
  I spend a lot of time thinking about playing video games 27.0% 18.3% 13.3% 22.3% 10.9% 8.2%
  I feel the need to play video games more and more 47.9% 15.5% 15.8% 11.9% 4.8% 4.0%
  I play video games so I can forget about my problems 54.9% 11.6% 9.3% 12.3% 5.9% 6.0%
  I’ve tried to play video games less but I can’t 62.2% 12.5% 9.2% 9.1% 3.6% 3.5%
  I’ve become stressed or upset if I am not allowed to play video games 60.5% 15.8% 8.3% 9.0% 3.2% 3.2%
  I play video games so much that it has had a bad effect on my schoolwork or job 78.5% 10.4% 4.9% 3.9% 1.3% 1.0%
Social Media Addiction Questionnaireb Never Very rarely Rarely Sometimes Often Very often
  I spend a lot of time thinking about social media apps or planning my use of social media apps 35.6% 24.8% 17.1% 16.1% 4.6% 1.8%
  I feel the need to use social media apps more and more 55.3% 17.4% 15.7% 8.2% 2.4% 1.1%
  I use social media apps so I can forget about my problems 61.9% 11.5% 8.3% 11.5% 4.0% 2.9%
  I’ve tried to use my social media apps less but I can’t 62.6% 12.9% 9.1% 9.1% 3.9% 2.5%
  I’ve become stressed or upset if I am not allowed to use my social media apps 68.4% 12.7% 7.2% 7.3% 2.3% 2.2%
  I use social media apps so much that it has had a bad effect on my schoolwork or job 78.8% 9.8% 5.3% 4.2% 1.3% 0.7%
Mobile Phone Involvement Questionnairec Strongly disagree Disagree Somewhat disagree Neither agree nor disagree Somewhat agree Agree Strongly agree
  I interrupt whatever else I am doing when I am contacted on my phone 17.7% 26.3% 14.3% 11.2% 19.5% 7.8% 3.3%
  I often use my phone for no particular reason 13.6% 23.1% 9.3% 10.1% 22.4% 15.8% 5.7%
  I feel connected to others when I am using my phone 12.0% 19.1% 9.9% 15.6% 21.8% 16.7% 5.0%
  Arguments have arisen with others because of my phone use 32.4% 33.0% 8.0% 9.3% 10.4% 4.9% 2.1%
  I lose track of how much I am using my phone. 15.0% 19.6% 9.3% 8.6% 19.8% 17.0% 10.7%
  I often think about my phone when I am not using it 28.5% 34.7% 9.8% 7.7% 9.6% 6.1% 3.7%
  I have been unable to reduce my phone use 25.3% 36.0% 10.0% 11.6% 8.3% 6.1% 2.7%
The thought of being without my phone makes me feel distressed 44.0% 32.2% 5.9% 6.6% 5.9% 3.2% 2.2%

ABCD propensity weights were applied based on the American Community Survey from the US Census.

aAsked among a subset who reported video game use (n = 7595).

bAsked among a subset who reported social media use (n = 5652).

cAsked among a subset who reported mobile use (n = 7361).

Table 3 shows linear regression analyses examining sociodemographic associations with problematic video game, social media, and mobile phone use. On average, boys reported higher scores for problematic video game use while girls reported higher scores for problematic social media and mobile phone use. Black adolescents reported higher scores for problematic screen use compared to white adolescents across the three modalities. Native American and Latinx/Hispanic adolescents reported higher scores for problematic video game and mobile phone use compared to white adolescents. Asian adolescents reported higher scores for problematic video game use compared to white adolescents. Lower income was associated with higher problematic screen use scores, while lower parent education was associated with higher problematic video game use. Adolescents whose parents were unmarried/unpartnered reported higher problematic mobile phone use scores compared to adolescents whose parents were married/partnered.

Table 3.

Sociodemographic associations with problematic screen use in the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study.

Video gamesa Social mediab Mobile phonesc
Sociodemographic characteristics B (95% CI) p B (95% CI) p B (95% CI) p
Age 0.02 (–0.02 to 0.05) 0.281 0.09 (0.04 to 0.13) <0.001 0.18 (0.14 to 0.23) <0.001
Sex
  Female reference reference reference
  Male 0.74 (0.69 to 0.79) <0.001 –0.11 (–0.16 to –0.05) <0.001 –0.18 (–0.24 to –0.12) <0.001
Race/ethnicity
  White reference reference reference
  Latinx/Hispanic 0.12 (0.04 to 0.20) 0.004 0.06 (–0.04 to 0.15) 0.250 0.11 (0.004 to 0.21) 0.041
  Black 0.17 (0.10 to 0.25) <0.001 0.11 (0.02 to 0.19) 0.018 0.21 (0.12 to 0.31) <0.001
  Asian 0.14 (0.04 to 0.25) 0.008 0.02 (–0.12 to 0.15) 0.814 –0.16 (–0.32 to 0.00) 0.050
  Native American 0.25 (0.12 to 0.38) <0.001 0.14 (–0.06 to 0.33) 0.167 0.29 (0.11 to 0.47) 0.002
  Other 0.14 (–0.12 to 0.40) 0.281 0.04 (–0.40 to 0.49) 0.848 –0.03 (–0.51 to 0.45) 0.902
Primary language of adolescent
  Non-English reference reference reference
  English 0.06 (–0.04 to 0.16) 0.219 0.03 (–0.09 to 0.14) 0.648 –0.11 (–0.23 to 0.02) 0.092
Household income
  $75,000 and greater reference reference reference
  Less than $75,000 0.17 (0.11 to 0.23) <0.001 0.12 (0.05 to 0.20) 0.001 0.15 (0.07 to 0.23) <0.001
Parents’ highest education
  College education or more reference reference reference
  High school education or less 0.10 (0.02 to 0.18) 0.014 0.01 (–0.08 to 0.10) 0.774 0.06 (–0.04 to 0.16) 0.228
Parent marital status
  Married/partnered reference reference reference
  Unmarried/unpartnered 0.03 (–0.03 to 0.09) 0.286 0.07 (–0.00 to 0.14) 0.054 0.10 (0.02 to 0.18) 0.011

Bold indicates p < 0.05. ABCD propensity weights were applied based on the American Community Survey from the US Census.

All models include age, sex, race/ethnicity, primary language, household income, parent education, parent marital status, and site.

aAsked among a subset who reported video game use (n = 7595).

bAsked among a subset who reported social media use (n = 5652).

cAsked among a subset who reported mobile use (n = 7361).

We conducted additional linear regression analyses examining sociodemographic associations with problematic video game use stratified by income given evidence of significant effect modification by income (p < 0.05). There was no evidence of significant effect modification by income for problematic social media or mobile phone use (p > 0.05). There were some notable differences by race/ethnicity and income level. For black adolescents (compared to white adolescents), those in high-income households reported a B = 0.32 (95% CI 0.18–0.45) higher problematic video game use score; however, in low-income households, the difference in problematic video game use score was not significant (B = 0.07; 95% CI –0.05 to 0.19).

Discussion

In this demographically diverse sample of 10–14-year-old early adolescents in the United States, we found multiple noteworthy sociodemographic factors associated with problematic screen use. Boys reported higher problematic video game use scores while girls reported higher problematic social media and mobile phone use scores. Native American, black, and Latinx/a adolescents reported higher problematic video game and mobile phone use measures compared to white adolescents. While higher household income was generally protective against problematic screen use, these associations were weaker for black than white adolescents, which is in line with the expected MDRs theory.27,28

Our finding of sex differences in problematic screen use reflects general screen use trends. Boys on average spend more time playing video games than girls, while girls on average spend more time on social media and texting than boys, which was previously shown in the baseline ABCD cohort.16 Significant sex differences occurred in all domains of problematic screen use, but the effect was most prominent in problematic video game use. These differences could be informed by children facing increased pressure to conform to culturally sanctified gender roles from an early age.29 Furthermore, while boys spend less time on social media than girls,16 they may simply foster social connections through different means (i.e., video games instead of social media). Prior studies support this phenomenon such that boys begin to identify themselves as gamers in early adolescence, when gender disparities typically increase.29,30 Our findings extend known sex differences in time spent on screens to problematic screen use.

Across the video game, social media, and mobile phone categories, black adolescents reported higher problematic screen use than white adolescents. Native American and Latinx/Hispanic adolescents reported higher problematic video game and mobile phone use compared to white adolescents. Adolescents from lower-income households reported higher problematic screen use. Racial and socioeconomic differences may be explained by residential differences. Black and Latinx/Hispanic parents are more likely to be essential workers and therefore may have less supervision time for their children.31 Certain neighborhood environments intrinsically have fewer opportunities for outdoor physical activity in predominantly minority neighborhoods.32 Negative perceptions of safety within a neighborhood have been associated with decreased physical activity and increased screen time.33,34 Race/ethnicity and socioeconomic status may be a proxy for context and place,35 which may narrow options for recreational activities.

Socioeconomic disparities could be partially explained by “escapism,” or the tendency to seek relief and distraction from realities perceived as being unpleasant, especially by entertaining or engaging in a fantasy.3638 For instance, multiple studies demonstrate that video game addiction may permit the user to escape from reality.36,39 Prior studies have identified escaping through video games as a preference for virtual over real-life stimuli, and this was associated with higher engagement in video game usage as assessed through an Affect Misattribution Procedure.39 While video gaming can be a nonproblematic leisure pursuit or even a passion for many, it becomes problematic when children use video games as a potential coping strategy.39 The questions asked in the VGAQ suggest a strong component of problematic coping, potentially exacerbated by psychological escapism. While speculative, this may be associated with the higher problematic video game use among children of parents with lower formal education and lower household income.

Interestingly, income modified race and sex disparities in problematic video game use. In high-income households, there were greater disparities in problematic video game use for black compared to white adolescents, relative to low-income households. This may reflect MDRs, a phenomenon where higher socioeconomic status does not remove disparities between black and white children.40 For instance, black adolescents in high-income households experience higher odds of racism than black adolescents in low-income households, compared to white adolescents.41 These MDRs then can be measured at the level of function and structure of the brain, and become a vehicle for the trans-generational transition of poverty and low socioeconomic status.27,28

Other factors that could explain disparities in problematic screen use are parent–child interactions, where lower socioeconomic status is associated with fewer parent–child interactions, assessed by the frequency of field trips and screen-free conversations.42 Parents underestimate adolescents’ social media use.43 Heavy parent screen use has been shown to predict child screen use and can lead to distracted parenting, also known as “technoference” or “technology interference.”44 Technology interference may be influenced by parental education status, household income, and race/ethnicity.

There are several limitations and strengths of this study worth noting. Directions of causality are unable to be determined due to the cross-sectional nature of the study. Although several potential confounding variables were controlled for, there is a possibility of residual confounders. Measures were also self-reported, which increased subjectivity to reporting and recall bias. Social media and mobile phone behavior may also include a certain degree of overlap.45 It is also important to note that the effect sizes of some sociodemographic factors were relatively small. The possibility of selection bias may be exemplified by participants with missing data more likely to be ethnic/racial minorities, born outside of the US, and from lower socioeconomic strata (Appendix A). The strengths of this study are derived from the diverse, large, population-based sample. Furthermore, the novel measures captured contemporary problematic screen use behaviors.

Our findings have significant public health, policy, and clinical implications, particularly to inform the adaptation and implementation of existing video game, social media, and mobile phone guidance for children. This research may further inform targeted screen-related guidance for clinicians, educators, and parents. The American Academy of Pediatrics advocates for a family media use plan, which could be individualized based on some of the disparities in problematic screen use noted in this study. Studies show that parental oversight is critical in adolescence, so educating and informing parents on the warning signs of problematic screen use could be helpful.46 Moreover, school and community-level efforts implemented to engage families of color may incorporate tailoring of culturally pertinent messages, mobilizing social networks, and building community coalitions to facilitate culturally competent guidance.47 One example would be the decision for communities and schools to provide interested parents with guidance on education and novel research to develop and modify their own family media use plans.

This study represents an advance in our understanding of video game, social media, and mobile phone use among early adolescents, and how problematic patterns of usage are associated with sociodemographic factors. These factors should be incorporated into guidance and policies to individualize efforts on counseling and implementation. Greater knowledge of risk and protective factors for problematic video game, social media, and mobile phone behavior can strengthen our preventive strategies by informing future children-focused or family-based interventions across numerous technological platforms while individualizing the approaches for children in this age range. Comprehension of the social epidemiology of problematic video game, social media, and mobile phone use is crucial, especially given the unprecedented rise of technology usage during the coronavirus disease 2019 pandemic.48 Future research may integrate randomized control trials, prospective studies, and individual interviews to discern lifestyle factors that are disproportionately associated with problematic video game, social media, and mobile phone use.

Supplementary information

Supplementary Information (18.4KB, docx)

Acknowledgements

The authors thank Anthony Kung and Levi Cervantez for editorial assistance.

Author contributions

J.M.N.: conception and design, analysis and interpretation of data, drafting the article, and final approval. G.S.: drafting and revising the article, final approval. O.M.S.: drafting and revising the article, final approval. K.T.G.: conception, critically revising article, final approval. A.T.: critically revising article, final approval. D.B.J.: critically revising article, final approval. S.A.: critically revising article, final approval. S.B.M.: critically revising article, final approval. K.B.-D.: critically revising article, final approval. F.C.B.: conception and design, data acquisition, critically revising article, final approval.

Funding

J.M.N. was supported by the American Heart Association Career Development Award (CDA34760281) and the National Institutes of Health (K08HL159350). S.B.M. was supported by the National Institutes of Health (K23 MH115184). K.B.-D. is supported by the National Institutes of Health (K24DK103992). The ABCD Study was supported by the National Institutes of Health and additional federal partners under award numbers U01DA041022, U01DA041025, U01DA041028, U01DA041048, U01DA041089, U01DA041093, U01DA041106, U01DA041117, U01DA041120, U01DA041134, U01DA041148, U01DA041156, U01DA041174, U24DA041123, and U24DA041147. A full list of supporters is available at https://abcdstudy.org/nihcollaborators. A listing of participating sites and a complete listing of the study investigators can be found at https://abcdstudy.org/principal-investigators.html. ABCD consortium investigators designed and implemented the study and/or provided data but did not necessarily participate in the analysis or writing of this report. The funders had no role in the study analysis, decision to publish the study, or the preparation of the manuscript.

Data availability

Data used in the preparation of this article were obtained from the ABCD Study (https://abcdstudy.org), held in the NIMH Data Archive (NDA). Investigators can apply for data access through the NDA (https://nda.nih.gov/).

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Consent for publication

Written assent was obtained from participants, and written informed consent was obtained from their caregivers.

Footnotes

The original online version of this article was revised: some of the descriptive screen time measures in Table 1 were underestimated and have been corrected. The data of the correlation table in Appendix B has also been updated to reflect these changes.

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Change history

9/22/2022

A Correction to this paper has been published: 10.1038/s41390-022-02311-5

Supplementary information

The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41390-022-02176-8.

References

  • 1.Twenge JM, Campbell WK. Associations between screen time and lower psychological well-being among children and adolescents: evidence from a population-based study. Prev. Med Rep. 2018;12:271–283. doi: 10.1016/j.pmedr.2018.10.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Anderson, M. & Jiang, J. Teens, social media, and technology 2018. https://www.pewresearch.org/internet/wp-content/uploads/sites/9/2018/05/PI_2018.05.31_TeensTech_FINAL.pdf (2018).
  • 3.Stiglic, N. & Viner, R. M. Effects of screentime on the health and well-being of children and adolescents: a systematic review of reviews. BMJ Open.9. 10.1136/bmjopen-2018-023191 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 4.Nagata JM, et al. Contemporary screen time usage among children 9–10-years-old is associated with higher body mass index percentile at 1-year follow-up: A prospective cohort study. Pediatr. Obes. 2021;16:e12827. doi: 10.1111/ijpo.12827. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Nagata Jason M., et al. Contemporary screen time modalities among children 9–10 years old and binge-eating disorder at one-year follow-up: A prospective cohort study. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 2021;54:887–892. doi: 10.1002/eat.23489. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Aşut Ö, Abuduxike G, Acar-Vaizoğlu S, Cali S. Relationships between screen time, internet addiction and other lifestyle behaviors with obesity among secondary school students in the Turkish Republic of Northern Cyprus. Turk. J. Pediatr. 2019;61:568–579. doi: 10.24953/turkjped.2019.04.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Kuss DJ, Lopez-Fernandez O. Internet addiction and problematic Internet use: a systematic review of clinical research. World J. Psychiatry. 2016;6:143. doi: 10.5498/wjp.v6.i1.143. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Aydin, S. et al. Investigation of the effect of social media addiction on adults with depression. Healthcare9, 450. 10.3390/healthcare9040450 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 9.Gros, L., Debue, N., Lete, J. & van de Leemput, C. Video game addiction and emotional states: possible confusion between pleasure and happiness? Front. Psychol.10, 2894. 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02894 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 10.Kuss, D. J. & Griffiths, M. D. Social networking sites and addiction: ten lessons learned. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health.14, 311. 10.3390/ijerph14030311 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 11.Turkle, S. Alone Together: Why We Expect More from Technology and Less from Each Other. (New York, NY: Basic Books, 2012).
  • 12.Haug S, et al. Smartphone use and smartphone addiction among young people in Switzerland. J. Behav. Addict. 2015;4:299–307. doi: 10.1556/2006.4.2015.037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Lin, Y. H. et al. Development and validation of the Smartphone Addiction Inventory (SPAI). PLoS One9, e98312. 10.1371/journal.pone.0098312 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 14.Fiechtner L, et al. Determinants of binge eating symptoms in children with overweight/obesity. Child Obes. 2018;14:510–517. doi: 10.1089/chi.2017.0311. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Hoyos Cillero I, Jago R. Systematic review of correlates of screen-viewing among young children. Prev. Med. (Balt.). 2010;51:3–10. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2010.04.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Nagata JM, et al. Sociodemographic correlates of contemporary screen time use among 9-10-year-old children. J. Pediatr. 2022;240:213–220.e2. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2021.08.077. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Carson N, Lê Cook B, Chen C-N, Alegria M. Racial/ethnic differences in video game and internet use among us adolescents with mental health and educational difficulties. J. Child Media. 2012;6:450–468. doi: 10.1080/17482798.2012.724592. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Barch DM, et al. Demographic, physical and mental health assessments in the adolescent brain and cognitive development study: rationale and description. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2018;32:55–66. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2017.10.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Andreassen CS, TorbjØrn T, Brunborg GS, Pallesen S. Development of a Facebook Addiction Scale. Psychol. Rep. 2012;110:501–517. doi: 10.2466/02.09.18.PR0.110.2.501-517. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Hou Y, Xiong D, Jiang T, Song L, Wang Q. Social media addiction: its impact, mediation, and intervention. Cyberpsychology J. Psychosoc. Res Cybersp. 2019;13:4. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Simsek A, Elciyar K, Kizilhan T. A comparative study on social media addiction of high school and university students. Contemp. Educ. Technol. 2019;10:106–119. doi: 10.30935/cet.554452. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Walsh SP, White KM, Young RM. Needing to connect: the effect of self and others on young people’s involvement with their mobile phones. Aust. J. Psychol. 2010;62:194–203. doi: 10.1080/00049530903567229. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Mrazek AJ, et al. Teenagers’ smartphone use during homework: an analysis of beliefs and behaviors around digital multitasking. Educ. Sci. 2021;11:713. doi: 10.3390/educsci11110713. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Bagot KS, et al. Current, future and potential use of mobile and wearable technologies and social media data in the ABCD Study to increase understanding of contributors to child health. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2018;32:121–129. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2018.03.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Semega, J., Kollar, M., Creamer, J. & Mohanty, A. Income and Poverty in the United States: 2018. https://www.census.gov/library/publications/2019/demo/p60-266.html (2019).
  • 26.Heeringa, S. & Berglund, P. A guide for population-based analysis of the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study Baseline Data. Preprint at bioRxiv 2020.02.10.942011 (2020).
  • 27.Assari S. Parental education and nucleus accumbens response to reward anticipation: minorities’ diminished returns. Adv. Soc. Sci. Cult. 2020;2:p132. doi: 10.22158/assc.v2n4p132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Assari S. Health disparities due to diminished return among black Americans: public policy solutions. Soc. Issues Policy Rev. 2018;12:112–145. doi: 10.1111/sipr.12042. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Hill, J. P. & Lynch, M. E. The intensification of gender-related role expectations during early adolescence. In: Girls at puberty (ed Brooks-Gunn, J. P. A. C.) 201–208 (Boston, Springer, 1983).
  • 30.Leonhardt M, Overå S. Are there differences in video gaming and use of social media among boys and girls?–A mixed methods approach. Int J. Environ. Res. Public Heal. Artic. Public Heal. 2021;18:6085. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18116085. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics. Labor force characteristics by race and ethnicity, 2018: BLS Reports (accessed 9 Dec 2021). https://www.bls.gov/opub/reports/race-and-ethnicity/2018/home.htm (2019).
  • 32.Whitaker, K. M. et al. Associations of accelerometer-measured sedentary time and physical activity with prospectively assessed cardiometabolic risk factors: the CARDIA study. J. Am. Heart. Assoc.8, e010212. 10.1161/JAHA.118.010212 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 33.Datar A, Nicosia N, Shier V. Parent perceptions of neighborhood safety and children’s physical activity, sedentary behavior, and obesity: evidence from a national longitudinal study. Am. J. Epidemiol. 2013;177:1065–1073. doi: 10.1093/aje/kws353. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Lenhart, C. M., Wiemken, A., Hanlon, A., Perkett, M. & Patterson, F. Perceived neighborhood safety related to physical activity but not recreational screen-based sedentary behavior in adolescents. BMC Public Health17, 722. 10.1186/s12889-017-4756-z (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 35.Abdel Magid HS, Milliren CE, Gabriel KP, Nagata JM. Disentangling school and neighborhood effects on screen time among adolescents in the United States. Prev. Med. 2021;142:106357. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2020.106357. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Chen CY, Chang SL. Moderating effects of information-oriented versus escapism-oriented motivations on the relationship between psychological well-being and problematic use of video game live-streaming services. J. Behav. Addict. 2019;8:564–573. doi: 10.1556/2006.8.2019.34. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Jouhki, H. & Oksanen, A. To get high or to get out? Examining the link between addictive behaviors and escapism. Subst. Use Misuse57, 202–211. 10.1080/10826084.2021.2002897 (2022). [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 38.Hagström D, Kaldo V. Escapism among players of MMORPGs-conceptual clarification, its relation to mental health factors, and development of a new measure. Cyberpsychology, Behav. Soc. Netw. 2014;17:19–25. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2012.0222. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Deleuze J, et al. Escaping reality through videogames is linked to an implicit preference for virtual over real-life stimuli. J. Affect Disord. 2019;245:1024–1031. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2018.11.078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Assari S. American children’s screen time: diminished returns of household income in black families. Information. 2020;11:1–10. doi: 10.3390/info11110538. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Nagata JM, Ganson KT, Sajjad OM, Benabou SE, Bibbins-Domingo K. Prevalence of perceived racism and discrimination among US children aged 10 and 11 years: The Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study. JAMA Pediatr. 2021;175:861–863. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2021.1022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Wong RS, et al. Parent technology use, parent–child interaction, child screen time, and child psychosocial problems among disadvantaged families. J. Pediatr. 2020;226:258–265. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2020.07.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Nagata JM, et al. Parent-adolescent discrepancies in adolescent recreational screen time reporting during the Coronavirus Disease 2019 pandemic. Acad. Pediatr. 2021;S1876-2859:00623–00629. doi: 10.1016/j.acap.2021.12.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.McDaniel BT, Radesky JS. Technoference: longitudinal associations between parent technology use, parenting stress, and child behavior problems. Pediatr. Res. 2018;84:210–218. doi: 10.1038/s41390-018-0052-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Brunette MF, et al. Use of smartphones, computers and social media among people with SMI: opportunity for intervention. Community Ment. Health J. 2019;55:973–978. doi: 10.1007/s10597-019-00431-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Worthman CM, Tomlinson M, Rotheram-Borus MJ. When can parents most influence their child’s development? Expert knowledge and perceived local realities. Soc. Sci. Med. 2016;154:62–69. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2016.02.040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Yancey, A. K. et al. Peer reviewed: population-based interventions engaging communities of color in healthy eating and active living: a review. Prev. Chronic Dis.1, A09 (2004). [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 48.Nagata JM, Abdel Magid HS, Gabriel KP. Screen time for children and adolescents during the Coronavirus Disease 2019 pandemic. Obesity. 2020;28:1582–1583. doi: 10.1002/oby.22917. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary Information (18.4KB, docx)

Data Availability Statement

Data used in the preparation of this article were obtained from the ABCD Study (https://abcdstudy.org), held in the NIMH Data Archive (NDA). Investigators can apply for data access through the NDA (https://nda.nih.gov/).


Articles from Pediatric Research are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES