Abstract
Objective.
To test the hypotheses that people with Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment have increased frequency of vestibular impairments and decreased hippocampal volume compared to healthy age-matched controls.
Study design:
Retrospective, with some historical controls
Setting:
Out-patient, tertiary care center
Subjects:
People with mild to moderate dementia diagnosed with Alzheimer’s disease and with mild cognitive impairment.
Main outcome measures: A standard clinical battery of objective tests of the vestibular system, and screening for balance; available clinical diagnostic MRIs were reviewed and post-processed to quantify the left and right hippocampal volumes utilizing both manual segmentation and computer automated segmentation.
Results.
Study subjects (N=26) had significantly more vestibular impairments, especially on Dix-Hallpike maneuvers and cervical vestibular evoked myogenic potentials (cVEMP), than historical controls. No differences were found between mild and moderate dementia subjects. Independence on instrumental activities of daily living in subjects with age-normal balance approached statistical differences from subjects with age-abnormal balance. MRI data were available for 11 subjects. Subjects with abnormal cVEMP had significantly reduced left hippocampal MRI’s using manual segmentation compared to subjects with normal cVEMP.
Conclusion.
The data from this small sample support and extend previous evidence for vestibular impairments in this population. The small MRI sample set should be considered preliminary evidence, and suggests the need for further research, with a more robust sample and high-resolution MRIs performed for the purpose of hippocampal analysis.
Keywords: Dementia, vestibular disorders, MRI, hippocampus, VEMP
Introduction
Several studies suggest that vestibular impairment may be related to Alzheimer’s Disease (AD), although the results are mixed. Harun et al. reported that AD patients who were tested on cervical vestibular evoked myogenic potentials (cVEMP) and head impulse tests had impaired spatial skills, and spatial skills were worse among subjects who appeared to have vestibular disorders (1, 2). This finding is significant because AD patients have reduced hippocampal volume (3), and the hippocampus is involved in spatial orientation. By contrast, a small sample of AD patients were shown to have balance deficits and impaired scores on the Fukuda stepping test, but they had no deficits on bi-thermal caloric tests of the vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR) (4). AD subjects have been shown to have deficits on static posturography (5). These studies are problematic because the Fukuda stepping test has been discredited (6–8) and static posturography has limited uses.
Direct and indirect evidence indicates the presence of vestibular input on hippocampal place cells and head direction cells, reviewed elsewhere (9, 10). Vestibular input probably has an anatomical distribution that is greater in the dorsal hippocampus and dorsal dentate gyrus than the ventral hippocampus or in CA1 (11). Rats with chemically induced bilateral peripheral vestibular loss show increased long term potentiation in the dentate gyrus and upregulation of NMDA receptor expression in the dentate gyrus and in CA1 (12). These studies suggest that vestibular input may have a direct or indirect influence on spatial memory functions mediated by the hippocampus. Those functions are often impaired in AD (12, 13). By comparison, however, patients with surgical unilateral vestibular loss showed no changed in hippocampal volume (14). Therefore, the extent that vestibular projections contribute to hippocampal volume is unclear.
Previc suggested that vestibular loss contributes to AD via degeneration of medial-temporal regions and related pathways in the caudate nucleus and hippocampus (15). Vestibular input may project to the caudate nucleus (16), and vestibularly-mediated path integration – a type of spatial orientation skill -- has been shown to be impaired after vestibular and caudate nucleus lesions (17). Also, two studies of vestibular nuclei show changes that are characteristic of changes in the hippocampus of AD patients. Neurofibrillary tangles have been found in the lateral vestibular nuclei of AD patients but only minimally in control subjects (18). Also, plaques, decreased dendritic spines, impairments of the Golgi apparatus, and decreased neuronal vesicles have been found in the medial vestibular nuclei of AD patients (19).
These studies suggest that deficits in vestibular function may contribute to deficits in spatial orientation functions mediated by the vestibular system and the hippocampus and may contribute to decreased input to the hippocampus, resulting in decreased hippocampal volume. The findings are not clear-cut, however. In the present study we tested two hypotheses: that individuals with AD and with mild cognitive impairment (MCI) would have decreased performance on standard, clinical objective tests of the vestibular system and well-normed balance screening tests; and that individuals with decreased vestibular test results would have decreased hippocampal volumes.
Methods
Subjects
Subjects were adults diagnosed with either AD (n=16) or MCI (n=10). They were all recruited from among the caseload of the Alzheimer’s Disease and Memory Disorder Center at Baylor College of Medicine. Patients who present to the Center with memory complaints undergo an evaluation by a neurologist specializing in dementia and they complete a standardized workup that includes a detailed history and interview with the patient and informant, neurological and physical examinations, an MRI of the brain, neuropsychological testing, APOE genotyping, and screening laboratory studies. They or their care givers completed the Physical Self-Maintenance Scale (PSMS) and Independent Activities of Daily Living scale (IADL) to rate the patient’s ability to function independently (20). Therefore, data from those scales were already available for this study.
Due to cost, amyloid status is only determined in patients who screen for clinical trials so that information was not available. Diagnoses are made by a panel of physicians and neuropsychologists who met weekly at a consensus conference. Consensus diagnoses for AD and MCI are determined using well-established criteria (21–23). We used their most current diagnoses.
Subjects were recruited between October 2019 and January 2021. The final sample included 26 people, mean age 76.1, range 64.6 to 91.0 years, with 14 males and 12 females. The mean Mini Mental Status Exam score on the most recent test was 21.0, SD 3.8. Subjects were all ambulatory and able to stand unsupported on the floor for at least 60 seconds, had cervical range of motion within functional limits, and had no significant musculoskeletal impairments. Although an MRI was part of the standard diagnostic work-up, some people had had their MRI’s outside of the institution and the raw data were not available. Some others had incomplete data. Therefore, MRI data were available for only 11 subjects.
To participate in this study, subjects had to have a Mini Mental State Score > 10 (mild-moderate AD or MCI) and be capable of following the instructions during testing. All subjects were accompanied by their legal representatives, who gave written informed consent if the subject was unable to do so.
We wanted to be able to compare subjects in this study to individuals of the same age who did not have MCI or AD, and who had been tested in the same laboratory with the same equipment. Therefore, we compared the vestibular test findings from the new subjects to data from subjects who were previously tested in this lab (24). Details are provided in the Results.
MRI description
In patients with symptoms related to Alzheimer’s, an anatomical brain MRI is performed to assess atrophy of the medial temporal lobe structures. This atrophy is highly associated with the presence of neurofibrillary tangles and neuronal loss (25–27). A retrospective review of the clinical anatomic MRIs was performed on all subjects with specific inclusion criteria: a high resolution T1 sequence with oblique coronal orientation that is perpendicular to the long axis of the hippocampus; and date of the MRIs after January 1, 2017 to ensure common availability of high resolution imaging. Data from any subject was excluded primarily if the images had motion or if the quality of imaging had inadequate resolution as deemed by the neuroradiologist (CML). The routine clinical practice MRI parameters used for the T1 weighted sequence done for patients with memory loss or cognitive impairment included: Sagittal 3D T1W TFE scan with field of view 256mm (frequency) x 244mm (phase) x 190, repetition time of 7.1msec, echo time of 3.2msec, resolution voxel 1 × 1 × 1mm, total number of slice sections at 190, matrix 256 × 232 and NEX of 1. These parameters satisfy the need for clinical qualitative assessment that is usually subject to ambiguity and interobserver inconsistency.
Once a subject’s MRI met the inclusion criteria, post processing of the imaging data set was performed with quantitative analysis that utilized both computer automated and manually segmented techniques. The computer-automated technique employed a free, online MRI brain volumetry, VolBrain (http://volbrain.upv.es) (28). Manual segmentation was performed with MIPAV (Medical Image Processing, Analysis, and Visualization; version 10.0.0), a free medical imaging processing software package from the National Institutes of Health. Each technique allowed for quantifying the individual hippocampi, i.e. right and left and in total. The computer-automated segmentation technique required uploading the T1 MRI images into VolBrain and, after several hours, the software extracted the volumes and emailed a report of the individual volume of each hippocampus and in total in cubic centimeters. By comparison, for the manual segmentation method, T1 imaging was loaded into MIPAV software, the entire hippocampus was identified, and was manually traced on contiguous slices by the neuroradiologist (CML). The contiguous voxel information was used to calculate volume in cubic centimeters. Reference data for hippocampal volumes were taken from the paper by Sanchez-Benavides et al (29).
Balance testing
All subjects were tested on standard, normed screening tests of walking and standing balance using Tandem Walking with eyes closed (TW) (30) and the modified Romberg test, also known as the modified Clinical Test of Sensory Integration on Balance (CTSIB) (31). To standardize footwear and to ensure good hygiene, all subjects were tested without shoes but wearing socks, per standard practice with these tests. Per standard procedure, to perform TW subjects practiced 5 heel-to-toe steps with eyes open and then with eyes closed. Then they were asked to perform 10 steps with eyes closed and arms crossed. The dependent measure was the number of steps performed without stepping out of line, taking an extra step, moving the arms, or opening the eyes.
To perform the modified CTSIB subjects stood on 10 cm thick, medium density, continuously compliant foam (Sunmate, Dynamic Systems, Leicester, NC) with feet together and arms crossed, looking straight ahead (31). All trials were performed with eyes closed. Trial 1 was performed without additional head movements (head still). Trial 2 was performed with head moving in yaw (shaking left/right about the vertical axis) in time to an oscillating sound played at 0.3 Hz. Trial 3 was performed with head moving in pitch (nodding up/ down about the intra-aural axis) in time to the 0.3 Hz sound. The dependent measure for each trial was the amount of time the subject could stand without opening the eyes, moving the arms, or taking a step.
Objective vestibular testing.
Subjects were tested on a standard clinical battery. For cervical vestibular evoked myogenic potentials (cVEMP), surface Ag/AgCl electrodes were placed over the bulk of the sternocleidomastoid, with the reference electrode over the sternum and the ground electrode on the forehead. Testing was performed at 500 Hz, maximum 100 dB nHL, 200 msec interval testing. Responses were considered abnormal if thresholds were < 70 dB or showed marked asymmetry ≥ 100%, and were unmeasurable if no response could be detected.
For all other tests, eye movements were recorded with infra-red video-oculography. They were tested on spontaneous nystagmus in the dark with eyes open and then with Dix-Hallpike maneuvers, supine roll tests and bi-thermal caloric tests with water at 30° C and 43°C. The cut-point for bi-thermal caloric testing was set at ≥20% unilateral weakness. Results of Dix-Hallpike maneuvers and supine roll tests were considered abnormal if three or more beats of nystagmus were recorded.
For Dix-Hallpike maneuvers, responses were categorized as either normal (no nystagmus), classical nystagmus, or non-classical nystagmus. Classical nystagmus included components that were vertical up-beating, horizontal ipsilaterally-beating, and torsional ipsilaterally beating, which began after a delay of a few seconds (32, 33). All other nystagmus in response to Dix-Hallpike testing was classified as non-classical.
Statistical methods
To compare the entire sample to previously published data on healthy control subjects (historical controls), only the 22 study subjects ages ≥ 70 years were used, to make the mean ages of the groups comparable. For the subgroup analyses comparing AD to MCI study subjects on balance and vestibular function tests, all study subjects were used. MRI data were available for only 11 study subjects, so all of those subjects were used. Study subjects and historical controls were compared by Chi-square/Fisher exact tests for grouped data, t-tests or ANOVA for continuous normally distributed data, and Wilcoxon rank sum test for non-normal data. P < 0.05 was considered significant. Statistical analyses were performed using SAS statistical software (version 9.4; Cary, NC).
Results
Comparison to historical healthy controls
When vestibular test results from subjects in this study were compared to historical controls (24), to make the groups most comparable we used only the 22 study subjects aged ≥ 70, mean age 77.8 years (SD 5.7), and 62 controls also aged ≥ 70 years, mean age 75.8 years (SD4.2), including 41 (65%) females, 21 (35%) males. The ages of the groups did not differ significantly.
Several differences were found. 1) Study subjects had significantly more spontaneous nystagmus than historical controls. 2) Study subjects had significantly more tests on cVEMP with responses that were too small to be detectable. 3) Study subjects had fewer normal and nonclassical responses than the historical controls but the differences were not statistically significant; study subjects had significantly more classical positive responses to Dix-Hallpike testing than historical controls. 4) Study subjects showed significantly more abnormalities on the summary scores indicating abnormal responses on any vestibular subtests, compared to historical controls. No significant differences were found between the two groups on bi-thermal caloric testing, TW, or any CTSIB trials. See Table 1 for details.
Table 1.
Test | Study subjects | Healthy controls | p-values |
---|---|---|---|
| |||
Bi-thermal caloric weakness, abnormal responses | 31.8 (N=7) | 16.1 (N=10) | 0.21 |
| |||
Dix-Hallpike responses | 0.005 | ||
a) normal | a) 59.1 (N=13) | a) 65.0 (N=39) | |
b) classical | b) 22.7 (N=5) | b) 1.7 (N=1) | |
c) nonclassical | c) 18.2 (N=4) | c) 33.3 (N=20) | |
| |||
cVEMP responses | 0.03 | ||
a) normal | a) 53.9 (N=14) | a) 76.7 (N=46) | |
b) abnormal but measurable | b) 0 | b) 0 | |
c) unmeasurable | c) 46.2 (N=12) | c) 23.3 (N=14) | |
| |||
Summary VNG score, abnormal | 95.5 (N=21) | 59.7 (N=37) | 0.002 |
| |||
Spontaneous nystagmus present | 90.91 (N=20) | 16.1 (N=1) | <0.001 |
| |||
TW eyes closed, abnormal | 47.6 (N=10) | 48.4 (N=30) | 0.99 |
| |||
CTSIB head still, abnormal | 72.7 (N=16) | 67.7 (N=42) | 0.70 |
| |||
CTSIB head yaw, abnormal | 86.4 (N=19) | 66.1 (41) | 0.10 |
| |||
CTSIB head pitch, abnormal | 45.4 (N=10) | 87.1 (N=54) | 0.52 |
Association between degree of cognitive impairment and test results
We performed analyses of the vestibular and balance tests from the entire cohort of 26 study subjects. For statistical analyses we divided the group into MCI or early AD, i.e., MMSE score < 22 (range 10–21), and moderate AD, MMSE score ≥ 22 (range 22–30) (34). Because we excluded patients with severe impairment, we chose an MMSE cut-point of 22, rather than 20, to maximize the contrast between those with relatively mild cognitive impairment and those with more advanced impairment. No significant differences were found between groups on any tests. See Table 2 for details.
Table 2.
Test | Mild MMSE score (N=15) | Moderate MMSE score (N=11) | P-values |
---|---|---|---|
| |||
Bi-thermal caloric test | P=0.99 | ||
a) Normal | 60.0% (9) | 63.6% (7) | |
b) Abnormal | 40.0% (6) | 36.4% (4) | |
| |||
Dix-Hallpike responses | P=0.65 | ||
a) normal | 13.3% (2) | 27.3% (3) | |
b) classical | 26.7% (4) | 18.2% (2) | |
c) nonclassical | 60.0% (9) | 54.6% (6) | |
| |||
cVEMP responses | P=0.95 | ||
a) Normal | 53.3% (8) | 54.6% (6) | |
b) Abnormal | 46.7% (7) | 45.5% (5) | |
| |||
Spontaneous nystagmus | P=0.99 | ||
a) Normal | 6.7% (1) | 9.1% (1) | |
b) Abnormal | 93.3% (14) | 90.9% (10) | |
| |||
TW eyes closed | P=0.46 | ||
a) Normal | 40.0% (6) | 54.6% (6) | |
b) Abnormal | 60.0% (9) | 45.4% (5) | |
| |||
CTSIB head still | P=0.99 | ||
a) Normal | 26.7% (N=4) | 18.2% (2) | |
b) Abnormal | 73.3% (N=11) | 81.8% (9) | |
| |||
CTSIB head yaw | P=0.99 | ||
a) Normal | 20.0% (3) | 18.2% (2) | |
b) Abnormal | 80.0% (12) | 81.8% (9) | |
| |||
CTSIB head pitch | P=0.99 | ||
a) Normal | 20.0% (3) | 18.2% (2) | |
b) Abnormal | 73.3% (12) | 81.8% (9) |
Comparison to ADL scores
Data from the PSMS and IADL scales were tested separately against the VNG summary scores and balance summary scores. These scales use ordinal scoring with lower scores closer to normal or independence. PSMS scores did not differ significantly by normal and abnormal balancer groups. IADL scores approached significance, p< 0.08, however: subjects with normal balance (N=3), mean IADL score = 10.3 (SD, 4.6); subjects with abnormal balance scores (N=23), mean IADL score = 17.6 (SD, 6.7). If the sample had been larger, the IADL scores in normal- and abnormal-balance groups would probably have differed significantly. PSMS and IADL scores did not differ significantly by normal- and abnormal-VNG summary score groups.
MRI results
All 11 study subjects for whom MRI data were available had decreased right-side hippocampus volumes. Some, but not all, had decreased left-side hippocampal volumes. Therefore, we used data only for the left sides. Because radiologic data can be analyzed by hand, i.e. manual segmentation, and by computer program, i.e. computer automated segmentation, we analyzed data using both types of analyses. As shown in Table 3, significant differences between subgroups with normal and reduced left hippocampus values were found only for cVEMP calculated with manual segmentation by the neuroradiologist. The values from computer automated segmentation for cVEMP approached significance.
Table 3.
Test (N for normal and abnormal test results) |
Mean (SD) hippocampal values | Mean Hippocampal volume MS P-values | Mean Hippocampal volume CAS P=values |
---|---|---|---|
| |||
Bi-thermal caloric tests | P=0.67 | P=0.66 | |
(Normal N=8, abnormal N=3) | |||
Normal MS | 1.22 (0.68) | ||
Abnormal MS | 1.43 (0.82) | ||
Normal CAS | 1.27 (0.47) | ||
1.14 (0.13) | |||
| |||
Dix-Hallpike maneuvers | P=0.028 | P=0.097 | |
(Normal N=3, abnormal classical N=4, abnormal nonclassical N=4) | |||
Normal MS | 1.35 (1.04) | ||
Abnormal classical MS | 1.12 (0.50) | ||
Abnormal nonclassical MS | 1.39 (0.73) | ||
Normal CAS | 1.16 (0.36) | ||
Abnormal classical CAS | 1.31 (0.42) | ||
1.21 (0.52) | |||
| |||
Abnormal nonclassical segmented | |||
| |||
cVEMP | P=0.86 | P=0.90 | |
(Normal N=4, abnormal N=7) | |||
Normal MS | 1.85 (0.67) | ||
Abnormal MS | 0.95 (0.47) | ||
Normal CAS | 0.96 (0.22) | ||
Abnormal CAS | 1.39 (0.42) | ||
| |||
Spontaneous nystagmus | P=0.78 | P=0.81 | |
(Normal scores, N=2; abnormal scores, N=9) | |||
Normal MS | 1.15 (0.62) | ||
Abnormal MS | 1.31 (0.73) | ||
Normal CAS | 1.17 (0.01) | ||
Abnormal CAS | 1.25 (0.45) | ||
| |||
Tandem walking | P=0.98 | P=0.55 | |
(Normal scores, N=7; abnormal scores, N=4) | |||
Normal MS | 1.29 (0.64) | ||
Abnormal MS | 1.27 (0.87) | ||
Normal CAS | 1.29 (0.47) | ||
Abnormal CAS | 1.13 (0.30) | ||
| |||
CTSIB, head still | P=0.98 | P=0.93 | |
| |||
(Normal scores, N=4; abnormal scores, N=7) | |||
Normal CSTIB MS | 1.27 (0.42) | ||
Abnormal CTSIB MS | 1.28 (0.66) | ||
Normal CTSIB CAS | 1.25 (0.52) | ||
Abnormal CTSIB CAS | 1.22 (0.37) | ||
| |||
CTSIB head yaw | P=0.62 | P=0.84 | |
(Normal scores, N=3; abnormal scores, N=8) | |||
Normal MS | 1.46 (0.91) | ||
Abnormal MS | 1.21 (0.64) | ||
Normal CAS | 1.28 (0.64) | ||
Abnormal CAS | 1.22 (0.34) | ||
| |||
CTSIB head pitch | P=0.62 | P=0.84 | |
Normal scores, N=3; abnormal scores, N=8) | |||
Normal MS | 1.46 (0.91) | ||
Abnormal MS | 1.21 (0.64) | ||
Normal CAS | 1.28 (0.64) | ||
Abnormal CAS | 1.22 (0.34) |
Discussion
Between groups findings
The results from the comparison between study subjects and historical controls suggest that study subjects had more vestibular pathology than healthy controls. The presence of significantly more spontaneous nystagmus in the study subjects indicates the occurrence of central pathology in people without vertigo, because spontaneous nystagmus is never normal. Classical Dix-Hallpike responses may indicate benign paroxysmal positional vertigo of the posterior semicircular canal (BPPV), although we cannot be sure from test results, alone. That canal is innervated by the inferior branch of the vestibular nerve, which projects to at least the medial and lateral vestibular nuclei (35, 36) and possibly other parts of the vestibular nuclear complex. The significant differences between study subjects and historical controls is a new finding, and might represent some susceptibility in these patients to the underlying causes of BPPV or some retrograde impairment in responses due to pathological changes in the vestibular nuclei.
The reason for the relatively large percentage of study subjects who had unmeasurable responses to cVEMP might mean one of two things. Either many subjects had conductive hearing loss or, as previous evidence suggests, these patients have impaired vestibulosaccular function. The saccule is innervated by part of the inferior branch of the vestibular nerve and is known to project to parts of the vestibular nuclei (37–39) although the exact pathways remain poorly understood. Thus, these responses might represent retrograde changes in the vestibular nerve due to AD-related changes in the vestibular nuclei. Alternatively, these changes may indicate some susceptibility to local vascular changes or to viral damage, an exploration of which is beyond the scope of this research.
An alternative possibility, i.e. a high prevalence of conductive hearing loss, is unlikely. No evidence indicates the occurrence of an unusual level of conductive hearing loss in this population. All subjects had otoscopy before testing and they had their ears cleaned by an otolaryngologist if cerumen occluded the tympanic membranes. Subjects who used hearing aids were able to hear adequately with them. All subjects had cervical range of motion within functional limits. No subjects had difficulty following instructions to lift their heads from the treatment for the few seconds of each test run.
Therefore, the cVEMP results probably indicate the absence of responses or responses so small that they were beyond the capability of the equipment to detect them without using unsafe levels of sound stimuli. This finding supports previous evidence of decreased cVEMP responses in this population (2). This finding is of more than just academic interest. Previous investigators have shown a relationship between decreased cVEMP responses in AD patients and decreased driving performance (40). Therefore, this finding has implications for public safety.
The lack of difference in bi-thermal caloric responses but the finding of differences in cVEMP is interesting. Responses to bi-thermal caloric testing are thought to come primarily from the lateral semicircular canal/ superior portion of the vestibular nerve, although saccular input does affect the response (41, 42). Responses from cVEMP are thought to be primarily saccular, and thus primarily through the inferior portion of the vestibular nerve. This finding may resolve the differences in the literature. Differences between AD patients and healthy controls have been reported on bi-thermal caloric tests (4) but other research has shown significant decrements on cVEMP compared to healthy controls (2).
We have confirmed the finding of differences in cVEMP between dementia and controls subjects but not the finding of bi-thermal caloric differences. A possible explanation is that the differences lie in the blood supply to the regions of the vestibular nuclei that receive projections from those parts of the vestibular nerve, such as the descending vestibular nucleus or the Y nucleus, or to the presence of other AD-related changes such as Beta-amyloid or the presence of neurofibrillary plaques and tangles. Looking into that question is beyond the scope of this study.
The finding of no differences between study subjects and historical controls on screening tests of balance, i.e. TW and CTSIB, is not surprising. All subjects were ambulatory. The literature does not strongly support the idea of balance problems in this population and does not indicate the occurrence of particular problems with peripheral neuropathy or musculoskeletal problems.
Between subgroup findings
We found no statistically significant differences between the AD and MCI subjects. Some differences may exist but the small sample size precluded finding them. On some tests, however, the differences may not exist. We have no reason to believe that we should have found differences on bi-thermal caloric testing, for example.
The finding that IADL scores approached differing between subjects with normal and abnormal balance is not surprising. Many instrumental skills require good balance and weight-shifting ability of the type that was tested in this study. Vestibular and balance impairments are known to cause ADL deficits in personal care skills that require mobility and balance skill, on specific mobility skills and on many IADL skills (43–46). Quite likely had the sample size been larger a significant difference would have been found. These data are important because they suggest the functional implications of vestibular impairments in the Alzheimer’s population.
MRI findings
In this study, most MRI findings were not statistically significant, probably due to the small sample size. The significant finding of MRI results for cVEMP with manual segmentation is new, however, and supports the behavioral finding of reduced cVEMP scores in this population. The finding with computer automated segmentation approached significance. The difference may be due to the greater clinical sensitivity of the neuroradiologist’s clinical judgement. This finding for cVEMP evidence supports previous findings in the literature and suggests that this relationship between vestibular input and hippocampal volume may underlie the spatial orientation deficits that have been found in AD patients (1).
Potential functional implications
The finding of vestibular impairments in some subjects has some functional implications. Patients with vestibular disorders have decreased independence in activities of daily living (ADLs), in self-care, mobility, and instrumental ADLs (43, 44, 47–53). These problems are caused by the spatial disorientation, balance problems, and blurred vision that are characteristic of vestibular disorders. Also, vestibular disorders have been reported to be associated with some executive function problems (54), although not to the same level as AD and MCI. AD and MCI are well-known to cause ADL deficits (55). Furthermore, patients with AD have increased problems with sleep. The finding of increased positive responses to Dix-Hallpike testing might indicate an increase in benign paroxysmal positional vertigo, which is known to affect sleep. Thus, the functional limitations caused by vestibular impairments may add to the ADL deficits that this population already has, decreasing their functional skills further.
Limitations of the study.
Due to difficulty recruiting participants during the COVID-19 pandemic, our sample size was relatively small for this highly variable population. We had planned on a larger sample but were unable to recruit more subjects for a study that did not include medically necessary tests. Therefore, this work should be considered a pilot study. A larger sample might have provided more statistically significant findings.
Because patients in the Alzheimer’s Disease Center are not required to have their MRI’s performed at our institution results were unavailable for more than half the cohort, thus yielding a very small sample. A larger sample may have yielded more significant results. Nevertheless, the finding with cVEMP is interesting, and would bear following up with larger samples. The quality of some MRI’s was adequate for clinical care but not for research. Some evidence suggests that vestibular input is greater to the dorsal hippocampus and dorsal dentate gyrus than to the ventral hippocampus and to CA1 (11). Therefore, we would have expected decreased hippocampal volumes in the dorsal hippocampus in subjects with abnormal cVEMP scores, but the reduced quality of the images did not allow for that level of refinement in the analyses.
Finally, the vestibular and balance tests were not performed at the same time as the MMSE and MRI. Those results might have changed over time. We used the most current test results available and were unable to obtain more current MMSE and MRI results.
Conclusion.
This study shows that patients with AD have increased frequency of positive findings on Dix-Hallpike testing but not on bi-thermal caloric testing, compared to age-matched controls. This study also suggests that they have reduced hippocampal volume, in the area that receives vestibular input.
Acknowledgements
Many thanks for technical support from Nathan Silver, Melody Fregia, Eveleen Darby and Aisha Ansari.
Supported by National Institutes of Health grant DC009031–10S1.
References
- 1.Wei EX, Oh ES, Harun A, Ehrenburg M, Agrawal Y. Vestibular loss predicts poorer spatial cognition in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. J Alzheimer’s Dis. 2018;61:995–1003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Harun A, Oh ES, Bigelow RT, Studenski S, Agrawal Y. Vestibular impairment in dementia. Otol Neurotol. 2016;37:1137–42. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Leung KK, Barnes J, Ridgway GR, Bartlett JW, Clarkson MJ, Macdonald K, et al. Automated cross-sectional and longitudinal hippocampal volume measurement in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Neuroimage. 2010;51:1345–59. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Nakamagoe K, Fujimiya S, Koganezawa T, Kadono K, Shimizu K, Fukizuka N, et al. Vestibular function impairment in Alzheimer’s disease. J Alzheimer’s Dis. 2015;47:185–96. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Leandri M, Cammisuli S, Cammarata S, Baratto L, Campbell J, Simonini M, et al. Balance features in Alzheimer’s disease and amnestic mild cognitive impairment. J Alzheimer’s Dis. 2009;16:113–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Cohen HS, Sangi-Haghpeykar H, Ricci NA, Kampangkaew J, Williamson RA. Utility of stepping, wallking and head impulses for screening patients for vestibular impairments. Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg. 2014;151:131–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Honaker JA, Boismier TE, Shepard NP, Shepard NT. Fukuda stepping test: sensitivity and specificity. J Am Acad Audiol. 2009;20:311–4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Honaker JA, Shepard NT. Performance of Fukuda stepping test as a function of the severity of caloric weakness in chronic dizzy patients. J Am Acad Audiol. 2012;23:616–22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Hitier M, Besnard S, Smith PF. Vestibular pathways involved in cognition. Front Integr Neurosci. 2014;8:59. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Agrawal Y, Smith PF, Rosenberg PB. Vestibular impairment, cognitive decline and Alzheimer’s disease: balancing the evidence. Aging Ment Health. 2020;24:705–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Hitier M, Sato G, Zhang Y-F, Besnard S, Smith PF. Effects of electrical stimulation of the rat vestibular labyrinth on c-Fos expression in the hippocampus. Neurosci Lett. 2018;677:60–4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Truchet B, Benoit A, Chaillan F, Smith PF, Philoxene B, Guillamin M, et al. Hippocampal LTP modulation and glutamatergic receptors following vestibular loss. Brain Struct Funct. 2019;224:699–711. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Tu S, Spiers HJ, Hodges JR, Piguet O, Hornberger M. Egocentric versus allocentric spatial memory in behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. J Alzheimer’s Dis. 2017;59:883–92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Hufner K, Hamilton DA, Kalla R, Stephan T, Glasauer S, Ma J, et al. Spatial memory and hippocampal volume in humans with unilateral vestibular deafferentation. Hippocampus. 2007;17:471–81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Previc FH. Vestibular loss as a contributor to Alzheimer’s disease. Med Hypotheses. 2013;80:360–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Stiles L, Zheng Y, Smith PF. The effects of electrical stimulation of the peripheral vestibular system on neurochemical release in the rat striatum. PLoS One. 2018;13(10):e0205869. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Abraham L, Potegal M, Miller S. Evidence for caudate nucleus involvement in an egocentric spatial task: return from passive transport. Physiol Psychol. 1983;11(1):11–7. [Google Scholar]
- 18.Ransmayr G, Benesch H, Nowakowski C, Kunig G, Heinstein H, Riederer P, et al. Neurofibrillary tangles without cell loss in the lateral vestibular nucleus of patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Neurosci Lett. 1994;177:11–4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Baloyannis SJ, Manolidis SL, Manolidis LS. Synaptic alterations in the vestibulocerebvellar system in Alzheimer’s disease — a Golgi and eletron microsope study. Acta Otolaryngologica. 2000;120:247–50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Lawton MP, Brody EM. Assessment of older people: self-maintaining and instrumental activities of daily living. Gerontologist. 1969;9:179–86. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.McKhann GM, Knopman DS, Chertkow H, Hyman BT, Jack CR Jr., Kawas CH, et al. The diagnosis of dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the National Institite on Aging - Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2011;7:263–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Petersen RC, Doody R, Kurtz A, Mohs RC, Morris JC, Rabins PV, et al. Current concepts in mild cognitive impairment. Arch Neurol. 2001;58:1985–92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Petersen RC, Smith GE, Waring SC, Ivnik RJ, Tangalos EG, Kokmen E. Mild cognitive impairment: clinical characterization and outcome. Arch Neurol. 1999;56:303–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Cohen HS, Plankey MW, Sangi-Haghpeykar H. Vestibular impairments on objective diagnositc tests in HIV+ women and control men and women. Laryngoscope. 2021;131:E2318–E22. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Jack CRj. Alliance for aging research AD biomarkers work group: structural MRI. Neurobiol Aging. 2011;32(Suppl 1 (0 1)):S48–S57. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Whitwell JL, Josephs KA, Murray ME, Kantarci K, Przybelski SA, Weigand SD, et al. MRI correlates of neurofibrillary tangle pathology at autopsy: a voxel-based morphometry study. Neurology. 2008;71:743–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Vemuri P, Whitwell JL, Kantarci K, Josephs KA, Parisi JE, Shiung MS, et al. Antemortem MRI based structural abnormality index (STAND) - scores correlated with postmortem Braak neurofibrillary tangle stage. Neuroimage. 2008;42:559–67. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.volBrain: An Online MRI Brain Volumetry System [Internet]. 2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 29.Sanchez-Benavides G, Gomez-Anson B, Sainz A, Vives Y, Defino M, Pena-Casanova J. Manual validation of FreeSurfer’s automated hippocampal segmentation in normal aging, mild cognitive impairment, and Alzheimers Disease subjects. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging. 2010;181:219–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Cohen HS, Stitz J, Sangi-Haghpeykar H, Williams SP, Mulavara AP, Peters BT, et al. Tandem walking as a quick screening test for vestibular disorders. Laryngoscope. 2018;128:1687–91. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Cohen HS, Mulavara AP, Stitz J, Sangi-Haghpeykar H, Williams SP, Peters BT, et al. Screening for vestibular disorders using the modified Clinical Test of Sensory Interaction and Balance and Tandem Walking with eyes closed. Otol Neurotol. 2019;40:658–65. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Dix MR, Hallpike CS. The pathology, symptomatology and diagnosis of certain common disorders of the vestibular system. Proc Royal Soc Med. 1952;45:341–54. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Coats AC. ENG examination technique. Ear Hearing. 1986;7:143–50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. Mini-mental state: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12:189–98. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Diaz C, Puelles L. Segmental analysis of the vestibular nerve and the efferencts of the vestibular complex. Anat Reco. 2019;302:472–84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Kushiro K, Bai R, Kitajima N, Sugita-Kitajima A, Uchino Y. Properties and axonal trajectories of posterior semicircular canal nerve-activated vestibulospinal neurons. Experimental Brain Resarch. 2008;191:257–64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Imagawa M, Graf W, Sato H, Suwa H, Isu N, Izumi R, et al. Morphology of single afferents of the saccular macular in cats. Neurosci Lett. 1998;240:127–30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Naito Y, Newman A, Lee WS, Beykirch K, Honrubia V. Projections of the individual vestibular end-organs in the brain stem of the squirrel monkey. Hear Res. 1995;87:141–56. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Maklad A, Kamel S, Wong EC, Fritzsch B. Development and organization of polarity-specific segregation of primary vestibular afferent fibers in mice. Cell Tissue Res. 2010;340:303–21. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Wei EX, Oh ES, Harun A, Ehrenburg M, Agrawal Y. Saccular impairment in Alzheimer’s disease is associated with driving difficulty. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2018;44:294–302. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Coats AC, Smith SY. Body position and the intensity of caloric nystagmus. Acta Otolaryngol. 1967;63:515–32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Cohen HS. Influence of otolith input on bithermal caloric responses: re-analyses of the data of Coats and Smith. Acta Otolaryngol. 2004;124:223–4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Cohen HS, Kimball KT, Adams AD. Application of the Vestibular Disorders Activities of Daily Living Scale. Laryngoscope. 2000;110:1204–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Cohen H. Vestibular rehabilitation reduces functional disability. Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg. 1992;107:638–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Cohen HS, Kimball KT. Increased independence and decreased vertigo after vestibular rehabilitation. Otolaryngology - Head and Neck Surgery. 2003;128:56–66. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Cohen HS, Kimball KT. Decreased ataxia and improved balance after vestibular rehabilitation. Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg. 2004;130:418–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Cohen HS. Disability in vestibular disorders. In: Herdman SJ, editor. Vestibular Rehabilitation. 2nd ed. Philadelphia: Davis; 2000. p. 373–86. [Google Scholar]
- 48.Cohen HS, Adams AD, Kimball KT, editors. An ADL evaluation for vestibular disorders. Twelfth International Congress of the World Federation of Occupational Therapy; 1998; Montreal, Canada. [Google Scholar]
- 49.Cohen HS, Kimball KT. Development of the Vestibular Disorders Activities of Daily Living Scale. Arch Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg. 2000;126:881–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Ricci NA, Aratani MC, Caovilla HH, Cohen HS, Ganança FF. Evaluation of properties of the Vestibular Disorders Activities of Daily Living Scale (Brazilian version) in an elderly population. Braz J Phys Ther. 2014;18:174–82. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Jacobson GP, Newman CW. The development of the Dizziness Handicap Inventory. Archives of Otolaryngology - Head and Neck Surgery. 1990;116:424–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Wei EX, Agrawal Y. Vestibular dysfunction and difficulty with driving: data from the 2001–200 National Health and Nutrition Examinaton Surveys. Front Neurol. 2017;8:557. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Cohen HS, Wells J, Kimball KT, Owsley C. Driving disability in dizziness. J Safety Res. 2003;34:361–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Smith PF. Why dizziness is likely to increase the risk of cognitive dysfunction and dementia in elderly adults. NZ Med J. 2020;133:112–27. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Altieri M, Garramone F, Santangelo G. Functional autonomy in dementia of the Alzheimer’s type, mild cognitive impairment, and healthy aging: a meta-analysis. Neurol Sci. 2021;42:1773–83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]