Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2022 Jun 22;11:e67573. doi: 10.7554/eLife.67573

Perceptual restoration fails to recover unconscious processing for smooth eye movements after occipital stroke

Sunwoo Kwon 1,2,3, Berkeley K Fahrenthold 3,4, Matthew R Cavanaugh 3,4, Krystel R Huxlin 2,3,4,, Jude F Mitchell 2,3,†,
Editors: Miriam Spering5, Chris I Baker6
PMCID: PMC9255960  PMID: 35730931

Abstract

The visual pathways that guide actions do not necessarily mediate conscious perception. Patients with primary visual cortex (V1) damage lose conscious perception but often retain unconscious abilities (e.g. blindsight). Here, we asked if saccade accuracy and post-saccadic following responses (PFRs) that automatically track target motion upon saccade landing are retained when conscious perception is lost. We contrasted these behaviors in the blind and intact fields of 11 chronic V1-stroke patients, and in 8 visually intact controls. Saccade accuracy was relatively normal in all cases. Stroke patients also had normal PFR in their intact fields, but no PFR in their blind fields. Thus, V1 damage did not spare the unconscious visual processing necessary for automatic, post-saccadic smooth eye movements. Importantly, visual training that recovered motion perception in the blind field did not restore the PFR, suggesting a clear dissociation between pathways mediating perceptual restoration and automatic actions in the V1-damaged visual system.

Research organism: Human

Introduction

Human observers use eye movements to bring targets of interest to central vision for detailed inspection. For moving targets, they do so effortlessly, with a combination of rapid saccades and smooth eye movements. When observers acquire a moving target via a saccade, they can continue to track it with smooth eye movements that match the eye’s velocity to the motion of the target (Buizza and Schmid, 1986; Lisberger and Westbrook, 1985; Lisberger et al., 1987; Rashbass, 1961; Tychsen and Lisberger, 1986). Those pursuit movements can show accurate velocity tracking, matched to the target from the moment of saccade landing, indicating motion integration and predictive planning for the target prior to the saccade (Gardner and Lisberger, 2001). Other smooth eye movements can occur involuntarily, as in ocular following, where the eyes drift after a saccade in response to the onset of wide-field motion (Gellman et al., 1990; Miles et al., 1986).

Recently, we found that saccade planning to peripheral, static apertures containing motion involuntarily generates predictive, smooth eye movements at the saccade target, even when there are no task demands to follow the target’s motion (Kwon et al., 2019). These involuntary smooth eye movements anticipate the post-saccadic motion in the target aperture, generating a low-gain, following response along the target’s motion direction, which we named the ‘post-saccadic following response’ (PFR). The PFR appears to reflect automatic, unconscious visual processing that occurs during a saccade target’s selection – that is, during pre-saccadic planning (Kwon et al., 2019). Previous studies of pre-saccadic attention have shown that perceptual enhancements for the saccade target are automatic and obligatory (Deubel and Schneider, 1996; Kowler et al., 1995; Rolfs and Carrasco, 2012; Rolfs et al., 2011), and involve selection of target features such as spatial frequency and orientation (Li et al., 2016), as well as motion features (White et al., 2013), which can drive smooth eye movements. Thus, the PFR may represent an automatic consequence of attentional selection for the motion target prior to the saccade; alternatively, it may play a role in priming the motor system for subsequent tracking of that target. Critically for the current study, the automatic selection of stimulus motion during pre-saccadic attention results in PFR, which we will use as a read-out of the motion processing for smooth eye movements at specific peripheral locations. Both voluntary and involuntary smooth eye movements are thought to rely on processing of stimulus motion mediated through neural pathways in the middle temporal (MT) area (Bakst et al., 2017; Mustari et al., 2009; Newsome et al., 1985; Nuding et al., 2008). Area MT receives strong cortical inputs, routed through primary visual cortex (area V1), but it also receives direct input from sub-cortical centers, which bypass V1 (Glickstein et al., 1980; Maunsell and van Essen, 1983; Rodman et al., 1989; Sincich et al., 2004; Tamietto and Morrone, 2016; Ungerleider et al., 1984; Van Essen et al., 1981). To what extent these different routes of information that are transferred to MT contribute to voluntary and involuntary, smooth eye movements, and to perception, remains to be fully elucidated. Notably, prior studies have suggested that motion pathways driving involuntary smooth eye movements differ from those mediating perception (Glasser and Tadin, 2014; Price and Blum, 2014; Simoncini et al., 2012; Spering and Carrasco, 2012; Spering et al., 2011). As such, the fact that MT receives inputs from sub-cortical centers and from other cortical areas (via V1) prompted the hypothesis that sub-cortical pathways to MT may support smooth eye movements while conscious visual motion perception relies predominantly on input routed via V1 (Spering et al., 2011). Damage to V1 as a result of unilateral occipital stroke offers a unique opportunity to test this hypothesis in humans. Indeed, unilateral V1-strokes cause a loss of conscious visual perception in the contralateral visual hemifield (Smith, 1962; Teuber et al., 1960), but sub-cortical projections to MT are generally spared. Importantly, these projections are thought to underlie the preservation of unconscious residual abilities such as blindsight (Mazzi et al., 2019; Sanchez-Lopez et al., 2019; Tamietto and Morrone, 2016; Weiskrantz et al., 1974). A key role of MT in blindsight has also been inferred from the particular stimulus properties needed to elicit blindsight: visual targets presented in the blind field have to be relatively large, coarse, moving, or flickering (Weiskrantz et al., 1995), containing high luminance contrasts, low spatial frequencies, and high temporal frequencies (Sahraie et al., 2008) – stimulus properties that elicit strong responses from MT neurons (Born and Bradley, 2005; Movshon and Newsome, 1996). However, the impact of V1 damage on unconscious, motion-dependent visual processes used to guide smooth eye movements, such as the PFR, has not been investigated.

Moreover, although visual training can restore conscious visual motion perception in parts of the blind field of V1-stroke patients (Cavanaugh et al., 2019; Cavanaugh et al., 2015; Das et al., 2014; Elshout et al., 2016; Huxlin et al., 2009; Saionz et al., 2020; Vaina et al., 2014), we do not know how effectively patients can use such restored percepts to guide actions. Here, we used a cued, saccade task to demonstrate remarkably preserved ability of trained, V1-stroke patients to correctly target motion-containing peripheral stimuli presented in both their intact and blind fields. However, by continuously tracking eye movements, we were also – for the first time – able to capture the impact of V1 damage on the PFR. Our unique data reveal a key role for V1 in this unconscious, automatic, oculomotor behavior. By the same token, they provide new insights into the likely neural pathways mediating restored, conscious, motion perception after V1 damage versus those involved in the predictive processing necessary for a normal PFR.

Results

To investigate how V1 damage impacts unconscious motion processing for smooth eye movements, we contrasted the PFR for saccades made to motion stimuli placed in the intact and the blind fields of eleven cortically blind (CB) V1-stroke patients (Figure 1) after visual restoration training at some of these blind field locations.

Figure 1. Occipital lesions, Humphrey visual field maps, and post-saccadic following response (PFR) testing locations.

Figure 1.

Single radiographic images through the brains of each V1-stroke participant (N=11), illustrating region(s) of occipital damage (red arrows), shown with right brain hemispheres on image right. The location, size, and shape of visual stimuli presented during the PFR testing protocol are indicated by colored circles superimposed on initial 24-2 Humphrey visual field maps acquired for the tested eye in each case. Red circles: blind field testing locations; blue circles: intact field locations; OS: left eye; OD: right eye; DWI: diffusion-weighted imaging; FLAIR: T2-weighted fluid-attenuated inversion recovery; CT: computed tomography.

We identified four optimal equi-eccentric testing locations in the intact and blind fields of each participant, assessed from Humphrey perimetry (Figure 1), and measured both the PFR and motion integration thresholds using random dot stimuli (Figure 2A) at these locations. Our main goal was to assess how PFR tracked with perception in blind and intact regions of the visual field, rather than specifically at trained, blind field locations. As such, because PFR testing locations had to be within a certain range of eccentricities, and mirror symmetric across the four visual field quadrants, they did not always fall directly on all trained, blind field location. The tight location and task specificity of training-induced global motion recovery in the blind field (Huxlin et al., 2009; Das et al., 2014; Saionz et al., 2020) allowed us to attain a large range of perceptual thresholds at blind field PFR test locations, against which to plot PFR gain.

Figure 2. Experimental paradigms for measuring motion perception and oculomotor functions.

Figure 2.

(A) Trial sequence for assessing global motion perception: trials started with a fixation period of 1000 ms, followed by appearance of a random dot stimulus either in the blind or intact field for 500 ms. Dots moved globally to the right or left, with a range of directions defined an adaptive staircase. On each trial, subjects were asked to report the stimulus’ global direction of motion by pressing the left or right arrow keys on a keyboard. They received auditory feedback on the correctness of each response. (B) Trial sequence for assessing oculomotor behavior: each trial started with a variable fixation period after which, participants were presented with four equi-eccentric motion apertures and a spatial cue at fixation. Dot motion apertures were Gaussian-enveloped and contained 100% coherent motion along a randomly assigned direction (clockwise or counter-clockwise) in each aperture, that was tangential to the center-out saccade to the aperture. The spatial cue (50 ms) indicated a peripheral target aperture toward which the participant was instructed to initiate a saccade as fast as possible. In half the trials, the dot motion stimuli persisted for 500 ms, and were thus present upon saccade offset. In remaining trials, stimuli disappeared during saccade flight, such that no stimulus motion was present at the fovea upon saccade landing.

Global motion perception at PFR tested locations was assessed by measuring normalized direction range (NDR) thresholds (Table 1; additional details in Materials and methods). In brief, at each location of interest, patients were asked to perform a two-alternative, direction discrimination (left versus right) task using random dot stimuli in which the range of dot directions was varied in a 3:1 staircase procedure, from 0° (coherent motion) to 360° (random motion). We computed direction range thresholds using all trials in a given session, by fitting a Weibull function, which defined the direction range level at which performance reached 75% correct. Direction range thresholds were then normalized to the maximum possible range of motion of 360° (see Materials and methods) such that they ranged from 0 (good integration performance) to 1 (poor integration performance, or unmeasurable threshold). All participants in our sample obtained NDR thresholds at or below 0.3 for their intact visual fields and the next increment in the staircase procedure fell at 0.4. Thus, we set the upper limit of the normal threshold range at 0.35 – half way between those values.

Table 1. Demographics and global motion integration thresholds in retrained, V1-stroke participants.

M, male; F, female; NDR, normalized direction range (low value = good performance, 1 = bad performance/no measurable threshold). Each NDR threshold denotes performance measured at a single blind or intact field location in each patient (see Figure 1 for positioning of these locations relative to the pre-training Humphrey visual field). Pre-training NDR thresholds were always 1 in blind fields.

Subject Sex Age (years) Time post-stroke (months) Blind field NDR thresholds Intact field NDR thresholds
CB1 F 27 65.0 1 0.3
CB2 F 68 24.8 0.2 0.2
CB3 F 57 48.6 0.7, 0.8 0.3
CB4 M 66 32.2 0.5 0.2, 0.2, 0.2
CB5 M 54 52.2 1 0.3, 0.3
CB6 M 79 22.7 1, 0.1 0.1, 0.2
CB7 M 53 36.8 1 0.3
CB8 M 52 65.5 1, 0.2 0.1, 0.2
CB9 F 65 56.5 0.3 0.1, 0.1, 0.02
CB10 F 31 66.8 0.3 0.2
CB11 F 43 6.0 0.1 0.3, 0.2, 0.2

In the blind fields, depending on whether the tested locations overlapped with a trained location, performance was either at chance or had improved to measurable and sometimes near-normal NDR thresholds, assessed in each patient’s own intact visual field (Table 1). The end result was a set of 14 blind field locations across 11 patients, where perceptual thresholds ranged from unmeasurable (reflecting inability to do the task) to near-normal.

Oculomotor behavior was measured using a cued-saccade task for motion stimuli at four peripheral locations (colored circles in Figure 1; Figure 2B). Of particular note, the motion direction inside the apertures was irrelevant to the task and subjects were not asked to track it or report it. Additionally, in half the trials, the motion stimulus disappeared during saccade flight to disambiguate the contributions of pre-saccadic motion processing to the PFR.

Basic saccade behavior of V1-stroke participants – accuracy and latency

Stroke participants were generally able to use central spatial cues at fixation to plan saccades to peripheral aperture locations. We first quantified saccade accuracy in a binary fashion by whether saccade end-points fell within the diameter of the stimulus aperture. We then also assessed spatial accuracy by the mean absolute error in position of the saccade end-point from the aperture center. A saccade was labelled ‘correct’ when it fell less than 3.5° from the saccade target center within 90 ms of the eye leaving the fixation window. Stroke participants made slightly more correct saccades to targets in their intact field (mean 96.8% ± 3.7%) than to those in their blind field (mean 89.4% ± 15.5%), likely reflecting more reliable target identification in the intact field (t42=2.5078, p=0.016, BF = 3.4414). However, when correctly selecting targets in their blind fields, there was high spatial accuracy of saccades to those targets, as measured by the location of the end-points relative to the aperture center. Specifically, stroke participants had a mean absolute landing error relative to the stimulus center of 1.59°±0.07° (SEM) in the intact field and 1.59°±0.08° (SEM) in the blind field, which was not significantly different (t10=0.1946, p=0.8496, BF = 0.3024). The latency of saccades was also similar for blind and intact field targets (intact field latencies: 360±45 ms; blind field latencies: 382±57 ms; t42=−1.3593, p=0.181, BF = 0.6472).

Finally, we compared saccade accuracy and latency of stroke patients to those of visually intact controls from a previous study (Kwon et al., 2019). Saccade accuracies in the intact fields of stroke patients were slightly impaired compared to those of visually intact controls (t36=1.5634, p=0.1267, BF = 0.8863) who exhibited saccade accuracy of 98.7% ± 0.8% as compared to 96.8% ± 3.7% for stroke patients. However, stroke patients had significantly longer (t36=−5.4737, p<0.0001, BF > 1000) saccade latencies of 360±45 ms in their intact fields, compared to visually intact controls, whose latencies averaged 260±10 ms (Kwon et al., 2019). We consider several possible causes for these differences in the Discussion, including age, and challenges specific to saccade planning in the presence of a blind field.

Predictive oculomotor behavior in the intact field of V1-stroke participants

In intact portions of their visual fields, stroke patients’ post-saccadic smooth eye movements reflected the direction of target motion of the pre-saccadic stimulus immediately upon saccade offset. In a typical trial, a saccade made to a target aperture in the intact field exhibited a smooth drift in eye position from the saccade end-point along the direction of target motion (see example for a single stroke patient in Figure 3A). We quantified the time course of the drift in eye position by computing the eye velocity projected along the direction of target motion, where positive values reflect following of motion. We term this the PFR velocity. Across patients, we observed a net positive PFR velocity (Figure 3C). By including a stimulus manipulation in which the motion target disappeared during saccade flight, we were able to confirm that the PFR velocity was driven exclusively by pre-saccadic motion in the peripheral aperture. Within the first 100 ms after saccade offset (the ‘open-loop’ period) the PFR velocity did not differ depending on whether the stimulus remained present after the saccade (red trace in Figure 3C) or if it was removed during saccade flight (blue trace in Figure 3C). By including trials where we removed the stimulus in-flight, we could eliminate direct post-saccadic stimulation of motion at the fovea, and thus isolate the predictive, ‘open-loop’ component of the PFR velocity. After 100 ms from saccade offset, the presence or absence of foveal motion did influence post-saccadic eye movements. Specifically, the PFR velocity continued to increase along the target motion direction in the foveal-motion-present condition (red trace in Figure 3C), whereas it decreased when no stimulus was present upon saccade landing (blue trace in Figure 3C).

Figure 3. Oculomotor behavior in perceptually trained, 11 V1-stroke participants.

(A) Eye movement traces to a cued target in the intact field (white background) of a single V1-stroke patient in a stimulus absent condition (i.e. with no foveal motion upon saccade landing at the cued target). Small, connected black dots denote the raw eye movement sampled from our eye tracker; the green circle represents the electronic window around the fixation spot; random dot stimuli were presented inside the four dashed circles and their global motion direction is indicated by large arrows inside each circle. Note the accurate saccade to the target center, and how the eye follows the pre-saccadic target motion direction (red arrow) upon saccade landing. (B) Raw eye movement traces to a cued target in the blind field (gray background) of a single V1-stroke patient in a stimulus absent condition. Note successful saccade landing onto the cued target but how the eye fails to follow the pre-saccadic target motion direction. Labelling conventions as in A. (C) Eye velocity traces for saccades to intact portions of the visual field, averaged across intact field locations per patient then averaged across all 11 stroke patients. In half the trials, stimuli were present upon saccade landing, resulting in foveal motion (red trace). In the remaining trials, stimuli were absent – that is, there was no foveal motion upon saccade landing on the target (blue trace). Error bars represent ± 2 SEM across subjects. Average eye velocities were projected along the target motion direction time-locked prior to the saccade onset (−100 to 0 ms) and offset (0 to 200 ms), such that positive values reflected motion consistent with the stimulus, while negative values reflected motion opposite. (D) Eye velocity traces for saccades to blind portions of the visual field, averaged across blind field locations per patient and then averaged across all 11 stroke patients (same conventions as in A). Error bars represent ± 2 SEM across subjects. Note the near-zero eye movement velocity during the ‘open-loop’ period, reflecting the lack of post-saccadic following response (PFR).

Figure 3.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1. Post-saccadic following response (PFR) gain in the intact and blind field of V1-stroke patients during the foveally-guided period.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1.

When stimulus motion was presented foveally after the saccade on trials where the stimulus remained present, patients exhibited following movements for both intact and blind visual fields. Following was evidenced by a positive PFR gain that emerged after a visuomotor delay of about 100 ms (Figure 3D, red curves). To quantify the smooth following, PFR gain was computed for each patient measured during the ‘foveally guided period’, which was defined as 100–200 ms after the saccade offset for motion targets presented in cortically blind (CB) patients’ intact and blind fields (N=11 patients). Individual dots represent the mean PFR gains for each participant. Error bars represent ±2 SEM across subjects. The PFR gain is positive and significant for both the intact (18.6% gain, CI95 = [18.5%, 18.6%], t10=4.2314, p=0.0017, BF = 25.4627) and the blind fields (15.0% gain, CI95 = [14.9%, 15.0%], t10=8.4130, p<0.0001, BF > 1000) but no difference between intact and blind fields is found in this later period (t10=1.2041, p=0.2563, BF = 0.5355). Thus, CB patients generate smooth, following eye movements for stimulus motion at the fovea after a visual response delay. The data suggests that occipital stroke patients retain functional, smooth following eye movements at the fovea.

For subsequent analyses, we focused on the ‘open-loop’ period (i.e. within 100 ms after saccade offset), as the PFR velocity in this period depends on pre-saccadic motion information accumulated from the peripheral target aperture (Kwon et al., 2019). The average velocity of these open-loop following responses, both for stimulus-present and -absent trials, was small in magnitude relative to the stimulus speed (10 °/s), ranging from 5% to 15% relative velocity gain. These lower gain responses are consistent with an involuntary following response, such as ocular following (Gellman et al., 1990; Miles et al., 1986), rather than voluntary pursuit of a target. Overall, the pattern of oculomotor behavior in the intact field of stroke patients was highly similar in its time course and magnitude to that previously measured in visually intact controls (Kwon et al., 2019).

Visually intact controls in our prior study had a net positive PFR gain in the open-loop epoch that differed significantly from zero (t7=4.31, p=0.004, BF = 14.2833 – dark gray bar in Figure 4A). PFR gain in the intact field of stroke patients (white bar in Figure 4A, 15.7% gain, CI95 = [0.1564, 0.1576]) was not significantly different from PFR gain in our prior, visually intact controls (t17=−0.577, p=0.5715, BF = 0.4578), and also showed a net positive effect that differed significantly from zero (t10=5.4058, p<0.0001, BF > 100). Finally, we noted a significant correlation (R2=0.351, F18=9.73, p=0.0059, BF = 7.3164) between perceptual performance measured by NDR thresholds, and the magnitude of the PFR (white circles, Figure 4B) in the intact field of stroke participants.

Figure 4. Post-saccadic following response (PFR) gain in the intact and blind field of V1-stroke participants.

(A) Plot of mean PFR gain in eight visually intact controls (Kwon et al., 2019), and in 11 V1-stroke patients’ intact and blind fields during the open-loop period. The PFR gain is represented as a proportion of pursuit gain along the target motion direction (1=perfect following, 0=no following, –1=following in the opposite direction). Individual dots represent the mean PFR for each participant. There was no significant difference in PFR gain between the intact fields of stroke patients and visually intact controls. PFR gain approximated 0 for saccades to targets in stroke patients’ blind fields. Individual gray lines represent PFR gains in intact and blind fields belonging to the same patients. Each error bars represent ± 2 SEM across subjects. (B) PFR gain for individual intact field locations (open circles, N=20) and (C) blind field locations (gray circles, N=14) as a function of the normalized direction range (NDR) threshold measured at each location in stroke participants. The solid line and dashed line represent the line of best fit for intact and blind field PFR gain as a function of NDR threshold. Within intact regions of the visual field, there was a negative correlation between this global motion threshold and PFR gain: the lower the threshold (i.e. the better the motion perception), the higher the PFR gain. This relationship was lost in the blind field of stroke patients, with no significant correlation between NDR thresholds and PFR gain – the latter remaining abnormally low. Error bars represent 2 standard deviations per visual field locations.

Figure 4.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1. Post-saccadic following response (PFR) gain as a function of normalized direction range (NDR) thresholds.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1.

An exponential fit to the intact visual fields was computed to describe the dependence of PFR on the NDR threshold performance (goodness of fit: SSE = 0.1717, R2=0.3502, and RMSE = 0.0977). The plot shows PFR gains for individual intact field locations (open circles, N=20) and blind field locations (gray circles, N=14) as a function of the NDR threshold measured at each location in stroke patients. The bold red line represents the model fit based on the intact visual field data. The dashed red lines represent the confidence interval boundaries at 95% and the error bars represent ± 2 SD. Out of a total of six blind field data points, five lie outside these confidence intervals, and also demonstrate respective confidence intervals on their PFR gain that are overlapping or below zero. The probability that five of six points would lie outside the 95% confidence intervals, assuming they came from the same distribution as intact fields is highly improbable (p ≤ 1.79 × 10–6, binomial test). A single point, which fell within the range of the intact visual fields, was unusual in other ways. Unlike other blind field locations in the study, this point (Figure 1, CB6, lower left quadrant) did exhibit significant detection performance in Humphrey visual field tests (spared Humphrey visual field performance – i.e. luminance detection), even though global motion discrimination at that location was impaired and thus selected for training (see Materials and methods). However, this point was included to be consistent and more conservative in the analyses. Nonetheless, even when this potential outlier was included, there exists a quantitative evidence to support a distinction in PFR gain between intact and blind fields within the range where normal motion discrimination performance (NDR <0.35) has recovered.
Figure 4—figure supplement 2. Post-saccadic following response (PFR) gain as a function of normalized direction range (NDR) thresholds pooled per V1-stroke patients.

Figure 4—figure supplement 2.

The plots show PFR gain for individual intact field locations (Panel A, open circles) and blind field locations (Panel B, gray circles) as a function of the NDR threshold measured per each stroke patient (N=11). The error bars represent ± 2 SEM. The solid line and dashed line represent the line of best fit for intact and blind field PFR gain as a function of NDR threshold. After pooling all the fields per subject, the previous the trend in which the lower NDR is correlated with higher PFR gain in the intact fields (R2=0.568, F9=11.8334, p=0.0074, BF = 8.0855) persists and there is no significant correlation between NDR and PFR gain in the blind fields (R2=0.038, F9=0.3552, p=0.5659, BF = 2.650). Further, for NDR <0.35, the correlation between PFR and performance remains significant for intact fields (R2=0.568, F9=11.8334, p=0.0074, BF = 8.0855) but not for blind fields (R2=0.5406, F2=2.3538, p=0.2647, BF = 0.7678). When the slopes from each linear regression with respect to intact fields and blind fields under NDR <0.35 were compared, the slopes (–1.012 for intact and –0.1845 for blind) were significantly different from each other (t11=−2.6039, p=0.0245). Thus, the main findings hold when visual fields are pooled per participant to insure independence.
Figure 4—figure supplement 3. Luminance contrast-dependence of the post-saccadic following response (PFR) gain in visually intact controls.

Figure 4—figure supplement 3.

A logistic function (sigmoid fit) was used to represent the best fit to the average PFR data across contrasts. Visually intact participants (N=7) performed the same task as described in the main PFR experiment except that in each session, the target aperture was presented with a fixed stimulus luminance contrast between 2.5% and 50%. Large black symbols and error bars represent ± 2 SEM of PFR gain across participants. Small gray dots represent individual participant PFR gains at each luminance level. PFR gain increased steeply with stimulus contrast, starting around 5–7.5% contrast and reaching saturation quickly between 12.5% and 25% contrast (R2=0.9862, SSE = 0.000293, RMSE = 0.0086). This is comparable to neuronal responses in cortical area middle temporal (MT) which are sensitive to low stimulus contrasts and saturate in response at roughly 10% or higher contrasts (Heuer and Britten, 2002; Kohn and Movshon, 2003; Sclar et al., 1990). Next, to what extent behavior of cortically blind (CB) patients in the blind field might reflect a response to a stimulus of effectively reduced contrast was considered. For comparison, when CB patients performed the same test in their intact field with 100% contrast, the observed mean PFR was 0.1448, a value that matched the performance of intact controls for contrasts >25% (Figure 4—figure supplement 4). The lowest contrast for which the PFR was significantly different from zero in controls was 7.5% (t6=0.3150, p=0.0202). Thus, if recovered motion perception in the blind field resembles a lower-contrast stimulus representation, then we would estimate its contrast to be less than 7.5%.
Figure 4—figure supplement 4. Saccade angular deviations in the intact and blind field of cortically blind (CB) participants.

Figure 4—figure supplement 4.

Previous studies showed that location of an aperture is perceived as shifted along the direction of target motion contained in the aperture (De Valois and De Valois, 1991; Kwon et al., 2015; Ramachandran and Anstis, 1990). This reflects a perceptual mis-localization error along the target motion direction that also influences saccade programming by causing saccade end-points to be shifted along the target motion (Kosovicheva et al., 2014). Consistent with previous studies, another earlier study in visually intact controls found the position of saccade end-points to be displaced along the direction of dot motion contained in a peripheral target aperture (Kwon et al., 2019). Thus, like the post-saccadic following response (PFR), saccade end-points are also influenced by pre-saccadic selection of target motion, providing another measure of the predictive influence of target motion. A previous study also found that saccade end-points provide a measure that correlates well with perception (Kosovicheva et al., 2014). Thus, the deviations of saccade end-points were analyzed to investigate if they were biased by the direction of motion in both the blind and intact fields of our CB participants, and whether restoration of motion perception in the blind field influenced these saccade parameters. For each saccade, the angle of the line from fixation to the saccade end-point relative to the line from fixation to the center of the target aperture was computed. Positive angular deviations were interpreted to reflect a bias along the target motion. The mean saccade angular deviations in 8 visually intact controls (from Kwon et al., 2019), and for motion targets presented in 11 CB patients’ intact and blind fields were computed as shown in the figure. Individual dots represent the mean saccade angular deviations for each participant. Error bars represent ± 2 SEM across subjects.The visually intact controls (from Kwon et al., 2019) showed a net positive saccade angular deviations that differed significantly from 0 (t7=11.53, p<0.001 – leftmost gray bar). This was also observed in CB patients, in intact portions of their visual fields (3.33°, CI95 = [3.32°, 3.34°], t10=6.0170, p<0.0001, BF > 100 – white bar). In fact, there were no significant differences between saccade end-point deviations between the two groups (t17=−0.7607, p=0.4573, BF = 0.4982). In the blind field of CB participants, saccade angular deviations were smaller than in their intact fields (t10=3.3586, p=0.0073, BF = 7.8454 – light gray bar), but unlike the PFRs, they were greater than 0 (1.12°, CI95 = [1.11°, 1.13°], t10=3.6538, p=0.044, BF = 11.7368), providing positive evidence of pre-saccadic motion integration within the blind field. Thus, occipital stroke does not abolish motion-induced perceptual shifts reflected by saccade targeting.
Figure 4—figure supplement 5. Experimental paradigm for measuring oculomotor functions with a single saccade target.

Figure 4—figure supplement 5.

To test for deficits in attention allocation into the blind field, a control experiment was conducted in two new participants. This experiment tested whether post-saccadic following response (PFR) gain might be recovered when competing distractors in the visual field were removed. This control was designed to rule out the possibility that attention impairments with respect to the blind field might emerge because attention is preferentially pulled/allocated toward stimuli that appear within intact fields, due to their higher saliency. (A) Trial sequence of assessing oculomotor behavior for a single saccade target without distractors. The paradigm followed the main experimental sequence as defined in Figure 2B except only a single target that matched the spatial cue was presented at every trial. A central line cue directed patients for where to saccade, and as in the main experiment, their saccades remained accurate for both intact (98.60% ± 1.15%) and blind field (98.72% ± 0.24%) locations. (B) PFR gain across two patients’ intact and blind fields for the four aperture and single target experiments. The open symbols represent the intact field data while filled symbols represent the blind field data. The circles represent the four apertures experiment and the triangles represent the single aperture experiment. The color (gray or black) represents each participant. For intact visual fields, each data point represents the average across the three intact fields. The error bars denote the mean ± 2 SEM. There was no measurable PFR in the blind fields (t3=−1.8996, p=0.1537, BF = 1.1120), while there was robust PFR in the intact fields (t3=3.7698, p=0.0327, BF = 3.1780). PFR in intact fields was significantly larger than in their blind fields (t3=3.9793, p=0.0284, BF = 6.8888), and the blind fields still had PFR that was not significantly different from zero (t3=-1.8996, p=0.1537, BF = 1.1120). (C) PFR gain as a function of NDR thresholds in each individual visual field location. The solid line and dashed line represent the line of best fit for intact and blind field PFR gain as a function of NDR threshold. Note, there are more data points because individual intact fields are shown across NDR rather than averaged as a single point. There remained a weak correlation between PFR and normalized direction range (NDR) threshold for intact fields (R2=0.2818, F10=3.9243, p=0.0757, BF = 1.0577) when compared with the blind fields (R2=0.0010, F2=0.0019, p=0.9690, BF = 0.3368). Thus, removing competing distractors in the other three aperture locations had little impact on the PFR suggesting the deficit was sensory in nature rather than involving a misallocation of attention, indicating that attention allocation appears to be intact in V1-stroke patients.
Figure 4—figure supplement 6. Post-saccadic following response (PFR) gain in the intact and blind field of V1-stroke patients driven by motion stimuli in the distractor apertures.

Figure 4—figure supplement 6.

As a final control analysis, the control analyses was performed to test what extent PFR was driven by the motion in distractor apertures. If the motion in the other three apertures produced any measurable influence on the PFR, then one might expect that influence to be stronger when saccades were made into the blind field because attention could not be properly allocated to its location, and instead would be allocated to intact field distractor locations with more salient motion targets. To test for this we repeated our PFR analyses but instead projected it onto each of the distractor aperture’s motion directions, averaging across all three of the distractor apertures. Plot of mean PFR gains driven by motion in distractor apertures when cortically blind (CB) patients made a saccade either into an intact or blind field. PFR gains driven on average by the other three distractor apertures. Error bars represent ± 2 SEM across subjects. Motion in distractor apertures contributed to a weak PFR gain when saccades were performed to a target in an intact visual field (6.85%, CI95 = [6.81%, 6.88%], t10=3.4926, p=0.0058, BF = 9.4228), but for saccades made into the blind field there was no measurable PFR gain for distractor locations (1.16%, CI95 = [1.14%, 1.19%], t10=0.8594, p=0.4102, BF = 0.4054), which does not support the hypothesis that greater attention was allocated to distractors for saccades made into the blind field. Although the PFR gains were not significantly different when saccades were made into the intact or blind fields (t10=2.0156, p=0.0715, BF = 1.3046). Thus, analysis of PFR for distractor apertures does not support that attention was mis-allocated for saccades made into the blind field. It seems unlikely that the lack of PFR in the blind field reflects an impairment to engage pre-saccadic attention.

Predictive oculomotor behavior in the blind field of V1-stroke participants

Although saccades landed correctly on target stimuli in the blind field of our stroke participants ~89% of the time, post-saccadic eye movements differed dramatically from those in the same participants’ intact field. Specifically, in the open-loop period, the eyes no longer moved along the direction of motion in the target (see example in Figure 3B) – in other words, there was no positive PFR velocity. This pattern was reflected in the average PFR velocity across all 11 stroke patients’ blind fields (Figure 3D), irrespective of visual rehabilitation training. In contrast to oculomotor behavior when making a saccade to targets in their intact fields, stroke patients did not show any positive PFR velocity within the first 20–100 ms after saccade offset (‘open-loop’ period), whether the motion stimulus was present (red trace) or absent (blue trace) post-saccadically (Figure 3D).

Beyond 100 ms from the saccade offset, post-saccadic foveal motion – when present – was sufficient to drive an ocular following response (red trace, Figure 3D). The PFR during this latter foveally-guided period (100–200 ms) was driven by foveal motion for blind fields as much as the intact fields (Figure 3—figure supplement 1). PFR gain was positive and significant both for the intact fields (18.6% gain, CI95 = [18.5%, 18.6%], t10=4.2314, p=0.0017, BF = 25.4627) and the blind fields (15.0% gain, CI95 = [14.9%, 15.0%], t10=8.4130, p<0.0001, BF > 1000) with no significant difference between them (t10=1.2041, p=0.2563, BF = 0.5355). This is an important observation, as it confirms that post-saccadic ocular following remained functional in these patients. Only the predictive component during the open-loop period was abnormal, reflected by the absence of the PFR velocity in stimulus-absent trials (blue trace, Figure 3D). Consistent with these observations, PFR gain in the blind field of our stroke patients (light gray bar in Figure 4A, 0.45% gain, CI95 = [0.43%, 0.47%]) was significantly lower than PFR gain in their intact field (t10=5.6451, p=0.0001, BF = 150), and was not significantly different from zero (t10=0.3796, p=0.7122, BF = 0.3166).

Since portions of the blind fields of stroke patients underwent training that restored global motion perception, we next asked if such training, when it was coincident with locations tested on the PFR task, restored PFR gains. Perceptual recovery did not restore PFR: there was no significant correlation between NDR and PFR gains (R2=0.0655, F12=0.841, p=0.377, BF = 0.3020) in the blind field of stroke patients (gray circles, Figure 4C). At blind field locations where post-training stroke patients attained NDR thresholds < 1, the PFR gain was not significantly different from 0 (mean PFR gain = 0.0177 ± 0.07, t8=0.7524, p=0.4734, BF = 0.4066). Even when we isolated blind field locations where training restored ‘normal’ NDR thresholds (<0.35), contrasting them against blind field locations where NDR thresholds were >0.35, PFR gain was not significantly different between those groups (t12=−0.4140, p=0.6862, BF = 0.4749). Since many blind field locations failed to recover perception to the same range as the intact fields (NDR <0.35), we examined a subset of blind fields (N=6) where discrimination was matched to performance of intact fields. Still we found there was no significant PFR for those blind fields (t5=0.5411, p=0.6117, BF = 0.4206) and that subset differed significantly from the intact fields (t24=2.7424, p=0.0057, BF = 9.0755), and further the correlation remains non-significant for blind fields in that range (R2=0.362, F4=2.267, p=0.2066, BF = 0.6993). In an additional analysis, we fit an exponential curve to describe how PFR varied with NDR for the intact fields, and asked if PFR at blind field location that recovered NDR (i.e. NDR <0.35) fell within its confidence intervals. Only one of six such blind field locations overlapped the intact field confidence intervals (Figure 4—figure supplement 1), providing strong evidence against those distributions being matched across NDR (p ≤ 1.79 × 10–6, binomial test).

Finally, we should note that the above analyses treated individual visual field locations from the same participant as independent samples. Prior work in CB patients suggests that both baseline/pre-training performance and the ability of a location to improve are not uniform across blind fields, even at identical eccentricities; training-induced changes in discrimination performance are tightly restricted to the trained, blind field locations and do not spread by more than 1° outside the boundaries of each trained location (Das et al., 2014; Huxlin et al., 2009; Sahraie et al., 2006; Saionz et al., 2020). Nonetheless, we repeated our analyses by pooling the intact and blind fields within each participant and confirmed that our results remained consistent (Figure 4—figure supplement 2). In conclusion, over several tests, we found no significant recovery of PFR gain with recovery in perceptual motion performance.

Discussion

For the first time, the present study measured the impact of V1 damage and subsequent, training-induced, visual restoration on both voluntary saccadic behavior and a class of unconscious, automatic, post-saccadic smooth eye movements: the PFR. Our findings revealed an unexpected, critical reliance of pre-saccadic visual motion processing on visual pathways that include V1. We first confirmed that V1-stroke patients exhibit normal saccade accuracy and normal PFR when saccading to motion targets in intact regions of their visual fields, where vision is mediated by intact V1. However, while the same patients exhibited normal saccade accuracies for targets presented in their blind fields, they had no measurable PFR, even after visual discrimination training recovered global motion perception at those blind field locations. Thus, restoration of motion perception did not automatically restore the unconscious visual motion processing necessary for the PFR. This is surprising because traditionally, patients with V1 damage are well known for having preserved, unconscious visual processing in their blind fields – under the umbrella of blindsight phenomena (reviewed in Weiskrantz, 1996; Weiskrantz, 2009). That visual discrimination training can recover the ability to perform the relatively complex computations needed to integrate motion direction into a global percept available to consciousness – even to the extent of attaining normal NDR thresholds – inside chronic blind fields is remarkable. That this could occur without automatically restoring the unconscious global motion processing necessary for predictive smooth eye movements was unexpected. Our findings suggest that primary visual cortex (V1) may be key for both conscious visual perception (Tong, 2003) and unconscious visual processes that influence smooth eye movements induced by peripherally presented motion targets. They also suggest that visual restoration, after V1 damage, recruits different neural circuits than are normally used for these processes in the intact visual system; finally, it suggests that these newly engaged circuits now dissociate conscious and unconscious visual motion processing.

The extrastriate visual area critical for many aspects of visual motion processing – area MT – receives strong inputs from V1 as well as from sub-cortical projections that bypass V1 (Glickstein et al., 1980; Hagan et al., 2019; Maunsell and van Essen, 1983; Rodman et al., 1989; Sincich et al., 2004; Ungerleider et al., 1984; Van Essen et al., 1981). This diversity of inputs to MT likely explains why, after V1 damage, residual visual motion processing persists inside the resulting blind fields (reviewed in Das and Huxlin, 2010; Melnick et al., 2016; Tamietto and Morrone, 2016). Key for rehabilitation efforts, this residual processing can be leveraged by intensive visual training to recover both simple and complex motion perception (Cavanaugh et al., 2019; Das et al., 2014; Huxlin et al., 2009; Saionz et al., 2020).

In non-human primates, activity in area MT has been causally linked to perceptual reports of motion in discrimination and detection tasks (Britten et al., 1992; Newsome et al., 1985; Salzman et al., 1990; Siegel and Andersen, 1986), and to accuracy in pursuit eye movements (Huang and Lisberger, 2009; Newsome et al., 1985; Osborne et al., 2005; Salzman et al., 1990; Siegel and Andersen, 1986). The perception of velocity is also well correlated with velocity gain in voluntary pursuit, supporting the notion that pursuit and perception share common motion processing at the neural level (Gegenfurtner et al., 2003; Spering et al., 2005; Stone and Krauzlis, 2003). Voluntary pursuit and involuntary ocular following responses, such as the PFR, are also thought to rely on motion processing by the dLGN, V1, as well as area MT and the medial superior temporal (MST) area (Bakst et al., 2017; Mustari et al., 2009; Nuding et al., 2008; Takemura et al., 2007). Just as pursuit is modulated by stimulus contrast (Spering and Gegenfurtner, 2007; Spering et al., 2005), the PFR also exhibits a dependence on stimulus contrast; in visually intact humans, we saw a steep rise of the PFR contrast response function starting right below 10% luminance contrast, quickly reaching saturation at or above 15% contrast (Figure 4—figure supplement 3). This is comparable to the contrast response function of neurons in macaque area MT (Heuer and Britten, 2002; Kohn and Movshon, 2003; Sclar et al., 1990). However, pathways for ocular following are thought to be at least partly non-overlapping with those involved in motion perception (Glasser and Tadin, 2014; Price and Blum, 2014; Simoncini et al., 2012). Other studies in humans also support a distinction between neural circuits underlying smooth eye movements and conscious motion percepts (Spering and Carrasco, 2012; Spering and Gegenfurtner, 2007; Spering et al., 2011). Consistent with those studies, we now find that despite recovery of processing used for accurate, global motion perception in the blind field of V1-damaged humans, the PFR remains absent at trained, blind field locations.

One possible explanation for this outcome (disassociation between perception and PFR) is that training post-stroke lowered the thresholds used for reading out motion information in area MT for perception but not smooth eye movements. If motion perception in the blind field was consistent with weak residual sensory signals, and the PFR response has a higher contrast threshold than perception, then we could observe a lack of PFR while still retaining motion discrimination at blind field locations. Contrast thresholds for global motion stimuli in normal participants range between 5% and 10% contrast (Fine et al., 2004). We examined PFR as a function of contrast in normal participants to determine if it might have a higher threshold than that for perception, but found thresholds were in a comparable range (Figure 4—figure supplement 3). Thus, we consider it unlikely that the lack of PFR reflects a difference in threshold present in normal populations. However, there remains a possibility that perceptual training in patients may cause lower the threshold used for perception to accommodate weaker motion signals in area MT following V1 damage, but that a similar change in threshold did not transfer to the read-out used for oculomotor signals. This would reflect a disassociation between perception and eye movements at the level of read-out from area MT, which would still have important implications for visual recovery and understanding resulting differences in action and perception.

Another possible explanation for this disassociation is that while training post-stroke improved processing for perception, it did not correct problems with pre-saccadic attention and/or other aspects of saccade planning. It is well established that sensory processing among neurons in MT and MST can be strongly influenced by attention (Treue and Maunsell, 1996; Treue and Trujillo, 1999) and also by target selection immediately prior to saccades (Ferrera and Lisberger, 1997; Recanzone and Wurtz, 2000). Recent studies suggest that like target selection in voluntary pursuit, selective attention can modulate ocular following responses (Souto and Kerzel, 2014), and our findings here and previously (Kwon et al., 2019) support this notion. Pre-saccadic attention is thought to operate through feedback from oculomotor planning areas to visual cortex (Moore and Armstrong, 2003; Moore and Fallah, 2004), and while its impact has been studied mainly in visual area V4, it is also thought to occur in MT/MST. Indeed, electrical micro-stimulation in an oculomotor area, the frontal eye fields (FEF), influences selection of motion signals prior to saccades and can alter subsequent saccade trajectories to favor stimulus motion (Schafer and Moore, 2007). That the FEF and its projections to area MT are intact in V1-stroke patients suggests preservation of pre-saccadic planning and attention selection for the saccade target even when visual input is weak or abnormal in a blind field.

Although the effects of attention have not been studied extensively in V1-damaged patients, work to date suggests that some attentional mechanisms remain functional within cortical blind fields; as such, they could modulate motion signals at the level of MT/MST in the current behavioral paradigm. For instance, covert spatial attention was reported to improve stimulus detection in the blind field (Poggel et al., 2006) and in a separate study, it was shown to significantly decrease reaction times in V1-stroke patients performing an orientation discrimination task without any speed-accuracy trade-off (Kentridge et al., 2004). Feature-based attention was also able to improve fine direction discrimination training in cortically blinded fields (Cavanaugh et al., 2019). One piece of evidence suggesting that pre-saccadic attention remains functional in the current experiments is that other aspects of saccade pre-planning related to perceptual shifts in the position of motion targets remain in the blind field. Previous studies reported a motion-induced perceptual shift for stimulus location along the direction of stimulus motion (De Valois and De Valois, 1991; Nishida and Johnston, 1999; Ramachandran and Anstis, 1990; Whitney and Cavanagh, 2000), which for saccades is reflected by a shift in their end-points along the direction of target motion (Kosovicheva et al., 2014; Kwon et al., 2019; Schafer and Moore, 2007). For stroke patients, we confirmed similar shifts in saccade end-points in their intact fields, as well as significant, albeit reduced shifts along the target motion in their blind fields (Figure 4—figure supplement 4). We also considered if the lack of PFR in the blind field may have resulted from inability to allocate attention for the saccade into the blind field, against the competing influence of distractor apertures within intact portions of the visual field. First, we ran a control experiment in two patients to compare PFR when competing distractors were absent (single aperture task) against the original design with four apertures (Figure 4—figure supplement 5A). A central line cue directed patients to the saccade target, and as in the main experiment, their saccades remained accurate for both intact (98.6% ± 1.2%) and blind field (98.7% ± 0.24%) locations. Importantly, we did not observe a measurable PFR in the blind fields when distractor motion was removed, and PFR remained robust in the intact fields comparable to the original design (Figure 4—figure supplement 5B). There also remained a correlation between PFR and NDR thresholds for intact fields, but not for the blind fields (Figure 4—figure supplement 5C). Thus, removing distractors had little impact on the PFR, suggesting the deficit was sensory in nature rather than involving a misallocation of attention. As a final control analysis, we also measured to what extent PFR was driven by the motion in distractor apertures. If the motion in the other three apertures produced any measurable influence on PFR, then one might expect that influence to be stronger when saccades were made into the blind field because attention could not be properly allocated to its location, resulting in higher attention to distractor locations. While we did find that motion in distractor apertures contributed to a weak PFR gain that was significant and measurable, contrary to that hypothesis, the gain was stronger for saccades made into intact visual fields as compared to blind fields (Figure 4—figure supplement 6). Thus, it seems unlikely that the lack of PFR reflects an impairment to engage pre-saccadic attention into the blind field. Instead, we posit that perceptual recovery through repetitive discrimination training did not entrain the specific motion processing pathways that support post-saccadic following.

An important consideration for the present experiments was whether failures to elicit PFRs in cortically blinded portions of the visual field might simply reflect a motor deficit for accurately targeting peripherally presented motion apertures. Because the PFR requires pre-saccadic attention to select the target motion, any loss in target localization accuracy could impair selection. Previous studies in monkeys with V1 lesions have found reduced spatial accuracy for saccades made into the blind field with larger end-point errors from 0.2° to 0.6° at matched eccentricities for intact and blind fields (estimated from data in Figure 2 of Yoshida et al., 2008). In the present study, we observed that stroke patients were less likely to correctly select a target aperture in their blind versus intact fields when given a central spatial cue (89% versus 97%). However, when the target was correctly selected, spatial accuracy of the saccade end-points was normal (1.31° versus 1.38° absolute error relative to the target’s center). A key difference in the prior study is that Yoshida et al., 2008, used smaller stimuli, measuring only 0.45° in diameter, while we used large, dot motion fields (5.5° in diameter, Gaussian enveloped – see Materials and methods). Because spatial accuracy of saccade landings was similar for blind and intact fields of our stroke participants, we conclude that a motor deficit for targeting peripheral motion apertures in CB regions of the visual field was not a likely explanation for the absence of a PFR.

The slightly larger number of errors made by stroke patients when selecting cued targets in their blind fields could also be consistent with a reduction in their relative, perceived salience. Although all four motion apertures appeared simultaneously, iso-eccentrically and had equal (~100%) luminance contrast, it is possible that stroke participants required extra effort to ignore blind field-related perceptual inhomogeneities between the four apertures. Indeed, a prior study in chronic V1-stroke patients showed depressed luminance contrast sensitivity for motion and orientation discriminations at trained, blind field locations, in spite of normal NDR thresholds at these locations (Das et al., 2014). Thus, when patients are cued to saccade to a presumably less salient target in their blind field, this may require additional effort to suppress a reflexive saccade to the more salient targets. In an anti-saccade task where a salient target is ignored in order to plan a cued movement to the opposite (but empty) visual field, there is typically a reduction in saccadic reaction time (Hallett, 1978; Munoz and Everling, 2004). If saccadic reaction times slow down by 140 ms or more due to task difficulty, this impacts saccades to both visible and anti-saccade locations due to the extra volitional demands (Hallett and Adams, 1980). In line with this observation, we found that saccade reaction times were slower for stroke patients by roughly 100–130 ms, for both the intact and blind fields, relative to visually intact controls. Controls did differ in age from stroke patients (~20 years versus ~57 years of age), and prior work showed a correlation between slower saccade latency and growing age; however, the typical reduction from 20 to 80 years of age was 40–45 ms (Abel et al., 1983; Pirozzolo and Hansch, 1981; Spooner et al., 1980). Therefore, it appears unlikely that age alone would account for the >100 ms reduction in saccadic reaction times in stroke patients. Rather, we posit that patients had to exert greater volitional control to select the cued target inside blind regions of their visual field.

In summary, V1 damage in humans, such as occurs from occipital stroke, causes a dramatic loss of conscious visual perception across large regions of the visual field, impairing most aspects of daily living. Paradoxically, this condition was famously known for its relative preservation of unconscious visual processes, such as those mediating blindsight. With the advent of visual restoration training for this patient population, an important question in the field has been to ascertain what aspects of visual processing can recover, which cannot, and why. Peripheral visual motion processing is key to many aspects of daily living. Not only is it critical for accurate perception and identification of targets, it is also essential for our motor actions and reactions to these targets. Here, we show that visual training that can restore perceptual discrimination of peripheral motion does not automatically recover the PFR (or normal saccade targeting to peripheral motion stimuli). Our findings support a dissociation between smooth eye movements, saccade targeting, and perception following V1 damage, and suggest that V1 is critical for driving smooth eye movements such as the PFR. A key realization emerging from these results is that alternative pathways, which convey motion information from sub-cortical centers directly to area MT, are insufficient to support predictive oculomotor behaviors when V1 is damaged, even if they are sufficient to mediate recovery of conscious motion perception.

A second insight attained presently is that repetitive motion discrimination training in CB fields might only influence circuits and processes that support conscious perception, without transfer to those driving motion-dependent, unconscious behaviors, such as the smooth eye movements involved in the PFR. Importantly, prior to global motion training, CB patients are typically unable to describe or even correctly guess global motion direction of random dot stimuli in their blind fields – as such, they have no demonstrable blindsight for the direction integration task used here (Das et al., 2014; Huxlin et al., 2009; Saionz et al., 2020), and for other global motion tasks (Azzopardi and Cowey, 2001; Vaina et al., 2014). By the end of training, CB patients in the present study could correctly describe random dot stimuli and attain relatively normal NDR threshold levels of performance – implying recovery of conscious perception and not simply more effective blindsight. A dissociation between processing for conscious vision and action in the context of restoring vision in CB naturally leads to parallels with the work of Goodale and Milner, 1992, which suggested that ventral stream projections from striate cortex to the inferotemporal cortex play a major role in perception, while dorsal stream projections from striate cortex to posterior parietal regions mediate the required sensorimotor transformations for visually guided actions. Our current results suggest that after V1 damage, psychophysical training enables residual motion processing, likely in area MT and related dorsal pathway areas, to be used to recover perception without recovering the necessary processing for smooth eye movements. This suggests a dissociation between perception and visually guided actions in this rehabilitation paradigm.

It remains to be determined if deliberate training on tasks that focus on saccade planning to motion targets might recover predictive motor behaviors. Rehabilitation of predictive ocular behaviors remains an uncharted area of research for V1-stroke patients, even though saccade training is one of the few forms of rehabilitation more readily available to these patients (Kerkhoff, 1999; Kerkhoff, 2000; Kerkhoff et al., 1992; Mannan et al., 2010; Nelles et al., 2001; Ong et al., 2015; Pambakian et al., 2004; Roth et al., 2009; Sahraie et al., 2016; Spitzyna et al., 2007; Trauzettel-Klosinski, 2010; Weinberg et al., 1977; Zihl, 1980). Of relevance to our observation of an apparent dissociation between perception and eye movements after V1 damage, training patients to saccade to targets in their blind field does not induce perceptual recovery (Campion et al., 1983; Pollock et al., 2019). Nonetheless, it is conceivable that an approach combining perceptual training with training of ocular behaviors could improve the efficiency with which patients use information from their blind fields in everyday life.

Materials and methods

Participants

Eleven participants with long-standing cerebral blindness (CB) were recruited 2–5 years after a stroke that damaged their V1 unilaterally or in one case, bilaterally (see Table 1 for details). The location and nature of V1 damage was verified from clinical brain imaging performed as part of each patient’s standard of care. Homonymous visual field defects were confirmed using monocular, Humphrey automated perimetry performed at the Flaum Eye Institute of the University of Rochester (Figure 1). Participants suffering from neglect, cognitive impairments or ocular diseases were excluded from enrollment, as were those using psychoactive drugs. All participants had their visual acuity corrected to normal (with glasses or contact lenses) during testing.

Testing of V1-stroke participants occurred following completion of separate, visual training studies whereby they underwent visual training at one or more blind field locations. Some of these trained, blind field locations overlapped with PFR testing locations – some did not. The end result was a set of 14 blind field locations from 11 patients, where pre-training performance was initially at chance – that is, participants were unable to reliably discriminate left from right coherent, global motion. Post-training, however, depending on whether the tested locations overlapped with a trained location, performance either remained at chance or improved, generating measurable and occasionally near-normal direction integration (NDR) thresholds (Table 1). For comparison we also measured performance at a set of 13, iso-eccentric, intact field locations (Table 1; see below for details of global motion assessment methods).

PFR data from stroke patients were contrasted with a previously published data set obtained from eight visually intact controls (18–22 years of age; four females and four males) who had normal or corrected-to-normal vision (Kwon et al., 2019).

All procedures were approved by the Institutional Review Board of the University of Rochester and adhered to the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki. Written, informed consent was obtained from each participant, and participation was at all times completely voluntary.

Testing and visual training to recover perception

In each participant, visual field deficits were first estimated from Humphrey visual perimetry (Cavanaugh and Huxlin, 2017). This served as a starting point to map the position of the blind field border and select locations for PFR testing. It is important to note, however, that spared Humphrey visual field performance (i.e. luminance detection) was not necessarily representative of spared motion discrimination at a given blind field location – some patients were unable to attain normal global motion thresholds at locations partially spared on Humphrey perimetry (i.e. CB6 blind field location 2, Figure 1, Table 1, Figure 4—figure supplement 1). Blind field training was performed as previously described (Cavanaugh et al., 2022; Cavanaugh et al., 2015; Das et al., 2014; Huxlin et al., 2009; Saionz et al., 2020). In brief, participants were assigned to train daily at home using a custom MATLAB-based software (Cavanaugh et al., 2021) on their personal computers and displays. They were supplied with chin/forehead rests and instructed to position them such that the eyes were 42 cm away from their displays during training. They performed 300 trials per training location per day, at least 5 days per week, and emailed their auto-generated data log files back to the laboratory for analysis weekly. Session thresholds were calculated by fitting a Weibull function with a threshold criterion of 75% correct performance. Once performance reached levels comparable to equivalent intact field locations, training moved 1° deeper into the blind field along the x-axis (Cartesian coordinate space). When subjects returned to the lab, discrimination performance at all home-trained locations was verified with online fixation control using the Eyelink 1000 eye tracker. In addition, we measured NDR thresholds at blind and intact field locations chosen to undergo testing on the PFR task (see below). Intact field measurements were used to obtain a normative range of NDR thresholds that was specific to each participant (internal control for blind field performance). Ultimately, this enabled us to assess whether PFR was tied to perceptual motion integration performance in both intact and impaired regions of the visual field.

The locations used for PFR testing were mirror symmetric across the vertical and horizontal meridians, and ranged in eccentricity from 5.8° to 10.4° with a mean of 7.7°. This was done to maintain consistency with the original study (Kwon et al., 2019) that measured PFR in visually intact participants, whose stimuli uniformly sampled ordinal and cardinal directions at 7.1° eccentricity. Partly because of this experimental requirement, PFR testing locations did not always coincide with all trained locations in CB patients’ blind fields. However, this was not a problem, as our main goal here was to ascertain if and how PFR gain tracked with NDR performance. Thus, we sampled regions of the blind field that underwent training and recovered normal NDR performance (CB2, CB6 location 2, CB8 location 2, CB9, CB10, CB11), as well as locations that were not directly trained or were trained on a non-global motion task, causing NDR thresholds to remain at 1 (CB1, CB5, CB6 location 1, CB7, CB8 location 1) or improve only partially (CB3 locations 1 and 2, CB4). In sum, the specificity of recovery to trained locations and tasks in CB fields (Das et al., 2014; Huxlin et al., 2009; Sahraie et al., 2006; Saionz et al., 2020) allowed us to attain a wide range of NDR threshold outcomes, against which to map PFR gain.

Apparatus and eye tracking for assessing global motion perception

Participants were asked to perform 100 trials of a two-alternative, forced-choice, left-versus-right, global direction discrimination task at two to four, equi-eccentric, peripheral visual field locations chosen for testing of predictive oculomotor behavior (circles superimposed on Humphrey visual fields in Figure 1; red: blind field locations, blue: intact field locations). All blind field locations were tested in each patient (red circles in Figure 1). Time limitations restricted our ability to measure performance at every intact field location (blue circles in Figure 1), but at least one intact field location was assessed in each participant. Across intact field locations tested, we saw normal NDR thresholds that varied from 0.1 to 0.3 (Table 1). Percent correct and direction range thresholds were measured during in-lab testing, with central fixation enforced using an Eyelink 1000 eye tracker (SR Research, Mississagua, Ontario, Canada). Tracking was binocular for all participants except for CB3, who was tested monocularly because she exhibited convergence issues. As such, she had her dominant (right) eye tracked and the non-dominant eye patched both for motion perception and for PFR testing. Stimuli were presented in a gaze-contingent manner in either intact or blind regions of the visual field. Viewing distance to a luminance-calibrated CRT monitor (HP 7217A, 48.5 × 31.5 cm, 1024 × 640p, refresh rate 120 Hz) was 42 cm, enforced by a chin/forehead rest. Experiments were conducted using MATLAB (the MathWorks, Natick, MA) and the Psychophysics toolbox (Brainard, 1997; Kleiner et al., 2007; Pelli, 1997). At the start of each trial, subjects were asked to fixate a small target at the center of the CRT monitor. The Eyelink 1000 eye tracker was accurate to within 0.25°, with a sampling frequency of 1000 Hz (nominal, standard values as reported by SR Research). Subjects were allowed a fixation window of only ±1° around the fixation spot. If gaze moved outside this window during stimulus presentation, the trial was aborted, reshuffled and patients received a noxious auditory tone as feedback, reminding them to improve their fixation accuracy.

Following accurate fixation of the central spot for 1000 ms, a random dot stimulus appeared in a 5° diameter circular aperture, at one of the pre-determined locations in the peripheral visual field (see colored circles in Figure 1; NDR thresholds in Table 1). Black dots moved on a mid-gray background with a 250 ms lifetime, a speed of 10 °/s, and with a density of 3 dots/°2. Stimuli were presented for 500 ms, accompanied by a tone to indicate stimulus onset. Dots moved globally with a variable range of directions, uniformly distributed around the left- or rightward vectors (Das et al., 2014; Huxlin et al., 2009; Saionz et al., 2020). On each trial, subjects were asked to report the stimulus’ global direction of motion by pressing the left or right arrow keys on a keyboard (Figure 2A). Task difficulty was adjusted using an adaptive staircase (Levitt, 1971), which increased the range of dot directions from 0° to 360° in 40° steps after each set of three consecutive, correct responses; direction range was decreased by one 40° step for every incorrect response (Das et al., 2014; Huxlin et al., 2009; Saionz et al., 2020). Auditory feedback was provided on each trial, indicating the correctness of each response. Participants completed 100 trials at each of the four visual field locations. For each location tested, we fit a Weibull function to the data to extract the direction range threshold corresponding to 75% correct performance. This value was then normalized to the maximum possible range of dot directions (360°), generating an NDR threshold (Das et al., 2014; Huxlin et al., 2009), defined as:

NDR threshold = 360 o- Weibull fitted direction range threshold360 o

Apparatus and eye tracking for PFR measurements

Stimuli were generated using the Psychophysics toolbox in MATLAB 2015b on a PC computer (Intel i7 CPU, Windows 7, 8 GB RAM, GeForce Ti graphics card). They were presented on a gamma corrected display (BenQ X2411z LED Monitor, resolution: 1920 × 1080p, refresh rate: 120 Hz, gamma correction: 2.2) which had a dynamic luminance range from 0.5 to 230 cd/m2, at a distance of 95.25 cm in a dark room. Brightness on the display was set to 100% and contrast to 50%, and additional visual features of the monitor such as blur reduction and low blue light were turned off. Gamma corrections were verified with measurement by a photometer. Position of the left eye was recorded continuously in all participants except for CB3, who had her right eye tracked (see above). Eye position was recorded at 220 Hz using an infrared eye tracker (USB-220, Arrington Research, Scottsdale, AZ). The accuracy of the Arrington Eye Tracking system was 0.25°, with a precision of 0.15° (nominal, standard values as reported by Arrington Research). To minimize potential head movements, participants performed the task using a bite bar.

PFR stimulus and task

CB patients performed a centrally cued saccade task toward peripheral motion apertures (Figure 2B) as previously described in visually intact controls (Kwon et al., 2019). In brief, and as schematically illustrated in Figure 2B, trials were initiated by fixation of a small, dark, fixation spot presented on a gray background. After a variable fixation period of 150–200 ms, a saccade cue appeared at fixation together with four dot motion apertures in a square configuration (colored circles, Figure 1). The cue (dark bar, 1° in length, extending from fixation) was used to indicate the target aperture to which the participant should saccade. The target aperture was 5.5° in diameter and was consistent for all the participants. For CB1, the target apertures were centered at (± 3°, ± 5°). For CB2-8, the target apertures were centered at (± 5°, ± 5°). For CB9, the target apertures were centered at (± 5°, ± 10°). For CB10, the target apertures were centered at (± 8°, ±5°). For CB11, the target apertures were centered at (± 3°, ± 10°). There were 180 dots total in each aperture, with dot luminance set to 0.5 cd/m2 (100% contrast) and dot velocity fixed at 10 °/s. Following parameters from our previous study (Kwon et al., 2019), a Gaussian envelope was applied to each dot motion aperture to create a gradient in dot contrast from the center of the aperture (sigma = 1°).

To avoid stereotyped eye movements, we varied saccade directions across trials. Thus, the spatially cued motion aperture could appear in the intact or blind field of a given participant, on any given trial. Of particular note, the motion itself or its direction were irrelevant to the task. The motion within the aperture was 100% coherent and ran along a direction that was tangential to an imaginary line from the fixation point to the aperture. For each aperture, the motion was selected independent of the other apertures in one of the two tangential directions relative to the center-out saccade, either clockwise or counter-clockwise relative to the screen center.

We first compared eye movements in which the peripheral motion aperture was either present or absent upon saccade offset. Participants were instructed to make a saccade to the peripheral aperture as quickly as possible following the movement cue. A saccadic grace period (i.e. a maximum latency) was allowed for participants to initiate the saccade. In half the trials, selected at random, the stimulus motion remained present in all four apertures for 300 ms following detection of the eye landing within 3.5° from the center of an aperture. In the other half of trials, the stimulus was removed as soon as the eye had been detected leaving the fixation window, thus leaving a blank screen through the post-saccadic period. A saccade was labelled ‘correct’ when it fell at least 3.5° from the saccade target center within 90 ms of the eye leaving the fixation window. Participants completed as many trials as possible during a 1–1.5 hr session that sampled equally from the four locations and different stimulus conditions. During a typical session, stroke participants correctly completed on average 75.5 ± 32.0 (1 SD) trials in each blind field and 75.0 ± 31.8 (1 SD) trials in each intact field. For comparison against a set of eight normal controls, we analyzed eye movement data from a previous study (Kwon et al., 2019). The previous study employed a task with a subset of trials identical to the current study, but also in another half of trials tested an alternative stimulus configuration with the four apertures along the cardinal axes rather than the quadrants, thus a total of eight locations. During a typical session, normal controls completed on average 59.6 ± 13.8 (1 SD) trials in each of the eight fields.

Eye movement recordings and PFR analysis

Eye position data were collected as participants performed saccades from fixation to the peripheral target. Eye tracking and saccade detection procedures were identical to those previously published (Kwon et al., 2019). We sub-sampled eye position using the ViewPoint Matlab toolbox (Arrington Research) at the display refresh rate (120 Hz) to initiate gaze-contingent task events. For offline detection of saccadic eye movements, we used the full eye position data recorded at 220 Hz and applied an automatic procedure that detected deviations in 2D horizontal and vertical eye velocity space (Engbert and Mergenthaler, 2006; Kwon et al., 2019). Only the trials where the saccade was labelled ‘correct’ were included in the PFR analysis. We then focused our analysis by time locking eye velocity traces on intervals 200 ms prior to saccade onset and 200 ms following saccade offset. Details for eye position filtering, smoothing, and saccade detection were as previously described (Kwon et al., 2019). In brief, the 2D eye velocity was computed from smoothed eye position traces and then projected onto the motion vector in the target aperture on each trial. These projected velocity traces were then aligned to saccade onset or offset, and averaged across trials for each participant. Notably, the stimulus motion in our task was selected such that it was tangent to the radial direction of the saccade from fixation out to the aperture (thus either clockwise or counter-clockwise directions of motion), as in the original study (Kwon et al., 2019). This minimizes the influence of velocity transients from the saccade itself in measuring the PFR. We have focused analyses on the eye velocity projected onto stimulus motion for this reason, as it provides a measure of pre-saccadic motion integration that is less contaminated by saccade-induced velocity. To quantify the net target-related eye velocity in each trial, we used a second measure of eye velocity that did not involve any filtering or smoothing of eye position. We computed a vector for the PFR in units of velocity (°/s) as the 2D vector difference in the raw (non-smoothed) eye position from 20 to 100 ms after saccade offset normalized by that time interval. Excluding the first 20 ms after saccade offset from this analysis interval reduced the influence of saccade-related effects to instead focus on post-saccadic smooth movements. Like velocity traces, we projected this 2D vector onto the vector of the target’s motion to produce a single velocity value along the axis of stimulus motion, which we term the ‘open-loop’ PFR (Kwon et al., 2019). To assess the average PFR across trials, we computed each CB patients’ eye movements relative to the target motion direction so that positive average eye velocities meant that the eye was moving along the target motion direction, and negative average eye velocities meant that the eye was moving opposite to the target motion direction.

Finally, we considered to what extent the PFR tracked target velocity by quantifying the PFR gain: the eye velocity computed from the open-loop PFR normalized to the target velocity, with +1 indicating a perfect match of eye velocity to the target motion, and negative values indicating eye velocity in the opposite direction.

Statistics

To evaluate the significance of PFR gain we computed the one-sample t-test to verify it was greater than zero and we also computed the two-sample t-test to compare whether the PFR gains differed between conditions comparing either intact versus blind fields for CB participants, or intact fields for CB participants versus normal controls. We used the Pearson correlation to assess the relationship between the PFR gains and NDR thresholds within each stroke patient for intact and blind field visual locations. Confidence intervals of 95% were computed by bootstrapping the respective data at 10,000 times. Further statistical significance was computed by calculating the Bayes factor for the respective t-tests and ANOVA.

Acknowledgements

The authors wish to thank Martin Rolfs, Duje Tadin, Ralf Haefner, and Gregory DeAngelis for enlightening discussions and comments on the manuscript. The authors also wish to thank Terrance Schaefer, who performed the Humphrey visual field tests presented here and Christine Callan for her excellent work as research coordinator. The present study was funded by NIH (EY027314 and EY021209 to KRH; EY030998 to JFM; P30 EY001319 to the Center for Visual Science), by start-up funds to JFM from the University of Rochester, and by an unrestricted grant from the Research to Prevent Blindness (RPB) Foundation to the Flaum Eye Institute.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Jude F Mitchell, Email: jmitch27@ur.rochester.edu.

Miriam Spering, The University of British Columbia, Canada.

Chris I Baker, National Institute of Mental Health, National Institutes of Health, United States.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • National Eye Institute EY027314 to Krystel R Huxlin.

  • National Eye Institute EY021209 to Krystel R Huxlin.

  • National Eye Institute EY030998 to Jude F Mitchell.

  • Research to Prevent Blindness to Krystel R Huxlin.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

co-inventor on US Patent No. 7,549,743.

Author contributions

Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Software, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review and editing.

Data curation, Writing – review and editing.

Data curation, Writing – review and editing.

Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review and editing.

Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review and editing.

Ethics

All experimental protocols were conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by The Research Subjects Review Board at the University of Rochester Medical Center (#00021951). Informed written consent was obtained from all participants prior to participation. Participants were compensated $15/hour.

Additional files

Transparent reporting form

Data availability

Data for all figures has been shared on the Dryad. https://doi.org/10.6078/D1W69T.

The following dataset was generated:

Kwon S, Farenthold BK, Cavanaugh MR, Mitchell JF, Huxlin KR. 2021. Data from: Perceptual restoration fails to recover unconscious processing for smooth eye movements after occipital strokes. Dryad Digital Repository.

References

  1. Abel LA, Troost BT, Dell’Osso LF. The effects of age on normal saccadic characteristics and their variability. Vision Research. 1983;23:33–37. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(83)90038-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Azzopardi P, Cowey A. Motion discrimination in cortically blind patients. Brain: A Journal of Neurology. 2001;124:30–46. doi: 10.1093/brain/124.1.30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bakst L, Fleuriet J, Mustari MJ. FEFsem neuronal response during combined volitional and reflexive pursuit. Journal of Vision. 2017;17:1–14. doi: 10.1167/17.5.13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Born RT, Bradley DC. Structure and function of visual area MT. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2005;28:157–189. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.26.041002.131052. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Brainard DH. The Psychophysics Toolbox. Spatial Vision. 1997;10:433–436. doi: 10.1163/156856897X00357. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Britten KH, Shadlen MN, Newsome WT, Movshon JA. The analysis of visual motion: a comparison of neuronal and psychophysical performance. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1992;12:4745–4765. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.12-12-04745.1992. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Buizza A, Schmid R. Velocity characteristics of smooth pursuit eye movements to different patterns of target motion. Experimental Brain Research. 1986;63:395–401. doi: 10.1007/BF00236858. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Campion J, Latto R, Smith YM. Is blindsight an effect of scattered light, spared cortex, and near-threshold vision? Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 1983;6:423–448. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00016861. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  9. Cavanaugh MR, Zhang R, Melnick MD, Das A, Roberts M, Tadin D, Carrasco M, Huxlin KR. Visual recovery in cortical blindness is limited by high internal noise. Journal of Vision. 2015;15:9. doi: 10.1167/15.10.9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Cavanaugh MR, Huxlin KR. Visual discrimination training improves Humphrey perimetry in chronic cortically induced blindness. Neurology. 2017;88:1856–1864. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000003921. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Cavanaugh MR, Barbot A, Carrasco M, Huxlin KR. Feature-based attention potentiates recovery of fine direction discrimination in cortically blind patients. Neuropsychologia. 2019;128:315–324. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.12.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Cavanaugh MR, Fahrenthold BK, Huxlin KR. Huxlin lab home training codes. 8b0d278Github. 2021 https://github.com/huxlinlab/TrainingCodes
  13. Cavanaugh MR, Tadin D, Carrasco M, Huxlin KR. Benefits of Endogenous Spatial Attention During Visual Double-Training in Cortically-Blinded Fields. Frontiers in Neuroscience. 2022;16:e623. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2022.771623. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Das A, Huxlin KR. New Approaches to Visual Rehabilitation for Cortical Blindness: Outcomes and Putative Mechanisms. The Neuroscientist. 2010;16:374–387. doi: 10.1177/1073858409356112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Das A, Tadin D, Huxlin KR. Beyond Blindsight: Properties of Visual Relearning in Cortically Blind Fields. Journal of Neuroscience. 2014;34:11652–11664. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1076-14.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. De Valois RL, De Valois KK. Vernier acuity with stationary moving Gabors. Vision Research. 1991;31:1619–1626. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(91)90138-u. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Deubel H, Schneider WX. Saccade target selection and object recognition: evidence for a common attentional mechanism. Vision Research. 1996;36:1827–1837. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(95)00294-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Elshout JA, van Asten F, Hoyng CB, Bergsma DP, van den Berg AV. Visual rehabilitation in chronic cerebral blindness: a randomized controlled crossover study. Frontiers in Neurology. 2016;7:e92. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2016.00092. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Engbert R, Mergenthaler K. Microsaccades are triggered by low retinal image slip. PNAS. 2006;103:7192–7197. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0509557103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Ferrera VP, Lisberger SG. Neuronal responses in visual areas MT and MST during smooth pursuit target selection. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1997;78:1433–1446. doi: 10.1152/jn.1997.78.3.1433. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Fine I, Anderson CM, Boynton GM, Dobkins KR. The invariance of directional tuning with contrast and coherence. Vision Research. 2004;44:903–913. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2003.11.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Gardner JL, Lisberger SG. Linked target selection for saccadic and smooth pursuit eye movements. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2001;21:2075–2084. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-06-02075.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Gegenfurtner KR, Xing D, Scott BH, Hawken MJ. A comparison of pursuit eye movement and perceptual performance in speed discrimination. Journal of Vision. 2003;3:865–876. doi: 10.1167/3.11.19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Gellman RS, Carl JR, Miles FA. Short latency ocular-following responses in man. Visual Neuroscience. 1990;5:107–122. doi: 10.1017/s0952523800000158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Glasser DM, Tadin D. Modularity in the motion system: independent oculomotor and perceptual processing of brief moving stimuli. Journal of Vision. 2014;14:28. doi: 10.1167/14.3.28. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Glickstein M, Cohen JL, Dixon B, Gibson A, Hollins M, Labossiere E, Robinson F. Corticopontine visual projections in macaque monkeys. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1980;190:209–229. doi: 10.1002/cne.901900202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Goodale MA, Milner AD. Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Trends in Neurosciences. 1992;15:20–25. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(92)90344-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Hagan MA, Chaplin TA, Huxlin KR, Rosa MGP, Lui LL. Altered Sensitivity to Motion of Area MT Neurons Following Long-Term V1 Lesions. Cerebral Cortex (New York, N.Y. 2019;30:451–464. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhz096. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Hallett PE. Primary and secondary saccades to goals defined by instructions. Vision Research. 1978;18:1279–1296. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(78)90218-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Hallett PE, Adams BD. The predictability of saccadic latency in a novel voluntary oculomotor task. Vision Research. 1980;20:329–339. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(80)90019-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Heuer HW, Britten KH. Contrast dependence of response normalization in area MT of the rhesus macaque. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2002;88:3398–3408. doi: 10.1152/jn.00255.2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Huang X, Lisberger SG. Noise correlations in cortical area MT and their potential impact on trial-by-trial variation in the direction and speed of smooth-pursuit eye movements. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2009;101:3012–3030. doi: 10.1152/jn.00010.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Huxlin KR, Martin T, Kelly K, Riley M, Friedman DI, Burgin WS, Hayhoe M. Perceptual relearning of complex visual motion after V1 damage in humans. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29:3981–3991. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4882-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Kentridge RW, Heywood CA, Weiskrantz L. Spatial attention speeds discrimination without awareness in blindsight. Neuropsychologia. 2004;42:831–835. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.11.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Kerkhoff G, Münßinger U, Haaf E, Eberle-Strauss G, Stögerer E. Rehabilitation of homonymous scotomata in patients with postgeniculate damage of the visual system: saccadic compensation training. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience. 1992;4:245–254. doi: 10.3233/RNN-1992-4402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Kerkhoff G. Restorative and compensatory therapy approaches in cerebral blindness - a review. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience. 1999;15:255–271. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Kerkhoff G. Neurovisual rehabilitation: recent developments and future directions. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry. 2000;68:691–706. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.68.6.691. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Kleiner M, Brainard D, Pelli D, Ingling A, Murray R, Broussard C. What’s new in Psychtoolbox-3? Perception. 2007;36:1–16. [Google Scholar]
  39. Kohn A, Movshon JA. Neuronal adaptation to visual motion in area MT of the macaque. Neuron. 2003;39:681–691. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00438-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Kosovicheva AA, Wolfe BA, Whitney D. Visual motion shifts saccade targets. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics. 2014;76:1778–1788. doi: 10.3758/s13414-014-0680-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Kowler E, Anderson E, Dosher B, Blaser E. The role of attention in the programming of saccades. Vision Research. 1995;35:1897–1916. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(94)00279-u. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Kwon OS, Tadin D, Knill DC. Unifying account of visual motion and position perception. PNAS. 2015;112:8142–8147. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1500361112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Kwon S, Rolfs M, Mitchell JF. Presaccadic motion integration drives a predictive postsaccadic following response. Journal of Vision. 2019;19:11–19. doi: 10.1167/19.11.12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Levitt H. Transformed Up‐Down Methods in Psychoacoustics. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 1971;49:467–477. doi: 10.1121/1.1912375. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Li HH, Barbot A, Carrasco M. Saccade preparation reshapes sensory tuning. Current Biology. 2016;26:1564–1570. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.04.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Lisberger SG, Westbrook LE. Properties of visual inputs that initiate horizontal smooth pursuit eye movements in monkeys. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1985;5:1662–1673. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.05-06-01662.1985. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Lisberger SG, Morris EJ, Tychsen L. Visual motion processing and sensory-motor integration for smooth pursuit eye movements. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 1987;10:97–129. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.10.030187.000525. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Mannan SK, Pambakian ALM, Kennard C. Compensatory strategies following visual search training in patients with homonymous hemianopia: an eye movement study. Journal of Neurology. 2010;257:1812–1821. doi: 10.1007/s00415-010-5615-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Maunsell JH, van Essen DC. The connections of the middle temporal visual area (MT) and their relationship to a cortical hierarchy in the macaque monkey. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1983;3:2563–2586. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.03-12-02563.1983. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Mazzi C, Savazzi S, Silvanto J. On the “blindness” of blindsight: What is the evidence for phenomenal awareness in the absence of primary visual cortex (V1)? Neuropsychologia. 2019;128:103–108. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.10.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Melnick MD, Tadin D, Huxlin KR. Relearning to See in Cortical Blindness. The Neuroscientist. 2016;22:199–212. doi: 10.1177/1073858415621035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Miles FA, Kawano K, Optican LM. Short-latency ocular following responses of monkey. I. Dependence on temporospatial properties of visual input. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1986;56:1321–1354. doi: 10.1152/jn.1986.56.5.1321. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Moore T, Armstrong KM. Selective gating of visual signals by microstimulation of frontal cortex. Nature. 2003;421:370–373. doi: 10.1038/nature01341. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Moore T, Fallah M. Microstimulation of the frontal eye field and its effects on covert spatial attention. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2004;91:152–162. doi: 10.1152/jn.00741.2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Movshon JA, Newsome WT. Visual response properties of striate cortical neurons projecting to area MT in macaque monkeys. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1996;16:7733–7741. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-23-07733.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Munoz DP, Everling S. Look away: the anti-saccade task and the voluntary control of eye movement. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2004;5:218–228. doi: 10.1038/nrn1345. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Mustari MJ, Ono S, Das VE. Signal processing and distribution in cortical-brainstem pathways for smooth pursuit eye movements. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2009;1164:147–154. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.03859.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Nelles G, Esser J, Eckstein A, Tiede A, Gerhard H, Diener HC. Compensatory visual field training for patients with hemianopia after stroke. Neuroscience Letters. 2001;306:189–192. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(01)01907-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Newsome WT, Wurtz RH, Dürsteler MR, Mikami A. Deficits in visual motion processing following ibotenic acid lesions of the middle temporal visual area of the macaque monkey. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1985;5:825–840. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.05-03-00825.1985. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Nishida S y., Johnston A. Influence of motion signals on the perceived position of spatial pattern. Nature. 1999;397:610–612. doi: 10.1038/17600. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Nuding U, Ono S, Mustari MJ, Büttner U, Glasauer S. A theory of the dual pathways for smooth pursuit based on dynamic gain control. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2008;99:2798–2808. doi: 10.1152/jn.90237.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Ong YH, Jacquin-Courtois S, Gorgoraptis N, Bays PM, Husain M, Leff AP. Eye-Search: A web-based therapy that improves visual search in hemianopia. Annals of Clinical and Translational Neurology. 2015;2:74–78. doi: 10.1002/acn3.154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Osborne LC, Lisberger SG, Bialek W. A sensory source for motor variation. Nature. 2005;437:412–416. doi: 10.1038/nature03961. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Pambakian ALM, Mannan SK, Hodgson TL, Kennard C. Saccadic visual search training: a treatment for patients with homonymous hemianopia. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry. 2004;75:1443–1448. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2003.025957. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Pelli DG. The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. Spatial Vision. 1997;10:437–442. doi: 10.1163/156856897X00366. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Pirozzolo FJ, Hansch EC. Oculomotor reaction time in dementia reflects degree of cerebral dysfunction. Science (New York, N.Y.) 1981;214:349–351. doi: 10.1126/science.7280699. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Poggel DA, Kasten E, Müller-Oehring EM, Bunzenthal U, Sabel BA. Improving residual vision by attentional cueing in patients with brain lesions. Brain Research. 2006;1097:142–148. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.04.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Pollock A, Hazelton C, Rowe FJ, Jonuscheit S, Kernohan A, Angilley J, Henderson CA, Langhorne P, Campbell P, Cochrane Stroke Group Interventions for visual field defects in people with stroke. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. 2019;1:e858. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD008388.pub3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Price NSC, Blum J. Motion perception correlates with volitional but not reflexive eye movements. Neuroscience. 2014;277:435–445. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.07.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Ramachandran VS, Anstis SM. Illusory displacement of equiluminous kinetic edges. Perception. 1990;19:611–616. doi: 10.1068/p190611. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Rashbass C. The relationship between saccadic and smooth tracking eye movements. The Journal of Physiology. 1961;159:326–338. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1961.sp006811. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Recanzone GH, Wurtz RH. Effects of Attention on MT and MST Neuronal Activity During Pursuit Initiation. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2000;83:777–790. doi: 10.1152/jn.2000.83.2.777. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Rodman HR, Gross CG, Albright TD. Afferent basis of visual response properties in area MT of the macaque. I. Effects of Striate Cortex Removal. Journal of Neuroscience. 1989;9:2033–2050. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.09-06-02033.1989. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Rolfs M, Jonikaitis D, Deubel H, Cavanagh P. Predictive remapping of attention across eye movements. Nature Neuroscience. 2011;14:252–256. doi: 10.1038/nn.2711. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Rolfs M, Carrasco M. Rapid simultaneous enhancement of visual sensitivity and perceived contrast during saccade preparation. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32:13744–52a. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2676-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Roth T, Sokolov AN, Messias A, Roth P, Weller M, Trauzettel-Klosinski S. Comparing explorative saccade and flicker training in hemianopia: A randomized controlled study. Neurology. 2009;72:324–331. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000341276.65721.f2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Sahraie A, Trevethan CT, MacLeod MJ, Murray AD, Olson JA, Weiskrantz L. Increased sensitivity after repeated stimulation of residual spatial channels in blindsight. PNAS. 2006;103:14971–14976. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0607073103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Sahraie A, Trevethan CT, MacLeod MJ. Temporal properties of spatial channel of processing in hemianopia. Neuropsychologia. 2008;46:879–885. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.11.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Sahraie A, Smania N, Zihl J. to Improve Eye Movement Efficacy in Patients with Homonymous Visual Field Loss. BioMed Research International. 2016;2016:e6461. doi: 10.1155/2016/5186461. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Saionz EL, Tadin D, Melnick MD, Huxlin KR. Functional preservation and enhanced capacity for visual restoration in subacute occipital stroke. Brain: A Journal of Neurology. 2020;143:1857–1872. doi: 10.1093/brain/awaa128. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Salzman CD, Britten KH, Newsome WT. Cortical microstimulation influences perceptual judgements of motion direction. Nature. 1990;346:174–177. doi: 10.1038/346174a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Sanchez-Lopez J, Pedersini CA, Di Russo F, Cardobi N, Fonte C, Varalta V, Prior M, Smania N, Savazzi S, Marzi CA. Visually evoked responses from the blind field of hemianopic patients. Neuropsychologia. 2019;128:127–139. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.10.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Schafer RJ, Moore T. Attention governs action in the primate frontal eye field. Neuron. 2007;56:541–551. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.09.029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Sclar G, Maunsell JHR, Lennie P. Coding of image contrast in central visual pathways of the macaque monkey. Vision Research. 1990;30:1–10. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(90)90123-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Siegel R, Andersen R. Motion perceptual deficits following ibotenic acid lesions of the middle temporal area (MT) in the behaving rhesus monkey. Soc Neurosci Abstr. 1986;12:1183. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.05-03-00825.1985. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  86. Simoncini C, Perrinet LU, Montagnini A, Mamassian P, Masson GS. More is not always better: adaptive gain control explains dissociation between perception and action. Nature Neuroscience. 2012;15:1596–1603. doi: 10.1038/nn.3229. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Sincich LC, Park KF, Wohlgemuth MJ, Horton JC. Bypassing V1: a direct geniculate input to area MT. Nature Neuroscience. 2004;7:1123–1128. doi: 10.1038/nn1318. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Smith JL. Homonymous hemianopia: A review of one hundred cases. American Journal of Ophthalmology. 1962;54:616–623. doi: 10.1016/0002-9394(62)92192-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Souto D, Kerzel D. Ocular tracking responses to background motion gated by feature-based attention. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2014;112:1074–1081. doi: 10.1152/jn.00810.2013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Spering M, Kerzel D, Braun DI, Hawken MJ, Gegenfurtner KR. Effects of contrast on smooth pursuit eye movements. Journal of Vision. 2005;5:455–465. doi: 10.1167/5.5.6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Spering M, Gegenfurtner KR. Contrast and assimilation in motion perception and smooth pursuit eye movements. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2007;98:1355–1363. doi: 10.1152/jn.00476.2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Spering M, Pomplun M, Carrasco M. Tracking without perceiving: a dissociation between eye movements and motion perception. Psychological Science. 2011;22:216–225. doi: 10.1177/0956797610394659. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Spering M, Carrasco M. Similar effects of feature-based attention on motion perception and pursuit eye movements at different levels of awareness. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32:7594–7601. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0355-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Spitzyna GA, Wise RJS, McDonald SA, Plant GT, Kidd D, Crewes H, Leff AP. Optokinetic therapy improves text reading in patients with hemianopic alexia: a controlled trial. Neurology. 2007;68:1922–1930. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000264002.30134.2a. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Spooner JW, Sakala SM, Baloh RW. Effect of aging on eye tracking. Archives of Neurology. 1980;37:575–576. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1980.00500580071012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Stone LS, Krauzlis RJ. Shared motion signals for human perceptual decisions and oculomotor actions. Journal of Vision. 2003;3:725–736. doi: 10.1167/3.11.7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Takemura A, Murata Y, Kawano K, Miles FA. Deficits in short-latency tracking eye movements after chemical lesions in monkey cortical areas MT and MST. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27:529–541. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3455-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Tamietto M, Morrone MC. Visual plasticity: blindsight bridges anatomy and function in the visual system. Current Biology. 2016;26:R70–R73. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.11.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Teuber HL, Battersby WS, Bender MB. Visual Field Defects after Penetrating Missile Wounds of the Brain. Harvard University Press; 1960. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  100. Tong F. Primary visual cortex and visual awareness. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience. 2003;4:219–229. doi: 10.1038/nrn1055. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Trauzettel-Klosinski S. Rehabilitation for visual disorders. Journal of Neuro-Ophthalmology. 2010;30:73–84. doi: 10.1097/WNO.0b013e3181ce7e8f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Treue S, Maunsell JHR. Attentional modulation of visual motion processing in cortical areas MT and MST. Nature. 1996;382:539–541. doi: 10.1038/382539a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Treue S, Trujillo JCM. Feature-based attention influences motion processing gain in macaque visual cortex. Nature. 1999;399:575–579. doi: 10.1038/21176. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Tychsen L, Lisberger SG. Visual motion processing for the initiation of smooth-pursuit eye movements in humans. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1986;56:953–968. doi: 10.1152/jn.1986.56.4.953. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Ungerleider LG, Desimone R, Galkin TW, Mishkin M. Subcortical projections of area MT in the macaque. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1984;223:368–386. doi: 10.1002/cne.902230304. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Vaina LM, Soloviev S, Calabro FJ, Buonanno F, Passingham R, Cowey A. Reorganization of retinotopic maps after occipital lobe infarction. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2014;26:1266–1282. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00538. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Van Essen DC, Maunsell JH, Bixby JL. The middle temporal visual area in the macaque: myeloarchitecture, connections, functional properties and topographic organization. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1981;199:293–326. doi: 10.1002/cne.901990302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Weinberg J, Diller L, Gordon WA, Gerstman LJ, Lieberman A, Lakin P, Hodges G, Ezrachi O. Visual scanning training effect on reading-related tasks in acquired right brain damage. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation. 1977;58:479–486. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.12.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Weiskrantz L, Warrington EK, Sanders MD, Marshall J. Visual capacity in the hemianopic field following a restricted occipital ablation. Brain. 1974;97:709–728. doi: 10.1093/brain/97.1.709. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Weiskrantz L, Barbur JL, Sahraie A. Parameters affecting conscious versus unconscious visual discrimination with damage to the visual cortex (V1. PNAS. 1995;92:6122–6126. doi: 10.1073/pnas.92.13.6122. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Weiskrantz L. Blindsight revisited. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 1996;6:215–220. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(96)80075-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Weiskrantz L. Blindsight: A case study spanning 35 years and new developments. Oxford University Press; 2009. [Google Scholar]
  113. White AL, Rolfs M, Carrasco M. Adaptive deployment of spatial and feature-based attention before saccades. Vision Research. 2013;85:26–35. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2012.10.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Whitney D, Cavanagh P. Motion distorts visual space: shifting the perceived position of remote stationary objects. Nature Neuroscience. 2000;3:954–959. doi: 10.1038/78878. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Yoshida M, Takaura K, Kato R, Ikeda T, Isa T. Striate cortical lesions affect deliberate decision and control of saccade: implication for blindsight. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2008;28:10517–10530. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1973-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Zihl J. “Blindsight”: improvement of visually guided eye movements by systematic practice in patients with cerebral blindness. Neuropsychologia. 1980;18:71–77. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(80)90085-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Editor's evaluation

Miriam Spering 1

In this unique and meticulous study, Kwon, Huxlin and Mitchell add yet another twist to the story of dissociation between perception and action in the human brain. They found a sort of reverse blindsight: occipital stroke patients who lost conscious vision in part of the visual field could regain the ability to discriminate stimulus motion but did not make a specific form of eye movement that typically follows the stimulus motion. This result has implications for theories of perception and motor control, and for neurological rehabilitation.

Decision letter

Editor: Miriam Spering1
Reviewed by: Alex White2

Our editorial process produces two outputs: i) public reviews designed to be posted alongside the preprint for the benefit of readers; ii) feedback on the manuscript for the authors, including requests for revisions, shown below. We also include an acceptance summary that explains what the editors found interesting or important about the work.

Decision letter after peer review:

Thank you for submitting your article "Perceptual restoration fails to recover unconscious processing for smooth eye movements after occipital stroke" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by 3 peer reviewers, and the evaluation has been overseen by a Miriam Spering as the Reviewing Editor and Chris Baker as the Senior Editor. The following individual involved in review of your submission has agreed to reveal their identity: Alex White (Reviewer #3).

The reviewers and reviewing editor have discussed the reviews with one another. The Reviewing Editor has drafted this letter to help you prepare a revised submission. In this letter, you will also find a summary of the post-review discussion.

Essential revisions:

As you will be able to read below, all reviewers appreciate the novelty of the question, the elegance of the study design, and the richness and high value of the obtained patient dataset. There is no question that the obtained results, should they indeed support the claimed dissociation, would be highly interesting to the vision sciences community and to the reader with a clinical background. However, as you will also see, all reviewers raised significant and major concerns with regard to data analysis and data interpretation. The reviewers share a concern that perhaps the issues cannot be addressed without collecting additional data, but – given the interest in the research question and value of the dataset – would like to give the authors a chance at rebuttal.

1. Throughout the paper, findings are interpreted to indicate a "clear dissociation between pathways mediating perceptual restoration and automatic actions", but the evidence for this dissociation is not clear and appears relatively overstated throughout. Specifically, this interpretation is based on a small subsample of the dataset (e.g., the regression of PRF gain on NDR thresholds in the intact field [Figure 4b] predicts that there should be a substantial PRF gain only at NDR thresholds below about 0.3. For the affected field this applies only to three data points of which one shows a substantial PFR and is fully compatible with the data in the intact field; there is also a concern about the ability to fit an exponential function here). In sum, the key finding of the paper appears to be based on an analysis that is drastically underpowered (for a theoretical coefficient r of 0.5, 80% power requires 29 subjects, far more than n=8).

In addition to recommendations listed below in individual reviews, two additional recommendations to potentially strengthen the finding of a dissociation were raised during the discussion: (a) the authors could add to Figure 4A a measure of the gain of post-saccadic ocular following for trials when the motion stimulus remains visible. On p 15, line 266-269 they make the "important observation" that the stroke patients are capable of making smooth following eye movements when fixating the moving stimulus. However, that effect has not been quantified. (b) Rather than relying on a correlation across subjects, the authors could run statistics on individual patient data and show that the PFR gain of at least some patients is significantly worse than would be predicted from their motion discrimination thresholds. For instance, they could use the data from the intact fields to build a model of how motion discrimination predicts PFR, and then test whether each data point from a blind field falls outside the confidence interval of that model.

In sum, without clear quantitative evidence for the claimed dissociation the overall impact of this paper would be relatively weak.

2. In terms of data analysis, the authors should report the mean values and confidence intervals for all quantitative measures (in addition to the reported statistics for the comparisons, exact p-values, t-values etc…). Importantly, the authors are encouraged to go beyond traditional null-hypothesis tests (t-tests and correlations) to make binary judgements of whether each effect or difference is "significant" (p<0.05). Some of the conclusions would be more convincing if supplemented with power analyses, bootstrapped confidence intervals, and Bayes factors to evaluate the strength of evidence.

3. Overall, the results might allow for a different interpretation: instead of assuming separate pathways for motion perception and oculomotor control in patients, the results might also be explained by a different read-out of the same motion signal for perception and oculomotor control, where oculomotor control applies a more conservative threshold and requires a higher internal signal strength than the motion perception. Further to the concern about data interpretation, a reviewer pointed out the potential role for presaccadic attentional mechanisms, which are currently not taken into account.

Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

Unit of analysis: It seems that the unit of analysis was sometimes individual patients (N=8) and sometimes locations in the affected visual field (N=11). I think that treating multiple locations of one patient as independent samples is a form of pseudo replication (e.g. Lazic, 2010) and therefore the unit of analysis should always be individual patients.

Consciousness of motion perception: The authors interpret their motion discrimination task in terms of conscious motion perception, but strictly speaking the patients might also perform the task in a blindsight manner, where they have no conscious awareness of the motion itself but still are able to indicate the correct direction of motion.

Relevant literature: It might be interesting to relate the results to the theory and work by Milner and Goodale, who postulate a dissociation between perception and action as well.

Definition and values of NDR thresholds: I don't quite understand why the NDR thresholds in Table 1 and in Figure 4 range between 0 and 1 % although they should range between 0 and 100 % according to the equation in the methods.

Restoration training: It would be useful to provide some information in what kind of training the patients participated.

Reference

Lazic, S. E. (2010). The problem of pseudo replication in neuroscientific studies: is it affecting your analysis?. BMC neuroscience, 11(1), 1-17.

Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

In order to comply as much as possible with the journal recommendations, the authors should report the mean values and confidence intervals for all quantitative measures (in addition to the reported statistics for the comparisons, exact p-values, t-values etc…)

Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

(1) Throughout the manuscript, you rely on null-hypothesis tests that treat the outcome as binary, either significant if p<0.05, or completely supporting the null hypothesis if p>0.05. This is an outdated approach known to be problematic. For instance, consider the correlations in Figure 4B. One is test treated as significant and the other as showing no relationship. On line 244: "this relationship was lost in the blind field"; and line 276, "there was no significant correlation between restored NDR and PFR gains." If you take p>0.05 as your criterion for no relationship in a correlation, then yes. But the slope in Figure 4B doesn't appear to be 0. It seems quite possible that with more subjects from the same population, this correlation would be "significant." An example that goes in the other direction is in Supplementary Figure 2, the saccade angular deviations in the blind field: p=0.03, which is taken as strong evidence of normal motion perception in the blind field.

To improve your statistical analysis I would suggest several complementary approaches: (i) power analyses for "null" results to estimate how many participants would be required to get to p<0.05; (ii) Bayes Factors for the null vs alternate hypotheses. If you're using Matlab I recommend Bart Krekelberg's toolbox: https://klabhub.github.io/bayesFactor/ (ii) For one-sample t-tests I also recommend reporting 95% confidence intervals of the mean (obtained via bootstrapping), and for two-sample t-tests I would recommend reporting 95% bootstrapped confidence intervals for the mean difference.

(2) Some important details about the participants and the study design aren't explained until the Methods, which come at the end. Three details in particular confused me: (i) what the motion discrimination thresholds are, and how they're normalized. After I read the Methods section I better understood what the thresholds are, but I didn't understand the rationale for normalizing them the way you did. Why not just report the threshold direction range in degrees? (ii) The training that some patients underwent. It comes as a surprise in the Results that some visual field locations were trained and some weren't. Even in the abstract, the narrative is a little confusing. (iii) The two ways in which saccade "accuracy" is computed. One measure is reported in percent correct, and the other in degrees. You may just need to add a few words in the Results to clarify that first you categorized each saccade in a binary fashion as either landing near the target or near a different stimulus.

(3) A related question that should be clarified: when patients improve their motion discrimination after training, do they have a conscious percept of the motion, or is it another form of blindsight?

(4) I find these results to be very cool and indeed surprising. But I wonder whether they are limited to a particular set of unusual conditions, and a reader who spends less time thinking about oculomotor control may be puzzled by the particular set of conditions set up in this experiment. It does seem rather niche, in terms of the patient population, the particular eye movement, and the particular task and stimulus arrangement. Regarding the latter, your display contained 4 static apertures full of moving dots that don't really translate across the screen. Why did you need 4 apertures, rather than just one, and why not use single objects that actually moved? Does it matter that the target stimulus is 'competing' with three other irrelevant motion stimuli? Do these results generalize to more realistic settings when a person makes a saccade to a moving object that they then must pursue, like a bird flying across the sky? I myself have relied on such "unnatural" stimuli and I know there can be good reasons to use them carefully, but they need to be justified. And I imagine you had only a short amount of time with each patient, but we must also acknowledge that the whole story would be stronger if the phenomenon were demonstrated in a few different ways.

So overall, it would help if you could justify these peculiarities of your study and explain the ways in which they do and do not apply to other conditions – other people, other settings outside your particular lab experiment, and other behaviors.

eLife. 2022 Jun 22;11:e67573. doi: 10.7554/eLife.67573.sa2

Author response


Essential revisions:

As you will be able to read below, all reviewers appreciate the novelty of the question, the elegance of the study design, and the richness and high value of the obtained patient dataset. There is no question that the obtained results, should they indeed support the claimed dissociation, would be highly interesting to the vision sciences community and to the reader with a clinical background. However, as you will also see, all reviewers raised significant and major concerns with regard to data analysis and data interpretation. The reviewers share a concern that perhaps the issues cannot be addressed without collecting additional data, but – given the interest in the research question and value of the dataset – would like to give the authors a chance at rebuttal.

We acknowledge the concern that the original sample size was not adequate and as such, we have collected additional data in 3 new patients in an attempt to address it.

1. Throughout the paper, findings are interpreted to indicate a "clear dissociation between pathways mediating perceptual restoration and automatic actions", but the evidence for this dissociation is not clear and appears relatively overstated throughout. Specifically, this interpretation is based on a small subsample of the dataset (e.g., the regression of PRF gain on NDR thresholds in the intact field [Figure 4b] predicts that there should be a substantial PRF gain only at NDR thresholds below about 0.3. For the affected field this applies only to three data points of which one shows a substantial PFR and is fully compatible with the data in the intact field; there is also a concern about the ability to fit an exponential function here). In sum, the key finding of the paper appears to be based on an analysis that is drastically underpowered (for a theoretical coefficient r of 0.5, 80% power requires 29 subjects, far more than n=8).

As described above, we have collected additional data from 3 new patients who attained training-induced recovery of global direction perception and integration below an NDR threshold of 0.35, the rough upper limit of the normal range. After several months required for recruiting, training and post-testing these patients, we were able to double the blind-field locations where normal performance was restored (from 3 to 6 independent locations). Although still a small sample size, the effects in the additional data remained highly consistent, showing no significant PFR. Using the combined data, there is still a significant difference in PFR between intact and blind-field locations under a variety of tests, including standard pairwise comparison tests, but also Bayes Factor t-tests as suggested by the reviewers. We have also repeated an analysis of the correlation between PFR and NDR threshold, but limited to the range of normal performance (NDR < 0.35) and find that the correlation remains non-significant for blind-field locations in that range (R2=0.362, F4=2.267, p=0.2066, BF=0.6993). However, as the reviewers anticipated, even with the added patients (now n=6) testing the difference in slopes for those line fits in the correlation would require a much larger n, and the slopes fit in this range are not significantly different (slopes of -0.8616 vs -0.5611, t22=-0.6478, p=0.5238). By contrast, if slopes are compared over the full NDR range, including all points, they are significantly different (slopes -0.8616 vs -0.0385, t30=-2.9461, p=0.0062). Given the difficulty obtaining the 3 additional patients who recovered back into the normal NDR range, we have instead adopted the approach suggested by Reviewer 1 for this issue: we fit an exponential curve to the intact-field points and evaluated if the PFR for blind-fields fall outside that distribution. For that analysis, we did find robust support for a significant difference, as detailed below in our responses to Reviewer 1. We have updated Results to include the additional statistics for the restricted range and the exponential fit analyses on lines 310-323.

In addition to recommendations listed below in individual reviews, two additional recommendations to potentially strengthen the finding of a dissociation were raised during the discussion: (a) the authors could add to Figure 4A a measure of the gain of post-saccadic ocular following for trials when the motion stimulus remains visible. On p 15, line 266-269 they make the "important observation" that the stroke patients are capable of making smooth following eye movements when fixating the moving stimulus. However, that effect has not been quantified.

We now provide quantification of occipital stroke patients’ smooth following eye movements when motion remains visible at saccade end. Velocity traces after saccade landing exhibit smooth following of foveal stimulus motion, which for blind-fields occurs with a roughly 100 ms visuomotor delay (Figure 3D, red curve) as compared to intact-fields, where pre-saccadic information influences an earlier predictive response (Figure 3C, red curve). The quantified PFR gain and updated analyses are now presented in Results (lines 290-295, Figure 3—figure supplement 1). CB patients have no problem generating smooth, following eye movements for stimulus motion at the fovea after a visual response delay. It is only the predictive “open-loop” period (from 20-100 ms post-saccade) that differs between intact and blind-fields. That early period relies on motion processing from peripheral locations before the saccade is initiated, and this remains the focus of our main results.

(b) Rather than relying on a correlation across subjects, the authors could run statistics on individual patient data and show that the PFR gain of at least some patients is significantly worse than would be predicted from their motion discrimination thresholds. For instance, they could use the data from the intact fields to build a model of how motion discrimination predicts PFR, and then test whether each data point from a blind field falls outside the confidence interval of that model.

In sum, without clear quantitative evidence for the claimed dissociation the overall impact of this paper would be relatively weak.

We now show results of an exponential fit to the intact visual field data to describe the dependence of PFR on motion integration performance as a supplemental analysis (Figure 4—figure supplement 1). In this figure, the solid red line represents the model fit based on the intact-field data. The dotted red lines represent the confidence interval boundaries at 95%. Out of a total of 6 blind-field points (including data from our newly-recruited patients) within the normal range of NDR performance in the intact-field (white data points), 5 lie outside these confidence intervals, and have individual confidence intervals on their PFR gain that are instead overlapping or below zero. The probability that 5 of 6 points would lie outside the 95% confidence intervals, assuming they came from the same distribution as intact-fields is highly improbable (p <= 1.79x10-6, binomial test). We also note that the single point which fell within the range of the intact visual fields was unusual in other ways. Unlike other blind-field locations in our study, this point (Figure 1, CB6, lower left quadrant) did exhibit significant detection performance in Humphrey visual field tests (spared Humphrey visual field performance – i.e. luminance detection), even though global motion discrimination at that location was impaired and thus selected for training (see Methods). Nonetheless, even when this potential outlier was included in our analysis, we still found clear quantitative evidence to support the distinction in PFR gain within the range where normal motion discrimination performance has been recovered. This new analysis is included in Results (lines 310-323) and with Figure 4—figure supplement 1.

2. In terms of data analysis, the authors should report the mean values and confidence intervals for all quantitative measures (in addition to the reported statistics for the comparisons, exact p-values, t-values etc…).

The manuscript has now been modified to include these additional values throughout.

Importantly, the authors are encouraged to go beyond traditional null-hypothesis tests (t-tests and correlations) to make binary judgements of whether each effect or difference is "significant" (p<0.05). Some of the conclusions would be more convincing if supplemented with power analyses, bootstrapped confidence intervals, and Bayes factors to evaluate the strength of evidence.

We have expanded our analyses to provide boot-strapped confidence intervals for PFR across subjects (Figure 4A). In addition, we now include Bayes Factor t-tests for all the pair-wise comparisons in the revised manuscript.

3. Overall, the results might allow for a different interpretation: instead of assuming separate pathways for motion perception and oculomotor control in patients, the results might also be explained by a different read-out of the same motion signal for perception and oculomotor control, where oculomotor control applies a more conservative threshold and requires a higher internal signal strength than the motion perception.

This alternative interpretation merits consideration – we have expanded the Discussion to include it by adding a new paragraph (lines 393-408).

Further to the concern about data interpretation, a reviewer pointed out the potential role for presaccadic attentional mechanisms, which are currently not taken into account.

We considered the possibility that impairments of attentional mechanisms might impact PFR in our original manuscript (see original Discussion), but dismissed it as highly unlikely for two reasons: first, prior literature shows that attention can be directed into blind-fields and improve performance (Poggel et al., 2006; Kentridge et al., 2004; Cavanaugh et al., 2019). Second, we also find in the current paradigm, evidence for attentional selection of target motion in the deviation of saccade end-points along the motion direction of the target. We quantified this effect by measuring the angular deviations of saccades along target motion (the angle from the fixation point to the target center and to the saccade end-point). Saccades were positively deviated, reflecting pre-saccadic selection of target motion for both the intact and blind-fields (Figure 4—figure supplement 4). Similar shifts in saccade end-point have been correlated with changes in the perception of target location in previous studies (Schafer and Moore, 2007; Kosovicheva et al., 2014; Kwon et al., 2019), and may be more tightly correlated with perception than are smooth eye movements. Thus, we do find some positive evidence for attentional selection, although saccade deviations were weaker for blind that intact-fields, which could reflect sensory or attention deficits, and thus we sought additional controls.

To further test for deficits in attention allocation, we ran a new, control experiment in 2 of our new CB participants and implemented an analysis suggested by both Reviewers 2 and 3. These additional results appear in the Discussion where we expand the previous section on the role of attention (lines 442-462). In short, we asked if the lack of PFR in the blind-field was caused by an inability to allocate pre-saccadic attention to a blind-field target, against the competing influence of distractor apertures in intact regions of the visual field. We found no measurable PFR in the blindfield, even when distractor motion was removed, and PFR remained robust in the intact-fields comparable to the original design (Figure 4—figure supplement 5B). These additional results, in combination with the original positive result for saccade end-points, support that the lack of PFR does not reflect an impairment to allocate attention to the pre-saccadic target location.

Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

Unit of analysis: It seems that the unit of analysis was sometimes individual patients (N=8) and sometimes locations in the affected visual field (N=11). I think that treating multiple locations of one patient as independent samples is a form of pseudo replication (e.g. Lazic, 2010) and therefore the unit of analysis should always be individual patients.

We understand the reviewer’s concern. However, while one may claim inter-dependence of visual field locations within a given participant when dealing with visually-intact participants, there is considerable evidence in the CB literature to show that chronic cortically-blinded fields do not behave normally in this respect (Sahraie et al., 2006; Huxlin et al., 2009; Das et al., 2014; Saionz et al., 2020). Baseline performance and the ability of a location to improve are not uniform across CB fields, even at identical eccentricities. More relevant to the present comparisons, training-induced changes in discrimination and motion integration performance in the blind-field are tightly restricted to the trained, blind-field location and do not spread by more than 1° outside the boundaries of each trained location (Huxlin et al., 2009; Das et al., 2014; Saionz et al., 2020). This is unique – to our knowledge – to cortically-blinded fields and motivates why we maintain that it is appropriate to treat different blind-field locations as independent sites for training assessments. Nonetheless, we now present subject data in all figures except for Figure 4B and 4C, where we compare NDR thresholds and PFR for individual intact (n=20) and blind (n=14) field locations. In addition, we have included additional figures (Figure 4—figure supplement 2A and 2B) to show the outcome of re-analyzing the latter data set while pooling results per CB patient (11 patients = 11 intact vs. 11 blind-fields). Even when pooling data per subject, we still observe the trends shown in Figure 4B and 4C: lower NDR is correlated with higher PFR gain in the intact-fields (R2=0.568, F9=11.8334, p=0.0074, BF=8.0855), and no significant correlation between NDR and PFR gain in the blind-fields (R2=0.038, F9=0.3552, p=0.5659, BF=2.650) across the entire range of NDR. Further, for NDR < 0.35, the correlation between PFR and performance remains significant for intact-fields (R2=0.568, F9=11.8334, p=0.0074, BF=8.0855) but not for blindfields (R2=0.5406, F2=2.3538, p=0.2647, BF=0.7678). When we compared whether the slopes from each linear regression with respect to intact-fields and blind-fields under NDR < 0.35, we found that slopes (-1.012 for intact and -0.1845 for blind) were significantly different from each other (t11=-2.6039, p=0.0245). Thus, our main findings, where NDR thresholds and PFR gains were correlated per location, still hold when locations are pooled per subject. We have added this information in the revised manuscript’s Results section (lines 324-332).

Consciousness of motion perception: The authors interpret their motion discrimination task in terms of conscious motion perception, but strictly speaking the patients might also perform the task in a blindsight manner, where they have no conscious awareness of the motion itself but still are able to indicate the correct direction of motion.

We thank the reviewer for these suggestions. With respect to the first point, the Huxlin lab has previously described the properties of visual perception associated with training-induced recovery of NDR thresholds in cortically-blinded fields (a few key papers include Huxlin et al., 2009; Das et al., 2014; Saionz et al., 2020). In particular, the paper by Das and colleagues illustrated how direction integration of motion information presented by our random dot stimuli is not capable of eliciting blindsight. That global motion does not elicit blindsight was also shown by Azzopardi and Cowey (2001) and Vaina et al. (2014) among others. Prior to training, our patients were unable to describe or even correctly guess global motion direction in these stimuli in their blind-field – as such, they had no demonstrable blindsight for our global direction integration task. By the end of training, they could both correctly describe their conscious percept of the random dot stimuli and attain normal/near-normal NDR threshold levels of performance. The fact that patients are fully aware and can describe the stimulus in detail as well as how that percept changes when direction range within the stimulus is increased or decreased at different steps of the staircase within a session further implies conscious perception and not blindsight. We have added this information to the text of the Discussion (lines 525532).

Relevant literature: It might be interesting to relate the results to the theory and work by Milner and Goodale, who postulate a dissociation between perception and action as well.

We agree that perceptual therapies which restore conscious vision in CB appear to exhibit a dissociation with processing for action that naturally leads to parallels with the work of Milner and Goodale (TINS, 1992) where it was suggested that ventral stream projections from the striate cortex to the inferotemporal cortex play a major role in perception, while dorsal stream projections from the striate cortex to posterior parietal regions mediates the required sensorimotor transformations for visually-guided actions. Our current results suggest that after V1 damage, psychophysical training enables residual motion processing, likely in area MT and related dorsal pathway areas, to be used to recover perception without recovering the necessary processing for smooth eye movements. This suggests a dissociation between perception and visually-guided actions in this rehabilitation paradigm. We have added this information to the text of the Discussion (lines 532-541).

Definition and values of NDR thresholds: I don't quite understand why the NDR thresholds in Table 1 and in Figure 4 range between 0 and 1 % although they should range between 0 and 100 % according to the equation in the methods.

Our apologies – we fixed the equation in the method to reflect that NDR thresholds should range from 0 to 1 instead of 0 to 100%.

Restoration training: It would be useful to provide some information in what kind of training the patients participated.

Reference: Lazic, S. E. (2010). The problem of pseudo replication in neuroscientific studies: is it affecting your analysis?. BMC neuroscience, 11(1), 1-17.

We added some information about the training administered to participants in the

Methods section, along with references in which training protocols are described in even more detail (lines 584-607). We address the Lazic reference as described above with a supplemental analysis done per subject (Figure 4—figure supplement 2).

Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

In order to comply as much as possible with the journal recommendations, the authors should report the mean values and confidence intervals for all quantitative measures (in addition to the reported statistics for the comparisons, exact p-values, t-values etc…)

We have revised the manuscript to include the requested values in addition to the Bayes Factor analysis.

Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

(1) Throughout the manuscript, you rely on null-hypothesis tests that treat the outcome as binary, either significant if p<0.05, or completely supporting the null hypothesis if p>0.05. This is an outdated approach known to be problematic. For instance, consider the correlations in Figure 4B. One is test treated as significant and the other as showing no relationship. On line 244: "this relationship was lost in the blind field"; and line 276, "there was no significant correlation between restored NDR and PFR gains." If you take p>0.05 as your criterion for no relationship in a correlation, then yes. But the slope in Figure 4B doesn't appear to be 0. It seems quite possible that with more subjects from the same population, this correlation would be "significant." An example that goes in the other direction is in Supplementary Figure 2, the saccade angular deviations in the blind field: p=0.03, which is taken as strong evidence of normal motion perception in the blind field.

The conclusions regarding Figure 4B (now split into Figures 4B and 4C) have been substantially strengthened by collecting additional data and performing requested analyses (see above, revisions in Results on lines 303-323). We have also revised the Discussion to consider other supporting data and alternatives with less binary outcomes.

To improve your statistical analysis I would suggest several complementary approaches: (i) power analyses for "null" results to estimate how many participants would be required to get to p<0.05; (ii) Bayes Factors for the null vs alternate hypotheses. If you're using Matlab I recommend Bart Krekelberg's toolbox: https://klabhub.github.io/bayesFactor/ (ii) For one-sample t-tests I also recommend reporting 95% confidence intervals of the mean (obtained via bootstrapping), and for two-sample t-tests I would recommend reporting 95% bootstrapped confidence intervals for the mean difference.

Other reviewers also suggested the inclusion of Bayes Factors and boot-strapped confidence intervals, which we have now implemented throughout the manuscript. We find that all the results initially reported stand using these tests and the additional data we have collected.

(2) Some important details about the participants and the study design aren't explained until the Methods, which come at the end. Three details in particular confused me: (i) what the motion discrimination thresholds are, and how they're normalized. After I read the Methods section I better understood what the thresholds are, but I didn't understand the rationale for normalizing them the way you did. Why not just report the threshold direction range in degrees?

We have revised the Results to define how NDR thresholds are computed when they are first introduced (lines 127-135). The Huxlin and Pasternak labs have used direction range measures extensively in their research on motion processing in humans and animal models with cortical damage. The reason direction range in degrees has increasingly been converted to NDR thresholds in more recent publications from both labs is because direction range scales from low to high values as performance improves, and this is the inverse of the scaling for thresholds (where lower values mean better performance). NDR is a simple means of aligning integration performance to standards for reporting thresholds in the field.

(ii) The training that some patients underwent. It comes as a surprise in the Results that some visual field locations were trained and some weren't. Even in the abstract, the narrative is a little confusing.

We apologize for the confusion and have now clarified (Results: lines 117-126; Methods: lines 591-622) that our main goal was to assess whether and how PFR tracked with perception in the blind- and intact-fields. As such, PFR testing locations (which had to be within a specific eccentricity range and mirror symmetric across the 4 quadrants of the visual field) did not always fall on a trained, blind-field location (the requirements for selecting training locations are quite different than those for PFR measurements). This did not matter at all. In fact, the tight location and task-specificity of training-induced NDR recovery in the blind-field allowed us to attain a large range of NDR thresholds at blind-field PFR test locations, against which we were able to plot PFR gain.

(iii) The two ways in which saccade "accuracy" is computed. One measure is reported in percent correct, and the other in degrees. You may just need to add a few words in the Results to clarify that first you categorized each saccade in a binary fashion as either landing near the target or near a different stimulus.

We thank the reviewer for noting this point – we have clarified the two different measures of saccade accuracy where they are first introduced in Results (lines 174-178).

(3) A related question that should be clarified: when patients improve their motion discrimination after training, do they have a conscious percept of the motion, or is it another form of blindsight?

Please see our earlier response to Reviewer 1, and new information added to the Discussion (lines 525-532). In sum, our data support recovery of conscious perception and not just blindsight.

(4) I find these results to be very cool and indeed surprising. But I wonder whether they are limited to a particular set of unusual conditions, and a reader who spends less time thinking about oculomotor control may be puzzled by the particular set of conditions set up in this experiment. It does seem rather niche, in terms of the patient population, the particular eye movement, and the particular task and stimulus arrangement. Regarding the latter, your display contained 4 static apertures full of moving dots that don't really translate across the screen. Why did you need 4 apertures, rather than just one, and why not use single objects that actually moved? Does it matter that the target stimulus is 'competing' with three other irrelevant motion stimuli?

A key advantage of the current paradigm is that it enables one to measure the postsaccadic following response (PFR) to a motion stimulus that is spatially localized, and at the same time, enables one to plan a saccade to a specific spatial location, even if perception at that location is impaired. The paradigm was adapted from an earlier study (Kwon et al., 2019) which examined the effects of pre-saccadic attention when saccades were cued by a central line at fixation towards one of 4 apertures. We retained that design for consistency with the previous study (allowing comparison between patients and previously-tested controls) and because of the control it provides both in directing the saccade and localizing motion to a specific visual field location. However, we agree that it is not critical to have four apertures to obtain similar spatial localization and spatial cueing for saccades. As such, we ran an additional control experiment in two new patients using a single stimulus (Figure 4—figure supplement 5) and we were able to validate similar results regardless of whether one or four apertures were used. This suggest that observed effects do not rely on the complexity of the 4-stimulus apertures, or complexities in how attention is allocated between them, as discussed earlier under “Essential revisions for authors”, which is included in Discussion (lines 442-454).

Do these results generalize to more realistic settings when a person makes a saccade to a moving object that they then must pursue, like a bird flying across the sky? I myself have relied on such "unnatural" stimuli and I know there can be good reasons to use them carefully, but they need to be justified. And I imagine you had only a short amount of time with each patient, but we must also acknowledge that the whole story would be stronger if the phenomenon were demonstrated in a few different ways.

The rationale for our stimulus design is outlined in our response above, but as noted the findings do appear to generalize if a single aperture is used, and we believe they would generalize further to natural stimuli without the control afforded by using an aperture. If anything, the controlled conditions afforded by presenting stimuli within an aperture would limit our ability to measure the PFR and provide conservative estimates of its magnitude under natural conditions.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Data Citations

    1. Kwon S, Farenthold BK, Cavanaugh MR, Mitchell JF, Huxlin KR. 2021. Data from: Perceptual restoration fails to recover unconscious processing for smooth eye movements after occipital strokes. Dryad Digital Repository. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]

    Supplementary Materials

    Transparent reporting form

    Data Availability Statement

    Data for all figures has been shared on the Dryad. https://doi.org/10.6078/D1W69T.

    The following dataset was generated:

    Kwon S, Farenthold BK, Cavanaugh MR, Mitchell JF, Huxlin KR. 2021. Data from: Perceptual restoration fails to recover unconscious processing for smooth eye movements after occipital strokes. Dryad Digital Repository.


    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES