Skip to main content
Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience logoLink to Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience
. 2022 Jun 28;14:885787. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2022.885787

Sex-Specific Associations of Diabetes With Brain Structure and Function in a Geriatric Population

Elias G Thomas 1,2,*, Hanneke Rhodius-Meester 1,3, Lieza Exalto 4, Sanne A E Peters 5,6,7, Liselotte van Bloemendaal 1, Rudolf Ponds 8, Majon Muller 1
PMCID: PMC9273850  PMID: 35837485

Abstract

Introduction

Globally, women with dementia have a higher disease burden than men with dementia. In addition, women with diabetes especially are at higher risk for cognitive impairment and dementia compared to men with diabetes. Differences in the influence of diabetes on the cerebral vasculature and brain structure may contribute to these sex-specific differences. We examined sex-specific patterns in the relationship between diabetes and brain structure, as well as diabetes and cognitive function.

Methods

In total, 893 patients [age 79 ± 6.6 years, 446 (50%) women] from the Amsterdam Ageing Cohort with available data on brain structures (assessed by an MRI or CT scan) and cognitive function were included. All patients underwent a thorough standardized clinical and neuropsychological assessment (including tests on memory, executive functioning, processing speed, language). Brain structure abnormalities were quantified using visual scales.

Results

Cross-sectional multivariable regression analyses showed that diabetes was associated with increased incidence of cerebral lacunes and brain atrophy in women (OR 2.18 (1.00–4.72) but not in men. Furthermore, diabetes was associated with decreased executive function, processing speed and language in women [B −0.07 (0.00–0.13), −0.06 (0.02–0.10) and −0.07 (0.01–0.12) resp.] but not in men.

Conclusions

Diabetes is related to increased risk of having lacunes, brain atrophy and impaired cognitive function in women but not in men. Further research is required to understand the time trajectory leading up to these changes and to understand the mechanisms behind them in order to improve preventive health care for both sexes.

Keywords: sex-specific analysis, brain structure, diabetes, cognitive function, vascular aging, vascular cognitive impairment and dementia

Introduction

The prevalence of diabetes is increasing worldwide, with an expected rise from 537 million adults in 2021 to 783 million in 2045 (Sun et al., 2021). This not only leads to high mortality – more than 6.7 million deaths in 2021 alone – but also to high morbidity, including an increased risk of cognitive impairment and dementia (Arvanitakis et al., 2004; Yaffe et al., 2004; Liu, J. et al., 2018). However, not all individuals are similarly affected by the complications of diabetes. As early as 1979, and as confirmed more recently by cohort studies, it was shown that type 2 diabetes is a stronger risk factor for ischemic heart disease and stroke in women than in men (Kannel and McGee, 1979; Peters et al., 2014, 2020, 2021). Women with type 2 diabetes also have a higher excess risk of cognitive decline and vascular dementia, than their male counterparts, although the extent of these differences are dependent of study populations and their characteristics (Verhagen et al., 2022).

To date, it is unclear why men and women with diabetes are dissimilarly impacted by dementia. Since sex-related differences in the incidence of dementia are only present in the group with vascular dementia – not in Alzheimer's dementia – sex-specific patterns of cerebral vascular pathology may play a role in mediating these differences (Hayden et al., 2006; Chatterjee et al., 2016; Liu et al., 2018). Cerebral small vessel disease (cSVD) – including the presence of microbleeds, white matter hyperintensities and lacunes – is more prevalent in individuals with type 2 diabetes than those without (Troncoso et al., 2008; Moran et al., 2013; Geijselaers et al., 2015; Ter Telgte et al., 2018; Wardlaw et al., 2019). Although little is known about sex-specific susceptibility for cSVD, it seems plausible that the increased susceptibility of women to cerebrovascular complications of diabetes is manifested as an increased susceptibility to disease of the smaller cerebral vessels (Jiménez-Sánchez et al., 2021). In addition, diabetes is associated with increased rates of atrophy (Moran et al., 2013). Again, it is not known whether there are sex-related differences in this association, but it is known that atrophy and cSVD are closely related, and they are sometimes even collectively referred to as “brain structure” or “markers for brain health” (Mahammedi et al., 2021). Our primary goal in the present analysis is to assess if there are sex-specific pattern in the relationship between diabetes and brain structure, including cSVD and atrophy, and cognitive function as their clinical correlates.

Methods

Study Population

The Amsterdam Aging Cohort is an ongoing longitudinal cohort study which includes patients from the outpatient geriatric clinic at the Amsterdam University Medical Center, location VUmc (Rhodius-Meester et al., 2021). We included 893 patients with brain imaging who attended the memory clinic seeking medical care between February 2016 and June 2021. During this period, almost a patients visiting the memory clinic (89%) were willing to participate in the study. All patients were given a complete standardized comprehensive geriatric assessment (CGA) by trained nurses and doctors. This included an assessment of multiple geriatric domains, including cognition, physical function, nutrition, revision of medication in use and detailed medical history. Cognitive diagnosis – such as dementia (McKhann et al., 1984; Román et al., 1993; Neary et al., 1998; McKeith et al., 2005; Dubois et al., 2007; Rascovsky et al., 2011), mild cognitive impairment (MCI) (Albert et al., 2013), or subjective cognitive decline (SCD) (Studart and Nitrini, 2016) – were evaluated in a multidisciplinary consensus meeting. Our analysis included only patients who underwent brain Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI) or Computed Tomography (CT) as part of the diagnostic work-up. All patients gave written informed consent for their data to be used and the study was approved by the local Medical Ethics Committee.

Cardiovascular Risk and Disease

Diabetes mellitus (DM) was defined either as having a history of diabetes or using antidiabetic medication. Other cardiovascular diseases – including coronary disease, heart failure, atrial fibrillation, and peripheral artery disease – were assessed on the basis of medical history and double checked with the patient and their family or carer. We dichotomized smoking status (never smoked v. ever smoked). Blood pressure and gait speed, as well as patient height and weight, were measured during the visit (Odden et al., 2012). Venous blood was drawn from all patients to measure cholesterol levels and non-fasting glucose. Medication as provided by the patient's pharmacy was reviewed with the patient, and with a partner, family member or carer if necessary.

Cerebral Small Vessel Disease

Brain imaging was performed during a patient's first visit using CT (n = 238), 1.5T MRI (n = 162) and 3T MRI (n = 478) devices. The scans were reviewed and scored visually by two trained experts supervised by a clinical radiologist. Atrophy was scored on T1 sequence using visual rating scales ranging from 0 to 4 for medial temporal lobe atrophy (MTA), and from 0 to 3 for global cortical atrophy (GCA) (Harper et al., 2015). The average of the left and right side was used for MTA. White matter hyperintensities were scored on FLAIR/T2 sequence using the Fazekas scale (0–3) (Fazekas et al., 1987), and the number of microbleeds (on susceptibility-weighted imaging) and lacunes were counted. In this manuscript, we refer to either brain atrophy, white matter hyperintensities, lacunes or microbleeds collectively as “brain structure abnormalities.”

Cognitive Function

To assess whether the observed differences in brain structure also had a functional impact, cognition was included in the analysis. Cognitive performance was assessed in a standardized manner by trained neuropsychologists and divided into four domains: memory, language, executive function, and processing speed. All patients were assessed using the Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) and the Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS). Memory was tested with the auditory verbal learning test (Van Der Elst et al., 2005) and Visual Association Test (VAT) (Lindeboom et al., 2002). Language was tested using the Category Fluency Animals Test (Van Der Elst et al., 2006) and the VAT naming test, a component of the VAT. Processing speed was examined with the Stroop Color-Word test (SCWT) (Van der Elst et al., 2006) and the Trail Making Test-A (TMT-A) (Reitan, 1955). Finally, executive function was assessed with the Behavioral Assessment of the Dysexecutive Rule-changing test (BADS) (Burrell and Piguet, 2015) while correcting for speed using the SCWT and the TMT. For the purposes of the analysis, all test results were converted to Z-scores or inverse Z-scores. A higher Z-score indicates poorer performance.

Statistical Analysis

Baseline characteristics for men, women, and the total population are reported as mean (SD), or median (interquartile range) for categorical variables. Differences between groups were analyzed using Student's T-test, the Mann-Whitney U-test, the Kruskal Wallis test, ANOVA and chi-square testing where appropriate. First, logistic regression analyses were performed to assess the association of diabetes with brain structures separately for men and women. For the logistic regression analysis, we dichotomized the scores of the visual rating scale and the values for microbleeds and lacunes. A cut-off value of two or more was used for the imaging scores of atrophy (MTA and/or CGA) and WMH (Rhodius-Meester et al., 2017). Microbleeds were dichotomized as present or not present, and a value of one or more was adopted a cut-off for lacunes (Henneman et al., 2009; Jokinen et al., 2011). Second, linear regression analyses were performed to assess the association of diabetes with cognitive functioning separately for men and women. All analyses were adjusted for age (model 1), and additionally for smoking and alcohol consumption (model 2), and hypertension and cardiovascular disease (coronary disease, heart failure, atrial fibrillation, stroke or TIA and peripheral arterial disease) (model 3). In addition, we adjusted for presence of subjective complaints, mild cognitive impairment, or dementia (data not shown). For the analyses of functional cognitive measures, furthermore, we corrected all models for level of education. A p-value < 0.05 was considered statistically significant. Data were analyzed with SPSS software, version 26 (IBM Corp, Armonk, NY, USA).

To determine whether male or female sex and CVD was associated with a higher risk of cSVD to a greater degree than these factors individually, we added an interaction term to the regression analysis, testing multiplicative interaction. To assess additive interaction, we calculated RERI (Relative Risk due to Interaction) (Knol et al., 2007; Knol and VanderWeele, 2012). For this analysis, when the combined risk of sex and CVD was higher than the sum of the risks associated with the individual factors, the interaction between sex and CVD was considered to constitute an additional risk factor. A RERI above zero indicated that interaction between female sex and cardiac disease had an additional effect on the outcome; a RERI below zero indicated that this was the case for the interaction between male sex and cardiac disease. In the RERI analysis, we also corrected for age, smoking, and alcohol consumption, analogous to the logistic regression analyses. These analyses were performed in R (R Core Team, 2020).

Results

A total of 893 patients were included in the analysis (Table 1). The mean (SD) age was 79.6 years (73–86.2) and 50% were women. The prevalence of diabetes was 23.3% in men and 15.7% in women (p = 0.004), and men with diabetes were more often insulin-dependent compared to women (6.3% for men, 3.1% for women, p = 0.03). Women lived alone more often than men and their level of education was lower. Further, women had a lower prevalence of cardiovascular disease, consumed less alcohol, smoked less, had a lower body mass index (BMI) and a slightly higher diastolic BP, and they used less statins and anticoagulation drugs (Table 1). Women had slightly lower Mini-Mental State Examination scores, GDS, and men had higher brain atrophy scores. No differences in cognitive diagnosis were observed between men and women. Stratified analyses for sex and diabetes showed that differences in cardiovascular risk between men and women were more pronounced in those with diabetes compared to the total population (Supplementary Table 1).

Table 1.

Patient characteristics stratified for sex.

Total Men Women P-value
n = 893 n = 447 n = 446
Age in years 79.6 ± 6.6 79.1 ± 6.4 79.9 ± 6.7 0.096
Living situation 487 (54.5%) 330 (74.0%) 157 (35.1%) <0.001
   Independent, with partner 329 (36.8%) 89 (20.0%) 240 (53.7%)
   Independent, alone 32 (3.6%) 10 (2.2%) 22 (4.9%)
   Institutionalized 45 (5.0%) 17 (3.8%) 28 (6.3%)
   Other
Level of education <0.001
   Low education 180 (20.1%) 81 (18.1%) 99 (22.2%)
   Medium level education 317 (35.1%) 137 (30.7%) 176 (39.4%)
   Higher education or university 396 (44.3% 225 (50.4%) 171 (38.3%)
Diabetesa 176 (19.7%) 105 (23.4%) 71 (15.9%) 0.004
Antidiabetic medication
   Oral 112 (12.5%) 69 (15.4%) 43 (9.6%) 0.009
   Insulin 42 (4.7%) 28 (6.3%) 14 (3.1%) 0.028
Cardiovascular diseases
   Coronary disease 225 (25.2%) 155 (34.8%) 70 (15.7%) <0.001
   Heart failure 90 (10.1%) 56 (12.6%) 34 (7.6%) 0.014
   Atrial fibrillation 153 (17.1%) 87 (19.5%) 66 (14.8%) 0.060
   CVA/TIA 186 (20.8%) 111 (24.9%) 75 (16.8%) 0.003
   Peripheral artery disease 33 (3.6%) 16 (3.6%) 17 (3.8%) 0.861
Cardiovascular risk factors
   Alcohol consumption in units/week 1 (0–5) 2 (0–7) 1 (0–5) 0.003
   Smokers or ex-smokers 505 (56.6%) 289 (64.8%) 216 (48.3%) <0.001
   Hypertension 466 (52.2%) 228 (51.1%) 238 (53.2%) 0.525
   Hypercholesterolemia 221 (24.7%) 115 (25.8%) 106 (23.7%) 0.473
   Glucose in mmol/L 6.9 ± 2.6 6.9 ± 2.9 6.4 ± 2.5 0.302
   BMI in kg/m2 25.7 ± 4.6 26.1 ± 4.2 25.3 ± 4.9 0.012
   Systolic BP in mmHg 145.8 ± 21.8 144.5 ± 21.3 147.0 ± 22.3 0.100
   Diastolic BP in mmHg 80.6 ± 10.4 79.7 ± 10.5 81.4 ± 10.3 0.014
   LDL in mmol/L 2.56 ± 0.98 2.62 ± 0.98 2.47 ± 0.97 0.959
   HDL in mmol/L 1.58 ± 0.47 1.58 ± 0.47 1.58 ± 0.47 0.101
eGFR CKD-EPI in ml/min/1.73 m2 67.2 ± 16.2 67.0 ± 16.4 67.4 ± 16.0 0.755
Statin use 525 (58.8%) 208 (46.6%) 160 (35.8%) 0.001
Blood pressure lowering agents
Diuretics 147 (16.4%) 71 (15.9%) 76 (17.0%) 0.663
   RAAS-inhibition 171 (19.1%) 90 (20.2%) 81 (18.1%) 0.434
   Calcium-antagonists 63 (7.0%) 37 (8.3%) 26 (5.8%) 0.148
   Beta-blockers 105 (11.7%) 61 (13.7%) 44 (9.8%) 0.075
Anticoagulation
   DOAC/VKA 168 (18.8%) 96 (21.5%) 72 (16.1%) 0.038
   Platelet inhibition 288 (32.2%) 169 (37.9%) 119 (26.6%) <0.001
MMSE 24 (21–26) 25 (22–28) 24 (21–28) 0.008
GDS 3 (1–5) 3 (1–5) 3 (1–5) 0.093
Brain imaging
   MTA or GCA > 2 493 (55.2%) 282 (63.2%) 211 (47.2%) 0.007
   WMH >2 227 (25.4%) 108 (24.2%) 119 (26.6%) 0.398
   Lacunes ≥ 1 204 (22.8%) 112 (25.1%) 92 (20.6%) 0.098
   Microbleeds ≥ 3 163 (18.3%) 90 (20.2%) 73 (16.3%) 0.213
Cognitive diagnosis 0.085
   SCD 124 (13.8%) 62 (13.9%) 62 (13.9%)
   MCI 267 (29.8%) 148 (33.2%) 119 (26.6%)
   Dementia 502 (56.2%) 236 (52.9%) 266 (59.5%)

Data are presented as mean ± SD, n (%) or median [interquartile range]. Differences were tested with independent t-test for continuous variables and chi-square tests for categorical and for not normally distributed continuous variables.

BMI, body mass index; BP, blood pressure; CVA, cerebrovascular accident; DOAC, Direct Oral Anti-Coagulant; GCA, Global Cortical Atrophy; MCI, Mild Cognitive Impairment; MTA, Medial Temporal lobe Atrophy; TIA, Transient Ischemic Attack; SCD, Subjective Cognitive Decline; VKA, Vitamin K Antagonist; WMH, White Matter Hyperintensities.

atype I or II diabetes, not specified in our data collection.

The bold values indicate the p values which are statistically significant.

Sex Differences in the Relationship Between Diabetes and Brain Structure

The sex-specific logistic regression analyses of the relation between diabetes and brain structures showed that in women, the presence of diabetes was significantly associated with an increased risk of having brain atrophy and lacunes (Table 2). Age-adjusted odds ratios were 2.16 (95% CI 1.00–4.67) and 2.60 (95% CI 1.24–2.46). However, in men, diabetes was not associated with an increased risk of having brain structure abnormalities. Additional adjustments for cardiovascular risk factors and disease (model 2 and 3) did not change these effect estimates (Table 2). Adjusting for cognitive diagnosis (subjective complaints, mild cognitive impairment, or dementia) did not change the effect estimates (data not shown). Diabetes was not associated with an increased risk of WMH and microbleeds. When adding an interaction term to the regression, we did not find a significant interaction of sex with diabetes. When assessing additive interaction using RERI analysis, a trend was seen toward an increased risk for women with diabetes of atrophy and lacunes (RERI 0.45 for the presence of atrophy and female sex, and 0.48 for the presence of lacunes and female sex) (Supplementary Table 2).

Table 2.

The sex-specific relation of diabetes with changes in brain structure in older men and women (N = 893).

Men, n = 447 Women, n = 446 Interactiona p-valueb
OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI)
Atrophy
Non-diabetic Ref Ref Ref
Diabetic (model 1) 1.14 (0.60–2.18) 2.16 (1.00–4.67)* 1.22 (0.50–2.94) p = 0.66
Diabetic (model 2) 1.17 (0.60–2.29) 2.46 (1.11–5.42)* 1.32 (0.46–2.79) p = 0.77
Diabetic (model 3) 1.00 (0.51–1.96) 2.18 (1.00–4.72)* 1.17 (0.48–2.87) p = 0.71
WMH
Non-diabetic Ref Ref
Diabetic (model 1) 1.20 (0.76–1.89) 1.11 (0.65–1.89) 0.88 (0.47–4.00) p = 0.70
Diabetic (model 2) 1.15 (0.72–1.84) 1.09 (0.63–1.88) 0.85 (0.45–1.60) p = 0.61
Diabetic (model 3) 1.01 (0.63–1.62) 1.04 (0.61–1.79) 0.78 (0.41–1.49) p = 0.45
Microbleeds
Non-diabetic Ref Ref
Diabetic (model 1) 0.59 (0.26–1.32) 0.82 (0.31–2.20) 1.04 (0.32–3.34) p = 0.67
Diabetic (model 2) 0.65 (0.28–1.49) 0.86 (0.31–2.32) 0.97 (0.30–3.13) p = 0.95
Diabetic (model 3) 0.46 (0.20–1.00 0.75 (0.28–2.03) 0.93 (0.29–3.01) p = 0.90
Lacunes
Non-diabetic Ref Ref
Diabetic (model 1) 1.68 (0.92–3.04) 2.60 (1.24–2.46)* 1.10 (0.49–6.91) p = 0.82
Diabetic (model 2) 1.64 (0.87–3.07) 2.72 (1.24–5.93)* 1.00 (0.41–2.10) p = 0.84
Diabetic (model 3) 1.38 (0.74–2.56) 2.40 (1.13–5.07)* 0.95 (0.37–1.91) p = 0.63

WMH, White Matter Hyperintensities.

Data are presented as OR with (95% CI).

Model 1: adjusted for age.

Model 2: adjusted for age, smoking and alcohol consumption.

Model 3: Adjusted for age, hypertension, hypercholesterolemia, and presence of CVD (coronary disease, heart failure, atrial fibrillation, CVA or TIA, peripheral arterial disease).

*p-value < 0.05.

aInteraction term (sex multiplied by diabetes) added to the logistic regression.

bStatistical significance of the interaction term in the logistic regression analysis.

Sex Differences in the Relationship Between Diabetes and Cognitive Performance

The sex-specific linear regression analyses of the relation between diabetes and cognitive performance showed that diabetes in women was associated with a significantly lower score of executive function (beta z-score 0.07; 95% CI 0.00–0.14), processing speed (beta 0.06; 95% CI 0.90–0.95), and language (beta 0.07; 95% CI 0.01–0.12) (Table 3). In men, diabetes was not associated with cognitive performance. Additional adjustments for cardiovascular risk and disease, and cognitive diagnosis did not change the effect estimates (data for cognitive diagnosis not shown). We observed an interaction of sex and diabetes: women with diabetes were at increased risk for impaired processing speed (B 0.17 (0.03–0.33), p = 0.04). We observed no interaction between diabetes and sex in the other cognitive domains.

Table 3.

The sex specific relation of diabetes with cognitive performance in older men and women (N = 893).

Men Women Interactiona p-valueb
B (95% CI) B (95% CI) B (95% CI)
Memory
Non-diabetic Ref Ref Ref
Diabetic (model 1) 0.02 (−0.09 to 0.03) 0.02 (−0.07 to 0.03) 0.09 (−0.26 to 0.46) 0.59
Diabetic (model 2) 0.03 (−0.09 to 0.02) 0.02 (−0.07 to 0.03) 0.10 (−0.26 to 0.47) 0.57
Diabetic (model 3) 0.03 (−0.08 to 0.02) 0.01 (−0.06 to 0.03) 0.10 (−0.27 to 0.47) 0.59
Executive function
Non-diabetic Ref Ref
Diabetic (model 1) −0.02 (−0.09 to 0.04) −0.07 (−0.14 to 0.00)* −0.04 (−0.36 to 0.28) 0.81
Diabetic (model 2) −0.02 (−0.09 to 0.04) −0.06 (−0.13 to 0.01) −0.03 (−0.32 to 0.32) 0.98
Diabetic (model 3) −0.01 (−0.06 to 0.06) −0.07 (−0.13 to 0.00)* −0.01 (−0.33 to 0.32) 0.97
Processing speed
Non-diabetic Ref Ref
Diabetic (model 2) −0.03 (−0.08 to 0.01) −0.06 (−0.10 to 0.02)* 0.19 (0.03 to 0.34) 0.02
Diabetic (model 3) −0.03 (−0.08 – 0.02) −0.06 (−0.10 to 0.01)* 0.18 (0.02 to 0.33) 0.03
Diabetic (model 1) −0.02 (−0.07 to 0.01) −0.06 (−0.10 to 0.02)* 0.17 (0.03 to 0.33) 0.04
Language
Non-diabetic Ref Ref
Diabetic (model 1) −0.01 (−0.06 to 0.04) −0.07 (−0.12 to 0.01)* −0.18 (−0.53 to 0.17) 0.31
Diabetic (model 2) −0.01 (−0.05 to 0.07) −0.06 (−0.12 to 0.00) −0.12 (−0.48 to 0.22) 0.14
Diabetic (model 3) 0.00 (−0.05 to 0.04) −0.07 (−0.012 to 0.01)* −0.14 (−0.49 to 0.21) 0.43

Data are presented as unstandardized B with (95% CI).

A negative B signifies a correlation with worse Z-scores, e.g. with worse cognitive performance.

Model 1: adjusted for age and level of education; Model 2: adjusted for age, level of education, smoking and alcohol consumption; Model 3: Adjusted for age, level of education, presence of CVD (coronary disease, heart failure, atrial fibrillation, CVA or TIA, peripheral arterial disease), hypertension and hypercholesterolemia.

*p-value linear regression < 0.05.

aInteraction term (sex multiplied by diabetes) added to the logistic regression.

bStatistical significance of the interaction term in the logistic regression analysis.

The bold values indicate the p values which are statistically significant.

Discussion

In this study of 893 patients attending a geriatric outpatient memory clinic, we found that the presence of diabetes was associated with an increased risk of having brain structure abnormalities, specifically lacunes and atrophy, in women but not in men. We also found an additive interaction between female sex and these brain structure abnormalities, as tested by a RERI analysis. This finding complements previous studies which showed that women with diabetes may be more at risk of multiple forms of vascular pathology than men with diabetes, including coronary heart disease, stroke, and vascular dementia (Huxley et al., 2006; Peters et al., 2014; Chatterjee et al., 2016). It could be argued that other age-mediated cardiovascular risk factors such as hypertension and cardiac disease play a role in mediating the relationship between diabetes and brain structure abnormalities. However, we show that this relationship was independent of age, lifestyle, cardiovascular risk factors, and cardiac disease.

Additionally, we found that diabetes was significantly associated with worse cognitive performance in terms of executive function, processing speed and language, in the women in our population but not in the men. An interaction between sex and diabetes was also observed for processing speed, further strengthening the hypothesis of a true sex difference. These findings are in line with a recent study showing that women with diabetes have a higher risk of accelerated cognitive decline than men with diabetes (Verhagen et al., 2022). Sex-dependent physiology, as well as socio-cultural differences between men and women, may be the cause of these differences. We postulate a number of hypotheses below which may explain the association between diabetes and brain structure abnormalities as it is seen in women but not in men.

Pathophysiological Differences

Mechanisms which may affect susceptibility to the vascular complications of diabetes include altered coagulation, oxidative stress, endothelial dysfunction and impaired vasodilation (Kautzky-Willer et al., 2016; de Ritter et al., 2020). Women with diabetes might be in a more pro-thrombotic state than men, which may lead to lacunes and atrophy, and a more general decline in brain health, even when the prevalence of diabetes is similar in both sexes (Smith et al., 2012; Neergaard-Petersen et al., 2014). They generally also have greater levels of systemic inflammation and more oxidative stress than men with diabetes, leading to impaired vascular reactivity, which is specifically associated with the occurrence of lacunes (Mrgan et al., 2018). Sex-dependent differences in vascular physiology may therefore render women more susceptible to the cerebrovascular complications of diabetes, and also lead to functional decline.

Levels of central adiposity in men and women with diabetes may also be of importance. There is evidence to suggest that women have a poorer cardiovascular risk profile than men when they are diagnosed with diabetes, especially when central adiposity is measured (Paul et al., 2012; Peters et al., 2016a). This may be the result of a longer period of development of diabetes in women: women are more insulin-sensitive in middle age, and their insulin sensitivity deteriorates more than in men before they reach the diagnosis of diabetes. A longer period of time before a formal diagnosis of diabetes can be made may also lead to an increased occurrence of other risk factors such as abdominal adiposity, and to higher levels of subclinical damage mediated by hyperglycemia (Woodward et al., 2015; Peters et al., 2016b). Abdominal adiposity is independently related to brain structure abnormalities, including silent lacunary infarcts, and therefore may mediate the increased prevalence in women with diabetes by comparison with men (Yamashiro et al., 2014).

Furthermore, there is little awareness in the field of geriatrics about the long-term cardiovascular effects of pregnancy-related complications, as well as other women-specific factors such as timing of menopause and gestational diabetes (Keskin et al., 2015; Kuh et al., 2018). These important cardiovascular parameters are therefore often not registered in medical files, as is the case in our dataset (Wilkins-Haug et al., 2015). It has long been known that menopausal status and the timing of menopause influence cardiovascular risk, abdominal obesity, occurrence of DM, and clinical course of dementia (Archer, 2009; Gong et al., 2021; Hickey and Mishra, 2021). They may therefore also have affected the clinical outcome in our study. Further studies investigating the long-term effects of DM should therefore include sex-specific cardiovascular risk factors to assess their impact on brain structure and cognitive function.

Sex-Related Differences in Current Care

As a consequence of ongoing lower inclusion rates of women in studies investigating the long-term effects of diabetes, at least in part, it is unclear which mechanisms lead to the poorer clinical outcome of women with diabetes (Norhammar and Schenck-Gustafsson, 2013). However, there are several scientific findings which may play a role. Social gender norms have a profound influence on patients' disease perception, moment of referral, interpretation of symptoms and the likelihood of receiving guideline-recommended treatment. In the case of diabetes, evidence shows that women diagnosed with diabetes attain glycemic targets less often, and are screened less for the complications of diabetes (Choe et al., 2018). Risk factor targets for co-morbid cardiovascular disease are also less often achieved in women (Ferrara et al., 2008; Wannamethee et al., 2012; Rossi et al., 2013; de Jong et al., 2020). Since early intervention in diabetes improves long-term outcome, this may also have implications for the incidence of (cerebral) complications (Group UPDS, 1998). The same truth holds for cognitive impairment: women with dementia are generally referred later than men (Howard et al., 1998; Sourial et al., 2020). Women might therefore experience delay in receiving adequate supporting care when experiencing cognitive impairment. Hence, inequities in the recognition and treatment of (cardiovascular) risk factors for dementia, as well as the recognition and treatment of dementia itself, may play a role in the occurrence of sex-related differences in the cerebral complications of diabetes.

Strengths and Limitations

A major strength of our study is the standardized work-up of a large group of “real-life” patients. We included a multi-domain assessment which was part of medical routine care. Because of this integration in regular care practice, routinely used measurements and tools were used, facilitating the translation of our research to clinical practice. In addition, we combined these clinically used parameters with imaging markers as well as extensive neuropsychological testing, bridging the gap between etiological research and clinical practice. Our study has also several limitations. Firstly, because of our cross-sectional design, we cannot draw conclusions about the causality of our findings. We balanced this with logistic regression models in which we corrected for confounding factors. Related to this, we have no data showing possible differences between brain structure at the time of diagnosis of diabetes, and later life. It is possible that women with diabetes already have worse brain health at this time, and that it is not a direct consequence of diabetes, but merely coexists due to other pathological processes. Furthermore, HbA1c and time since diagnosis of DM were not included in our dataset. We were therefore unable to assess the influence of glycemic regulation. We did include non-fasted glucose, which was similar for men and women, in our baseline characteristics. Also, it is remarkable that the mean age in our sample is similar for men and women. Since the life expectancy in women exceeds the life expectancy in men, we would expect a higher mean age for women. The similar age for men and women may reflect underlying gender bias in referral to our clinic. However, it may also be explained by other forms of sampling bias not related sex or gender. Furthermore, as mentioned before, women-specific pathology was hardly registered at all in our patient files. A history of gestational diabetes, polycystic ovary syndrome and premature menopause contribute to the excess risk of diabetic complications and diabetes (Soedamah-Muthu et al., 2004; Huxley et al., 2006; Peters et al., 2014). Furthermore, preeclampsia is related to structural brain damage later in life (Siepmann et al., 2017). Lastly, premature menopause is additionally associated with poorer cognitive performance and a higher risk of dementia later in life (Ryan et al., 2014). However, the precise relationship between cerebrovascular disease and pregnancy-related cardiovascular disease remains unclear, since a recent review concluded that gestational hypertension is not related to cerebral stroke (Lo et al., 2020). Finally, other factors that affect the social position of patients and therefore their quality of care – such as class, cultural background, and variables associated with poorer referral and poorer health care provision in general – were not available in our study (Vaccarino et al., 2002; LaVeist et al., 2003).

In conclusion, this sex-specific analysis of the association between diabetes and its cerebrovascular complications shows that diabetes is significantly associated with brain structure abnormalities and function in women but not in men. We can only speculate on the nature of these differences, and whether they are dependent on gender or sex. Although we did not find a statistically significant interaction between female sex and diabetes, the differences in associations for men and women are striking and underline the importance of the sex-specific analysis of clinical data. Further research should at least include data on abdominal obesity and female-specific risk factors such as pregnancy-related complications and menopause, and more studies are needed to elucidate the mechanisms which contribute to the association between diabetes, brain structure and cognition in women. Elucidating the sex-specific relationships between diabetes and cSVD may help to understand the gap in burden of dementia and help to achieve more equity in the care for this group of patients.

Data Availability Statement

The data analyzed in this study is subject to the following licenses/restrictions: The data is not publicly available since it contains clinical privacy-sensitive patient information. Requests to access these datasets should be directed to majon.muller@amsterdamumc.nl.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Medical Ethical Committee of the Amsterdam UMC, location VUmc. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

Study conception and design and data collection: ET, MM, and HR-M. Analysis and interpretation of the results: ET, MM, HR-M, and SP. Draft manuscript preparation: ET, MM, HR-M, LE, SP, RP, and LB. All authors reviewed the results and approved the final version of the manuscript.

Funding

HR-M was recipient of the Memorabel Dementia Fellowship 2021 (ZonMw project number 10510022110004). Also, we acknowledge the support from the Netherlands CardioVascular Research Initiative: the Dutch Heart Foundation (CVON 2018-28 and 2012-06 Heart Brain Connection), Dutch Federation of University Medical Centres, the Netherlands Organisation for Health Research and Development, and the Royal Netherlands Academy of Sciences.

Conflict of Interest

HR-M performs contract research for Combinostics. All funding is paid to her institution. The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Dr. S.P. Thomas for his proofreading of the manuscript and perpetual moral support.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnagi.2022.885787/full#supplementary-material

References

  1. Albert M. S., DeKosky S. T., Dickson D., Dubois B., Feldman H. H., Fox N. C., et al. (2013). The diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment due to Alzheimer's disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer's Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer's disease. Focus 11, 96–106. 10.1176/appi.focus.11.1.96 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Archer D. (2009). Premature menopause increases cardiovascular risk. Climacteric 12(sup1), 26–31. 10.1080/13697130903013452 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Arvanitakis Z., Wilson R. S., Bienias J. L., Evans D. A., Bennett D. A. (2004). Diabetes mellitus and risk of Alzheimer's disease and decline in cognitive function. Arch. Neurol. 61, 661–666. 10.1001/archneur.61.5.661 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Burrell J. R., Piguet O. (2015). Lifting the veil: how to use clinical neuropsychology to assess dementia. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 86, 1216–1224. 10.1136/jnnp-2013-307483 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Chatterjee S., Peters S. A., Woodward M., Arango S. M., Batty G. D., Beckett N., et al. (2016). Type 2 diabetes as a risk factor for dementia in women compared with men: a pooled analysis of 2.3 million people comprising more than 100,000 cases of dementia. Diabetes Care. 39, 300–307. 10.2337/dc15-1588 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Choe S.-A., Kim J. Y., Ro Y. S., Cho S.-I. (2018). Women are less likely than men to achieve optimal glycemic control after 1 year of treatment: a multi-level analysis of a Korean primary care cohort. PLoS One 13, e0196719. 10.1371/journal.pone.0196719 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. de Jong M., Oskam M. J., Sep S. J., Ozcan B., Rutters F., Sijbrands E. J., et al. (2020). Sex differences in cardiometabolic risk factors, pharmacological treatment and risk factor control in type 2 diabetes: findings from the Dutch Diabetes Pearl cohort. BMJ Open Diabetes Res. Care 8, e001365. 10.1136/bmjdrc-2020-001365 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. de Ritter R., de Jong M., Vos R. C., van der Kallen C. J., Sep S. J., Woodward M., et al. (2020). Sex differences in the risk of vascular disease associated with diabetes. Biol. Sex Differ. 11, 1–11. 10.1186/s13293-019-0277-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Dubois B., Feldman H. H., Jacova C., DeKosky S. T., Barberger-Gateau P., Cummings J., et al. (2007). Research criteria for the diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease: revising the NINCDS–ADRDA criteria. Lancet Neurol. 6, 734–746. 10.1016/S1474-4422(07)70178-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Fazekas F., Chawluk J. B., Alavi A., Hurtig H. I., Zimmerman R. A. (1987). MR signal abnormalities at 1.5 T in Alzheimer's dementia and normal aging. Am. J. Neuroradiol. 8, 421–426. 10.2214/ajr.149.2.351 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Ferrara A., Mangione C. M., Kim C., Marrero D. G., Curb D., Stevens M., et al. (2008). Sex disparities in control and treatment of modifiable cardiovascular disease risk factors among patients with diabetes: Translating Research Into Action for Diabetes (TRIAD) Study. Diabetes Care. 31, 69–74. 10.2337/dc07-1244 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Geijselaers S. L., Sep S. J., Stehouwer C. D., Biessels G. J. (2015). Glucose regulation, cognition, and brain MRI in type 2 diabetes: a systematic review. Lancet Diabetes Endocrinol.. 3, 75–89. 10.1016/S2213-8587(14)70148-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Gong J., Harris K., Peters S. A., Woodward M. (2021). Reproductive factors and the risk of incident dementia: results from the UK Biobank. 10.21203/rs.3.rs-359787/v1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Group UPDS (1998). Intensive blood-glucose control with sulphonylureas or insulin compared with conventional treatment and risk of complications in patients with type 2 diabetes (UKPDS 33). Lancet 352, 837–853. 10.1016/S0140-6736(98)07019-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Harper L., Barkhof F., Fox N. C., Schott J. M. (2015). Using visual rating to diagnose dementia: a critical evaluation of MRI atrophy scales. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 86, 1225–1233. 10.1136/jnnp-2014-310090 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Hayden K. M., Zandi P. P., Lyketsos C. G., Khachaturian A. S., Bastian L. A., Charoonruk G., et al. (2006). Vascular risk factors for incident Alzheimer's disease and vascular dementia: the Cache County study. Alzheimer's Dis. Assoc. Disord. 20, 93–100. 10.1097/01.wad.0000213814.43047.86 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Henneman W. J., Sluimer J. D., Cordonnier C., Baak M. M., Scheltens P., Barkhof F., et al. (2009). MRI biomarkers of vascular damage and atrophy predicting mortality in a memory clinic population. Stroke 40, 492–498. 10.1161/STROKEAHA.108.516286 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Hickey M., Mishra G. D. (2021). Timing and type of menopause and risk of cardiovascular disease. Menopause 28, 477–479. 10.1097/GME.0000000000001747 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Howard B. V., Cowan L. D., Go O., Welty T. K., Robbins D. C., Lee E. T., et al. (1998). Adverse effects of diabetes on multiple cardiovascular disease risk factors in women: the Strong Heart Study. Diabetes care. 21, 1258–1265. 10.2337/diacare.21.8.1258 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Huxley R., Barzi F., Woodward M. (2006). Excess risk of fatal coronary heart disease associated with diabetes in men and women: meta-analysis of 37 prospective cohort studies. BMJ. 332, 73–78. 10.1136/bmj.38678.389583.7C [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Jiménez-Sánchez L., Hamilton O. K., Clancy U., Backhouse E. V., Stewart C. R., Stringer M. S., et al. (2021). Sex differences in Cerebral Small Vessel Disease: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Front. Neurol. 12, 756887. 10.3389/fneur.2021.756887 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Jokinen H., Gouw A., Madureira S., Ylikoski R., Van Straaten E., Van Der Flier W., et al. (2011). Incident lacunes influence cognitive decline: the LADIS study. Neurology. 76, 1872–1878. 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31821d752f [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Kannel W. B., McGee D. L. (1979). Diabetes and cardiovascular risk factors: the Framingham study. Circulation. 59, 8–13. 10.1161/01.CIR.59.1.8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Kautzky-Willer A., Harreiter J., Pacini G. (2016). Sex and gender differences in risk, pathophysiology and complications of type 2 diabetes mellitus. Endocr. Rev. 37, 278–316. 10.1210/er.2015-1137 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Keskin F., Ozyazar M., Pala A., Elmali A., Yilmaz B., Uygunoglu U., et al. (2015). Evaluation of cognitive functions in gestational diabetes mellitus. Exp. Clin. Endocrinol. Diabetes. 123, 246–251. 10.1055/s-0034-1395634 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Knol M. J., van der Tweel I., Grobbee D. E., Numans M. E., Geerlings M. I. (2007). Estimating interaction on an additive scale between continuous determinants in a logistic regression model. Int. J. Epidemiol. 36, 1111–1118. 10.1093/ije/dym157 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Knol M. J., VanderWeele T. J. (2012). Recommendations for presenting analyses of effect modification and interaction. Int. J Epidemiol. 41, 514–520. 10.1093/ije/dyr218 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Kuh D., Cooper R., Moore A., Richards M., Hardy R. (2018). Age at menopause and lifetime cognition: findings from a British birth cohort study. Neurology. 90:e1673–e81. 10.1212/WNL.0000000000005486 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. LaVeist T. A., Nuru-Jeter A., Jones K. E. (2003). The association of doctor-patient race concordance with health services utilization. J. Public Health Policy 24, 312–323. 10.2307/3343378 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Lindeboom J., Schmand B., Tulner L., Walstra G., Jonker C. (2002). Visual association test to detect early dementia of the Alzheimer's type. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 73, 126–133. 10.1136/jnnp.73.2.126 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Liu C.-L., Lin M.-Y., Hwang S.-J., Liu C.-K., Lee H.-L., Wu M.-T. (2018). Factors associated with type 2 diabetes in patients with vascular dementia: a population-based cross-sectional study. BMC Endocr. Disord. 18, 1–7. 10.1186/s12902-018-0273-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Liu J., Rutten-Jacobs L., Liu M., Markus H. S., Traylor M. (2018). Causal impact of type 2 diabetes mellitus on cerebral small vessel disease: a Mendelian randomization analysis. Stroke. 49, 1325–1331. 10.1161/STROKEAHA.117.020536 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Lo C. C. W., Lo A. C., Leow S. H., Fisher G., Corker B., Batho O., et al. (2020). Future cardiovascular disease risk for women with gestational hypertension: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Am. Heart Assoc. 9, e013991. 10.1161/JAHA.119.013991 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Mahammedi A., Wang L., Williamson B., Khatri P., Kissela B., Sawyer R., et al. (2021). Small vessel disease, a marker of brain health: what the radiologist needs to know. Am. J. Neuroradiol. 43, 650–660. 10.3174/ajnr.A7302 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. McKeith I. G., Dickson D. W., Lowe J., Emre M., O'brien J., Feldman H., et al. (2005). Diagnosis and management of dementia with Lewy bodies: third report of the DLB Consortium. Neurology 65, 1863–1872. 10.1212/01.wnl.0000187889.17253.b1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. McKhann G., Drachman D., Folstein M., Katzman R., Price D., Stadlan E. M. (1984). Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease: Report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group* under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer's Disease. Neurology 34, 939-. 10.1212/WNL.34.7.939 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Moran C., Phan T. G., Chen J., Blizzard L., Beare R., Venn A., et al. (2013). Brain atrophy in type 2 diabetes: regional distribution and influence on cognition. Diabetes Care 36, 4036–4042. 10.2337/dc13-0143 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Mrgan M., Gram J., Hecht Olsen M., Dey D., Linde Nørgaard B., Gram J., et al. (2018). Sex differences in coronary plaque composition evaluated by coronary computed tomography angiography in newly diagnosed Type 2 diabetes: association with low-grade inflammation. Diabetic Med. 35, 1588–1595. 10.1111/dme.13768 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Neary D., Snowden J. S., Gustafson L., Passant U., Stuss D., Black S., et al. (1998). Frontotemporal lobar degeneration: a consensus on clinical diagnostic criteria. Neurology 51, 1546–1554. 10.1212/WNL.51.6.1546 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Neergaard-Petersen S., Hvas A.-M., Kristensen S. D., Grove E. L., Larsen S. B., Phoenix F., et al. (2014). The influence of type 2 diabetes on fibrin clot properties in patients with coronary artery disease. Thromb. Haemost. 112, 1142–1150. 10.1160/th14-05-0468 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Norhammar A., Schenck-Gustafsson K. (2013). Type 2 diabetes and cardiovascular disease in women. Diabetologia 56, 1–9. 10.1007/s00125-012-2694-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Odden M. C., Peralta C. A., Haan M. N., Covinsky K. E. (2012). Rethinking the association of high blood pressure with mortality in elderly adults: the impact of frailty. Arch. Intern. Med. 172, 1162–1168. 10.1001/archinternmed.2012.2555 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Paul S., Thomas G., Majeed A., Khunti K., Klein K. (2012). Women develop type 2 diabetes at a higher body mass index than men. Diabetologia 55, 1556–1557. 10.1007/s00125-012-2496-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Peters S. A., Carcel C., Millett E. R., Woodward M. (2020). Sex differences in the association between major risk factors and the risk of stroke in the UK Biobank cohort study. Neurology 95, e2715–e2726. 10.1212/WNL.0000000000010982 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Peters S. A., Huxley R. R., Woodward M. (2014). Diabetes as a risk factor for stroke in women compared with men: a systematic review and meta-analysis of 64 cohorts, including 775 385 individuals and 12 539 strokes. The Lancet. 383, 1973–1980. 10.1016/S0140-6736(14)60040-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Peters S. A., Huxley R. R., Woodward M. (2016a). Sex differences in body anthropometry and composition in individuals with and without diabetes in the UK Biobank. BMJ Open 6, e010007. 10.1136/bmjopen-2015-010007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Peters S. A., Singhateh Y., Mackay D., Huxley R. R., Woodward M. (2016b). Total cholesterol as a risk factor for coronary heart disease and stroke in women compared with men: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Atherosclerosis 248, 123–131. 10.1016/j.atherosclerosis.2016.03.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Peters T. M., Holmes M. V., Richards J. B., Palmer T., Forgetta V., Lindgren C. M., et al. (2021). Sex differences in the risk of coronary heart disease associated with type 2 diabetes: a mendelian randomization analysis. Diabetes Care. 44, 556–562. 10.2337/dc20-1137 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. R Core Team. (2020). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. Available online at: https://www.R-project.org/
  50. Rascovsky K., Hodges J. R., Knopman D., Mendez M. F., Kramer J. H., Neuhaus J., et al. (2011). Sensitivity of revised diagnostic criteria for the behavioural variant of frontotemporal dementia. Brain 134, 2456–2477. 10.1093/brain/awr179 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Reitan R. M. (1955). The relation of the trail making test to organic brain damage. J. Consult. Psychol. 19, 393. 10.1037/h0044509 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Rhodius-Meester H. F., Benedictus M. R., Wattjes M. P., Barkhof F., Scheltens P., Muller M., et al. (2017). MRI visual ratings of brain atrophy and white matter hyperintensities across the spectrum of cognitive decline are differently affected by age and diagnosis. Front. Aging Neurosci. 9, 117. 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00117 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Rhodius-Meester H. F. M., van de Schraaf S. A., Peters M. J., Kleipool E. E., Trappenburg M. C., Muller M. (2021). Mortality risk and its association with geriatric domain deficits in older outpatients: the amsterdam ageing cohort. Gerontology 67, 194–201. 10.1159/000512048 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Román G. C., Tatemichi T. K., Erkinjuntti T., Cummings J., Masdeu J., Garcia J., et al. (1993). Vascular dementia: diagnostic criteria for research studies: report of the NINDS-AIREN International Workshop. Neurology 43, 250. 10.1212/WNL.43.2.250 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Rossi M. C., Cristofaro M. R., Gentile S., Lucisano G., Manicardi V., Mulas M. F., et al. (2013). Sex disparities in the quality of diabetes care: biological and cultural factors may play a different role for different outcomes: a cross-sectional observational study from the AMD Annals initiative. Diabetes Care 36, 3162–3168. 10.2337/dc13-0184 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Ryan J., Scali J., Carriere I., Amieva H., Rouaud O., Berr C., et al. (2014). Impact of a premature menopause on cognitive function in later life. BJOG 121, 1729–1739. 10.1111/1471-0528.12828 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Siepmann T., Boardman H., Bilderbeck A., Griffanti L., Kenworthy Y., Zwager C., et al. (2017). Long-term cerebral white and gray matter changes after preeclampsia. Neurology. 88, 1256–1264. 10.1212/WNL.0000000000003765 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Smith E. E., Schneider J. A., Wardlaw J. M., Greenberg S. M. (2012). Cerebral microinfarcts: the invisible lesions. Lancet Neurol. 11, 272–282. 10.1016/S1474-4422(11)70307-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Soedamah-Muthu S. S., Chaturvedi N., Toeller M., Ferriss B., Reboldi P., Michel G., et al. (2004). Risk factors for coronary heart disease in type 1 diabetic patients in Europe: the EURODIAB Prospective Complications Study. Diabetes Care. 27, 530–537. 10.2337/diacare.27.2.530 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Sourial N., Arsenault-Lapierre G., Margo-Dermer E., Henein M., Vedel I. (2020). Sex differences in the management of persons with dementia following a subnational primary care policy intervention. Int. J. Equity Health 19, 1–6. 10.1186/s12939-020-01285-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Studart A. N., Nitrini R. (2016). Subjective cognitive decline: the first clinical manifestation of Alzheimer's disease? Dement Neuropsychol. 10, 170–177. 10.1590/S1980-5764-2016DN1003002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Sun H., Saeedi P., Karuranga S., Pinkepank M., Ogurtsova K., Duncan B. B., et al. (2021). IDF diabetes Atlas: global, regional and country-level diabetes prevalence estimates for 2021 and projections for 2045. Diabetes Res. Clin. Pract. 183, 109119. 10.1016/j.diabres.2021.109119 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Ter Telgte A., van Leijsen E. M., Wiegertjes K., Klijn C. J., Tuladhar A. M., de Leeuw F.-E. (2018). Cerebral small vessel disease: from a focal to a global perspective. Nat. Rev. Neurol. 14, 387–398. 10.1038/s41582-018-0014-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Troncoso J. C., Zonderman A. B., Resnick S. M., Crain B., Pletnikova O., O'Brien R. J. (2008). Effect of infarcts on dementia in the Baltimore longitudinal study of aging. Ann. Neurol. 64, 168–176. 10.1002/ana.21413 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Vaccarino V., Abramson J. L., Veledar E., Weintraub W. S. (2002). Sex differences in hospital mortality after coronary artery bypass surgery: evidence for a higher mortality in younger women. Circulation 105, 1176–1181. 10.1161/hc1002.105133 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Van Der Elst W., Van Boxtel M. P., Van Breukelen G. J., Jolles J. (2005). Rey's verbal learning test: normative data for 1855 healthy participants aged 24–81 years and the influence of age, sex, education, and mode of presentation. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 11, 290–302. 10.1017/S1355617705050344 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Van der Elst W., Van Boxtel M. P., Van Breukelen G. J., Jolles J. (2006). The Stroop color-word test: influence of age, sex, and education; and normative data for a large sample across the adult age range. Assessment. 13, 62–79. 10.1177/1073191105283427 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Van Der Elst W., Van Boxtel M. P., Van Breukelen G. J., Jolles J. (2006). Normative data for the Animal, Profession and Letter M Naming verbal fluency tests for Dutch speaking participants and the effects of age, education, and sex. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 12, 80–89. 10.1017/S1355617706060115 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Verhagen C., Janssen J., Biessels G. J., Johansen O. E., Exalto L. G. (2022). Females with type 2 diabetes are at higher risk for accelerated cognitive decline than males: CAROLINA-COGNITION study. Nutr. Metab. Cardiovasc. Dis. 32, 355–364. Available online at: 10.1016/j.numecd.2021.10.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Wannamethee S., Papacosta O., Lawlor D., Whincup P., Lowe G., Ebrahim S., et al. (2012). Do women exhibit greater differences in established and novel risk factors between diabetes and non-diabetes than men? The British Regional Heart Study and British Women's Heart Health Study. Diabetologia 55, 80–87. 10.1007/s00125-011-2284-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Wardlaw J. M., Smith C., Dichgans M. (2019). Small vessel disease: mechanisms and clinical implications. Lancet Neurol. 18, 684–696. 10.1016/S1474-4422(19)30079-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Wilkins-Haug L., Celi A., Thomas A., Frolkis J., Seely E. W. (2015). Recognition by women's health care providers of long-term cardiovascular disease risk after preeclampsia. Obstet. Gynecol. 125, 1287–1292. 10.1097/AOG.0000000000000856 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Woodward M., Peters S. A., Huxley R. R. (2015). Diabetes and the female disadvantage. Women's Health 11, 833–839. 10.2217/whe.15.67 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Yaffe K., Blackwell T., Kanaya A., Davidowitz N., Barrett-Connor E., Krueger K. (2004). Diabetes, impaired fasting glucose, and development of cognitive impairment in older women. Neurology. 63, 658–663. 10.1212/01.WNL.0000134666.64593.BA [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Yamashiro K., Tanaka R., Tanaka Y., Miyamoto N., Shimada Y., Ueno Y., et al. (2014). Visceral fat accumulation is associated with cerebral small vessel disease. Eur. J. Neurol. 21, 667–673. 10.1111/ene.12374 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Data Availability Statement

The data analyzed in this study is subject to the following licenses/restrictions: The data is not publicly available since it contains clinical privacy-sensitive patient information. Requests to access these datasets should be directed to majon.muller@amsterdamumc.nl.


Articles from Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Frontiers Media SA

RESOURCES