Abstract
Objective
Researchers have suggested that psychotherapy may be enhanced by the addition of 3,4‐methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA), particularly in the treatment of disorders wherein interpersonal dysfunction is central, such as social anxiety disorder. We review literature pertaining to three potential processes of change that may be instigated during sessions involving MDMA administration in the treatment of social anxiety disorder.
Design
This is a narrative review that integrates research on the etiology and maintenance of social anxiety disorder and mechanisms of action of MDMA to examine how MDMA may enhance psychotherapy outcomes.
Results
We first outline how MDMA may enhance memory reconsolidation in social anxiety disorder. We then discuss how MDMA may induce experiences of self‐transcendence and self‐transcendent emotions such as compassion, love, and awe; and how these experiences may be therapeutic in the context of social anxiety disorder. We subsequently discuss the possibility that MDMA may enhance the strength and effectiveness of the therapeutic relationship which is a robust predictor of outcomes across many disorders as well as a potential key ingredient in treating disorders where shame and social disconnection are central factors.
Conclusion
We discuss how processes of change may extend beyond the MDMA dosing sessions themselves.
Keywords: MDMA, memory reconsolidation, processes of change, self‐transcendence, social anxiety disorder, therapeutic relationship
1. INTRODUCTION
Social anxiety disorder (SAD) is a prevalent and disabling disorder characterized by intense fear of being scrutinized or negatively evaluated by others in social situations (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2013). SAD is the fourth most commonly diagnosed psychological disorder in the United States (Kessler et al., 2005) with onset typically occurring in adolescence and assuming a chronic course even following treatment (Morris et al., 2005). SAD has significant public health costs including increased workplace absenteeism, lower worker productivity, and unemployment (APA, 2013; Stuhldreher et al., 2014). Furthermore, it often precedes the development of other psychiatric conditions, for example, increasing the risk for the development of major depressive disorder (Adams et al., 2016), and doubling the likelihood of developing an alcohol use disorder (Magee et al., 1996).
Although evidence‐based treatments for SAD exist, including medications and therapy, a significant proportion of patients either do not respond or remain considerably symptomatic at the end of treatment (Loerinc et al., 2015; Mayo‐Wilson et al., 2014). In terms of pharmacotherapy, meta‐analytic evidence supports the conclusion that SSRIs are superior to placebo but also that SSRIs are associated with higher treatment dropout than placebo over the treatment course (Williams et al., 2017). In terms of psychotherapy, cognitive behavioral therapy for social anxiety disorder demonstrates the strongest outcomes (Mayo‐Wilson et al., 2014), with other approaches, such as acceptance and commitment therapy (Niles et al., 2014), interpersonal psychotherapy (Stangier et al., 2011), emotion‐focused therapy (Shahar et al., 2017), and psychodynamic treatment (Leichsenring et al., 2013) also showing some efficacy.
In the past decade, there has been increased interest in pharmacotherapies to augment the effectiveness of psychotherapy for SAD. For example, d‐cycloserine has been tested in several trials and preliminary evidence suggests it may provide a small augmentation effect for exposure therapy targeting social anxiety (Mataix‐Cols et al., 2017). In addition, MDMA (3,4‐methylenedioxymethamphetamine) has been tested as a means to augment psychotherapy for social anxiety in adults with autism (Danforth et al., 2018). In a randomized, placebo‐controlled trial, 12 participants with autism and SAD were treated with two sessions of MDMA‐assisted therapy (MDMA‐AT) along with non‐drug preparatory and integrative psychotherapy. Participants in the MDMA‐AT group showed dramatically reduced SAD symptom severity at the post‐treatment primary outcome point (between group effect size: d = 1.4) after two doses of MDMA, providing preliminary evidence for the efficacy of MDMA as a possible treatment augmentation strategy for SAD.
2. MDMA AND MDMA‐ASSISTED THERAPY
MDMA primarily affects the serotonin, norepinephrine, and dopamine systems (Liechti & Vollenweider, 2001) and stimulates hormonal responses related to oxytocin, vasopressin, prolactin, and cortisol (Emanuele et al., 2006; Mas et al., 1999; Passie et al., 2005). Until MDMA was made illegal in the U.S.A., it had been used as an adjunct to psychotherapy for about a decade. Therapists at the time noted that MDMA created heightened feelings of empathy, reduced patient defensiveness, facilitated direct communication, and helped the processing of traumatic events (Passie, 2018). The first controlled study of MDMA‐AT was published in 2011 (Mithoefer et al., 2011) and, in total, five clinical trials of MDMA‐AT have been published including one on SAD and four on PTSD (Luoma et al., 2020). A recent pooled analysis of 103 patients from six Phase II clinical trials (Mithoefer et al., 2019) found a large between group difference (g = 0.8) compared to placebo at primary end point.
In established trials, MDMA‐AT is typically delivered by a team of two therapists using a combination of non‐drug psychotherapy sessions and MDMA dosing sessions. Typically, there are three preparation sessions which involve setting expectations and intentions for treatment, reviewing the patient's history and presenting problem, establishing safety and trust in the therapeutic alliance, the teaching of skills that may be used in dosing sessions, and educating about likely events that might occur in dosing sessions. This is then typically followed by two to three dosing sessions, with three integration sessions occurring after each dosing session. Dosing sessions last approximately 8 h, during which the client receives the investigational drug and spends the session transitioning between states of internally focused attention facilitated by the use of eyeshades and music and externally focused interactions with the therapists. Integration sessions involve processing what occurred in the dosing session, concretizing gained insights or new learning, and translating the experience into meaningful life changes.
3. PREVIOUS WORK ON MDMA‐AT PROCESSES OF CHANGE
The primary goal of this paper is to outline potential processes of change in proposed MDMA‐AT for SAD. We choose to use the term 'processes of change' instead of mechanisms of action as it is less restrictive, allowing for feedback loops and bidirectionality, in contrast with the term 'mechanisms', which implies a more linear causal pattern (Hayes et al., 2019). Processes of change can be studied at many levels of analysis, ranging from molecular, neurological, psychological or even sociological levels (Hofmann et al., 2020). Each of these levels of analysis has utility in guiding future treatment development in this domain; however, this paper focuses on the psychological level as it has the most direct relevance to the ongoing development of the psychotherapy component of MDMA‐AT. The practical aim of a scientific understanding of processes of change is to guide therapist efforts toward a focus on variables most likely to maximize therapeutic outcomes.
Though many experimental, non‐clinical studies have examined the effects of MDMA in humans (for reviews see Feduccia & Mithoefer, 2018; Jungaberle et al., 2018), some of which are reviewed below, processes of change associated with MDMA have been notably understudied. Quantitative data on the subject are sparse, with only one available randomized, controlled trial showing that MDMA‐AT resulted in larger changes in the personality trait of openness to experience compared to a placebo condition among people with PTSD (Wagner et al., 2017). The authors proposed that MDMA may have facilitated a tendency toward seeking out new experiences and openness to self‐examination, which may have led to improved PTSD outcomes. No authors to date have outlined potential processes of change that might be specific to SAD, but some researchers have noted that MDMA, because of its effects on social perception and cognition and on social hormones such as oxytocin and prolactin, seems particularly well suited to treat disorders wherein interpersonal dysfunction is central, such as social anxiety, autism, or major depressive disorder (Heifets & Malenka, 2016).
Due to the paucity of the direct study of processes of change in MDMA‐AT, this narrative review largely relies on experimental and correlational findings on MDMA and anecdotal reports to hypothesize possible processes of change occurring during MDMA‐AT. Where relevant, we also discuss the findings of a qualitative study of perceived benefits of treatment among 19 participants who completed MDMA‐AT for PTSD (Barone et al., 2019). It is important to keep in mind that the generalizability of research on MDMA use in experimental and uncontrolled settings to clinical settings may be limited as the effect of MDMA has been shown to be dependent upon the social context in which the drug is used. Drug effects are not independent of the social environment (Alboni et al., 2017) and multiple studies in rodents show that this is also true with MDMA (Hake et al., 2019; Nardou et al., 2019). For example, Nardou et al. (2019) showed that MDMA only increased sensitivity to social reinforcement among mice who received MDMA when in the presence of another mouse, but not in mice receiving the drug while alone. In humans, an experimental study found that participants who were assigned to take a low‐dose of MDMA in the presence of other participants reported increased intensity of acute effects of MDMA, such as higher subjective ratings of “feeling the drug” and increased cardiovascular activity, compared to those taking the drug alone (Kirkpatrick & de Wit, 2015). These results imply that the social context of MDMA administration is essential in understanding the effects of MDMA and that the effects of the drug cannot be understood without taking this context into account (Carhart‐Harris et al., 2018).
4. POSSIBLE PROCESSES OF CHANGE IN MDMA‐AT FOR SOCIAL ANXIETY DISORDER
This paper reviews literature relevant to three interdependent processes of change we hypothesize may be instigated during dosing sessions and further consolidated during integration sessions. We focus first on the possibility that MDMA may enhance memory reconsolidation in SAD, an idea that has been previously discussed in relation to PTSD (Feduccia & Mithoefer, 2018). We then discuss how MDMA may induce experiences of self‐transcendence and self‐transcendent emotions, such as compassion, love, and awe and how these experiences may be therapeutic in the context of SAD. Finally, we discuss the possibility that MDMA may enhance the strength and effectiveness of the therapeutic relationship (Johansen & Krebs, 2009), potentiating corrective emotional and interpersonal experiences in which socially anxious patients feel safe enough to behave authentically, facilitating greater openness to acceptance and warmth from their therapist. While these processes are presented separately for clarity in this paper, we hypothesize that they are interrelated and influence each other.
This paper focuses primarily on events that occur during dosing sessions. Yet the content of integration sessions is also likely to be central in further reinforcing and elaborating processes initiated during dosing sessions. Similarly, the content of preparation sessions is likely to influence the chance that these processes are likely to occur during dosing sessions. In addition, there may be drug‐related effects that are more delayed, such as those found in a series of studies in mice by Nardou et al. (2019), which showed that MDMA increased sensitivity to social rewards for at least two weeks post‐dosing. This suggests that, among humans, MDMA may affect responses to social reinforcement for an undetermined period after the acute drug has left the body and that this potential process of change may interact with the person's social context in the weeks after dosing. Due to space constraints, we only focus on these three processes and how they may unfold during dosing sessions; we do not mean to imply that activities in integration sessions are not important or that other processes of change may not also be central.
4.1. Enhanced memory reconsolidation
Memory reconsolidation refers to a type of neuroplasticity that occurs when a memory is reactivated, initiating a critical period during which the memory trace is destabilized and then can be modified and updated with new information before being consolidated again into long term memory (Elsey et al., 2018). Memory reconsolidation has been proposed as a common mechanism across many psychotherapy approaches (e.g., Lane et al., 2015) and has been specifically proposed as a mechanism in the context of MDMA‐AT (Feduccia & Mithoefer, 2018). We seek to outline how this process may relate to SAD.
People with SAD have a large bank of readily accessible implicit and explicit memories of being shamed, humiliated, rejected, ostracized and generally devalued. Experiences with maltreatment are a generalized risk factor for the development of SAD (Bruce et al., 2012; Simon et al., 2009; Wong & Rapee, 2016), with emotional abuse and neglect being associated with symptom severity in patient samples with SAD (Bruce et al., 2012; Simon et al., 2009) and prospectively predicting the onset of SAD (Kessler et al., 1997). Social victimization among peers may be a particularly strong risk factor for the development of social anxiety (Norton & Abbott, 2017a, 2017b; Sansen et al., 2015). One study of people with SAD found that 98% reported aversive early experiences with peers (Norton & Abbott, 2017b).
At least partially as a result of these salient experiences, people with SAD exhibit enhanced retrieval of negative autobiographical memories and reduced availability of positive autobiographical memories (Stopa & Jenkins, 2007). Compared to non‐anxious controls, people with SAD recall positive memories with impoverished detail (Moscovitch et al., 2011) and negative memories with greater internal detail and a higher degree of self‐relevance (Moscovitch et al., 2011, 2018). People with greater social anxiety also tend to recall more self‐defining negative memories and memories related to social anxiety (Krans et al., 2014). Recall biases may also affect recent memories, with research showing that people higher in social anxiety tend to recall positive feedback about a social task more negatively over time (Glazier & Alden, 2017) compared to people low in social anxiety. The recall of negative memories, particularly those related to victimization, has been shown to heighten physiological arousal among people with SAD, with those who reported past experiences of peer victimization demonstrating higher skin conductance to recall of negative memories (Sansen et al., 2015).
More easily accessible negative memories also appear to be related to the prevalent intrusive negative self‐imagery found in most people with SAD (Chiupka et al., 2012). The vast majority of people with SAD report negative self‐imagery in social situations (Homer & Deeprose, 2017; Moscovitch et al., 2011, 2018), with most reporting that the imagery is linked to identifiable autobiographical memories (Hackmann et al., 1998, 2000; Moscovitch et al., 2011). Preliminary evidence suggests that the occurrence of this negative self‐imagery linked to autobiographical memory might be a causal factor in the maintenance of SAD (Makkar & Grisham, 2011). Together, the above findings implicate the important role of autobiographical memory and self‐imagery in maintaining SAD and suggest that targeting these memories and attempting to alter parameters associated with them (their meaning, current levels of intrusiveness) may be particularly important in treatment for SAD. Relatedly, several studies have shown preliminary benefits of a technique called imagery rescripting that involves engaging with negative autobiographical memories while inserting novel imagery and emotional experiences (Knutsson et al., 2019; Langkaas et al., 2017; Nilsson et al., 2012; Reimer & Moscovitch, 2015; Wild & Clark, 2011). Rescripting interventions have been shown to alter imagery and memory details and the appraisal of the meaning of the memory, as well as reduce intrusiveness, vividness, and negative affect associated with target memories (Reimer & Moscovitch, 2015; Romano et al., 2020).
Experimental studies of fear conditioning in rats and mice have suggested that MDMA may facilitate memory reconsolidation of fear memories (Hake et al., 2019; Young et al., 2015). Possible neurobiological mechanisms for how MDMA may enhance memory reconsolidation include altered action in the medial pre‐frontal cortex and amygdala (Carhart‐Harris et al., 2015) and a 5‐HT‐dependent reduction in Brain Derived Neurotropic Factor (BDNF; Hake et al., 2019), a neurotropin important for memory reconsolidation (Bekinschtein et al., 2014). In addition, basic science shows that MDMA alters memory functioning in a variety of ways (for a review see Feduccia & Mithoefer, 2018). In humans, MDMA appears to diminish the encoding of emotional information (Doss et al., 2018) and enhance the vividness of positive autobiographical memories (Carhart‐Harris et al., 2014), with MDMA potentially reducing the recollection of specific details of emotional events, rather than the ability to recall the event itself (Doss et al., 2018). This could be helpful in the treatment of intrusive imagery commonly found in SAD (Chiupka et al., 2012) if upsetting memories are re‐encoded with weakened associations to emotionally evocative details.
We posit that MDMA‐AT may facilitate memory reconsolidation through a) enhancing the activation of relevant memory traces and b) intensifying the prediction errors that are central to updating these memory traces. With respect to the former, the activation of a relevant memory trace is necessary for reconsolidation. Several etiological models of SAD have recently emphasized that the fear most central to the maintenance of SAD is that the authentic, core self is undesirable, deficient, or inadequate and that this self, if revealed to others, will trigger ostracism, rejection, and further shame (Elliott & Shahar, 2018; Moscovitch, 2009). Accordingly, the therapeutic context needs to facilitate the activation of memory traces consistent with this core fear in order for SAD‐relevant reconsolidation to occur, but it is important to note that activation of the memory trace in session does not necessarily need to entail the recall of explicit autobiographical memories. In humans, activated memory traces may be primarily explicit or implicit in nature, with implicit memory referring to the influence of prior events on current experience and behavior in the absence of explicit recall or awareness of the experience that led to that learning (Lane et al., 2015), and explicit memory referring to events that are recalled with awareness (e.g., autobiographical memory). Regardless of the cue, when this fear is activated, autobiographical memories are likely to arise since this central fear involves a perceived inadequate self and is linked autobiographical (traumatic) memories.
We hypothesize that MDMA may serve both to elicit the cue for the core fear memories that need reconsolidation while also fostering a strong prediction error. A prediction error is a mismatch between expected and current event and is thought to be essential for reconsolidation to occur (Fernández et al., 2017). If the core fear of people with SAD is that revealing the authentic self will lead to ostracism, rejection, and shame, then a strong prediction error would conceivably occur if the authentic self is revealed and these events do not occur. MDMA tends to elicit an experience of authenticity (Baggott et al., 2016; Passie, 2018) in its users, thus conceivably activating the core memory traces that need to be reconsolidated in SAD. However, under the effects of MDMA, rather than the typically accompanying feelings of fear, the person with SAD is instead likely to experience feelings of peace, safety, or even love (Studerus et al., 2010). This would conceivably foster a strong prediction error that may facilitate memory reconsolidation. A prediction error in which outcomes of current events do not match with expectations is thought to be a particularly strong signal to update an aversive memory trace (Elsey et al., 2018) and the experiences of safety, love, and increased perception of empathy from others (Jungaberle et al., 2018) typically elicited by MDMA, simultaneously occurring in a context of felt authenticity, would run counter to the expectations of people with SAD. In addition, because MDMA can induce this experience for several hours, this may allow for a more prolonged period of learning compared to the more limited moments of feeling connected and cared for in more typical talk therapy. In addition, because MDMA increases people's focus on social and emotional content (Kamilar‐Britt & Bedi, 2015; Wardle et al., 2014), this may further facilitate memory reconsolidation relevant to core fears. The extended focus on social and emotional content related to social fears might also facilitate the elaboration of autobiographical memories with new, more adaptive information that may not have been available at the time the original memory was laid down, such as an awareness of the social context surrounding the events or the development of a more compassionate, nuanced perspective of the events. Later integrations sessions might then help bridge this learning to other parts of the person's life and social situations.
In sum, we hypothesize that MDMA‐AT dosing sessions may elicit relevant core fears that serve to maintain SAD while simultaneously promoting new emotional experiences that then lead to a new memory trace via reconsolidation (Lane et al., 2015). We further elaborate on how MDMA might enhance this prediction error in the section on the therapeutic relationship. Effectively studying memory reconsolidation in the context of MDMA‐AT likely requires measurement across a variety of levels including cognition, behavior, genetic, and peripheral and central physiology as well as across multiple domains with each level (Fernández et al., 2017). Furthermore, it may require multiple conditions aimed specifically at experimentally manipulating the variables thought to lead to reconsolidation. Acknowledging these complexities, an example of a face valid measurement approach at the cognitive level may be the use of structured interview methods to assess changes in the content (i.e., specific positive or negative details) and appraisals (i.e., the meaning ascribed to the event) related to core memories of shame and rejection identified by the client before and after dosing and integration sessions (Romano et al., 2020).
4.2. Self‐transcendent experiences and emotions
The functioning of the self is considered central to most models of SAD (Clark & Wells, 1995; Elliott & Shahar, 2018; Gilboa‐Schechtman et al., 2020; Hofmann, 2000; Moscovitch, 2009). While conceptualizations of the self are numerous, one common distinction is between: 1) the self as content, sometimes referred to as the self‐as‐object (Gilboa‐Schechtman et al., 2020), conceptualized self (Luoma et al., 2017) or simply as “me;” and 2) the self‐as‐subject (Gilboa‐Schechtman et al., 2020), sometimes referred to as the observer self (Luoma et al., 2017), perspective‐taking‐self (Mchugh & Stewart, 2012), or the “I.” Most models of SAD focus on the content of the self, with less attention paid to the functioning of the subjective self or the I from which we experience. Repeated experiences of social mistreatment and shame, combined with difficulty in achieving adequate levels of interpersonal validation and reduced social efficacy result in the development of a sense of self as inadequate, inferior, and flawed (Elliott & Shahar, 2018). Research has demonstrated that SAD is more highly correlated with shame than are other anxiety disorders (Cândea & Szentagotai‐Tăta, 2018) and that the self‐concept of people with SAD is generally negative (Anderson et al., 2008) and manifested in heightened self‐criticism (Cox et al., 2004), beliefs relating to social inferiority (Gilbert, 2000), greater negative self‐related imagery (Makkar & Grisham, 2011), and heightened access to negative autobiographical memories (Stopa & Jenkins, 2007).
In contrast, much less research focuses on the functioning of the subjective self or the 'I' in SAD, despite its potential importance. A notable exception is research on attentional biases, which show that SAD is associated with a general tendency to attend to threat‐related cues (Bar‐Haim et al., 2007) and a difficulty focusing attention on positive and affiliative cues (Taylor et al., 2011). These biases may be due to the documented tendency for people who perceive themselves as low in social rank to monitor more for social threat (Gilboa‐Schechtman et al., 2020). People with SAD tend to focus more on their own behavior, whether that is in the form of heightened self‐consciousness or increased self‐focused attention to interoceptive cues in social situations (Norton & Abbott, 2015). Neuroscientific findings also show that the insula, which is associated with interoceptive processing, is more active in people with SAD (Stein, 2015). In sum, there are various alterations in the function of the self that are associated with SAD both at a content and process level.
MDMA may help address some of these self‐related difficulties through fostering experiences with self‐transcendence. MDMA often produces strong positive emotions and, most importantly in this discussion, self‐transcendent positive emotions. Self‐transcendent positive emotions represent a subset of positive emotions and include compassion, awe, gratitude, appreciation, inspiration, admiration, and love. Experimental research on MDMA administration shows that the single most commonly experienced phenomenon under MDMA is a blissful state characterized by feelings of pleasure, peace, and love (Studerus et al., 2010). Clinical observations indicate that MDMA often fosters an attitude toward the self and one's experiences characterized by positive feelings such as compassion (Passie, 2018) as shown in participant reports such as, “…with MDMA, I feel, even though I'm crying, I feel good” (Barone et al., 2019).
Self‐transcendent positive emotions have been shown to increase perceptions of social connectedness (Shiota et al., 2007), prosocial behavior (Goetz et al., 2010; Piff et al., 2015), and encourage individuals to transcend their own momentary needs and desires and focus on those of another (Stellar et al., 2017). Self‐transcendent emotions are also central to binding people to others, whether that is with a partner, children, or larger groups. Self‐transcendent emotions generated through practices such as lovingkindness meditation have been shown to result in increased high frequency heart rate variability, an indicator of improved cardiac response (Lumma et al., 2015), reduced self‐criticism (Shahar et al., 2015), and greater social connectedness (Hutcherson et al., 2008; Kok & Fredrickson, 2015; Kok & Singer, 2017). Since self‐transcendent positive emotions focus attention on the needs and welfare of others, they may be helpful in reducing the rigid and excessive self‐directed attention found in SAD (Stellar et al., 2017). To be clear, we would not necessarily expect that MDMA would reduce self‐focused attention during dosing session, since an internal focus is typically encouraged during such treatment (Mithoefer, 2014). However, we would anticipate that this internal self‐focusing during dosing sessions would be less characterized by the critical self‐monitoring aimed at threat management typical of SAD, but rather a more mindful and open state of self‐reflection that occurs in a state of felt safety (Kirschner et al., 2019; Liotti & Gilbert, 2011). In addition, neuroscience has found that receptors connected to oxytocin and prolactin are particularly prevalent in brain regions related to self‐transcendent emotions (Landgraf & Neumann, 2004), suggesting the possibility that the oxytocin and prolactin release documented in MDMA administration may explain, at least partially, how MDMA fosters self‐transcendent positive emotions.
MDMA also appears to foster self‐transcendent experiences (STEs), which incorporate self‐transcendent positive emotions, but are broader in scope. STEs involve periods of reduced self‐salience and an increased sense of connectedness (Yaden, 2017). They may also help decrease internal focus and enhance positive self‐representations of people with SAD (Gilboa‐Schechtman et al., 2020). During STEs, the normative experience of being a bounded, separate self is reduced; the sense of a separate self may even dissolve completely in more extreme STEs, such as mystical experiences (Nour et al., 2016). In Yaden et al.‘s view (2017), self‐transcendence has both an “annihilational” component that refers to a dissolution of a bodily sense of self and self‐boundaries and a “relational” component characterized by a sense of oneness with something beyond the self, often with other people or one's environment. This second aspect of self‐transcendence appears most relevant to the experience of MDMA as it commonly evokes an experience of unity characterized by feeling part of a larger whole, feeling one with the environment, and a sense that the future and past are dissolving into an all‐encompassing now (Studerus et al., 2010). Induction of strong experiences of interconnection and feelings of closeness with others may create more enduring shifts toward a sense of shared humanity and interconnection with others, which may be helpful for people with SAD who typically feel different from and inferior to others, disconnected, prone to rejection, and lonely. One study of people in treatment for SAD found that people who reported seeing themselves as closer to others had better treatment outcomes, suggesting that shifts toward a greater sense of closeness and similarity to others may improve outcomes (Meuret et al., 2016)
Some of the self‐transcendent emotions and experiences in MDMA‐AT may occur in the form of self‐compassion, which is likely to be particularly helpful in relation to the high levels of shame experienced by people with SAD. Experimental research has shown that self‐compassion increases when people take MDMA/ecstasy (Kamboj et al., 2015, 2018) and experiences with self‐compassion are commonly reported in MDMA‐AT as shown in qualitative reports like, “It was really that first MDMA session that we had…where I was able to clearly see that I had a big disconnect in compassion that I had for myself. In fact, I didn't have, really, any compassion for myself, other than maybe enough just to want to try to get help,” (Barone et al., 2019).
Self‐compassion refers to experiencing feelings of caring and kindness toward oneself, an understanding, nonjudgmental attitude toward one's inadequacies and failures, and a recognition that one's own experience is part of the common human experience (Neff, 2003). This last aspect of self‐compassion is transcendent in that it involves seeing or experiencing oneself as part of a larger whole of humanity in which one's own perceived flaws or inadequacies lose salience when embedded in a larger context. Individuals with SAD report lower self‐compassion compared to healthy controls and among those with a SAD diagnosis, greater self‐compassion has been related to decreased fear of positive and negative evaluation (Werner et al., 2012) and reduced social anxiety severity (Makadi & Koszycki, 2020). Further, increases in self‐compassion through meditation practice have been shown to reduce anticipatory anxiety related to a public speaking task among individuals high, but not low, in social anxiety (Hake et al., 2017) and an experimental study showed that women who participated in a self‐compassion exercise prior to a public speaking task demonstrated lower physiological markers of social stress than women across two other control conditions (Arch et al., 2014). There is also some preliminary data suggesting that treatments incorporating self‐compassion practices may be helpful in the treatment of SAD (Boersma, et al., 2015; Koszycki et al., 2016). Taken together, these findings suggest a beneficial buffering effect of self‐compassion from which socially anxious individuals were not previously benefiting.
In sum, MDMA‐AT regularly induces experiences of self‐transcendence and self‐transcendent emotions, which may have a range of therapeutic effects on people with SAD, including reducing sensitivity to social‐evaluative threat, increasing self‐compassion, reducing self‐focused attention, increasing a sense of interconnection with and similarity to others, and increasing attention to positive social events. Researchers wishing to study self‐transcendent experiences in MDMA‐AT might consider measures such as state and trait versions of the Self‐Compassion Scale (Neff, 2003), Ego Dissolution Inventory (Nour et al., 2016), Nondual Awareness Dimensional Assessment (Hanley et al., 2018), Gratitude Questionnaire (McCullough et al., 2002), the blissful, unity, and percepts subscales of the Altered States of Consciousness Rating Scale (Studerus et al., 2010), the Inclusion Of Other In The Self Scale (Aron et al., 1992), Small Self Scale (Piff et al., 2015), or modified Differential Emotions Scale (Fredrickson et al., 2003).
4.3. Enhanced therapeutic relationship
A strong therapeutic relationship has been identified as centrally important for all forms of psychotherapy (Norcross & Lambert, 2018). The therapeutic relationship has many qualities, including those related to the working environment (agreement on task and goals) and those related to the therapist‐patient emotional bond (empathy, positive regard, congruence, attachment). Meta‐analytic studies have consistently shown that all of these relationship elements are associated with favorable treatment outcomes. A robust set of meta‐analytic studies has shown that the therapeutic alliance, a construct that includes both the therapist‐patient bond and agreement on tasks and goals, is predictive of positive outcomes (Flückiger et al., 2018). Other meta‐analytic studies specifically focusing on relationship qualities show similar associations with outcome, such as empathy (Elliott et al., 2018), positive regard and affirmation (Farber et al., 2018) and congruence/genuineness (Kolden et al., 2018). In addition, some models of therapy hold that the alliance is central in fostering therapeutic change in SAD (Elliott & Shahar, 2018; Leichsenring et al., 2013).
Only few studies directly examine the association between the alliance and outcomes within SAD samples. These studies have shown mixed results, with some studies showing a positive association between the alliance and outcome (Haug et al., 2016; Kivity et al., 2020) and some studies reporting no such association (Mörtberg, 2014; Woody & Adessky, 2002). However, the sample sizes of these studies on alliance in SAD were relatively small, some of them were on group therapy, and some showed limited variability in some aspects of the alliance (e.g., Mörtberg, 2014), thus limiting the statistical power to detect effects typically found in the rest of the literature and introducing potential statistical confounds related the measurement of alliance in group treatments. It is also possible that the alliance, especially the bond component, may matter less in structured treatments like CBT that make up the bulk of the research cited above (Mörtberg, 2014). Given the strength of the findings about outcome‐alliance associations transdiagnostically, it seems likely the alliance is important in treating SAD, though this has not been directly proven.
Our hypothesis is that MDMA may particularly enhance the therapeutic relationship, and specifically the therapist‐patient emotional bond by initiating several processes. MDMA typically induces a state in which people feel safe around others and less sensitive to rejection, as shown in studies showing that MDMA reduces the effects of simulated rejection experiences (Bershad et al., 2016). Anecdotal reports suggest that this extends to therapeutic situations as shown in patient reports that MDMA was “very calming. That's the safest I've probably ever felt” and “I (felt) like you [the therapist] would be able to walk through hell with me.” (Barone et al., 2019). MDMA‐AT has been observed to reduce threat sensitivity (Baggott et al., 2016), increase disclosure in therapy, and enable people to receive both praise and criticism with more acceptance (Passie, 2018). This may help reduce the keeping of secrets that can interfere with therapy (Farber et al., 2019), which can also help the client feel more authentic. The tendency for MDMA to reduce fear in social situations may also generalize to reduced fear of compassion that is common in people with SAD and often functions as a barrier to receiving compassion from the therapist (Gilbert et al., 2011) and from oneself.
MDMA also induces friendly, playful, and loving feelings towards others (Bedi et al., 2010) and may promote prosocial behavior and generosity (Kamilar‐Britt & Bedi, 2015). Accordingly, clients ingesting MDMA are likely to engage in warm and positive expressions toward therapists that will in turn trigger greater expressions of affiliative emotions from therapists, as research shows that affiliative behavior cues affiliative behavior in others (Markey et al., 2003; Sadler et al., 2009), at least in part through the operation of the mirror neuron system (Jeon & Lee, 2018). The tendency for MDMA to strengthen responses (measured at both a subjective and neurophysiological level and often called emotional empathy) to positive interpersonal situations and emotional expressions of others (Bedi et al., 2009; Hysek et al., 2014; Jungaberle et al., 2018; Kuypers et al., 2017; Wardle & De Wit, 2014; Wardle et al., 2014) may make therapist expressions of positive emotion such as compassion and liking more salient at both a visceral and cognitive level. This may also be enhanced by the tendency for MDMA to make people perceive others as more empathic of them (Bershad et al., 2016).
This enhanced therapeutic relationship should potentiate new (corrective) interpersonal and emotional learning (Christian et al., 2012) and memory reconsolidation. As discussed above, memory reconsolidation first requires that the memory is activated. If the core fear of people with SAD is that the exposure of a seemingly flawed and inferior self will result in rejection and shaming from others, then the therapeutic situation in MDMA‐AT is likely to enhance and prolong contact with cues that normally elicit this fear, thus potentiating learning. In addition, MDMA may enhance the expectancy mismatch needed for memory change through reconsolidation. If the person with SAD expects that revealing their authentic self will result in rejection, mistreatment, or shame, a strong prediction error would presumably occur as MDMA enhances the likelihood the therapist will be experienced as fully and unconditionally accepting, caring, warm, safe, and compassionate.
Additionally, some models propose that optimal reconsolidation and learning is activated in the context of enough arousal to activate relevant memory traces, but also moderate enough to allow an experience of safety during the therapeutic interaction (Lane et al., 2015; Siegel, 1999). MDMA may help keep the person in this optimal zone of learning by fostering a sense that one is being authentic, thereby triggering core fears related to shame, while simultaneously creating a sense of safety in the relationship. As illustrated by one MDMA‐AT patient, “I felt like I had the ability and tools, whereas before I was unarmed, unarmored, and had no support. And this type of environment, with [the therapists], the catalyst drug, and everything else, it felt as though I had backup. Now it was safe, and I had my tools and weapons to be able to tackle the obstacles that I never had before.” (Barone et al., 2019).
Finally, the extended period of authentic connection with the therapist and intense experience of a dosing session could lead to lasting improvements in the therapeutic relationship. The resulting stronger relationship could be subsequently helpful in facilitating whatever additional gains could be made during following non‐dosing integration sessions. This deeper connection and corrective experience may also serve as a prototype of more meaningful social relationships for people with SAD in their own lives. Researchers examining therapeutic alliance in MDMA dosing sessions might consider state measures of authenticity such as the Self Experiences Questionnaire (Plasencia et al., 2011), closeness with therapists such as by utilizing the Inclusion of Other in the Self Scale (Aron et al., 1992), or measures of therapeutic alliance such as the Working Alliance Inventory (Tracey & Kokotovic, 1989) or the Agnew Relationship Measure (Cahill et al., 2012), a less commonly used measure of the therapeutic alliance that seems more appropriate to the relatively non‐directive context of dosing sessions and which includes a subscale related to feeling free to disclose personal material to the therapist.
5. CONCLUSION
Now that clinical trials are establishing the efficacy of MDMA‐AT, it is time that researchers pay more attention to identifying possible processes of change relating to MDMA‐AT. Scientific progress cannot come exclusively from randomized trials showing that a treatment works at the group level, it must also come from examining how different interventions affect different, theory‐driven processes of change hypothesized to occur during treatment. This approach could enable clinical scientists to more effectively tailor in‐session interventions delivered in dosing sessions to capitalize on relevant therapeutic processes enhanced by MDMA, thereby potentially improving clinical outcomes. Current trials of MDMA‐AT generally feature a non‐directive approach that emphasizes the role of an inner‐healing intelligence purported to be activated by MDMA (Mithoefer, 2014). However, the one pilot trial that incorporated a more structured approach, cognitive‐behavioral conjoint therapy, appeared promising, suggesting more structured interventions might be successfully coupled with MDMA (Monson et al., 2020). An empirical understanding of processes of change could guide how to incorporate other psychotherapy models into MDMA‐AT that target processes of change found to be important in research. Research on process‐outcome relationships holds the potential to guide therapist intervention based on empirical observation, an important supplement to current practice that is largely guided by clinical experience and theory. For example, if evidence eventually supported the change processes described in this paper, findings related to memory reconsolidation might be capitalized on by facilitating the activation of explicit autobiographical memories, implicit memory traces, or feelings related to the core fear of shame and rejection in SAD, while coupled with elements aimed at maximizing expectancy violations, such as imagery rescripting (Reimer & Moscovitch, 2015) or similar in‐session imaginal exposure work. Alternately, if self‐transcendent emotions were linked to positive outcomes, this could suggest that integration sessions might attempt to capitalize on self‐transcendent experiences to build more self‐compassion through experiential exercises such as chair work or meditative practices (Luoma & LeJeune, 2020). As another example, therapists might utilize an enhanced therapeutic bond to facilitate in‐session self‐transcendent emotions via strategic self‐disclosure. We hope that by integrating a variety of lines of research, this paper will guide new predictions that stimulate future research on MDMA‐AT for SAD and beyond.
CONFLICTS OF INTEREST
No conflicts of interest have been declared.
Supporting information
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
The authors wish to express appreciation to Sarah M. Smith of Portland Psychotherapy Clinic, Research, and Training Center for her help in editing the manuscript and preparing it for publication, and Angelica Spata of Portland Psychotherapy for her work composing the graphical abstract. This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not‐for‐profit sectors.
Luoma, J. B. , Shahar, B. , Lear, M. K. , Pilecki, B. , & Wagner, A. (2022). Potential processes of change in MDMA‐Assisted therapy for social anxiety disorder: Enhanced memory reconsolidation, self‐transcendence, and therapeutic relationships. Human Psychopharmacology: Clinical and Experimental, 37(3), e2824. 10.1002/hup.2824
DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENT
Data sharing not applicable to this article as no datasets were generated or analyzed during the current study.
REFERENCES
- Adams, G. C. , Balbuena, L. , Meng, X. F. , & Asmundson, G. J. G. (2016). When social anxiety and depression go together: A population study of comorbidity and associated consequences. Journal of Affective Disorders, 206, 48–54. 10.1016/j.jad.2016.07.031 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alboni, S. , Van DIjk, R. M. , Poggini, S. , Milior, G. , Perrotta, M. , Drenth, T. , Brunello, N. , Wolfer, D. P. , Limatola, C. , Amrein, I. , Cirulli, F. , Maggi, L. , & Branchi, I. (2017). Fluoxetine effects on molecular, cellular and behavioral endophenotypes of depression are driven by the living environment. Molecular Psychiatry, 22(4), 552–561. 10.1038/mp.2015.142 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- American Psychiatric Association . (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: Author. [Google Scholar]
- Anderson, B. , Goldin, P. R. , Kurita, K. , & Gross, J. J. (2008). Self‐representation in social anxiety disorder: Linguistic analysis of autobiographical narratives. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46, 1119–1125. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arch, J. J. , Brown, K. W. , Dean, D. J. , Landy, L. N. , Brown, K. D. , & Laudenslager, M. L. (2014). Self‐compassion training modulates alpha‐amylase, heart rate variability, and subjective responses to social evaluative threat in women. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 42, 49–58. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aron, A. , Aron, E. N. , & Smollan, D. (1992). Inclusion of other in the self scale and the structure of interpersonal closeness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63(4), 596–612. 10.1037/0022-3514.63.4.596 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Baggott, M. J. , Coyle, J. R. , Siegrist, J. D. , Garrison, K. J. , Galloway, G. P. , & Mendelson, J. E. (2016). Effects of 3,4‐methylenedioxymethamphetamine on socioemotional feelings, authenticity, and autobiographical disclosure in healthy volunteers in a controlled setting. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 30(4), 378–387. 10.1177/0269881115626348 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bar‐Haim, Y. , Lamy, D. , Pergamin, L. , Bakermans‐Kranenburg, M. J. , & Van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (2007). Threat‐related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: A meta‐analytic study. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 1–24. 10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barone, W. , Beck, J. , Mitsunaga‐Whitten, M. , & Perl, P. (2019). Perceived benefits of MDMA‐assisted psychotherapy beyond symptom reduction: Qualitative follow‐up study of a clinical trial for individuals with treatment‐resistant PTSD. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs, 51(2), 199–208. 10.1080/02791072.2019.1580805 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bedi, G. , Hyman, D. , & De Wit, H. (2010). Is ecstasy an “empathogen”? Effects of ±3,4‐ methylenedioxymethamphetamine on prosocial feelings and identification of emotional states in others. Biological Psychiatry, 68(12), 1134–1140. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.08.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bedi, G. , Phan, K. L. , Angstadt, M. , & De Wit, H. (2009). Effects of MDMA on sociability and neural response to social threat and social reward. Psychopharmacology, 207(1), 73–83. 10.1007/s00213-009-1635-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bekinschtein, P. , Cammarota, M. , & Medina, J. H. (2014). BDNF and memory processing. Neuropharmacology, 76(PART C), 677–683. 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2013.04.024 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bershad, A. K. , Miller, M. A. , Baggott, M. J. , & De Wit, H. (2016). The effects of MDMA on socio‐emotional processing: Does MDMA differ from other stimulants? Journal of Psychopharmacology, 30(12), 1248–1258. 10.1177/0269881116663120 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boersma, K. , Håkanson, A. , Salomonsson, E. , & Johansson, I. (2015). Compassion focused therapy to counteract shame, self‐criticism and isolation. A replicated single case experimental study for individuals with social anxiety. Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy, 45, 89–98. [Google Scholar]
- Bruce, L. C. , Heimberg, R. G. , Blanco, C. , Schneier, F. , & Liebowitz, M. R. (2012). Childhood maltreatment and social anxiety disorder: Implications for symptom severity and response to pharmacotherapy. Depression and Anxiety, 29(2), 132–139. 10.1038/jid.2014.371 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cahill, J. , Stiles, W. B. , Barkham, M. , Hardy, G. E. , Stone, G. , Agnew‐Davies, R. , & Unsworth, G. (2012). Two short forms of the Agnew relationship measure: The ARM‐5 and ARM‐12. Psychotherapy Research, 22(3), 241–255. 10.1080/10503307.2011.643253 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cândea, D. M. , & Szentagotai‐Tăta, A. (2018). Shame‐proneness, guilt‐proneness and anxiety symptoms: A meta‐analysis. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 58, 78–106. 10.1016/j.janxdis.2018.07.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carhart‐Harris, R. L. , Leech, R. , Hellyer, P. J. , Shanahan, M. , Feilding, A. , Tagliazucchi, E. , Chialvo, D. R. , & Nutt, D. (2014). The entropic brain: A theory of conscious states informed by neuroimaging research with psychedelic drugs. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 1–22. 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carhart‐Harris, R. L. , Murphy, K. , Leech, R. , Erritzoe, D. , Wall, M. B. , Ferguson, B. , Williams, L. T. J. , Roseman, L. , Brugger, S. , De Meer, I. , Tanner, M. , Tyacke, R. , Wolff, K. , Sethi, A. , Bloomfield, M. A. P. , Williams, T. M. , Bolstridge, M. , Stewart, L. , Morgan, C. , … Nutt, D. J. (2015). The effects of acutely administered 3,4‐methylenedioxymethamphetamine on spontaneous brain function in healthy volunteers measured with arterial spin labeling and blood oxygen level‐dependent resting state functional connectivity. Biological Psychiatry, 78(8), 554–562. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.12.015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carhart‐Harris, R. L. , Roseman, L. , Haijen, E. , Erritzoe, D. , Watts, R. , Branchi, I. , & Kaelen, M. (2018). Psychedelics and the essential importance of context. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 32(7), 1–731. 10.1177/0269881118754710 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chiupka, C. A. , Moscovitch, D. A. , & Bielak, T. (2012). In vivo activation of anticipatory vs. post‐event autobiographical images and memories in social anxiety. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 31. [Google Scholar]
- Christian, C. , Safran, J. D. , & Muran, J. C. (2012). The corrective emotional experience: A relational perspective and critique. In Transformation in psychotherapy: Corrective experiences across cognitive behavioral, humanistic, and psychodynamic approaches (pp. 51–67). American Psychological Association. 10.1037/13747-004 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Clark, D. M. , & Wells, A. (1995). A cognitive model of social phobia. In R. Heimberg , M. Liebowitz , D. A. Hope , & F. R. Schneier (Eds.), Social phobia: Diagnosis, assessment and treatment (pp. 69–93). New York: Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
- Cox, B. J. , Fleet, C. , & Stein, M. B. (2004). Self‐criticism and social phobia in the US national comorbidity survey. Journal of Affective Disorders, 82(2), 227–234. 10.1016/j.jad.2003.12.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Danforth, A. L. , Grob, C. S. , Struble, C. , Feduccia, A. A. , Walker, N. , Jerome, L. , Yazar‐Klosinski, A. , & Emerson, A. (2018). Reduction in social anxiety after MDMA‐assisted psychotherapy with autistic adults: A randomized, double‐blind, placebo‐controlled pilot study. Psychopharmacology, 235(11), 3137–3148. 10.1007/s00213-018-5010-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Doss, M. K. , Weafer, J. , Gallo, D. A. , & De Wit, H. (2018). MDMA impairs both the encoding and retrieval of emotional recollections. Neuropsychopharmacology, 43(4), 791–800. 10.1038/npp.2017.171 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elliott, R. , Bohart, A. C. , Watson, J. C. , & Murphy, D. (2018). Therapist empathy and client outcome: An updated meta‐analysis. Psychotherapy, 55(4), 399–410. 10.1037/pst0000175 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elliott, R. , & Shahar, B. (2018). Emotion‐focused therapy for social anxiety. In Clinical handbook of emotion‐focused therapy (pp. 337–360). American Psychological Association. 10.1037/0000112-015 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Elsey, J. W. B. , Van Ast, V. A. , & Kindt, M. (2018). Human memory reconsolidation: A guiding framework and critical review of the evidence. Psychological Bulletin, 144(8), 797–848. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Emanuele, E. , Arra, M. , & Pesenti, S. (2006). Vasopressin and oxytocin as neurohormonal mediators of MDMA (ecstasy) sociosexual behavioural effects. Medical Hypotheses, 67, 1250–1251. 10.1016/j.mehy.2006.05.021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Farber, B. A. , Blanchard, M. , & Love, M. (2019). Secrets and lies in psychotherapy. Secrets and lies in psychotherapy. American Psychological Association. 10.1037/0000128-000 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Farber, B. A. , Suzuki, J. Y. , & Lynch, D. A. (2018). Positive regard and psychotherapy outcome: A meta‐analytic review. Psychotherapy, 55(4), 411–423. 10.1037/pst0000171 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feduccia, A. A. , & Mithoefer, M. C. (2018). MDMA‐assisted psychotherapy for PTSD: Are memory reconsolidation and fear extinction underlying mechanisms? Progress in Neuro‐Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 84, 221–228. 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2018.03.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fernández, R. S. , Pedreira, M. E. , & Boccia, M. M. (2017). Does reconsolidation occur in natural settings? Memory reconsolidation and anxiety disorders. Clinical Psychology Review, 57(July), 45–58. 10.1016/j.cpr.2017.08.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Flückiger, C. , Del, A. C. , Wampold, B. E. , & Horvath, A. O. (2018). The alliance in adult psychotherapy: A meta‐analytic synthesis. Psychotherapy, 55(4), 316–340. 10.1037/pst0000172 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fredrickson, B. L. , Tugade, M. M. , Waugh, C. E. , & Larkin, G. R. (2003). What good are positive emotions in crisis? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(2), 365–376. 10.1037/0022-3514.84.2.365 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gilbert, P. (2000). The relationship of shame, social anxiety and depression: The role of the evaluation of social rank. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 7(3), 174–189. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Gilbert, P. , McEwan, K. , Matos, M. , & Rivis, A. (2011). Fears of compassion: Development of three self‐report measures. Psychology and Psychotherapy, 84(3), 239–255. 10.1348/147608310X526511 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gilboa‐Schechtman, E. , Keshet, H. , Peschard, V. , & Azoulay, R. (2020). Self and identity in social anxiety disorder. Journal of Personality, 88(1), 106–121. 10.1111/jopy.12455 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glazier, B. L. , & Alden, L. E. (2017). Social anxiety and biased recall of positive information: It's not the content, it's the valence. Behavior Therapy, 48(4), 533–543. 10.1016/j.beth.2016.08.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goetz, J. L. , Keltner, D. , & Simon‐Thomas, E. (2010). Compassion: An Evolutionary analysis and empirical review. Psychological Bulletin, 136(3), 351–374. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hackmann, A. , Clark, D. M. , & McManus, F. (2000). Recurrent images and early memories in social phobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38(6), 601–610. 10.1016/S0005-7967(99)00161-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hackmann, A. , Surawy, C. , & Clark, D. M. (1998). Seeing yourself through others’ eyes: A study of spontaneously occurring images in social phobia. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 26(1), 3–12. 10.1017/S1352465898000022 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hake, H. S. , Davis, J. K. , Wood, R. R. , Tanner, M. K. , Loetz, E. C. , Sanchez, A. , Ostrovskyy, M. , Oleson, E. B. , Grigsby, J. , Doblin, R. , & Greenwood, B. N. (2019). 3,4‐methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) impairs the extinction and reconsolidation of fear memory in rats. Physiology & Behavior, 199, 343–350. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2018.12.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hake, H. S. , Davis, J. K. P. , Wood, R. R. , Tanner, M. K. , Loetz, E. C. , Sanchez, A. , Harwood, E. M. , & Kocovski, N. L. (2017). Self‐compassion induction reduces anticipatory anxiety among socially anxious students. Mindfulness, 8, 1544–1551. 10.1007/s12671-017-0721-2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hanley, A. W. , Nakamura, Y. , & Garland, E. L. (2018). The Nondual Awareness Dimensional Assessment (NADA): New tools to assess nondual traits and states of consciousness occurring within and beyond the context of meditation. Psychological Assessment, 30(12), 1625–1639. 10.1037/pas0000615 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haug, T. , Nordgreen, T. , Öst, L. G. , Tangen, T. , Kvale, G. , Hovland, O. J. , Havik, O. E. , & Havik, O. E. (2016). Working alliance and competence as predictors of outcome in cognitive behavioral therapy for social anxiety and panic disorder in adults. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 77, 40–51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hayes, S. C. , Hofmann, S. G. , Stanton, C. E. , Carpenter, J. K. , Sanford, B. T. , Curtiss, J. E. , & Ciarrochi, J. (2019). The role of the individual in the coming era of process‐based therapy. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 117, 40–53. 10.1016/j.brat.2018.10.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heifets, B. D. , & Malenka, R. C. (2016). MDMA as a probe and treatment for social behaviors. Cell, 166(2), 269–272. 10.1016/j.cell.2016.06.045 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hofmann, S. G. (2000). Self‐focused attention before and after treatment of social phobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38(7), 717–725. 10.1016/S0005-7967(99)00105-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hofmann, S. G. , Curtiss, J. E. , & Hayes, S. C. (2020). Beyond linear mediation: Toward a dynamic network approach to study treatment processes. Clinical Psychology Review, 76, 101824. 10.1016/j.cpr.2020.101824 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Homer, S. R. , & Deeprose, C. (2017). Voluntary and involuntary imagery in social anxiety. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 45, 285–299. 10.1017/S1352465817000017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hutcherson, C. A. , Seppala, E. M. , & Gross, J. J. (2008). Loving‐kindness meditation increases social connectedness. Emotion, 8(5), 720–724. 10.1037/a0013237 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hysek, C. M. , Schmid, Y. , Simmler, L. D. , Domes, G. , Heinrichs, M. , Eisenegger, C. , Preller, K. H. , Quednow, B. B. , & Liechti, M. E. (2014). MDMA enhances emotional empathy and prosocial behavior. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(11), 1645–1652. 10.1093/scan/nst161 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jeon, H. , & Lee, S. H. (2018). From neurons to social beings: Short review of the mirror neuron system research and its socio‐psychological and psychiatric implications. Clinical Psychopharmacology and Neuroscience, 16, 18–31. 10.9758/cpn.2018.16.1.18 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johansen, P. , & Krebs, T. (2009). How could MDMA (ecstasy) help anxiety disorders? A neurobiological rationale. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 23(4), 389–391. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jungaberle, H. , Thal, S. , Zeuch, A. , Rougemont‐Bücking, A. , von Heyden, M. , Aicher, H. , & Scheidegger, M. (2018). Positive psychology in the investigation of psychedelics and entactogens: A critical review. Neuropharmacology, 142, 179–199. 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2018.06.034 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kamboj, S. K. , Kilford, E. J. , Minchin, S. , Moss, A. , Lawn, W. , Das, R. K. , Falconer, C. J. , Gilbert, P. , Curran, H. V. , & Freeman, T. P. (2015). Recreational 3,4‐methylenedioxy‐N‐methylamphetamine (MDMA) or ecstasy and self‐focused compassion: Preliminary steps in the development of a therapeutic psychopharmacology of contemplative practices. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 29(9), 961–970. 10.1177/0269881115587143 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kamboj, S. K. , Walldén, Y. S. E. , Falconer, C. J. , Alotaibi, M. R. , Blagbrough, I. S. , Husbands, S. M. , & Freeman, T. P. (2018). Additive effects of 3,4‐methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) and compassionate imagery on self‐compassion in recreational users of ecstasy. Mindfulness, 9(4), 1134–1145. 10.1007/s12671-017-0849-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kamilar‐Britt, P. , & Bedi, G. (2015). The prosocial effects of 3,4‐methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA): Controlled studies in humans and laboratory animals. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 57, 433–446. 10.1016/J.NEUBIOREV.2015.08.016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kessler, R. C. , Berglund, P. , Demler, O. , Jin, R. , Merikangas, K. R. , & Walters, E. E. (2005). Lifetime prevalence and age‐of‐onset distributions of DSM‐IV disorders in the national comorbidity survey replication. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62, 593. 10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.593 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kessler, R. C. , Davis, C. G. , & Kendler, K. S. (1997). Childhood adversity and adult psychiatric disorder in the US National Comorbidity Survey. Psychological Medicine, 27(5), 1101–1119. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kirkpatrick, M. G. , & de Wit, H. (2015). Mdma: A social drug in a social context. Psychopharmacology, 232, 1155–1163. 10.1007/s00213-014-3752-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kirschner, H. , Kuyken, W. , Wright, K. , Roberts, H. , Brejcha, C. , & Karl, A. (2019). Soothing your heart and feeling connected: A new experimental paradigm to study the benefits of self‐compassion. Clinical Psychological Science, 7(3), 545–565. 10.1177/2167702618812438 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kivity, Y. , Strauss, A. Y. , Elizur, J. , Weiss, M. , Cohen, L. , & Huppert, J. D. (2020). The alliance mediates outcome in cognitive–behavioral therapy for social anxiety disorder, but not in attention bias modification. Psychotherapy Research, 31, 589–603. 10.1080/10503307.2020.1836423 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knutsson, J. , Nilsson, J. E. , Eriksson, Å. , & Järild, L. (2019). Imagery rescripting and exposure in social anxiety: A randomized trial comparing treatment techniques. Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy, 50, 233–240. 10.1007/s10879-019-09448-1 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Kok, B. E. , & Fredrickson, B. L. (2015). Evidence for the upward spiral stands steady: A response to heathers, Brown, coyne, and friedman (2015). Psychological Science, 26, 1–1146. 10.1177/0956797615584304 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kok, B. E. , & Singer, T. (2017). Effects of contemplative dyads on engagement and perceived social connectedness over 9 months of mental training a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Psychiatry, 74(2), 126–134. 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.3360 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kolden, G. G. , Wang, C. C. , Austin, S. B. , Chang, Y. , & Klein, M. H. (2018). Congruence/genuineness: A meta‐analysis. Psychotherapy, 55(4), 424–433. 10.1037/pst0000162 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koszycki, D. , Thake, J. , Mavounza, C. , Daoust, J.‐P. , Taljaard, M. , & Bradwejn, J. (2016). Preliminary investigation of a mindfulness‐based intervention for social anxiety disorder that integrates compassion meditation and mindful exposure. Journal of Alternative & Complementary Medicine, 22(5), 363–374. 10.1089/acm.2015.0108 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krans, J. , de Bree, J. , & Bryant, R. A. (2014). Autobiographical memory bias in social anxiety. Memory, 22(8), 890–897. 10.1080/09658211.2013.844261 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuypers, K. P. C. , Dolder, P. C. , Ramaekers, J. G. , & Liechti, M. E. (2017). Multifaceted empathy of healthy volunteers after single doses of MDMA: A pooled sample of placebo‐controlled studies. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 31(5), 589–598. 10.1177/0269881117699617 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Landgraf, R. , & Neumann, I. D. (2004). Vasopressin and oxytocin release within the brain: A dynamic concept of multiple and variable modes of neuropeptide communication. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology, 25, 150–176. 10.1016/j.yfrne.2004.05.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lane, R. D. , Ryan, L. , Nadel, L. , & Greenberg, L. (2015). Memory reconsolidation, emotional arousal, and the process of change in psychotherapy: New insights from brain science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 38. 10.1017/S0140525X14000041 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Langkaas, T. F. , Hoffart, A. , Øktedalen, T. , Ulvenes, P. G. , Hembree, E. A. , & Smucker, M. (2017). Exposure and non‐fear emotions: A randomized controlled study of exposure‐based and rescripting‐based imagery in PTSD treatment. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 97, 33–42. 10.1016/j.brat.2017.06.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leichsenring, F. , Salzer, S. , Beutel, M. E. , Herpertz, S. , Hiller, W. , Hoyer, J. , Huesing, J. , Joraschky, P. , Nolting, B. , Poehlmann, K. , Ritter, V. , Stangier, U. , Strauss, B. , Stuhldreher, N. , Tefikow, S. , Teismann, T. , Willutzki, U. , Wiltink, J. , & Leibing, E. (2013). Psychodynamic therapy and cognitive‐behavioral therapy in social anxiety disorder: A multicenter randomized controlled trial. American Journal of Psychiatry, 170(7), 759–767. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liechti, M. E. , & Vollenweider, F. X. (2001). Which neuroreceptors mediate the subjective effects of MDMA in humans? A summary of mechanistic studies. Human Psychopharmacology, 16(8), 589–598. 10.1002/hup.348 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liotti, G. , & Gilbert, P. (2011). Mentalizing, motivation, and social mentalities: Theoretical considerations and implications for psychotherapy. Psychology and Psychotherapy, 84(1), 9–25. discussion 98–110. 10.1348/147608310X520094 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Loerinc, A. G. , Meuret, A. E. , Twohig, M. P. , Rosenfield, D. , Bluett, E. J. , & Craske, M. G. (2015). Response rates for CBT for anxiety disorders: Need for standardized criteria. Clinical Psychology Review, 42, 72–82. 10.1016/j.cpr.2015.08.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lumma, A. L. , Kok, B. E. , & Singer, T. (2015). Is meditation always relaxing? Investigating heart rate, heart rate variability, experienced effort and likeability during training of three types of meditation. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 97(1), 38–45. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2015.04.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luoma, J. B. , Chwyl, C. , Bathje, G. J. , Davis, A. K. , & Lancelotta, R. (2020). A meta‐analysis of placebo‐controlled trials of psychedelic‐assisted therapy. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs, 52, 289–299. 10.1080/02791072.2020.1769878 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luoma, J. B. , Hayes, S. C. , & Walser, R. D. (2017). Learning ACT: An acceptance and commitment therapy skills training manual for therapists (Vol. 36, p. 634). Oakland, CA: New Harbinger. 10.1017/S1352465808004670 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Luoma, J. B. , & LeJeune, J. T. (2020). Incorporating affective science into ACT to treat highly self‐critical and shame‐prone clients. In M. E. Levin, Twohig M. P., & Krafft J. (Eds.), Innovations in acceptance and commitment therapy: Clinical advancements and applications in ACT. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger. [Google Scholar]
- Magee, W. J. , Eaton, W. W. , Wittchen, H. U. , McGonagle, K. A. , & Kessler, R. C. (1996). Agoraphobia, simple phobia, and social phobia in the National Comorbidity Survey. Archives of General Psychiatry, 53(2), 159–168. 10.1001/archpsyc.1996.01830020077009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Makadi, E. , & Koszycki, D. (2020). Exploring connections between self‐compassion, mindfulness, and social anxiety. Mindfulness, 11(2), 480–492. 10.1007/s12671-019-01270-z [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Makkar, S. R. , & Grisham, J. R. (2011). Social anxiety and the effects of negative self‐imagery on emotion, cognition, and post‐event processing. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 49(10), 654–664. 10.1016/j.brat.2011.07.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Markey, P. M. , Funder, D. G. , & Ozer, D. J. (2003). Complementarity of interpersonal behaviors in dyadic interactions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29(9), 1082–1090. 10.1177/0146167203253474 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mas, M. , Farré, M. , de la Torre, R. , Roset, P. N. , Segura, J. O. J. , & Camí, J. (1999). Cardiovascular and neuroendocrine effects and pharmacokinetics of 3,4‐methylenedioxymethamphetamine in humans. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, 290(1), 136–145. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mataix‐Cols, D. , De La Cruz, L. F. , Monzani, B. , Rosenfield, D. , Andersson, E. , Pérez‐Vigil, A. , Rück, C. , de Kleine, R. A. , Difede, J. , Dunlop, B. W. , Farrell, L. J. , Geller, D. , Gerardi, M. , Guastella, A. J. , Hofmann, S. G. , Hendriks, G.‐J. , Kushner, M. G. , Lee, F. S. , Lenze, E. J. , … Thuras, P. (2017). D‐cycloserine augmentation of exposure‐based cognitive behavior therapy for anxiety, obsessive‐compulsive, and posttraumatic stress disorders a systematic review and meta‐analysis of individual participant data. JAMA Psychiatry, 74, 501. 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.3955 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mayo‐Wilson, E. , Dias, S. , Mavranezouli, I. , Kew, K. , Clark, D. M. , Ades, A. E. , & Pilling, S. (2014). Psychological and pharmacological interventions for social anxiety disorder in adults: A systematic review and network meta‐analysis. The Lancet Psychiatry, 1(5), 368–376. 10.1016/S2215-0366(14)70329-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCullough, M. E. , Emmons, R. A. , & Tsang, J. A. (2002). The grateful disposition: A conceptual and empirical topography. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82(1), 112–127. 10.1037/0022-3514.82.1.112 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mchugh, L. , & Stewart, I. (2012). The self and perspective taking: Contributions and applications from modern behavioral science. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger. [Google Scholar]
- Meuret, A. E. , Chmielewski, M. , Steele, A. M. , Rosenfield, D. , Petersen, S. , Smits, J. A. J. , Simon, N. M. , Otto, M. W. , Marques, L. , Pollack, M. H. , & Hofmann, S. G. (2016). othersThe desire to belong: Social identification as a predictor of treatment outcome in social anxiety disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 81, 21–34. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mithoefer, M. (2014). MDMA‐assisted psychotherapy: How different is it from other psychotherapy. In Doblin R. & Burge B. (Eds.), Manifesting minds: A review of psychedelics in science, medicine, sex, and spirituality (p. 125). North Atlantic Books. Mulitdisciplinary Association for Psychedelic Studies. [Google Scholar]
- Mithoefer, M. C. , Feduccia, A. A. , Jerome, L. , Mithoefer, A. , Wagner, M. , Walsh, Z. , Hamilton, S. , Yazar‐Klosinski, B. , Emerson, A. , & Doblin, R. (2019). MDMA‐assisted psychotherapy for treatment of PTSD: Study design and rationale for phase 3 trials based on pooled analysis of six phase 2 randomized controlled trials. Psychopharmacology, 236, 2735–2745. 10.1007/s00213-019-05249-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar] [Retracted]
- Mithoefer, M. C. , Wagner, M. T. , Mithoefer, A. T. , Jerome, L. , & Doblin, R. (2011). The safety and efficacy of ±3,4‐methylenedioxymethamphetamine‐ assisted psychotherapy in subjects with chronic, treatment‐resistant posttraumatic stress disorder: The first randomized controlled pilot study. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 25(4), 439–452. 10.1177/0269881110378371 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Monson, C. M. , Wagner, A. C. , Mithoefer, A. T. , Liebman, R. E. , Feduccia, A. A. , Jerome, L. , Mithoefer, M. C. , Emerson, A. , Doblin, R. , & Mithoefer, M. C. (2020). MDMA‐facilitated cognitive‐behavioural conjoint therapy for posttraumatic stress disorder: An uncontrolled trial. European Journal of Psychotraumatology, 11(1), 1840123. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morris, E. P. , Stewart, S. H. , & Ham, L. S. (2005). The relationship between social anxiety disorder and alcohol use disorders: A critical review. Clinical Psychology Review, 25(6), 734–760. 10.1016/j.cpr.2005.05.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mörtberg, E. (2014). Working alliance in individual and group cognitive therapy for social anxiety disorder. Psychiatry Research, 220(1–2), 716–718. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moscovitch, D. A. (2009). What is the core fear in social phobia? A new model to facilitate individualized case conceptualization and treatment. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 16, 123–134. 10.1016/j.cbpra.2008.04.002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Moscovitch, D. A. , Gavric, D. L. , Merrifield, C. , Bielak, T. , & Moscovitch, M. (2011). Retrieval properties of negative vs. positive mental images and autobiographical memories in social anxiety: Outcomes with a new measure. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 49(8), 505–517. 10.1016/j.brat.2011.05.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moscovitch, D. A. , Vidovic, V. , Lenton‐Brym, A. P. , Dupasquier, J. R. , Barber, K. C. , Hudd, T. , Zabara, N. , & Romano, M. (2018). Autobiographical memory retrieval and appraisal in social anxiety disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 107, 106–116. 10.1016/j.brat.2018.06.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nardou, R. , Lewis, E. M. , Rothhaas, R. , Xu, R. , Yang, A. , Boyden, E. , & Dölen, G. (2019). Oxytocin‐dependent reopening of a social reward learning critical period with MDMA. Nature, 569(7754), 116–120. 10.1038/s41586-019-1075-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neff, K. (2003). Self‐compassion: An alternative conceptualization of a HealthyAttitudeToward oneself. Self and Identity, 2, 85–101. 10.1080/15298860390129863 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Neff, K. D. (2003). The development and validation of a scale to measure self‐compassion. Self and Identity, 2, 223–250. 10.1080/15298860390209035 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Niles, A. N. , Burklund, L. J. , Arch, J. J. , Lieberman, M. D. , Saxbe, D. , & Craske, M. G. (2014). Cognitive mediators of treatment for social anxiety disorder: Comparing acceptance and commitment therapy and cognitive‐behavioral therapy. Behavior Therapy, 45(5), 664–677. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nilsson, J. E. , Lundh, L. G. , & Viborg, G. (2012). Imagery rescripting of early memories in social anxiety disorder: An experimental study. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 50(6), 387–392. 10.1016/j.brat.2012.03.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Norcross, J. C. , & Lambert, M. J. (2018). Psychotherapy relationships that work III. Psychotherapy, 55(4), 303–315. 10.1037/pst0000193 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Norton, A. R. , & Abbott, M. J. (2015). Self‐focused cognition in social anxiety: A review of the theoretical and empirical literature. Behaviour Change, 33(1), 44–64. 10.1017/bec.2016.2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Norton, A. R. , & Abbott, M. J. (2017a). Bridging the gap between aetiological and maintaining factors in social anxiety disorder: The impact of socially traumatic experiences on beliefs, imagery and symptomatology. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 24(3), 747–765. 10.1002/cpp.2044 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Norton, A. R. , & Abbott, M. J. (2017b). The role of environmental factors in the aetiology of social anxiety disorder: A review of the theoretical and empirical literature. Behaviour Change, 34(2), 76–97. 10.1017/bec.2017.7 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Nour, M. M. , Evans, L. , Nutt, D. , & Carhart‐Harris, R. L. (2016). Ego‐dissolution and psychedelics: Validation of the ego‐dissolution inventory (EDI). Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 10, 1–13. 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00269 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Passie, T. (2018). The early use of MDMA (‘Ecstasy’) in psychotherapy (1977–1985). Drug Science, Policy and Law, 4, 205032451876744. 10.1177/2050324518767442 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Passie, T. , Hartmann, U. , Schneider, U. , Emrich, H. M. , & Krüger, T. H. C. (2005). Ecstasy (MDMA) mimics the post‐orgasmic state: Impairment of sexual drive and function during acute MDMA‐effects may be due to increased prolactin secretion. Medical Hypotheses, 64(5), 899–903. 10.1016/j.mehy.2004.11.044 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Piff, P. K. , Dietze, P. , Feinberg, M. , Stancato, D. M. , & Keltner, D. (2015). Awe , the small self, and prosocial behavior, 108(6), 883–899. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 108(6), 883–899. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Plasencia, M. L. , Alden, L. E. , & Taylor, C. T. (2011). Differential effects of safety behaviour subtypes in social anxiety disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 49(10), 665–675. 10.1016/j.brat.2011.07.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reimer, S. G. , & Moscovitch, D. A. (2015). The impact of imagery rescripting on memory appraisals and core beliefs in social anxiety disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 75, 48–59. 10.1016/j.brat.2015.10.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Romano, M. , Moscovitch, D. A. , Huppert, J. D. , Reimer, S. G. , & Moscovitch, M. (2020). The effects of imagery rescripting on memory outcomes in social anxiety disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 69, 102169. 10.1016/j.janxdis.2019.102169 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sadler, P. , Ethier, N. , Gunn, G. R. , Duong, D. , & Woody, E. (2009). Are we on the same wavelength? Interpersonal complementarity as shared cyclical patterns during interactions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(6), 1005–1020. 10.1037/a0016232 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sansen, L. M. , Iffland, B. , & Neuner, F. (2015). The trauma of peer victimization: Psychophysiological and emotional characteristics of memory imagery in subjects with social anxiety disorder. Psychophysiology, 52(1), 107–116. 10.1111/psyp.12291 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shahar, B. , Bar‐Kalifa, E. , & Alon, E. (2017). Emotion‐focused therapy for social anxiety disorder: Results from a multiple‐baseline study. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 85(3), 238–249. 10.1037/ccp0000166 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shahar, B. , Levit‐Binnun, N. , Szepsenwol, O. , Zilca, S. , Zamir, O. , Haim, N. , & Levit‐Binnun, N. (2015). A wait‐list randomized controlled trial of loving‐kindness meditation program for self‐criticism. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 22, 346–356. 10.1002/cpp.1893 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shiota, M. N. , Keltner, D. , & Mossman, A. (2007). The nature of awe: Elicitors, appraisals, and effects on self‐concept. Cognition & Emotion, 21(5), 944–963. 10.1080/02699930600923668 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Siegel, D. J. (1999). The developing mind. New York: Guilford. [Google Scholar]
- Simon, N. M. , Herlands, N. N. , Marks, E. H. , Mancini, C. , Letamendi, A. , Li, Z. , Pollack, M. H. , Van Ameringen, M. , & Stein, M. B. (2009). Childhood maltreatment linked to greater symptom severity and poorer quality of life and function in social anxiety disorder. Depression and Anxiety, 26(11), 1027–1032. 10.1002/da.20604 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stangier, U. , Schramm, E. , Heidenreich, T. , Berger, M. , & Clark, D. M. (2011). Cognitive therapy vs interpersonal psychotherapy in social anxiety disorder: A randomized controlled trial. Archives of General Psychiatry, 68(7), 692–700. 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.67 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stein, D. J. (2015). Social anxiety disorder and the psychobiology of self‐consciousness. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 489. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stellar, J. E. , Gordon, A. M. , Piff, P. K. , Cordaro, D. , Anderson, C. L. , Bai, Y. , Maruskin, L. A. , & Keltner, D. (2017). Self‐transcendent emotions and their social functions: Compassion, gratitude, and awe bind us to others through prosociality. Emotion Review, 9(3), 200–207. 10.1177/1754073916684557 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Stopa, L. , & Jenkins, A. (2007). Images of the self in social anxiety: Effects on the retrieval of autobiographical memories. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 38(4), 459–473. 10.1016/j.jbtep.2007.08.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Studerus, E. , Gamma, A. , & Vollenweider, F. X. (2010). Psychometric evaluation of the altered states of consciousness rating scale (OAV). PLoS One, 5(8), e12412. 10.1371/journal.pone.0012412 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stuhldreher, N. , Leibing, E. , Leichsenring, F. , Beutel, M. E. , Herpertz, S. , Hoyer, J. , Konnopka, A. , Salzer, S. , Strauss, B. , Wiltink, J. , & König, H.‐H. (2014). The costs of social anxiety disorder: The role of symptom severity and comorbidities. Journal of Affective Disorders, 165, 87–94. 10.1016/j.jad.2014.04.039 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor, C. T. , Bomyea, J. , & Amir, N. (2011). Malleability of attentional bias for positive emotional information and anxiety vulnerability. Emotion, 11(1), 127–138. 10.1037/a0021301 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tracey, T. J. , & Kokotovic, A. M. (1989). Factor structure of the working alliance inventory. Psychological Assessment, 1(3), 207–210. 10.1037/1040-3590.1.3.207 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Wagner, M. T. , Mithoefer, M. C. , Mithoefer, A. T. , MacAulay, R. K. , Jerome, L. , Yazar‐Klosinski, B. , & Doblin, R. (2017). Therapeutic effect of increased openness: Investigating mechanism of action in MDMA‐assisted psychotherapy. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 31(8), 967–974. 10.1177/0269881117711712 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wardle, M. C. , & De Wit, H. (2014). MDMA alters emotional processing and facilitates positive social interaction. Psychopharmacology, 231(21), 4219–4229. 10.1007/s00213-014-3570-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wardle, M. C. , Kirkpatrick, M. G. , & de Wit, H. (2014). “Ecstasy” as a social drug: MDMA preferentially affects responses to emotional stimuli with social content. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(8), 1076–1081. 10.1093/scan/nsu035 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Werner, K. H. , Jazaieri, H. , Goldin, P. R. , Ziv, M. , Heimberg, R. G. , & Gross, J. J. (2012). Self‐compassion and social anxiety disorder. Anxiety, Stress & Coping, 25(5), 543–558. 10.1080/10615806.2011.608842 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wild, J. , & Clark, D. M. (2011). Imagery rescripting of early traumatic memories in social phobia. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 18(4), 433–443. 10.1016/j.cbpra.2011.03.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williams, T. , Hattingh, C. J. , Kariuki, C. M. , Tromp, S. A. , van Balkom, A. J. , Ipser, J. C. , & Stein, D. J. (2017). Pharmacotherapy for social anxiety disorder (SAnD). Cochrane Database of Systematic Review. 2017(10), CD001206. Retrieved October 27, 2021, from 10.1002/14651858.CD001206.pub3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wong, Q. J. , & Rapee, R. M. (2016). The aetiology and maintenance of social anxiety disorder: A synthesis of complementary theoretical models and formulation of a new integrated model. Journal of Affective Disorders, 203, 84–100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Woody, S. R. , & Adessky, R. S. (2002). Therapeutic alliance, group cohesion, and homework compliance during cognitive‐behavioral group treatment of social phobia. Behavior Therapy, 33(1), 5–27. [Google Scholar]
- Yaden, D. B. (2017). The varieties of self‐transcendent experience. Review of General Psychology, 21(2), 143–160. 10.1037/gpr0000102 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Young, M. B. , Andero, R. , Ressler, K. J. , & Howell, L. L. (2015). 3,4 Methylenedioxymethamphetamine facilitates fear extinction learning. Translational Psychiatry, 5(9), 1–e634. 10.1038/tp.2015.138 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
Data sharing not applicable to this article as no datasets were generated or analyzed during the current study.