Abstract
Modern society is reviving the practice of aromatherapy, and lavender is reported being the most worldwide purchased plant for essential oil (EO) extraction. Since recent studies reported cognitive enhancing effects of lavender besides the hypno‐inducing effects, a literature review is needed. Considering EO quality and diffusion devices, we conducted a systematic review on the effects of lavender EO inhalation on arousal, attention and memory in healthy subjects. Starting from this new multidisciplinary perspective, cognitive effects were reviewed to link outcomes to effective and reproducible protocols. A systematic search on MEDLINE, ERIC, PsycInfo, Google Scholar, and Scopus databases using Cognitive Atlas and plant‐related keywords was conducted. Among the 1,203 articles yielded, 11 met eligibility criteria. Subjects administered with lavender EO displayed arousal decrease and sustained attention increase. Controversial results emerged regarding memory. Lack of EO quality assessment and protocols heterogeneity did not allow assessing whether different EO composition differentially modulates cognition and whether placebo effect can be discerned from EO effect itself. However, GABAergic pathway modulation exerted by linalool, a major lavender EO constituent, might explain cognitive functions empowerment. We speculate aromatherapy could be a burgeoning cognition enhancing tool, although further investigation is required to reach robust conclusions.
Keywords: arousal, attention, cognitive functions, inhalation, lavender, memory
Abbreviation
- EO
essential oils
INTRODUCTION
Over the last decade, scientific literature revived the traditional medical practice of aromatherapy, defined as “the controlled use of plant essences for therapeutic purposes” (Ernst, 2000; Hoffmann, 2003), and contributed to make it rise to the role of the most frequently used complementary medicine.
Indeed, the International Federation of Essential Oils and Aroma Trades (IFEAT) reported a rapid growth in aromatic plants cultivation and lavender as the most worldwide purchased raw material for essential oils (EO) extraction. EO are a complex mixture of chemical volatile components obtained by different extraction techniques (distillation or cold pressure) avoiding any sort of chemical solvent (Petersen, 2013). Currently, since the fortuitous discovery of lavender's soothing effects in the early 1900s by the French chemist Gattefossé (Miller & Miller, 1999), lavender aromatherapy has widely spread among alternative or complementary usages, ranging from antiemetic to soothing and pain treatment up to lately discovered sedative and cognitive effects. Indeed, Gattefossé began to study aromatherapy and inspired the physician Jean Valnet to lay the foundations of plant‐based medicine, by implementing lavender EO to soothe the injuries of the veterans of the war in Indochina in 1948. Afterwards, he expanded the use of lavender EO to treat agitation in psychiatric patients (Thomas, 2002). Recently, ethno‐pharmacological studies reported the medical usage of lavender EO to treat stress, anxiety and depression throughout the world (McIntyre et al., 2016; Xu et al., 2008).
However, the composition of EO still remains an issue to be disentangled, since each component could produce different psychoactive effects. To overcome this problem, the International Standard Organization (ISO) established specific standards for extraction and analytical composition in order to define EO quality grades (Baser et al., 2011; Lawrence, 1993; Maštovská & Lehotay, 2003), and in 2012, the European Medicines Agency (EMA) Committee on Herbal Medicinal Products (HMPC) adopted a community herbal monograph on Lavandula angustifolia Miller aetheroleum, providing specific posologies and oral administration methods. The specific HMPC assessment concluded that lavender oil showed a possible effect on several clinical conditions, asserting that its use in the medical field can be based on its long‐standing usage (EMA & HMPC, 2012). A promising way to exploit the beneficial effects of lavender is the inhalatory administration due to the fact that the olfactory pathway is a direct and rapid way to the brain (Erdő et al., 2018). The nasal route of drug administration has several advantages over oral or intravenous administration, which include non‐invasiveness, self‐administration, shorter time of effects onset and higher bioavailability due to avoidance of hepatic first‐pass metabolism. Moreover, bypassing the blood brain barrier may potentially increase central nervous system (CNS) availability of the drug (Dufes et al., 2003).
From a cognitive standpoint, the above‐mentioned processes might be involved in the frequently reported anxiolytic effects induced by lavender (Arslan et al., 2020; Kianpour et al., 2016; Yayla & Ozdemir, 2019), and, at the same time, this could be linked to the empowerment of cognitive functions, as some studies investigating the effects of anesthetic drugs on cognition reported an improvement in attention and memory domains (Mirski et al., 2010). Since according to the Yerkes–Dodson law (Yerkes & Dodson, 1908) cognitive performance is known to be optimal when arousal is at an intermediate level and, if that threshold is overtaken, performance decreases, low doses of lavender EO might induce arousal decrease to achieve more optimal levels in cognitive tasks, while higher doses of lavender EO might induce the well‐known anxiolytic effects.
Furthermore, a systematic and integrated study on the impact of lavender EO inhalation on cognitive functions is needed, since most studies focused on compartmentalized outcomes ranging from phytochemical composition of EO to physiological and behavioral measures. As regards lavender EO composition, linalool and linalyl acetate are the two main components whose mechanism of action is responsible for central (i.e., brain rhythms) and peripheral (i.e., autonomic tone) effects (Umezu et al., 2006; Zhong et al., 2019). Based on these premises, a systematic characterization of the effects of lavender EO on cognition from a multidisciplinary perspective would help in the comprehension of the cognitive enhancing effects of lavender EO inhalation, meaning a reduction in reaction times or an increase in task accuracy. In this context, different types of EO diffusion devices may also have an impact on administration procedure, since EO quality can be differentially affected by the employed device (Halligudi & Al Ojaili, 2013). Thus, taking into account the EO phytochemical parameters and administration methods, the aim of this systematic review is to assess in healthy subjects whether different lavender EO compositions can differentially influence core cold cognitive functions such as vigilance and arousal, attention and memory or whether these alleged effects hold true regardless of predetermined EO composition.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
We carried out this systematic review according to the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta‐Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines, which comprises a checklist to ensure the quality of systematic reviews (Moher et al., 2009), reported in Table S1. The protocol for the present systematic review has been submitted for registration (ID number 180709) to PROSPERO international prospective register for systematic reviews database (Moher et al., n.d.).
In order to develop an effective search strategy, we adopted the Population, Intervention, Comparison, Outcomes and Study design (PICOS) strategy (da Costa Santos et al., 2007), reported in Table S2. The literature search was conducted in August 2021 and included three phases: (1) identification: a systematic search based on queries on the electronic databases MEDLINE, ERIC, PsycInfo, Google Scholar, and Scopus of articles until August 2021 (an example of the publication trend of articles on aromatherapy retrieved until 2021 is provided in Figures S1 and S2); (2) screening: a manual screening of the articles yielded in phase (1) by evaluating title and abstract only; (3) final eligibility: a more in‐depth assessment of the full text of the remaining papers on the basis of the following eligibility criteria: (i) healthy humans of any age; (ii) inhalation as method of lavender EO administration because inhalation is the only non‐invasive method of administration through which the highest bioavailability of the substance is reached; (iii) behavioral and/or physiological assessment in order to exclude biases ascribable to self‐report measures; (iv) randomized‐controlled design; (v) articles published in peer‐reviewed journals; (vi) articles written in English language. At phase 1, boolean operators “AND” and “OR” were applied to combine a list of keywords related to lavender and a list of keywords related to cognitive functions retrieved from Cognitive Atlas website (Poldrack, 2011), reported in Table S3.
Table S4 summarizes the search steps performed on the database. Articles retrieved from MEDLINE, ERIC, PsycInfo, Google Scholar, and Scopus databases were merged into the Mendeley database (Mendeley Support Team, 2011), and duplicates were automatically removed using the desktop Mendeley reference manager. During the inclusion phase, we excluded articles containing words related to agricultural and botanical fields and to medical treatments in order to do a preliminary selection of the studies by focusing on procedures that employed EO in the field of cognitive functions research; to this aim, we used a list of excluding words related to the aforementioned fields to automatically exclude such studies (list of excluding words is reported in Table S5)
To assess the quality of evidence of the studies, we used the Newcastle‐Ottawa Scale (NOS, Table 2) for the evaluation of randomized‐controlled studies (Wells et al., 2000). The scale scores the quality of reporting with a maximum of 9 stars for higher quality. According to NOS, each study is judged for the selection of the study groups, comparability of the groups, and ascertainment of either the exposure or outcome of interest. In this process, two independent reviewers (E.M. and V.C.) scored each study according to NOS criteria. Any disagreement was resolved through consensus of all authors.
TABLE 2.
Author and Year | Selection | Comparability | Exposure | Total score quality | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Case definition | Representativeness of the cases | Selection of controls | Definition of controls | Comparability of cases and control | Ascertainment of exposure | Same method of ascertainment for cases and controls | Non‐Response rate | ||
Kiecolt‐Glaser et al., 2008 | ⋆ | ⋆ | ⋆ | ⋆⋆ | ⋆ | ⋆ | 7 | ||
Kim et al., 2011 | ⋆ | ⋆ | ⋆ | ⋆ | ⋆ | 5 | |||
Saiorwan et al., 2012 | ⋆ | ⋆ | ⋆ | ⋆ | ⋆ | 5 | |||
Chamine et al., 2016 | ⋆ | ⋆ | ⋆ | ⋆⋆ | ⋆ | ⋆ | 7 | ||
Colzato et al., 2014 | ⋆ | ⋆ | ⋆ | ⋆ | ⋆ | ⋆ | 6 | ||
Heuberger & Ilmberger, 2010 | ⋆ | ⋆ | ⋆ | ⋆ | ⋆ | ⋆ | 6 | ||
Moss et al., 2003 | ⋆ | ⋆ | ⋆ | ⋆ | ⋆ | ⋆ | 6 | ||
Sakamoto et al., 2005 | ⋆ | ⋆ | ⋆ | ⋆ | ⋆ | 5 | |||
Sellaro et al., 2015 | ⋆ | ⋆ | ⋆ | ⋆ | ⋆ | ⋆ | 6 | ||
Shimizu et al., 2008 | ⋆ | ⋆ | ⋆ | ⋆ | ⋆ | 5 | |||
Yue et al., 2016 | ⋆ | ⋆ | ⋆ | ⋆ | ⋆ | ⋆ | 6 |
RESULTS
Our search yielded 1,203 articles. After duplicates removal and screening phase, 31 full‐text articles were retained according to the eligibility criteria. Twenty of them were excluded for the following reasons: 14 studies included mixed EO; one study did not investigate cognitive functions; one study used lavender EO inhalation as a control condition; 2 studies used topical application of lavender EO; 2 studies were based on a within‐group without randomized‐controlled design. The articles selection process is illustrated in Figure 1. After the inclusion phase, by evaluating the studies' outcomes, articles were divided into three macro‐categories regarding psychological macro‐domains: arousal, attention, and memory. Detailed information is provided in Table 1. Finally, 11 articles were reviewed. NOS scores were adequate for all studies and ranged from 5 to 7 (Table 2). According to Table 1, all studies recruited young adults and adults, except for two studies (Chamine & Oken, 2016; Heuberger & Ilmberger, 2010) in which elderly subjects were recruited too. None of the reviewed studies investigated either an age effect or effects elicited by different EO administration timing. Only two studies (Kim et al., 2011; Shimizu et al., 2008) investigated the temporal range in which cognitive effects are still detectable from inhalation onset.
TABLE 1.
Study | Design | Subjects number | Age | Groups | Procedure | Measurement | Results for all groups | Odor awareness | EO characteristics | EO quali‐quantitative analyses | Diffusion system |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
AROUSAL | |||||||||||
Kiecolt‐Glaser et al., 2008 | Single blind/not blind, crossover | 18 | Young adults, adults | Lavender, lemon, control (blind and primed subgroups for each group) | Physiological parameters were assessed before and after subjects underwent cold pressor test for one minute while inhaling EO | Norepinephrine | Decrease in lavender group compared to lemon group (p = 0.019) | Aware | PVE | GC–MS | c |
BP | No changes | ||||||||||
HR | Increase in primed groups compared to blind groups (p = 0.002) | ||||||||||
Salivary cortisol | Decrease in blind groups compared to not blind groups (p = 0.002) | ||||||||||
Blastogenesis response: lymphocyte response to mitogens | Decrease in primed groups compared to water group (p = 0.001) and lemon group (p = 0.009) | ||||||||||
Interleukin‐6 and ‐10 | No changes | ||||||||||
Delayed hypersensitivity to candida | Decrease in lavender group compared to water group (p = 0.02) | ||||||||||
Kim et al., 2011 | Cross‐sectional, double blind, RCT | 15 | Young adults, adults | Lavender, control (passive) | Bispectral index was assessed before and at 5, 10, 15 and 20 min after 5 min of EO administration while subjects were resting | EEG bispectral index | Decrease at 5, 10, 15 and 20 min after inhalation, but not at 25 min in lavender group compared to control group (p < 0.001) | Aware | DVE | Not specified | cm |
Saiorwan et al., 2012 | Single blind, crossover | 10 | Young adults | Lavender, control (passive) | Physiological parameters were assessed at baseline and after 4 7‐min sessions of EO administration while subjects were resting | RR | No changes | Aware | DVE | GC–MS | cm |
HR | Decrease in lavender group compared to control group (p < 0.001) | ||||||||||
SBP | Decrease in lavender group compared to control group (p < 0.001) | ||||||||||
DBP | Decrease in lavender group compared to control group (p < 0.001) | ||||||||||
Skin temperature | Decrease in lavender group compared to control group (p < 0.001) | ||||||||||
EEG | Decrease in lavender group compared to control group (p < 0.05) | ||||||||||
ATTENTION | |||||||||||
Chamine et al., 2016 | Single blind/not blind, RCT | 31 | Adults, elderly | Lavender, placebo (water or coconut), control (primed and not primed subgroups for each group) | Performance on a battery of cognitive tests was evaluated at baseline and after exposure to a 30‐min stress battery during which aromatherapy was administered | Stroop Color‐Word Test | No changes | Aware/unaware | DED | Not specified | c |
WAIS‐III: Simple reaction times | No odor effect, improvement in primed subgroups compared to not primed subgroups only (p < 0.05) | ||||||||||
Shimizu et al., 2008 | Single blind, crossover | 7 | Young adults | Lavender, eucalyptus, control (passive) | Performance on a 30‐min vigilance task was assessed every 2 min during EO administration | Long‐term vigilance task | Attention increase at 14, 16, 26 and 30 min after EO administration onset in lavender group compared to control group (p < 0.001) | Aware | PVE/TCD | Not specified | cm |
Sakamoto et al., 2005 | Single blind, RCT | 12 | Young adults | Lavender, jasmine, control (passive) | Subjects performed 5 60‐min sessions of a sustained attention task and were administered with EO during 20‐min intervals between every session | Sustained attention task: reaction times | Attention increase in lavender group compared to control and jasmine groups (p < 0.05) | Aware | PVE | Not specified | g |
Sustained attention task: errors number | Attention increase in lavender group compared to control group (p < 0.007) and control group (p < 0.044) | ||||||||||
Heuberger & Ilmberger, 2010 | Single blind, RCT | 30 | Young adults, adults, elderly | 1,8 cineole, jasmine, peppermint, linalyl acetate 5 μl and 20 μl, control (passive) | Performance on a vigilance task was assessed during EO administration | Standard visual vigilance task: reaction times difference | Attention increase in 20‐μl linalyl acetate group compared to control group (p < 0.05) | Aware | PVE | Not specified | cm |
Standard visual vigilance task: scores of misses | No changes | ||||||||||
Moss et al., 2003 | Single blind, RCT | 48 | Young adults | Lavender, rosemary, control (passive) | Performance on CDR battery was assessed during EO administration | CDR battery: speed of attention | Attention decrease in lavender group compared to control group (p < 0.05) | Unaware | PED | not specified | g |
CDR battery: accuracy of attention | No changes | ||||||||||
Colzato et al., 2014 | Single blind, RCT | 22 | Young adults | Lavender, peppermint, control (passive) | Attentional blink in a 10‐min RSVP task was assessed during EO administration | RSVP task | Less pronounced attentional blink in lavender group compared to peppermint group (p = 0.043) | Unaware | PED/DED | Not specified | g |
Sellaro et al., 2015 | Single blind, RCT | 24 | Young adults | Lavender, peppermint, control (passive) | Performance at Joint Simon task was evaluated during EO administration | Joint Simon task (correspondence trials): errors | No changes | Unaware | PED | Not specified | g |
Joint Simon task (correspondence trials): reaction times | No changes | ||||||||||
Joint Simon task (noncorrespondence trials): errors | Attention decrease in lavender group compared to control group (p < 0.05) and peppermint group (p < 0.005) | ||||||||||
Joint Simon task (noncorrespondence trials): reaction times | No changes | ||||||||||
Joint Simon task: Simon effect (errors) | Attention decrease in lavender group compared to control and peppermint groups (p < 0.05) | ||||||||||
Yue et al., 2016 | Single blind, RCT | 23 | Young adults | Lavender, jasmine, garlic, control (passive) | Performance on a perception of time task was assessed during EO administration | Time reproduction task | Attention increase in lavender group compared to jasmine group (p < 0.01) and garlic group (p < 0.05) | Unaware | Not applicable | Not specified | g |
MEMORY | |||||||||||
Chamine et al., 2016 | Single blind/not blind, RCT | 31 | Adults, elderly | Lavender, placebo (water or coconut), control (primed and not primed subgroups for each group) | Performance on a battery of cognitive tests was evaluated at baseline and after exposure to a 30‐min stress battery during which aromatherapy was administered | WAIS‐III: digit span backward | Memory improvement in lavender group compared to coconut group (p = 0.008) and water group (p = 0.021) | Aware/unaware | DED | Not specified | c |
Moss et al., 2003 | Single blind, RCT | 48 | Young adults | Lavender, rosemary, control (passive) | Performance on CDR battery was assessed during EO administration | CDR battery | Memory decline in lavender group compared to control group (p < 0.01) and rosemary group (p < 0.05) | Unaware | PED | Not specified | g |
Abbreviations: EO, essential oil; BP, blood pressure; EEG, electroencephalogram; WAIS‐III, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale‐III; PVE, pure volatile essential oil; CDR, cognitive drug research; RSVP, rapid serial visual presentation; PED, pure essential oil drops; RCT, randomized‐controlled trial; HR, heart rate; RR, respiratory rate; SBP, systolic blood pressure; DBP, diastolic blood pressure; DVE, diluted volatile essential oil; DED, diluted essential oil drops; TCD, target compound diffusion; GC‐MS, gas‐chromatography mass‐spectrometry; c, cotton pad/gauze/linen fabric (two layers under nose, direct diffusion system); cm, cotton pad/gauze/linen fabric/ball in surgical mask and oxygen flow/inhalers (direct diffusion system); g, candle diffuser/soaked pad under bench/tank‐becker (no electrical diffuser, indirect diffusion system).
Phytochemical and diffusion parameters
In the revision process, the selected articles were reviewed by taking into account four key points of EO selection and aroma diffusion: (i) quality parameters reported either in commercial documentation or lab‐scale distilled EO (i.e., pure EO samples obtained by distillation using lab equipment such as Clevenger apparatus recommended by European Directorate, 2019), (ii) sample preparation procedures, (iii) diffusion devices and (iv) subjects' awareness towards administration procedure depending on the device used. Selection process is illustrated in Figure 2.
Quality parameters and sample preparation procedures were not always reported in the selected studies (Colzato et al., 2014; Moss et al., 2003; Sellaro et al., 2015). Different aromatic products were used by Heuberger and Ilmberger (2010) such as commercial singular volatile compounds, EO and absolute ether extract, which are referred to by the general term “fragrances”. The authors did not specify either if the EO were synthetic or isolated from plant material or the quality analytical results of EO purity and composition. Although Kiecolt‐Glaser et al. (2008) did not report compositional data, theirs was the only experimental procedure based on lavender EO monitoring in order to avoid compositional variability. In Saiorwan and colleagues' study (2012), the quality control of commercial lavender EO (i.e., pure or diluted EO or mixtures of volatile chemicals sold on the current international market) was carried out by gas chromatography–mass spectrometry analysis, which is an analytical technique used both in industrial and research laboratories to analyse EO samples. It combines chromatographic separation on a selected column with mass spectrometry detection in order to define the EO chemical profile by both a fixed retention time for each volatile on that selected column and a specific mass spectrum. Identification process is performed by the comparison of the obtained experimental retention time/mass spectrum with those of pure compounds. However, only the relative percentages of linalool (31.9%) and linalyl acetate (32.5%) were shown. Despite these two biomarkers (i.e., the typical volatile components of lavender EO linked to a specific bio‐pharmacological action) being detected in the same amount, the authors considered only linalool as the main compound whose effects on cognitive functions were studied. In other cases, the scientific botanical name of the lavender raw material was the only certified information and the EO composition was not reported at all (Chamine & Oken, 2016; Sellaro et al., 2015). Shimizu et al. (2008) reported no information about linalyl acetate level in the tested EO sample, although the study considered the singular EO compound linalyl acetate as the main active lavender constituent.
Regarding diffusion devices, a variety of facilities with different diffusion methods have been described in the studies. According to EO administration method, two types of devices were identified: (1) direct administration (face systems) including steam diffusion, mask, natural volatile system lab‐scale and cotton pad under nose; (2) indirect administration (environmental systems) including cotton pad under seat, candle and electric devices. Direct administration methods allowed subjects to be aware of the procedure, while indirect administration methods prevented subjects from being aware of the protocol they had been involved in (see Table 1).
Six studies employed direct EO administration systems (Chamine & Oken, 2016; Heuberger & Ilmberger, 2010; Kiecolt‐Glaser et al., 2008; Kim et al., 2011; Saiorwan et al., 2012; Shimizu et al., 2008). Shimizu et al. (2008) used a lab‐scale Natural Volatile Delivery System to spread lavender EO and linalyl acetate in the room environment, avoiding dishomogeneous EO dispersion. This device was placed 10 centimeters below the nose of each subject and allowed EO flow at constant pressure and quantity (amount per liter of air). Heuberger and Ilmberger (2010), Kim et al. (2011) and Saiorwan et al. (2012) administered EO by means of inhalation from a face mask. In particular, Heuberger and Ilmberger (2010) applied to subjects a surgical mask coated with lavender EO; Saiorwan et al. (2012) mixed volatile components of EO with oxygen through a pump connected to an oxygen mask; Kim et al. (2011) applied a cotton swab soaked with EO to an oxygen mask. No indication about the operating oxygen mask flow has been described. Kiecolt‐Glaser et al. (2008) and Chamine and Oken (2016) used tissue diffusion method by applying a cotton swab below the subjects' nose. In their study, Kiecolt‐Glaser et al. (2008) used a commercial lavender EO, identified by its specific tradename and producer, diluted in water (four drops/30 ml).
Five studies used indirect EO administration systems (Colzato et al., 2014; Moss et al., 2003; Sakamoto et al., 2005; Sellaro et al., 2015; Yue et al., 2016). Sakamoto et al. (2005) and Yue et al. (2016) used general evaporative methods to diffuse lavender and jasmine EO samples. Authors also described the adequate room parameters to guarantee insulation conditions and homogenous EO concentration in the room. A testing aromatic delimited room was also used by Moss et al. (2003), who employed a dedicated cubicle for a more controlled scent room by applying a soaked cotton pad in a container under the subject's seat. Colzato et al. (2014) and Sellaro et al. (2015) administered EO by means of a candle for a fixed exposure time.
Effects of lavender EO inhalation on arousal
Three studies investigated lavender aromatherapy effects on arousal in healthy subjects (Kiecolt‐Glaser et al., 2008; Kim et al., 2011; Saiorwan et al., 2012). Physiological measurements were assessed at baseline and after exposure to EO. In the studies by Kim et al. (2011) and by Saiorwan et al. (2012), participants inhaled EO while resting, whereas, in Kiecolt‐Glaser and colleagues' study (2008), participants underwent a stress‐response inducing test.
Kiecolt‐Glaser et al. (2008) found complex results while studying physiological responses to the cold pressor test as a function of inhaled EO while manipulating odor expectancy (priming). In their experiment, they found that norepinephrine levels following the cold pressor test diminished in water and lavender conditions compared to lemon condition; delayed hypersensitivity to candida, an index of T‐cells immunity, and blastogenesis response to mitogens, that are markers of stress‐arousing response, were smaller after lavender condition compared to water condition. Primed and blind groups showed no differences. Blastogenesis response to mitogens was higher in lavender primed group than in water and lemon primed groups. EO did not alter the following stress‐related parameters: interleukin‐6 and interleukin‐10 levels, salivary cortisol, heart rate (HR) or blood pressure (BP); however, expectancy effect was revealed for HR, since primed groups showed higher HR increase after cold pressor test, but also larger HR decrease during recovery as compared to blind groups.
Saiorwan et al. (2012) assessed central and peripheral correlates of arousal level. EEG measures revealed fronto‐central and temporal increase of power in alpha and theta bands, which, as the authors state, are indices of arousal decrease,while autonomic parameters showed reduction of HR, systolic and diastolic BP and skin temperature in the lavender group compared to the control group (administered with base oil), but no differences were shown in respiratory rate.
Kim et al. (2011) assessed arousal with the bispectral index (BIS), an electroencephalogram derivative index which reflects the level of consciousness during sedation and acupuncture (Fassoulaki et al., 2003; Liu et al., 1996) while inserting a needle into the forearm skin to induce a stress‐arousing state in healthy participants. Authors found a decrease of arousal in the lavender EO group compared to the control group, whose participants were administered with oxygen only. Between‐group differences were significant at 5, 10, 15, and 20 but not at 25 min after the end of EO inhalation.
Effects of lavender EO inhalation on attention
Eight studies investigated the effects of lavender EO on attention in healthy subjects (Chamine & Oken, 2016; Colzato et al., 2014; Heuberger & Ilmberger, 2010; Moss et al., 2003; Sakamoto et al., 2005; Sellaro et al., 2015; Shimizu et al., 2008; Yue et al., 2016). All studies assessed performance on cognitive tasks while subjects were inhaling EO, except for Sakamoto and colleagues' study (2005), in which participants were administered with EO during intervals between cognitive task sessions.
Chamine and Oken (2016) implemented a mixed design in which lavender and coconut groups were divided into two subgroups: one group was told it would receive a relaxing aroma, the other was told that the aroma might have relaxing effects or not. Control group was administered with water and received no priming. Participants then underwent a stress battery including emotional and physical stressors while inhaling lavender or coconut EO or distilled water. Authors conducted cognitive and physiological assessment to detect the effects of EO controlling for expectation. Authors did not find significant changes after EO inhalation for Stroop Color‐Word test (Stroop, 1935) (a task in which the color of the ink of a list of color names does not match with the semantic significance of the words and subjects are required to say the name of the ink, instead of reading the color name) and WAIS‐III Letter‐Number Sequencing (Wechsler, 1997) (a task in which participants must first say the numbers in ascending order and then the letters in alphabetical order), but faster reaction times were detected among primed subgroups, regardless of the EO. Physiological assessment showed changes in cortisol level and in respiratory rate, but chromogranin A level, a reliable marker of psychological stress, was found to be lower in the control group as compared to both EO groups.
Shimizu et al. (2008) reported faster reaction times during a sustained attention task after lavender EO administration, compared to the control group that received no odor. Between‐group differences were significant at 14, 16, 26, and 30 min after EO administration onset.
In Sakamoto et al.'s (2005) study, a sustained attention task was performed by participants, showing that, compared to jasmine and no‐odor groups, lavender EO produced faster reaction times and improved accuracy during all task sessions.
The study by Heuberger and Ilmberger (2010) showed faster reaction times during a vigilance task in participants who had inhaled 20 μl of linalyl acetate as compared to those in the control group who were administered with water. This result was not significant for 5 μl of linalyl acetate. Reaction times decrease correlated with subjective ratings of fragrance: the more pleasant the lavender fragrance was rated, the faster reaction times were. Few errors correlated with subjective ratings too: the more stimulating the water, the fewer the errors.
On the contrary, Moss et al. (2003) reported slower reaction times induced by lavender EO inhalation during a vigilance task, compared to rosemary and no‐odor groups.
Colzato et al. (2014) investigated the effects of lavender and peppermint EO inhalation on the attentional blink, that reflects temporal limitations in visual attention allocation, assessed with RSVP task (Potter, 1975), a task in which subjects are required to detect a target stimulus embedded in a stream of visual stimuli. Attentional blink was found to be less pronounced among participants in the lavender group as compared to the control group, which did not undergo any intervention. The authors of the study interpreted the result as a less focused attentional state induced by lavender.
Sellaro et al. (2015) investigated Simon effect (Simon & Rudell, 1967) in the following three groups: participants inhaling lavender EO; participants inhaling peppermint EO; participants exposed to no aroma. This effect is the difference in accuracy or reaction times between trials in which stimulus and response are on the same side and trials in which they are on opposite sides, with responses being generally slower and less accurate when stimulus and response are on opposite sides; namely, the subject is required to manage spatially‐conflicting stimuli to provide the correct motor response: indeed, the task assesses the ability to suppress automatic response when the localization of the stimulus is different from the localization in which the response is required. More errors in the stimulus/response locations discordance condition were found in the lavender group, compared to peppermint or control groups.
Yue et al. (2016) investigated how time perception varied depending on different EO inhalation, as a measure of odor‐dependent attentional deployment. The authors found that, during the time tracking task, subjects perceived longer time duration than the actual time duration in the lavender group, compared to garlic and jasmine groups.
Effects of lavender EO inhalation on memory
Two studies investigated lavender EO effects on memory in healthy subjects, reporting different results (Chamine & Oken, 2016; Moss et al., 2003). Performance on cognitive tasks was assessed during EO administration.
Chamine and Oken (2016) found working memory performance improvement after lavender inhalation as compared to placebo and water inhalation; memory was assessed by WAIS‐III digit span backward (Wechsler, 1997). Neither priming nor placebo effects were reported. However, Moss et al. (2003) showed that participants who had inhaled lavender EO had worse working memory performance as compared to the rosemary group and to the control group that received no odor. Working memory was assessed by CDR battery (Wesnes et al., 1999), which comprises numerical working memory and spatial working memory tasks; for both tasks, speed and accuracy were measured.
DISCUSSION
This systematic review aims to provide a comprehensive qualitative synthesis of psychophysiological outcomes of lavender EO inhalation, taking into consideration its quality issues as well as diffusion systems (Figure 3).
Lavender EO and cognition
As regards the effects of lavender EO inhalation on attention, the most frequent result emerging from the studies was reaction times decrease during sustained attention tasks (Sakamoto et al., 2005; Shimizu et al., 2008; Yue et al., 2016). These findings, together with less pronounced attentional blink found by Colzato et al. (2014), suggest that lavender might induce a temporally distributed attentional state, which positively influences performance in sustained attention tasks, at the expense of focused attentional state, as shown in Sellaro and colleagues' study (2015): in fact, lavender produced a more pronounced Simon effect, which is elicited by a task implying conflict monitoring abilities (Simon & Rudell, 1967).
However, Chamine and Oken (2016) showed that faster reaction times in a vigilance task were induced by the expectancy effect, since high expectations led to cognition enhancement, regardless of intrinsic properties of lavender. These results are coherent with Heuberger and Ilmberger's (2010) study, which found that increase of performance speed correlated with subjective ratings of odor. Therefore, these studies suggest that priming and judgement of the odor value were responsible for cognition enhancement instead of lavender EO itself.
The aforementioned findings might be related to the arousal outcomes. The experimental procedures of these studies implied a stress‐inducing situation leading to arousal increase, which led to performance detriment and which was found to be reversed by lavender inhalation. Indeed, Saiorwan et al. (2012) found a decrease in autonomic parameters and an increase of power in alpha, beta and theta bands during lavender EO inhalation, indicating arousal decrease. These findings are corroborated by one EEG study which reported increased alpha waves after lavender EO inhalation, suggesting a relaxing effect (Park et al., 2019). Moreover, three studies (Diego et al., 1998; Dimpfel et al., 2004; Songsamoe et al., 2020) showed a positive correlation between lavender EO inhalation and beta power increase, indicating arousal decrease; in the same study, arousal decline allowed the subjects to better perform sustained attention tasks. It is worth noting that Diego et al. (1998), Dimpfel et al. (2004), Park et al. (2019) and Songsamoe et al. (2020) also studied cognitive effects of lavender EO; however, these studies were not included in this systematic review because they were based on a within‐subject design. Moreover, beta power is reported to be also related to less pronounced attentional blink (Gross et al., 2004; Shapiro et al., 2017), and this supports the hypothesis of a possible causal relation between lavender EO inhalation, arousal decrease and higher tonic attention performance. Indeed, as the Yerkes and Dodson (1908) law states, arousal decrease induced by lavender EO inhalation might have produced performance empowerment in sustained attention tasks.
As regards the few and controversial results about working memory, its improvement reported by Chamine and Oken (2016) might be explained by Yerkes and Dodson law (Yerkes & Dodson, 1908), as well. Indeed, arousal decline induced by lavender can be considered as a cognitive substrate which promotes the deployment of higher cognitive functions, including working memory, whose structural and functional correlates partly overlap with those of attention (deBettencourt et al., 2019; Petersen & Posner, 2012). However, Moss et al. (2003) did not confirm the previous findings, reporting working memory decline after lavender EO inhalation. This incoherence might reflect the use of different neuropsychological tools to assess working memory: Chamine and Oken (2016) employed WAIS‐III backward digit span (Wechsler, 1997), while Moss et al. (2003) used CDR battery (Wesnes et al., 1999). It is well stated that a certain degree of task impurity can affect neuropsychological tasks. In this context, working memory subscale of CDR battery might be more specific for working memory assessment than WAIS‐III backward digit span.
The previously‐mentioned results suggest potential central neural correlates of odor‐induced effects. Focusing on lavender EO mechanism of action, the principal component of lavender is the acyclic monoterpene linalool, which has been shown to have GABA A receptors as main targets (Hossain et al., 2002; Milanos et al., 2017). In fact, volatile components are reported to bind to odorant receptors at the olfactory epithelium (Buck, 1993). Harada et al. (2018) confirmed that linalool effect was mediated by GABA, since it was antagonized by flumazenil. Since the GABAergic circuit has been found to actively modulate arousal in locus coeruleus (Breton‐Provencher & Sur, 2019), it is possible to assume that lavender EO might induce arousal decrease acting through the GABAergic pathway. Moreover, this effect was not observed in anosmic mice, indicating that it was triggered by olfactory input evoked by linalool (Harada et al., 2018).
We can speculate that arousal decrement might be related to top‐down processes: since orbitofrontal cortex receives projections from piriform cortex (Plailly et al., 2008; Reiner et al., 2020) and, together with anterior cingulate cortex, represents the only cortical structure to send efferent projections to locus coeruleus (Aston‐Jones & Cohen, 2005), arousal decrease might be induced by the modulation of locus coeruleus GABAergic neurons exerted by orbitofrontal cortex. In fact, neocortex is thought to be the main target of the sedative effects of volatile anesthetics, since its neurons are more sensitive to these chemical compounds (Becker et al., 2012; Hentschke et al., 2005). However, other possible neurobiological mechanisms of action of lavender volatile components are not to be excluded: since some evidence reported linalool to also act through the serotonergic pathway (Chioca et al., 2013; Lee et al., 2018), its effect could be ascribable to raphe nucleus activation by olfactory input from olfactory bulb (Miro et al., 2001), and subsequent bottom‐up process to cortical areas (Arguinchona & Tadi, 2021; Kandel, 1985).
However, since only one study (Chamine & Oken, 2016) included placebo condition, a placebo effect or an overlap between lavender EO mechanism of action and placebo effect are not to be definitely ruled out. Indeed, Masaoka et al. (2013) reported a decrease in pain perception induced by expectancy of the analgesic effects of lavender; as compared to the control group and subjects inhaling aroma without knowing it, this effect was more prominent in subjects who were informed they were inhaling an analgesic aroma and actually inhaled lavender EO, but it was also present among subjects who believed they were inhaling an analgesic EO but received no treatment. This study was not included in the systematic review because it investigated a psychological domain that does not concern the present study. Similarly, since placebo effect derives from subjective interpretation of context information and most of medical treatments benefits are caused by the brain's response to the treatment context (Wager & Atlas, 2015), visible EO diffusion devices might act as contextual cues, inducing an expectancy effect in subjects. However, the mere effect of aromatherapy is supported by some studies that highlighted cognitive enhancing effects of lavender EO in several clinical conditions that imply cognitive impairment, such as dementia and Alzheimer's disease (Ayaz et al., 2017).
Anatomo‐functional evidence corroborates the role of lavender EO inhalation in modulating cognitive functions through specific neuroanatomical pathways triggered by olfactory bulb. A recent study reported anatomical and functional connectivity between olfactory system and subcortical hubs, suggesting an indirect role of olfactory stimulation in arousal, reward learning, emotions and memory (Zelano et al., 2016); in particular, the stimulation of olfactory epithelium mechanoreceptors was found to lead to electrical activity synchronization of olfactory bulb, piriform cortex, hippocampus, and amygdala as well (Heck et al., 2019; Piarulli et al., 2018). Furthermore, the high degree of functional connectivity between olfactory systems and neocortex suggests that olfactory stimulation may play an important role in the modulation of higher cognitive functions, such as executive functions and attention (Cohen et al., 2015; Smith & Bhatnagar, 2019).
EO quality control and intervention protocols
Due to the EO compositional complexity, chemical profile assessment should be considered as a preliminary crucial step to ensure quality and safety of research in aromatherapy.
Despite the importance of providing the gas chromatography–mass spectrometry composition, batch number and trade name of EO sample, in most of the reviewed papers, understanding the EO quality of the inhaled sample was not possible. In a few cases (Heuberger & Ilmberger, 2010; Saiorwan et al., 2012; Shimizu et al., 2008), only two main major constituents, linalool and linalyl acetate, have been described. However, the presence of other typical minor compounds has not been provided. In general, monitoring the EO compositional data is crucial in order to manage adverse effects, such as hepatotoxic effects exerted by some volatile constituents, namely, pulegone, menthofuran, camphor, limonene, zederone, and germacrone. These well‐known natural compounds are frequently used in traditional medicine, pharmaceutical industry and cosmetics. Furthermore, they exhibit high bioavailability through several ways of administration, including inhalation (Zárybnický et al., 2018). For this reason, in the case of lavender EO, chemical composition should be specifically reported even when the singular chemical compounds linalool or linalyl acetate are used, to guarantee purity and to exclude the presence of hazard or allergenic additives (ISO TC). Regarding the current official documentation, the community herbal monograph on L. angustifolia Miller aetheroleum, adopted by EMA, provided specific posologies, including oral administration methods, based on its long‐standing usage (EMA & HMPC, 2012). In addition, a further assessment report has also been recently required on L. angustifolia Mill. by the EMA to update research outcomes on safety of lavender in relieving stress and anxiety. No relevant further changes to the monograph were deemed necessary in consideration of lavender confirmed safety (EMA & HMPC, 2020). The World Health Organization restated that oral use of lavender is safe (WHO monographs on selected medicinal plants, 2007, 2018). Lavender scent has been tested in many research studies without any reported side effects (Kritsidima et al., 2010; Muzzarelli et al., 2006) and adverse skin reactions (Khan & Abourashed, 2011) have been observed with topical use only.
Lavender medical properties are especially due to its EO and in particular to its major volatile constituents linalool and linalyl acetate, which have recently been reported to prevent damages to liver (hepatoprotective properties) and kidneys (nephroprotective properties) (Altınok‐Yipel et al., 2020; Mazani et al., 2020; Mohamed et al., 2020). In addition, they showed no mutagenic activity in different Salmonella typhimurium strains with or without metabolic activation (Evandri et al., 2005).
Considering lavender EO composition complexity and variability, quali‐quantitative characterization is necessary to determine the contribution of each component. Focusing on the main lavender constituents, the ratio between linalool and linalyl acetate in the EO batch sample should be specified, as linalyl acetate derives from linalool during the extraction process. Furthermore, the monoterpene linalool (3,7‐dimethylocta‐1,6‐dien‐3‐ol) has generally been reported to influence the transmission of odor stimulation. In fact, the oxygen atom's structure allows hydrogen bond formation with a hydrogen atom on the olfactory receptor. In addition, the chemical relationship between linalyl acetate and linalool is really tight, because linalool is also the common and direct product of linalyl acetate hydrolysis together with acetic acid. Therefore, lavender EO activity could be strongly influenced by the linalyl acetate hydrolysis in favor of linalool levels followed by the unpleasant pungent odor of acetic acid (Akira, 2007).
Among the reviewed papers, Kiecolt‐Glaser et al. (2008) used lavender EO extracted by steam distillation in lab‐scale and analysed it before the intervention by gas chromatography–mass spectrometry. They monitored lavender EO also during the successive experimental sessions in order to avoid any compositional variability, but without reporting compositional data. Colzato et al. (2014) considered linalool as the main component of lavender EO and, most importantly, they suggested its role in attention increase: in this context, the authors only measured linalyl acetate as reference control. Both linalool and linalyl acetate have been reported to have complementary mechanisms which seem to be responsible for lavender EO cognitive effects (Cavanagh & Wilkinson, 2002; Degel & Koster, 1999). On the other hand, other studies reported only linalool to be responsible for nervous system modulation. Regarding the qualification of linalyl acetate as character impact compound reported in Heuberger and Ilmberger (2010), L. angustifolia Miller EO shows quite similar amounts of both linalyl acetate and linalool (ranges 7–56%, 6–50% respectively; generally 30–35%), following Pharmacopoeia (Pharmacopée Française X Édition, 1983, 1990) or ISO/AFNOR guidelines (ISO TC). Therefore, both these compounds have been believed to be active substances. Nevertheless, Heuberger and Ilmberger (2010) asserted linalyl acetate to be the main psychotropic active principle. In light of the linalool and linalyl acetate structure–activity relationship, the EO profile in toto has to be considered, due to the fact that minor constituents can also play a crucial role in olfactory stimulation, too. In addition, the authors used EO and an absolute ether extract introducing all of them by the general terms “fragrances”. They specified the related provider, but no mention was made of their being either synthetic or isolated from plant material, or of the quality control results of these materials in terms of EO composition.
Another important drawback is the wide variety of vehicles used to dilute the inhaled sample, since they could interfere with the lavender EO olfactory stimulation. In this context, Diego et al. (1998) diluted and administered lavender EO using grapeseed oil (10%), while Saiorwan et al. (2012) diluted lavender EO in sweet almond oil (10%) and Kim et al. (2011) used jojoba oil (2%) as vehicle.
To ensure homogeneous experimental procedures and treatment, the established and controlled exposition time should be associated with dimensioned room, air ventilation parameters and administration device, in order to allow protocol reproducibility and results comparability.
As regards diffusion systems, indirect administration represents a practical and cheap method that guarantees blindness towards procedure; however, while the oils volatile components are gradually released into the air, aroma quick dissipation may cause chemical degradation of some natural lavender constituents (Halligudi & Al Ojaili, 2013). For example, circumscribed steam diffusion (a tank with boiled water), used by Sakamoto et al. (2005), allows a quick release of aroma into the room, but it does not guarantee a long‐lasting and constant permanence of aroma in the air, since the use of heat may invalidate EO quality by disrupting its constituents (Halligudi & Al Ojaili, 2013). Furthermore, in order to guarantee a sufficient amount of aroma for a proper temporal range into the room, the tank used in the study was not small enough to ensure subjects' blindness towards procedure. Therefore, in this case, indirect administration method did not guarantee subjects' unawareness towards protocol. Concerning direct administration systems, cotton tissue generally supports the diffusion in other highly practical methods. Despite preventing EO from decomposition, cotton does not promote aroma diffusion into the air (Halligudi & Al Ojaili, 2013). This procedure has the advantage of avoiding volatile components dispersion, but it does not guarantee blindness towards procedure; moreover, a preliminary sensitivity test is recommended to avoid skin reactions. In fact, Kiecolt‐Glaser et al. (2008) considered their experimental system (soaked cotton ball with a fixed volume of EO tapped between nose and upper‐lip) better than environmental room inhalation, as it guarantees more continuous and homogeneous exposure. Heuberger and Ilmberger (2010), Kim et al. (2011) and Saiorwan et al. (2012) administered EO using a face mask. In particular, Heuberger and Ilmberger (2010) applied a surgical mask coated with lavender EO to subjects; Saiorwan et al. (2012) mixed volatile components of EO with oxygen through a pump connected to an oxygen mask; Kim et al. (2011) applied a cotton swab soaked with EO to an oxygen mask. These methods allow the maintenance of EO quality, preventing the environmental dispersion and possible contamination with other indoor volatiles. However, these advantages did not lead to subjects' blindness with respect to aroma diffusion. Therefore, direct and indirect EO diffusion systems can be considered as two complementary administration methods: on the one hand, if direct administration systems prevent EO from dispersion but may manipulate subjects' expectancy towards the procedure, on the other hand, indirect administration systems guarantee subjects' blindness but preclude EO components integrity.
Limitations and Future Perspectives
Among the reviewed studies, several issues arise from a wide and unclear variety of terms used for the description of the administered EO. These issues are frequently observed in the field of complementary and alternative medicine, since several systematic reviews highlighted methodological flaws and heterogeneity of protocols (Kantor, 2009; Ng et al., 2018). This fact has an important impact on the evaluation of the effectiveness of the inhalation protocol. The selection of lab‐scale distilled EO, commercial lavender EO and marker composition should be based on the relative certificate of analysis in order to provide information linked to supplier and transformation process (plat raw material, extraction, carrier type, dilution ratio, process, batch number). Due to the frequent market adulteration for many plant derivatives, it is not possible to assume that every sold EO has been checked and passed quality requirements. In fact, botanical authenticity and certification results are crucial to guarantee the subject's safety to better understand the relationship between EO volatile components and odor‐induced effects on cognitive functions. In addition, batch‐to‐batch quali‐quantitative chemical analysis, which is the quantification of the EO volatiles to guarantee their quality and their required composition in the transformation process, should be required in each intervention session in order to check the chemical stability of the tested EO sample before its administration. Furthermore, the carrier oil used in the aromatic sample preparation should be described to study the stability and the correct EO posology in each exposure experimental session.
Regarding diffusion systems, EO stability should be guaranteed under controlled temperature and sample oxygen exposure in order to avoid any sort of EO degradation. Considering the direct or indirect administration systems during the exposure to EO, an oxygen mask with controlled EO flow and soaked cotton pad could be considered the most effective method to reduce this problem as compared to room diffusion through candles or electric systems. However, the lab‐scale oxygen mask is generally described without any information about the operative flow. In this case, it is extremely important to focus on the composition of EO and the vehicle suitability to adjust the right posology but also to avoid any allergic or undesirable reaction. For this reason, in the case of facial masks, more than in other types of diffusion systems, different doses of lavender EO should be administered in order to shed light on the minimum quantity needed to induce an effect on cognition. Although facial masks might guarantee a better controlled EO flow in administration protocols compared to environmental diffusion, its alleged placebo effect on the subject is not to be neglected in the evaluation of lavender EO inhalation effects.
Concerning cognitive enhancing effects, the experimental heterogeneity of the selected studies did not allow conducting a meta‐analysis investigation. However, in the light of the foregoing evidence, we can suggest that aromatherapy might be considered a burgeoning practical and non‐invasive method to enhance cognitive functions, for example to prevent work‐related lapses of cognitive performance, such as car accidents, medication errors, diagnosis failure (Hanowski et al., 2009; Nichols et al., 2008). Stronger conclusions can be obtained with the adoption of more specific and accurate experimental protocols for EO inhalation, since NOS for quality assessment revealed adequate but low scores, due to lack of information provided with regard to EO source, extraction and analyses and due to lack of control of biasing variables such as placebo or expectancy effect. In particular, the investigation of cognitive effects duration and the introduction of appropriate placebo conditions are required to better understand the underlying mechanisms of EO inhalation. To disentangle this issue, a placebo group with inert odor administration should be included and biasing contextual cues such as diffusion devices should be controlled. In this context, to avoid subject biasing and simultaneously prevent EO dispersion, treatment administration in delimited areas such as cubicles and the employment of a hidden diffusion device with a flowmeter should be considered.
Generalization of cognitive enhancing effects to clinical conditions that imply cognitive impairment, such as dementia, was not possible, since one of the eligibility criteria of the present systematic review was to include studies on healthy subjects.
CONCLUSIONS
From this systematic review, it was not possible to determine whether different EO components ratios have different impacts on cognitive function modulation, although cognition enhancement induced by lavender EO inhalation was observed. However, the employment of different diffusion devices might have contaminated the pure effect of EO on cognition, due to EO degradation in indirect systems and expectancy effect induced by subjects' awareness of aroma inhalation in direct systems. Nevertheless, despite these biasing variables, lavender EO inhalation was shown to produce a decrease of arousal level and an improvement of sustained component of attention. Robust conclusion as regards memory domain cannot be stated, since only two studies investigated this function, reporting opposite results.
Whatever further research outcomes in the field of aromatherapy will be achieved, selecting lavender EO by its quality in order to perform a suitable posology and avoid any adverse effect will be crucial as a starting point for any experimental procedure.
CONFLICT OF INTEREST
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
Supporting information
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
This work was supported by MUR (Ministry of University and Research) and by the University of Pisa funding V.C. and S.F. PhD scholarship. Open Access Funding provided by Universita degli Studi di Pisa within the CRUI‐CARE Agreement.
[Correction added on 25 May 2022, after first online publication: CRUI‐CARE funding statement has been added.]
Malloggi, E. , Menicucci, D. , Cesari, V. , Frumento, S. , Gemignani, A. , & Bertoli, A. (2022). Lavender aromatherapy: A systematic review from essential oil quality and administration methods to cognitive enhancing effects. Applied Psychology: Health and Well‐Being, 14(2), 663–690. 10.1111/aphw.12310
Funding information Università di Pisa; MUR (Ministry of University and Research)
Contributor Information
Eleonora Malloggi, Email: eleonoramalloggi@gmail.com.
Danilo Menicucci, Email: danilo.menicucci@unipi.it.
DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENT
N/A
REFERENCES
- Akira, N. (2007). Physiological background of aromatherapy. Japanese Soc Aromatherapy, 6, 13–21. [Google Scholar]
- Altınok‐Yipel, F. , Ozan Tekeli, İ. , Özsoy, Ş. Y. , Güvenç, M. , Kaya, A. , & Yipel, M. (2020). Hepatoprotective activity of linalool in rats against liver injury induced by carbon tetrachloride. International Journal for Vitamin and Nutrition Research, 90(3–4), 302–308. 10.1024/0300-9831/a000581 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arguinchona, J. H. , & Tadi, P. (2021). Neuroanatomy, reticular activating system. In StatPearls [Internet]. StatPearls Publishing. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arslan, I. , Aydinoglu, S. , & Karan, N. B. (2020). Can lavender oil inhalation help to overcome dental anxiety and pain in children? A randomized clinical trial. European Journal of Pediatrics, 179(6), 985–992. 10.1007/s00431-020-03595-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aston‐Jones, G. , & Cohen, J. (2005). An integrative theory of locus coeruleus‐norepinephrine function: Adaptive gain and optimal performance. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 28, 403–450. 10.1146/annurev.neuro.28.061604.135709 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ayaz, M. , Sadiq, A. , Junaid, M. , Ullah, F. , Subhan, F. , & Ahmed, J. (2017). Neuroprotective and anti‐aging potentials of essential oils from aromatic and medicinal plants. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 9, 168. 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00168 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baser, K. H. C. , Özek, T. , & Konakchiev, A. (2011). Enantiomeric distribution of linalool, linalyl acetate and camphor in Bulgarian lavender oil. Journal of EO Research, 17, 135–136. [Google Scholar]
- Becker, K. , Eder, M. , Ranft, A. , von Meyer, L. , Zieglgänsberger, W. , Kochs, E. , & Dodt, H. U. (2012). Low dose isoflurane exerts opposing effects on neuronal network excitability in neocortex and hippocampus. PLoS One, 7(6), e39346. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Breton‐Provencher, V. , & Sur, M. (2019). Active control of arousal by a locus coeruleus GABAergic circuit. Nature Neuroscience, 22(2), 218–228. 10.1038/s41593-018-0305-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buck, L. B. (1993). Receptor diversity and spatial patterning in the mammalian olfactory system. Ciba Foundation Symposium, 179, 51–64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cavanagh, H. M. A. , & Wilkinson, J. M. (2002). Biological activities of lavender EO. Phytotherapy Research, 16, 301–308. 10.1002/ptr.1103 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chamine, I. , & Oken, B. S. (2016). Aroma effects on physiologic and cognitive function following acute stress: A mechanism investigation. The Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, 22(9), 713–721. 10.1089/acm.2015.0349 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chioca, L. R. , Ferro, M. M. , Baretta, I. P. , Oliveira, S. M. , Silva, C. R. , Ferreira, J. , Losso, E. M. , & Andreatini, R. (2013). Anxiolytic‐like effect of lavender essential oil inhalation in mice: Participation of serotonergic but not GABA A/benzodiazepine neurotransmission. Journal of Ethnopharmacology, 147(2), 412–418. 10.1016/j.jep.2013.03.028 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen, Y. , Putrino, D. , & Wilson, D. A. (2015). Dynamic cortical lateralization during olfactory discrimination learning. The Journal of Physiology, 593(7), 1701–1714. 10.1113/jphysiol.2014.288381 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Colzato, L. S. , Sellaro, R. , Paccani, C. R. , & Hommel, B. (2014). Attentional control in the attentional blink is modulated by odor. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 76, 1510–1515. 10.3758/s13414-014-0733-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- da Costa Santos, C. M. , de Mattos Pimenta, C. A. , & Nombre, M. R. (2007). The PICO strategy for the research question construction and evidence search. Revista Latino‐Americana de Enfermagem, 15(3), 508–511. 10.1590/S0104-11692007000300023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- deBettencourt, M. T. , Keene, P. A. , Awh, E. , & Vogel, E. K. (2019). Real‐time triggering reveals concurrent lapses of attention and working memory. Nature Human Behaviour, 3(8), 808–816. 10.1038/s41562-019-0606-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Degel, J. , & Koster, E. P. (1999). Odors: implicit memory and performance. Chemical Senses, 24, 317–325. 10.1093/chemse/24.3.317 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diego, M. A. , Jones, N. A. , Field, T. , Hernandez‐Reif, M. , Schanberg, S. , Kuhn, C. , McAdam, V. , Galamaga, R. , & Galamaga, M. (1998). Aromatherapy positively affects mood, EEG patterns of alertness and math computations. International Journal of Neuroscience, 96(3–4), 217–224. 10.3109/00207459808986469 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dimpfel, W. , Pischel, I. , & Lehnfeld, R. (2004). Effects of lozenge containing lavender oil, extracts from hops, lemon balm and oat on electrical brain activity of volunteers. European Journal of Medical Research, 9(9), 423–431. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dufes, C. , Olivier, J.‐C. , Gaillard, F. , Gaillard, A. , Couet, W. , & Muller, J.‐M. (2003). Brain delivery of vasoactive intestinal peptide (VIP) following nasal administration to rats. International Journal of Pharmaceutics, 255(1‐2), 87–97. 10.1016/s0378-5173(03)00039-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- EMA, HMPC . (2012). Community herbal monograph on Lavandula angustifolia , 1–5.
- Erdő, F. , Bors, L. A. , Farkas, D. , Bajza, Á. , & Gizurarson, S. (2018). Evaluation of intranasal delivery route of drug administration for brain targeting. Brain Research Bulletin, 143, 155–170. 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2018.10.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ernst, E. (2000). The role of complementary and alternative medicine. BMJ, 321(7269), 1133–1135. 10.1136/bmj.321.7269.1133 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- European Directorate . (2019). European Pharmacopoeia (10th ed.).
- Evandri, M. G. , Battinelli, L. , Daniele, C. , Mastrangelo, S. , Bolle, P. , & Mazzanti, G. (2005). The antimutagenic activity of Lavandula angustifolia (lavender) essential oil in the bacterial reverse mutation assay. Food and Chemical Toxicology, 43(9), 1381–1387. 10.1016/j.fct.2005.03.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fassoulaki, A. , Paraskeva, A. , Patris, K. , Pourgiezi, T. , & Kostopanagiotou, G. (2003). Pressure applied on the extra 1 acupuncture point reduces bispectral index values and stress in volunteers. Anesthesia & Analgesia, 96(3), 885–890. 10.1213/01.ANE.0000048713.41657.D3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gross, J. , Schmitz, F. , Schnitzler, I. , Kessler, K. , Shapiro, K. , Hommel, B. , & Schnitzler, A. (2004). Modulation of long‐range neural synchrony reflects temporal limitations of visual attention in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 101(35), 13050–13055. 10.1073/pnas.0404944101 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Halligudi, N. , & Al Ojaili, M. (2013). The science and art of aromatherapy: A brief review. Journal of Biomedical and Pharmaceutical Research, 2(2), 6–14. [Google Scholar]
- Hanowski, R. J. , Hickman, J. S. , Olson, R. L. , & Bocanegra, J. (2009). Evaluating the 2003 revised hours‐of‐service regulations for truck drivers: The impact of time‐on‐task on critical incident risk. Accident; Analysis and Prevention, 41(2), 268–275. 10.1016/j.aap.2008.11.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harada, H. , Kashiwadani, H. , Kanmura, Y. , & Kuwaki, T. (2018). Linalool odor‐induced anxiolytic effects in mice. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 12, 241. 10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00241 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heck, D. H. , Kozma, R. , & Kay, L. M. (2019). The rhythm of memory: How breathing shapes memory function. Journal of Neurophysiology, 122(2), 563–571. 10.1152/jn.00200.2019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hentschke, H. , Schwarz, C. , & Antkowiak, B. (2005). Neocortex is the major target of sedative concentrations of volatile anaesthetics: Strong depression of firing rates and increase of GABA A receptor mediated inhibition. The European Journal of Neuroscience, 21, 93–102. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03843.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heuberger, E. , & Ilmberger, J. (2010). The influence of essential oils on human vigilance. Natural Product Communications, 5(9), 1441–1446. 10.1177/1934578X1000500919 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoffmann, D. (2003). Medical herbalism. Healing Arts Press. [Google Scholar]
- Hossain, S. J. , Hamamoto, K. , Aoshima, H. , & Hara, Y. (2002). Effects of tea components on the response of GABA(A) receptors expressed in Xenopus Oocytes. Journal of Agriculture and Food Chemistry, 50, 3954–3960. 10.1021/jf011607h [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kandel, E. R. (1985). JH Schwartz principles of neural science (Vol. 2). Elsevier. [Google Scholar]
- Kantor, M. (2009). The role of rigorous scientific evaluation in the use and practice of complementary and alternative medicine. Journal of the American College of Radiology, 6(4), 254–262. 10.1016/j.jacr.2008.09.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Khan, A. I. , & Abourashed, A. E. (2011). Leung's Encyclopedia of common natural ingredients: Used in food, drugs and cosmetics (Vol. 3) (pp. 50–52). John Wiley & Sons. [Google Scholar]
- Kianpour, M. , Mansouri, A. , Mehrabi, T. , & Asghari, G. (2016). Effect of lavender scent inhalation on prevention of stress, anxiety and depression in the postpartum period. Iranian Journal of Nursing and Midwifery Research, 21(2), 197–201. 10.4103/1735-9066.178248 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kiecolt‐Glaser, J. K. , Graham, J. E. , Malarkey, W. B. , Porter, K. , Lemeshow, S. , & Glaser, R. (2008). Olfactory influences on mood and autonomic, endocrine, and immune function. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 33, 328–339. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2007.11.015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim, S. , Kim, H.‐J. , Yeo, J.‐S. , Hong, S.‐J. , Lee, J.‐M. , & Jeon, Y. (2011). The effect of lavender oil on stress, bispectral index values, and needle insertion pain in volunteers. The Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, 17(9), 823–826. 10.1089/acm.2010.0644 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kritsidima, M. , Newton, T. , & Asimakopoulou, K. (2010). The effects of lavender scent on dental patient anxiety levels: A cluster randomised‐controlled trial. Community Dentistry and Oral Epidemiology, 38(1), 83e87. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lawrence, B. M. (1993). Progress in EOs, lavender oils. Perfumer and Flavorist, 18(1), 58–61. [Google Scholar]
- Lee, B. K. , Jung, A. N. , & Jung, Y. S. (2018). Linalool ameliorates memory loss and behavioral impairment induced by REM‐sleep deprivation through the serotonergic pathway. Biomolecules & Therapeutics (Seoul), 26(4), 368–373. 10.4062/biomolther.2018.081 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu, J. , Singh, H. , & White, P. F. (1996). Electroencephalogram bispectral analysis predicts the depth of midazolam‐induced sedation. The Journal of the American Society of Anesthesiologists, 84(1), 64–69. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Masaoka, Y. , Takayama, M. , Yajima, H. , Kawase, A. , Takakura, N. , & Homma, I. (2013). Analgesia is enhanced by providing information regarding good outcomes associated with an odor: Placebo effects in aromatherapy? Evidence‐Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine, 1(8), 921802. 10.1155/2013/921802 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maštovská, K. , & Lehotay, S. J. (2003). Practical approaches to fast gas chromatography–mass spectrometry. Journal of Chromatography A, 1000(1–2), 153–180. 10.1016/S0021-9673(03)00448-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mazani, M. , Rezagholizadeh, L. , Shamsi, S. , Mahdavifard, S. , Ojarudi, M. , Salimnejad, R. , & Salimi, A. (2020). Protection of CCl4‐induced hepatic and renal damage by linalool. Drug and Chemical Toxicology, 13, 1–9. 10.1080/01480545.2020.1792487 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McIntyre, E. , Saliba, A. J. , Wiener, K. K. , & Sarris, J. (2016). Herbal medicine use behaviour in Australian adults who experience anxiety: A descriptive study. BMC Complementary and Alternative Medicine, 16, 60. 10.1186/s12906-016-1022-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mendeley Support Team . (2011). Getting started with Mendeley. Mendeley Desktop (pp. 1–16). Mendeley Ltd. http://www.mendeley.com [Google Scholar]
- Milanos, S. , Elsharif, S. A. , Janzen, D. , Buettner, A. , & Villmann, C. (2017). Metabolic products of linalool and modulation of GABA A receptors. Frontiers in Chemistry, 5, 46. 10.3389/fchem.2017.00046 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller, L. , & Miller, B. (1999). Ayurveda and aromatherapy. Lotus Press. [Google Scholar]
- Miro, X. , Perez‐Torres, S. , Palacios, J. M. , Puigdomènech, P. , & Mengod, G. (2001). Differential distribution of cAMP‐specific phosphodiesterase 7A mRNA in rat brain and peripheral organs. Synapse, 40, 201–214. 10.1002/syn.1043 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mirski, M. A. , Lewin, J. J. 3rd , Ledroux, S. , Thompson, C. , Murakami, P. , Zink, E. K. , & Griswold, M. (2010). Cognitive improvement during continuous sedation in critically ill, awake and responsive patients: The Acute Neurological ICU Sedation Trial (ANIST). Intensive Care Medicine, 36(9), 1505–1513. 10.1007/s00134-010-1874-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mohamed, M. E. , Abduldaium, Y. S. , & Younis, N. S. (2020). Ameliorative effect of linalool in cisplatin‐induced nephrotoxicity: The role of HMGB1/TLR4/NF‐κB and Nrf2/HO1 Pathways. Biomolecules, 10(11), 1488. 10.3390/biom10111488 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moher, D. , Ghersi, D. , Dooley, G. , Stewart, L. , Clarke, M. & Macleod, M. (n.d.). PROSPERO: International prospective register of systematic reviews. https://www.crd.york.ac.uk/prospero/ [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Moher, D. , Liberati, A. , Tetzlaff, J. , & Altman, D. G. (2009). Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta‐analyses: The PRISMA statement. Annals of Internal Medicine, 151(4), 264–269. 10.7326/0003-4819-151-4-200908180-00135 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moss, M. , Cook, J. , Wesnes, K. , & Duckett, P. (2003). Aromas of rosemary and lavender EOs differentially affect cognition and mood in healthy adults. International Journal of Neuroscience, 113, 15–38. 10.1080/00207450390161903 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muzzarelli, L. , Force, M. , & Sebold, M. (2006). Aromatherapy and reducing preproceduralanxiety: A controlled prospective study. Gastroenterology Nursing, 29(6), 466–471. 10.1097/00001610-200611000-00005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ng, Q. X. , Peters, C. , Ho, C. Y. X. , Lim, D. Y. , & Yeo, W. S. (2018). A meta‐analysis of the use of probiotics to alleviate depressive symptoms. Journal of Affective Disorders, 228, 13–19. 10.1016/j.jad.2017.11.063 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nichols, P. , Copeland, T. S. , Craib, I. A. , Hopkins, P. , & Bruce, D. G. (2008). Learning from error: identifying contributory causes of medication errors in an Australian hospital. The Medical Journal of Australia, 188(5), 276–279. 10.5694/j.1326-5377.2008.tb01619.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- European Medicine Agency (EMA), Committee on Herbal Medicinal Products (HMPC) (2020). Addendum to Assessment report on Lavandula angustifolia Mill. (L.officinalis Chaix), flos.
- Park, K. H. , Kim, H. J. , Oh, B. , Seo, M. , Lee, E. , & Ha, J. (2019). Evaluation of human electroencephalogram change for sensory effects of fragrance. Skin Research and Technology, 25(4), 526–531. 10.1111/srt.12682 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petersen, D. (2013). Aromatherapy: Characteristics of the market. Conference Proceedings of the 2013 International Federation of Essential Oils and Aroma Trades Conference, San Francisco, CA, USA.
- Petersen, S. E. , & Posner, M. I. (2012). The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 35, 73–89. 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150525 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Piarulli, A. , Zaccaro, A. , Laurino, M. , Menicucci, D. , De Vito, A. , Bruschini, L. , Berrettini, S. , Bergamasco, M. , Laureys, S. , & Gemignani, A. (2018). Ultra‐slow mechanical stimulation of olfactory epithelium modulates consciousness by slowing cerebral rhythms in humans. Scientific Reports, 8(1), 6581. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Plailly, J. , Howard, J. D. , Gitelman, D. R. , & Gottfried, J. A. (2008). Attention to odor modulates thalamocortical connectivity in the human brain. The Journal of Neuroscience, 28(20), 5257–5267. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5607-07.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poldrack, R . (2011). Cognitive Atlas. http://www.cognitiveatlas.org/
- Potter, M. C. (1975). Meaning in visual search. Science, 187(4180), 965–966. 10.1126/science.1145183 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reiner, D. J. , Lofaro, O. M. , Applebey, S. V. , Korah, H. , Venniro, M. , Cifani, C. , Bossert, J. M. , & Shaham, Y. (2020). Role of projections between piriform cortex and orbitofrontal cortex in relapse to fentanyl seeking after palatable food choice‐induced voluntary abstinence. The Journal of Neuroscience, 40(12), 2485–2497. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2693-19.2020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saiorwan, W. , Siripornpanich, V. , Piriyapunyaporn, T. , Hongratanaworakit, T. , Kotchabhakdi, N. , & Ruangrungsi, N. (2012). The effects of lavender oil inhalation on emotional states, autonomic nervous system, and brain electrical activity. Journal of the Medical Association of Thailand, 95(4), 598–606. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sakamoto, R. , Minoura, K. , Usui, A. , Ishizuka, Y. , & Kanba, S. (2005). Effectiveness of aroma on work efficiency: Lavender aroma during recesses prevents deterioration of work performance. Chemical Senses, 30, 683–691. 10.1093/chemse/bji061 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sellaro, R. , Hommel, B. , Rossi Paccani, C. , & Colzato, L. S. (2015). With peppermints you're not my prince: Aroma modulates self‐other integration. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 77, 2817–2825. 10.3758/s13414-015-0955-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shapiro, K. L. , Hanslmayr, S. , Enns, J. T. , & Lleras, A. (2017). Alpha, beta: The rhythm of the attentional blink. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 24(6), 1862–1869. 10.3758/s13423-017-1257-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shimizu, K. , Gyokusen, M. , Kitamura, S. , Kawabe, T. , Kozaki, T. , Ishibashi, K. , Izumi, R. , Mizunoya, W. , Ohnuki, K. , & Kondo, R. (2008). Essential oil of lavender inhibited the decreased attention during a long‐term task in humans. Bioscience, Biotechnology, and Biochemistry, 72(7), 1944–1947. 10.1271/bbb.70835 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simon, J. R. , & Rudell, A. P. (1967). Auditory S–R compatibility: The effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing. Journal of Applied Psychology, 51, 300–304. 10.1037/h0020586 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith, T. D. , & Bhatnagar, K. P. (2019). Anatomy of the olfactory system. Handbook of Clinical Neurology, 164, 17–28. 10.1016/B978-0-444-63855-7.00002-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Songsamoe, S. , Koomhin, P. , & Matan, N. (2020). The effects of Michelia alba oil against mould on brown rice and assessing the brain response using electroencephalogram (EEG). Journal of Food Science and Technology, 6, 1–12. 10.1007/s13197-020-04689-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18(6), 643–662. 10.1037/h0054651 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Thomas, D. V. (2002). Aromatherapy: Mythical, magical, or medicinal? Holistic Nursing Practice, 16(5), 8–16. 10.1097/00004650-200210000-00005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Umezu, T. , Nagano, K. , Ito, H. , Kosakai, K. , Sakaniwa, M. , & Morita, M. (2006). Anticonflict effects of lavender oil and identification of its active constituents. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior, 85(4), 713–721. 10.1016/j.pbb.2006.10.026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wager, T. D. , & Atlas, L. Y. (2015). The neuroscience of placebo effects: Connecting context, learning and health. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 16(7), 403–418. 10.1038/nrn3976 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler, D. (1997). WAIS‐III Administration and Scoring Manual. Psychological Corp. [Google Scholar]
- Wells, G. A. , Shea, B. , O'Connell, D. A. , Peterson, J. , Welch, V. , Losos, M. , & Tugwell, P. (2000). The Newcastle‐Ottawa Scale (NOS) for assessing the quality of nonrandomized studies in meta‐analyses.
- Wesnes, K. A. , Wand, T. , Ayre, G. , & Pincock, C. (1999). Validity and utility of the cognitive drug research (CDR) computerized assessment system: A review following fifteen years of usage. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 9(sup. 5), S368. [Google Scholar]
- World Health Organization (WHO) . (2018). The International Pharmacopoeia (8th ed.). World Health Organization, Dept. of Essential Medicines and Pharmaceutical Policies. [Google Scholar]
- World Health Organization, WHO monographs on selected medicinal plants . (2007). http://apps.who.int/medicinedocs/en/m/abstract/Js14213e/
- Xu, F. , Uebaba, K. , Ogawa, H. , Tatsuse, T. , Wang, B. H. , Hisajima, T. , & Venkatraman, S. (2008). Pharmaco‐physio‐psychologic effect of Ayurvedic oil‐dripping treatment using an essential oil from Lavendula angustifolia. Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, 14(8), 947–956. 10.1089/acm.2008.0240 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yayla, E. M. , & Ozdemir, L. (2019). Effect of inhalation aromatherapy on procedural pain and anxiety after needle insertion into an implantable central venous port catheter: A quasi‐randomized controlled pilot study. Cancer Nursing, 42(1), 35–41. 10.1097/NCC.0000000000000551 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yerkes, R. M. , & Dodson, J. D. (1908). The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit‐formation. Journal of Comparative Neurology and Psychology, 18, 459–482. 10.1002/cne.920180503 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Yue, Z. , Gao, T. , Chen, L. , & Wu, J. (2016). Odors bias time perception in visual and auditory modalities. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 535. 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00535 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zárybnický, T. , Boušová, I. , Ambrož, M. , & Skálová, L. (2018). Hepatotoxicity of monoterpenes and sesquiterpenes. Archives of Toxicology, 92, 1–13. 10.1007/s00204-017-2062-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zelano, C. , Jiang, H. , Zhou, G. , Arora, N. , Schuele, S. , Rosenow, J. , & Gottfried, J. A. (2016). Nasal respiration entrains human limbic oscillations and modulates cognitive function. The Journal of Neuroscience, 36(49), 12448–12468. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2586-16.2016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhong, Y. , Zheng, Q. , Hu, P. , Huang, X. , Yang, M. , Ren, G. , Du, Q. , Luo, J. , Zhang, K. , Li, J. , Wu, H. , Guo, Y. , & Liu, S. (2019). Sedative and hypnotic effects of compound Anshen essential oil inhalation for insomnia. BMC Complementary and Alternative Medicine, 19(1), 306. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
N/A