Abstract
The X‐chromosome is often excluded from genome‐wide association studies because of analytical challenges. Some of the problems, such as the random, skewed, or no X‐inactivation model uncertainty, have been investigated. Other considerations have received little to no attention, such as the value in considering nonadditive and gene–sex interaction effects, and the inferential consequence of choosing different baseline alleles (i.e., the reference vs. the alternative allele). Here we propose a unified and flexible regression‐based association test for X‐chromosomal variants. We provide theoretical justifications for its robustness in the presence of various model uncertainties, as well as for its improved power when compared with the existing approaches under certain scenarios. For completeness, we also revisit the autosomes and show that the proposed framework leads to a more robust approach than the standard method. Finally, we provide supporting evidence by revisiting several published association studies. Supporting Information for this article are available online.
Keywords: confounding, dominance, interaction, model uncertainty, regression
1. INTRODUCTION
Genome‐wide association studies (GWAS) are ubiquitous, delivering significant insights into the genetic determinants of complex traits over the past decade (Visscher et al., 2017). For this reason, it is surprising that it is not a common practice to include the X‐chromosome in GWAS (Konig et al., 2014; Wise et al., 2013). The X‐chromosome differs from the autosomes in that males have only one copy of the X‐chromosome while females have two, and at any given genomic location one of the two copies in females may be silenced (Gendrel & Heard, 2011), referred to as X‐chromosome inactivation (XCI). The choice of the silenced copy could be random or skewed toward a specific copy (Wang et al., 2014). These unique aspects lead to more complex analytic considerations for genetic association analysis of X‐chromosomal variants, such as bi‐allelic single‐nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs).
A bi‐allelic SNP has two alleles, and , of which one is the reference allele and the other is the alternative allele with allele frequency . An autosomal SNP has three genotypes regardless of sex, namely . In association analysis of an autosomal SNP, the common practice is to simply model a binary or continuous phenotype as an additive function of the number of copies of the non‐baseline allele present in ; that is, coding additively as . Here, without loss of generality, is chosen to be the baseline allele in a statistical model and the non‐baseline allele. When is binary, this regression‐based additive test is also equivalent to the Cochran–Armitage trend test (Wellek & Ziegler, 2012). Although both dominant and recessive genetic models of inheritance are possible, among these one degrees of freedom (1 df) models, a common practice for GWAS is to use the additive model, because it has reasonable power to detect both additive and dominance effects at a causal variant, and at variants in linkage disequilibrium (LD) with the causal variant (Bush & Moore, 2012; Hill et al., 2008). An alternative parameterization is the 2 df genotypic model that includes both the additive term and the dominance term. In the case of recessive genetic inheritance, Zhou et al. (2017) showed that the 2 df genotypic test outperforms the 1 df additive test for binary outcomes, and Dizier et al. (2017) reached the same conclusion for continuous traits. In the case of additive genetic inheritance being true, the genotypic test is known to be less powerful than the additive test due to the increased df, which is unnecessary. The preferred test for unknown genetic inheritance in terms of power and robustness to different genetic models is, however, not clear across different true genetic effect sizes, sample sizes, and significance levels.
For an X‐chromosomal SNP, the most commonly used approach assumes additivity and XCI. However, recent work (Tukiainen et al., 2017) showed that up to one‐third of X‐chromosomal genes are expressed from both the active and inactive X‐chromosomes in female cells, with varying degrees of “escape” from inactivation between genes and individuals. Several additional points also require attention. Table 1 describes eight analytical considerations and challenges (C1–C8) present in an X‐chromosome‐inclusive GWAS, including a method's suitability for analyzing both binary and continuous traits (C1), which is related to the type of method used, that is, genotype‐based or allelic association tests (C2); the (under‐appreciated) consequence of the choice of the baseline allele on association analysis of an X‐chromosomal SNP (C3); the importance of including sex as a covariate (C4) and its analytical connection with C3; the value in considering gene‐sex interaction effect (C5) and its connection with the assumption of XCI (C6); and the assumption of random versus skewed XCI (C7) and its connection with nonadditive effects (C8).
Table 1.
Problem | Solution | Relevant sections |
---|---|---|
C1: Quantitative traits vs. binary outcomes | ||
C2: Genotype‐based vs. allele‐based association methods Allele‐based association tests, comparing allele frequency differences between cases and controls, are locally most powerful. However, they analyze binary outcomes only and are sensitive to the Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium (HWE) assumption (Sasieni, 1997). |
Genotype‐based regression models, ‐on‐, support various types of outcome data, account for covariate effects with ease, and are robust to the HWE assumption. | Sections 1 and 2 |
C3: The choice of the baseline allele for association analysis, r vs. R For the autosomes, switching the two alleles does not affect the association inference. Is this true for the X‐chromosome? |
It is not always true for the X‐chromosome, unless is included in the model. | Sections 2.1 and 2.2, and C4 |
C4: Sex as a covariate vs. no S main effect Unlike the autosomes, sex is a confounder when analyzing the X‐chromosome for traits exhibiting sexual dimorphism (e.g., height and weight). Even for the autosomes, sex can be a confounder if allele frequencies differ significantly between males and females.× |
To maintain the correct type I error rate control, the sex main effect must be considered particular when analyzing the X‐chromosome. The resulting association test is also invariant to the choice of the baseline allele. | Section 2.2 and C3 |
C5: Gene–sex interaction vs. no G × S interaction effect Gene–sex interaction might exist, but there is a concern over loss of power due to increased degrees of freedom. In addition, what is the interpretation of gene–sex interaction effect in the presence of X‐inactivation? |
Under no interaction, power loss of modeling interaction is capped at 11.4%. Models including the covariate also lead to tests invariant to the assumption of X‐chromosome inactivation status. | Sections 2.3 and 3, and C6 |
C6: X‐chromosome inactivation (XCI) vs. no XCI XCI occurs if one of the two alleles in a genotype of a female is silenced. Individual‐level XCI status requires additional biological information that are not typically available to genetic association studies. Assuming XCI or no XCI at the sample level leads to different genotype coding strategies (Table 2), and it was thought that this will always lead to different association results. |
XCI uncertainty implies sex‐stratified genetic effect which can be analytically represented by the interaction effect. Teasing apart these different biological phenomenon require other “omic” data and additional analyses. | Sections 2.3 and 5, and C5 |
C7: If XCI, random vs. skewed X‐inactivation If the choice of the silenced allele in females is skewed toward a specific allele, the average effect of the genotype is no longer the average of those of and . |
XCI skewness is statistically equivalent to a dominance genetic effect. | Section 2.4, and C8 |
C8: Dominance effect vs. no GD dominance effect For both the autosomes and X‐chromosome, the most common practice is to use the additive test which has better power than the genotypic test under (approximate) additivity, but it cannot capture dominance effects. The exact trade‐off, however, is not clear. |
We provide analytical and empirical evidence supporting the use of genotypic model when analyzing either the autosomes or X‐chromosome. For an X‐chromosomal variant, including the dominance effect term has the added benefit of resolving of the skewed X‐inactivation uncertainty issue. | Sections 2.4, 3 and 4, and C7 |
Several association methods have been developed for the X‐chromosome, and they are computationally efficient for conducting X‐chromosome‐wide association analysis. However, each method solves only some of the C1–C8 challenges. For example, Zheng et al. (2007) considered only binary outcomes for which both genotype‐ and allele‐based association tests are applicable. The classical allelic association test, comparing allele frequencies between case and control groups, is locally most powerful but sensitive to the Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium (HWE) assumption and not applicable to continuous traits (Sasieni, 1997; Zhang & Sun, 2021; Zheng, 2008). Clayton (2008, 2009) discussed analytical strategies assuming the X‐chromosome is always inactivated. Hickey and Bahlo (2011) and Loley et al. (2011) performed simulation studies, each providing a thorough method comparison, for example, between tests of Zheng et al. (2007) and Clayton (2008). Konig et al. (2014) gave detailed guidelines for including the X‐chromosome in GWAS, recommending different tests for different model assumptions (e.g., presence or absence of an interaction effect or XCI), but it is difficult to validate these assumptions in practice. Gao et al. (2015) developed a toolset for conducting X‐chromosome association studies, implementing some of the existing methods. More recently Z. Chen et al. (2017) improved sex‐stratified analysis by eliminating genetic model assumptions, but their method is limited to analyzing genetic main effects on binary traits. Focusing on XCI uncertainty, Wang et al. (2014) proposed a frequentist maximum likelihood solution to deal with no, random or skewed X‐inactivation, and in their follow‐up work Wang et al. (2017) provided a model selection method. In contrast, B. Chen et al. (2020) applied the Bayesian model averaging principle (Draper, 1995) to deal with the XCI uncertainty problem. However, these approaches assumed additive genetic effects. The value in considering dominance and gene‐sex interaction effects, and the inferential consequence of defining different baseline allele (i.e., the reference or the alternative allele) when analyzing an X‐chromosomal SNP, have received little to no attention.
Here we propose a theoretically justified and robust X‐chromosome association method that can simultaneously deal with all eight challenges (C1–C8) outlined in Table 1. We emphasize the robustness of the proposed method to genetic assumptions as our understanding is evolving. For example, although most published X‐chromosome‐inclusive GWAS assumed XCI, recent work has shown that up to a third of genes “escape” XCI (Tukiainen et al., 2017).
The proposed method is regression‐ and genotype‐based (robust to departure from HWE), analyzing either a continuous or binary trait while adjusting for covariate effects. The recommended test has three degrees of freedom, including both additive and dominance genetic effects, as well as a gene–sex interaction effect. We show analytically why the proposed method is robust to the various model uncertainties, including no, random or skewed XCI, as well as the choice of the baseline allele. Desirably, the power of the proposed test is robust to different alternative genetic models, despite its increased degrees of freedom over a simple additive test. We note that the work here focuses on efficient association testing, not parameter estimation or model selection which requires additional biological data (Busque et al., 1996).
We first present our main theory to address the eight challenges associated with X‐chromosome‐inclusive GWAS in Section 2. We then provide analytical results of power study across all possible genetic models, sample sizes and type I error rates, as well as empirical results from simulation studies in Section 3. For methodology completeness, this section also briefly discusses merit of the genotypic model in the familiar context of analyzing autosomal SNPs. We then provide corroborating evidence from several applications in favor of the proposed approach in Section 4. Finally we discuss the limitations of our approach and possible future work in Section 5.
2. METHOD FOR X‐CHROMOSOME‐INCLUSIVE ASSOCIATION ANALYSIS
The proposed method relies on the generalized linear model (McCullagh & Nelder, 1989) as it is flexible, analyzing both binary and continuous traits (C1 of Table 1). As a result, the method is a genotype‐based approach (C2) that is robust to the assumption of HWE by regressing the phenotype data () on genetic data () while accounting for other covariate effects.
For robust and powerful association analysis of a bi‐allelic X‐chromosomal SNP, we recommend the following model:
(1) |
and the corresponding 3 df test, jointly testing
(2) |
where notations for the covariates are defined in Table 2. Other relevant covariates such as environmental factors (s) should also be included in the model but omitted here for notation simplicity.
Table 2.
Effect interpretation | Covariate notation | Non‐baseline allele | X‐chromosome inactivation (XCI) status | Coding schemes | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Females | Males | ||||||||||
|
|
|
|
|
|||||||
|
|
Yes | 0 | 0.5 | 1 | 0 | 1 | ||||
Additive GA |
|
|
Yes | 1 | 0.5 | 0 | 1 | 0 | |||
|
|
|
No | 0 | 1 | 2 | 0 | 1 | |||
|
|
No | 2 | 1 | 0 | 1 | 0 | ||||
Dominance |
|
Either | Either | 0 | 1 | 0 | 0 | 0 | |||
Gene–sex interaction |
|
|
Either | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 1 | |||
|
|
|
Either | 0 | 0 | 0 | 1 | 0 | |||
Sex |
|
Either | Either | 0 | 0 | 0 | 1 | 1 |
Note: The subscripts and represent additive and dominance effects, or represents the non‐baseline allele of which we count the number of copies present in a genotype, and or denotes X‐chromosome inactivated or not inactivated.
We show later (a) why the association result from the proposed approach is invariant to the different (e.g., or ) and (e.g., or ) coding schemes as defined in Table 2, and (b) why the proposed method also solves the C3–C8 issues simultaneously. But before we do so, we first provide more details about the notations presented in Table 2.
2.1. X‐chromosome specific genotype and covariate coding schemes
Table 2 summarizes the various covariate coding schemes for analyzing an X‐chromosomal SNP, when considering all the analytical challenges outlined in Table 1. Note that when the choice of the baseline allele is varied (i.e., either or ) and the XCI status is unknown, there are four ways to code the additive covariate , and two ways to code the gene–sex interaction covariate . The specific coding for sex does not have an impact on our proposed method. In Table 2, a female is coded as 0 and a male as 1, and the interaction term vanishes. If a female were coded as 1 and a male as 0, then is the same as . Thus, in either case it is redundant to include in our proposed regression model.
Using the notations in Table 2, it is immediately clear why the choice of the baseline allele (C3) matters for association analysis of an X‐chromosomal SNP. Under no XCI, if were assumed to be the baseline allele there would be one copy of allele in genotype of a female, and of a male. Thus, genotypes and would be grouped together for association analysis. However, if were chosen to be the baseline allele, genotypes and would be grouped together, resulting in different inference. In contrast, the choice of the baseline allele does not affect association evidence when analyzing an autosomal SNP. It is well‐known that although the estimate of the effect size changes direction, the magnitude of the association remains the same when analyzing an autosomal SNP. But, this is not always true when analyzing an X‐chromosomal SNP.
2.2. Sex as a confounder (C4) and its connection with the choice of the baseline allele (C3)
Sex is a confounder for phenotype‐genotype association analysis of an X‐chromosomal SNP for traits displaying sexual dimorphism. When sex, but not the SNP, is associated with a trait of interest, omitting sex in the analysis leads to false positives. This is because sex is inherently associated with the genotypes of an X‐chromosomal SNP (Table 2); see Ozbek et al. (2018) for empirical evidence from simulation studies. Thus, accuracy of a test provides the first argument for always including as a covariate in association analysis of an X‐chromosomal SNP.
The second advantage of modeling the main effect is more subtle. As shown in Table 2, the coding of depends on the choice of the baseline allele (i.e., or ) and the X‐inactivation status ( for XCI and for no XCI), resulting in a total of four different ways of coding the five genotype groups, namely (0, 0.5, 1, 0, 1)′, (1, 0.5, 0, 1, 0)′, (0, 1, 2, 0, 1)′, and (2, 1, 0, 1, 0)′. Furthermore, and yield different test statistics, because the two coding schemes lead to different groupings of the genotypes as discussed in 2.1. Note that, in contrast to under XCI, under no XCI there is no linear transformation that makes and equivalent. An inference that is invariant to the coding choices may seem difficult, but we show that this is achievable for models that include sex as a covariate.
Theorem 1
Let and be two generalized linear models (McCullagh & Nelder, 1989) with the same link function and , where is the response vector of length and are two design matrices, and and are the corresponding parameter vectors of length . Let , where and are and matrices corresponding to, respectively, the secondary covariates not being tested and the primary covariates of interest, and similarly for , and partition the regression coefficients accordingly as and . If there exists an invertible matrix
where and are, respectively, invertible and matrices, then any of the Wald, Score or LRT tests for testing
are identical under the two models and , resulting in the same association inference for evaluating the primary covariates of interest. Note that given the structure of matrix implies .
We provide the proof of Theorem 1 in Supporting Information Appendix A. Here we emphasize that the two sets of primary covariates being tested, and , are not required to be linear transformation of each other, for example, between and . Instead, and , corresponding to the secondary covariates (including the unit vector if modeling the intercept), that are not being tested must be invertible linear transformations of each other, , in addition to . This result may seem surprising, but the two requirements imply that the two design matrices are equivalent to each other either in general or under the null, resulting in identical F‐test statistics; see Supporting Information Appendix A for technical details.
In our setting when sex is included in the model, consider only the additive effect for the moment, . Then the two design matrices, corresponding to or being the baseline allele and under no XCI, are
In this case, and satisfy the two requirements. Thus, even though and are not linked by a linear transformation, Theorem 1 allows us to conclude that a Wald, Score or LRT test of is invariant to the two coding schemes and , if sex is included as a covariate.
Note that the known result that two tests are equivalent to each other if and , corresponding to primary covariates, are linear transformation of each other is a special case of Theorem 1, where all elements except the first row of are zero; the exception allows for a location shift. For example, under the XCI assumption, and , and . Thus, and satisfy the requirements.
At this point in the methodology development, the preferred model controls the type I error rate and is invariant to the choice of the baseline allele. However, in practice the XCI status is unknown and if we assume there is XCI, and , and
In this case, it is not difficult to show that a matrix satisfying the requirements of Theorem 1 does not exist, because implies that the linear system has no solution, and the XCI uncertainty remains a challenge.
2.3. Gene–sex interaction effect (C5) and its connection with unknown XCI status (C6)
Throughout the paper, we define the interaction term as . Depending on the choice of the baseline allele, has two different codings, namely and (Table 2). In the previous section, we have shown that when is included in the model, that is, , the choice of the baseline allele is no longer of a concern if we test within a particular XCI assumption. Interestingly, when both and are included in the model, , by applying Theorem 1 again, testing is statistically equivalent between the different choices of the baseline allele and the assumption of the XCI status. For example, consider
respectively, for a model assuming XCI and choosing as the baseline allele (i.e., tracking the number of copies of allele ), and for a model assuming no XCI and choosing as the baseline allele, we can show that
satisfy the linear transformation requirements of Theorem 1. That is, for association analysis of an X‐chromosomal SNP, testing based on is invariant to the choice of the baseline allele and the assumption of the X‐inactivation status. Figure 1 summarizes the equivalency between the design matrices that correspond to the different coding schemes studied so far; all the theoretical results have been confirmed empirically via simulations. Our findings are supported by a recent simulation study (Song et al., 2021), where estimation biases in SNP coefficient estimate were observed “in several situations, particularly if the assumptions about XCI made by the coding scheme used and the assumptions made about sex differences in SNP effect of the fitted model were incorrect,” while “Fitting models with SNP Sex interaction terms can avoid reliance on assumptions.”
2.4. Random versus skewed X‐inactivation (C7) and its connection with genetic dominance effect (C8)
Similar to analyzing an autosomal SNP, the first reason for modeling the dominance effect is to capture potential departure from additivity; see Section 3 for additional discussion. For an X‐chromosomal SNP, another important reason is that the dominance effect can also statistically capture skewness of X‐inactivation, if present.
Intuitively, if we assume the effects of and to be, respectively, 0 and 1, the effect of will be either 0 or 1 for each individual, depending on the inactivated allele of the sample collected. If the two alleles are equally likely to be inactivated (i.e., random XCI) across all individuals, the average statistical effect of is 1/2. If is more (or less) likely to be inactivated (i.e., skewed XCI), the average effect of is greater (or less) than 1/2. However, this XCI skewness is analytically equivalent to a dominance effect (i.e., effect of deviating from 1/2), even though dominance effect is at the population level whereas skewed XCI is a sample‐specific property. This analytical equivalency also shows that knowing the true underlying biological model requires more than the standard GWAS data.
Table 3 summarizes the statistical behaviors of all the regression models and corresponding tests discussed in this section. Notably, jointly testing , based on the model , ensures that the inference is invariant to the assumptions of the XCI status and baseline allele, and accounts for dominance effect and XCI‐skewness if present.
Table 3.
Model, | Testing | C3/C4 | C5/C6 | C7/C8 | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
|
|
|
|
|
|||||
|
|
|
|
|
|||||
|
|
|
|
|
|||||
|
|
|
|
|
Note: Whole‐genome considerations such as C1 (continuous vs. binary traits) and C2 (Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium vs. disequilibrium) are naturally dealt with by the genotype‐based regression approach. X‐chromosome‐specific considerations include C3 (choice of the baseline allele), C4 (sex as a confounder and type I error control), C5 (gene‐sex interaction), C6 (X‐chromosome inactivation (XCI) vs. no XCI), C7 (random vs. skewed XCI), and C8 (the dominance effect). In the table, indicates a problem for the corresponding model and test, and means no problem. Relevant covariates 's should be included in the model but omitted here for notation simplicity. Joint testing of based on is the recommended, most robust approach; see Figures 2 and S5 for power comparisons among –.
3. ANALYTICAL AND SIMULATION‐BASED METHOD EVALUATION
The proposed method is easy‐to‐implement and has good type I error control, because regression‐based approach is known to be well behaved, as long as sample size is not too small and allele frequency is not too low, which are satisfied by most GWAS of common variants. Thus, we focus on evaluating power of the proposed method. We first provide a general analytical finding then present some simulation‐based results.
3.1. Using the general theory of χ 2 distributions
One concern with the use of the proposed 3 df test is the potential loss of power due to the increased degrees of freedom. Indeed, if the true model for an X‐chromosomal SNP is without a dominance effect and skewed inactivation, without gene‐sex interaction, and the true inactivation status is known so that the additive genotype variable can be correctly coded, then the corresponding 1 df test will be more powerful than the proposed 3 df test. However, we show that even under the worst‐case scenario and irrespective of sample size and the nominal type I error level, the maximum power loss of the proposed 3 df is, surprisingly, capped at 18.8%, while the potential maximum power gain is (i.e., close to 100%).
Let , and be the 1, 2, and 3 df test statistics derived from the different regression models listed in Table 3. The power difference between the different 's depends on both the non‐centrality parameters and . When all the 's are close to 0, all tests have no power. At the other extreme when all 's are sufficiently large or close to 1, all tests have power close to 1. Thus, we expect meaningful power comparison when 's, and have moderate values.
First, we assume that there are no dominance or interaction effects and the true XCI status is known to study the maximum power loss induced by unnecessarily including the and terms. In that case, and , derived from with the correct genotype coding, is the optimal test. Varying and values, we numerically compute the power of the 's for and . Figure S1 provides a heat plot for power as a function of and for the two tests. Results show that the maximum power loss of compared with is capped at 18.8%, regardless of the true additive effect size, sample size and the level. The maximum occurs at and (Figure S1). At the genome‐wide significance level (Dudgridge & Gusnanto, 2008), the maximum power loss is 17.7% occurring at .
Notably, the maximum of 18.8% power loss holds for comparing any 3 df test with a 1 df test that was derived from the correctly specified 1 df model. This is because the derivation is based on and alone. Second, we emphasize that although a 18.8% loss of power is substantial, the fact that this is the maximum power loss for the 3 df test, under any true 1 df genetic model and regardless of the true genetic effect size, sample size, and significance level, is encouraging, as the potential power gain of the proposed 3 df test under other models can be much greater than 18.8% as we show next.
In the presence of dominance effect/skewed XCI or interaction effect/misspecified XCI, , where . Compared with the maximum power loss of using the proposed 3 df for a 1 df (correctly specified) model, the maximum power gain under other genetic models can be theoretically as large as . To provide specific numerical results, we consider (the worse‐case scenario derived above for the 3 df test), , 10 or 15, and ranging from 0 to 10. Results in Figure S2 show that once is as large as half of (i.e., ), the 3 df test is more powerful than the 1 df test.
Together these two observations suggest that the proposed 3 df test is not only robust to the various model uncertainties associated with analyzing X‐chromosomal variants, but it is reasonably powered as compared with the standard 1 df additive test. Compared with a 2 df test derived from correctly specified or , the global maximum power loss of the proposed 3 df test is capped at 7.7%, occurring at and . At , the maximum power loss is 7.5% occurring at (Figure S1). In contrast, if the 2 df model is misspecified the potential power gain of the proposed 3 df test can be greater than 95%.
Power comparison between a 1 df test and a 2 df test is more relevant to the analysis of an autosomal SNP, but the conclusion is similar to above. For example, under additivity, the maximum power loss of a 2 df genotypic test is capped at 11.4% across all parameter values and sample sizes. The maximum occurs at and , and at the genome‐wide significance level of (Dudgridge & Gusnanto, 2008), the maximum power loss is 10.3% when (Figure S1). Supporting Information Appendix B also provides power comparison between the additive and genotypic tests for association analysis of an autosomal SNP across a range of dominance effects and allele frequencies (Figure S3). For each combination of parameter values considered, Figure S4 and Table S1 also provide the corresponding and .
3.2. Using different genetic models for the X‐chromosome
Here we provide some empirical results based on different genetic models for an X‐chromosomal variant and sample sizes. Note that tests derived from models that do not include sex as a covariate are susceptible to type I error rate inflation. Thus, power comparisons here focus on – as specified in Table 3.
To compare the empirical power, we first derive the non‐centrality parameters of the tests as functions of sample size, additive, dominance, and interaction effects, and under different assumptions of the baseline allele and X‐inactivation status. We provide the technical details in Supporting Information Appendices C and D. We then considered (the worst case scenario for the 3 df test as shown in Section 3.1), and allele frequency or 0.5. Results for other parameter values, including differential allele frequency values between males and females, are provided as Supporting Information; sex‐specific allele frequencies may occur due to sex‐specific selection.
Because of the various analytical equivalencies between interaction and XCI status, and between dominance effect and skewed XCI, the corresponding interaction, dominance, and skewed effect sizes are statistically confounded with each other. Thus, we specified the averaged statistical effect size for each of the five genotype groups, that is, , and . We fixed and , and varied and from to 0.6. Note that fixing and is equivalent to fixing the additive effect under XCI or under no XCI; varying is equivalent to varying the dominance effect from to 0.6. The link with the interaction effect is less clear. Under the XCI assumption, , while under the no XCI assumption, . Thus, for the values considered here, ranged from to 0.3 under XCI, and from to 0.45 under no XCI. For ease of interpretation, Figure 2 uses the “dominance” and “interaction” terms to denote the varying degrees of and .
Results in Figure 2 demonstrate the merits of the proposed method (testing jointly, the red curves). While there could be some power loss in the worse case scenario (no dominance or interaction effects), it is theoretically capped at 18.8% regardless of the parameter values. On the other hand, compared with the standard 1 df additive test (testing and assuming the correct genotype coding, the black curves), power gain can be 70% for the cases considered here. When the allele frequency is 0.2 (Figure 2a), the performance of the 2 df additive and interaction test (testing , the blue curves) is close to the proposed 3 df test. However, that is no longer the case when (Figure 2b), where the 2 df additive and dominance test (testing , the green curves) is better and close to the proposed 3 df test. Figures S5 provides additional results for other parameter values, all showing the robustness of the proposed method, which is testing based on , . In practice the regression model should include relevant 's, which are omitted here for notation simplicity.
The proposed method not only resolves all C1–C8 analytical challenges simultaneously, but also has the best overall performance across the different underlying genetic models. However, we note that our robust method cannot identify the underlying true genetic model. This is true for any method that uses GWAS data alone, because we have shown, for example, XCI uncertainty is analytically equivalent to a gene–sex interaction effect, while XCI skewness is analytically equivalent to dominance effect. The lack of model identifiability of the proposed method, however, does not prevent a robust and powerful association analysis of X‐chromosomal SNPs.
4. APPLICATIONS TO THREE PREVIOUSLY PUBLISHED ASSOCIATION STUDIES
4.1. Reanalyses of the X‐chromosome‐inclusive GWAS of Sun et al. (2012)
This data set consists of 3199 unrelated individuals with cystic fibrosis (CF) and 570,724 genome‐wide bi‐allelic SNPs after standard quality control (Sun et al., 2012). Among the 570,724 SNPs, 14,279 are for the X‐chromosome and 556,445 are from the autosomes. And among the 3199 CF subjects, 574 are cases with meconium ileus, an intestinal obstruction at birth seen in of CF patients (Dupuis et al., 2016), and the remaining 2625 CF subjects are controls; 1722 are males and 1477 are females. The rates of meconium ileus are 17.7% and 18.3%, respectively, in the male and female groups, which is not statistically different.
A previous X‐chromosome‐inclusive GWAS of meconium ileus in CF has been conducted based on this data set (Sun et al., 2012), where the standard 1 df additive test was used for analyzing the autosomal SNPs, and X‐chromosome being inactivated was further assumed for analyzing the X‐chromosomal SNPs (i.e., using model in Table 3 with genotype coding under the assumption of XCI). Here we reanalyze both the autosomal and X‐chromosomal SNPs to demonstrate the utility of the proposed approach.
For the X‐chromosome, we compared the – models and their corresponding tests as detailed in Table 3 in Section 2. For each SNP, we performed six different association tests, depending on which of the – models was used and if the XCI status needed to be specified, because (a) sex must be included to ensure correct type I error rate control and models including are invariant to the choice of the baseline allele (Section 2.2), and (b) models including the gene‐sex interaction effect are invariant to the assumption of XCI (Section 2.3). Figure 3a shows the results for the top 15 ranked X‐chromosomal SNPs, ordered by the minimal p value of all six tests; the lines connecting the SNPs are used only for visualization purposes to demonstrate the robustness of a method.
The application results here are consistent with our earlier analytical and simulation results in Section 3, showing that joint modeling and testing the additive, dominance, and gene–sex interaction effects is the most robust association approach for analyzing X‐chromosomal variants. For example, the result of Sun et al. (2012) ( and assuming XCI) marked by the black curve is clearly less “stable” than the red curve (the proposed ) across different SNPs. In this particular application, we observed that the performance of the orange curve ( assuming no XCI) is similar to the proposed method. However, interestingly, the green curve (also but assuming XCI) is noticeably different from the orange curve.
For the autosomal SNPs, we contrast the standard 1 df additive test with the proposed 2 df genotypic test as briefly discussed in Section 3.1 and detailed in Supporting Information Appendix B. Figure 3b shows the results for the top 15 ranked autosomal SNPs, ordered by the minimal p value of additive and genotypic tests; Figures S7 and S8 provide genome‐wide results. It is clear that if the p values of the standard 1 df additive test (the black curve) are smaller, then those from the recommended 2 df genotypic test (the red curve) are close in magnitude, while the reverse is not true. For example, p value of the recommended 2 df genotypic test for the 6th SNP () in the plot is more than four orders of magnitude smaller than that of the 1 df additive test; there is no evidence for genotyping error at this SNP as the p value of HWE test in the control group is 0.026. The genotype counts for , and are in the case group and in the control group, which yields case/control ratios of , clearly suggesting a dominance pattern. Whether this is a true new finding, however, requires further investigation.
4.2. Evidence from the first (autosome only) GWAS of WTCCC (2007)
We then examined the results of the first (autosome only) GWAS, conducted by the Wellcome Trust Case Control Consortium (WTCCC, 2007). Their tab. 3 lists regions of the genome showing the strongest association signals and provides results from both the 1 df trend test (statistically equivalent to the additive test considered here) and the 2 df genotypic tests.
Consistent with the autosomal results of the CF meconium ileus application above, the results in tab. 3 of WTCCC (2007) also show that if the 1 df additive test provides a smaller p value, the p value of the 2 df genotypic test is at most one order of magnitude larger. For example, the p values are and , respectively, for the 1 df additive and 2 df genotypic tests, testing association between coronary artery disease and rs1333049, the second SNP in Table 3 of WTCCC (2007). On the other hand, the p value of the 2 df genotypic test can be several orders of magnitude smaller that of the 1 df additive test. For example, the p values are and , respectively, for the 1 df additive and 2 df genotypic tests, testing association between bipolar disorder and rs420259, the first SNP in Table 3 of WTCCC (2007); the association between rs420259 and bipolar disorder has since been replicated by other studies (Gonzalez et al., 2016; Tesli et al., 2010). We can draw similar conclusions based on the Bayes factors provided in their tab. 3, obtained under the 1 df additive or 2 df genotypic models.
4.3. Re‐analyses of the 60 autosomal SNPs potentially associated with various complex traits, selected by Wittke‐Thompson et al. (2005)
Finally, we re‐analyzed the 60 autosomal SNPs selected by Wittke‐Thompson et al. (2005) from 41 case‐control association studies of various complex traits, including Alzheimer's disease and breast cancer; the genotype count data are available from Table 1 of Wittke‐Thompson et al. (2005). Although these SNPs were originally selected by Wittke‐Thompson et al. (2005) for a study of departure from HWE, genotype‐based methods are robust to the HWE assumption (Sasieni, 1997; Zhang & Sun, 2021). Here we focused on comparing the standard 1 df additive test with the recommended 2 df genotypic test for analyzing these 60 autosomal SNPs, which are presumed to be associated with complex traits based on the earlier 41 studies.
We observed that the genotypic test leads to 31 SNPs with p values less than , while the additive test results in 22 SNPs (Figure S6). Using the Bonferroni threshold of the numbers are 7 and 6, respectively, for the genotypic and additive tests. Although these autosomal SNPs can only be presumed to be associated with the various complex traits, the empirical evidence here is consistent with the analytical and simulation results in Section 3.1 and Supporting Information Appendix B.
5. DISCUSSION
We have shown that in association analysis of an X‐chromosomal variant, the sex main effect must be included to achieve correct type I error rate control. The inclusion of sex also addresses the complication of baseline allele specification that otherwise affects association inference for an X‐chromosomal SNP, in contrast to an autosomal SNP. Although the method developed here is motivated by genetic association studies of the X‐chromosome, Theorem 1 is applicable to other settings where model uncertainty plays a role. For association studies of autosomal variants, sex is not routinely included. However, sex can be a confounder for an autosomal SNP as well, for example, when there is sex difference in allele frequency due to sex‐specific selection. When the allele frequency difference is small, including sex does not substantively change the association result, because sex is not directly included in the genetic association test. Thus, we recommend to always include sex as a covariate in association analysis of either autosomal or X‐chromosomal variants.
We have also shown that modeling the genetic dominance effect is beneficial for analyzing both the autosomes and X‐chromosome. The proposed model can significantly increase test power when is large. When is close to 0, the model is still robust and maintains ‘comparable’ power with that of the additive model; “comparable” is in the context of the trade‐off between the maximum power loss and gain across different models. For an autosomal SNP, we have shown analytically that even under true additivity, compared with the classical 1 df additive test, the maximum power loss of the 2 df genotypic test is capped at 11.4%, regardless of the sample, genetic effect and test sizes, but the power gain can be as high as . Similarly, for an X‐chromosome SNP, with a 3 df test that includes , and interaction effects, power loss is capped at 18.8%; this assumes that the standard 1 df additive test used the correct XCI model and there is no skewed XCI or dominance effect. If these assumptions do not hold, the potential power gain of the 3 df test can be as high as . However, not all alternative genetic models are equally likely in practice. Consistent with the earlier work of Hill et al. (2008) and Bush and Moore (2012), two recent studies showed that “genetic variance for complex traits is predominantly additive” (Hivert et al., 2021; Pazokitoroudi et al., 2021). To this end, a Bayesian alternative that incorporates prior evidence for the different genetic models can be considered.
When the true genetic model is unknown, one alternative frequentist's approach is to consider all possible models and use the “best” or weighted average. But, such an approach is difficult to implement in practice; see Bagos (2013) for a review. For example, selection bias inherent in choosing the best‐fitted model must be corrected for, often through computationally intensive simulation studies, and power of this bias‐corrected inferential procedure is not clear. On the other hand, ways to obtain a weighted average of the test statistics or p values across all models can be quite ad hoc, and the optimal weighting factors are difficult to derive. The recent Cauchy method can be used to combine correlated p values derived from all possible genetic models (Liu & Xie, 2020). Finally, the method proposed here is tailored for analyzing one common SNP at a time, and joint analysis of multiple common or rare SNPs (Derkach et al., 2014) requires further consideration.
When the true genetic model is unknown, another alternative is to use sex‐stratified analysis, followed by meta‐analysis combining the female and male groups (Willer et al., 2010). This approach appears to be robust to the XCI assumption when analyzing an X‐chromosomal variant, because association evidence in females are the same between the XCI and no‐XCI assumptions. However, the two assumptions lead to different effect size estimates by a factor of two (the standard errors also differ by a factor of two), resulting in different results using the inverse‐variance‐based meta‐analysis. The sample‐size‐based meta‐analysis can overcome this limitation, but other issues remain including difficulty of modeling non‐additive or gene–sex interaction effects.
Summary statistics from the proposed 3 df test for X‐chromosome SNPs can be used to perform meta‐analysis by using, for example, Fisher's combined p‐value approach. The classical inverse‐variance‐based method, however, is not applicable for two reasons. Firstly, there are multiple genotype‐related estimates, , and . Secondly, and more importantly, some of the estimates are not meaningful on their own as we have shown that skewed XCI and dominance effects are statistically confounded with each other, so are the interaction effect and the assumption of XCI. Even if we limit our attention to the genetic main additive effect, the effect size estimate changes by a factor of two depending on the XCI assumption (i.e., the genotype coding scheme). Thus, our work here also highlights new challenges associated with other analyses of X‐chromosomal SNPs. For example, how to aggregate association evidence across multiple SNPs or multiple traits (Zhao & Sun, 2021), and how to perform X‐chromosome‐inclusive polygenic risk score (PRS) analysis (Dudbridge et al., 2018), both of which we will address in future research.
The proposed full model for analyzing an X‐chromosomal SNP, , is robust to various model uncertainties, analytically. However, as noted earlier it is not capable of differentiating between the scenarios. Indeed, the recent work by Song et al. (2021), focusing on effect size estimation as opposed to association testing, showed that genetic effect estimates are sensitive to model assumptions. Using the available genetic association data, Ma et al. (2015) proposed a variance‐based test for detecting X‐inactivation by comparing phenotypic variance of the group with that of the and groups in females, but this method is limited to a continuous trait (Deng et al., 2019; Soave & Sun, 2017). Wang et al. (2014) explicitly introduced a parameter to represent the amount of skewness of X‐inactivation. Our work here, however, shows that the interpretation of their parameter is statistically confounded with dominance genetic effect using GWAS data alone. How to incorporate additional ‘omic’ data (Carrel & Willard, 2005) to tease apart different biological phenomenon is an interesting problem that deserves further investigation.
CONFLICT OF INTERESTS
The authors declare that there are no conflict of interests.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
Bo Chen developed the method and designed the study with significant input from Lei Sun and Radu Craiu. Bo Chen performed the data analysis with significant input from Lei Sun and Lisa J. Strug. Bo Chen wrote the manuscript with significant contributions from all co‐authors. All authors have reviewed and approved the final manuscript.
Supporting information
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
The authors thank the two referees for their helpful and constructive comments, which have substantially improved the presentation of the paper. The authors thank cystic fibrosis patients and their families who participate in the International CF Gene Modifier studies, the US and Canadian CF Foundations for the genotyping and clinical data of the International CF Gene Modifier Consortium, and the members of the consortium Johanna Rommens, Garry Cutting, Michael Knowles, Mitchell Drumm, and Harriet Corvol. The authors would also like to thank Prof. Keith Knight and Prof. Nancy Reid for their helpful comments, and Mr. Bowei Xiao for assisting the CF application. This study is funded by the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (NSERC) to R. V. C. (RGPIN‐249547) and L. S. (RGPIN‐04934; RGPAS‐522594), the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR) to L. S. (MOP‐310732) and L. J. S. (MOP‐258916), and the Cystic Fibrosis Canada to L. J. S. (2626).
Chen, B. , Craiu, R. V. , Strug, L. J. , & Sun, L. (2021). The X factor: A robust and powerful approach to X‐chromosome‐inclusive whole‐genome association studies. Genetic Epidemiology, 45, 694–709. 10.1002/gepi.22422
DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENT
The meconium ileus application data is available by application to the Cystic Fibrosis Canada National data registry for researchers who meet the criteria for access to confidential clinical data for the purpose of CF research. The 60‐SNP application data is publicly available from Table 1 of Wittke‐Thompson et al. (2005). The WTCCC application used only the summary statistics reported in Table 3 of WTCCC (2007).
REFERENCES
- Bagos, P. G. (2013). Genetic model selection in genome‐wide association studies: Robust methods and the use of meta‐analysis. Statistical Applications in Genetics and Molecular Biology, 12(3), 285–308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bush, W. S. , & Moore, J. H. (2012). Chapter 11: Genome‐wide association studies. PLOS Computational Biology, 8(12), e1002822. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Busque, L. , Mio, R. , Mattioli, J. , Brais, E. , Blais, N. , Lalonde, Y. , Maragh, M. , & Gilliland, D. G. (1996). Nonrandom X‐inactivation patterns in normal females: lyonization ratios vary with age. Blood, 88(1), 59–65. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carrel, L. , & Willard, H. F. (2005). X‐inactivation profile reveals extensive variability in X‐linked gene expression in females. Nature, 434, 400–404. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen, B. , Craiu, R. V. , & Sun, L. (2020). Bayesian model averaging for the X‐chromosome inactivation dilemma in genetic association study. Biostatistics, 21(2), 319–335. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen, Z. , Ng, H. K. T. , Li, J. , Liu, Q. , & Huang, H. (2017). Detecting associated single‐nucleotide polymorphisms on the X chromosome in case control genome‐wide association studies. Statistical Methods in Medical Research, 26(2), 567–582. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clayton, D. G. (2008). Testing for association on the X chromosome. Biostatistics, 9, 593–600. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clayton, D. G. (2009). Sex chromosomes and genetic association studies. Genome Medicine, 1, 110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deng, W. , Mao, S. , Kalnapenkis, A. , Esko, T. , Magi, R. , Paré, G. , & Sun, L. (2019). Analytical strategies to include the X‐chromosome in variance heterogeneity analyses: Evidence for trai‐specific polygenic variance structure. Genetic Epidemiology., 43(7), 815–830. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Derkach, A. , Lawless, J. F. , & Sun, L. (2014). Pooled association tests for rare genetic variants: A review and some new results. Statistical Science, 29(2), 302–321. [Google Scholar]
- Dizier, M. H. , Demenais, F. , & Mathieu, F. (2017). Gain of power of the general regression model compared to Cochran‐Armitage trend tests: simulation study and application to bipolar disorder. BMC Genetics, 18 1(24). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Draper, D. (1995). Assessment and propagation of model uncertainty. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society. Series B (Methodological), 57(1), 45–97. [Google Scholar]
- Dudbridge, F. , Pashayan, N. , & Yang, J. (2018). Predictive accuracy of combined genetic and environmental risk scores. Genetic Epidemiology, 42(1), 4–19. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dudgridge, F. , & Gusnanto, A. (2008). Estimation of significance thresholds for genomewide association scans. Genetic Epidemiology, 32(3), 227–234. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dupuis, A. , Keenan, K. , Ooi, C. Y. , Dorfman, R. , Sontag, M. K. , Naehrlich, L. , Castellani, C. , Strug, L. J. , Rommens, J. M. , & Gonska, T. (2016). Prevalence of meconium ileus marks the severity of mutations of the Cystic Fibrosis Transmembrane Conductance Regulator (CFTR) gene. Genetics in Medicine, 18(4), 333–340. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gao, F. , Chang, D. , Biddanda, A. , Ma, L. , Guo, Y. , Zhou, Z. , & Keinan, A. (2015). XWAS: A software toolset for genetic data analysis and association studies of the X chromosome. Journal of Heredity, 106(5), 666–671. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gendrel, A. V. , & Heard, E. (2011). Fifty years of X‐inactivation research. Development, 138, 5049–5055. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gonzalez, S. , Gupta, J. , Villa, E. , Mallawaarachchi, I. , Rodriguez, M. , Ramirez, M. , Zavala, J. , Armas, R. , Dassor, A. , Contreras, J. , Flores, D. , Jerez, A. , Ontiveros, A. , Nicolini, H. , & Escamilla, M. (2016). Replication of genome‐wide association study (GWAS) susceptibility loci in a Latino bipolar disorder cohort. Biopolar Disorders, 18(6), 520–527. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hickey, P. F. , & Bahlo, M. (2011). X chromosome association testing in genome wide association studies. Genetic Epidemiology, 35, 664–670. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hill, W. G. , Goddard, M. E. , & Visscher, P. M. (2008). Data and theory point to mainly additive genetic variance for complex traits. PLOS Genetics, 4(2), e1000008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hivert, V. , Sidorenko, J. , Rohart, F. , Goddard, M. E. , Yang, J. , Wray, N. R. , Yengo, L. , & Visscher, P. M. (2021). Estimation of non‐additive genetic variance in human complex traits from a large sample of unrelated individuals. American Journal of Human Genetics, 108(5), 786–798. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Konig, I. R. , Loley, C. , Erdmann, J. , & Ziegler, A. (2014). How to include chromosome X in your genome‐wide association study. Genetic Epidemiology, 38, 97–103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu, Y. , & Xie, J. (2020). Cauchy combination test: A powerful test with analytic p‐value calculation under arbitrary dependency structures. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 115(529), 393–402. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Loley, C. , Ziegler, A. , & Konig, I. R. (2011). Association tests for X‐chromosomal markers—A comparison of different test statistics. Human Heredity, 71, 23–36. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ma, L. , Hoffman, G. , & Keinan, A. (2015). X‐inactivation informs variance‐based testing for X‐linked association of a quantitative trait. BMC Genomics, 16, 241. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCullagh, P. , & Nelder, J. A. (1989). Generalized linear models (2nd ed.). Chapman & Hall. [Google Scholar]
- Ozbek, U. , Lin, H. M. , Lin, Y. , Weeks, D. E. , Chen, W. , Shaffer, J. R. , Purcell, S. M. , & Feingold, E. (2018). Statistics for X‐chromosome associations. Genetic Epidemiology, 42(6), 539–550. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pazokitoroudi, A. , Chiu, A. M. , Burch, K. S. , Pasaniuc, B. , & Sankararaman, S. (2021). Quantifying the contribution of dominance deviation effects to complex trait variation in biobank‐scale data. American Journal of Human Genetics, 108(5), 799–808. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sasieni, P. D. (1997). From genotypes to genes: Doubling the sample size. Biometrics, 53(4), 1253–1261. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Soave, D. , & Sun, L. (2017). A generalized Levene's scale test for variance heterogeneity in the presence of sample correlation and group uncertainty. Biometrics, 73, 960–971. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Song, Y. , Biernacka, J. , & Winham, S. (2021). Testing and estimation of x‐chromosome SNP effects: Impact of model assumptions. Genetic Epidemiology, 45(6), 577–592. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sun, L. , Rommens, J. M. , Corvol, H. , Li, W. , Li, X. , Chiang, T. A. , Lin, F. , Dorfman, R. , Busson, P. F. , Parekh, R. V. , Zelenika, D. , Blackman, S. M. , Corey, M. , Doshi, V. K. , Henderson, L. , Naughton, K. M. , O'Neal, W. K. , Pace, R. G. , Stonebraker, J. R. , … Strug, L. J. (2012). Multiple apical plasma membrane constituents are associated with susceptibility to meconium ileus in individuals with cystic fibrosis. Nature Genetics, 44(5), 562–569. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tesli, M. , Athanasiu, L. , Mattingsdal, M. , Kahler, A. K. , Gustafsson, O. , Andreassen, B. K. , Werge, T. , Hansen, T. , Mors, O. , Mellerup, E. , Koefoed, P. , Jönsson, E. G. , Agartz, I. , Melle, I. , Morken, G. , Djurovic, S. , & Andreassen, O. A. (2010). Association analysis of palb2 and brca2 in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia in a Scandinavian case‐control sample. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 13B(7), 1276–1282. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tukiainen, T. , Villani, A.‐C. , Yen, A. , Rivas, M. A. , Marshall, J. L. , Satija, R. , Aguirre, M. , Gauthier, L. , Fleharty, M. , Kirby, A. , Cummings, B. B. , Castel, S. E. , Karczewski, K. J. , Aguet, F. , Byrnes, A. , GTEx Consortium , Lappalainen, T. , Regev, A. , Ardlie, K. G. , … MacArthur, D. G. (2017). Landscape of X chromosome inactivation across human tissues. Nature, 550(7675), 244–248. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Visscher, P. M. , Wray, N. R. , Zhang, Q. , Sklar, P. , McCarthy, M. I. , Brown, M. A. , & Yang, J. (2017). 10 years of GWAS discovery: Biology, function, and translation. American Journal of Human Genetics, 101(1), 5–22. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang, J. , Talluri, R. , & Shete, S. (2017). Selection of X‐chromosome inactivation model. Cancer Informatics, 16, 1–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang, J. , Yu, R. , & Shete, S. (2014). X‐chromosome genetic association test accounting for X‐inactivation, skewed X‐inactivation, and escape from X‐inactivation. Genetic Epidemiology, 38, 483–493. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wellek, S. , & Ziegler, A. (2012). Cochran‐Armitage test versus logistic regression in the analysis of genetic association studies. Human Heredity, 73(1), 14–17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Willer, C. J. , Li, Y. , & Abecasis, G. R. (2010). METAL: Fast and efficient meta‐analysis of genomewide association scans. Bioinformatics, 26(17), 2190–219. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wise, A. L. , Gyi, L. , & Manolio, T. A. (2013). eXclusion: Toward integrating the X chromosome in genome‐wide association analyses. American Journal of Human Genetics, 92, 643–647. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wittke‐Thompson, J. K. , Pluzhnikov, A. , & Cox, N. J. (2005). Rational inferences about departures from Hardy‐Weinberg equilibrium. American Journal of Human Genetics, 76, 967–986. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- WTCCC . (2007). Genome‐wide association study of 14,000 cases of seven common diseases and 3,000 shared controls. Nature, 447, 661–678. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang, L. , & Sun, L. (2021). A generalized robust allele‐based genetic association test. Biometrics. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhao, Y. , & Sun, L. (2021). On set‐based association tests: Insights from a regression using summary statistics. The Canadian Journal of Statistics. [Google Scholar]
- Zheng, G. (2008). Can the allelic test be retired from analysis of case‐control association studies? Annals of Human Genetics, 72, 848–851. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zheng, G. , Joo, J. , Zhang, C. , & Geller, N. L. (2007). Testing association for markers on the X chromosome. Genetic Epidemiology, 31, 834–843. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhou, Z. , Ku, H. C. , Huang, Z. , Xing, G. , & Xing, C. (2017). Differentiating the Cochran‐Armitage trend test and Pearson's chi‐squared test: Location and dispersion. Annals of Human Genetics, 81(5), 184–189. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The meconium ileus application data is available by application to the Cystic Fibrosis Canada National data registry for researchers who meet the criteria for access to confidential clinical data for the purpose of CF research. The 60‐SNP application data is publicly available from Table 1 of Wittke‐Thompson et al. (2005). The WTCCC application used only the summary statistics reported in Table 3 of WTCCC (2007).