Abstract
Wondrously elaborate weapons and displays that appear to be counter to ecological optima are widespread features of male contests for mates across the animal kingdom. To understand how such diverse traits evolve, here we develop a quantitative genetic model of sexual selection for a male signaling trait that mediates aggression in male‐male contests and show that an honest indicator of aggression can generate selection on itself by altering the social environment. This can cause selection to accelerate as the trait is elaborated, leading to runaway evolution. Thus, an evolving source of selection provided by the social environment is the fundamental unifying feature of runaway sexual selection driven by either male‐male competition or female mate choice. However, a key difference is that runaway driven by male‐male competition requires signal honesty. Our model identifies simple conditions that provide clear, testable predictions for empirical studies using standard quantitative genetic methods.
Keywords: aggression, honest signals, indirect genetic effects, male‐male competition, quantitative genetics, runaway evolution, sexual selection, social evolution, weapons
Biologists and the public are captivated by extravagant traits used during male‐male contests for mates. Elaborations such as weapons, showy signals, and complex courtship are curious because they don't obviously contribute to survival, and in many instances would seem to work to attract predation. We provide a quantitative genetic model that shows how such elaboration can evolve, and when populations will shoot past an optimum leading to runaway evolution.
INTRODUCTION
Darwin (1859, 1871) chronicled the amazing diversity of traits associated with success in mating, proposing the theory of sexual selection to explain the diverse exaggerated, spectacular, and bizarre structures and behaviors found in males of many species. Darwin suggested that such traits evolve either because they enhance success in contests between males for access to females or because they are preferred by females when choosing mates. As Darwin (1871) wrote, “It is certain that amongst almost all animals there is a struggle between the males for the possession of the female. This fact is so notorious that it would be superfluous to give examples.” In contrast, the ability of females to influence evolution through choice of partners was almost immediately questioned and continued to be controversial for decades after Darwin (Huxley, 1938; Wallace, 1889). However, theoretical models of evolution via female choice (Kirkpatrick, 1982; Lande, 1981; Mead & Arnold, 2004) and empirical research documenting female preference in nature (Andersson, 1982, 1994; Rosenthal, 2017) eventually led to mate choice becoming the dominant paradigm in studies of sexual selection. The development of formal mathematical models showing that male traits and female preferences coevolve in self‐reinforcing fashion, an idea first proposed by Fisher (1915, 1930), was particularly crucial to the acceptance of mate choice as an important evolutionary mechanism. The key component of the Fisher process is that female preference and a preferred male trait become genetically correlated as a result of assortative mating that generates linkage disequilibrium between the preference and male trait. This can cause sexually selected male traits to evolve at ever‐increasing speed, a pattern that has been referred to as an evolutionary “runaway” (Bailey & Moore, 2012; Fisher, 1930; Lande, 1981).
Despite the current bias towards studies focused on mate choice, Darwin was not wrong about male‐male competition. Members of entire taxa are characterized by highly modified sexually dimorphic structures that function only in male contests (e.g., Dermaptera, Briceño & Eberhard, 1995). Weapons can evolve to be massive and create real functional constraints for the males that bear them, and such bizarrely elaborate and diverse structures associated with duels are indeed found across the animal kingdom (Emlen, 2008, 2014; McCullough et al., 2016; O'Brien et al., 2018). In fact, male‐male competition remains a more common source of selection shaping male traits that influence mating success, and traits expressed in male‐male interactions can be as elaborate as those that are the target of female preferences (Andersson, 1994; Darwin, 1871; Emlen, 2008, 2014; Huxley, 1938; McCullough et al., 2016; Moore & Moore, 2006; O'Brien et al., 2018). However, we still lack robust genetic models that generate testable predictions for the evolution of sexually selected traits via male‐male competition. Notably, the potential for male‐male competition to result in a runaway process that drives extreme trait elaboration remains unresolved.
Many elaborate male traits used in male‐male contests, such as showy plumage (Hagelin, 2002), color (Seehausen & Schluter, 2004), pheromones (Moore, Reagan‐Wallin, et al., 1997), and structures such as antlers (Wilkinson & Dodson, 1997), horns (Emlen et al., 2005), forceps (Briceño & Eberhard, 1995), and claws (Sneddon et al., 1997) function as signals that may provide information about some underlying qualities of the individuals, such as the willingness or ability to fight (Emlen, 2008, 2014; Maynard Smith & Harper, 1988, 2003; Parker, 1974). For example, there is often a positive association between signals or weapons and other traits such as body size (McCullough et al., 2016; O'Brien et al., 2018), making the signal or weapon an honest indicator of potential threat to an opponent. As such, males are expected to adaptively modulate their behavior in response to these signaling traits, escalating contests they are more likely to win and withdrawing from ones they are more likely to lose. Because the effect of signaling traits inherently depends on social context, such traits serve as both targets and sources of selection, potentially leading to self‐reinforcing and accelerating selection as occurs in the runaway process driven by female preference (Bailey & Kölliker, 2019; Lande, 1981). However, despite insights from game theory models (Maynard Smith & Brown, 1986; Maynard Smith & Harper, 1988, 2003; Parker, 1974; Rutte et al., 2006), how this fundamental feature of extreme elaboration—an evolving source of selection—may arise within male‐male contests is unclear.
Here, we utilize a framework that explicitly incorporates socially contingent trait expression and fitness (McGlothlin et al., 2010; Moore, Brodie, et al., 1997; Wolf et al., 1999) to model trait evolution arising from male‐male competition. We show that when honest signals are used to modulate the behavior of competitors, male‐male competition leads to evolutionary elaboration of male traits. We identify the necessary and sufficient conditions for trait elaboration to become a runaway process and outline predictions that can be empirically tested to evaluate this scenario in natural systems. We show that sexual selection by male‐male competition can have features that are analogous to those of runaway sexual selection by female choice; just as in female mate choice, the social environment in male‐male contests may generate a self‐reinforcing source of selection on the traits that mediate the interaction, potentially leading to self‐sustaining and escalating selection.
MODEL
To capture the influence of the social environment in a model of male‐male competition, we assume that individuals adjust their behavior in response to the signaling trait values of their social partners, an assumption that is supported empirically and theoretically (Maynard Smith, 1982; Moore, Brodie, et al., 1997; O'Brien et al., 2018; Parker, 1974; Rico‐Guevara & Hurme, 2019; Tinghitella et al., 2018; West‐Eberhard, 1979, 1983, 1984, 2003; Wiens & Tuschhoff, 2020). Because the social context (i.e., the social environment) is constructed from traits of conspecifics, this flexible response to social context provides the opportunity for indirect genetic effects (Moore, Brodie, et al., 1997), which allow the social environment itself to evolve (Bijma & Wade, 2008; McGlothlin et al., 2010; Moore et al., 2002; Wolf, 2003). Evolutionary changes in the social environment can lead to concerted evolution because the social environment can be a source of selection on the traits that themselves compose the social environment (Araya‐Ajoy et al., 2020; Bailey & Kölliker, 2019; McGlothlin et al., 2022; West‐Eberhard, 1979; Wolf et al., 1999). Such “social selection” (Bijma & Wade, 2008; McGlothlin et al., 2010; West‐Eberhard, 1979, 1983, 1984; Wolf et al., 1999) is expected to arise whenever traits act as both agents and targets of selection.
Our model assumptions are based on common conditions observed in male‐male contests (Eberhard et al., 2018; Emlen, 2008, 2014). Although we model the outcome of pairwise duels between males drawn at random from the population, our model is easily generalized to include multiple interactions between males (Supporting Appendix). First, we assume that males possess a trait (designated by the subscript S) that is used as a signal conveying potential threat in social contests. There are many diverse examples include traits such as plumage patches, exaggerated weapons, or vocal or chemical signals. Elaboration of the signal may consist of an increase in size or complexity, although for heuristic simplicity, we discuss the evolution of increased signal size. Second, we assume that males vary in the underlying quality trait that reflects their fighting ability or some other aspect of their phenotype that determines the potential interaction cost they represent to their opponent in a contest. We describe this trait as body size (designated by the subscript B) for simplicity (see the discussion of male quality in Eberhard et al., 2018). Finally, we assume males respond to the assessment of the signal by modulating their behavioral response of aggression toward their opponent (designated by the subscript A) within the contest because the signal provides information on the likelihood that they would win an escalated contest (see below).
We assume that both signal size () and body size () are normally distributed metric traits influenced by many loci of small effect. Expression of these traits can be partitioned into heritable additive genetic effects (denoted and ) and general non‐heritable (environmental and nonadditive genetic) effects (denoted and ). We assume that neither signal nor body size changes as a result of the social interaction. An individual's total phenotypic value for each trait is then described by a simple sum of the heritable and non‐heritable components:
(1) |
where is normally distributed with mean and variance and is normally distributed with mean and variance . We make the standard quantitative genetic assumption that heritable and non‐heritable components are uncorrelated.
We model male‐male competition where the larger males will defeat smaller males in a fight. We therefore further assume that the phenotypic value for aggressive behavior () associated with a given genotype depends on social context, influenced by their rival's signal size relative to their own, as suggested by West‐Eberhard (1979, 1983, 1984). This effect is captured in our model as a term in which aggression scales with the magnitude of the size difference between opponents, which is supported by optimality models and empirical studies (Emlen, 2008, 2014; Huxley, 1938; Maynard Smith & Harper, 2003; Parker, 1974; Riechert, 1984; Sneddon et al., 1997). The phenotypic value of aggression can thus be written:
(2) |
where and represent standard additive genetic and uncorrelated non‐heritable components, respectively. Here and elsewhere, terms with primes indicate a value assigned to the focal individual's opponent so is the phenotypic value of the signal of the focal individual and of their opponent. The coefficient measures the influence of the difference in signal size on the expression of aggressive behavior. Thus, is analogous to the term in standard interacting phenotype models (Moore, Brodie, et al., 1997), but differs because it depends upon the value of an interactant's phenotype relative to the focal individual. Because signal size is heritable, the phenotype value of aggression for the focal individual includes modifications arising from both direct genetic effects of their own genotype and indirect genetic effects ), which is defined as the effect of a social interactant's genotype on the focal individual's phenotype (Moore, Brodie, et al., 1997). We describe the relationship between this model and the standard model of indirect genetic effects in the Supporting Appendix.
The underlying genetic value of aggression is assumed to be genetically uncorrelated to both that of signal size and body size (i.e., there is no direct pleiotropic relationship between the traits such that genetic covariances ). This is a conservative assumption as a positive correlation would result in even faster evolution. However, the signal may be genetically correlated to body size, providing signal honesty, which is quantified by the covariance between signal size and body size (). Because the level of aggression displayed is conditional on the social context, a correlation within the population is generated if males with larger signals and/or larger body size are more aggressive on average (and vice versa). Hence, aggression can be correlated to the signal and body size traits despite the absence of a direct pleiotropic link (or linkage disequilibrium) because the flexible behavioral response creates a relationship between these traits through the social interaction.
Selection imposed by male‐male competition
In social interactions, associations between traits and fitness may cause selection via two pathways: nonsocial selection (quantified by the gradient , which arises from effects of a focal individual's traits on its own fitness, and social selection (quantified by the gradient , which arises from the effects of an opponent's traits on the fitness of a focal individual (Wolf et al., 1999). From Wolf et al. (1999), when both nonsocial and social selection are present, individual relative fitness can be written:
(3) |
where is an intercept, and z′ are column vectors of focal and opponent traits, and are vectors of nonsocial and social selection gradients, is an uncorrelated error term, and the superscript T denotes transposition. Expressing relative fitness using Equation 3 has two distinct advantages. First, selection gradients can be estimated in natural populations using multiple regression (Fisher & Pruitt, 2019; Formica et al., 2011; Lande & Arnold, 1983; Wolf et al., 1999), allowing our model to generate testable predictions. Second, selection gradients can be combined with genetic parameters to predict short‐term evolutionary response to selection (Bijma & Wade, 2008; Lande & Arnold, 1983; McGlothlin et al., 2010).
To understand how these selection gradients arise from male‐male contests with signaling, we can use evolutionary game theory (see Supporting Appendix) to write a mechanistic expression for relative fitness:
(4) |
where terms including b represent fitness benefits and terms including c represent fitness costs. In Equation 4, the benefit term and the first cost term derive from the hawk‐dove model of evolutionary game theory (Appendix; Maynard Smith, 1982; Maynard Smith & Price, 1973; McGlothlin et al., 2022). The coefficient is the fitness benefit of winning a contest, which we assume derives from greater access to females. In a contest, access to females is determined by a focal individual's aggression relative to its opponent. Multiplying by reflects the fact that the probability of winning a contest increases as a male can become increasingly more aggressive than its opponent. This benefit, however, does not come without a cost. The term is the fitness cost of aggression associated with escalation of encounters. Logically, an individual pays a cost for acting aggressive that depends on the level of aggression shown by its opponent. In the hawk‐dove model, this corresponds to the cost associated with a hawk player facing a hawk opponent, which increases in likelihood as both players act increasingly aggressive. A second fitness cost () reflects the fact that the fitness impact of aggression by an opponent () depends on the size of the opponent (). This cost, which we call the threat of the opponent, derives from the fact that larger males impose a greater risk of harm than do smaller males. Finally, we assume that a third cost () arises from natural selection favoring some optimal trait value (), which therefore will oppose signal elaboration. Following a Gaussian model of selection (Lande, 1976, 1979), selection against elaborate signals becomes stronger as the population mean of the signal () becomes further away from its naturally selected optimum (). Although we do not do so here, this term could be replaced with a multivariate Gaussian term (Lande, 1979) to add naturally selected optima for aggression and body size.
Taking partial derivatives of with respect to focal and opponent traits (evaluated at the population mean) allows us to translate the fitness model in Equation 4 into nonsocial and social selection gradients (McGlothlin et al., 2022; Supporting Appendix). The nonsocial gradients are:
(5a) |
(5b) |
(5c) |
and social selection gradients:
(6a) |
(6b) |
(6c) |
Thus, males with large signals are selected against via nonsocial selection (Equation 5a) but interacting with such males does not directly impose social selection (Equation 6a). Body size is not under direct nonsocial selection (Equation 5b) but imposes a fitness cost via social selection that increases with the population mean of aggression (Equation 6b). Nonsocial selection favors aggression until the benefits of aggression are outweighed by the costs (Equation 5c), while social selection imposed by opponent's aggression is always negative, representing a net fitness cost (Equation 6c). This gradient becomes increasingly negative as the population mean aggression and body size increase. These selection gradients suggest that signal size itself experiences no direct sexual selection. If signal size increases, it must do so as an indirect response to selection on a correlated trait.
Evolutionary response to selection
Selection within a generation is translated into an evolutionary response across generations through the association between the phenotype, upon which selection acts, and the genotype, which contributes to the inheritance of the traits across generations. In quantitative genetics, this genotype‐phenotype relationship is most often summarized by the additive genetic variance, which is used to predict evolutionary response to selection across generations (Arnold, 1994; Lande & Arnold, 1983). However, for traits expressed in social interactions, we must also consider social pathways to fitness, which arise from indirect genetic effects and social selection, when calculating response to selection (Bijma & Wade, 2008; McGlothlin et al., 2010; Moore, Brodie, et al., 1997). Because the model of phenotypic modification described in Equation 2 deviates from the standard model of indirect genetic effects, we develop a general equation for response to selection in the Supporting Appendix (Equation A10). Using this equation, the response to selection for the three traits in our model can in general be written:
(7a) |
(7b) |
(7c) |
The multiplier ½ in Equation 7 arises because selection is acting only on males. Equation 7a shows that modification of aggressive behavior in response to the signaling trait () causes both nonsocial and social selection gradients for aggression to contribute to signal evolution. This behavioral modification also contributes to evolution of body size when the signal is honest, which is captured by the covariance between signal size and body size (; Equation 7b). This is easily shown by setting the modification of aggression based on the signaling trait () to 0, which recovers standard quantitative genetic expressions for evolution. In contrast, behavioral modification never contributes to evolution of aggression (Equation 7c).
By substituting Equations 5–6 into Equation 7, we can predict evolutionary change using our mechanistic fitness model (Equation 4):
(8a) |
(8b) |
(8c) |
Equation 8a shows that when fitness is defined as in Equation 4, evolution of the signaling trait beyond its naturally selected optimum depends crucially on modification of aggression. If males do not change their aggression to the signal (i.e., if ), the population mean of the signaling trait cannot increase. From Equation 8b, the evolution of body size depends on both and the presence of signal honesty (i.e., ). Equations 8a,b also show that evolution of the signaling trait and of male body size is potentially open‐ended because the evolutionary response to selection for each trait becomes stronger as the population mean body size increases. In contrast, from Equation 8c, the evolution of aggression is self‐limiting because selection depends on the balance of the benefits and costs of aggression, the latter of which become more intense as mean aggression intensifies. This observation suggests that both signal size and body size may experience runaway evolution if the benefits of aggression and the threat of the opponent are strong enough to outweigh natural selection against elaborate signals, whereas aggression should always tend to quickly evolve to an equilibrium value.
To solve for equilibrium and to explore the conditions for such a runaway, we set Equations 8a–c equal to zero and solve for the equilibrium mean of each trait ():
(9a) |
(9b) |
(9c) |
As predicted, aggression will always reach a stable equilibrium whenever there is a cost of aggression (Equation 9c, Figure 1). Equations 9a,b predicts a line of equilibria for signal size and body size, because their evolutionary change is completely intertwined with the relationship (Figure 1). The slope of the line of equilibria predicting mean signal size from mean body size, and hence the evolutionary allometry of signal size, is . This relationship indicates that when comparing population means through time or across space, positive allometry (i.e., a slope greater than unity) is predicted when the strength of behavioral modification multiplied by the threat of the opponent () is greater than the strength of natural selection on signal size (). In general, when male behavior is more strongly dependent on the signal of their opponent (i.e., when is larger), more elaborate signals are expected at equilibrium (Figure 2).
FIGURE 1.
Evolution of a male signal, body size, and aggression in response to male‐male competition. Panels (a) and (b) show evolutionary trajectories for each trait over 200 generations, and panels (c) and (d) show predicted lines of equilibria (heavy line) and their stability (gray arrows). In all panels, all three traits have the same genetic variance (), benefit () and cost of aggression (), fitness cost deriving from the threat of a male's opponent (), cost of signal size (; with naturally selected optimum ), and a responsiveness of aggression to body size (). The line of equilibria is calculated from Equation 9a using these values. In panels (a) and (c), signal size is weakly correlated with body size (), while in panels (b) and (d), the two traits are more strongly correlated (). When the genetic correlation between signal size and body size is weak, all three traits reach equilibria (a), with equilibrium aggression predicted solely by costs and benefits. Signal size and body size reach a point on the predicted line of equilibrium (c) that differs depending on their starting values. When the genetic correlation is strong, aggression still reaches an equilibrium, but signal size and body size run away together (b), overshooting the predicted line of equilibria (d). As in Fisherian selection from female mate choice (Lande, 1981), male‐male competition can drive traits to runaway elaboration or extinction when the line of equilibria is unstable (d)
FIGURE 2.
Stronger dependence of male aggressive behavior leads to more elaborate traits at equilibrium. Panel (a) illustrates a relatively weak influence of opponent signal on male aggression (), while panel (b) illustrates a stronger influence (). In each panel, we use starting values for traits relevant to the highly sexually dimorphic earwig Labidura riparia, which uses its forceps as a signaling trait and is shown to the right of each panel (drawing modified from Lucas, 1920). Other parameters are the same as Figure 1a. When the influence of opponent signal is weak (a), both body size and signal show a moderate evolutionary increase. When the influence is stronger (b), both body size and signal increase more, but the final signal size is much larger relative to body size. The highly elaborate elongate forceps in panel (b) may be found in other earwig species like Forcipula gariazzi
Whether an evolving population will reach a predicted equilibrium (no runaway) or overshoot it (runaway) also depends on the rate of evolution of body size versus natural selection on signal size. Specifically, from Equation 8a, for runaway evolution of signal size, body size must evolve fast enough so that . Because and are constants, this occurs when , or equivalently:
(10) |
This result is also achievable by solving for the condition generating a negative eigenvalue of the Jacobian of , which indicates an unstable equilibrium (Bailey & Kölliker, 2019; Lande, 1981).
The first term in Equation 10 is the regression of body size on signal size, which is typically large for weapons and signals (Eberhard et al., 2018; McCullough et al., 2016). As a regression coefficient, this term measures the degree to which body size can be predicted from signal size, which captures the logic of why the term measures signal honesty. In addition, Equation 10 indicates that runaway evolution of a signal is most likely to occur when three conditions exist: the signal is honest ( is large and positive), it modifies aggressive behavior of social partners (), and aggression imposes a fitness cost that increases when opponents are larger (). Figure 1 illustrates a scenario in which the predicted outcome (equilibrium or runaway) depends upon the value of the genetic covariance .
DISCUSSION
Our model provides explicit conditions for sexual selection arising from male‐male competition to result in elaborate signals and runaway evolution. We model the origin of costs and benefits associated with male traits mediating male‐male interactions using considerations from evolutionary game theory, which allows us to derive expressions for natural and social selection gradients that reflect the mechanistic properties of male contests (Equations 5–6). We incorporate these expressions for selection into a model of trait genetics based on the interacting phenotypes framework, which accounts for the influence of indirect genetic effects arising from interactions with an opponent (Equation 2). Elaboration of a signal occurs whenever males adjust their level of aggression based on the signal of an opponent; i.e., (Equation 8a). This elaboration becomes runaway evolution when the signal is honest and when the cost imposed by aggression in an opponent increases with their body size (Equations 9a, 10; Figures 1, 2). In contrast, aggression always reaches an equilibrium, both because the fitness benefit of aggression is relative to that of the opponent and because of the fitness costs of escalated contests (Equation 9c). Limits to runaway evolution of the signaling trait depend on the strength of natural selection opposing signal elaboration, which may arise through costs of producing or bearing the signal.
Our model does not specify the nature of the costs and benefits associated with aggression, the signaling trait, and body size (condition). These are important variables, likely ecologically contingent, and empirical work that quantifies these costs and benefits will provide context for the generality of our model. However, one of the strengths of this quantitative genetic modeling approach is that it provides predictions that are testable in natural populations. Specifically, we expect the evolution of elaborate signaling traits that resolve duels between males to evolve when three conditions are present. First, signals should be reliable predictors of body size or some other proxy of fighting ability. Indeed, such signal honesty, which is often characterized as positive allometry (McCullough et al., 2016; O'Brien et al., 2018) or a positive genetic correlation between size and signal (Clark & Moore, 1995; Laidre & Johnstone, 2013; McGlothlin et al., 2005), is a common feature of traits involved in male‐male competition. Second, males must modify their behavior in response to their opponent's signal. We assume that males increase their aggression when encountering an opponent with a smaller signal than their own and reduce their aggression when encountering an opponent with a larger signal. Such adjustment is common in species that resolve contests via limited fights or displays (Darwin, 1871; Emlen, 2008, 2014; West‐Eberhard, 1979, 1983). In our model, this phenomenon alters the relationship between genotype and phenotype, causing a net force of social selection to contribute to signal evolution (Equations 7a, 8a). Finally, we expect social selection to be imposed via the aggression of opponents. This selection becomes stronger as male body size or fighting ability evolves due to the threat of escalation of fights with large opponents. Mean level of aggression need not change if the threat escalates. Our model makes specific predictions for the signs of these gradients when selection on signal size, body size, and aggression can all be measured (Equations 5–6). Most crucially, our model predicts negative social selection gradients for both body size and aggression, which reflect the costs of escalated contests. In populations that are experiencing an evolutionary runaway, these gradients should become stronger as body size and signal size coevolve. Although few studies have measured social selection gradients, the limited evidence that exists supports the existence of negative social selection gradients imposed by competitors (Fisher & Pruitt, 2019; Formica et al., 2011).
Parallels to Lande's model of female choice
The results of our model are conceptually analogous to Lande’s (1981) model of runaway sexual selection via female choice, suggesting some key parallels between the processes. Both our model and Lande's, which was the first formal model of Fisher's runaway process, result in lines of equilibria that may be stable or unstable depending on the genetic parameters. For the scenario of relative mate preference in Lande's model, the line of equilibria for a male trait () and a female preference () can be written:
(11) |
where is the naturally selected optimum, is the strength of sexual selection, and is the strength of natural selection. Equation 11 directly parallels Equation 9a from our model and emphasizes that in male‐male competition, the force of sexual selection is provided not by direct female choice but by male body size (or some other measure of willingness or ability to engage in aggression). In male‐male competition, the threat of the opponent () leads to social selection, which is indirectly translated into evolutionary change in male signals via the parameter , measuring the dependence of aggression on relative signal size of two competing males.
Similarly, the condition for runaway evolution of male traits and female preference driven by mate choice in Lande's model can be written:
(12) |
where represents the genetic covariance between male trait and female preference and represents genetic variance of the male trait. The condition in Equation 12 directly parallels the condition in Equation 10, emphasizing again that in male‐male competition, provides the force of social selection that indirectly leads to an evolutionary increase in male signal size. Both types of runaway evolution are driven by genetic covariance. In mate choice, runaway is driven by the covariance between the sexes that arises from choosy females mating with attractive males, but in male‐male competition, this effect arises directly from signal honesty, i.e., the genetic covariance between a signaling trait and the threat (willingness or ability to fight) it signals. Moreover, if the mean level of aggression does not change, as when the aggression plateau is reached (Figures 1, 2), increasing costs during male‐male competition are associated only with the increasingly elaborated signal. This may occur when limited fights settle contests (Maynard Smith & Harper, 1988, 2003). These are common conditions (Andersson, 1994; Maynard Smith & Harper, 1988; Parker, 1974; West‐Eberhard, 1983, 1984), suggesting that runaway from male‐male competition may occur frequently (McCullough et al., 2016; Rico‐Guevara & Hurme, 2019). Finally, the genetic covariance in Lande's model arises from linkage disequilibrium that accumulates via nonrandom mating whereas ours reflects pleiotropy between body size and signal size. Thus, the genetic covariance driving runaway from male‐male competition is likely to be much larger both because recombination efficiently erodes linkage disequilibrium and positive allometry (e.g., signals and body size) is common and reflects pleiotropy. Indeed, in models of mate choice where there is a pleiotropic relationship between direct benefits or good genes, runaway is difficult. In our model, however, the pleotropic nature of the honest signal leads to runaway.
There are many other models of mate choice in the literature, and a full comparison to them all is beyond the scope of this paper. Our goal here is simply to highlight potentially common features of runaway evolution, the most important is that runaway sexual selection by both male‐male competition and female mate choice appears to be an evolving source of selection provided by the social environment. A more expansive comparison may well stimulate modifications or additions to the model we present here. In addition, there would be much to gain by combining studies of female mate choice and male‐male competition to simultaneously test models of sexual selection (Hunt et al., 2009). This may be especially enlightening when traits serve as both ornaments and armaments (Berglund et al., 1996) or when mate choice opposes male‐male competition (Moore & Moore, 1999).
CONCLUSION
Ritualized displays and elaborated signals associated with the potential for aggression are readily observed in nature and their importance often obvious and spectacular (Darwin, 1871; Emlen, 2008, 2014; Maynard Smith & Harper, 1988, 2003; Parker, 1974). Yet the details of how these might evolve have been unclear. Previous game theory models have shown that overt aggression can be ameliorated by conventional signals (Maynard Smith, 1982; Maynard Smith & Harper, 1988, 2003; Parker, 1974; Rutte et al., 2006), and verbal models have proposed that signaling traits associated with male‐male competition evolve exaggerated expression because social selection is intense (West‐Eberhard, 1979, 1983, 1984). Male‐male competition may well result in intense selection (Maynard Smith & Brown, 1986), as mating can be highly skewed toward one or a few males in a population (Andersson, 1994; Darwin, 1871; Shuster & Wade, 2003), but this alone is insufficient to result in exaggerated traits. Our model shows that feedback between the behavioral and morphological traits mediating male‐male competition create runaway evolution.
Sexual selection arising from male‐male competition is prevalent and so the consequences of such selection important for understanding the generation of biological diversity. We hope our model stimulates empiricists in much the same way that the model of mate choice stimulated research on mating preferences. Ultimately, our understanding of the consequences of sexual selection arising from male‐male competition will come from empirical research. Our hope is that this model helps direct and focus some of that research.
AUTHORS CONTRIBUTIONS
AJM, JWM, and JBW conceived the idea, developed the model, and wrote the paper.
PEER REVIEW
The peer review history for this article is available at https://publons.com/publon/10.1111/ele.13921.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
We thank our collaborators, students, and postdoctoral scientists who over the years provided discussions and comments on various earlier versions of this idea and model. This work was supported by grants from NERC (UK, NE/C510659/1 to JBW) and NSF (USA, IOS‐1354358 to AJM, DEB‐1457463 to JWM).
FITNESS MODEL FOR MALE‐MALE INTERACTIONS
To model the fitness consequences of an interaction between two males, we use a modification of the classic hawk‐dove game, which involves competition over a resource (Maynard Smith & Price 1973, Maynard Smith 1982). In a traditional hawk‐dove game, there are two strategies. Hawks tend to start fights, while doves tend to flee. When two hawks meet, a fight determines the outcome of the contest, with one hawk winning and receiving a resource of value , while the other hawk pays a cost , which may be related to injury or other costs of aggression. The average payoff for a hawk in a hawk‐hawk encounter is thus . When two doves meet, they either divide the resource evenly or decide the contest without aggression and its associated costs, leading to an average payoff for each dove of . When a hawk meets a dove, the hawk wins the contest, receiving a fitness payoff of , while the dove receives a payoff of 0, reflecting the fact that the dove neither wins the resource nor pays a cost for being aggressive. If represents playing hawk and represents playing dove, this fitness model can be written in terms of the phenotypes of two interactants expressing traits and z′:
(A1) |
Following McGlothlin et al. (2022), we generalize this equation to a continuous scale, allowing , representing aggression, to take on any non‐negative value. This means that individuals that are more aggressive (i.e., that employ a more hawkish strategy) will tend to win interactions and that the fitness cost of being aggressive will increase as both individuals become more aggressive (i.e., more hawkish). The fitness model in Equation 4 differs from Equation A1 by being expressed in terms of relative fitness ( ) rather than absolute fitness ( ). Thus, the translations between the parameters in the general hawk‐dove fitness model and the fitness effects of aggression in Equation (4) are
(A2a) |
(A2b) |
(A2c) |
In words, represents access to resources (here, mates) achieved by winning a contest, while represents the costs paid by the loser of a contest when both individuals are aggressive. We also include two additional unique costs. We assume that the cost of an encounter is proportional to the body size (or fighting ability) of an opponent, resulting in a cost proportional to . Importantly, this cost is paid regardless of an individual’s own behavior. Second, bearing a signal larger than the naturally selected optimum imposes a cost proportional to , which is presumably paid outside the context of male‐male encounters. Nonsocial and social selection gradients may be calculated from fitness functions such as Equation A1 and Equation 4 by taking the partial derivatives of relative fitness with respect to focal and social phenotypes, respectively (McGlothlin et al., 2022). These derivatives are then evaluated at the population mean. For linear fitness functions, selection gradients will be constant, but quadratic and higher‐order functions may lead to selection gradients that change with the population mean.
GENERAL EQUATION FOR RESPONSE TO SELECTION
Here, we develop a general model for evolution when trait expression depends upon the difference between a focal individual’s own traits and traits of another individual encountered in the context of a social interaction. This model is directly applied to male‐male contests in the main text and may be useful for considering many other types of social interactions. First, consider a vector of traits () whose expression can be decomposed into three components: a vector of additive genetic effects (), a vector of environmental effects (), and a social response term that depends on the difference between the traits of the focal individual and those of a social interactant (z′):
(A3) |
The matrix consists of components that translate the effect of differences in trait j into expression of trait i. Similarly, the phenotype of the social partner can be written as
(A4) |
As we show below, because the term in Equations (A3), (A4) contains phenotypes of both individuals, it consists of a combination of direct and indirect genetic effects.
To calculate a response to selection for traits expressed as in Equation A3, we first solve for the multivariate phenotypic mean. Assuming that environmental effects have a mean of zero, the trait mean is
(A5) |
which means that the population trait mean will depend only on the mean additive genetic value. The vector of total breeding values ( ), which represents the genetic contribution to the population mean and is used to calculate evolutionary responses to selection, is equivalent to the vector of additive genetic effects ( ). Next, to derive an explicit definition of the phenotype, we first use substitution to write
(A6) |
where is the identity matrix. After some algebra, Equation A6 allows us to write explicit definitions of the two phenotypes as
(A7) |
and
(A8) |
Response to selection can then be calculated following (McGlothlin et al., 2010) as
(A9) |
Substituting (6b), (6c) into Equation A9 and simplifying yields
(A10) |
where is the additive genetic (co)variance matrix and is relatedness. Equation A10 can be used to generate the specific model in the text by setting parameters as
(A11) |
and multiplying the result by to indicate selection acting only on males.
RELATIONSHIP TO INDIRECT GENETIC EFFECTS MODELS
The model described above differs from previous models of indirect genetic effects in that phenotypes may be adjusted in relation to both the phenotypes of a social partner and other phenotypes of the focal individual. The standard model of phenotypic expression used in trait‐based genetic effects models (Moore, Brodie, et al., 1997) is:
(A12) |
The relationship between the two models can be seen by adding an additional term to the standard model:
(A13) |
The term , which is similar to developmental interaction effects (Wolf et al. 2001), contains a conditional modification of the direct genetic effect in response to other phenotypic traits of the same individual, while the term contains indirect genetic effects (Moore, Brodie, et al., 1997). When , Equation 13 is equivalent to Equation A12, and when , Equation A13 is equivalent to Equation A3.
Incorporating conditional direct genetic effects is a way to mechanistically represent genetic covariances in a quantitative genetic model (Wolf et al. 2001). Combining such effects with indirect genetic effects allows exploration of a wide variety of models of phenotypic adjustment, including behavioral modification, in evolutionary quantitative genetic models. For full generality, we give the equation for multivariate response to selection derived from Equation A13. First, the vector of total breeding values derived from the trait mean is
(A14) |
By substituting Equations (A13), (A14) into Equation A9,
(A15) |
This equation is unwieldy in its multivariate form, but one can follow the approach we take here for male‐male competition and use Equation A15 to generate much simpler models of the evolution of systems of responsive traits given assumptions about fitness and trait expression.
EXTENSION TO MULTIPLE OPPONENTS
Our results can be extended to incorporate interactions with multiple opponents. Suppose that a male encounters opponents sequentially. The phenotype of a focal individual averaged over all encounters:
(A16) |
and the average of his social partners:
(A17) |
where an overbar is now taken to indicate an average over an individual male’s encounters. If we assume that the fitness effects of individual encounters accrue additively, then Equation A10 may still be used to predict the evolution of the population mean, with and now representing vectors of partial regression slopes of fitness on focal individual mean and social group mean phenotypes, respectively. Specific fitness models may be substituted into Equation A10 in the same way as for models of single pairwise interactions.
Moore, A.J. , McGlothlin, J.W. & Wolf, J.B. (2022) Runaway evolution from male‐male competition. Ecology Letters, 25, 295–306. 10.1111/ele.13921
DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENT
There are no data associated with this manuscript.
REFERENCES
- Andersson, M. (1982) Female choice selects for extreme tail length in a widowbird. Nature, 299, 818–820. [Google Scholar]
- Andersson, M. (1994) Sexual selection. Princeton: Princeton Univ. Press. [Google Scholar]
- Araya‐Ajoy, Y.G. , Westneat, D.F. & Wright, J. (2020) Pathways to social evolution and their evolutionary feedbacks. Evolution, 74, 1894–1907. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arnold, S.J. (1994) Multivariate inheritance and evolution: a review of concepts. In: Boake, C.R. (Ed.) Quantitative genetic studies of behavioral evolution. Chicago, IL: Univ. of Chicago Press Chicago, pp. 17–48. [Google Scholar]
- Bailey, N.W. & Kölliker, M. (2019) Social runaway: fisherian elaboration (or reduction) of socially selected traits via indirect genetic effects. Evolution, 73, 1549–1563. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bailey, N.W. & Moore, A.J. (2012) Runaway sexual selection without genetic correlations: social environments and flexible mate choice initiate and enhance the Fisher process. Evolution, 66, 2674–2684. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berglund, A. , Bisazza, A. & Pilastro, A. (1996) Armaments and ornaments: an evolutionary explanation of traits of dual utility. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 58, 385–399. [Google Scholar]
- Bijma, P. & Wade, M.J. (2008) The joint effects of kin, multilevel selection and indirect genetic effects on response to genetic selection. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 21, 1175–1188. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Briceño, R.D. & Eberhard, W.G. (1995) The functional morphology of male cerci and associated characters in 13 species of tropical earwigs (Dermaptera: Forficulidae, Labiidae, Carcinophoridae, Pygidicranidae). Washington, DC: Smithsonian Institution Press. [Google Scholar]
- Clark, D.C. & Moore, A.J. (1995) Genetic aspects of communication during male‐male competition in the Madagascar hissing cockroach: honest signaling of size. Heredity, 75, 198–205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Darwin, C. (1859) The origin of species. London: John Murray. [Google Scholar]
- Darwin, C. (1871) The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex. London: John Murray. [Google Scholar]
- Eberhard, W.G. , Rodríguez, R.L. , Huber, B.A. , Speck, B. , Miller, H. , Buzatto, B.A. et al. (2018) Sexual selection and static allometry: the importance of function. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 93, 207–250. [Google Scholar]
- Emlen, D.J. (2008) The evolution of animal weapons. Annual Review of Ecology Evolution and Systematics, 39, 387–413. [Google Scholar]
- Emlen, D.J. (2014) Animal weapons: the evolution of battle. New York: Henry Holt & Co. [Google Scholar]
- Emlen, D.J. , Marangelo, J. , Ball, B. & Cunningham, C.W. (2005) Diversity in the weapons of sexual selection: horn evolution in the beetle genus Onthophagus (Coleoptera : Scarabaeidae). Evolution, 59, 1060–1084. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fisher, D.N. & Pruitt, J.N. (2019) Opposite responses to selection and where to find them. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 32, 505–518. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fisher, R.A. (1915) The evolution of sexual preference. Eugen Rev, 7, 184–192. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fisher, R.A. (1930) The genetical theory of natural selection. Oxford: Clarendon Press. [Google Scholar]
- Formica, V.A. , McGlothlin, J.W. , Wood, C.W. , Augat, M.E. , Butterfield, R.E. , Barnard, M.E. et al. (2011) Phenotypic assortment mediates the effect of social selection in a wild beetle population. Evolution, 65, 2771–2781. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hagelin, J.C. (2002) The kinds of traits involved in male‐male competition: a comparison of plumage, behavior, and body size in quail. Behavioral Ecology, 13, 32–41. [Google Scholar]
- Hunt, J. , Breuker, C.J. , Sadowski, J.A. & Moore, A.J. (2009) Male‐male competition, female mate choice and their interaction: determining total sexual selection. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 22, 13–26. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huxley, J.S. (1938) Darwin's theory of sexual selection and the data subsumed by it, in the light of recent research. American Naturalist, 72, 416–433. [Google Scholar]
- Kirkpatrick, M. (1982) Sexual selection and the evolution of female mate choice. Evolution, 36, 1–12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laidre, M.E. & Johnstone, R.A. (2013) Animal signals. Current Biology, 23, R829–R833. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lande, R. (1976) Natural selection and random genetic drift in phenotypic evolution. Evolution, 30, 314–334. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lande, R. (1979) Quantitative genetic analysis of multivariate evolution, applied to brain: body size allometry. Evolution, 33, 402–416. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lande, R. (1981) Models of speciation by sexual selection on polygenic traits. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 78, 3721–3725. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lande, R. & Arnold, S.J. (1983) The measurement of selection on correlated characters. Evolution, 37, 1210–1226. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lucas, W.J. (1920) A Monograph of the British Orthoptera. London: The Ray Society. [Google Scholar]
- Maynard Smith, J. (1982) Evolution and the theory of games. Cambridge: Cambridge Univ. Press. [Google Scholar]
- Maynard Smith, J. & Brown, R.L.W. (1986) Competition and body size. Theoretical Population Biology, 30, 166–179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maynard Smith, J. & Harper, D.G.C. (1988) The evolution of aggression: can selection generate variability? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 319, 557–570. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maynard Smith, J. & Harper, D. (2003) Animal signals. Oxford: Oxford Univ. Press. [Google Scholar]
- Maynard Smith, J. & Price, G.R. (1973) The logic of animal conflict. Nature, 246, 15–18. [Google Scholar]
- McCullough, E.L. , Miller, C.W. & Emlen, D.J. (2016) Why sexually selected weapons are not ornaments. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 31, 742–751. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGlothlin, J.W. , Akçay, E. , Brodie, E.D. III , Moore, A.J. & Van Cleve, J. (2022) A synthesis of evolutionary game theory and quantitative genetic models of social evolution. Journal of Heredity, 10.1101/2021.03.27.437341. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGlothlin, J.W. , Moore, A.J. , Wolf, J.B. & Brodie, E.D. III . (2010) Interacting phenotypes and the evolutionary process. III. social evolution. Evolution, 64, 2558–2574. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGlothlin, J.W. , Parker, P.G. , Nolan, V. Jr & Ketterson, E.D. (2005) Correlational selection leads to genetic integration of body size and an attractive plumage trait in dark‐eyed juncos. Evolution, 59, 658–671. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mead, L.S. & Arnold, S.J. (2004) Quantitative genetic models of sexual selection. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 19, 264–271. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moore, A.J. , Brodie, E.D. III & Wolf, J.B. (1997) Interacting phenotypes and the evolutionary process: I Direct and Indirect Genetic Effects of Social Interactions. Evolution, 51, 1352–1362. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moore, A.J. , Haynes, K.F. , Preziosi, R.F. & Moore, P.J. (2002) The evolution of interacting phenotypes: genetics and evolution of social dominance. American Naturalist, 160, S186–S197. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moore, A.J. & Moore, P.J. (1999) Balancing sexual selection through opposing mate choice and male competition. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences, 266, 711–716. [Google Scholar]
- Moore, A.J. & Moore, P.J. (2006) The genetics of sexual selection. In: Fox, C.W. & Wolf, J.B. (Eds.) Evolutionary Genetics. Oxford: Oxford Univ. Press, pp. 339–349. [Google Scholar]
- Moore, P.J. , Reagan‐Wallin, N.L. , Haynes, K.F. & Moore, A.J. (1997) Odour conveys status on cockroaches. Nature, 389, 25.9288961 [Google Scholar]
- O'Brien, D.M. , Allen, C.E. , Van Kleeck, M.J. , Hone, D. , Knell, R. , Knapp, A. et al. (2018) On the evolution of extreme structures: static scaling and the function of sexually selected signals. Animal Behavior, 144, 95–108. [Google Scholar]
- Parker, G.A. (1974) Assessment strategy and the evolution of fighting behaviour. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 47, 223–243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rico‐Guevara, A. & Hurme, K.J. (2019) Intrasexually selected weapons. Biological Reviews, 94, 60–101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Riechert, S.E. (1984) Games spiders play. III. Cues underlying context‐associated changes in agonistic behavior. Animal Behavior, 32, 1–15. [Google Scholar]
- Rosenthal, G.G. (2017) Mate choice: the evolution of sexual decision making from microbes to humans. Princeton: Princeton Univ. Press. [Google Scholar]
- Rutte, C. , Taborsky, M. & Brinkhof, M.W.G. (2006) What sets the odds of winning and losing? Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 21, 16–21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seehausen, O. & Schluter, D. (2004) Male‐male competition and nuptial‐colour displacement as a diversifying force in Lake Victoria cichlid fishes. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences, 271, 1345–1353. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shuster, S.M. & Wade, M.J. (2003) Mating systems and strategies. Princeton: Princeton Univ. Press. [Google Scholar]
- Sneddon, L.U. , Huntingford, F.A. & Taylor, A.C. (1997) Weapon size versus body size as a predictor of winning in fights between shore crabs, Carcinus maenas (L.). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 41, 237–242. [Google Scholar]
- Tinghitella, R.M. , Lackey, A.C.R. , Martin, M. , Dijkstra, P.D. , Drury, J.P. , Heathcote, R. et al. (2018) On the role of male competition in speciation: a review and research agenda. Behavioral Ecology, 29, 783–797. [Google Scholar]
- Wallace, A.R. (1889) Darwinism. New York: Macmillan and Co. [Google Scholar]
- West‐Eberhard, M.J. (1979) Sexual selection, social competition, and evolution. Proceedings of the American Philosophical Society, 123, 222–234. [Google Scholar]
- West‐Eberhard, M.J. (1983) Sexual selection, social competition, and speciation. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 58, 155–183. [Google Scholar]
- West‐Eberhard, M.J. (1984) Sexual selection, competitive communication and species‐specific signals. In: Lewis, T. (Ed.) Insect communication. New York: Academic Press, pp. 283–324. [Google Scholar]
- West‐Eberhard, M.J. (2003) Developmental plasticity and evolution. Oxford: Oxford Univ. Press. [Google Scholar]
- Wiens, J.J. & Tuschhoff, E. (2020) Songs versus colours versus horns: what explains the diversity of sexually selected traits? Biological Reviews, 95, 847–864. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilkinson, G.S. & Dodson, G.N. (1997) Function and evolution of antlers and eye stalks in flies. In: Choe, J.C. & Crespi, B.J. (Eds.) The evolution of mating systems in insects and arachnids. Cambridge: Cambridge Univ. Press, pp. 310–327. [Google Scholar]
- Wolf, J.B. (2003) Genetic architecture and evolutionary constraint when the environment contains genes. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 100, 4655–4660. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolf, J.B. , Brodie, E.D. III & Moore, A.J. (1999) Interacting phenotypes and the evolutionary process. II. Selection resulting from social interactions. American Naturalist, 153, 254–266. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolf, J.B. , Frankino, W.A. , Agrawal, A.F. , Brodie, E.D. III & Moore, A.J. (2001) Developmental interactions and the constituents of quantitative variation. Evolution, 55, 232–245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
There are no data associated with this manuscript.