Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2022 Jul 21.
Published in final edited form as: Soc Networks. 2019 Sep 24;61:78–86. doi: 10.1016/j.socnet.2019.08.008

Who is Dropped and Why? Methodological and Substantive Accounts for Network Loss

Claude S Fischer 1, Shira Offer 2
PMCID: PMC9302580  NIHMSID: NIHMS1816099  PMID: 35875821

Abstract

High rates of egocentric network turnover are frequently observed but not well explained. About 1,000 respondents to the UCNets survey named an average of 10 names in each of two waves a year apart. Consistent with prior studies, respondents in wave 2 failed to relist about half of the names they provided in wave 1. Asked why, respondents explained that they had forgotten the alter for about 40 percent of the missing names. Other common answers, such as no “occasion... to be in touch,” also suggest that the true rate of alters being dropped is probably under 20 percent. Multilevel logit models identified the predictors of alters being dropped (neither relisted nor forgotten) versus retained. Immediate kin were likeliest to be retained and roommates, coworkers, and acquaintances to be dropped. Alters who provided companionship, confiding, advice, and emergency help were especially likely to be retained, as were those to whom respondents felt close. Little about the respondents themselves affected drop rates: having moved recently, having a close friend die, or having had an important relationship break up. Results are consistent with the argument that a tie’s degree of constraint (notably being close family) and its balance of rewards determine the likelihood of it being dropped or demoted.


Panel studies of egocentric networks commonly report a paradox: Respondents report much turnover among their alters–typically, about half of those named are not renamed–and yet respondents’ repopulated networks display notable constancy in traits such as size, composition, supportiveness, and structure (see review below). Our purpose here is to expand our understanding of this phenomena by focusing on the first part of this process: whom respondents drop. We use two waves of UCNets, a survey that administered many name-eliciting questions to about 1,000 adult respondents. Methodologically, we found that (a) many apparently dropped alters were not really dropped, but were simply forgotten; (b) many alters whom respondents failed to re-list nonetheless remained “dormant” and available to the respondents; and (c) respondents positively dropped or demoted alters for several distinct reasons. Substantively, we found that (a) alters who were really dropped--neither re-listed nor simply forgotten--tended to be: extended kin, coworkers, or acquaintances; alters who had not been named as providing key support; and alters who were considered “difficult” (unless they were immediate kin). (b) Respondents who dropped many of their alters were particularly likely to have moved outside the region between waves.

These findings suggest, methodologically, that the “true” rate of dropped alters in an adult population is nearer to 20 percent than the conventional 50 percent (depending on how the analyst classifies dropped alters who seemed to have been moved toward the periphery of ego’s network). Theoretically, the findings suggest that the likelihood of being dropped is a function, first, of context and changes in context. Most sharply, immediate kin are hard to drop—or to forget. And being dropped is a function, second, of the balance of support and burden that the relationship entails.

Framework

Perry, et al. (2018, 251) underline a “foundational issue” in studying egocentric network dynamics: “the ability to distinguish real network change from reporting error.” So, the first task is to address matters of measurement before addressing matters of substance.

Measurement.

At the level of specific ties, we propose that two general sources of error generate “false” dropped alters in repeated name-eliciting surveys. One source is the usual noise of survey research, such as respondent distraction, satisficing, self-presentation, and fatigue, as well as recency and interviewer effects. The more interesting other source is variation in respondents’ mental sampling. That is, when asked to name alters who meet a general category–such as “five best friends” or “people you could ask for advice”–respondents do not, indeed cannot, provide a complete inventory (unless the probe is very bounded, such as “List your brothers”). Respondents instead draw some names out of the relevant population on the spur of the moment. This non-random1 sampling process would generate apparent “churn” from t1 to t2, but it would also generate apparent constancy because each sampling, if presumably elicited the same way, would on average represent, with similar distortion, the same underlying population of alters. Both sources of error imply that networks have substantially less real churn than apparent, because there are qualifying alters unnamed at either t1 or t2 who are in the network but are overlooked at one time or the other.

Other methodological issues also affect the probability that a t1 alter’s absence at t2 is accurate: the instrument--how exhaustive the set of name-eliciting questions are, how many names it accepts, and how probing the interview is (Brewer 2000; Fischer and Bayham, 2019); the inclusion of what may have be fleeting ties (Desmond 2012; Small, 2017; Torres 2019);2 selective attrition in the respondent sample; and possible panel conditioning (Silber et al., 2019). But the process respondents use to, in effect, “sample” the alters in their networks seems fundamental to the process and implies that real culling of alters–and thus the real level of “churn”--may be significantly less common than existing results imply.

Alter and Tie Differences.

Beyond measurement error and sampling variation, respondents fail to re-list some alters for substantive reasons. Those reasons vary in their implications for understanding the network. Whether a t1 alter goes unnamed at t2 because, say, the respondent has not seen her in a while is different than if she goes unnamed because of a bitter falling out. In the former case, the alter is most likely still available–a “dormant” tie (Marin and Hampton, 2019)–and in the latter, not. Thus, the bright line between being in or out of ego’s network is better re-imagined as a broad gray area where alters remain with some degree of connection.

Certain types of alters and certain types of relationships may be prone to being dropped totally or being demoted into the gray zone. By alter types, we refer to their ages, genders, occupations, and the like. More often, however, analysis of turnover has focused on the qualities of the relationships: kinship, distance, duration, homophily, contact frequency, embeddedness, and the like. Two theoretical approaches to understanding these network dynamics inform our expectations.

One approach stresses the openness or constraint of the social contexts or “foci” within which relationships operate (Bidart et al., 2011; Blau and Schwartz 1984; Feld, 1982; Fischer 1982; Lazarsfeld and Merton 1954; Mollenhorst, et al., 2008, 2014). Contexts can constrain ties materially as in the case of workplaces, schools, and co-housing, and can constrain them normatively, as in the case of immediate family and old friendship circles. Other things be equal, relationships in more constraining contexts, such as immediate family and workplace, should dissolve less often than others.

Another approach stresses that individuals strategically build and maintain networks. This is explicit in, for example, exchange theory (Homans 1950; Thibaut and Kelly 1959; Blau, 1964) and “social capital” theory (Bourdieu, 1984; Coleman, 1990). Such an agentic approach suggests that alters who support ego should be less likely and alters who burden ego should be more likely to be purposefully dropped.

This strategic approach is also consistent with a different, substantive, rather than methodological, interpretation for why the profiles of individuals’ networks tend to remain stable even as membership in their networks “churns.” The process is homeostatic: Individuals have distinct and stable preferences for networks—“signatures” is one term (Heydari, et al., 2018; Saramäki, et al., 2013) for this—and upon dropping or being dropped by alters, they form new ties so as to maintain the general character of their ego networks. “Continuity theory” in gerontology (Atchley, 1989; Badawy, et al., 2018; Cornwell, 2014) similarly stresses individuals’ efforts to sustain or reconstruct familiar social connections.

Both contextual and strategic dynamics probably affect dropping and may interact, such that relatively unrewarding ties are nonetheless likely to be retained if they operate in contexts that are constraining (such as the immediate family).

Ego and Network Traits.

Finally, at a higher level, we can ask what sorts of egos with what sorts of networks are likelier to drop alters, holding alter and tie traits constant. Approaches emphasizing contexts or foci suggest that life events that change ego’s contexts–such as divorce, job starts, graduation, disability, and, in particular, moving (because it entails multiple context changes)–would increase the odds of alters being dropped. Network attributes that might affect retention or dropping include network size–presumably, egos with large networks have more alters to drop and less need to retain them–and also density–under the assumption that triadic connections make it harder to let alters go.

Overview.

In the remainder of this paper, we (1) review the research on network loss; (2) explain our data; (3) describe how many and what kinds of wave 1 alters respondents failed to re-list in wave 2 and (4) examine how respondents explained why they did not rename those alters. This analysis leads to the conclusion that purposeful drops are much less common than is apparent in the literature. (5) Further using respondents’ explanations for the drops, we examine those deliberately unlisted alters, describing the roles that some of them still played in respondents’ social lives or the nature of their “dormancy.” (6) Using multivariate analyses, we determine what sorts of alters at wave 1 were likely to have been dropped at wave 2 and what sorts of respondents tended to drop alters for which reasons. (7) Discussion and conclusion.

What We Know About Dropping Ties

Researchers using some version of name-eliciting methods (Laumann, 1965; Wellman, 1979; Fischer 1982; Marsden, 1987; and many others) have generally found that there is much “churn” in network membership, but that the overall content of egos’ networks–say, overall size, availability of support, or kin composition–remains stable. For examples: Wellman, et al’s (1997) East Yorkers failed to rename two-thirds of their ties; van Duijin, et al’s (1999) Dutch older sample failed to rename one-third; NSHAP’s older Americans dropped about 43 percent of their few confidants (National Social Life, Health, and Aging Project (Badawy, et al., 2018; Cornwell, 2014; Cornwell et al., 2014); and Marin and Hampton’s (2019) Boston respondents failed to rename about half of their alters. Burt (2000) and Suitor et al. (1997) summarized the studies up to about 20 years ago by estimating that one-third to two-thirds of nominated ties are dropped within a single year. Yet, Morgan et al. (1997) concluded that, except perhaps for most elderly (Lang 2000; Bowling and Farquhar, 1995), “the stability of the aggregate properties in personal networks is much greater than the stability of the membership in these networks” (see also Bidart et al., 2011; Bignami-Van Assche, 2005; Cornwell and Laumann, 2018; Lubbers 2010; Marin and Hampton 2019; Mollenhorst, et al., 2014; Saramäki 2013; Schwartz and Litwin, 2017; Vriens and Inge, 2017). Such alter-level churn plus network-level stability is, as noted earlier, consistent with a homoeostatic or personality-driven model of network formation: people seek alters like the alters they lost so as to sustain the kinds of networks they prefer. This pattern is, however, also consistent with our suggestion that respondents engage in “alter sampling” at each wave and in that way also maintain continuity in their networks, perhaps unconsciously.

Which alters are retained or dropped?

Many studies find that alters who were more central to egos’ lives–as indicated by attributes such as being immediate kin, embeddedness, years known, role multiplexity, and some kinds of homophily –are less likely than others to be dropped between waves of interviews (e.g., Bidart, et al., 2011; Burt, 2000; Cornwell et al., 2014; Cornwell and Laumann, 2018; Degenne and Lebeaux, 2005; Lang, 2000; Lubbers et al., 2010; Marin and Hampton, 2019; O’Malley and Christakis, 2011; Schafer and Vargas, 2016; Suitor, et al., 1997; van Duijin et al., 1999; van Tilburg 1998). Similarly, alters’ more active involvement with ego predicts persistence. The greater the volume and variety of material and emotional support, frequency of interaction, emotional closeness, and the lower the level of conflict, the higher the chances of an alter being retained (see, for example, in addition to many of the studies just cited, Bello and Rolfe, 2013; Ikkink and van Tilburg, 1999; Saramaki et al. 2013). We know much less about how alters’ personal traits affect retention, although a couple of studies found that higher-status alters were less likely to be dropped (Burt, 2000; Suitor and Keeton, 1997). These findings are consistent with the general expectations we noted that both the context, particularly its degree of constraint, and the net value of a tie determines its chances of being dropped.

Which egos are likeliest to keep ties or drop ties?

Research suggests that between-ego differences are much less significant contributors to patterns of alter loss than are between-tie differences (e.g., van Duijin et al. 1999; Marin and Hampton, 2019), but a few attributes do stand out. People undergoing life transitions–marriage, widowhood, graduation, moving, retirement and nearing the end of life–report more drops and more churn generally (e.g., Badawy, et al., 2018; Bidart and Cacciuttolo, 2013; Bidart and Lavenu, 2005; Bidart, et al., 2011; Bloem, et al., 2008; Carstensen, et al., 1999; Cornwell et al., 2014; Lang, 2000; Mollenhorst, 2014; Schafer and Vargas, 2016; Small, et al., 2015; van Dujian, et al., 1999; Wellman et al., 1997). Thus, both young and very elderly adults tend to have high drop rates. People with lower social standing appear to lose more ties (e.g., Cornwell 2014; Fischer and Beresford, 2015; Goldman and Cornwell 2018; van Dujian et al. 1999; van Tilburg 1998). The findings on ego’s health are revealing. Illness can lead to more ties or more active ones because health crises mobilize dormant relationships and even generate new ones for a while (e.g., Badawy, 2018; Perry and Pescosolido, 2012; van Tilburg and van Groenou, 2002; vs. Cornwell, 2014), even if in the long term more peripheral ties are lost (as one reviewer noted). Finally, personality plays a role, particularly as extroverts are especially likely to get and keep ties (Asendorpf and Wipers, 1998; Mund, et al., 2018; Sasovova et al., 2010; Zhu et al., 2013).

People with larger networks seem to experience more drops, perhaps because large networks require more maintenance, or because losing any one tie matters less, or perhaps simply because of regression to the mean (van Tilburg, 1992). Dense networks and networks with many kin seem to retain ties more often (Lubbers et al., 2010; Martin and Yeung, 2006; Schafer and Vargas, 2016; versus Marin and Hampton, 2013), which is understandable in terms of constraining contexts.

Liminal Ties.

Rarely does the literature address a key question preliminary to that of who gets dropped by whom: In what sense are alters who are not renamed actually dropped from respondents’ networks? Some studies show that many alters who are not re-listed nonetheless remain somehow connected or available. In Mollenhorst’s (2014) large Dutch sample, 60 percent of alters who were not mentioned after a seven-year gap were nonetheless still in touch with ego. Yet others may not be in touch but remain “dormant” (Marin and Hampton, 2019), “latent” (Perry, et al., 2018, 249), or “weak” (Grannoveter, 1973) ties. It is important to establish a more nuanced understanding of what being unnamed at t2 means. Wave 2 of the current survey asked respondents to explain why they had not renamed an alter from wave 1.

One answer is “I forgot.” Wright and Pescosolido (2001) found rates of forgetting to be trivial, accounting for only five percent of dropped names, and Mollenhorst (2014) reported eight percent, but Brewer’s (2000, 40) conclusion from a review of studies was that “people forget a substantial proportion of their social contacts when asked to recall them. Even studies with relatively weak test–retest designs show noteworthy levels of forgetting” (see also Bell et al. 2007; Lang, 2000; on forgetting to list spouses, Stueve and Lein, 1979.) Forgotten alters, even if slighted, remain in the network. At the other end of the spectrum, some t1 alters die before t2. Between those extremes are ambiguous cases, such as egos and alters who have “drifted apart.” The present study allowed respondents several ways to explain the status of the alter not renamed.

In addition to catching nuances in what dropping an alter means, UCNets’ first two waves offer an unusually large scale of relevant data: many adult respondents, about 1,000, reporting twice on many ties (an average of about 10 with as many as 26 alters each time) generated by seven name-eliciting questions and describing those ties in many ways. The sample entails two specific age cohorts at distinct periods in the life cycle, allowing deep exploration of how life transitions affect ties. These UCNets features provide significant views of network loss.3

Data and Methods

UCNets Data.

The University of California Social Networks Study (UCNets)4 is a longitudinal egocentric network survey on personal relationships, life events, and well-being. In 2015, we drew participants from two distinct age groups in the greater six-county San Francisco Bay Area: 50- to 70-year-olds and 21- to 30-year-olds. The study focused on these two relatively understudied cohorts in order to maximize the number and variety of key life events respondents would likely experience between survey waves, as well as to test the robustness of results in two parts of the life cycle. We randomly drew households from 30 randomly-selected Bay Area census tracts. Solicitation letters invited a qualified member of the household to join the multi-year panel (for pay). Assessing the yield rate is difficult, because—in addition to the resistance to polling in the current era (National Research Council 2013)—UCNets required would-be panelists to reach out to its fieldwork center, enroll for three waves, and in most cases arrange an in-person interview. About five percent of potential older panelists contacted the field office and in the end about three percent of them completed the entire survey. This “cumulative completion rate” for a panel uptake is comparable to or higher than the yields of other contemporary panel studies (e.g., the Pew panel5; see also Callegaro and DiSogra, 2009; MacInnis et al., 2018, table 1). Household sampling sufficed to fill the 50-to-70 subsample, but not the younger one. We added a few dozen 21-to-30 year-olds by referral from existing panelists and 290 more through targeted Facebook advertisements. Thus, the 21-to-30 panel combines a probability sample plus an opt-in sample of Facebook users. Analysis indicates few differences between the young sample recruited in the household sample and those recruited through Facebook (Lawton and Wilson, 2018). In any event, we control for the recruitment procedure in all the multivariate analyses.

Table 1.

Sample’s sociodemographic characteristics and prevalence of life events (weighted).

Demographics at wave 1 (n = 1,152) %

Male 48.1
Age
 21–30 38.0
 50–70 62.0
Race/ethnicity
 White 51.1
 Asian 23.5
 Latino 15.7
 Black and other 9.6
Married 46.3
Education
 Less than BA degree 54.4
 BA degree 28.1
 More than BA degree 17.6
Poor health 17.3
Mode: web survey (vs. in-person interview) 37.7
Sample: Facebook recruit (vs. household sample, referred) 20.3
Experience of life event between wave 1 and wave 2
 Moved but nearby (in Bay area) 14.4
 Moved far (outside Bay area) 7.5
 New job or started school 12.7
 Stopped working (retirement and unemployment) 6.4
 Graduated or left school 9.3
 Financial, work or school problems 30.9
 Change in marital or partnered relationship 11.1
 New child or grandchild 13.9
 Close kin died 25.0
 Close nonkin died 17.1
 Major break in relationship or friendship 28.0
 Health issue 26.0
Other Attributes (mean / SD)
 Extraversion −2.31 / 2.77
 Neuroticism −3.83 / 2.40
 Network size 10.2/4.3

As part of a mode experiment (Fischer and Bayham, 2019), screeners randomly assigned non-Facebook respondents to either a face-to-face interview or to a web version of the survey at a 3:1 ratio. All Facebook-recruited respondents did the survey online. The online and in-person instruments are substantively identical and we control for any mode effects in all analyses (in-person: 625; web: 379). Roughly a year later, about 88 percent of the wave 1 respondents repeated the survey. All but one respondent who had done wave 1 online did so again; a mostly random 120 of those who had been interviewed in person in wave 1 were assigned to do the wave 2 survey online6; the remainder of the face-to-face wave 1 interviewees did in-person interviews again.

Table 1 below provides a full description of respondents’ demographic and socioeconomic characteristics in each wave. All analyses use weights for wave 2 that adjust for combinations of gender, age, race, Hispanic ethnicity, marital status, and education to match the corresponding age-specific population of the region and in that way accounting for our modest attrition.

Measures.

UCNets used an extended egocentric name-eliciting method to draw a detailed map of respondents’ personal networks and collect information about their social connections. The instrument asked respondents to name the people to whom they were connected in seven possible ways.7 Research shows that multiple items yield greater reliability than do methods that use one or just a few name-eliciting questions (Bernard et al. 1990; Marin 2004; Marin and Hampton 2007; McCallister and Fischer, 1978). The seven questions asked respondents to name the people with whom they were involved in various ways, including sociability, emotional support, material support, and being a burden (see appendix for full text). The instrument asked several name-interpreting questions to obtain descriptions of every alter and of every ties, including role relationship, felt closeness, geographic proximity, and homophily in gender, and age. (More details appear in the appendix to Fischer and Bayham, 2019.) The survey also measured respondents’ demographic, socioeconomic, and health characteristics.

The wave 2 instrument was essentially identical except when identical repetition would not make sense,8 but added a set of questions about what happened to each alters from wave 1 whom respondents did not name in wave 2.

Why Alter was Dropped.

The first key measure for this study is the simple dichotomy of whether an alter named in wave 1 was not named in wave 2. The other is the respondents’ explanation. The instrument asks:

Before finishing our discussion of your social ties, we ask about people you named in the last interview but did not name in this one.

[For each name listed in wave 1 but not wave 2:]

  1. You did mention [name inserted], but gave a different name [this time];

  2. You just forgot to mention [name inserted];

  3. [Name inserted] passed away;

  4. There has not been any occasion for you to be in touch; or

  5. Your relationship changed.

If respondents gave answer 1, the software joined the wave 2 record for that alter with the matching wave 1 record; the alter is considered retained. If respondents answered 5, “your relationship changed,” the instrument asked them to choose an explanation:

  1. One or both of you moved;

  2. One or both of you went through a major life change, like graduation, parenthood, or retirement;

  3. One or both of you had health issues;

  4. You just drifted apart;

  5. You had disagreements;

  6. Other: [open-ended text]

These close-ended answers, combined with coded open-ended replies, generated 16 distinct accounts for the failure to rename, which we reduced to eight categories (see table 2). Our list is similar to that generated by coding of open-ended answers in the NSHAP study (Cornwell et al, 2014).

Table 2.

Wave 1 Alters by Fate in Wave 2: Listed and Dropped, and by Reason for Drop (weighted).

Fate of Alters Listed in W1 TOTAL Immed. Kinc Extended Kin Non-Kin
 Listed in W2 .537 .806 .451 .437
 Not Listed in W2 .463 .194 .549 .563
Total of W1 Alters 9678 2609 1032 5985
Reason W1 Alter Not in W2
 Forgottena .412 .623 .571 .355
 Died .022 .085 .035 .011
 Disagreement / Breakup .055 .039 .063 .057
 Drifted Apart .125 .063 .028 .150
 Someone Moved .128 .043 .069 .150
 Other Life Eventb .071 .026 .034 .085
 No Occasion to Get Together .166 .101 .173 .175
 Miscellaneous Answers .020 .020 .026 .018
Total of w1 Alters Not in W2 4477 507 567 3370
Treating Forgotten as Retained
 Died .038 .225 .082 .017
 Disagreement / Breakup .094 .105 .160 .088
 Drifted Apart .212 .168 .066 .233
 Someone Moved .219 .115 .160 .233
 Other Life Eventb .121 .068 .078 .131
 No Occasion to Get Together .283 .267 .403 .272
 Miscellaneous Answers .034 .052 .062 .027
Total of W1 Alters Not in W2 and Not Forgotten 2631 191 243 2175

Notes:

a.

Includes 11 alters for whom respondents said that there were not enough spaces to list them.

b.

Includes 51 alters with “someone’s health” as the answer.

c.

Spouse, parent, child, or sibling.

Interactions.

We measure the nature of respondents’ interactions and interdependencies with alters through the specific name-eliciting questions that produced the alter. The seven questions (for full text, see Appendix) ask respondents to name the people they...

  • typically do social and leisure activities with (up to 9 names);

  • confide in about “personal matters” (up to 6 names for this and remaining questions);

  • seek out for advice in making important decisions;

  • recently received modest practical help from;

  • could ask for major help if seriously injured or ill;

  • help out in these sorts of ways; and

  • find especially demanding or difficult.

Respondents could provide up to nine names for the social activities question and up to six for each of the others.

Contexts.

Our basic measure of the contexts within which ties exist is the following question (as phrased for the in-person interview; the online version is a slight variant):

People can be connected to each other in a few different ways, even family members. Here is a list of the ways people can be connected. When I read a name to you, please tell me all the different ways that you are connected to that person nowadays. What are all the ways that you are connected to [Name]?

Family:

  • Spouse9

  • My parent

  • My step-parent

  • My child

  • My step-child

  • My brother/sister

  • My step-brother/step-sister

  • My half-brother/half-sister

  • Other relatives, including ex’s (please specify):

Other connections:

  • Romantic partners

  • Housemate/roommate

  • Neighbor

  • Know at work

  • Know at school

  • Know at church, synagogue, temple, or mosque

  • Friend

  • Acquaintance

  • Know another way (please specify):

Alter Traits.

Presenting respondents with a list of all the names they had mentioned, the instrument asks them to indicate which of those on the list fit several criteria, of which we use: same gender as ego; same age as ego (+/− 6 years); older than ego; was first met in the previous year; is someone they feel “especially close to”; and lives over an hour’s drive away.10 Each alter has a dichotomous indicator for each measure.

Ego Traits.

Besides controlling for a standard list of demographic and social traits of the respondents at wave 1, we constructed measures of life events that could change the contexts of respondents’ activities. We drew these from a series of questions about what had happened to the respondents between wave 1 and wave 2, such as a move, graduation, birth of a child, and so on (see Table 3 and see Appendix for texts).

Table 3.

Alters dropped and forgotten from network by role relationship and interaction in the network (weighted).

N % Dropped % Forgotten

Role relationship reported in wave 1
Immediate kin
 Wife 215 3.8 2.0
 Husband 228 0.8 1.6
 Mother 454 7.3 14.9
 Father 286 10.8 16.5
 Adult daughter 343 2.3 10.0
 Adult son 278 6.7 10.7
 Sister 493 12.1 14.1
 Brother 295 16.0 18.8
Extended Kin 1,012 22.9 30.7
Non-kin
 Nonkin Romantic partner 210 18.9 1.0
 Nonkin Housemate 310 45.9 5.4
 Nonkin Neighbor 489 29.6 26.1
 Nonkin Workmate 833 44.3 18.1
 Nonkin Schoolmate 487 40.9 18.5
 Nonkin Churchmate 487 28.4 26.4
 Nonkin Acquaintance 309 59.3 17.3
 Nonkin Friend (Labeled only as friend) 2,500 32.3 19.9
Type of interaction in wave 1
Social companionship 5,416 23.6 15.7
Confide 2,903 17.4 11.6
Advise 2,595 15.4 13.3
Provided recent practical help 1,847 23.4 15.1
Would provide emergency help 2,871 16.8 11.0
Receives support from respondent 3,832 23.6 13.2
Demanding or difficult alter 1,277 29.7 13.1

Notes:

All percentages are calculated at the alter level.

Kin categories include step- and half-relationships. Nonkin categories exclude kin.

Significance: All comparisons between alters in vs. out of a category are p<.001.

Plan of Analysis.

We begin by presenting the descriptive data on dropped names–the numbers and the reasons given. From this, we draw some methodological points. After briefly discussing patterns of forgetting, we proceed to analyze the predictors of alters being actually dropped, counting those whom respondents said they simply forgot as retained. Those models will test our expectations that the odds of being dropped are largely a function of contextual constraints on respondents and on the rewardingness of the alter tie. We use multilevel models with ties nested within individuals, which allows us to properly attribute covariance with dropping to traits of the ties and alters versus to traits of the respondents. For clarity, we present analyses with 21-to-30 year-olds and the 50-to-70 year-olds pooled, because there were minor differences between cohorts in patterns, noting where appropriate any major differences.

Results

Who Was Dropped and Why.

Table 2 presents the results of the question series posed to wave 2 respondents about why they failed to rename an alter from wave 1.11 Using the open-ended answers as well as the closed-ended ones, we started with 16 categories of reasons and combined them into eight. The unit of analysis is the alter. (The numbers in Table 2 confound between-respondent and between-alter differences, which we do examine later, but they are within a few percentage points of those derived by calculating within-respondent averages.)

Note, first, the column for totals. About half of those named in wave 1 did not appear in wave 2. This finding is roughly comparable to the average of egocentric panel studies, despite its own particularities. However, according to the respondents, 41 percent of the dropped alters—19 percent of all alters named in wave 1--were ones whom they had simply forgotten. For most of this paper, we treat forgotten alters as still members of the network, but pause here to report some basic findings on forgetting. The older cohort was slightly likelier to report forgetting, by six points, which may (as a reviewer suggested) point to “senioritis,” but the difference goes away in a multi-level, multivariate model predicting whether an alter was reported as forgotten rather than being re-listed. The model (not shown) reveals that immediate kin, romantic partners, alters deemed emotionally close, and alters living with an hour’s drive were significantly (p<.01) less likely to be forgotten rather than relisted. Nothing about the respondents independently predicts forgetting an alter.

If we pool forgotten alters with re-listed alters as being still present in respondents’ networks, the drop rate becomes 27 percent, not the observed 46 percent, a major revision of preceding estimates. Then, there are the gray zone explanations for dropping, ones that vary in how definitively the ties were sundered. If we take the explanation “we had no occasion to get together” as also indicating that the tie persisted, then the estimated drop rate goes down to 19 percent. One might assume that other categories, aside from death and disagreement, include some ties that remain latent. Our first key finding, then, is methodological: These data suggest that true rates of dropping are much lower than most past research suggests, closer to 20 percent than 50 percent.

Although older and younger respondents dropped roughly the same proportion of wave 1 alters, 50 and 44 percent, the older ones were much likelier to report having forgotten the alters--49 percent compared to 30 percent for the young (data not shown), while the younger respondents were much likelier to have explained dropped names as the result of someone moving, 19 to 9 percent. Treating the forgotten as still in the network, the ”real” drop-or-demote rate for the 21–30-year-olds was 35 percent compared to 22 percent for the 50-to-70-year-olds, an unsurprising difference considering their stages in the life cycle.

The remainder of Table 2 shows the sorts of contextual effects we will explore in detail later. It compares the fates of immediate kin, extended kin, and nonkin alters. Overall, immediate kin were less than half as likely to be dropped than were extended kin or than nonkin, 19 versus about 55 percent. Only nine percent of immediate kin were dropped compared to 24 percent of extended kin and 36 percent of nonkin. Immediate kin were notably likely to have been dropped (neither relisted nor forgotten) because of death (22 percent), extended kin by no occasion to get together (40 percent), and nonkin by literal moving or metaphorical “drifting apart” (both 23 percent).

In sum, by analyzing respondents’ explanations for having not re-listed alters, we find, first, that many of those seemingly missing actually remained in the networks as far as ego was concerned; we don’t know what the alters would have said. About 54 percent were re-listed. Adding back in those alters whom respondents said they had simply forgotten, 73 percent of alters remained presumably active members. (That is, respondents implied that they should have named the alter to one of the name-eliciting questions.) If we enter the gray zone and add back to the networks those missing because of no occasion to get together, about 80 percent were retained. And if we further consider those dropped because of life events–particularly, having moved–as latent ties many of which might be mobilized, we come to 90 percent as a high estimate for the year-to-year continuity.

Multivariate Analysis of Which Alters are Dropped.

We use HLM modeling to assess our theoretical expectations that–all else equal–the probability of being dropped is lower the more constraining the context of the tie and the more rewarding the tie. We control for many other attributes of the alter, the relationships, and ego.12 We define as “retained” all alters who were either renamed in wave 2 or, if not renamed, whose absence respondents explained as forgotten; the rest of the wave 1 names are treated as having been explicitly dropped.

Table 3 shows the percentage of alters in each category of role context and each category of exchange context who were explicitly dropped and who were forgotten. There are major variations. For example, wave 1 romantic partners missing in wave 2 were almost always explicitly dropped and almost never forgotten (19 vs. 1 percent), while wave 1 sons missing in wave 2 were more likely to have been forgotten than explicitly dropped (11 vs. 7 percent). Emergency helpers, advisors, and confidants were likeliest to remain in the network, while alters whom respondents found difficult were likeliest to be explicitly dropped (30 percent).

Table 4 examines these predictors simultaneously, together with alter traits, ego traits, and ego life events that occurred between the waves, as well as controlling for any methods effects. First, we should note that between-ego variation is minor (see lower part of table 4). Nothing about respondents’ sociodemographic profile is independently associated with dropping an alter. Nor does network size and nor do methods variations matter. A few life events do predict a higher probability of dropping ties. Most important of these is moving, especially out of the region (and especially for the younger cohort; data not shown). A respondent who had moved out of the region was about four times likelier to drop an alter than a respondent who had stayed put. (The effect of moving is also highlighted in other studies, such as Badawy et al., 2018; Bloem et al., 2008.)The alter of a respondent who reported the death of a close nonkin associate or who had reported a major break in a relationship was at about 67 percent elevated risk of being dropped.13 (Examination of interaction effects—not shown—suggest that the death mattered only for the young and the relationship breakup only for the older respondents.)

Table 4.

Multilevel results predicting the log-odds an alter would be dropped from the network (weighted)

b (SE) Odds Ratio

Intercept −.882*** (.252) .41
Alter-level variables
Role relationship in Wave 1
Kin
 Wife −1.145 (.764) .32
 Husband −3.204** (1.049) .04
 Mother −1.731*** (.311) .18
 Father −1.357*** (.340) .26
 Adult daughter −2.264*** (.360) .10
 Adult son −1.307*** (.341) .27
 Sister −.900*** (.258) .41
 Brother −.666* (.309) .51
 [Extended kin, ref.]
Nonkin
 Roommate .689*** (.189) 1.99
 Neighbor .257 (.150) 1.29
 Workmate .648*** (.139) 1.99
 Schoolmate .235 (.164) 1.26
 Churchmate .224 (.214) 1.25
 Romantic partner .023 (.282) 1.02
 Friend (labeled only as friend) .347** (.117) 1.42
 Acquaintance 1.078*** (.198) 2.94
Type of interaction in Wave 1
 Socialize −.478*** (.083) .62
 Confide −.332*** (.104) .72
 Advise −.330** (.110) .72
 Recent practical help −.228* (.104) .83
 Available for emergency help −.365*** (.089) .69
 Respondent provides support to alter −.088 (.083) .92
 Difficult alter −.002 (.119) 1.00
Alter descriptors in Wave 1
Female (for non-kin) −.006 (.086) .99
Same age −.052 (.123) .95
Older .045 (.149) 1.05
Met in last year .333* (.170) 1.40
Emotionally close −.406*** (.092) .67
Live over one hour away .060 (.109) 1.06
Ego-level variables
Life events between wave 1 and wave 2
 Moved away but nearby (in Bay area) .500** (.162) 1.65
 Moved far (outside Bay area) 1.460*** (.191) 4.31
 New job or started school .140 (.151) 1.15
 Retired or became unemployed .049 (.207) 1.05
 Graduated or left school −.124 (.224) .88
 Financial, work or school problems .222 (.119) 1.25
 Change in marital or partnered relationship −.074 (.148) .93
 New child or grandchild −.249 (.159) .78
 Close kin died −.017 (.120) .98
 Close nonkin died .570*** (.147) 1.77
 Major break in friendship or relationship .493*** (.120) 1.64
 Health issue −.125 (.138) .88
Sociodemographic variables in wave 1
 Male −.100 (.123) .90
 Age 50–70 −.312 (.176) .73
 Asian .238 (.153) 1.27
 Latino .059 (.151) 1.06
 Black and other .120 (.200) 1.13
 Married .019 (.124) 1.02
 Education less than BA .201 (.142) 1.22
 Education BA .147 (.130) 1.16
 Poor health .122 (.157) 1.13
 Extroversion .020 (.018) 1.02
 Neuroticism .033 (.021) 1.03
Network size in wave 1 −.023 (.014) .98
Web survey in wave 1 .049 (.125) 1.05
Personal reference or Facebook recruit −.025 (.175) .98
N alters in wave 1 10,138
N respondents in waves 1 and 2 997
Variance components
Between-ego variance .581
Chi-square (intercept) 1959.710***
*

p < .05.

**

p < .01.

***

p < .001 (two-tailed tests)

Much more important in predicting which alters were dropped are the alters’ relationships to ego, as displayed in the top part of table 4. Immediate kin were far less likely than extended kin to be dropped, ranging from one-twenty-fifth as likely for husbands to one-half as likely for brothers. This certainly is evidence–given that other attributes of the actual ties, such as their supportiveness and emotional closeness, are held constant in this model–of the importance of the structural and normative constraints against sundering ties with close relatives.

All sorts of nonkin were likelier to be dropped than extended kin, substantially so for roommates, workmates, and acquaintances (double or more the odds of being dropped as extended kin). We expected workmates to be less vulnerable to being dropped, but they were more vulnerable. Open-ended responses suggest that this may be explained by a change of contexts, that either the respondent or the workmate changed jobs or work sites. Respondents volunteered such explanations for four percent of the dropped workmates and, more generally, respondents more often explained the dropping of workmates as the result of “life events” or of moving rather than having cut off those workplace ties.14

The results for relationship content show the importance of receiving support for retaining ties. All else held constant, providing social companionship, a confidant’s ear, advice, or availability in an emergency reduced the odds of being dropped by about 30 to 40 percent. (Recently having provided practical help mattered as well, but not as much because, close examination suggests, many of these helpers were casual acquaintances or even previously unknown ties who just happened to be available [cf. Small, 2017].) Receiving help from ego and being demanding of ego seemed not to affect retention, although that has to be qualified: When the object of ego’s help or the person named as difficult was an immediate relative, then they were much less likely to be dropped than an alter in that situation who was not close kin (data not shown). Finally, the alter descriptors section of table 4 shows, everything else equal, that alters whom respondents marked as emotionally close carried, for that alone, two-thirds the risk of being dropped than did other alters. (An indirect indicator of closeness, suggested by a reviewer, is whether an alter is listed early in answer to the questions. That modestly predicts, all else equal, retention.15) These findings reflects in another way the importance of how rewarding the tie is to ego in determining retention.

Discussion

High rates of network churn have long been a concern and puzzle for network analysts. By surveying a large and diverse sample of adults twice asked to answer a large and diverse set of name-eliciting and name-describing questions, the UCNets data provide a rich view of which alters are dropped from one year to another and why. About half of wave 1 names did not appear on wave 2 lists. Respondents chose from a range of explanations to account for each of the missing wave 1 alters. We took the answer “I forgot to name X” to mean that the missing alter X remained roughly in the same place in the respondents’ networks. Other accounts, such as “we had no occasion to get together” or “we drifted apart,” suggest that many dropped alters remained at least on the periphery of respondents’ networks. Depending on how analysts choose to interpret these other explanations, the percentage of truly sundered ties could be as high as 27 percent (all those neither relisted nor forgotten) or as low as four percent (dropped only by death or disagreement) or nine percent (adding in those who “drifted” away).

The methodological implications of this analysis include the suggestion that researchers ought, given time and resources, at least find out whether dropped names were simply forgotten. A second is to appreciate even more the difficult task we pose to respondents (and interviewers [Paik and Sanchagrin, 2013]) when we ask them to recall lists of names. The recall process, as some researchers have described (e.g., Brashears, 2013; Brashears et al. 2016), is subject to all sorts of influences. If, as we suggested earlier, each question prompts a partly-random draw from a pool of qualifying ties, then there is greater stability in network membership than previously presumed. And such a process helps account, along with other processes such egos trying to sustain their individual network “signatures,” for the over-time constancy in general network features. A third methodological implication is, as others have suggested, that networks have zones of ambiguity in which alters may be disfavored but yet remain latently connected and perhaps available. It is not clear that network analysts have good tools to deal with such ambiguity. Antonucci’s depiction of networks as concentric circles of greater to lower involvement (e.g., Antonucci et al. 2010) is relevant, but even there, specific alters are either in or out of a circle. Perhaps, like Schrödinger’s cat, many ties both exist and do not exist until egos look at them.

The substantive implications of these findings are, first, that contexts matter considerably, with constraining contexts–notably the immediate families of descent and of procreation–reducing the odds of ties being dropped. Changing contexts–notably moving–increases the odds of dropping ties. (We anticipated that the workplace would also constrain ties to persist, but the reverse seemed true, probably because egos and alters often change work sites.) Other studies discussed earlier reported rates of dropping ties to other life events such as marriage, widowhood, and retirement. Perhaps we did not because of the one-year lag did not allow us to cumulate enough such events. By one calculation, in our data about 60 percent of the drops resulted from contextual change of one kind or another–a move, a death, or the lack of an occasion to meet. Unanticipated by us, but consistent with much network literature on triadic structures and bridging ties, we found some evidence that losing a non-redundant tie by death or break-up led to losing connection with further nonkin alters.

The second general substantive implication of the findings is that retaining a tie depended on the net balance of support versus burden the tie entailed. Alters who were confidants, advisers, social partners, likely helpers in an emergency, or considered emotionally “close” were likelier to be retained. Alters who got help from ego or who were demanding of ego were either no likelier to be kept or were likelier to be dropped. Finally, these two general factors–context and exchange quality–conditioned one another. Notably, respondents kept ties to alters who received their help or who were difficult if those burdensome people were close kin. Normative context seemed to matter more than the quality of the tie.

This study contributes to our understanding of the dynamics of personal networks by analyzing which ties are likely to disappear from the network lists, dropped by whom, and why. Understanding who is dropped is only one part of understanding network change. Feld, et al. (2007) point out that network change entails several dimensions, not only in membership turnover, for example, but also change in what continuing members do and who is added (see, e.g., Cornwell, et al., 2014). We can only cover member losses in this space. In future research, we plan to examine who was added to networks in waves 2 and 3 and to what extent dropped alters reappeared and why.

Acknowledgements:

This research was supported by National Institute of Aging Grant R01 AG041955-01 to the senior author. We thank the UCNets project’s executive director, Leora Lawton, and UCNets’ many participants for support and comments; the Center for Economic and Social Research at the University of Southern California for development and data management, especially programmer Bart Orriens; and Nexant fieldwork managers, Kenric Erickson and Cat Conti. And we thank reviewers for helpful suggestions.

APPENDIX

Name-Eliciting Questions

Please think about people you typically do these sorts of things [referring to sharing meals or other social activities] with – or other social things as well, such as going shopping, out for drinks, to the park, or just hanging out. Who are the people you usually do these sorts of things with?

Sometimes personal matters come up that concern people, like issues about relationships, important things in their lives, or difficult experiences. Do you ever confide in someone about these sorts of things or do you never confide in anyone? [IF YES:] Who do you confide in about these sorts of things?

When you have to make important decisions – for example, about taking a job, family issues, or health problems – are there any people whose advice you seek out or would seek out in making those decisions? They can be family, friends, or professional advisors. [IF YES:] Whose advice do you or would you seek out?

In the last few months, have any friends, relatives, or acquaintances[ (who do not live with you)] given you any practical help like moving furniture, doing repairs, picking up something at the store, looking after a child, giving you a ride, or things like that? [IF YES:] Please give us the names of people who have done things like this for you in the last few months.

If you were seriously injured or sick and needed some help for a couple of weeks with things such as preparing meals and getting around, who would you ask? Would you ask particular people you know personally for help, ask a group for help, pay for help, or get help some other way? Or would you do more than one of these? [IF ASK PARTICULAR PERSON:] Who would those people be? These can be people you have named before or new people. [IF NOT:] But if you needed to, are there particular people you could ask for help? [IF YES:] Who would those people be? These can be people you have named before or new people.

We have been asking about people who help you out in different ways. Now, let’s turn things around. Who are the people that you help out practically, or with advice, or in other kinds of ways at least occasionally? They can be people you’ve already named or new people.

There are sometimes people we know who ask a lot of us, who are sometimes demanding or difficult. Who are the people that you sometimes find demanding or difficult? They can be people you’ve already named or new people.

Life Events Between Wave 1 and Wave 2

Moving Near, Far: Respondents who answered yes to “Have you moved since the previous interview in [month, year]?” then answered “How far is where you live now from where you lived at the time of the previous interview? Is it...1 In the same neighborhood; 2 In a different neighborhood but in the same town; 3 In a different town but still in the Bay Area; 4 Out of the Bay Area, but still in California; 5 Out of California; 6 Other.” Near was defined as answers 1, 2, and 3.

New job or started school: Respondents reported how long they had been at their current job and, for students, current school. If less than two years, they were coded as having a new job or school.

Stopped working (retirement and unemployment): Respondents who were retired or unemployed in wave 2 but not in wave 1 were coded as having stopped working since wave 1.

Graduated or left school: If respondents were in school in wave 1 but not wave 2; if they had advanced a degree.

Financial, work or school problems: Respondents who answered affirmatively to “Since the last interview in [month, year], have you had any major problems at work?” or a similar question about school.

Change in marital or partnered relationship: Respondents answered affirmatively to “Since the last interview in [[month, year]], has there been any important change in [your marriage/your relationship/any romantic relationship you might have had16]?

New child or grandchild: Respondents who answered affirmatively to “Since the last interview in [month, year], have you [had a child/had a child or a grandchild]?”

Close kin died, Close nonkin died: Respondents answered affirmatively to “Since the last interview in [month, year], did anyone you felt close to pass away?” Follow-up questions determined whether the deceased included kin and/or nonkin.

Major break in relationship or friendship: Respondents who answered affirmatively to “Since [month, year], has there been any major break in a relationship between yourself and a relative or close friend?”

Health issue: Respondents who answered affirmatively to any of these options (besides “none”): “Did any of these happen to you since the last interview in [month, year]? Tell me all of the ones that apply: 1 Diagnosed with a serious illness; 2 Disabled by an illness or injury; 3 Hospitalized 4 Had or have another serious health issue 5 None -- no serious health issues.”

Footnotes

1

Logic and past research suggest that the sampling process would be systematically skewed toward more central alters and also toward more cognitively available alters–for example, people whom the respondent saw recently and people who are connected to one another in a “chunk” (Brashears and Quintane, 2015), such as coworkers, although there is a large random component (Marin, 2004).

2

We found, for example, that occasionally respondents in wave 2 would not recognize the name of an alter from wave 1 whom they had listed as having helped them with a practical chore. Those people seemed often to be friends of friends or casual neighbors who happened to be on hand at the moment but otherwise out of mind (see Small, 2017).

3

Other important large-scale studies have used just one or two name-eliciting probes or focused on only one slice of the population, or both (e.g., Cornwell and Laumann, 2018; Mollenhorst, 2014; Schafer and Vargas, 2016; Schwartz and Litwin 2017).

4

UCNets data are available to researchers at ICPSR. Consult also the UCNets website, http://ucnets.berkeley.edu/researcher-resources/.

5

Pew (2015) estimates their cumulative response rate for participating in any given panel survey—that is, qualifying for and joining the panel and then qualifying for and joining a particular survey--as 3.5 percent. Some panels attain higher rates by recruiting all the teenage and adult members of a household, maximizing participation (Callegaro and DiSogra, 2009).

6

Those who had moved out of the region were assigned to the online condition.

7

Two other kinds of questions also elicited names (name spouse or partner; name co-residents), but they are not part of the battery analyzed here.

8

For example, the wave 2 survey did not re-ask respondents if their mothers were alive when respondents had answered “no” in wave 1, nor did it re-ask country of origin.

9

Spouses, non-marital partners, and housemates were also identified, earlier in the interview, by specific name-eliciting questions.

10

We explored other alter traits, but they ended up with insubstantial associations to being dropped.

11

We exclude from analysis the small number of alters who, in wave 1, were named only in the household census. And we exclude (given limitations of a multilevel model) the ten respondents who named no or only one alter in wave 1.

12

We do not control for alter centrality or network density because, for practical reasons, UCNets measured those for only a subset of up-to-five alters in each network.

13

It could be that these effects are mechanical in that the differences simply reflect the specifically dropped alters–the close nonkin who died or the estranged friend. But we suspect that they reflect more: the disruption of triadic ties when one edge disappears. That the death of a close relative does not have the same effect suggests as much, because connections to close kin are redundant.

14

Forty-eight percent of dropped nonkin workmates (neither re-listed nor forgotten) were explained by life events or moving versus only 31 percent of non-workmate alters who were dropped (and versus 34 percent of nonkin non-workmates).

15

We created an indicator of whether an alter was named first of second to any name-eliciting question. That indictaor, added to the model in table 4, is significant at p <.03, OR = 0.83.

16

The instrument applied the correct wording depending on whether the respondent was married or partnered or neither.

Declarations of interest: None.

Contributor Information

Claude S. Fischer, University of California, Berkeley

Shira Offer, Bar-Ilan University.

REFERENCES

  1. Antonucci Toni C., Fiori Katherine L., Birditt Kira and Jackey Lisa M. H.. 2010. “Convoys of Social Relations: Integrating Life-Span and Life-Course Perspectives.” in The Handbook of Life-Span Development, Volume 2: Social and Emotional Development, edited by Lerner RM, Lamb ME and Freund AM: Wiley. [Google Scholar]
  2. Asendorpf Jens B. and Wilpers Susanne. 1998. “Personality Effects on Social Relationships.”. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 74(6):1531–44. [Google Scholar]
  3. Atchley Robert C. 1989. “A Continuity Theory of Normal Aging.” The Gerontologist 29(2):183–90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Badawy Philip J., Schafer Markus H. and Sun Haosen. 2018. “Relocation and Network Turnover in Later Life: How Distance Moved and Functional Health Are Linked to a Changing Social Convoy.” Research on Aging. doi: 10.1177/0164027518774805. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bell David C., Belli-McQueen Benedetta and Haider Ali. 2007. “Partner Naming and Forgetting: Recall of Network Members.” Social Networks 29(2):279–99. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bello Jason and Rolfe Meredith. 2013. “Is Influence Mightier Than Selection? Forging Agreement in Political Discussion Networks During a Campaign.” Social Networks 36:134–46. [Google Scholar]
  7. Bernard H. Russell, Johnsen Eugene C., Killworth Peter D., McCarty Christopher, Shelley Gene A. and Robinson Scott. 1990. “Comparing Four Different Methods for Measuring Personal Social Networks.” Social Networks 12(3):179–215. doi: 10.1016/0378-8733(90)90005-T. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bidart Claire and Cacciuttolo Patrice. 2013. “Combining Qualitative, Quantitative and Structural Dimensions in a Longitudinal Perspective. The Case of Network Influence.” Quality & Quantity 47(5):2495–515. doi: 10.1007/s11135-012-9667-6. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  9. __________, Degenne Alain and Grossetti Michel. 2011. La Vie En Réseau: Dynamique Des Relations Sociales. Paris: Presses Universitaires De France. [Google Scholar]
  10. __________ and Lavenu Daniel. 2005. “Evolutions of Personal Networks and Life Events.” Social Networks 27(4):359–76. [Google Scholar]
  11. Assche Bignami-Van, Simona. 2005. “Network Stability in Longitudinal Data: A Case Study from Rural Malawi.” Social Networks 27(3):231–47. [Google Scholar]
  12. Blau Peter M. 1964. Exchange and Power in Social Life. New York: Wiley. [Google Scholar]
  13. __________, and Schwartz Joseph E.. 1984. Crosscutting Social Circles. Orlando, FL: Academic. [Google Scholar]
  14. Bloem Brigitte, van Tilburg Theo and Thomése Fleur. 2008. “Changes in Older Dutch Adults’ Role Networks after Moving.” Personal Relationships 15(4):465–78. [Google Scholar]
  15. Bourdieu Pierre. 1984. Distinction: A Social Critique of the Judgment of Taste, trans. Nice Richard. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
  16. Bowling Ann Grundy and Farquhar Morag. 1995. “Changes in Network Composition among the Very Old Living in Inner London.” Journal of Cross-Cultural Gerontology 10:331–47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Brashears Matthew E. 2013. “Humans Use Compression Heuristics to Improve the Recall of Social Networks.” Scientific Reports 3. doi: 10.1038/srep01513. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. _________, Hoagland Emily and Quintane Eric. 2016. “Sex and Network Recall Accuracy.” Social Networks 44:74–84. [Google Scholar]
  19. _________ and Quintane Eric. 2015. “The Microstructures of Network Recall: How Social Networks Are Encoded and Represented in Human Memory.” Social Networks 41:113–26. doi: 10.1016/j.socnet.2014.11.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  20. Brewer Devon D. 2000. “Forgetting in the Recall-Based Elicitation of Personal and Social Networks.” Social Networks 22(1):29–43. [Google Scholar]
  21. Burt Ronald S. 2000. “Decay Functions.” Social Networks 22(1):1–28. doi: 10.1016/S0378-8733(99)00015-5. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  22. Callegaro Mario and DiSogra Charles. 2009. “Computing Response Metrics for Online Panels.” Public Opinion Quarterly 72(5):1008–32. doi: 10.1093/poq/nfn065. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  23. Carstensen Laura L., Isaacowitz Derek M. and Charles Susan T.. 1999. “Taking Time Seriously: A Theory of Socioemotional Selectivity.” American Psychologist 54(3):165–81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Coleman JS 1990. Foundations of Social Theory. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press [Google Scholar]
  25. Cornwell Benjamin. 2014. “Social Disadvantage and Network Turnover.” J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbu078. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. ___________ and Laumann Edward O.. 2018. “Structure by Death: Social Network Replenishment in the Wake of Confidant Loss.” Pp. 343–66 in Social Networks and the Life Course: Integrating the Development of Human Lives and Social Relational Networks, edited by Alwin DF, Felmlee DH and Kreager DA. Cham, Switzerland: Springer. [Google Scholar]
  27. __________, Philip Schumm L, Laumann Edward O., Kim Juyeon and Kim Young-Jin. 2014. “Assessment of Social Network Change in a National Longitudinal Survey.” Journal of Gerontology B: S75–S82. 69 (Suppl 2). doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbu037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Desmond Matthew. 2012. “Disposable Ties and the Urban Poor.” American Journal of Sociology 117(5):1295–335. [Google Scholar]
  29. Feld Scott L. 1981. “The Focused Organization of Social Ties.” American Journal of Sociology 86(5):1015–35. [Google Scholar]
  30. _________, Suitor J. Jill and Hoegh Jordana Gartner. 2007. “Describing Changes in Personal Networks over Time.” Field Methods 19 (2):218–36. [Google Scholar]
  31. Fischer Claude S. 1982. To Dwell Among Friends: Personal Networks in Town and City, Chicago: University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
  32. _________ and Bayham Lindsay. 2019. “Mode and Interviewer Effects in Egocentric Network Research.” Field Methods, In Press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. __________ and Beresford Lauren. 2015. “Changes in Support Networks in Late Middle Age: The Extension of Gender and Educational Differences.” Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences 70(1):123–31. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Goldman Alyssa W. and Cornwell Benjamin. 2018. “Social Disadvantage and Instability in Older Adults’ Ties to Their Adult Children.” Journal of Marriage and Family 80(5):1314–32. doi: doi: 10.1111/jomf.12503. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Granovetter Mark. 1973. “The Strength of Weak Ties.” American Journal of Sociology 78 (May): 1360–80. [Google Scholar]
  36. Heydari Sara, Roberts Sam G., Dunbar Robin I. M. and Saramäki Jari. 2018. “Multichannel Social Signatures and Persistent Features of Ego Networks.” Applied Network Science 3(1):8. doi: 10.1007/s41109-018-0065-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Homans George Caspar. 1950. The Human Group. New York: Harcourt Brace. [Google Scholar]
  38. Ikkink Karen Klein and Van Tilburg Theo. 1999. “Broken Ties: Reciprocity and Other Factors Affecting the Termination of Older Adults’ Relationships “. Social Networks 21:131–46. [Google Scholar]
  39. Lang Frieder R. 2000. “Endings and Continuity of Social Relationships: Maximizing Intrinsic Benefits within Personal Networks When Feeling near to Death.” Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 17(2):155–82. [Google Scholar]
  40. Laumann Edward O. 1965. Bonds of Pluralism: The Form and Substance of Urban Social Networks. New York: Wiley. [Google Scholar]
  41. Lawton Leora and Wilson Willis. 2018. “Comparing Facebook and Address-Based Sampling to Recruit Young Adults.” Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the Pacific Association of Public Opinion Research, Denver, Co. [Google Scholar]
  42. Lazarsfeld Paul, and Merton Robert K.. 1954. “Friendship as a Social Process.” Pp. 18–66 in Freedom and Control in Modern Society, edited by Berger M et al. New York: Van Nostrand. [Google Scholar]
  43. Lubbers Miranda J., José Luis Molina Jürgen Lerner, Brandes Ulrik, Ávila Javier and McCarty Christopher. 2010. “Longitudinal Analysis of Personal Networks. The Case of Argentinean Migrants in Spain.” Social Networks 32(1):91–104. doi: 10.1016/j.socnet.2009.05.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  44. MacInnis Bo, Krosnick Jon A., Ho Annabell S. and Cho Mu-jung. 2018. “The Accuracy of Measurements with Probability and Nonprobability Survey Samples: Replication and Extension.” Public Opinion Quarterly 82(4):707–44. [Google Scholar]
  45. Marin Alexandra. 2004. “Are Respondents More Likely to List Alters with Certain Characteristics? Implications for Name Generator Data.” Social Networks 26(4):289–307. [Google Scholar]
  46. __________ and Hampton Keith N.. 2007. “Simplifying the Personal Network Name Generator.” Field Methods 19(2):163–93. [Google Scholar]
  47. __________ and Hampton Keith N.. 2019. “Network Instability in Times of Personal Stability.” Sociological Forum. In press. [Google Scholar]
  48. Marsden Peter V. 1987. “Core Discussion Networks of Americans.” American Sociological Review 52(1):122–31. doi: 10.2307/2095397. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  49. Martin John Levi and Yeung King-To. 2006. “Persistence of Close Personal Ties over a 12-Year Period.” Social Networks 28 331–62. [Google Scholar]
  50. McCallister L and Fischer CS. 1978. “A Procedure for Surveying Personal Networks.” Sociological Methods & Research 7(November):131–48. [Google Scholar]
  51. Mollenhorst Gerald, Volker Beate and Flap Henk. 2008. “Social Contexts and Personal Relationships: The Effect of Meeting Opportunities on Similarity for Relationships of Different Strength.” Social Networks 30:60–68. [Google Scholar]
  52. _________, Volker Beate and Flap Henk. 2014. “Changes in Personal Relationships: How Social Contexts Affect the Emergence and Discontinuation of Relationships.” Social Networks 37:65–80. [Google Scholar]
  53. Morgan David L., Neal Margaret B. and Carder Paula. 1997. “The Stability of Core and Peripheral Networks over Time.” Social Networks 19:9–25. [Google Scholar]
  54. Mund M, Jeronimus BF and Neyer FJ. 2018. “Personality and Social Relationships: As Thick as Thieves.” Pp. 153–83 in Personality and Disease: Scientific Proof Vs. Wishful Thinking, edited by Johansen Christoffer. San Diego: Elsevier. [Google Scholar]
  55. Paik Anthony and Sanchagrin Kenneth. 2013. “Social Isolation in America: An Artifact.” American Sociological Review 78(3):339–60. [Google Scholar]
  56. Perry Brea L. and Pescosolido Bernice A.. 2012. “Social Network Dynamics and Biographical Disruption: The Case of “First-Timers” with Mental Illness.” American Journal of Sociology 118(1). doi: 10.1086/666377. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  57. _________, Pescosolido Bernice A. and Borgatti Steve P.. 2018. Egocentric Network Analysis: Foundations, Methods, and Models: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  58. Pew Research Center. 2015. “Building Pew Research Center’s American Trends Panel.” Washington: Pew Research Center. [Google Scholar]
  59. Saramäki Jari, Leicht EA, López Eduardo, Roberts Sam G. B., Reed-Tsochas Felix and Dunbar Robin I. M.. 2013. “Persistence of Social Signatures in Human Communication.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 111 (3) 942–947. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Sasovova Zuzana, Mehra Ajay, Borgatti Stephen P. and Schippers Michaéla C.. 2010. “Network Churn: The Effects of Self-Monitoring Personality on Brokerage Dynamics.” Administrative Science Quarterly 55(4):639–70. doi: doi: 10.2189/asqu.2010.55.4.639. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  61. Schafer Markus H. and Vargas Nicholas. 2016. “The Dynamics of Social Support Inequality: Maintenance Gaps by Socioeconomic Status and Race?”. Social Forces 94(4):1795–1882. doi: 10.1093/sf/sow024. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  62. Schwartz Ella and Litwin Howard. 2017. “Are Newly Added and Lost Confidants in Later Life Related to Subsequent Mental Health?” International Psychogeriatrics:1–11. doi: 10.1017/S1041610217001338. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Silber Henning, Schröder Jette, Struminskaya Bella, Stocké Volker and Bosnjak Michael. 2019. “Does Panel Conditioning Affect Data Quality in Ego-Centered Social Network Questions?” Social Networks 56:45–54. doi: 10.1016/j.socnet.2018.08.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  64. Small Mario Luis. 2017. Someone to Talk To: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  65. _________, Pamphile Vontrese Deeds and McMahan Peter. 2015. “How Stable Is the Core Discussion Network?” Social Networks 40:90–102. doi: 10.1016/j.socnet.2014.09.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  66. Stueve CA and Lein L. 1979. “Problems in Network Analysis: The Case of the Missing Person.” Paper presented at the 32nd Annual Meeting of the Gerontological Society of America. [Google Scholar]
  67. Suitor Jill and Keeton Shirley. 1997. “Once a Friend, Always a Friend? Effects of Homophily on Women’s Support Networks across a Decade.” Social Networks 19:51–62. [Google Scholar]
  68. __________, Wellman Barry and Morgan David L.. 1997. “It’s About Time: How, Why, and When Networks Change.” Social Networks 19:1–7. [Google Scholar]
  69. Thibaut John W. and Kelly Harold H.. 1959. The Social Psychology of Groups. New York: Wiley. [Google Scholar]
  70. Torres Stacy. 2019. “On Elastic Ties: Distance and Intimacy in Social Relationships.” Sociological Science. doi: 10.15195/v6.a10. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  71. van Duijin Marijtje A.J., van Busschbach Jooske T. and Snijders Tom A.B.. 1999. “Multilevel Analysis of Personal Networks as Dependent Variables.” Social Networks 21:187–209. [Google Scholar]
  72. van Tilburg Theo G. 1992. “Support Networks before and after Retirement.” Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 9:433–45. [Google Scholar]
  73. _________. 1998. “Losing and Gaining in Old Age: Changes in Personal Network Size and Social Support in a Four-Year Longitudinal Study.” Journal of Gerontology: Social Sciences 53B:S313–S23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. _________ and Van Groenou Marjolein Broese. 2002. “Network and Health Changes among Older Dutch Adults.” Journal of Social Issues 58(4):697–713. [Google Scholar]
  75. Vriens Eva and van Ingen Erik. 2017. “Does the Rise of the Internet Bring Erosion of Strong Ties? Analyses of Social Media Use and Changes in Core Discussion Networks.” New Media & Society 0(0):1461444817724169. doi: 10.1177/1461444817724169. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Wellman Barry. 1979. “The Community Question: The Intimate Networks of East Yorkers.” American Journal of Sociology 84(5):1201–31. [Google Scholar]
  77. _________, Yuk-lin Wong Renita, Tindall David and Nazera Nancy. 1997. “A Decade of Network Change: Turnover, Persistence and Stability in Personal Communities.” Social Networks 19(1):27–50. [Google Scholar]
  78. Wright Eric R. and Pescosolido Bernice A.. 2001. ““Sorry, I Forgot: The Role of Recall Error in Longitudinal Personal Network Studies.” Advances in Medical Sociology 8:113–29. doi: 10.1016/S1057-6290(02)80023-7 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  79. Zhu Xiumei, Woo Sang Eun, Porter Caitlin and Brzezinski Michael. 2013. “Pathways to Happiness: From Personality to Social Networks and Perceived Support.” Social Networks 35(3):382–93. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES