Skip to main content
Nature Communications logoLink to Nature Communications
. 2022 Jul 22;13:4253. doi: 10.1038/s41467-022-31687-5

A macroscopic link between interhemispheric tract myelination and cortico-cortical interactions during action reprogramming

Alberto Lazari 1,, Piergiorgio Salvan 1, Lennart Verhagen 2,3, Michiel Cottaar 1, Daniel Papp 1, Olof Jens van der Werf 4,5, Bronwyn Gavine 1, James Kolasinski 6, Matthew Webster 1, Charlotte J Stagg 1,7,8, Matthew F S Rushworth 2, Heidi Johansen-Berg 1,
PMCID: PMC9307658  PMID: 35869067

Abstract

Myelination has been increasingly implicated in the function and dysfunction of the adult human brain. Although it is known that axon myelination shapes axon physiology in animal models, it is unclear whether a similar principle applies in the living human brain, and at the level of whole axon bundles in white matter tracts. Here, we hypothesised that in humans, cortico-cortical interactions between two brain areas may be shaped by the amount of myelin in the white matter tract connecting them. As a test bed for this hypothesis, we use a well-defined interhemispheric premotor-to-motor circuit. We combined TMS-derived physiological measures of cortico-cortical interactions during action reprogramming with multimodal myelin markers (MT, R1, R2* and FA), in a large cohort of healthy subjects. We found that physiological metrics of premotor-to-motor interaction are broadly associated with multiple myelin markers, suggesting interindividual differences in tract myelination may play a role in motor network physiology. Moreover, we also demonstrate that myelination metrics link indirectly to action switching by influencing local primary motor cortex dynamics. These findings suggest that myelination levels in white matter tracts may influence millisecond-level cortico-cortical interactions during tasks. They also unveil a link between the physiology of the motor network and the myelination of tracts connecting its components, and provide a putative mechanism mediating the relationship between brain myelination and human behaviour.

Subject terms: Decision, Myelin biology and repair, Human behaviour


Myelination is a key regulator of brain function. Here the authors use MR-based myelin measures to examine if cortico-cortical interactions, as assessed by paired pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation, are affected by variations in myelin in the human brain.

Introduction

Myelination of axonal projections is increasingly being appreciated as a key regulator of brain function. This relationship is widely believed to arise because the properties of an axon’s myelination influence many of its physiological properties. Conduction velocity, for instance, increases with myelin thickness and internode length, as demonstrated by observational studies13, interventional studies47 and in silico simulations810. Moreover, myelination has also been proposed to enable high-frequency conduction11 and to have a broader role in network physiology12, for example by synchronising neuronal activity13,14 and regulating the timing of action potentials in a circuit-dependent manner1517.

While recent advances have allowed the translation of insights on myelination across species18,19, specifically with regards to our understanding of brain plasticity20,21 and neuroanatomy2225, most current evidence directly linking myelination of a circuit to its neurophysiological properties has been derived from animal studies, often focusing on in vitro results. Little is known about how levels of myelination may influence properties of circuits in humans and how this may play out during behaviour. Moreover, many studies have focused on microscopic aspects of myelination, such as individual myelin sheaths, and microscopic aspects of physiology, such as action potentials. However, it is unclear how myelination levels at the macroscopic level of axon bundles influence macroscopic aspects of tract physiology, such as cortico-cortical interactions.

In recent years, magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) and non-invasive brain stimulation have provided new opportunities to tackle questions at macroscopic scales in humans in vivo, which are typically confined to animal or in vitro studies. MRI studies have traditionally aimed to probe properties of white matter through diffusion-weighted imaging (DWI)2632. DWI, however, provides metrics that are remarkably non-specific to myelination: although it can detect myelin sheath-driven hindered diffusion, it is also sensitive to fibre orientation and other cellular compartments33. Recently, several advances have improved our ability to study myelination through MRI34,35. Surging interest in quantifying myelin non-invasively has led to the development of MR sequences sensitive to myelin through a variety of different biophysical mechanisms: macromolecular tissue content in the myelin lipid bilayer can now be measured by magnetisation transfer (MT)3639, local concentrations of diamagnetic myelin and paramagnetic iron-rich oligodendrocytes can be detected through susceptibility-sensitive contrasts (such as R2*)40, and quantification of longitudinal relaxation rates (through R1) makes mapping anatomical variability in myelin content possible41,42. In addition to this new abundance of MR technologies for quantification of myelination, advances in statistics, such as the development of joint inference permutation testing43 also allow optimal pooling of multimodal data to extract reliable information across these complementary MR signals. While previous work has linked individual cortical myelin-sensitive markers to cortical physiology42, multimodal myelin imaging is a promising tool to draw stronger conclusions about myelination and study myelin’s relationship with tract physiology.

We hypothesised that MR measures of myelin would relate to physiological measures of cortico-cortical interactions. Although functional MRI (fMRI) has been widely deployed to study cortico-cortical interactions, even advanced computational models of fMRI signals44 cannot detect the directional, causal influence of an area’s neuronal activity on the activity of another area. fMRI’s low temporal resolution also precludes studying millisecond-level cortico-cortical interactions which are known to underly behaviour4547. Paired pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation (ppTMS), by contrast, can be delivered with millisecond precision to allow insights into directional interactions between two brain areas. This high temporal resolution allows ppTMS to detect subtle variation in rapid cortico-cortical interactions, which are not detectable in fMRI studies. We therefore hypothesised that myelin variability would have fine-grained effects on rapid cortico-cortical interactions, which could be quantified by the use of ppTMS.

Cortical projections from ventral premotor cortex (PMv) to primary motor cortex (M1) have been extensively characterised both in anatomical tract tracing and physiological studies, across human and non-human primates (for a review, see ref. 48), thus allowing us to formulate detailed hypotheses regarding their function. Behaviourally, these projections allow for inhibition of incorrect motor programmes4951 and are thus engaged during low-level motor tasks such as action reprogramming46, providing a clear read-out of participant behaviour to relate to their function52. Although there are several premotor areas, two regions on the lateral surface, the dorsal premotor cortex (PMd) and ventral premotor cortex (PMv) stand out because they provide the densest projections to primary motor cortex (M1)53. Of these two regions, however, PMv is special. First, its influence over M1 is the best studied; it has been repeatedly demonstrated PMv exerts a strong influence over M1 activity in both non-human and human primates5459. This influence can be studied not just in experiments in which PMv is directly stimulated electrically but in transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) experiments where the impact of the TMS effect in PMv is especially well characterised; even though the impact of the first pulse in PMv is spatially circumscribed60, it alters the activity in PMv neurons that project to M154,58,59. Second, PMv is special not just because it has a strong projection to M1 that is particularly well studied, but, in addition, compared to PMd, PMv receives the stronger projection from prefrontal cortex53. This means PMv is well-placed to mediate inhibitory influences exerted over motor control as a result of executive control processes in prefrontal cortical areas. Consistent with such a role, PMv projections to M1 are monosynaptic53,6163 but within M1, many, perhaps the majority, of connections are with inhibitory interneurons as opposed to pyramidal neurons ensuring that PMv is able to exert an inhibitory influence over M158,64. In accordance with such observations, the modulation of M1 by projections from PMv is dependent on behavioural state; PMv exerts a facilitatory influence over M1 during action initiation but an inhibitory influence when no movement is to be made or when an action is to be changed and reprogrammed45,46,55,65,66. Moreover, these inhibitory influences have been linked to specific white matter tracts connecting PMv and M146, thus providing a clear anatomical location not just in cortex but in underlying white matter at which to investigate PMv-to-M1 modulation. While many anatomical tracing studies focus only on ipsilateral connections within a hemisphere, PMv has many transcallosal connections with heterotopic areas in the other hemisphere67,68 and consistent with this anatomical observation it is established that PMv exerts similar facilitatory and inhibitory influences over the activity of M1 both ipsilaterally and contralaterally45,46,66,69,70. Therefore, the PMv-to-M1 circuit can also be studied interhemispherically, thus making it possible to probe mechanisms that involve fibres passing through the corpus callosum, and that may be unique to interhemispheric circuits23,7175. Taken together, these features of PMv-to-M1 projections (the presence of both clear behavioural and clear physiological readouts, the link to a defined interhemispheric white matter tract) make them an ideal test bed for hypotheses about how myelin shapes circuit function and behaviour in humans.

Here, we aimed to test whether inhibitory interactions between PMv and M1 (as measured through ppTMS) may be shaped by the amount of myelin in the white matter tract connecting them (as measured by multimodal myelin markers). We also aimed to probe the relationship between white matter myelination, cortico-cortical inhibition, and behavioural output in an action reprogramming task. Because previous experiments suggest that white matter myelination rarely relates directly to behavioural performance76, we hypothesised that during action reprogramming, PMv-to-M1 inhibition may link white matter myelination to behavioural performance. As PMv-to-M1 projections are known to modulate the balance between excitation and inhibition within M158,64, we also hypothesised that PMv-to-M1 inhibition and local inhibition within M1 may both contribute to behavioural output during action reprogramming.

Results

Multimodal myelin markers, interhemispheric cortico-cortical inhibition, and action reprogramming

We hypothesised that greater myelination of a long-range projection (measurable with multimodal MRI markers of myelin) is associated with stronger physiological interactions between cortical target areas (measurable with ppTMS), and that this, in turn, is associated with better behavioural performance. MRI allows collection of many quantitative markers that are known to be related to myelin (Fig. 1a). Taken independently, each marker is sensitive to other features of the tissue (e.g., R2* is also sensitive to iron), so a joint inference test was used to find common patterns across all MRI markers. Since myelin is the only common biological feature all sequences are sensitive to, we reasoned that any concordant, common trend in relationships with all these markers must signify strong evidence for a myelin-driven effect.

Fig. 1. Non-invasive measurement of myelination and of cortico-cortical interactions during behaviour.

Fig. 1

a Multiple myelin-sensitive MRI modalities were collected and jointly analysed through multimodal joint inference. b Motor-evoked potentials were collected from the same participants during an action reprogramming task. Stimulation of premotor cortex during the task allowed measurement of the modulatory effects of premotor cortex on motor-evoked potentials driven by M1 stimulation. More specifically, in a single pulse condition (SP, left) a test pulse (red) over left motor cortex elicits a motor-evoked potential in the right hand; in a paired pulse condition (PP, right) the test pulse (red) is preceded by a conditioning pulse (blue) over right ventral premotor cortex. The ratio of hand motor-evoked potential amplitude between the single pulse and paired pulse conditions during switch trials (switch PP/SP ratio) is our measure of cortico-cortical interaction. c Schematic of the action reprogramming task. The ratio between reaction times in stay trials and switch trials (switch RT cost) is our primary behavioural measure. d Transcranial magnetic stimulation set-up used to measure cortico-cortical interactions (left). Precise neuroanatomical targeting is achieved through continuous neuronavigation during stimulation, and confirmed with analysis of subject-specific target locations of stimulation (right).

ppTMS (Fig. 1b) was used to probe levels of directional inhibition from right ventral premotor cortex (PMv) to left primary motor cortex (M1) during the action reprogramming task (Fig. 1c, d). On half of all stimulation trials, participants were stimulated over M1 only, to determine their average motor-evoked potential (MEP) during single pulse stimulation. On the other half of trials, M1 stimulation was preceded by PMv stimulation, with the aim of quantifying the effect that activating PMv neurons would have on M1-driven MEP. By taking a ratio between MEPs in single pulse (M1 only) stimulation and paired stimulation (PMv and M1) during switch trials, we obtained the switch PP/SP ratio, a metric of the interaction between the two areas (Fig. 1b).

Physiological measures of interhemispheric cortico-cortical inhibition relate to myelination of the underlying long-range circuit

As hypothesised, we found that participants exhibiting the most inhibition from PMv to M1, as measured by a smaller switch PP/SP ratio, also have higher levels of myelin markers in white matter (Fig. 2a, peak pFisherFWE = 0.016), with correlations especially prominent in MT, R1 and R2* (r = −0.3839, r = −0.4120 and r = −0.4304 respectively, Fig. 2b). Within our whole-brain analysis, the areas where this correlation is significant are spatially asymmetric (Fig. 2a). In the left hemisphere, they cluster posteriorly, under the average location of the M1 coil; in the right hemisphere, they cluster slightly more anteriorly, under the average location of the PMv coil. Finally, the significant clusters in both hemispheres are connected by a cluster crossing the midline in the corpus callosum. This hints that myelination of the tract being stimulated may be uniquely important for PMv’s inhibition of M1 during action reprogramming.

Fig. 2. Multimodal joint inference reveals concordant relationships between interhemispheric cortico-cortical inhibition and myelin markers across modalities.

Fig. 2

a Result of joint inference across myelin markers. Clusters of significant voxels are shown in red (peak pFisherFWE = 0.016), overlaid on the white matter skeleton (green). Significance threshold for joint inference Fisher test is set at 0.05 after correcting for family-wise error correction. b Correlations between switch PP/SP ratio (paired pulse/single pulse ratio from ppTMS) and individual myelin markers. Each data point is a single participant; scatterplots and Spearman correlation effect sizes are presented for post hoc visualisation of the correlations, rather than for statistical inference.

To probe the specificity of the relationship between myelination and cortico-cortical interactions, we then ask whether this observed relationship is unique to the physiological interactions between cortical areas, or whether myelination also relates to local physiological inhibition in M1, or to behavioural performance during action reprogramming (Fig. S1). To test this, we used joint inference again and found that behavioural switch RT cost during the action reprogramming task does not significantly relate to myelin markers (peak pFisherFWE = 0.058). Switch M1 inhibition also does not significantly relate to myelin markers (peak pFisherFWE = 0.192). Finally, myelin markers are significantly less correlated to behavioural switch RT cost and switch M1 inhibition, than they are to switch PP/SP ratio (p = 0.0256 and p = 0.0014, respectively). In summary, correlations with myelin markers are not as strong for physiological measures of M1 inhibition or for behavioural measures that may reflect the integrated output of a broader set of brain regions rather than just PMv and M1.

Interhemispheric cortico-cortical inhibition mediates the link between myelin markers and action reprogramming behaviour

Cortico-cortical interactions are constrained by structural features of cortico-cortical connections77,78. Moreover, cortico-cortical connectivity shapes the dynamics of individual brain areas74,79, and in turn individual brain areas perform computations that directly regulate behavioural output. This is especially clear in the case of M1, whose computations directly determine movement80,81 and are input-driven82. Moreover, the inputs to M1 are also well-described, with excitatory premotor projections synapsing onto M1 interneurons45,48,64. Therefore, we take advantage of the well-established circuit properties of the motor network to hypothesise that cortico-cortical inhibition from right PMv to left M1 would relate to local cortical inhibition in M1, and that in turn M1 inhibition would relate to motor behaviour.

In support of this hypothesis (Fig. S2), we find that inhibition from PMv to M1, as measured by the switch PP/SP ratio, correlates with switch M1 inhibition (r = −0.5245, p < 0.0001), and that switch M1 inhibition correlates with behavioural output, as captured by switch RT cost (r = −0.3096, p = 0.0202), but we find no significant relationship between ppTMS inhibition and switch RT cost (r = 0.1925, p = 0.1551). Moreover, as mentioned previously, myelination correlates specifically with inhibition from PMv to M1, but not with switch M1 inhibition, nor with behavioural switch RT cost (pFisherFWE > 0.05, Fig. S1).

Given our strong, directional, a priori hypotheses on how myelination, cortico-cortical inhibition and action reprogramming behaviour would relate to each other, we formalise these relationships in a single statistical model, by means of a 2-mediator mediation analysis (Fig. 3). In cross-sectional designs, mediation analysis does not replace causal or pseudo-causal approaches, but is a powerful tool to test statistical inter-dependence between variables in a hypothesis-driven way, as we do here. In accordance with our hypothesis, we find that inhibition from PMv to M1, as measured by the switch PP/SP ratio (mediator 1) and switch M1 inhibition (mediator 2) sequentially mediate the link between myelination and action reprogramming behaviour (p = 0.0427, 95% boostrapped CI: −0.29 to −0.02, indirect link explaining 61.8% of the total effect). Finally, we find this mediation result to be specific to switch RT cost, as the same mediation does not explain variability in stay trial Reaction Times (p = 0.4488, Fig. S4).

Fig. 3. Motor network physiology links myelination to switching behaviour.

Fig. 3

a Mediation analysis reveals an indirect link between the myelin-related measure of interhemispheric cortico-cortical inhibition (Box X) and switch RT cost, a metric of performance during the action reprogramming task (Box Y). The 95% boostrapped CI for the indirect effect of sequential mediation of M1 and M2 is −0.29 to −0.02, which overall explained 61.8% of the total effect. b Inhibition from PMv to M1, as measured by ppTMS, correlates with switch M1 inhibition (r = −0.5245, p < 0.0001). Switch M1 inhibition correlates with action reprogramming behaviour, as measured by switch RT cost (r = −0.3096, p = 0.0202).

Tractography of stimulated white matter tracts based on individual stimulation sites

We then investigated the anatomical location of sites where myelin markers correlated with physiological measures of cortico-cortical interactions. By using information from individual participants’ cortical stimulation sites, we performed tractography to reconstruct estimates of the stimulated white matter fibres (Fig. 4 and Supplementary Movie 1). We find that in all individuals, the stimulated white matter fibres run interhemispherically through the body of the corpus callosum. Consistent with our hypotheses, the clusters where myelination markers correlate with stimulation-based physiological readouts are located in anatomical white matter areas that were consistently stimulated across all subjects.

Fig. 4. Tractography of stimulated white matter fibres.

Fig. 4

a We estimate individual-level and group-level maps of white matter fibres stimulated in our paradigm. b Left: the group-level map of tract overlap across subjects is shown in blue, overlaid on a coronal section. Locations for sagittal sections are indicated in green. Right: sagittal sections showing anatomical overlap between stimulated fibres (blue) and the cluster where myelin metrics correlate with stimulation-based metrics of cortico-cortical interactions (red).

Physiological measures of interhemispheric cortico-cortical inhibition do not correlate with demographic factors and features of M1 physiology

To probe whether the switch PP/SP ratio relates to non-specific individual differences across subjects, we tested correlations between switch PP/SP ratio, demographic factors, and features of primary motor cortex physiology (Fig. S4). We found that switch PP/SP ratio does not correlate with resting Motor Threshold (rMT, r = −0.2, p = 0.87), active Motor Threshold (aMT, r = 0.10, p = 0.46) or 1mV (r = −0.3, p = 0.82) measures. We also find that age and gender are not correlated with any other physiological metric, and in particular not with switch PP/SP ratio (age: r = 0.01, p = 0.94; gender: r = 0.004, p = 0.98).

Discussion

It has been repeatedly suggested that variability of white matter in the general population holds explanatory value concerning individual differences in brain physiology46,83, cognition84, and behaviour85,86. However, the features of white matter that are driving this meaningful variability are less well understood76. In recent years, white matter myelination has come to prominence as an underappreciated regulator of brain function, with studies demonstrating new roles such as trophic support of axons87,88 and also plastic potential21. Moreover, several strains of evidence have highlighted that myelination influences cognition and behaviour in ageing and in a range of pathologies8996. Therefore, it would make sense for interindividual variability in myelin to also play an important role in cognition and behaviour in health42, and potentially to underpin the previously reported relevance of white matter variability to wide-ranging behaviours. This study provides a direct test of that prediction, demonstrating that variability in white matter myelination of healthy adults holds meaningful explanatory information about physiological and behavioural processes, and opens the door to further studies exploring interindividual variability of myelination in the general population.

In the current study, we focused on the relationship between myelination of a well-defined white matter circuit (measured with multimodal MRI), the downstream physiological activity within that circuit (measured with ppTMS), and the resulting behaviour (measured with an Action Reprogramming Task). We find a link between metrics of physiological interactions between two brain areas and metrics of white matter myelination in the tract connecting the two areas. This relationship proves highly specific. It is common across multiple myelin markers, and a whole-brain voxelwise analysis reveals it to be localised to areas of the pathways stimulated by ppTMS. Taken together, these results argue for a specific link between the physiology of PMv to M1 projections, their myelination, and the action reprogramming behaviour they support.

Although the study highlights a robust relationship between the amount of myelin in a tract and its physiological properties, it is difficult to interpret which morphological aspects of a tract may be driving this effect. Levels of myelination in a voxel are influenced by a variety of factors, including myelin thickness, number of oligodendrocytes, length of Nodes of Ranvier (NoR), and axon packing density97. As a simplified example, a voxel with high MT could reflect a high number of myelinated axons, or thicker myelin sheaths on each axon18,33,97. Recent work has also highlighted the contributions that NoRs may play in dynamically regulating functional axonal properties14,98100, which our results are compatible with. To further the previous example, two voxels with the exact same number of axons may have different MT and R2* values if one of the voxels has shorter NoRs, therefore allowing more myelin to cluster within the same 3D volume97,101,102. In summary, our study is compatible with multiple morphological interpretations within white matter, but cannot distinguish between them. Future studies will be able to build upon our findings to tackle these biologically-driven questions by reverse-translation to rodent studies, where tools such as immunohistochemistry and electron microscopy would be able to answer more detailed cellular questions.

A key limitation of the study is that it employs non-invasive, MR-based metrics of myelination to quantify myelin. Drawing one-to-one relationships between biophysical signals detected from MR and the underlying biology is notoriously problematic33,97, and we took several steps to maximise the biological interpretability of our findings. Rather than focusing on an individual myelin marker, we collected a suite of four different myelin markers, each with a different profile of biophysical sensitivity35. We also used a joint inference framework when carrying out statistical analyses, so that we would only consider effects that are common across modalities. This minimises the risk that the link we discovered here is driven by other biological features of white matter: MT is also sensitive to oedema103, and R2* is also sensitive to vasculature104, but the only known signal that they both pick up is from tissue myelination. Although it comes with limitations, using non-invasive markers of myelination allows the gathering of insights and testing of hypotheses in humans that otherwise would be restricted to preclinical studies in animal models. In particular, taking this approach has allowed us to identify a new link between human behaviour and myelination that would have been inaccessible through invasive histology.

A foundational assumption of many in vitro studies of axonal properties is that the same principles that apply to relationships between an axon’s morphology and its physiological properties play out in similar ways at a larger scale in axon bundles within white matter tracts. The current results provide further confirmation of this assumption. Previous studies have found that increased myelination boosts conduction velocity and information transmission between individual neurons. Here, by probing cortico-cortical circuits with macroscopic imaging and stimulation methods, we show that a similar relationship exists for white matter tracts and cortex-to-cortex connectivity, whereby increased myelination of a tract also influences physiological interactions of one cortical area with another.

We studied brain-behaviour relationships in the context of a particular behavioural task—namely action reprogramming. Controlling what actions to execute, often termed ‘cognitive control’46, is a key function of the brain, and is likely an agglomeration of a variety of parallel and diverse mechanisms49,51,105107. We examined a specific component of ‘cognitive control’, i.e., action reprogramming52. Here, we show that at least some components of cognitive control can be explained in terms of physiological interactions, which are in turn driven by myelination of the underlying circuit. This constitutes a fundamental step forward in our understanding of the mechanisms underpinning cognitive control, and how we may be able to modulate them.

What implications do these results have for future studies on myelin and behaviour? While we found that the link between myelin and interhemispheric cortico-cortical inhibition is stronger than that between myelin and behavioural output, this is not evidence against the existence of a myelin-behaviour link. Rather, given our result finding a trend (p = 0.058) for the myelin-behaviour correlation of interest, it is possible that higher sample sizes would allow detection of significant myelin-behaviour correlations. This is in line with a previous study108 examining several behaviours and myelin markers, and concluding that while no significant myelin-behaviour relationships can be detected with 50 subjects, sample sizes between 50 and 200 subjects may be required to detect significant myelin-behaviour correlations in cross-sectional studies using multimodal MR-based myelin markers. Therefore, one implication of our study is that while around 50 participants are sufficient to detect significant myelin-physiology correlations, higher sample sizes (potentially up to 200 participants108), may be required to detect significant myelin-behaviour correlations.

Our findings also suggest that interindividual variability in tract myelination, tract physiology, and behaviour, are closely linked to each other. Subjects with higher levels of myelination exhibited stronger inhibition and lower switch RT costs during action reprogramming. Conversely, subjects with lower levels of myelination exhibited reduced inhibition and higher switch RT costs during action reprogramming. One potential interpretation of the finding could be that the chosen inter-pulse interval (IPI) of 6 ms is optimal in the participants with more myelin, and that participants with less myelin have delayed inhibition. This is in agreement with our data, as some subjects showed facilitation from right PMv to left M1, suggesting that their physiological inhibition is either lowered or delayed during action reprogramming. While additional imaging modalities (such as Magnetic Resonance Spectroscopy) would be needed to disentangle what precise features of cortical or white matter physiology lead to dampened inhibition in these subjects, our result suggest that participants displaying absent inhibition during action reprogramming also have higher switch RT cost, and that this phenomenon does therefore contribute to behavioural output.

These results illustrate a mechanistic link binding myelination of long-range circuits to their physiology and to action reprogramming. They highlight, for the first time, that myelin’s effects on axon conductance are also relevant at the larger spatial scales of tracts and translatable to the study of the live human brain. Taken together, this represents a first step towards translating studies on cellular-level properties of myelin to human-relevant knowledge. This is crucial for developing clinically-relevant insights into cortical circuits and to design therapeutic treatments aimed at modulating myelination.

Methods

Participants

Sixty-four healthy participants (36 female; mean age 24.69 years) underwent a single session of MRI and a separate TMS session which included task-based paired pulse TMS. TMS and MRI sessions were matched for time-of-day and were separated by a maximum of 1 week. All participants were self-assessed right-handed and their handedness was further confirmed through the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory109, (mean EHI score: 88.65). All participants were screened for TMS and MRI safety, received monetary compensation for their participation, and gave their informed consent to participate in this study. All study procedures were reviewed and approved by the local ethics committee at the University of Oxford (Central University Research Ethics Committee), and followed the Declaration of Helsinki.

Measuring myelination: acquisition of magnetic resonance imaging (MRI)

MRI data were collected with a 3.0-T Prisma Magnetom Siemens scanner, software version VE11C (Siemens Medical Systems, Erlangen, Germany). T1-weighted structural imaging (T1w), multi-parameter mapping (MPM) and DWI sequences were collected.

The T1w sequence (TR = 1900 ms, TE = 3.96 ms, voxel size = 1 mm isotropic, GRAPPA = 2) had a large field of view (FOV = 256 mm3)to allow for the nose and intertragic notches of the ears to be included in the image to facilitate later neuronavigation of the TMS coil to the target position.

Diffusion-weighted echo-planar imaging (EPI) scans (TR = 3070 ms, TE = 85.00 ms, FOV = 204 mm3, voxel size = 1.5 mm isotropic, multiband factor of 4) were collected for two b-values (500 and 2000 s/mm2), over 281 directions. An additional 23 volumes were acquired at b = 0, 15 in anterior-posterior phase-encoding direction and 8 in the posterior-anterior (PA) phase-encoding direction.

The MPM protocol40 included three multi-echo 3D FLASH (fast low-angle shot) scans with varying acquisition parameters, one RF transmit field map (B1 + map) and one static magnetic (B0) field map scan, for a total acquisition time of roughly 22 min. To correct for inter-scan motion, position-specific receive coil sensitivity field maps, matched in FOV to the MPM scans, were calculated and corrected for ref. 110. The three types of FLASH scans were designed to be predominantly T1-, PD-, or MT-weighted by changing the flip angle and the presence of a pre-pulse: 8 echoes were predominantly Proton Density-weighted (TR = 25 ms; flip angle = 6 degrees; TE = 2.3–18.4 ms), 8 echoes were predominantly T1-weighted (TR = 25 ms; flip angle = 21 degrees; TE = 2.3–18.4 ms) and 6 echoes were predominantly Magnetisation Transfer-weighted (MTw, TR = 25 ms; flip angle = 21 degrees; TE = 2.3–13.8 ms). For MTw scans, excitation was preceded by off-resonance Gaussian MT pulse of 4 ms duration, nominal flip angle, 2 kHz frequency offset from water resonance. All FLASH scans had 1 mm isotropic resolution and FOV of 256 × 224 × 176 mm3. The B1 map was acquired through an EPI-based sequence featuring spin and stimulated echoes (SE and STE) with 11 nominal flip angles, FOV of 192 × 192 × 256 mm3 and TR of 500 ms. The TE was 37.06 ms, and the mixing time was 33.8 ms. The B0 map was acquired to correct the B1+ map for distortions due to off-resonance effects. The B0 map sequence had a TR of 1020.0 ms, first TE of 10 ms, second TE of 12.46 ms, FOV of 192 × 192 × 256 mm3 and read-out bandwidth of 260 Hz/pixel.

MRI scan preprocessing, analysis and statistical comparisons were performed using FMRIB Software Library (FSL, v6.0), except for the MPM quantitative map estimation step which was carried out using the hMRI toolbox implemented in Matlab-based SPM, as described in ref. 111. All T1w images were preprocessed through a standard FreeSurfer-based pipeline112,113 to correct for bias field and achieve ACPC alignment (for use in Neuronavigation).

An automated custom pipeline based on existing FSL tools was also developed for our diffusion sequence. The topup tool was run on average images of AP b0 volumes and PA b0 volumes. The resulting susceptibility-induced off-resonance field was used as an input for the eddy tool114, which was run with options optimised for multiband diffusion data to correct for eddy currents and subject movement. To generate fractional anistropy (FA) maps, a diffusion tensor model was fit to each voxel through DTIFIT, optimised for multi-shell data processing with options such as –kurt.

MT saturation, R1 and R2* quantitative maps were estimated through the hMRI toolbox111. In order to register MPM volumes to FA volumes, we used the following steps. Boundary-based registration was used to calculate a DWI-to-T1w registration using AP b0 images (with high tissue boundary contrast). A customised pipeline was used to ACPC align the MPM maps and register them to T1w space. At this stage, one participant was excluded as the MPM scan was heavily corrupted due to movement artefacts; one participant was excluded due to lower quality signal in the MPM scan, which resulted in poor registrations with other modalities. Once registration matrices for MPM-T1w and DWI-T1w were calculated, they were inverted, concatenated and applied as needed to bring MPM volumes into DWI space with minimal interpolation.

Measuring myelination: joint inference across myelin-sensitive MRI modalities

To bring all volumes into a common space, native FA volumes were skeletonised with tract-based spatial statistics (TBSS), and the skeletonisation transforms were subsequently applied to MPM-to-DWI registered volumes. Group-level analyses were then conducted in skeleton space for all data, and using rank-based inverse-normal transformations on the regressors. To uncover common trends across modalities (i.e., trends compatible with an effect of myelin), voxelwise joint inference was performed through permutation analysis of linear models, which implements a voxelwise Fisher test with the following equation (as described in ref. 43):

2K=1Kln(pk) 1

with pk being each modality’s p value and K being the total number of modalities being combined (full code available here: https://git.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/alazari/macroscopic-link).

As a second step, to extract a single myelin metric from all myelin markers for use in the comparisons between correlations and in the mediation analyses, we used dimensionality reduction through principal component analysis (PCA). The follow-up analyses focused on voxels identified as significant through joint inference; within these voxels, fslmeants was used to extract the mean value for each modality. A PCA was then used in order to reduce the dimensionality of the normalised data, an approach already previously described for white matter data115. As the first principal component captured 69.22% of total variance, the subject-level scores for the first principal component were then used as the predictor variable in the mediation analysis (full code available here: https://git.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/alazari/macroscopic-link).

Measuring action reprogramming

The action reprogramming task was implemented based on a task originally developed for monkeys52,116, and then adapted for use in human TMS studies46,50. The task aimed to probe action execution and action reprogramming. Cues consisted of a central square (either red or green) with two ‘flanker’ squares (one red and one green). Participants were instructed to press the button on the side where the flanker colour matched the colour of the central square. The flankers kept switching sides at random, whereas the central square was always the same colour for 3–7 consecutive trials at the time. The central square was white when first appearing on the screen, and then turned green/red 450–600 ms after the flankers were presented. Thus the 450–600 ms interval allowed participants to generate a motor plan prior to motor execution towards either the left or right flanker given the expectation that they had about what the central cue colour change would be. This way, participants simply had to execute a pre-prepared movement when the central cue colour stayed the same (‘stay trials’), but they had to inhibit the movement and carry out a different one in the trials where the central cue colour had switched (‘switch trials’). All stimulus presentation timings mirrored ones used in previous literature46, including the inter-trial interval (1000 ms) and intervals between flanker and cue presentation (450–600 ms). Each participant underwent a session of 112 switch trials and corresponding stay trials (for a total of 678 trials), resulting in 28 TMS stimulations per condition in each participant117. Each session included three automatic breaks in between to ensure participants had a chance to rest if they wished to do so.

Participants were told to be as fast and accurate as they could. They received detailed task instructions in paper format at the beginning of each session; in addition, the instructions were reiterated in a computer-based fashion at the beginning of the task (full code available here: https://git.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/alazari/macroscopic-link). Before the task, they undertook roughly 100 trials to make sure first that they understood the rules of the task, that they had habituated to the task, and that they could tolerate TMS pulses while performing the task. Participants were pressing the two buttons with index fingers from their two hands, and were instructed to keep their hands relaxed between trials until the cue appeared; they were occasionally reminded to keep their hands relaxed if the experimenter observed noticeable muscle contractions between trials.

Our primary behavioural measure was the ratio between reaction time between switch trials and stay trials. We shall refer to this as ‘switch RT cost’. Switch RT cost is calculated as follows:

SwitchRTCost=μSwitchTrialRTsμStayTrialRTs 2

where μ is the mean of the reaction times (RTs) being considered. Switch RT cost always has a positive value; the higher the value, the higher the switch RT cost.

Measuring cortico-cortical inhibition: paired pulse TMS (ppTMS)

Detailed methods for TMS and neuronavigation set-ups are described in Supplementary Methods. In brief, two DuoMAG MP-Dual TMS monophasic stimulators (DeyMed DuoMag, Rogue Resolutions Ltd.) were used to deliver pulses via two figure-eight coils, one over left M1 and one over right PMv. All stimulation was delivered using continuous tracking of coil location with respect to subject neuroanatomy (i.e., neuronavigation) using a Polaris camera and the Brainsight (Rogue Resolutions, Inc.) software. Electromyography (EMG) was recorded from the participant’s right hand in a tendon-belly montage, in order to record TMS-induced MEPs from the hand’s First Dorsal Interosseus muscle (see Supplementary Methods). TMS was delivered during an Action Reprogramming task (see above).

Cortico-cortical interactions were measured by paired pulse TMS (ppTMS), as follows. On half of all stimulation trials, participants were stimulated over left M1 only; on the other half of trials, the same stimulation was preceded by a conditioning right PMv pulse 6 ms earlier. Although MEPs reflect the connectivity between M1 and the periphery, our focus in the current report is not on the MEPs per se but on their modulation by the induction of activity within PMv which a number of studies in both non-human and human primates demonstrate reflect the interactions between PMv and M15460. As both the M1 single pulse condition and the paired pulse PMv-M1 condition elicited the same level of peripheral stimulation, the overall paradigm allows us to control for corticospinal and peripheral influences118.

We decided on a 6 ms IPI based on previous work46 and after exclusion of other potential IPIs. We excluded higher IPIs, as IPIs of 9–18 ms have been shown to engage poly-synaptic subcortical pathways46. We also excluded lower IPIs, as they reflect variability in inhibitory intracortical circuits (which are engaged at 1–4 ms IPI) and would likely not engage transcallosal fibres sufficiently119,120. Together with evidence from other ppTMS studies on this circuit55,56, we concluded that focussing on a single IPI of 6 ms was the ideal strategy to isolate the contribution of transcallosal myelinated fibres to the ppTMS effect.

The pulse over M1 was always delivered 175ms after cue onset—based on previous experiments46—and set at 1mV intensity (see Supplementary Methods); the pulse over PMv was always delivered at 110% of resting Motor Threshold (rMT, see Supplementary Methods). Pulses were randomly assigned in an equal fashion to stay vs. switch trials (when pre-prepared actions were either executed or inhibited) and to right-hand vs. left-hand trials.

EMG analysis focused on TMS-induced MEPs, and more specifically on the peak-to-peak amplitude of MEPs. MEP recording, preprocessing and analysis procedures were identical for all experiments. MEPs were identified based on recorded TMS timings, preprocessed in MATLAB (Version R2018b, Mathworks, MA, USA) and visualised for quality control purposes. Absent or unusually small MEPs (<0.2 mV) or unusually high MEPs (>9 mV) were excluded from further analyses. The average rejection rate for unusually small MEPs was 1.25, and 0.84% for unusually high MEPs. Trials with incorrect, premature (<150 ms) or slow (>800 ms) responses were also excluded from further analyses. Trials with significant ‘precontraction’ (i.e., muscle contraction above 0.4 mV in the 100 ms before the MEP and the test pulse) were also excluded. Once all exclusion criteria were applied, Grubb’s outlier detection procedure was carried out on the data. Six participants had fewer than nine MEPs per condition which could be included, and were thus excluded from further analyses.

After preprocessing, MEP ratios (paired pulse TMS/single pulse TMS) were obtained from the median normalised MEP in switch trials. We shall refer to this as ‘switch PP/SP ratio’. Similar to previous studies46, calculation of this metric for switch trials did not involve any data from stay trials, in order to minimise the influence from excitatory processes happening during action execution45,46. Switch PP/SP ratio was used as our primary measures of cortico-cortical inhibition, and was calculated as follows:

SwitchPP/SPratio=MdSwitchTrialPairedPulseMEPsMdSwitchTrialSinglePulseMEPs 3

where Md is the median of the total MEPs being considered. Switch PP/SP ratio always has a positive value; the lower the value, the higher the interhemispheric inhibition.

Measuring task-dependent inhibition of primary motor cortex (M1)

To obtain a more fine-grained picture of dynamics in M1, we estimated the extent of its task-dependent inhibition. This was done by stimulating the motor hotspot at 1 mV with single pulse TMS; in half the TMS trials this was done during stay trials, and in the other half this was done during switch trials. The pulse delivered to M1 was always delivered 175 ms after cue onset and pulses were randomly assigned in an equal fashion to stay vs. switch trials. We find that MEPs driven by M1 stimulation decrease significantly during switch (action reprogramming) trials compared to stay (action execution) trials (Wilcoxon matched-pairs rank test: median of differences: −0.2849, 95% confidence interval of median of differences: −0.3955 to −0.1563, p < 0.001).

The ratio between median MEP on switch and stay trials was calculated for each subject, and used as our primary measure of task-dependent inhibition of M1. We shall refer to this as ‘switch M1 inhibition’. Switch M1 inhibition was calculated as follows:

SwitchM1inhibition=MdSwitchTrialSinglePulseMEPsMdStayTrialSinglePulseMEPs 4

where Md is the median of the total MEPs being considered.

Tractography of stimulated white matter fibres

White matter bundles connecting the stimulated cortical sites were estimated through Probtrackx121. Regions of interest (ROI) in the cortex were based on the neuronavigation-derived sites for each participant (see Supplementary Methods). This ensured that in each subject, the region of interest used for tractography corresponded to the site that was stimulated via TMS. As the motor hotspot does not always overlap with the postcentral gyral fold, and a larger coil was used for M1 compared to PMv, the motor hotspot ROI was enlarged to a 3 cm radius to improve the output tract quality. Tractography was run in native DWI space, with outputs in Montreal Neurological Institute space to enable pooling of results across all subjects. Individual-level maps of streamline densities were then thresholded at 1% of the number of valid streamlines and overlaid.

Statistical comparison between correlations and mediation analysis

Differences between correlations were tested in the R package psych, using Fisher’s z-transformed correlation coefficients. As all correlations tested originated from the same sample, a paired test was used to account for the dependency between correlations being compared.

Mediation analyses were run in PROCESS (to derive 95% confidence intervals122) and in BRAVO (to derive p values123, which cannot be derived in PROCESS for our model). In both toolboxes, we used a significant indirect (X-M) pathway as the key requirement to test mediation, and applied bootstrapping with 10,000 repetitions122,124. Two-tailed tests were used for all correlation and mediation analyses.

Reporting summary

Further information on research design is available in the Nature Research Reporting Summary linked to this article.

Supplementary information

Peer Review File (355.5KB, pdf)
41467_2022_31687_MOESM3_ESM.pdf (77.8KB, pdf)

Description of Additional Supplementary Files

Supplementary Movie 1 (7.3MB, mov)
Reporting Summary (297KB, pdf)

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Ioana Grigoras, Jack Miller, Tulika Nandi, Caroline Nettekoven, David Oliver, Marleen Schönfeld and Tom Smejka for their assistance with performing TMS sessions. We are grateful to Alex S. Bates, Antoine Cherix, Ilona Lipp and Laia Serratosa Capdevila for their input on previous versions of the manuscript. We would also like to thank Martina Callaghan from University College London for the MPM sequence; Juliet Semple, Nicola Aikin and Sebastian Rieger for their technical support and help with scanning participants; and Matthew Webster for wide-ranging help from IT to statistical issues. We acknowledge the IT-related support provided by David Flitney and Duncan Mortimer throughout the project. This work was supported by a PhD Studentship awarded to A.L. from the Wellcome Trust (109062/Z/15/Z) and by a Principal Research Fellowship from the Wellcome Trust to H.J.B. (110027/Z/15/Z). C.J.S. holds a Sir Henry Dale Fellowship, funded by the Wellcome Trust and the Royal Society (102584/Z/13/Z). B.G. is supported by funding from the Rhodes Trust. J.K. is supported by a Sir Henry Wellcome Postdoctoral Fellowship (204696/Z/16/Z). The project was supported by the NIHR Oxford Health Biomedical Research Centre. TMS work was supported by the University of Oxford John Fell Fund and the University of Oxford Wellcome Trust Institutional Strategic Support Fund (to C.J.S.). The Wellcome Centre for Integrative Neuroimaging is supported by core funding from the Wellcome Trust (203139/Z/16/Z).

Source data

Source Data (16.4KB, xlsx)

Author contributions

Conceptualisation: A.L., H.J.B. and M.F.S.R. Funding acquisition and project administration: A.L. and H.J.B. Investigation: A.L., O.J.W., B.G. and L.V. Formal analysis: A.L. Software and methodology: P.S., L.V., M.C., D.P., J.K., M.W. and C.J.S. Supervision: H.J.B., M.F.S.R., L.V. and P.S. Writing—original draft: A.L. and H.J.B. Writing—review and editing: all authors.

Peer review

Peer review information

Nature Communications thanks Julie Duque and the other, anonymous, reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work. Peer reviewer reports are available.

Data availability

Data used in this study are only available upon request due to data protection considerations. Source data are provided with this paper.

Code availability

Custom-written code used for data collection and for joint inference across myelin-sensitive modalities is available here: https://git.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/alazari/macroscopic-link.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Contributor Information

Alberto Lazari, Email: alberto.lazari@ndcn.ox.ac.uk.

Heidi Johansen-Berg, Email: heidi.johansen-berg@ndcn.ox.ac.uk.

Supplementary information

The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41467-022-31687-5.

References

  • 1.Rushton W. A theory of the effects of fibre size in medullated nerve. J. Physiol. 1951;115:101–122. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1951.sp004655. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Brill M, Waxman S, Moore J, Joyner R. Conduction velocity and spike configuration in myelinated fibres: computed dependence on internode distance. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry. 1977;40:769–774. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.40.8.769. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Waxman SG. Determinants of conduction velocity in myelinated nerve fibers. Muscle Nerve. 1980;3:141–150. doi: 10.1002/mus.880030207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Schauf C, Davis FA. Impulse conduction in multiple sclerosis: a theoretical basis for modification by temperature and pharmacological agents. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry. 1974;37:152–161. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.37.2.152. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Verhoeven K, et al. Slowed conduction and thin myelination of peripheral nerves associated with mutant rho guanine-nucleotide exchange factor 10. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 2003;73:926–932. doi: 10.1086/378159. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Caminiti R, et al. Diameter, length, speed, and conduction delay of callosal axons in macaque monkeys and humans: comparing data from histology and magnetic resonance imaging diffusion tractography. J. Neurosci. 2013;33:14501–14511. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0761-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Etxeberria A, et al. Dynamic modulation of myelination in response to visual stimuli alters optic nerve conduction velocity. J. Neurosci. 2016;36:6937–6948. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0908-16.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Goldman L, Albus JS. Computation of impulse conduction in myelinated fibers; theoretical basis of the velocity-diameter relation. Biophys. J. 1968;8:596–607. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3495(68)86510-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Smith RS, Koles ZJ. Myelinated nerve fibers: computed effect of myelin thickness on conduction velocity. Am. J. Physiol. Leg. Content. 1970;219:1256–1258. doi: 10.1152/ajplegacy.1970.219.5.1256. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Moore JW, Joyner RW, Brill MH, Waxman SD, Najar-Joa M. Simulations of conduction in uniform myelinated fibers. relative sensitivity to changes in nodal and internodal parameters. Biophys. J. 1978;21:147–160. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3495(78)85515-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Saab AS, et al. Oligodendroglial NMDA receptors regulate glucose import and axonal energy metabolism. Neuron. 2016;91:119–132. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.05.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Moore, S. et al. A role of oligodendrocytes in information processing independent of conduction velocity. 10.1101/736975 (2019).
  • 13.Seidl AH, Rubel EW, Harris DM. Mechanisms for adjusting interaural time differences to achieve binaural coincidence detection. J. Neurosci. 2010;30:70–80. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3464-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Ford MC, et al. Tuning of Ranvier node and internode properties in myelinated axons to adjust action potential timing. Nat. Commun. 2015;6:1–14. doi: 10.1038/ncomms9073. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Salami M, Itami C, Tsumoto T, Kimura F. Change of conduction velocity by regional myelination yields constant latency irrespective of distance between thalamus and cortex. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2003;100:6174–6179. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0937380100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Lang EJ, Rosenbluth J. Role of myelination in the development of a uniform olivocerebellar conduction time. J. Neurophysiol. 2003;89:2259–2270. doi: 10.1152/jn.00922.2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Pajevic S, Basser PJ, Fields RD. Role of myelin plasticity in oscillations and synchrony of neuronal activity. Neuroscience. 2014;276:135–147. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.11.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Kaller MS, Lazari A, Blanco-Duque C, Sampaio-Baptista C, Johansen-Berg H. Myelin plasticity and behaviour-connecting the dots. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2017;47:86–92. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2017.09.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Weiskopf, N., Edwards, L. J., Helms, G., Mohammadi, S. & Kirilina, E. Quantitative magnetic resonance imaging of brain anatomy and in vivo histology. Nat. Rev. Phys.3, 1–19 (2021).
  • 20.Sagi Y, et al. Learning in the fast lane: new insights into neuroplasticity. Neuron. 2012;73:1195–203. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.01.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Sampaio-Baptista C, et al. Motor skill learning induces changes in white matter microstructure and myelination. J. Neurosci. 2013;33:19499–19503. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3048-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Glasser MF, et al. A multi-modal parcellation of human cerebral cortex. Nature. 2016;536:171–178. doi: 10.1038/nature18933. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Assaf Y, Bouznach A, Zomet O, Marom A, Yovel Y. Conservation of brain connectivity and wiring across the mammalian class. Nat. Neurosci. 2020;23:805–808. doi: 10.1038/s41593-020-0641-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Movahedian Attar F, et al. Mapping short association fibers in the early cortical visual processing stream using in vivo diffusion tractography. Cereb. Cortex. 2020;30:4496–4514. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhaa049. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Kirilina E, et al. Superficial white matter imaging: Contrast mechanisms and whole-brain in vivo mapping. Sci. Adv. 2020;6:eaaz9281. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.aaz9281. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Moseley M, et al. Early detection of regional cerebral ischemia in cats: comparison of diffusion-and T2-weighted MRI and spectroscopy. Magn. Reson. Med. 1990;14:330–346. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910140218. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Basser PJ, Mattiello J, LeBihan D. Mr diffusion tensor spectroscopy and imaging. Biophysical J. 1994;66:259–267. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3495(94)80775-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Chenevert TL, Brunberg JA, Pipe JG. Anisotropic diffusion in human white matter: demonstration with MR techniques in vivo. Radiology. 1990;177:401–405. doi: 10.1148/radiology.177.2.2217776. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Doran M, et al. Normal and abnormal white matter tracts shown by MR imaging using directional diffusion weighted sequences. J. Comput. Assist. Tomogr. 1990;14:865–873. doi: 10.1097/00004728-199011000-00001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Cleveland G, Chang D, Hazlewood C, Rorschach H. Nuclear magnetic resonance measurement of skeletal muscle: anisotrophy of the diffusion coefficient of the intracellular water. Biophys. J. 1976;16:1043–1053. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3495(76)85754-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Le DB, Turner R, Douek P. Is water diffusion restricted in human brain white matter? An echo-planar NMR imaging study. Neuroreport. 1993;4:887–890. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199307000-00012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Moseley ME, et al. Diffusion-weighted MR imaging of anisotropic water diffusion in cat central nervous system. Radiology. 1990;176:439–445. doi: 10.1148/radiology.176.2.2367658. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Zatorre RJ, Fields RD, Johansen-Berg H. Plasticity in gray and white: neuroimaging changes in brain structure during learning. Nat. Neurosci. 2012;15:528–536. doi: 10.1038/nn.3045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Mancini M, et al. An interactive meta-analysis of mri biomarkers of myelin. Elife. 2020;9:e61523. doi: 10.7554/eLife.61523. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Lazari, A. & Lipp, I. Can MRI measure myelin? Systematic review, qualitative assessment, and meta-analysis of studies validating microstructural imaging with myelin histology. NeuroImage230, 117744 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 36.Sled JG, Pike GB. Quantitative imaging of magnetization transfer exchange and relaxation properties in vivo using MRI. Magn. Reson. Med. 2001;46:923–931. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1278. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Yarnykh VL. Pulsed z-spectroscopic imaging of cross-relaxation parameters in tissues for human MRI: theory and clinical applications. Magn. Reson. Med. 2002;47:929–939. doi: 10.1002/mrm.10120. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Tofts P, et al. Tozer et al. quantitative magnetization transfer mapping of bound protons in multiple sclerosis Magn Reson Med 2003;50:83–91. Magn. Reson. Med. 2005;53:492–493. doi: 10.1002/mrm.20370. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Heath F, Hurley SA, Johansen-Berg H, Sampaio-Baptista C. Advances in noninvasive myelin imaging. Dev. Neurobiol. 2018;78:136–151. doi: 10.1002/dneu.22552. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Weiskopf N, et al. Quantitative multi-parameter mapping of r1, pd*, mt, and r2* at 3t: a multi-center validation. Front. Neurosci. 2013;7:95. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2013.00095. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Lutti A, Dick F, Sereno MI, Weiskopf N. Using high-resolution quantitative mapping of r1 as an index of cortical myelination. Neuroimage. 2014;93:176–188. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.06.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Dubbioso, R., Madsen, K. H., Thielscher, A. & Siebner, H. R. The myelin content of the human precentral hand knob reflects inter-individual differences in manual motor control at the physiological and behavioural level. J. Neurosci.41, 3163–3179 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 43.Winkler AM, et al. Non-parametric combination and related permutation tests for neuroimaging. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2016;37:1486–1511. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Friston KJ, Harrison L, Penny W. Dynamic causal modelling. Neuroimage. 2003;19:1273–1302. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00202-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Buch ER, Mars RB, Boorman ED, Rushworth MF. A network centered on ventral premotor cortex exerts both facilitatory and inhibitory control over primary motor cortex during action reprogramming. J. Neurosci. 2010;30:1395–1401. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4882-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Neubert F-X, Mars RB, Buch ER, Olivier E, Rushworth MF. Cortical and subcortical interactions during action reprogramming and their related white matter pathways. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2010;107:13240–13245. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1000674107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Lazari, A., Giuffre, A. & Nandi, T. White matter damage and altered connectivity between primary motor cortices in chronic obstructive pulmonary disease. J. Physiol.599, 1367–1369 (2020). [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 48.Davare M, Rothwell JC, Lemon RN. Causal connectivity between the human anterior intraparietal area and premotor cortex during grasp. Curr. Biol. 2010;20:176–181. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.11.063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Mars RB, Piekema C, Coles MG, Hulstijn W, Toni I. On the programming and reprogramming of actions. Cereb. Cortex. 2007;17:2972–2979. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Mars RB, et al. Short-latency influence of medial frontal cortex on primary motor cortex during action selection under conflict. J. Neurosci. 2009;29:6926–6931. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1396-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Forstmann BU, et al. Function and structure of the right inferior frontal cortex predict individual differences in response inhibition: a model-based approach. J. Neurosci. 2008;28:9790–9796. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1465-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Isoda M, Hikosaka O. Switching from automatic to controlled action by monkey medial frontal cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 2007;10:240–248. doi: 10.1038/nn1830. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Dum RP, Strick PL. Frontal lobe inputs to the digit representations of the motor areas on the lateral surface of the hemisphere. J. Neurosci. 2005;25:1375–1386. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3902-04.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Cerri G, Shimazu H, Maier M, Lemon R. Facilitation from ventral premotor cortex of primary motor cortex outputs to macaque hand muscles. J. Neurophysiol. 2003;90:832–842. doi: 10.1152/jn.01026.2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Davare M, Lemon R, Olivier E. Selective modulation of interactions between ventral premotor cortex and primary motor cortex during precision grasping in humans. J. Physiol. 2008;586:2735–2742. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2008.152603. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Davare M, Montague K, Olivier E, Rothwell JC, Lemon RN. Ventral premotor to primary motor cortical interactions during object-driven grasp in humans. Cortex. 2009;45:1050–1057. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.02.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Kraskov A, Prabhu G, Quallo MM, Lemon RN, Brochier T. Ventral premotor–motor cortex interactions in the macaque monkey during grasp: response of single neurons to intracortical microstimulation. J. Neurosci. 2011;31:8812–8821. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0525-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Prabhu G, et al. Modulation of primary motor cortex outputs from ventral premotor cortex during visually guided grasp in the macaque monkey. J. Physiol. 2009;587:1057–1069. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2008.165571. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Shimazu H, Maier MA, Cerri G, Kirkwood PA, Lemon RN. Macaque ventral premotor cortex exerts powerful facilitation of motor cortex outputs to upper limb motoneurons. J. Neurosci. 2004;24:1200–1211. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4731-03.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Romero MC, Davare M, Armendariz M, Janssen P. Neural effects of transcranial magnetic stimulation at the single-cell level. Nat. Commun. 2019;10:1–11. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-07882-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Godschalk M, Lemon RN, Kuypers HG, Ronday H. Cortical afferents and efferents of monkey postarcuate area: an anatomical and electrophysiological study. Exp. brain Res. 1984;56:410–424. doi: 10.1007/BF00237982. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Jenny A. Commissural projections of the cortical hand motor area in monkeys. J. Comp. Neurol. 1979;188:137–145. doi: 10.1002/cne.901880111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Boussaoud D, Tanné-Gariépy J, Wannier T, Rouiller EM. Callosal connections of dorsal versus ventral premotor areas in the macaque monkey: a multiple retrograde tracing study. BMC Neurosci. 2005;6:67. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-6-67. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Tokuno H, Nambu A. Organization of nonprimary motor cortical inputs on pyramidal and nonpyramidal tract neurons of primary motor cortex: an electrophysiological study in the macaque monkey. Cereb. Cortex. 2000;10:58–68. doi: 10.1093/cercor/10.1.58. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Bäumer T, et al. Inhibitory and facilitatory connectivity from ventral premotor to primary motor cortex in healthy humans at rest–a bifocal tms study. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2009;120:1724–1731. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2009.07.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Buch ER, Johnen VM, Nelissen N, O’Shea J, Rushworth MF. Noninvasive associative plasticity induction in a corticocortical pathway of the human brain. J. Neurosci. 2011;31:17669–17679. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1513-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Dancause N, Barbay S, Frost SB, Mahnken JD, Nudo RJ. Interhemispheric connections of the ventral premotor cortex in a new world primate. J. Comp. Neurol. 2007;505:701–715. doi: 10.1002/cne.21531. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Lanz F, et al. Distant heterotopic callosal connections to premotor cortex in non-human primates. Neuroscience. 2017;344:56–66. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.12.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Johnen VM, et al. Causal manipulation of functional connectivity in a specific neural pathway during behaviour and at rest. Elife. 2015;4:e04585. doi: 10.7554/eLife.04585. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Sel, A. et al. Increasing and decreasing interregional brain coupling increases and decreases oscillatory activity in the human brain. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA118, e2100652118 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 71.Calford MB, Tweedale R. Interhemispheric transfer of plasticity in the cerebral cortex. Science. 1990;249:805–807. doi: 10.1126/science.2389146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.van der Knaap LJ, van der Ham IJ. How does the corpus callosum mediate interhemispheric transfer? A review. Behav. Brain Res. 2011;223:211–221. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.04.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Fling BW, Benson BL, Seidler RD. Transcallosal sensorimotor fiber tract structure-function relationships. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2013;34:384–395. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21437. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Bachtiar V, et al. Modulating regional motor cortical excitability with noninvasive brain stimulation results in neurochemical changes in bilateral motor cortices. J. Neurosci. 2018;38:7327–7336. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2853-17.2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Krupnik R, Yovel Y, Assaf Y. Inner hemispheric and interhemispheric connectivity balance in the human brain. J. Neurosci. 2021;41:8351–8361. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1074-21.2021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Lazari, A. et al. Heterogeneous relationships between white matter and behaviour. bioRxiv10.1101/2020.12.15.422826 (2020).
  • 77.Grandjean J, Zerbi V, Balsters JH, Wenderoth N, Rudin M. Structural basis of large-scale functional connectivity in the mouse. J. Neurosci. 2017;37:8092–8101. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0438-17.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Hermundstad AM, et al. Structural foundations of resting-state and task-based functional connectivity in the human brain. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2013;110:6169–6174. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1219562110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Lindenberg R, Nachtigall L, Meinzer M, Sieg MM, Flöel A. Differential effects of dual and unihemispheric motor cortex stimulation in older adults. J. Neurosci. 2013;33:9176–9183. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0055-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Churchland MM, Shenoy KV. Temporal complexity and heterogeneity of single-neuron activity in premotor and motor cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 2007;97:4235–4257. doi: 10.1152/jn.00095.2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Elsayed GF, Lara AH, Kaufman MT, Churchland MM, Cunningham JP. Reorganization between preparatory and movement population responses in motor cortex. Nat. Commun. 2016;7:1–15. doi: 10.1038/ncomms13239. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Sauerbrei BA, et al. Cortical pattern generation during dexterous movement is input-driven. Nature. 2020;577:386–391. doi: 10.1038/s41586-019-1869-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Boorman ED, O’Shea J, Sebastian C, Rushworth MF, Johansen-Berg H. Individual differences in white-matter microstructure reflect variation in functional connectivity during choice. Curr. Biol. 2007;17:1426–1431. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.07.040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Matejko AA, Ansari D. Drawing connections between white matter and numerical and mathematical cognition: a literature review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2015;48:35–52. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.11.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Kanai R, Rees G. The structural basis of inter-individual differences in human behaviour and cognition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2011;12:231–242. doi: 10.1038/nrn3000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Boekel W, et al. A purely confirmatory replication study of structural brain-behavior correlations. Cortex. 2015;66:115–133. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2014.11.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Nave K-A. Myelination and support of axonal integrity by glia. Nature. 2010;468:244–252. doi: 10.1038/nature09614. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Fünfschilling U, et al. Glycolytic oligodendrocytes maintain myelin and long-term axonal integrity. Nature. 2012;485:517–521. doi: 10.1038/nature11007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Shen S, et al. Age-dependent epigenetic control of differentiation inhibitors is critical for remyelination efficiency. Nat. Neurosci. 2008;11:1024–1034. doi: 10.1038/nn.2172. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Ruckh JM, et al. Rejuvenation of regeneration in the aging central nervous system. Cell Stem Cell. 2012;10:96–103. doi: 10.1016/j.stem.2011.11.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Zonouzi M, et al. Gabaergic regulation of cerebellar ng2 cell development is altered in perinatal white matter injury. Nat. Neurosci. 2015;18:674–682. doi: 10.1038/nn.3990. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Weil M-T, et al. Loss of myelin basic protein function triggers myelin breakdown in models of demyelinating diseases. Cell Rep. 2016;16:314–322. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2016.06.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Lakhani B, Hayward KS, Boyd LA. Hemispheric asymmetry in myelin after stroke is related to motor impairment and function. NeuroImage: Clin. 2017;14:344–353. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2017.01.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Hill RA, Li AM, Grutzendler J. Lifelong cortical myelin plasticity and age-related degeneration in the live mammalian brain. Nat. Neurosci. 2018;21:683–695. doi: 10.1038/s41593-018-0120-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Cabibel V, et al. Is bilateral corticospinal connectivity impaired in patients with chronic obstructive pulmonary disease? J. Physiol. 2020;598:4591–4602. doi: 10.1113/JP279560. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Forbes TA, et al. Environmental enrichment ameliorates perinatal brain injury and promotes functional white matter recovery. Nat. Commun. 2020;11:1–17. doi: 10.1038/s41467-020-14762-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Walhovd KB, Johansen-Berg H, Karadottir RT. Unraveling the secrets of white matter–bridging the gap between cellular, animal and human imaging studies. Neuroscience. 2014;276:2–13. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.06.058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Arancibia-Carcamo IL, et al. Node of ranvier length as a potential regulator of myelinated axon conduction speed. Elife. 2017;6:e23329. doi: 10.7554/eLife.23329. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Dutta DJ, et al. Regulation of myelin structure and conduction velocity by perinodal astrocytes. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2018;115:11832–11837. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1811013115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Lazari A, Koudelka S, Sampaio-Baptista C. Experience-related reductions of myelin and axon diameter in adulthood. J. Neurophysiol. 2018;120:1772–1775. doi: 10.1152/jn.00070.2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Andersson M, et al. Axon morphology is modulated by the local environment and impacts the noninvasive investigation of its structure–function relationship. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2020;117:33649–33659. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2012533117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Cottaar, M. et al. Diffusion-prepared phase imaging (DIPPI): quantifying myelin in crossing fibres. bioRxiv10.1101/2020.11.10.376657 (2020).
  • 103.Cook LL, Foster PJ, Mitchell JR, Karlik SJ. In vivo 4.0-T magnetic resonance investigation of spinal cord inflammation, demyelination, and axonal damage in chronic-progressive experimental allergic encephalomyelitis. J. Magn. Reson. Imaging. 2004;20:563–571. doi: 10.1002/jmri.20171. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Weisskoff RM, Kiihne S. MRI susceptometry: image-based measurement of absolute susceptibility of MR contrast agents and human blood. Magn. Reson. Med. 1992;24:375–383. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910240219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Ridderinkhof KR, Ullsperger M, Crone EA, Nieuwenhuis S. The role of the medial frontal cortex in cognitive control. Science. 2004;306:443–447. doi: 10.1126/science.1100301. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Chambers CD, et al. Executive “brake failure” following deactivation of human frontal lobe. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2006;18:444–455. doi: 10.1162/089892906775990606. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 107.MacDonald AW, Cohen JD, Stenger VA, Carter CS. Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. Science. 2000;288:1835–1838. doi: 10.1126/science.288.5472.1835. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Lazari, A. et al. Reassessing associations between white matter and behaviour with multimodal microstructural imaging. Cortex145, 187–200 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 109.Oldfield RC, et al. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. 1971;9:97–113. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 110.Papp D, Callaghan MF, Meyer H, Buckley C, Weiskopf N. Correction of inter-scan motion artifacts in quantitative R1 mapping by accounting for receive coil sensitivity effects. Magn. Reson. Med. 2016;76:1478–1485. doi: 10.1002/mrm.26058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Tabelow, K. et al. hMRI–a toolbox for quantitative mri in neuroscience and clinical research. NeuroImage194, 191–210 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 112.Fischl B, et al. Sequence-independent segmentation of magnetic resonance images. Neuroimage. 2004;23:S69–S84. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 113.Glasser MF, et al. The minimal preprocessing pipelines for the human connectome project. Neuroimage. 2013;80:105–124. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 114.Andersson JL, Sotiropoulos SN. An integrated approach to correction for off-resonance effects and subject movement in diffusion MR imaging. Neuroimage. 2016;125:1063–1078. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.10.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 115.Chamberland M, et al. Dimensionality reduction of diffusion MRI measures for improved tractometry of the human brain. NeuroImage. 2019;200:89–100. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.06.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 116.Isoda M, Hikosaka O. Role for subthalamic nucleus neurons in switching from automatic to controlled eye movement. J. Neurosci. 2008;28:7209–7218. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0487-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 117.Goldsworthy M, Hordacre B, Ridding M. Minimum number of trials required for within-and between-session reliability of TMS measures of corticospinal excitability. Neuroscience. 2016;320:205–209. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.02.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 118.Civardi C, Cantello R, Asselman P, Rothwell JC. Transcranial magnetic stimulation can be used to test connections to primary motor areas from frontal and medial cortex in humans. Neuroimage. 2001;14:1444–1453. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0918. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 119.Kujirai T, et al. Corticocortical inhibition in human motor cortex. J. Physiol. 1993;471:501–519. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1993.sp019912. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 120.Stagg C, et al. Relationship between physiological measures of excitability and levels of glutamate and gaba in the human motor cortex. J. Physiol. 2011;589:5845–5855. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2011.216978. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 121.Behrens TE, Berg HJ, Jbabdi S, Rushworth MF, Woolrich MW. Probabilistic diffusion tractography with multiple fibre orientations: what can we gain? Neuroimage. 2007;34:144–155. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.09.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 122.Hayes, A. F. Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-based Approach (Guilford, 2013).
  • 123.Wager TD, Davidson ML, Hughes BL, Lindquist MA, Ochsner KN. Prefrontal-subcortical pathways mediating successful emotion regulation. Neuron. 2008;59:1037–1050. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.09.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 124.Zhao X, Lynch Jr JG, Chen Q. Reconsidering Baron and Kenny: myths and truths about mediation analysis. J. Consum. Res. 2010;37:197–206. doi: 10.1086/651257. [DOI] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Peer Review File (355.5KB, pdf)
41467_2022_31687_MOESM3_ESM.pdf (77.8KB, pdf)

Description of Additional Supplementary Files

Supplementary Movie 1 (7.3MB, mov)
Reporting Summary (297KB, pdf)

Data Availability Statement

Data used in this study are only available upon request due to data protection considerations. Source data are provided with this paper.

Custom-written code used for data collection and for joint inference across myelin-sensitive modalities is available here: https://git.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/alazari/macroscopic-link.


Articles from Nature Communications are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES