Abstract
Objectives.
The relationships among depression, personality factors and cognitive decline in the elderly are complex. Depressed elders score higher in neuroticism than non-depressed older individuals. Presence of neuroticism worsens cognitive decline in depressed older adults. Yet little is known about changes in neuroticism among older adults being treated for depression and the impact of these changes on cognitive decline.
Design.
Longitudinal observational study.
Setting.
Academic Health Center
Participants.
We examined 68 participants in the Neurobiology of Late-life depression (NBOLD) study to test the hypothesis that older depressed subjects with more improvement in neuroticism would experience less cognitive decline compared with those with less change in neuroticism. Measurements. We measured neuroticism using the NEO-Personality Inventory-Revised at baseline and one year. Study psychiatrists measured depression using the Montgomery-Ǻsberg Depression Rating Scale. Global cognitive performance was measured using the Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD) battery at baseline and annually over three years. Regression models of one-year change in neuroticism and three-year change in CERAD included sex, age, race, education and one-year change in MADRS score as covariates.
Results.
We found that among older adults, one-year change in neuroticism was inversely associated with three-year change in CERAD total score.
Conclusions.
Our findings challenge the notion of longitudinal stability of measures of personality, especially among older depressed individuals. They highlight the importance of repeated personality assessment, especially of neuroticism, in the management of late-life depression. Future studies in larger samples followed for longer periods are needed to confirm our results and to extend them to examine both cognitive change and development of dementia.
The links between depression, the personality trait of neuroticism and cognitive decline are complex, though some research undertaken in the last two decades that has shed light on this area. Late-life depression is associated with later cognitive decline and development of dementia, and presence of neuroticism in older depressed patients has been shown to increase risk of cognitive decline and dementia (Manning et al., 2017; Terracciano et al., 2021). While personality has traditionally thought to be stable over time, increasing evidence suggests personality – and neuroticism in particular – may increase with age and as older adults transition across the spectrum of pathological aging (Roberts and Walton, 2006). For example, Caselli et al. found that an increase in neuroticism preceded the transition from cognitively normal to prodromal Alzheimer’s disease. Considering late-life depression is a risk factor for Alzheimer’s disease, and baseline neuroticism leads to cognitive decline in late-life depression, understanding how changes in neuroticism influence cognition in older depressed adults may help identify patients at risk of cognitive decline (Caselli et al., 2018).
Reported neuroticism may be influenced by concurrent depression. Therefore, repeated measures of personality may show decreases in neuroticism among patients whose depression is successfully treated (Renner et al., 2013). In a study of adult patients (mean age 4.19 years) seen in an Affective Disorders Program, the authors found that neuroticism correlated significantly with depression at baseline, and that over 12 weeks of treatment, both depression scores and levels of reported neuroticism decreased, with the change scores between the two measures being highly correlated Griens et al., 2002). However, while neuroticism may decrease in major depression following treatment, the absolute level of neuroticism in depressed patients who experience remission from depression at 12 weeks remains much higher than non-depressed comparison participants (Bagby et al., 1995). Less clear is the extent to which there are differential effects of depression changes on longitudinal measures of neuroticism among patients treated for depression, with some experiencing substantial decreased in levels of reported neuroticism, while others showing stability in reported neuroticism. Even less in known about the relationship between depression severity and neuroticism among older depressed adults.
Thus, examining neuroticism in older adults at two time points in the context of depression treatment may help identify those individuals with a persistent personality trait, as well as others for whom a state of depression may be exerting significant effects on their initial report of neuroticism. With this perspective, examination of the extent of neuroticism change with depression treatment becomes an important variable of interest in predicting cognitive decline. In the present investigation of older depressed adults enrolled in the Neurobiology of Late-Life Depression (NBOLD) study (Steffens et al., 2015), we examined the effect of one-year change in neuroticism as measured on the NEO Personality Inventory on three-year change in global cognitive performance as measured as on the Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD) cognitive battery. We hypothesized that change in neuroticism would be inversely associated with cognitive change such that older depressed patients with greater change (lowering) in neuroticism score over one year would be associated with less cognitive decline over three years.
Methods
Subjects
All subjects were enrolled in NBOLD, an NIMH funded study at UConn approved by its Institutional Review Board. After reviewing study information, all subjects provided written, informed consent to participate.
Depressed subjects were recruited from clinic referrals and newspaper advertisements. Inclusion criteria for depressed subjects were age 60 or above, ability to read and write English, Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) score 25 or greater and meeting criteria for major depression, single episode or recurrent. Study exclusion criteria were: current or recent alcohol or drug dependence; conditions associated with brain MRI abnormalities; physical or intellectual disability that may affect completion of self-rating instruments; established clinical diagnosis of dementia; other major DSM Axis 1 psychiatric disorders; and metal or pacemaker in the body or claustrophobia that might preclude MRI. In addition, current treatment with fluoxetine was an exclusion criterion for the depressed group given its long wash-out period.
The screening and assessment procedures used in NBOLD have been reported previously (Steffens, et al., 2015). Briefly, participants were screened for depression using the Center for Epidemiologic Studies- Depression (CES-D) scale, using a score of 16 or greater as a cut-off for depression. Upon enrollment and completion of baseline assessments, each participant was paid $100 for their time completing the MRI, cognitive test battery and experimental computerized measures. The clinical assessment procedures are summarized below.
Baseline Assessments
Trained clinical research assistants administered the Duke Depression Evaluation Schedule (DDES) to each participant via computer-assisted data entry. The DDES contains items covering demographic data, social variables, and the Diagnostic Interview Schedule (DIS) sections for depression, mania, generalized anxiety disorder, somatization symptoms, and alcohol use. A study psychiatrist interviewed each subject to establish a clinical diagnosis of major depression and then administered the Montgomery-Ǻsberg Depression Rating Scale. Each subject completed several self-report measures, including the NEO PI-R (Costa and McRae, 1992) as a measure of neuroticism. Within the neuroticism subscale, we also calculated the score for the facet vulnerability to stress.
Treatment Protocol
Study psychiatrists followed a treatment protocol that employed both structured and naturalistic components. All depressed subjects were offered open-label treatment with sertraline for 12 weeks. Individuals taking antidepressants at baseline who otherwise met inclusion criteria, underwent a study-related two-week medication washout with weekly telephone contact to assess clinical status and provide in-person assessments as warranted. Those who had prescribers or psychotherapists who did not wish to participate in study-based treatment could continue medication treatment or psychotherapy outside the study.
Neuropsychological Assessment
Subjects were administered a standardized cognitive assessment that is comprised of the Consortium to Establish a Registry in Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD ) neuropsychological battery (Morris et al., 1989), a collection of neuropsychological measures with normative standards for the elderly and established utility in longitudinal studies of cognitive impairment. While other cognitive assessments of various domains are also included in NBOLD, the current study focused on the CERAD, a composite measure of cognitive functioning. The CERAD measures include the MMSE; language tasks consisting of category fluency (animal naming) and object naming, constructional praxis and visual memory, requiring copy of 4 geometric designs, with delayed recall and delayed recognition procedures; and verbal learning and memory, consisting of immediate recall of 3 learning trials of a 10-item word list, delayed recall of the list, and recognition/discrimination of target words from nontarget foils. The CERAD measures (minus the MMSE) are tallied to create a composite measure of global cognitive functioning (maximum score=100).
Statistical analyses
Descriptive statistics were summarized for the total sample. Spearman’s correlation was used to assess the association between two continuous variables, neuroticism and MADRS.
The primary outcome variable was three-year change in CERAD total score (Year 3 minus baseline), and the predictor variable was one-year change in neuroticism (Year 1 minus baseline). Multiple linear regression was used controlling for age, one-year change in MADRS score, education, sex, and race to examine effects of change in neuroticism on change in cognition. All the analyses were performed using SAS 9.4 and significance level of 0.05 was used.
Results
The sample consistent of 68 older depressed subjects with complete three-year data. Demographic and clinical characteristics of the sample are shown in table 1. We found that baseline MADRS was associated with baseline total neuroticism score (r(S)=0.40, P=0.0006, n=68), that one-year MADRS was associated with one-year total neuroticism score (r(S)=0.36, P=0.0027, n=66), but that the association between one-year change in MADRS and one-year change in neuroticism did not reach statistical significance (r(S)=0.17, P=0.17, n=66). Nevertheless, we felt it was important to control for change in MADRS in subsequent analyses in addition to other covariates.
Table 1.
Total (N=68) | |
---|---|
Age, baseline, mean years (SD), | 71.5 (7.1) |
Gender, Female, N (%) | 48 (70.6%) |
Race, White, N (%) | 65 (95.6%) |
Educational level, mean years(SD) | 16.2 (2.5) |
NEO-PI Total Neuroticism score, baseline, mean(SD) | 108.1 (23.6) |
NEO-PI Total Neuroticism score Change (Year 1 minus baseline) , mean(SD) | −13.8 (15.1) |
MADRS total score, baseline, mean(SD) | 19.2 (5.8) |
MADRS total change score (Year 1 minus baseline) , mean(SD) | −9.3(6.4)* |
Outcome Variable | |
CERAD change score (Year 3 minus baseline) , mean(SD) | 1.19(7.91) |
NEO-PI = NEO Personality Inventory, MADRS = Montgomery-Ǻsberg Depression Rating Scale
2 subjects with missing one-year MADRS score
In multivariable linear regression analysis, one-year change in neuroticism was inversely associated with three-year change in CERAD total score in models controlling for age, sex, race, education and one-year change in MADRS score (B=−0.140 [95%CL: −0.275 to −0.004], p=0.040, see table 2).
Table 2.
Outcome: CERAD change score (Year 3 minus baseline) | ||
---|---|---|
Coefficient B(SE) | P Value | |
One-year NEO-PI neuroticism change (Year 1 minus baseline) | −0.140(0.068) | 0.043 |
MADRS total change score (Year 1 minus baseline) | 0.020(0.160) | 0.90 |
Age, years, baseline | −0.14(0.14) | 0.31 |
Educational level, years | 0.10(0.40) | 0.81 |
Gender, Female (Ref= Male) | −2.18(2.14) | 0.31 |
Race, Non-White (Ref= White) | 14.07(5.66) | 0.016 |
SE = Standard Error; NEO-PI = NEO Personality Inventory; Ref = Reference; MADRS = Montgomery-Ǻsberg
Discussion
The major finding of this study is that one-year change in total neuroticism as measured on the NEO-PI-R was negatively associated with three-year change in CERAD total score. The patients with more decline in neuroticism from baseline to Year 1 tended to have less cognitive decline from baseline to Year 3. For example, our model predicts an improvement of 20 (or 15) points on the total neuroticism scale would result in a favorable change of 2.8 (or 2.1) points on CERAD. The partial correlation coefficient between neuroticism change and cognitive change, derived from our multiple regression model was 0.26, which represented a near-medium effect size. Our results extend prior studies that found presence of higher neuroticism was associated with greater cognitive decline among older depressed individuals (Manning et al., 2017) and with incident dementia in a general population (Terracciano et al., 2021).
Our finding that patients with less decline in neuroticism over one-year experienced greater three-year cognitive decline has several important clinical implications. First, it highlights the role of neuroticism in cognitive outcomes of late life depression and impresses on our field the need to identify measures of neuroticism that may be employed during the assessment and management of depression in older adults. Second, our finding of significant improvement in self-reported neuroticism among many older patients receiving treatment for depression indicates that repeated measures of neuroticism may be quite variable in the context of geriatric depression, an observation that challenges the notion of stability of personality, especially over a relatively short period of time. For some older patients, higher neuroticism may simply reflect effects of the state of being depressed (i.e., depression influences responses on neuroticism measures), while in others, concurrent neuroticism may indicate a more enduring personality trait associated with cognitive decline. Our results support the use of neuroticism measures that can be incorporated into the management of depressed older adults. Neuroticism – particularly stable neuroticism as the current findings reveal – may be an easily identifiable risk factor for early nonresponse to psychotherapy and cognitive decline in older adults (Solomonov et al.,2021).
The current findings add to the literature that behavioral symptoms (e.g., depression, anxiety, and personality) predict cognitive decline and precede a clinical diagnosis of dementia. In recent years, the term Mild Behavioral Impairment (MBI) has been used to distinguish cognitively normal older adults who report apathy, affective dysregulation, or disinhibited symptoms concerning for a neurodegenerative illness (Ismail et al., 2016). While seemingly distinct constructs, there may be overlap between MBI and neuroticism. For example, in the Caselli et al. paper mentioned earlier the authors also found that an increase in neuroticism in prodromal Alzheimer’s disease was also accompanied by an increase in symptoms of depression and anxiety (Caselli et al., 2018). Changes in anxiety and depression also accompanied persistent changes in neuroticism in a large sample of middle aged adults from the Netherlands Study of Depression and Anxiety (Jeronimus et al, 2013). Considering the present analyses controlled for depression change, it is plausible that the persistence of state anxiety contributes to neuroticism stability in LLD. Future studies will help disentangle the psychological and behavioral constructs overlapping with neuroticism in LLD.
Biological mechanisms that might explain connection between neuroticism and cognitive decline are not clear. Previously, we noted that older depressed individuals high in neuroticism had smaller frontal lobe volumes than depressed subjects low in neuroticism and never-depressed subjects (Joseph et al., 2021), and that functional imaging studies implicated the prefrontal cortex in neuroticism among older depressives (Steffens, et al., 2017). These studies highlight the importance of frontal circuitry in the subgroup of older depressed individuals with comorbid neuroticism. Further studies are needed to understand the complex interactions among neuroticism, depression, and structural and functional frontal lobe changes in the development of cognitive decline. Another mechanism to consider involves the relationships among neuroticism and Alzheimer’s disease pathology. Early studies note that caregiver reports of neuroticism increase in patients following a diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease (Siegler et al., 1994). More recent studies found that neuroticism correlated with CSF markers of tau but not amyloid in a cohort of adults with autosomal dominant Alzheimer’s mutations (Aschenbrenner et al., 2020). Mutation carriers also exhibit higher levels of neuroticism compared to non-mutation carriers. Moreover, in this study, neuroticism remained stable in mutation carriers as time to dementia onset increased whereas neuroticism appeared to decrease in non-carriers over time. Together, these findings suggest Alzheimer’s disease progression may influence neuroticism change. Finally, chronic exposure to stress associated with neuroticism and depression has detrimental effects on brain function. Prior research linking depression and stress to hippocampal volume loss (Zannas et al., 2013) supports future investigations of neuroticism, hippocampal measures, and cognitive change.
This study has several limitations that should be noted. While few studies in late life depression report three-year cognitive data, our sample size of 68 is still relatively small and may limit the number of covariates we can include in our modelling. Our current cohort was predominantly white, so our findings may not generalize to other groups of older adults. Finally, with our three-year follow-up period, we are unlikely to be able to detect sufficient numbers of individuals whose cognitive decline has led to diagnosable dementia. Future studies with larger, more diverse samples followed over longer periods will allow us to include other potential confounding factors, to extend our findings in the elderly more broadly and include both cognitive change and development of dementia.
Acknowledgements
This study was supported by grants R01 MH108578 and P30 AG067988 from the U.S. National Institutes of Health.
Footnotes
Conflict of interest
None.
Contributor Information
David C. Steffens, Department of Psychiatry, University of Connecticut School of Medicine, Farmington, Connecticut, USA
Kevin J. Manning, Department of Psychiatry, University of Connecticut School of Medicine, Farmington, Connecticut, USA
Rong Wu, Public Health Sciences, University of Connecticut School of Medicine, Farmington, Connecticut, USA.
James J. Grady, Public Health Sciences, University of Connecticut School of Medicine, Farmington, Connecticut, USA
References
- Aschenbrenner AJ, et al. (2020). Relationships between big-five personality factors and Alzheimer’s disease pathology in autosomal dominant Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers and Dementia, 23, e12038. doi: 10.1002/dad2.12038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bagby RM, Joffe RT, Parker JDA, Kalemba V and Harkness KL (1995). Major depression and the five-factor model of personality. Journal of Personality Disorders, 9, 224–234. doi: 10.1521/pedi.1995.9.3.224. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Caselli RJ, et al. (2018). Personality changes during the transition from cognitive health to mild cognitive impairment. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 66, 671–678. doi: 10.1111/jgs.15182. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Costa PT Jr. and McCrae RR (1992). NEO PI-R professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc. [Google Scholar]
- Griens AMGF, Jonker K, Spinhoven P and Blom MBJ (2002). The influence of depressive state features on trait measurement. Journal of Affective Disorders, 70, 95–99. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0327(00)00371-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ismail Z, et al. (2016). Neuropsychiatric symptoms as early manifestations of emergent dementia: Provisional diagnostic criteria for mild behavioral impairment. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 12, 195–202. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2015.05.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jeronimus BF, Ormel J Aleman A, Penninx BWJH and Riese H (2013). Negative and positive life events are associated with small but lasting change in neuroticism. Psychological Medicine, 43, 2403–2415. doi: 10.1017/S0033291713000159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Joseph C, Wang L, Wu R, Manning KJ and Steffens DC (2021). Structural brain changes and neuroticism in late-life depression: a neural basis for depression subtypes. International Psychogeriatrics, 33, 515–520. doi: 10.1017/S1041610221000284. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manning KJ, Chan G and Steffens DC (2017). Neuroticism traits selectively impact long term illness course and cognitive decline in late-life depression. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 25, 220–229. doi: 10.1016/j.jagp.2016.10.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- British Journal of Psychiatry, 134, 382–389. [Google Scholar]
- Morris JC, et al. (1989). The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD). Part I. Clinical and neuropsychological assessment of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology, 39, 1159–1165. doi: 10.1212/wnl.39.9.1159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Renner F, Penninx BWJH, Peeters F, Cuijpers P, and Huibers MJH (2013). Two-year stability and change of neuroticism and extraversion in treated and untreated persons with depression: findings from the Netherlands Study of Depression and Anxiety (NESDA). Journal of Affective Disorders, 150, 201–208. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2013.03.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roberts BW, Walton KE, and Viechtbauer W (2006). Patterns of mean-level change in personality traits across the life course: a meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 1–25. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.1.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Siegler IC, Dawson DV and Welsh KA (1994). Caregiver ratings of personality change in Alzheimer’s disease patients: a replication. Psychology and Aging, 9, 464–466. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.9.3.464. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Solomonov N et al. (2021). Modifiable predictors of nonresponse to psychotherapies for late-life depression with executive dysfunction: a machine learning approach. Molecular Psychiatry, 26, 5190–5198. doi: 10.1038/s41380-020-0836-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steffens DC, Manning KJ, Wu R, Grady JJ, Fortinsky RH, and Tennen HA (2015). Methodology and preliminary results from the neurobiology of late-life depression study. International Psychogeriatrics, 27, 1987–1997. doi: 10.1017/S1041610215001386. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steffens DC, Wang L, Manning KJ, Pearlson GD (2017). Negative affectivity, aging, and depression: results from the Neurobiology of Late-Life Depression (NBOLD) study. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 25, 1135–1149. doi: 10.1016/j.jagp.2017.03.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Terracciano A, et al. (2021). Is neuroticism differentially associated with risk of Alzheimer’s disease, vascular dementia, and frontotemporal dementia? Journal of Psychiatric Research, 138, 34–40. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2021.03.039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yoneda T et al. (2020). Increases in neuroticism may be an early indicator of dementia: a coordinated analysis. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B, 75, 251–262, doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbw006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zannas AS, et al. (2013). Negative life stress and longitudinal hippocampal volume changes in older adults with and without depression. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 47, 829–34. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2013.02.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]