Abstract
As the literature highlights, many health behavior theories try to explain both social and psychological variables influencing an individual’s health behavior. This study integrates insights relative to the antecedents of getting vaccinated from health behavior theories, particularly including the health belief model (HBM), the theory of planned behavior (TPB), and the different socio-demographic factors. Furthermore, we considered the possible mechanism of impact of distrust in science on individuals’ hesitance and resistance to taking up SARS-CoV-2 vaccination in subjects living in Italy. A correlational study of 1095 subjects enrolled when the national vaccination campaign for the third dose was launched. A questionnaire was used to measure: Italian Risk Perception; subjective norm; trust in science, trust in the vaccine; fear of COVID-19; fear of the vaccine; perceived knowledge about SARS-CoV-2; booster vaccination intention. Principal results show that: (i) the positive relationship provided by HBM theory between perceptions of SARS-CoV-2 risk (vulnerability and severity) and intention to have the vaccine, through fear of COVID-19; (ii) the positive relationship between subjective norms and both trust in science and vaccination intention; (iii) that trust in science plays a crucial role in predicting vaccination intention. Finally, the results provided indications about a positive relationship between subjective norms and fear of COVID-19, and a full mediation role of trust in science in the relationships between determinants of both TPA and HBM, fear of COVID-19, and vaccination intention. In conclusion, an individual’s intention (not) to get vaccinated requires the consideration of a plethora of socio-psychological factors. However, overall, trust in science appears to be a key determinant of vaccination intention. Additional strategies promoting healthy behavior are needed.
Keywords: vaccination, booster, intention, vulnerability, severity, trust, SARS-CoV-2
1. Introduction
The importance of vaccination and boosters in a world scenario of an increasing likelihood of pandemics is fundamental both for supranational health policies and for the implementation of public and private communication campaigns and policies [1,2,3]. Vaccination behavior and its antecedent in terms of will (e.g., intention, willingness) are influenced by variables of different nature (e.g., individual, family, social, work, etc.) and this has directed the research of its determinants to an approach mostly by compartment ponds between the different theoretical frameworks [4,5]. Moreover, the intention to get vaccinated is influenced by a plethora of factors: (i) self-protection factors (on one hand, being vaccinated is an action to protect one’s health in the long term, but on the other, it is simultaneously seen as a possible health hazard) [6,7,8]; (ii) socio-relational factors [9]; (iii) political and ideological factors [10,11].
Much of the governmental storytelling around intention—or not—to get vaccinated against SARS-CoV-2 focused on the theme of freedom and individual rights: the act of vaccinating has become in effect a political action, a moment to do one’s duty as citizens in society or to protect the weakest. Conversely, the act of not getting vaccinated has also become the expression of a transversal distrust in government health policies, science, and medicine [12,13,14].
The availability of vaccines, booster frequency, and the complex organization of each national system for SARS-CoV-2 vaccination campaigns, have determined the need to continuously monitor the determinants of vaccination intention to reshape communication and nudging campaigns [15,16].
The importance of studying socio-psychological variables to understand vaccination intention and inform effective measures has been advocated [17]. A deeper understanding of the underlying psychology of vaccine-resistant and vaccine-hesitant groups can enhance the potential effectiveness of the public health messages targeting these groups [18].
Research in health psychology has generated multiple health behavior theories (HBT) that identify social and psychological variables influencing an individual’s health behavior [19,20,21,22,23]. In the present paper, we aimed to integrate insights relative to the antecedents of getting vaccinated against SARS-CoV-2 from HBTs, particularly including the health belief model (HBM) [24,25], the theory of planned behavior (TPB) [26,27], and the different socio-demographic factors. More specifically, the research intends, on the one hand, to test a theoretical model that integrates subjective norms, risk perceptions and fear of COVID-19, and trust to explain vaccination intention; on the other hand, understanding the peculiarities of the sub-sample with a low intention of vaccination.
2. Literature Background
Three main models have been proposed in the literature to explain the intention to get vaccinated: the 5Cs model of vaccines [28], the health belief model [24,25], and that of social norms, based on the theory of planned behavior [27,29]. Each of these models focuses on specific health protection factors, perceptions of utility, collective dimension, and social responsibility as antecedents of intention to get vaccinated. Almost all of the works that used determinants from all three theoretical models reasoned by assuming that the models were independent and that the variables could not relate to each other [23,30,31]; nevertheless, some authors have underlined the need to integrate insights from the three models to provide a more comprehensive explanation for such a complex behavior/behavioral intention as that represented by the intention/hesitancy to take up the SARS-CoV-2 vaccine [19,32,33].
2.1. Health Behavior Theories
Basically, the HBM [34] theorized that health-related behavior is determined by two main classes of variables: the value that each individual attaches to a particular goal and their estimate of the likelihood that a given action will achieve that goal. The model describes four dimensions that are likely to influence an individual’s behavior: perceived susceptibility, severity, benefits, and barriers (for a review, see [35]). HBM has been widely used as a predictor of SARS-CoV-2 behavioral vaccination intention [36,37]. According to HBM, people’s pro-health behaviors are significant predictors of intention to have SARS-CoV-2 vaccines and boosters [38,39]. Perceived vulnerability refers to the belief people have that they are at risk of getting infected with SARS-CoV-2 [40]; perceived susceptibility was identified as an important factor influencing the intention to get vaccinated against SARS-CoV-2 [37,41]. Perceived severity refers to the belief that the consequences resulting from getting the disease are serious for the self and others. Individuals that feel threatened or perceive high levels of risk from SARS-CoV-2 will be more likely to express higher levels of SARS-CoV-2 vaccination intention [42]. Perceived high susceptibility of contracting SARS-CoV-2 and perceived high severity of the negative effects of contracting the virus were associated with increased vaccination intention [37].
In addition to risk perceptions, both within the HBM, and in various contributions that have integrated different theoretical models, numerous authors have highlighted the important role of emotional factors resulting from SARS-CoV-2 in the intention to get vaccinated [25,31,43], and in particular on the fear of COVID-19 in the general population and healthcare workers (HCWs) [44,45,46]. Fear of COVID-19 is undoubtedly correlated with the intention to get vaccinated, but its effect seems to be mediated by attitudes of trust/distrust towards government, science, conspiracy, and so on [47].
From the review of the HBM literature, several confirmations emerge on the positive relationship between perceptions of SARS-CoV-2 risk (mostly concerning vulnerability and severity) and fear of COVID-19 [48,49,50,51,52], and on the positive relationship between fear of COVID-19 and vaccination intention [20,47,53]. Following these relationships, we can formulate several hypotheses (see Figure 1):
Hp 1a and Hp 1b: a higher risk perception (higher vulnerability and severity) will be positively related to fear of COVID-19; Hp 2a: a higher fear of COVID-19 will be associated with a lower intention to receive the booster;
Hp 2b: a higher fear of COVID-19 will be associated with lower Trust in Science.
Among the studies that analyzed the individual level, the relevance of psychological theories of behavior for vaccine acceptance was shown, such as the theory of planned behavior [54]. Shmueli [33] recently incorporated the TPB with the HBM to explain the intention to get SARS-CoV-2 vaccinated among Israeli adults and found the integration of TPB and HBM significantly increased the explained variance in behavioral intentions.
2.2. Theory of Planned Behavior
The TPB is an expectancy-value model used to predict and explain human behavior in specific contexts [29]. TPB posits those attitudes, subjective norms, and perceived behavior control as key antecedents of behavioral intention, which is a proximate predictor of behavior [55,56]. Its utility has been demonstrated in predicting various health-related behaviors including intentions to have genetic testing [57], flu vaccine [33], H1N1 vaccine amongst students [58], HPV vaccine amongst students [59], a future HIV [60] and SARS-CoV-2 vaccination [61,62,63], and to parental decision-making for children’s vaccination [64,65], and actual vaccine uptake [55,61,63,66]. Subjective norms represent the individual’s perceived social pressure to enact that behavior. People are heavily influenced by their perceptions of the beliefs and attitudes of relevant others (e.g., friends and family members) [67].
Subjective norms refer to a person’s beliefs about what relevant others think about them engaging in a particular behavior, and whether they would approve of it or not. Subjective norms refer to the extent to which people’s willingness to have the SARS-CoV-2 vaccine is influenced by whether their significant social others approve of them having the vaccine or not. TPB research shows that both perceived descriptive and injunctive norms are positively related to vaccine uptake [63,68,69,70,71]. Coronavirus SARS-CoV-2 vaccination intentions correlate positively with a positive attitude toward vaccination, with perceived subjective norms in favor of vaccination among friends, family [69,72], doctors [73,74], and with high perceived behavioral control [11,27,63,68].
Subjective norms play a role in the relationship between conspiratorial beliefs and vaccination. This is consequential, as research suggests that distrust in science—often underpinned by conspiracy beliefs [75] and specific ideologies [76]—affects health-related attitudes and behaviors. Along similar lines, individuals who distrust science might be less motivated to follow science-based advice about important behavioral guidelines to curb the pandemic [77,78].
From the review of TPB of vaccine intention, there is basically a certain uniformity in underlining how subjective norms, and trust in science, play a crucial role in predicting vaccination willingness and other prescribed norms and behaviors [11,67,68,79,80]. As a consequence of this, we formulated the following hypotheses:
Hp 3a: Subjective norms will be positively related to intention to get the booster;
Hp 3b: Subjective norms will be positively related to trust in science;
Hp 4: Trust in science will be positively related to intention to get the booster.
Some research has investigated the relationship between Subjective norms and Fear of COVID 19, but their relationship has not been fully clarified [11,63,81].
Integrating some contributions from the literature [77,82,83,84], it seems more appropriate to consider subjective norms as determining and the fear of COVID-19 as a resulting emotional factor. Considering that, we propose the following further hypothesis:
Hp 3c: Subjective norms will be positively related to fear of COVID-19.
2.3. 5C Model
Grounded in previous theoretical models, the 5C model aimed to reflect a broad scope of predictors of vaccination intention and booster behavior. The 5C model by Brewer and colleagues [85] includes five possible antecedents of vaccination intention, that is, confidence (trust in science, trust in vaccine effectiveness, safety, and necessity and the system that delivers it), complacency, the calculation, constraints, and collective responsibility [28,86].
Validated studies in vaccine hesitancy [87,88,89,90,91] focus primarily on confidence in vaccines and science/medicine and have stated that it plays a role in explaining vaccination behavior. Confidence “is defined as trust in (i) the effectiveness and safety of vaccines, (ii) the system that delivers them, including the reliability and competence of the health services and health professionals, and (iii) the motivations of policymakers who decide on the need for vaccines” [92] (p. 2). Individuals who lack trust in science and confidence have negative attitudes towards vaccination [1].
The trust in science factor has been transversally recognized as one of the factors capable of explaining different behaviors and attitudes of adherence to health behaviors prescribed in a pandemic situation [77,93]. Following literature indications [14,77,79,94,95,96,97], trust in science seems to be a key factor, able to modulate both the emotional and social factors responsible for the intention to get vaccinated. Because of this, we formulate the following hypotheses:
Hp 5: Trust mediates the relationship between subjective norms and intention to get vaccinated.
Hp 6: Trust mediates the relationship between fear of COVID-19 and intention to get vaccinated.
The proposed theoretical model is presented in Figure 1.
2.4. Socio-Demographic Factors
Individuals differed in their intention to get vaccinated according to a number of socio-demographic factors. For instance, males usually reported a higher intention to get vaccinated than females [37,40,98,99,100], although the patterns of such difference vary across national contexts [101].
Moreover, age, education, and income appear to influence the intention to get vaccinated, with older people with higher education and income levels often showing higher vaccination intention [88,102,103,104]. By contrast, unemployed individuals and those with a lower socioeconomic status have often been described as less likely to get vaccinated [105,106,107].
In addition to these factors, there are many other sociodemographic features that can predict vaccine hesitancy and uptake [68]. Unwillingness to have a vaccine is usually found at a higher level among religious minorities, those who are less educated, and residents of rural areas [108]. Regarding health-related predictors, it was found that respondents with chronic conditions at higher risk of SARS-CoV-2 or those who were overweight, as well as those who reported having been vaccinated against flu in the previous season were more likely to accept a SARS-CoV-2 vaccine and booster [33]. Nonetheless, other studies have shown no effect of such socio-demographic factors on the intention to take up SARS-CoV-2 vaccines [33,68,109].
3. Study Aim
Several authors have stressed the need to integrate insights from the separate models described above to better describe the intention to get vaccinated and, in the case of the present research, to get the SARS-CoV-2 booster vaccine. This integration should be directed to consider the interplay between the different factors that may induce people to accept being vaccinated vs. being reluctant to do so.
Based on the literature described above, in the present paper, we aimed to integrate some of the insights provided with regards to vaccination against the SARS-CoV-2 virus in a more comprehensive model in which subjective norms and risk perceptions are considered as antecedents, fear of COVID-19 and trust in science as underlying (emotional) mechanisms, and the intention to have the booster (i.e., the third dose) as the outcome.
4. Materials and Methods
4.1. Method
By integrating the Pagliaro [77] and Barattucci [82] models, and following literature indications, the model proposes risk perceptions (vulnerability and severity) and subjective norms as independent variables, fear of COVID-19 and trust in science as mediators, and vaccination intention as an outcome. A correlational study was designed with the aim of exploring the sets of variables associated with booster vaccination intention in an Italian sample, when the national vaccination campaign for the third dose had been launched in Italy.
Participants completed an online survey between 22 November 2021 (when booster dose administration began, including for people aged between 40 and 59) and 6 December. The overall sampling method was purposeful in that it aimed for diversity in a population that had not already had the third dose, to enhance generalizability.
Participants were recruited with a short poll (“Will you have the third dose of the SARS-CoV-2 vaccine?”) posted on social media networks (Facebook, LinkedIn, Instagram), direct mailing or through WhatsApp messaging, and by word of mouth, and then provided with a link to a Qualtrics form to fill in a closed-ended questionnaire. Participation was anonymous and voluntary, and there was no way of determining where the participants originated from; the Qualtrics form did track the IPs of participants only to ensure that the same participant did not respond more than once.
Before completing the questionnaire, participants read and accepted a consent form, instructions for participation, and a declaration on data processing in compliance with the Helsinki Declaration (WMA, 2013) and current Italian laws (GDPR).
The study is part of research that has been approved by the local academic ethics committee (Research project “Return to work and fragile workers”, RN: 03/2020, 28/12/2020). Cases without any data in the scales at use in the present study were considered invalid and the sample size was calculated as the total number of valid returned questionnaires.
4.2. Participants
One thousand and twenty participants began filling in the questionnaire; the final sample was composed of 1095 individuals who adequately completed it. Participants were mainly female (N = 864, 78.9%), mostly with a university or post-graduate degree (academic degree, N = 448, 40.9%; post-graduate, N = 240, 21.9%), currently in work (unemployed, N = 121, 11.1%; student, N = 130, 11.9%), and with a mean age of 40.06 years (SD = 13.8). From a health point of view, participants were mainly vaccinated (N = 1014, 92.6%); among the vaccinated, over 70% had had two vaccines (N = 788, 72%), largely the BioNTech, Pfizer vaccine (N = 609, 55.6%).
4.3. Measures
Participants completed the first section of the questionnaire with socio-demographic information and details about their own SARS-CoV-2 vaccine experiences; then, they filled out a questionnaire made up of the following measures:
Italian Risk Perception Questionnaire [110] adapted from the original development by Savadori et al. [111]. The researchers adapted the tool to measure 2 single-item dimensions of SARS-CoV-2 infection risk, on a seven-point scale ranging from 0 to 6: severity (perceived seriousness or harmfulness of SARS-CoV-2) and vulnerability (perception of the risk of being infected with SARS-CoV-2); item examples: “Considering the scale (from 1 “not exposed” to 7 “totally exposed”), in your opinion, to what extent do you think you are exposed to SARS-CoV-2 risk?” [112].
Subjective norms were measured with a single-item scale from the literature [113], on a seven-point scale ranging from 1 (totally disagree) to 7 (totally agree) (item example: “People whose opinion is important to me (friends, family, colleagues, etc.) think that I need to have the SARS-CoV-2 vaccine”).
Trust in science [77] was measured with an ad hoc 4-items scale, on a seven-point scale ranging from 1 (totally disagree) to 7 (totally agree) (item example: “We should trust the competence of scientists”). Cronbach’s Alpha = 0.84.
Trust in the vaccine [114,115] was measured with an ad hoc 6-items scale, on a seven-point scale ranging from 1 (totally disagree) to 7 (totally agree) (item example: “I believe the SARS-CoV-2 vaccine is important to reduce or eliminate SARS-CoV-2”). Cronbach’s Alpha = 0.86.
Fear of COVID-19 was measured with an ad hoc 2-item scale from the literature (fear of being infected and fear of spreading the virus to others) [82,116], on a seven-point scale ranging from 1 (not worried at all) to 7 (very worried) (item example: “How worried are you about the chance of being infected with the SARS-CoV-2 virus?”). Cronbach’s Alpha = 0.76.
Fear of the vaccine was measured with an ad hoc single-item scale from the literature [82,117,118] on a seven-point scale ranging from 1 (not worried at all) to 7 (very worried) (item example: “How concerned are you about SARS-CoV-2 vaccination?”).
Perceived knowledge about SARS-CoV-2 was measured with an ad hoc 3-items scale from the literature [2,117,118] on a seven-point scale ranging from 1 (no knowledge) to 7 (excellent knowledge) (item example: “How much do you think you know about the progress of the pandemic in recent weeks?”). Cronbach’s Alpha = 0.67.
Booster vaccination intention was measured with an ad hoc single-item on a seven-point scale ranging from 1 (absolutely not) to 7 (I definitely will), which asked participants: “Are you willing to have the third vaccine, even if you do not fall into those categories required to do so?”.
Common method variance and method biases were limited with procedures suggested in the literature [119]: the different scales were randomly inserted into the questionnaire and graphically separated from each other, and various scale formats and endpoints were utilized for each of the measured variables. Harman’s single factor test indicated that the first attribute contributes only 30% of the inconsistency in the data [120].
Gender, age, and education were included as control variables. Table 1 presents the descriptive statistics and zero-order correlations among the measured variables; through a Kolmogorov–Smirnov test for each of the variables involved, the distribution of all asymmetry and kurtosis values of the measured variables resulted between −1 and +1.
Table 1.
M (SD) | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. Vulnerability | 4.44 (1.5) | - | ||||||||
2. Severity | 4.65 (1.4) | 0.250 *** | - | |||||||
3. Fear of COVID-19 | 5.52 (1.4) | 0.290 *** | 0.522 *** | - | ||||||
4. Fear of vaccine | 3.06 (1.9) | –0.059 | –0.121 ** | –0.183 ** | - | |||||
5. COVID-19 knowledge | 4.70 (1.3) | 0.059 | 0.093 ** | 0.096 ** | 0.039 | - | ||||
6. Subjective norm | 5.58 (1.7) | 0.170 ** | 0.276 *** | –0.340 *** | –0.268 *** | 0.041 | - | |||
7. Trust in science | 5.72 (1.4) | 0.137 ** | 0.275 *** | 0.369 *** | –0.585 *** | –0.006 | 0.407 *** | - | ||
8. Trust in vaccine | 5.52 (1.4) | 0.116 ** | 0.331 *** | 0.368 *** | –0.719 *** | –0.017 | 0.389 *** | 0.763 *** | - | |
9. Intention to vaccine | 5.86 (1.9) | 0.119 ** | 0.206 ** | 0.271 *** | –0.482*** | 0.092 ** | 0.322 *** | 0.585 *** | 0.638 *** | - |
*** p < 0.001; ** p < 0.01.
4.4. Data Analysis
Data analysis was performed in three steps. In step 1, a series of individual control variables (gender, age, education, work status, vaccination status, etc.) were tested as predictors of intention to vaccine through multiple regression analysis. In step 2, some specific analyses (see next paragraph) were conducted for the description of the sub-sample of zero intention to get vaccinated. In step 3, the research tested measured variables as predictors of vaccination intention through multiple regression analysis. In step 3, the relationships between measured variables expressed by the proposed theoretical model and the main hypotheses were tested through a comprehensive structural equation model with AMOS22. Commonly reported fit statistics were: comparative fit index (CFI), Tucker–Lewis index (TLI), normed fit index (NFI), goodness of fit index (GFI), incremental fit index (IFI), root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), plus standardized root mean square residual (SRM) for measurement model fit.
5. Results
First of all, we proceeded to evaluate the effect of socio-demographic variables on the intention to vaccine through t-tests for independent samples, ANOVA, and multiple regressions. Neither gender nor age differences were found, while the educational qualification was a predictor of the intention to vaccine (t = 2.33, p < 0.02, R2 = 0.005), as well as the number of vaccines made (t = 7.97, p < 0.001, R2 = 0.06): as the number of vaccines already made and the qualification increases, a greater intention to vaccinate for the third dose is associated.
5.1. Predictors and Clusters for Vaccination Intention
The unvaccinated reported that they choose to be not vaccinated due: (a) to the vaccine risk for health (“The vaccine could have dangerous side effects”; N = 22, 27.2%); (b) to personal preferences regarding the management of personal health (“I prefer to develop immunity from infection rather than immunity through vaccination”, N = 14, 17.3%); (c) to the belief that they did not need it since they had already contracted SARS-CoV-2 in the past (N = 14, 17.3%). Among other factors, it is also necessary to consider other reasons for the declared non-vaccination: on one hand, real limitations, health problems, pregnancy, and breastfeeding; on the other, the SARS-CoV-2 vaccine is considered “experimental and unsafe”, non-vaccination is a way to express dissent with government restrictions and pandemic management, the vaccine gives rise to more variants and is not the appropriate method to contain a pandemic, current scientific paradigms are outdated.
The analytic setup was based on IBM SPSS Modeler, focusing on the target of subjects with a low propensity to have the third dose, and splitting the sample into two: high (5–7) propensity subjects (N = 895 individuals, 81.74%) and low (1) propensity subjects (N = 200 individuals, 18.26%). The education level distributions of the two sub-samples were significantly different (chi-square = 11.929, df = 3, p < 0.05): the lower the educational level, the lower the propensity to have the third dose. When dealing with a low propensity to have the third dose, trust in science is one of the most important factors (see Figure 2): more than 60% of the individuals having low propensity to have the third dose, had low trust in science (<3). Conversely, subjects showing a higher propensity to have the third vaccination (in Figure 2; target high propensity) also declared a higher level of trust in science (5 or more). High propensity to vaccination subjects had an average trust in science of 6.01 (SD = 1.16), while low propensity to vaccination individuals showed an average trust in science of 4.46 (SD = 1.67).
The figure above shows the incidence of high propensity vs. low propensity to vaccination individuals, with respect to trust in science levels (from 0 to 7, in 20 equal-spaced bins). The graph is normalized to 1 in each of the 20 bands in order to better depict the incidence of high propensity vs. low propensity individuals. Low levels of trust in science (≤4) clearly show a significant proportion of low propensity individuals (≥50%) and vice-versa for high propensity individuals with high levels of trust in science.
5.2. Measurement Model
Table 1 shows the descriptive statistics for all the measures and zero-order correlation between them. In order to explore the construct validity and the measurement model, a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) through SEM was conducted: a comparison of four nested models from one factor to a final model composed of the nine principal latent factors (risk perceptions, trust in science/the vaccine, fear of COVID-19/the vaccine, subjective norms, SARS-CoV-2 knowledge, vaccination intention) is presented in Table 2: considering that five dimensions were measured with single items, there was an evident amelioration of all indices from the first to the final model.
Table 2.
Chi-Square | df | RMSEA | CFI | IFI | SRMR | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Model 1: one factor | 3510.733 | 170 | 0.134 | 0.751 | 0.742 | 0.110 |
Model 2: three factors | 3464.783 | 167 | 0.123 | 0.756 | 0.793 | 0.096 |
Model 3: six factors | 3155.012 | 164 | 0.109 | 0.830 | 0.809 | 0.089 |
Model 4: nine factors | 2250.771 | 161 | 0.083 | 0.921 | 0.909 | 0.078 |
df = degrees of freedom; RMSEA = root mean square error of approximation; CFI = comparative fit index; IFI = incremental fit index; SRMR = standardized root mean square residual.
5.3. Tested Model
The proposed structural model was tested through SEM (Figure 3): the two risk perception dimensions (vulnerability and severity) and subjective norms as correlated antecedents, with direct relationships with fear of COVID-19 as the intermediate variable, which has direct links with trust in science (as a mediator) and vaccination intention (as an outcome). Consistently with our hypothesized relationships the model showed excellent goodness of fit: chi-square = 10.59 (df = 4; p < 0.001), RMSEA = 0.039, CFI = 0.995, IFI = 0.995, NFI = 0.992, GFI = 0.996, TLI = 0.974, with all significant relationships (p < 0.001).
Regression weights are presented in Table 3, while the path diagram of the final model is shown in Figure 3. All of the associations were highly significant (p < 0.001).
Table 3.
Direct Effects | |
---|---|
Vulnerability → fear of COVID-19 | 0.149 |
Severity → fear of COVID-19 | 0.430 |
Subjective norm → fear of COVID-19 | 0.197 |
Subjective norm → trust | 0.319 |
Subjective norm → vaccination intention | 0.120 |
Fear of COVID-19 → trust | 0.260 |
Fear of COVID-19 → vaccination intention | 0.086 |
Trust → vaccination intention | 0.541 |
Indirect Effects via Trust | |
Fear of COVID-19 → vaccination intention | 0.140 |
Subjective norm → vaccination intention | 0.217 |
As hypothesized (Hp 1a and Hp 1b), both dimensions of risk perception are positively related to fear of COVID-19, as well as for subjective norms (Hp 3c); taking into account direct and indirect effects of antecedents and intermediate variables on vaccination intention, results seem to clearly confirm that the expected effect of subjective norms, and risk perceptions through fear of COVID-19, is fully mediated by trust in science (Table 3; Hp 5 and Hp 6). Overall, the relationships expressed in the model explained 33% of the variance for fear of COVID-19, 23% for trust in science, and 36% for vaccination intention.
6. Discussion
Different theoretical frameworks focusing on antecedents of a different nature (social responsibility, beliefs, health protection factors, emotions, etc.) have been considered in the literature to explore the intention to get vaccinated, in particular in conjunction with the global SARS-CoV-2 pandemic scenario [121,122,123]. However, even the studies that considered determinants from the different models assumed that factors could not relate to each other (for example, social influencing factors and affect), highlighting the need for models that can provide a more inclusive explanation for behavioral vaccination intention, integrating cognitions and affect [19,23,32].
In the present paper, we tried to integrate insights from different theoretical frameworks to provide a better understanding of the psychological underpinnings of the intention to get vaccinated, with particular reference to the third booster dose. In particular, we proposed and tested a theoretical model where subjective norms have been integrated with risk perceptions and fear of COVID-19 to explain vaccination intention, through the mediation of trust in science.
In line with the abundant literature on health behaviors, our findings confirmed: (i) the positive relationship provided by HBM theory between perceptions of SARS-CoV-2 risk (vulnerability and severity) and vaccination intention, through fear of COVID-19 [20,47,48,49,50,51,52,53]; (ii) the positive relationship between subjective norms and both trust in science and vaccination intention [124,125]; (iii) that trust in science plays a crucial role in predicting vaccination intention (11,67,68,79,80,83,84]. The results also provided indications about a positive relationship between subjective norms and fear of COVID-19, and a possible full mediation role of trust in science in the relationships between determinants of both TPA and HBM, fear of COVID-19, and vaccination intention.
Some important results also emerge from the analysis of the intention to get vaccinated groups: first, those who did not get vaccinated claim the reason is that the vaccine is not considered safe, mostly due to personal beliefs regarding the danger of the vaccine, the importance of making choices independently, and to be able to freely express their dissent towards the government and health measures in response to the pandemic. Therefore, beyond the possible reasons of personal health (pregnancy, pathologies, past SARS-CoV-2 disease, etc.), people who are more resistant to SARS-CoV-2 vaccination and unvaccinated mostly have negative personal political thoughts that express dissent towards government actions and health policies. Finally, it is necessary to highlight, in line with other evidence in Italy, among the socio-demographic variables, the effect on the intention to vaccine of the educational qualification and the number of vaccines already made [95]. Compared to previous research in the same context, the results of this study, on the one hand, clearly confirm that the Italian is a population, compared to others, which has shown great compliance with vaccination and a high vaccination intention [126,127].
On the other hand, our results did not provide confirmation to other evidence that showed the effect of age and gender on the intention to vaccinate [128], while they are consistent with those that highlighted the effect of educational qualification, fear of COVID-19, and trust in science and governments [77,95,127].
6.1. Limitations and Future Directions
The study reported here has some limitations that should be acknowledged and overcome in future ad hoc studies. First and foremost, as the great bulk of research was conducted with regards to an individual’s reactions to the pandemic, the nature of this study is cross-sectional. Despite previous research demonstrating the causal relationship between the antecedents of the health behaviors that we have considered here and the behavioral intention/actual behavior, we should be cautious when interpreting the associations between variables that we have discussed here. Future ad hoc studies should be designed to ascertain the causal relationship between these variables, for instance by means of longitudinal designs.
In considering the results of this study, it is necessary to take into account the limitation due to the composition of the sample which does not seem to fully represent the population, due to a lack of homogeneity in the qualification and gender. It is not possible to exclude that the results on the effect of trust in science may have been somehow affected by the educational level of the sample collected.
Furthermore, it is necessary to report the possible presence of selection bias due to the fact that the sample has mostly vaccinated people.
A further avenue for future research is the extension of the model we proposed in this paper. Indeed, as we have stressed above, dealing with the pandemic requires the integration of a previously proposed model to provide a better understanding of an individual’s behavior. This means that further integrations are possible and desirable, and that we consider the proposed model as a starting point for a more comprehensive understanding of intention to get vaccinated, rather than the only possible one. In particular, the relationship between variables of the HBM and those of the TPB, and the possible interaction of emotional factors that result from the different cognitive and social assessment processes (SARS-CoV-2 risk, risk of vaccine damage, risk of social and work exclusion, trust in government, trust in vaccines, etc.) will need to be investigated more thoroughly.
6.2. Theoretical Implications
The current research contributes to the literature on the factors that most emerge as predictors of vaccination intention, and the possible mechanism of impact of distrust in science on individuals’ hesitance and resistance to the uptake of SARS-CoV-2 vaccination [128,129] in the Italian context. From a theoretical point of view, our results seem to confirm that the expected effect of subjective norms, and risk perceptions through fear of COVID-19, is mediated by trust in science. Following literature indications [14,77,79,94,95,96,97], our results also show the role of trust in science as a key factor, able to modulate both the emotional and social factors responsible for the intention to get vaccinated. Indeed, trust in science consistently emerged as a key determinant of adherence to government indications to avoid the spread of SARS-CoV-2 around the world, as several cross-national studies revealed: this has been found to be the case even regarding the most controversial issue, that is, whether to get vaccinated. As reported above, vaccine-hesitant people refer to distrust in science and scientists to explain their own choices, and this has also been related to conspiracy ideologies in other studies. Therefore, overall, the key role in (dis-)trust in understanding SARS-CoV-2 -related behaviors has been fully confirmed in our study: beyond the social, cognitive, and emotional determinants (fear), it is precisely the trust in science that acts as a driver of vaccination intention. This result also appears consistent in the light of those relating to socio-demographic variables: confidence in science and the intention to get vaccinated for the third dose is related both to the qualification and to the number of vaccine doses already made [95,126].
6.3. Practical Implications
Emergency situation conditions and various environmental factors such as critical events (lockdowns) have a major impact on peoples’ perceptions and behaviors [130,131], and since the beginning of the SARS-CoV-2 pandemic, there has been much misinformation and many conspiracy theories that may have increased an individuals’ hesitance and resistance to taking up SARS-CoV-2 vaccination [67,128].
The expectations of family, friends, and colleagues, knowledge of the risks of SARS-CoV-2, and the consequent fear of COVID-19, are modulated by the level of trust in science: it therefore seems clear that resistance to vaccination is a type of “political response” to the restrictions and obligations imposed on personal freedoms.
These results are consistent with those of the research which has shown overall that distrust in science, government, and the healthcare system [129,132,133], misinformation [68], belief in conspiracies [67,134], and conspiracy mentality [9,135,136] contribute significantly to a negative attitude to the vaccine. The lack of trust in science therefore appears to be an aspect that is linked to a wider mistrust and critical attitude of governments, politics, health systems, and corporations, which is often also associated with an affinity to conspiracy theories and the anti-scientific. From the point of view of institutional communication, in the future, it will be necessary firstly to separate health communication from governmental communication, and highlight the advantages in terms of health more than those of adherence to government restrictions (experienced negatively); thus, since a low propensity for having a third dose vaccination seems clearly associated with lower educational levels, particular attention should be given to targeting institutional communication to this section of the population.
7. Conclusions
Understanding an individual’s intention (not) to get vaccinated requires the consideration of a plethora of socio-psychological factors. In the present paper, we provided evidence that, among other factors, subjective norms, risk perceptions, fear of COVID-19, and, above all, trust in science and scientists appear to be key determinants of vaccination intention regarding the intention to have the third booster dose among a sample of Italians. The biggest health crisis in recent decades has required health and government interventions for the prevention and containment of the virus that has changed the social life, perceptions, and emotional responses of the population; therefore, understanding the psychological factors that guide and motivate the intentions to get vaccinated appears increasingly crucial and useful at the application level. In addition to the weight of the influence of the social reference groups and of the perceptions of risk as antecedents of the emotional responses that can influence the intention to get vaccinated, the feeling of trust in science (and in institutions, governments, etc.) seems to be central because it appears capable of making people go beyond the simple perception of affective factors.
Author Contributions
Conceptualization, T.R., M.B., S.P. and A.D.G.; methodology, T.R., S.P., M.B. and A.D.G.; formal analysis, M.B., C.C. and S.P.; investigation, T.R., M.B., S.P. and A.D.G.; data curation, A.G., C.B. and M.T.; writing—original draft preparation, T.R., M.B., S.P., M.T., C.B., A.G. and A.D.G.; writing—review and editing T.R., M.B., S.P., M.T., C.B., A.G. and A.D.G.; writing—result section, M.B., C.C. and S.P. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
Institutional Review Board Statement
The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Ethics Committee of E-Campus University (RN: 03/2020, 28/12/2020).
Informed Consent Statement
Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.
Conflicts of Interest
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
Funding Statement
This research received no external funding.
Footnotes
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Hornsey M.J., Harris E.A., Fielding K.S. The psychological roots of anti-vaccination attitudes: A 24-nation investigation. Health Psychol. 2018;37:307–315. doi: 10.1037/hea0000586. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.De Giorgio A. COVID-19 is not just a flu. Learn from Italy and act now. Travel Med. Infect. Dis. 2020;35:101655. doi: 10.1016/j.tmaid.2020.101655. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Su L., Du J., Du Z. Government Communication, Perceptions of COVID-19, and Vaccination Intention: A Multi-Group Comparison in China. Front. Psychol. 2022;12:783374. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.783374. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Fernandes N., Costa D., Costa D., Keating J., Arantes J. Predicting COVID-19 Vaccination Intention: The Determinants of Vaccine Hesitancy. Vaccines. 2021;9:1161. doi: 10.3390/vaccines9101161. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Wang Y., Liu Y. Multilevel determinants of COVID-19 vaccination hesitancy in the United States: A rapid systematic review. Prev. Med. Rep. 2021;25:101673. doi: 10.1016/j.pmedr.2021.101673. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Borah P., Hwang J., Hsu Y. COVID-19 Vaccination Attitudes and Intention: Message Framing and the Moderating Role of Perceived Vaccine Benefits. J. Health Commun. 2021;26:523–533. doi: 10.1080/10810730.2021.1966687. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Gerretsen P., Kim J., Caravaggio F., Quilty L., Sanches M., Wells S., Brown E.E., Agic B., Pollock B.G., Graff-Guerrero A. Individual determinants of COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy. PLoS ONE. 2021;16:e0258462. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0258462. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Sombultawee K., Boon-itt S., Bussanit V. The Adoption of Protective Health Behaviors During the COVID-19 Pandemic in Thailand. J. Behav. Sci. 2021;16:72–83. [Google Scholar]
- 9.Pivetti M., Di Battista S., Paleari F.G., Hakoköngäs E. Conspiracy beliefs and attitudes toward COVID-19 vaccinations: A conceptual replication study in Finland. J. Pac. Rim Psychol. 2021;15:18344909211039893. doi: 10.1177/18344909211039893. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Loomba S., De Figueiredo A., Piatek S.J., De Graaf K., Larson H.J. Measuring the impact of COVID-19 vaccine misinformation on vaccination intent in the UK and USA. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2021;5:337–348. doi: 10.1038/s41562-021-01056-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Thaker J., Ganchoudhuri S. The Role of Attitudes, Norms, and Efficacy on Shifting COVID-19 Vaccine Intentions: A Longitudinal Study of COVID-19 Vaccination Intentions in New Zealand. Vaccines. 2021;9:1132. doi: 10.3390/vaccines9101132. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Jin Q., Raza S.H., Yousaf M., Zaman U., Siang J.M.L.D. Can Communication Strategies Combat COVID-19 Vaccine Hesitancy with Trade-Off between Public Service Messages and Public Skepticism? Experimental Evidence from Pakistan. Vaccines. 2021;9:757. doi: 10.3390/vaccines9070757. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Petersen M.B., Bor A., Jørgensen F., Lindholt M.F. Transparent communication about negative features of COVID-19 vaccines decreases acceptance but increases trust. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2021;118:e2024597118. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2024597118. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Wynen J., Op de Beeck S., Verhoest K., Glavina M., Six F., Van Damme P., Beutels P., Hendrickx G., Pepermans K. Taking a COVID-19 Vaccine or Not? Do Trust in Government and Trust in Experts Help us to Understand Vaccination Intention? Adm. Soc. 2022:1–27. doi: 10.1177/00953997211073459. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Paul E., Steptoe A., Fancourt D. Attitudes towards vaccines and intention to vaccinate against COVID-19: Implications for public health communications. Lancet Reg. Health Eur. 2021;1:100012. doi: 10.1016/j.lanepe.2020.100012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Zheng H., Jiang S., Wu Q. Factors influencing COVID-19 vaccination intention: The roles of vaccine knowledge, vaccine risk perception, and doctor-patient communication. Patient Educ. Couns. 2022;105:277–283. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2021.09.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Dubé E., Gagnon D., Nickels E., Jeram S., Schuster M. Mapping vaccine hesitancy—Country-specific characteristics of a global phenomenon. Vaccine. 2014;32:6649–6654. doi: 10.1016/j.vaccine.2014.09.039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Kwok K.O., Li K., Wei W.I., Tang A., Wong S.Y.S., Lee S.S. Influenza vaccine uptake, COVID-19 vaccination intention and vaccine hesitancy among nurses: A survey. Int. J. Nurs. Stud. 2021;114:103854. doi: 10.1016/j.ijnurstu.2020.103854. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Chu H., Liu S. Integrating health behavior theories to predict American’s intention to receive a COVID-19 vaccine. Patient Educ. Couns. 2021;104:1878–1886. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2021.02.031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Karlsson L.C., Soveri A., Lewandowsky S., Karlsson L., Karlsson H., Nolvi S., Karukivi M., Lindfelt M., Antfolk J. Fearing the disease or the vaccine: The case of COVID-19. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2021;172:110590. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2020.110590. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Machingaidze S., Wiysonge C.S. Understanding COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy. Nat. Med. 2021;27:1338–1344. doi: 10.1038/s41591-021-01459-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Noar S.M., Zimmerman R.S. Health behavior theory and cumulative knowledge regarding health behaviors: Are we moving in the right direction? Health Educ. Res. 2005;20:275–290. doi: 10.1093/her/cyg113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Rosental H., Shmueli L. Integrating Health Behavior Theories to Predict COVID-19 Vaccine Acceptance: Differences between Medical Students and Nursing Students. Vaccines. 2021;9:783. doi: 10.3390/vaccines9070783. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Carpenter C.J. A meta-analysis of the effectiveness of health belief model variables in predicting behavior. Health Commun. 2010;25:661–669. doi: 10.1080/10410236.2010.521906. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Zampetakis L.A., Melas C. The health belief model predicts vaccination intentions against COVID-19: A survey experiment approach. Appl. Psychol. Health Well-Being. 2021;13:469–484. doi: 10.1111/aphw.12262. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Bosnjak M., Ajzen I., Schmidt P. The Theory of Planned Behavior: Selected Recent Advances and Applications. Eur. J. Psychol. 2020;16:352–356. doi: 10.5964/ejop.v16i3.3107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Wolff K. COVID-19 Vaccination Intentions: The Theory of Planned Behavior, Optimistic Bias, and Anticipated Regret. Front. Psychol. 2021;12:648289. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.648289. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Betsch C., Schmid P., Heinemeier D., Korn L., Holtmann C., Böhm R. Beyond confidence: Development of a measure assessing the 5C psychological antecedents of vaccination. PLoS ONE. 2018;13:e0208601. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0208601. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Ajzen I. The theory of planned behavior: Frequently asked questions. Hum. Behav. Emerg. Technol. 2020;2:314–324. doi: 10.1002/hbe2.195. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Hossain M.B., Alam M.Z., Islam M.S., Sultan S., Faysal M.M., Rima S., Hossain M.A., Mamun A.A. Health Belief Model, Theory of Planned Behavior, or Psychological Antecedents: What Predicts COVID-19 Vaccine Hesitancy Better Among the Bangladeshi Adults? Front. Public Health. 2021;9:711066. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2021.711066. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Patwary M.M., Bardhan M., Disha A.S., Hasan M., Haque Z., Sultana R., Hossain R., Browning M.H.E.M., Alam A., Sallam M. Determinants of COVID-19 Vaccine Acceptance among the Adult Population of Bangladesh Using the Health Belief Model and the Theory of Planned Behavior Model. Vaccines. 2021;9:1393. doi: 10.3390/vaccines9121393. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.An P.L., Nguyen H., Dang H., Huynh Q., Pham B., Huynh G. Integrating Health Behavior Theories to Predict Intention to Get a COVID-19 Vaccine. Health Serv. Insights. 2021;14:11786329211060130. doi: 10.1177/11786329211060130. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Shmueli L. Predicting intention to receive COVID-19 vaccine among the general population using the health belief model and the theory of planned behavior model. BMC Public Health. 2021;21:804. doi: 10.1186/s12889-021-10816-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Rosenstock I.M. Health Belief Model. In: Kazdin A.E., editor. Encyclopedia of Psychology. Volume 4. American Psychological Association; New York, NY, USA: 2000. pp. 78–80. [Google Scholar]
- 35.Campion V.L., Skinner C.S. The Health Belief Model. In: Kasprzyk D., editor. Health Behavior & Health Education. 4th ed. Jossey-Bass; San Francisco, CA, USA: 2008. pp. 45–64. [Google Scholar]
- 36.Lin Y., Hu Z., Zhao Q., Alias H., Danaee M., Wong L.P. Understanding COVID-19 vaccine demand and hesitancy: A nationwide online survey in China. PLoS Negl. Trop. Dis. 2020;14:e0008961. doi: 10.1371/journal.pntd.0008961. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Wong L.P., Alias H., Wong P.-F., Lee H.Y., AbuBakar S. The use of the health belief model to assess predictors of intent to receive the COVID-19 vaccine and willingness to pay. Hum. Vaccines Immunother. 2020;16:2204–2214. doi: 10.1080/21645515.2020.1790279. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Haque A., Pant A.B. Mitigating COVID-19 in the face of emerging virus variants, breakthrough infections and vaccine hesitancy. J. Autoimmun. 2022;127:102792. doi: 10.1016/j.jaut.2021.102792. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Lai X., Zhu H., Wang J., Huang Y., Jing R., Lyu Y., Zhang H., Feng H., Guo J., Fang H. Public Perceptions and Acceptance of COVID-19 Booster Vaccination in China: A Cross-Sectional Study. Vaccines. 2021;9:1461. doi: 10.3390/vaccines9121461. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Neumann-Böhme S., Varghese N.E., Sabat I., Barros P.P., Brouwer W., Van Exel J., Schreyögg J., Stargardt T. Once we have it, will we use it? A European survey on willingness to be vaccinated against COVID-19. Eur. J. Health Econ. 2020;21:977–982. doi: 10.1007/s10198-020-01208-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Mahmud I., Kabir R., Rahman M.A., Alradie-Mohamed A., Vinnakota D., Al-Mohaimeed A. The Health Belief Model Predicts Intention to Receive the COVID-19 Vaccine in Saudi Arabia: Results from a Cross-Sectional Survey. Vaccines. 2021;9:864. doi: 10.3390/vaccines9080864. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Ruiz J.B., Bell R.A. Predictors of intention to vaccinate against COVID-19: Results of a nationwide survey. Vaccine. 2021;37:1080–1086. doi: 10.1016/j.vaccine.2021.01.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Suess C., Maddock J.E., Dogru T., Mody M., Lee S. Using the Health Belief Model to examine travelers’ willingness to vaccinate and support for vaccination requirements prior to travel. Tour Manag. 2022;88:104405. doi: 10.1016/j.tourman.2021.104405. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Maraqa B., Nazzal Z., Rabi R., Sarhan N., Al-Shakhra K. COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy among health care workers in Palestine: A call for action. Prev. Med. 2021;149:106618. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2021.106618. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Chen H., Li X., Gao J., Liu X., Mao Y., Wang R., Zheng P., Xiao Q., Jia Y., Fu H., et al. Health Belief Model Perspective on the Control of COVID-19 Vaccine Hesitancy and the Promotion of Vaccination in China: Web-Based Cross-sectional Study. J. Med. Internet Res. 2021;23:e29329. doi: 10.2196/29329. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Coco M., Guerrera C.S., Santisi G., Riggio F., Grass R., Di Corrado D., Di Nuovo S., Ramaci T. Psychosocial Impact and Role of Resilience on Healthcare Workers during COVID-19 Pandemic. Sustainability. 2021;13:7096. doi: 10.3390/su13137096. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Scrima F., Miceli S., Caci B., Cardaci M. The relationship between fear of COVID-19 and intention to get vaccinated. The serial mediation roles of existential anxiety and conspiracy beliefs. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2022;184:111188. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2021.111188. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Birhanu Z., Ambelu A., Fufa D., Mecha M., Zeynudin A., Abafita J., Belay A., Doyore F., Oljira L., Bacha E., et al. Risk perceptions and attitudinal responses to COVID-19 pandemic: An online survey in Ethiopia. BMC Public Health. 2021;21:981. doi: 10.1186/s12889-021-10939-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Clavel N., Badr J., Gautier L., Lavoie-Tremblay M., Paquette J. Risk Perceptions, Knowledge and Behaviors of General and High-Risk Adult Populations Towards COVID-19: A Systematic Scoping Review. Public Health Rev. 2021;42:1603979. doi: 10.3389/phrs.2021.1603979. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.González-Castro J.L., Ubillos-Landa S., Puente-Martínez A., Gracia-Leiva M. Perceived Vulnerability and Severity Predict Adherence to COVID-19 Protection Measures: The Mediating Role of Instrumental Coping. Front. Psychol. 2021;12:674032. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.674032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Hilverda F., Vollmann M. The Role of Risk Perception in Students’ COVID-19 Vaccine Uptake: A Longitudinal Study. Vaccines. 2022;10:22. doi: 10.3390/vaccines10010022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Wake A.D. The Willingness to Receive COVID-19 Vaccine and Its Associated Factors: “Vaccination Refusal Could Prolong the War of This Pandemic”—A Systematic Review. Risk Manag. Healthc. Policy. 2021;14:2609–2623. doi: 10.2147/RMHP.S311074. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Sekizawa Y., Hashimoto S., Denda K., Ochi S., So M. Association between COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy and generalized trust, depression, generalized anxiety, and fear of COVID-19. BMC Public Health. 2022;22:126. doi: 10.1186/s12889-021-12479-w. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Ajzen I. The theory of planned behavior. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 1991;50:179–211. doi: 10.1016/0749-5978(91)90020-T. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Xiao X., Wong R.M. Vaccine hesitancy and perceived behavioral control: A meta-analysis. Vaccine. 2020;38:5131–5138. doi: 10.1016/j.vaccine.2020.04.076. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.La Barbera F., Ajzen I. Control Interactions in the Theory of Planned Behavior: Rethinking the Role of Subjective Norm. Eur. J. Psychol. 2020;16:401–417. doi: 10.5964/ejop.v16i3.2056. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Wolff K., Nordin K., Brun W., Berglund G., Kvale G. Affective and cognitive attitudes, uncertainty avoidance and intention to obtain genetic testing: An extension of the theory of planned Behaviour. Psychol. Health. 2011;26:1143–1155. doi: 10.1080/08870441003763253. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Agarwal V. A/H1N1 vaccine intentions in college students: An application of the theory of planned behavior. J. Am. Coll. Health. 2014;6:416–424. doi: 10.1080/07448481.2014.917650. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Catalano P.H., Richards K., Hawkins H.K. Theory of planned behavior-based correlates of HPV vaccination intentions and series completion among University Students in the Southeastern United States. Health Educ. 2017;49:35–44. doi: 10.1080/07448481.2016.1269771. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Gagnon M., Godin G. Young Adults and HIV Vaccine: Determinants of the Intention of Getting Immunized. Can. J. Public Health. 2000;91:432–434. doi: 10.1007/BF03404823. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Fan Y.V., Jiang P., Hemzal M., Klemeš J.J. An update of COVID-19 influence on waste management. Sci. Total Environ. 2021;275:142014. doi: 10.1016/j.scitotenv.2020.142014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Sherman S.M., Smith L.E., Sim J., Amlôt R., Cutts M., Dasch H., Rubin G.J., Sevdalis N. COVID-19 vaccination intention in the UK: Results from the COVID-19 vaccination acceptability study (CoVAccS), a nationally representative cross-sectional survey. Hum. Vaccines Immunother. 2020;17:1612–1621. doi: 10.1080/21645515.2020.1846397. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Yahaghi R., Ahmadizade S., Fotuhi R., Taherkhani E., Ranjbaran M., Buchali Z., Jafari R., Zamani N., Shahbazkhania A., Simiari H., et al. Fear of COVID-19 and Perceived COVID-19 Infectability Supplement Theory of Planned Behavior to Explain Iranians’ Intention to Get COVID-19 Vaccinated. Vaccines. 2021;9:684. doi: 10.3390/vaccines9070684. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Hamilton K., Van Dongen A., Hagger M. An extended theory of planned behavior for parent-for-child health behaviors: A metaanalysis. Health Psychol. 2020;39:863–878. doi: 10.1037/hea0000940. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Zhang K.C., Fang Y., Cao H., Chen H., Hu T., Chen Y.Q., Zhou X., Wang Z. Parental Acceptability of COVID-19 Vaccination for Children under the Age of 18 Years: Cross-Sectional Online Survey. [(accessed on 21 April 2021)];JMIR Pediatr. Parent. 2020 3:e24827. doi: 10.2196/24827. Available online: https://preprints.jmir.org/preprint/24827. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Sniehotta F.F., Presseau J., Ara jo-Soares V. Time to retire the theory of planned behaviour. Health Psychol. 2014;8:1–7. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2013.869710. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Winter K., Pummerer L., Hornsey M.J., Sassenberg K. Pro-vaccination subjective norms moderate the relationship between conspiracy mentality and vaccination intentions. Br. J. Health Psychol. 2022;27:390–405. doi: 10.1111/bjhp.12550. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Husain F., Shahnawaz M.G., Khan N.H., Parveen H., Savani K. Intention to get COVID-19 vaccines: Exploring the role of attitudes, subjective norms, perceived behavioral control, belief in COVID-19 misinformation, and vaccine confidence in Northern India. Hum. Vaccines Immunother. 2021;17:3941–3953. doi: 10.1080/21645515.2021.1967039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Rad R.E., Kahnouji K., Mohseni S., Shahabi N., Noruziyan F., Farshidi H., Hosseinpoor M., Kashani S., Takhti H.K., Azad M.H., et al. Predicting the COVID-19 vaccine receive intention based on the theory of reasoned action in the south of Iran. BMC Public Health. 2022;22:229. doi: 10.1186/s12889-022-12517-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Visser O., Hulscher M.E., Antonise-Kamp L., Akkermans R., Van der Velden K., Ruiter R.A., Hautvast J.L. Assessing determinants of the intention to accept a pertussis cocooning vaccination: A survey among healthcare workers in maternity and paediatric care. Vaccine. 2018;36:736–743. doi: 10.1016/j.vaccine.2017.12.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Xiao X., Borah P. Do norms matter? Examining norm-based messages in HPV vaccination promotion. Health Commun. 2020;36:1476–1484. doi: 10.1080/10410236.2020.1770506. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Shmueli L. The Role of Incentives in Deciding to Receive the Available COVID-19 Vaccine in Israel. Vaccines. 2022;10:77. doi: 10.3390/vaccines10010077. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Brambilla M., Sacchi S., Rusconi P., Goodwin G. The primacy of morality in impression development: Theory, research, and future directions. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2021;64:187–262. doi: 10.1016/bs.aesp.2021.03.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Chu A., Gupta V., Unni E.J. Utilizing the Theory of Planned Behavior to determine the intentions to receive the influenza vaccine during COVID-19: A cross-sectional survey of US adults. Prev. Med. Rep. 2021;23:101417. doi: 10.1016/j.pmedr.2021.101417. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Lewandowsky S., Gignac G.E., Oberauer K. The role of conspiracist ideation and worldviews in predicting rejection of science. PLoS ONE. 2013;8:e75637. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0075637. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Baumgaertner B., Carlisle J.E., Justwan F. The influence of political ideology and trust on willingness to vaccinate. PLoS ONE. 2018;13:e0191728. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0191728. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Pagliaro S., Sacchi S., Pacilli M.G., Brambilla M., Lionetti F., Bettache K., Bianchi M., Biella M., Bonnot V., Boza M., et al. Trust predicts COVID-19 prescribed and discretionary behavioral intentions in 23 countries. PLoS ONE. 2021;16:e0248334. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0248334. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Ramaci T., Pagliaro S., Teresi M., Barattucci M. Job Demands and Negative Outcomes after the Lockdown: The Moderating Role of Stigma towards Italian Supermarket Workers. Sustainability. 2021;13:7507. doi: 10.3390/su13137507. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Sturgis P., Brunton-Smith I., Jackson J. Trust in science, social consensus and vaccine confidence. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2021;5:1528–1534. doi: 10.1038/s41562-021-01115-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Kerr J.R., Schneider C.R., Recchia G., Dryhurst S., Sahlin U., Dufouil C., Arwidson P., Freeman A.L., Van der Linden S. Correlates of intended COVID-19 vaccine acceptance across time and countries: Results from a series of cross-sectional surveys. BMJ Open. 2021;11:e048025. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2020-048025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Bronfman N., Repetto P., Cisternas P., Castañeda J., Cordón P. Government Trust and Motivational Factors on Health Protective Behaviors to Prevent COVID-19 Among Young Adults. Int. J. Public Health. 2022;67:1604290. doi: 10.3389/ijph.2022.1604290. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Barattucci M., Chirico A., Kuvačić G., De Giorgio A. Rethinking the Role of Affect in Risk Judgment: What We Have Learned from COVID-19 During the First Week of Quarantine in Italy. Front. Psychol. 2020;11:554561. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.554561. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Liu M., Chen Y., Shi D., Yan T. The Public’s Risk Information Seeking and Avoidance in China During Early Stages of the COVID-19 Outbreak. Front. Psychol. 2021;12:649180. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.649180. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Godbersen H., Hofmann L.A., Ruiz-Fernández S. How People Evaluate Anti-Corona Measures for Their Social Spheres: Attitude, Subjective Norm, and Perceived Behavioral Control. Front. Psychol. 2020;11:567405. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.567405. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Brewer N.T., Hall M.E., Malo T.L., Gilkey M.B., Quinn B., Lathren C. Announcements versus conversations to improve HPV vaccination coverage: A randomized trial. Pediatrics. 2017;139:e20161764. doi: 10.1542/peds.2016-1764. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Mohamed Ghazy R., Abd ElHafeez S., Shaaban R., Elbarazi I., Shawky Abdou M., Ramadan A., Kheirallah K.A. Determining the Cutoff Points of the 5C Scale for Assessment of COVID-19Vaccines Psychological Antecedents among the Arab Population: A Multinational Study. J. Prim. Care Community Health. 2021;12:21501327211018568. doi: 10.1177/21501327211018568. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Jaffe A.E., Graupensperger S., Blayney J.A., Duckworth J.C., Stappenbeck C.A. The Role of Perceived Social Norms in College Student Vaccine Hesitancy: Implications for COVID-19 Prevention Strategies. Vaccine. 2022;40:1888–1895. doi: 10.1016/j.vaccine.2022.01.038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Larson H.J., Jarrett C., Eckersberger E., Smith D.M., Paterson P. Understanding vaccine hesitancy around vaccines and vaccination from a global perspective: A systematic review of published literature, 2007–2012. Vaccine. 2014;32:2150–2159. doi: 10.1016/j.vaccine.2014.01.081. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.MacDonald N.E., SAGE Working Group on Vaccine Hesitancy Vaccine hesitancy: Definition, scope and determinants. Vaccine. 2015;33:4161–4164. doi: 10.1016/j.vaccine.2015.04.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Ransing R., Kukreti P., Raghuveer P., Puri M., Paranjape A.D., Patil S., Hegde P., Padma K., Kumar P., Kishore J., et al. A brief psycho-social intervention for COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy among perinatal women in low-and middle-income countries: Need of the hour. Asian J. Psychiatry. 2022;67:102929. doi: 10.1016/j.ajp.2021.102929. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Thomson A., Robinson K., Vallée-Tourangeau G. The 5As: A practical taxonomy for the determinants of vaccine uptake. Vaccine. 2016;34:1018–1024. doi: 10.1016/j.vaccine.2015.11.065. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Anderson R.M., Vegvari C., Truscott J., Collyer B.S. Challenges in creating herd immunity to SARS-CoV-2 infection by mass vaccination. Lancet. 2020;396:1614–1616. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)32318-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Plohl N., Musil B. Modeling compliance with COVID-19 prevention guidelines: The critical role of trust in science. Psychol. Health Med. 2021;26:1–12. doi: 10.1080/13548506.2020.1772988. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Akther T., Nur T. A model of factors influencing COVID.19 vaccine acceptance: A synthesis of the theory of reasoned action, conspiracy theory belief, awareness, perceived usefulness, and perceived ease of use. PLoS ONE. 2022;17:e0261869. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0261869. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Giuliani M., Ichino A., Bonomi A., Martoni R., Cammino S., Gorini A. Who Is Willing to Get Vaccinated? A Study into the Psychological, Socio-Demographic, and Cultural Determinants of COVID-19 Vaccination Intentions. Vaccines. 2021;9:810. doi: 10.3390/vaccines9080810. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Bucchi M., Fattorini E., Saracino B. Public Perception of COVID-19 Vaccination in Italy: The Role of Trust and Experts’ Communication. Int. J. Public Health. 2022;67:1604222. doi: 10.3389/ijph.2022.1604222. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.De Giorgio A., Kuvacic G., Maleš D., Vecchio I., Tornali C., Ishac W., Ramaci T., Barattucci M., Milavi’c B. Willingness to Receive COVID-19 Booster Vaccine: Associations between Green-Pass, Social Media Information, Anti-Vax Beliefs, and Emotional Balance. Vaccines. 2022;10:481. doi: 10.3390/vaccines10030481. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Flanagan P., Dowling M., Gethin G. Barriers and Facilitators to Seasonal Influenza Vaccination Uptake among Nurses: A Mixed Methods Study. [(accessed on 11 March 2021)];J. Adv. Nurs. 2020 76:1746–1764. doi: 10.1111/jan.14360. Available online: https://publons.com/publon/10.1111/jan.14360. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Ramaci T., Barattucci M., Ledda C., Rapisarda V. Social Stigma during COVID-19and its Impact on HCWs Outcomes. Sustainability. 2020;12:3834. doi: 10.3390/su12093834. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Zhang J., While A.E., Norman I.J. Nurses’ Vaccination against Pandemic H1N1 Influenza and Their Knowledge and Other Factors. Vaccine. 2012;30:4813–4819. doi: 10.1016/j.vaccine.2012.05.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Nan X. Communicating to young adults about HPV vaccination: Consideration of message framing, motivation, and gender. Health Commun. 2012;27:10–18. doi: 10.1080/10410236.2011.567447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Chapman G.B., Coups E.J. Predictors of influenza vaccine acceptance among healthy adults. Prev. Med. 1999;29:249–262. doi: 10.1006/pmed.1999.0535. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Kumar S., Quinn S.C., Kim K.H., Musa D., Hilyard K.M., Freimuth V.S. The social ecological model as a framework for determinants of 2009 H1N1 influenza vaccine uptake in the United States. Health Educ. Behav. 2012;39:229–243. doi: 10.1177/1090198111415105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Machida M., Nakamura I., Kojima T., Saito R., Nakaya T., Hanibuchi T., Takamiya T., Odagiri Y., Fukushima N., Kikuchi H., et al. Trends in COVID-19 vaccination intent from pre- to post-COVID-19 vaccine distribution and their associations with the 5C psychological antecedents of vaccination by sex and age in Japan. Hum. Vaccines Immunother. 2021;17:3954–3962. doi: 10.1080/21645515.2021.1968217. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Malik A.A., McFadden S.M., Elharake J., Omer S.B. Determinants of COVID-19 vaccine acceptance in the US. EClinicalMedicine. 2020;26:100495. doi: 10.1016/j.eclinm.2020.100495. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Murphy J., Vallières F., Bentall R.P., Shevlin M., McBride O., Hartman T.K., McKay R., Bennett K., Mason L., Gibson-Miller J., et al. Psychological characteristics associated with COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy and resistance in Ireland and the United Kingdom. Nat. Commun. 2021;12:29. doi: 10.1038/s41467-020-20226-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Rhodes A., Hoq M., Measey M.-A., Danchin M. Intention to vaccinate against COVID-19 in Australia. Lancet Infect. Dis. 2020;21:e110. doi: 10.1016/S1473-3099(20)30724-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Salmon D.A., Dudley M.Z., Glanz J.M., Omer S.B. Vaccine hesitancy: Causes, consequences, and a call to action. Am. J. Prev. Med. 2015;49((Suppl. S4)):S391–S398. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2015.06.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Breslin G., Dempster M., Berry E., Cavanagh M., Nicola C., Armstrong N.C. COVID-19 vaccine uptake and hesitancy survey in Northern Ireland and Republic of Ireland: Applying the theory of planned behaviour. PLoS ONE. 2021;16:e0259381. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0259381. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110.Cafagna D., Barattucci M. Risk perception and personality: A study in the transportation sector. G. Ital. Med. Lav. Ergon. 2019;41:211–220. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Savadori L., Rumiati R., Bonini N., Pedon A. Percezione del rischio: Esperti vs non esperti. Arch. Psicol. Neurol. Psichiatr. 1998;59:387–405. [Google Scholar]
- 112.Keller C., Bostrom A., Kuttschreuter M., Savadori L., Spence A., White M. Bringing appraisal theory to environmental risk perception: A review of conceptual approaches of the past 40 years and suggestions for future research. J. Risk Res. 2012;15:237–256. doi: 10.1080/13669877.2011.634523. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 113.Fishbein M., Ajzen I. Predicting and Changing Behavior: The Reasoned Action Approach. Psychology Press; New York, NY, USA: 2010. [Google Scholar]
- 114.Istituto Superiore di Sanità . Relazione dell’Istituto Superiore di Sanità sui Risultati Dell’attività Svolta nel 2011. Istituto Superiore di Sanità; Roma, Italy: 2012. [Google Scholar]
- 115.Tamburrano A., Mellucci C., Galletti C., Vitale D., Vallone D., Barbara A., Sguera A., Zega M., Damiani G., Laurenti P. Improving Nursing Staff Attitudes toward Vaccinations through Academic Detailing: The HProImmune Questionnaire as a Tool for Medical Management. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2019;16:2006. doi: 10.3390/ijerph16112006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116.Slovic P., Finucane M.L., Peters E., MacGregor D.G. Risk as analysis and risk as feelings: Some thoughts about affect, reason, risk, and rationality. Risk Anal. 2004;24:311–322. doi: 10.1111/j.0272-4332.2004.00433.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117.Brown V.J. Risk perception: It’s personal. Environ. Health Perspect. 2014;122:A276–A279. doi: 10.1289/ehp.122-A276. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118.Slovic P.E. The Perception of Risk. Earthscan Publications; London, UK: 2000. [Google Scholar]
- 119.Podsakoff P., MacKenzie S.B., Lee J.-Y., Podsakoff N.P. Common Method Biases in Behavioral Research: A Critical Review of the Literature and Recommended Remedies. J. Appl. Psychol. 2003;88:879–903. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.88.5.879. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120.Richardson H.M., Simmering M.J., Sturman M. A Tale of Three Perspectives. Organ. Res. Methods. 2009;12:762–800. doi: 10.1177/1094428109332834. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 121.AlShurman B.A., Khan A.F., Mac C., Majeed M., Butt Z.A. What Demographic, Social, and Contextual Factors Influence the Intention to Use COVID-19 Vaccines: A Scoping Review. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2021;18:9342. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18179342. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122.Sallam M., Al-Sanafi M., Sallam M. A Global Map of COVID-19 Vaccine Acceptance Rates per Country: An Updated Concise Narrative Review. J. Multidiscip. Healthc. 2022;15:21–45. doi: 10.2147/JMDH.S347669. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 123.Patwary M.M., Alam M.A., Bardhan M., Disha A.S., Haque M.Z., Billah S.M., Kabir M.P., Browning M., Rahman M.M., Parsa A.D., et al. COVID-19 Vaccine Acceptance among Low- and Lower-Middle-Income Countries: A Rapid Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Vaccines. 2022;10:427. doi: 10.3390/vaccines10030427. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 124.Corace K.M., Srigley J.A., Hargadon D.P., Yu D., MacDonald T.K., Fabrigar L.R., Garber G.E. Using Behavior Change Frameworks to Improve Healthcare Worker Influenza Vaccination Rates: A Systematic Review. Vaccine. 2016;34:3235–3242. doi: 10.1016/j.vaccine.2016.04.071. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 125.Hopman C.E., Riphagen-Dalhuisen J., Looijmans-van den Akker I., Frijstein G., Van der Geest-Blankert A.D.J., Danhof-Pont M.B., De Jager H.J., Bos A.A., Smeets E., De Vries M.J.T., et al. Determination of Factors Required to Increase Uptake of Influenza Vaccination among Hospital-Based Healthcare Workers. J. Hosp. Infect. 2011;77:327–331. doi: 10.1016/j.jhin.2010.10.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 126.Folcarelli L., Miraglia del Giudice G., Corea F., Angelillo I.F. Intention to Receive the COVID-19 Vaccine Booster Dose in a University Community in Italy. Vaccines. 2022;10:146. doi: 10.3390/vaccines10020146. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 127.Santirocchi A., Spataro P., Costanzi M., Doricchi F., Rossi-Arnaud C., Cestari V. Predictors of the Intention to Be Vaccinated against COVID-19 in a Sample of Italian Respondents at the Start of the Immunization Campaign. J. Pers. Med. 2022;12:111. doi: 10.3390/jpm12010111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 128.French J., Deshpande S., Evans W., Obregon R. Key guidelines in developing a pre-emptive COVID-19 vaccination uptake promotion strategy. Int. J. Environ. Res. 2020;17:5893. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17165893. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 129.Simione L., Vagni M., Gnagnarella C., Bersani G., Pajardi D. Mistrust and Beliefs in Conspiracy Theories Differently Mediate the Effects of Psychological Factors on Propensity for COVID-19 Vaccine. Front. Psychol. 2021;12:683684. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.683684. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 130.Bliese P.D., Edwards J.R., Sonnentag S. Stress and well-being at work: A century of empirical trends reflecting theoretical and societal influences. J. Appl. Psychol. 2017;102:389–402. doi: 10.1037/apl0000109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 131.Morgeson F.P., Mitchell T.R., Liu D. Event System Theory: An Event-Oriented Approach to the Organizational Sciences. Acad. Manag. Rev. 2015;40:515–537. doi: 10.5465/amr.2012.0099. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 132.Egede L.E., Ellis C. Development and testing of the multidimensional trust in health care systems scale. Gen. Intern. Med. 2008;23:808–815. doi: 10.1007/s11606-008-0613-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 133.Griva K., Tan K., Chan F., Periakaruppan R., Ong B., Soh A., Chen M.I. Evaluating Rates and Determinants of COVID-19 Vaccine Hesitancy for Adults and Children in the Singapore Population: Strengthening Our Community’s Resilience against Threats from Emerging Infections (SOCRATEs) Cohort. Vaccines. 2021;9:1415. doi: 10.3390/vaccines9121415. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 134.Maftei A., Holman A.C. SARS-CoV-2 Threat Perception and Willingness to Vaccinate: The Mediating Role of Conspiracy Beliefs. Front. Psychol. 2021;12:672634. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.672634. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 135.Bruder M., Haffke P., Neave N., Nouripanah N., Imhoff R. Measuring individual differences in generic beliefs in conspiracy theories across cultures: Conspiracy Mentality Questionnaire. Front. Psychol. 2013;4:225. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00225. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 136.Zhang X., Guo Y., Zhou Q., Tan Z., Cao J. The Mediating Roles of Medical Mistrust, Knowledge, Confidence and Complacency of Vaccines in the Pathways from Conspiracy Beliefs to Vaccine Hesitancy. Vaccines. 2021;9:1342. doi: 10.3390/vaccines9111342. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]