Abstract
The aim of this paper is to demonstrate how Bandura's Social Cognitive Theory (1986) influenced the development of several complementary models of the construct of Self-Regulation. Building on the foundation of Self-Determination Theory, SDT (2000), and Zimmerman's Self-Regulation Theory, SR (2001), with their assumptions, contributions, goddesses, and limitations, we come to the Self- vs. External Regulatory Theory, SR-ER (2021). Finally, we integrate recent evidence demonstrating the explanatory adequacy of the SR vs. ER model for different psychological constructions in different settings related to education, health, clinical practice and social work. Complementary, a new theoretical and empirical research agenda is presented, to continue testing the adequacy of SR vs. ER assumptions, and to better understand the behavioral variability of the different constructs studied.
Keywords: Albert Bandura, social cognitive theory, self-determination, self-regulation, self- vs. external regulation
Preface
This article is dedicated to Prof. Albert E. Bandura (1925-2021), outstanding human being and one of the most influential psychologists of all time. Bandura's ground-breaking Bobo doll experiment gave rise to the field of social learning theory, later renamed social cognitive theory. The construct of self-efficacy was identified and described by Bandura. He challenged the core assertions of behaviorism and put forward his agentic theory of human behavior. A recent APA tribute (2021) to Albert Bandura summarizes highlights of his career: “Bandura was elected APA president in 1973 and encouraged our organization to pursue matters of public interest. Bandura's significant contributions to the field of psychology were recognized in 1980 with APA's Distinguished Scientific Contribution Award and in 2004 with our Award for Outstanding Lifetime Contribution to Psychology. He also received the Gold Medal Award for Distinguished Lifetime Contribution to Psychological Science from APF and the Lifetime Career Award from the International Union of Psychological Science. In 2016, he was awarded the National Medal of Science by President Barack Obama. Albert Bandura was a giant in the field, with work that influenced social, cognitive, developmental, educational, and clinical psychology. …Bandura's contribution is irreplaceable; without it, the current view of human educational and social processes would be impossible. His writings have always marked a before and after in our understanding of psychoeducational processes”.
Introduction
Every researcher knows that there is nothing more practical than a good theory—though not every theory can be equally applicable in practice (Berkman and Wilson, 2021). Bandura's Social Cognitive Theory (Bandura, 1986, 1991, 1999, 2004a,b, 2005, 2006) addresses the process by which a person acquires knowledge, beliefs, attitudes and ways of thinking in regard to the social environment. The foundation of this theory is that learning is an agentic, cognitive process that exists and is understood within a context of family, school, work or other (Bandura, 2006). This theoretical model established several explanatory mechanisms or types of learning -essentially human- such as learning through vicarious mechanisms or through self-regulatory mechanisms, thereby questioning and expanding the prevailing vision of the day, that of learning by classical and operant conditioning.
The aim of this manuscript is to comparatively analyze three existing theoretical models in educational psychology, all of which have adopted the construct of self-regulated behavior as a core element, but have established different explanatory mechanisms to explain its role in processes of human development and learning processes. Therefore, starting from a definition of the construct itself, the different theoretical positions will be analyzed (including goodnesses and limitations), to conclude with a prospective research proposal.
Self-Regulation Behavior
The construct of Self-Regulation (SR) is a personality-related construction (Mithaug, 1993; Boekaerts et al., 1999; Hoyle, 2010) that describes a person's ability to plan, monitor, and evaluate their own behavior (Brown, 1998; Vohs and Baumeister, 2016). Pervin (1988) study defined the classical understanding of this psychological construction. The initial conceptualization of self-regulation, situated at the molecular level of psychological analysis (de la Fuente et al., 2019a), adopts three principles:
1) SR is a variable pertaining to the subject and determined by other subject variables or factors, such as personality and metacognitive factors (Hoyle, 2010; Valikhani et al., 2020; Vega et al., 2020).
2) Contextual factors are considered indirectly, as having a more tangential role in explaining variability or defining the level of a person's behavioral regulation, whether referring to general behavior or specific, education- or health-related behavior.
3) People are assumed to have a higher or lower level of self-regulation, without attempting to define SR categories.
The plentiful previous research has documented numerous relationships with SR: personal adjustment factors are positively related (Wrosch et al., 2003); in personality factors, Conscientiousness is positively related and Neuroticism relates negatively (Guido et al., 2015; de la Fuente et al., 2020a); and SR is positively related to well-adjusted behavior in academic achievement (Blair and Raver, 2015; Bernardo et al., 2019).
Self-Regulation in Bandura's Theory
In Bandura's social cognitive theory (Bandura, 1986), there are interactions between personal factors (e.g., cognitions, feelings, skills), behavioral factors (e.g., strategy use, help-seeking actions), and environmental factors (e.g., classrooms, homes, work environments), through the concept of triadic reciprocal causality, all of which affect the individual's functioning (Usher and Schunk, 2018). The personal variable of self-efficacy (self-referential beliefs about the probability of adequate performance) results from these reciprocal influences. Prior research has demonstrated that behaviors like choice of tasks, persistence, effort, and achievement are influenced by self-efficacy beliefs (Schunk and DiBenedetto, 2016). Self-efficacy in turn is modified by students' behaviors. Students observe their progress toward learning goals as they work on their tasks. For example, assignments completed is one of many progress indicators that reinforce students' sense of capability for performing, and so increase their self-efficacy for further learning (Schunk and DiBenedetto, 2016).
Research has verified these reciprocal influences between self-efficacy and environmental variables in students with learning disabilities, who often have low self-efficacy for learning (Licht and Kistner, 1986). These individuals may react to their environment based on environment-related attributes instead of their own behavioral attributes. The learner's behaviors and the learner's environment can influence each other. The environment is influencing behavior when students pay attention to the visual without giving it much thought. Student behaviors, meanwhile, can also modify the instructional environment.
According to social cognitive theory, the individual pursues a sense of agency, that is, the purpose and skills to intervene and take action (Bandura, 1987, 1991), accompanied by the belief that they can exert substantial control over important aspects of their life. Self-regulation and self-efficacy are pathways to experiencing a greater sense of agency or agentic perspective (Bandura, 2001). Use of self-regulatory skills increases a students' feelings of efficacy about learning and performing well; this in turn leads to increased motivation, effort, persistence, and learning. Students' perception that they are learning enhances their agency beliefs.
Three Complementary Models of Self-Regulation Derived from Bandura's Theory
Different theoretical models have emerged from research rooted in Bandura's Social Cognitive Theory (1986). Addressing the concept of Self-Regulation (SR) either directly or indirectly, three models rise from different fields of Psychology. Summarized and presented below, they are the object of analysis in this paper (see Table 1).
Table 1.
Summary chart of the theoretical approaches toward Self-Regulation analyzed in this paper.
Theory | Year | Discipline | Object of study | Concepts/Paradigm | Motivation types | Applications | Limitations |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. Social Cognitive Theory | 1997 | Social psychology | Person, context | Self-efficacy, Self-regulation | Intrinsic, extrinsic | Social, educational, clinical | Micro-level analysis |
2. Self-Determination Theory (SDT) | 1985 | Developmental psych/learning | Person, context | SR (Autonomy) vs. ER (Heteronomy) | Introjected motiv /a-motivation/external motivation | Wellbeing/ psychopathology autonomy-competence | Human development process, external regulation = external dys-motivation |
3. Self-Regulated Learning Theory (SRL) | 2001 | Psychology of learning | Person, context | Self-Regulated Learning (SRL) | SR motivation | Learning process | Molar-level processes |
4. SRL vs. ERL Theory (SRL vs. ERL) | 2017 | Instructional psychology | Person x context | SR vs. ER Learning Processes | SRL vs. ERL motivation, regulatory | Teaching and Learning Process | Molar-level processes |
5. SR vs. ER Theory (SR vs. ER) | 2021 | Multiple spheres of psychology | Person x context | SR vs. ER in different contexts | SR x ER Factors | Different contexts | Micro- and molecular level process analysis |
Self-Determination Theory: Encouraging the Development of Autonomy
The first model, Self-Determination Theory (SDT) (Deci and Ryan, 1985a,b, 2008; Ryan and Deci, 2000b, 2006, 2020a,b) is a heuristic model of human development in interaction with the environment. SDT serves to explain how human motivation is largely determined by the needs for self-determination and autonomy. The impact of this theory in research and applied practice has been unquestionable, especially in the educational sphere of special educational needs. A Google Scholar search on self-determination and self-regulation yields a total of 63,000 documents (18-Oct-2021). This proposed theoretical framework has an indirect link to Albert Bandura's model because it gives shape to an interactive, combined conception of the mechanisms of motivation and human regulation. It concurs with Bandura's model in assuming that behavior and its development can be determined both internally and externally; furthermore, it establishes the sequential process for externally regulated behavior to become internalized. Consequently, both share the construct of self-regulation as a core explanatory element, and give importance to external factors as a regulatory mechanism.
Assumptions
SDT is a theoretical model of the molecular-molar order (de la Fuente et al., 2019a). Its focus is to explain human development and wellbeing using an explanatory philosophical paradigm that adopts the concepts of autonomous development, as opposed to heteronomous and anomic development (“autonomy” retains its primary etymological meaning of self-governance, or rule by self-control). Heteronomy, as the direct opposite, refers to “regulation from outside the phenomenal self, by forces experienced as alien or pressuring, be they inner impulses or demands, or external contingencies of reward and punishment” (Deci and Ryan, 1985a, p. 1562). In reaction to the external, behaviorist paradigm of twenty years ago, Self-Determination Theory is based on three essential concepts (Deci and Ryan, 1994; Deci et al., 1996; Ryan and Deci, 2017a,b, 2020a,b): (1) Autonomy involves initiative and ownership of one's actions. Experiences that correspond to a person's interest and value support autonomy, while external control, either by rewards or punishment, undermines autonomy. (2) Competence corresponds to a sense of mastery and of being able to succeed and grow. Competence is best promoted by optimal challenges, positive feedback, and growth opportunities, offered within well-structured settings. (3) Relatedness involves feelings of belonging and connection and is promoted by the expression of caring and respect.
This model is widely accepted and is backed by a large volume of empirical evidence (Deci and Ryan, 1985a,b,c, 2000; Deci et al., 1994, 1996; Ryan and Deci, 2017a,b, 2020a,b; Howard et al., 2022). A recent meta-analysis reported that ego-involved motives were positively related not only to persistence and performance goals, but also to indicators of well-being. By contrast, motivation driven by a desire to obtain rewards or avoid punishment was associated with decreased well-being, and there was no association with performance or persistence. Amotivation, for its part, was related to poor outcomes (Hagger and Hamilton, 2020).
Motivational and Regulatory Style
Self-Determination theory has elevated the role of the student in responding to their own motivations. It conceptualizes development on the basis of personal needs, and motivation as a progressive internalizing process from external influences to internal ones, where the person constructively defines their own personal needs and motivations. The theory is based on the following assumptions: (Ryan and Deci, 2000b, p.1; see Fig. 1):
(1) There are multiple types of motivation with their own unique characteristic phenomenology and dynamics. The concepts of amotivation, intrinsic motivation and extrinsic motivation are taken from this theory (Ryan and Deci, 2017a,b). Types of motivation can be ordered on a self-determination continuum (Howard et al., 2017; Ryan and Deci, 2017a,b, 2020a,b), where intrinsic motivation lies on the end of high self-determination, and amotivation at the opposite end, where self-determination is absent. Partially self-determined states, such as introjection, lie between the two extremes.
(2) Regulation styles result from the view of self-determination of motivation (Howard et al., 2017), and also range from extrinsic to intrinsic. Extrinsic regulation stems from externally imposed rewards and punishments and is typically experienced as controlled, non-autonomous motivation. When extrinsic motivation has become partly internalized, we refer to introjected regulation, or regulation by internal rewards of self-esteem for success and by avoidance of anxiety, shame, or guilt for failure. In academic activities, introjected regulation often involves the ego (Deci et al., 1982); self-esteem is contingent on outcomes, resulting in “internally controlled” regulation.
(3) Attributions of outcomes and the corresponding perceived causality are established according to type of motivation. A meta-analytic, structural equation model revealed total effects of autonomy orientation on behavior, comprising direct and indirect effects through autonomous motivation. There was also a positive direct effect of control orientation on behavior, and a negative indirect effect through controlled motivation (Hagger and Hamilton, 2020). This motivational model has also been transferred to other fields such as health (Ntoumanis et al., 2020; Vallerand, 2021).
Limitations
Limitations of these concepts have been recognized, in that they do not reflect a conceptual continuum, nor are they presented as complementary (not mutually exclusive). Moreover, the role of type of context as an influence in motivational processes has not been sufficiently accounted for.
(1) The authors themselves acknowledge this in their model, which emerges from the Psychology of Human Development, with extrapolations for improved learning and teaching (Ryan and Deci, 2000b). We find ample evidence and dissemination of this model in the study of special educational needs of students, including assessment and intervention (Almukhambetova and Hernández-Torrano, 2020). However, the model does not specify discrete processes of regulation of learning, nor the specific strategies of regulating motivation before, during and after the execution of a given task, as is reflected in the model by Zimmerman and Schunk (2001).
(2) The model's concept of external regulation focuses on control or application of external contingencies (a behavioral perspective) (Ryan and Deci, 2000b), and not on the possible external promotion or facilitation of the student's self-regulation. There is plentiful evidence that external regulation—understood in opposition to internal motivation or introjected motivation—produces poorer motivation in the behavior in question (Adams et al., 2017; Shum et al., 2021), even in the case of the COVID-19 pandemic (Morbée et al., 2021). However, research has also shown that people can operate with mixed motivational systems (de la Fuente, 2004), or changing back and forth from external to internal, according to the context (de la Fuente, 2004).
(3) The theoretical model does not incorporate a person's regulation state or style, which lies on a plausible continuum between self-regulated, deregulated (non-regulated), and dysregulated motivation (de la Fuente, 2017; Pachón-Basallo et al., 2021). There is no acknowledgment that a person may exhibit dysregulated behavior or motivation. However, clinical, healthcare and educational practice abound with reports showing this type of regulation to be real and pathological (Ryan et al., 2012).
(4) Also lacking is the possibility that the context may externally induce nonregulation. In fact, this aspect is yet to be defined in the theoretical model (Ryan and Deci, 2020a,b). Nor is this aspect established in the external inducement of dysregulation. Evidence has documented the existence of dysregulating contexts, in the personal and contextual realm (Pachón-Basallo et al., 2021).
Conclusion
SDT seeks to explain and predict self-determination processes in human beings—and has done so with abundant evidence and consistency. In different teaching-learning contexts, however, such processes: (1) are insufficiently associated with specific self-regulation mechanisms that are essential to explaining autonomy and self-management behaviors in humans (Bandura, 1986; Zimmerman and Schunk, 2001); (2) underestimate the possibility that external regulation can actually promote self-regulation; in other words, external regulation is considered only in its dysregulatory version (de la Fuente, 2017); nor do they consider that a person may be intrinsically motivated or self-regulated, without needing an externalization or internalization process to become so; (3) minimize the value of the context in promoting self-regulation, that is, an external regulatory value, not understood in opposition to internal regulation nor as external control (dysregulatory), but as a promoter and aid to self-regulation (externally regulatory).
Self-Regulated Learning Theory: Self-Regulated Learning
The theory of Self-Regulated Learning, developed by Zimmerman and Schunk (2001, 2011), offers detailed information about specific psychological processes that occur during academic/scholastic learning in reference to regulating one's own behavior. Plentiful evidence has been produced in support of this theoretical model, as well as its implications for intervening in student motivation (molecular analysis of learning). Though not addressed directly, certain principles of molar (or interaction with the context) analysis are suggested in this model. To complete this model, the processes it addresses must be incorporated within the larger, molar processes of teaching and learning. In this way, other possible types of regulation would be included along with self-regulation (Zimmerman and Labuhn, 2012).
Assumptions
The heuristic proposed by this theory offers an orderly, systematic view of students' cognitive and motivational processes during learning (Zimmerman, 2000). Referring specifically to motivation, it offers a discrete understanding (microanalysis) of motivational and meta-motivational processes throughout the circular, recurring sequence of the learning process (Cleary et al., 2012; Reindl et al., 2020). This heuristic model, given its explanatory potential, has been expanded to other fields of human learning (White and Bembenutty, 2014), such as skill training, assessment, and intervention in health (Hennessy et al., 2020) and in sports (Balk and Englert, 2020; Taylor et al., 2020; Wolff et al., 2021).
Zimmerman (2000) expanded Bandura's vision using a three-phase cyclical model that incorporates the individual's actions before and after task performance. This allows us to see more clearly how personal, behavioral, and social/environmental factors dynamically interact. Self-regulation is thus conceived along the three phases of forethought, performance, and self-reflection:
1) Prior to performance, the forethought phase is when learners set goals and select strategies for meeting them. The physical and social context is also addressed in the learner's forethought phase. Materials needed for task execution are acquired, and arrangements may be made to work with others. Time management is addressed, including decisions about when, where and how to work, and the overall time to be spent on the task and its components. Learners may actively motivate themselves to work on the task. For example, they may feel self-efficacy in being capable of success, and they may remind themselves that the task is valuable or important.
2) In the performance phase, learners work on the task; they self-instructions, and observe the results of their effort along the way. They consider how well their strategies are working, and whether they are making progress toward their goal.
3) Self-reflection takes place when the task is completed, although learners may also take time out for reflection during performance. Self-reflection is the learner's evaluation of how successful they have been. They made conclude that they need a change of strategy, or to arrange better conditions for working. In light of their outcomes, they may make attributions, that is, identify what they perceive to be causes. Attributions answer the question of why one was successful or not successful. These attributions and evaluations may prompt them to keep using the same strategy or to change it.
Students with learning disabilities, by way of illustration, often have difficulty in all three phases (Schunk and DiBenedetto, 2020a). Their forethought phase may be limited, without taking the time needed to plan out goals and strategies, and they may start the task with low self-efficacy of being able to successfully carry it out. In the performance phase, they may lack focused attention on the task, not overseeing their own work or considering their progress. In self-reflection, they may not properly evaluate their performance, and they may make non-motivating attributions. If they had trouble in doing the task, for example, they may attribute this to their own lack of ability instead of less-than-adequate effort.
Motivational and Regulatory Process
A central contribution of this model to the area of motivation is that it delimits the self-regulation variable at each motivational phase in cyclical learning, taking a metacognitive view, that is, becoming conscious of these processes and regulating them. This knowledge of meta-motivation or motivation regulation has been applied to many fields (Zimmerman, 2008; Monem, 2010; Panadero, 2017). At each phase of learning, the model proposes motivational behaviors that regulate the learning process:
1) At the start of the learning activity. The model establishes that it is possible to help students understand their own motivations and learning needs and establish learning goals, as well as plan their motivational and meta-motivational events: self-efficacy expectations (Bandura, 1987), academic behavioral confidence (Sander and de la Fuente, 2020a,b), personal improvement and achievement goals (Pintrich, 2000), and achievement emotions in anticipation of success or failure (Pekrun et al., 2014).
2) While carrying out the learning activity. This model has facilitated recent research for ascertaining specific behaviors of motivation (decisions, positive and negative emotions), and the degree of meta-motivational control: motivational strategies and self-instructions (Powers et al., 2020), strategies for coping with emotions (de la Fuente et al., 2017b), motivational decisions (self-reinforcement vs. self-punishment), perfectionism vs. procrastination (Garzón-Umerenkova et al., 2018).
3) At the end of the activity. The model establishes how self-assessment behaviors (Schunk, 1996; Zimmerman et al., 2011) and self-administration of emotions determine the final motivational state of engagement vs. burnout (de la Fuente et al., 2020e). The authors of the model establish that an adaptive evaluation supposes the recognition of errors but also a greater focus on successes. A maladaptive appraisal carries with it the self-dispensing of negative emotions. Also have causal or attributional explanations of success and failure adjusted to adjusted stability, internality, and controllability factors (Weiner, 1993).
This has represented a considerable advance in the study of regulatory processes in motivation, since it has identified concepts belonging to the meta-motivational realm, such as motivational and affective strategies, including coping strategies, which were not previously considered as belonging to models of self-regulated, academic learning, where the initial focus was on cognitive and meta-cognitive processes.
Contributions
Research on the construct of Self-Regulated Learning (SRL) that is based on Social Cognitive Theory (Bandura, 2006) has been yielding plentiful empirical evidence in relation to different variables and disciplines (Bembenutty et al., 2013):
1) In the sphere of Self-Regulated Learning (SRL), the relationship between SRL and Self-Efficacy has been amply demonstrated. For example, we have seen the roles of self-regulation and self-efficacy in students with learning disabilities (Schunk and DiBenedetto, 2021). SRL has also demonstrated its efficacy in the aspect of university students' work at home (Bembenutty and Hayes, 2016) and in delaying gratification (Bembenutty and Karabenick, 2004).
A large part of the research has focused on explaining and applying the SRL model to specific contexts of learning (Panadero, 2017), such as mathematics (Zimmerman et al., 2011), language arts and composition in students with behavioral maladjustment (Moohr et al., 2021), and in the sciences (Peters and Kitsantas, 2010). One essential contribution has come from the study of motivational processes and their self-regulated nature (Cleary and Zimmerman, 2004; Zimmerman and Kitsantas, 2005; Pintrich and Schunk, 2006; Wolters et al., 2011). There has also been plentiful research on the role and effect of self-regulation at university, especially in relation to assigned work (Ramdass and Zimmerman, 2011). In complementary fashion, research has also addressed improved teaching and learning through classroom practices of training in self-regulation (Zimmerman and Martinez-Pons, 1986, 1990; Zimmerman, 2008; Moos and Ringdal, 2012; Bembenutty et al., 2015; White and DiBenedetto, 2015; Zimmerman et al., 2015, 2017; White and Bembenutty, 2016; White, 2017; Schunk and DiBenedetto, 2020a,b, 2021).
2) As for SR and the realm of Health and Healthcare, the Self-Regulation construct (SR) has shown very consistent relationships with clinical and health issues. In Clinical Psychology specifically, recent research has shown self-regulation to be a cross-diagnostic variable of great importance. Its importance has also been reported from the perspective of Health Psychology (Bandura, 2004a,b, 2005). Specific examples include alcohol use and risk behaviors in adolescents (Crandall et al., 2018) and the role of SR in sports (Wolff et al., 2021).
Limitations
This model is therefore very adequate, parsimonious and powerful for assessment and intervention to train and improve motivational and meta-motivational processes, because it allows students to become aware of and put order in their cognitive-motivational processes. There is abundant evidence of intervention programs (Martínez-Vicente and de la Fuente, 2004) and the goodnesses of their application. However, the model is limited in several aspects:
1) Its explanatory domain focuses on molecular processes of learning. For this reason, it is especially adequate for training teachers and students in how to improve discrete, specific learning processes (Lombaerts et al., 2009). Specific meta-cognitive, meta-motivational and meta-emotional behavioral training is an example of the power of this model.
2) While the model can be considered to fall within the sphere of the psychology of learning, in the university context (Cassidy, 2011), it does not address in sufficient depth the role played by instructional processes, or by teaching in formal contexts. This approach would be characteristic of the domain of instructional psychology.
3) The concept of self-regulated learning does not take into account the specific concepts of deregulation (non-regulation) or dysregulation, as necessary types for explaining other, inadequate modalities of academic learning.
4) The SRL model is very focused on self-regulated, cyclical processes at the molecular level. It does not consider, however, the connection to self-regulation (SR) as a presage, personality variable in self-regulated learning (SRL), or the connection to aspects at the molar level, i.e., external regulatory processes from the context, as in regulatory teaching (de la Fuente, 2017). These limitations have prompted the development of the following theory, presented below.
SR vs. ER Theory: Self. vs. External- Regulation Behavior in Different Contexts
The General Model developed from SR vs. ER Theory (de la Fuente et al., 2020e) takes a molar-level approach to motivational analysis (de la Fuente et al., 2019a). It is an extrapolation of the Theory of Self-Regulated vs. Externally Regulated Learning, SRL vs. ERL (de la Fuente et al., 2013a,b, 2015, 2017a,b, 2019a,b, 2020a,b,c,d,e,f,g; de la Fuente, 2017), into different behavioral contexts. In the case of SRL vs. ERL, this analysis is contextualized within the processes of scholastic teaching and learning. With respect to their own learning, students may adopt self-regulation (as in Zimmerman's model), non-regulation, or dysregulation. The students' context (in interaction with the students' personal regulation type) may be externally regulating, externally non-regulating, or externally dysregulating. Motivational processes may then be contextualized within this new theoretical framework.
Assumptions
SR vs. ER Theory (de la Fuente et al., 2020e) seeks to explain the combination of external and internal conditions that predispose adequate behavior and motivation, in response to situations in different contexts. In summary, it proposes the following:
1) An individual's competence level in Self-Regulation may be classified as one of three options [3 = high (self-regulation or proactive self-regulation), 2 = medium (cessation of regulation or reactive regulation); 1 = low (dysregulation or dysfunctional regulation)]. Prior research shows that the level of self-regulation that a student exercises is an indicator of their competence in self-regulation, as a personal characteristic. It also correlates to competence and adequate use of meta-motivation, meta-emotion, and meta-behavior skills (de la Fuente et al., 2015, 2017a,b). Consequently, it would also be a good indicator of self-regulated learning (de la Fuente et al., 2017a,b). Numeric values are assigned across the range from a higher level of personal regulation, level 3, which is the most proactive self-regulation; to a medium or non-regulatory level 2, which is not proactive; to the lowest level of self-regulation (1), or the practice of dysregulation (procrastination behavior, etc.). See Figure 1.
2) Interpersonal contexts offer external regulation that can also be classified across three levels [3 = high (highly externally regulatory context); 2 = medium (or external de-regulatory or non-regulating context); 1 = low (dysregulating context or external dysfunctional context). This contextual level of external regulation identifies whether the context encourages or discourages use of oversight competencies like meta-motivation, meta-emotion and meta-behavior (de la Fuente et al., 2019a,b, 2020a,b,c,d,e,f,g). Consequently, high levels in this construct indicate an effective or regulatory context. Numeric values represent a range, from a context that more effectively facilitates personal regulation, Level 3, the most proactive in promoting self-regulation; to a medium or deregulatory Level (2), offering no external support for regulation; to the lowest level of external regulation, Level 1, or external dysregulation (e.g., the teaching process triggers stress, negative achievement emotions, surface learning approaches). See Figure 2.
3) By combining these two factors we may calculate an interactive regulation index, between 1 and 3, that is, the average of the two regulation types, with 5 possible results (de la Fuente et al., 2019a,b, 2020a,b,c,d,e,f,g). The proposed five-combination heuristic makes it possible to analyze the most common scenarios in the interactive regulation of learning behaviors. For example, if a student is low in self-regulation (1 point), and external regulation from the context is medium (2 points), the resulting regulation average will be 1.5 points (2 + 1 = 3/2 = 1.5 point average); likewise, if the student has a medium level of self-regulation (2 points), but the context is low in regulation (1 point), the same regulation average is produced (2 + 1 = 3/2 = 1.5 point average). Another example might be a student who is high in self-regulation (3 points), but their context is low in regulation (1 point); the regulation average will be 2 points (3 + 1 = 4/2 = 2 points). Regulation averages can thus be ordered across a regulation range where the person-context interaction progresses from least favorable to most favorable: from a minimum average of 1 point (1-point personal self-regulation and 1-point external regulation), to a maximum of 3 points (3-point self-regulation and 3-point external regulation). The possible regulation averages can then be ranked in order from 1 to 5, across the regulation range (regulation average of 1 = rank 1; regulation average of 1.5 = rank 2; regulation average of 2 = rank 3; regulation average of 2.5 = rank 4; regulation average of 3 = rank 5). See Table 2.
Figure 1.
Graphic representation of regulation types: SR, Self-regulation; NR, Non-regulation; DR, Dys-Regulation. The X axis represents the degree of regulation (high-medium-low), while the Y axis shows directionality (+1, 0, −1).
Figure 2.
Graphic representation of external regulation types: ER, External Regulation; ENR, External Non-regulation; EDR, External Dys-Regulation. The X axis represents the degree of external regulation (high-medium-low), while the Y axis shows the directionality of the external regulation (+1, 0, −1).
Table 2.
Combinations of levels | Regulation tendency | Stress | Stress | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
SR level (range) | ER level (range) | Avg. | Rank | Protection | Risk | |
3 (3.85–5.00) H | 3 (2.84–5.00) H | 3 | 5 | High-High: High-Regulation | High protection | Low risk |
2 (3.10–3.84) M | 3 (2.84–5.00) H | 2.5 | 4 | Medium-High: Regulation | M-H protection | M-L risk |
3 (3.85–5.00) H | 2 (2.35–2.83) M | 2.5 | 4 | High-Medium: Regulation | M-H protection | M-L risk |
2 (3.10–3.84) M | 2 (2.35–2.83) M | 2 | 3 | Medium: Non-Regulation | Medium protection | M risk |
2 (3.10–3.84) M | 1 (1.00–2.34) L | 1.5 | 2 | Medium-Low: Dysregulation | M-L protection | M-H risk |
1 (1.00–3.09) L | 2 (2.35–2.83) M | 1.5 | 2 | Low-Medium: Dysregulation | M-L protection | M-H risk |
1 (1.00–3.09) L | 1 (1.00–2.34) L | 1 | 1 | Low-Low: High Dysregulation | Low protection | High risk |
Motivational and Self-Regulation Concepts
Recent research has provided evidence of the value of this heuristic for determining the level of different motivational-affective variables in university students, as variables dependent on the student's level of self-regulation and the teacher's external regulation. Recent research reports have shown that the combination of the two factors (SR vs. ER) determine the more cognitive-strategic factors of motivation in university learning, that is, the student's learning approach. Thus, Rank 5 involves the highest level of deep approach (deep motivation and deep strategy), while Rank 1 represents a higher level of surface learning (surface motivation and surface strategy) (de la Fuente et al., 2017a, 2020c). In the same way, motivational-affective factors are also determined by these combination levels.
The heuristic levels presented in this study have proven to be a determining factor in many aspects, such as types of achievement emotions (de la Fuente et al., 2020a); perceived level of stress factors and symptoms in the teaching/learning process (de la Fuente et al., 2020b); coping strategies used to manage this stress (de la Fuente et al., 2020c); and attitudinal factors of motivation, such as academic behavioral confidence and procrastination (de la Fuente et al., 2020d). In all cases, Combination Rank 1 proves to be the most harmful: more negative emotions; higher levels of academic stress in factors and symptoms; more emotion-focused coping strategies, to the detriment of problem-focused strategies; lower academic behavioral confidence; and greater procrastination. By contrast, Combination Rank 5 proves to be the most desirable: more positive emotions; lower levels of academic stress factors and symptoms; more problem-focused coping strategies, without renouncing certain positive emotions; more academic behavioral confidence and less procrastination.
Limitations
This theoretical model also has certain limitations that must be addressed. On one hand, although levels of self-regulation (1 = low; 2 = medium; 3 = high) and external regulation (1 = low; 2 = medium; 3 = high) are both highly consistent constructs, assessed by two consolidated instruments, (1) the Short Self-Regulation Questionnaire (Pichardo et al., 2014) and (2) the Interactive Assessment of the Teaching and Learning Process, IATLP (de la Fuente et al., 2012), measurement of variables should be improved. In fact, new instruments of SR vs. ER Theory (de la Fuente, 2022; see Appendix I) have been developed for application in the spheres of education, clinical practice and ICT use, and are able to more accurately assess the constructs of self-regulation, non-regulation and dysregulation, as conceived in the present theory. Recent research findings are encouraging.
A Research Agenda For SR VS. ER Theory: Practical Applicability in Different Psychological Contexts
This manuscript has presented specific strategies for improving student self-regulation: (1) increasing introjected motivation and self-regulation, from the model of Self-Determination Theory, (2) increasing the student's level of self-regulation, adopting many principles from the Zimmerman cyclic model; (3) making changes in the type of personal, internal regulation that is affecting students' motivation (whether regulatory, deregulatory, or dysregulatory), following certain principles from the Self- vs. External- Regulation model; (4) increasing the teacher's level of external regulation in the classroom; (5) making changes in the type of external regulation that is affecting students' motivation (whether self-regulatory, de-regulatory, or dys-regulatory). Albert Bandura's Social Cognitive Theory, in conjunction with the two subsequent models, has been foundational to SR vs. ER Theory (de la Fuente, 2017).
This more recent theory faces numerous challenges. On one hand, there is the need for evidence that the assessment instrument is consistently associated with self-regulation in different languages and different populations (de la Fuente, 2022; see Annex I). Analyses performed to date have shown consistency and validity (Pachón-Basallo et al., 2021). On the other hand, it is very important to verify that this heuristic—on molecular and molar levels—is applicable and accounts for the variability in different behavioral constructs, in the main fields of Psychology and Psychiatry (Romer et al., 2021). This psychological model will allow a crossed and interactive analysis of the different personal self-regulation profiles of people, in interaction with the external regulatory characteristics of the contexts in which they operate. This is a general task of psychology, as a science and as a profession. historically excessively focused on explaining and making predictions only from the individual characteristics of the subjects. Our own previous research has documented the effect of levels of self-regulation and external regulation on different types of variables and contexts (see Table 3):
Table 3.
Summarized research agenda for Self- vs. External-Regulation Theory (SR vs. ER Theory), applied to different fields of the study of behavior in different contexts.
Self-regulation | Non-regulation | Dys-regulation | Construct | Research |
---|---|---|---|---|
1. Educational psychology area (Self- vs. External-Regulated Learning Theory; SRL vs. ERL Theory) | ||||
Individual variables | ||||
Self-regulation | Non-Regulation /Fatigue | Dys-regulation | Self-Regulation Behavior | de la Fuente, 2017; de la Fuente et al., 2021c |
Self-regulated learning | Non-regulated learning | Dys-regulated Learning | Self-regulated learning | de la Fuente, 2017; de la Fuente et al., 2017a,b; |
Self-control of study | Depletion toward study | Dys-control of Study | Self-control of study | Amate-Romera and de la Fuente, 2021 |
Problem focused coping | Emotion focused (+) | Emotion focused (−) | Academic coping strategies | de la Fuente et al., 2020d |
Self-regulation (non procrastination) | Passive procrastination | Active procrastination | Procrastination | Garzón-Umerenkova et al., 2018; de la Fuente et al., 2020a |
Self-motivation | Self non-motivation | Self-dysmotivation | Self-handicapping | Núñez et al., 2020 |
Non-anxiety | Mixed | Test-anxiety | Test anxiety | de la Fuente et al., 2017a; Amate-Romera and de la Fuente, 2021 |
Deep approaches | Mixed | Surface approaches | Learning approaches | de la Fuente et al., 2008, 2017a, 2020a,f |
Engagement | Mixed | Burnout | Engagement-Burnout | de la Fuente et al., 2021d |
Resilience high | Resilience medium | Resilience low | Resilience | |
Achievement emotions (+) | Mixed achiev. emotions (=) | Achievement emotions (-) | Achievement emotions | de la Fuente et al., 2017a, 2020b |
Competitive | Hard-working, impatience | Hostility/impatience | Action-emotion style | de la Fuente et al., 2016, 2017a |
No stress | Distress | High Stress | Academic Stress | de la Fuente et al., 2020c |
Confidence | No confidence | Dys-confidence | Academic Confidence | de la Fuente et al., 2017a, 2021a; Sander and de la Fuente, 2020a,b |
High | Medium | Low | Learning achievement | de la Fuente et al., 2017a, 2020f |
Strengths | Medium | Weaknesses | Character strengths | de la Fuente et al., in review |
Ext. Regulation | Ext. Non-regulation | Ext. Dys-regulation | Construct | Research |
Contextual variables | ||||
External regulatory teaching | External non-regulatory teaching | External dys-regulatory teaching | Regulatory teaching | de la Fuente et al., 2017a,b, 2021a |
Low (low factors) | Medium (medium factors) | High (high factors) | Contextual stress factors | de la Fuente et al., 2021a |
Parental involvement | Parental non involvement | Parental dys- involvement | Parental involvement | Sander et al., 2021 |
Authoritative style | Permissive style (laisser-faire) | Authoritarian style | Family style | Balaguer et al., 2021 |
Self-regulation | Non-regulation | Dys-regulation | Construct | Research |
2. Health psychology area (Self- vs. External-Regulated Health Behavior Theory; SRH vs. ERH Theory) | ||||
Individual variables | ||||
Self-regulation health | Non-regulation health/fatigue | Dys-regulation health | Self-regulation health | de la Fuente, 2017; de la Fuente et al., 2021e |
Problem focused (+) coping | Emotion focused (+) | Emotion focused (−) | Coping strategies | de la Fuente et al., 2020e,g |
Engagement | Mixed level medium | Burnout | Engagement-burnout | de la Fuente et al., 2020e, 2021d |
Acceptance of norms | Non-acceptance of norms | Reactance to norms | Psychological reactance | Pachón-Basallo et al., 2021 |
Resilience | Non-resilience | Weakness | Resilience | de la Fuente et al., 2017b |
Positivity | Mixed | Negativity | Positivity-negativity | de la Fuente et al., 2021d |
Flourishing | Non-flourishing | Flourishing | Garzón-Umerenkova et al., 2020 | |
Well-being (high) | Mixed (medium) | Discomfort (low) | Well-being | Becerra and Campitelli, 2013; López-Madrigal et al., 2021; de la Fuente et al., in review |
Strengths | Medium | Weaknesses | Character strengths | de la Fuente et al., in review, …… |
High adaptability | Medium adaptability | Dys-adaptability (low) | Adaptability | |
Contextual variables | ||||
External regulation of health | External non-regulation of health | External dys-regulation of health | Regulatory health context | de la Fuente et al., in review |
Low (low factors) | Medium (medium factors) | High (high factors) | Contextual stress factors | de la Fuente et al., 2021b |
Authoritative style | Laisser faire | Authoritarian style | Family | |
Self-regulation | Non-regulation | Dys-regulation | Construct | Research |
3. Clinical psychology area (Self- vs. External-Regulated Behavior Theory; SR vs. ER Theory) | ||||
Individual variables | ||||
Self-regulation | Non-regulation /fatigue | Dys-regulation | Self-regulation behavior | de la Fuente, 2017; de la Fuente et al., 2021e |
Self-control of behavior | Depletion | Dys-control of behavior | Self-control of behavior | |
Conscientiousness | Extraversion, openness | Agreeableness, neuroticism | Personality | Sander and de la Fuente, 2020a,b; de la Fuente et al., 2021f |
Engagement | Mixed | Burnout | Engagement-burnout | de la Fuente et al., 2021d |
Acceptance norms | Non- acceptance norms | Reactance norms | Psychological reactance | de la Fuente et al., 2021a |
Resilience | Non-resilience | Weakness | Resilience | de la Fuente et al., 2017b |
Positivity | Mixed | Negativity | Positivity-negativity | de la Fuente et al., 2021d |
Self-knowledge | Self-criticism | Depression | Kopala-Sibley and Zuroff, 2020 | |
Perfectionistic strivings | Medium | Perfectionistic concerns | Perfectionism | Frost and Marten, 1990; Stöber, 1998; Madigan, 2019; de la Fuente et al., 2020e |
Low emotional reactivity | Medium emotional reactivity | High emotional reactivity | Emotional reactivity | Becerra and Campitelli, 2013 |
Executive functions | De- executive function | Dys-executive function | Executive functions | de la Fuente et al., in review |
Character strengths (high) | (medium) | (low) | Character strengths | Seligman and Peterson, 2004 |
Well-being (high) | Mixed (medium) | Discomfort (low) | Psychological well-being | Becerra and Campitelli, 2013; de la Fuente et al., in review |
Self-regulation of ict use | Non-regulation ict /fatigue | Dys-regulation ict | Self-regulation ict | de la Fuente, 2017; Romer et al., 2021 |
Self-assessment | Self-avoidance | Self-rumination | Self-assessment | |
High adaptability | Medium adaptability | Dys-adaptability (low) | Adaptability | |
External regulation | External non-regulation | External dys-regulation | Construct | Research |
Contextual variables | ||||
External regulation of ict use | External non- regulation of ICTS | External Dys- Regulation of ICTs | Regulation of ICT use | |
Low (low factors) | Medium (medium factors) | High (high factors) | Contextual Stress Factors | de la Fuente et al., 2021a |
Self-regulation | Non-regulation | Dys-regulation | Construct | Research |
4. Social Psychology Area (Self- vs. External-Regulated Social Behavior Theory; SR vs. ER Social Theory) | ||||
Individual variables | ||||
Social self-regulation | Social non-regulation /fatigue | Social dys-regulation | Social self-regulation | de la Fuente, 2017 |
Competitive | Hard-working, impatience | Hostility/impatience | Action-emotion style | de la Fuente et al., 2016, 2017a |
Well-being (high) | Mixed (medium) | Discomfort (low) | Psychological well-being | Becerra and Campitelli, 2013 |
Engagement | Mixed | Burnout | Engagement-burnout | |
Assertiveness | Non-regulation | Aggressivity / inhibition | Social abilities | |
Strengths | Medium | Weaknesses | Character strengths | de la Fuente et al., in review |
External regulation | External non-regulation | External dys-regulation | Construct | Research |
Contextual variables | ||||
External social regulation | External social non-regulation | External social dys-regulation | Social regulation | de la Fuente et al., in review |
Low (low factors) | Medium (medium factors) | High (high factors) | Contextual stress factors | de la Fuente et al., 2021a |
External organizational regulation | External organizational non-regulation | External organizational dys-regulation | Organizational regulation | de la Fuente, 2017 |
Authoritative style | Laisser faire style | Authoritarian style | Family | de la Fuente et al., 2021b |
Self-regulation | Non-regulation | Dys-regulation | Construct | Research |
5. Traffic psychology area (self- vs. External- regulation of traffic behavior theory; sr vs. Er traffic theory) | ||||
Individual variables | ||||
Self-regulation | Non-regulation /fatigue | Dys-regulation | Self-regulated behavior | de la Fuente, 2017, 2021a,b; de la Fuente et al., 2021a,b,c,d,e,f |
Self-control of behavior | Depletion | Dys-control of behavior | Self-control of behavior | |
Competitive | Hard-working, impatience | Hostility/impatience | Action-emotion style | de la Fuente et al., 2016, 2017a |
Strengths | Medium | Weaknesses | Character strengths | de la Fuente et al., in review |
External regulation | External non-regulation | External dys-regulation | Construct | Research |
Contextual variables | ||||
Low (low factors) | Medium (medium factors) | High (high factors) | Contextual stress factors | de la Fuente et al., 2021a |
Self-regulation | Non-regulation | Dys-regulation | Construct | Research |
6. Moral psychology area (self- vs. External- regulation of moral behavior theory; moral sr vs. Er theory) | ||||
Individual variables | ||||
Self-regulation | Non-regulation /fatigue | Dys-regulation | Self-regulation behavior | de la Fuente, 2017; de la Fuente et al., 2021e |
Self-control behavior | Depletion behavior | Dys-control behavior | Self-control | Behavior |
Strengths | Medium | Weaknesses | Character strengths | Villacís et al., 2021 |
High | Medium | Low | Spirituality | |
External regulation | External non-regulation | External dys-regulation | Construct | Research |
Contextual variables | ||||
Low (low factors) | Medium (medium factors) | High (high factors) | Stress factors | de la Fuente et al., 2021a |
1) In the sphere of Educational Psychology, recent research has contributed evidence of the different effects of combined levels of Self- vs. External- Regulation (SR-ER) in education. Specifically, a combined effect has been observed in learning approaches (de la Fuente et al., 2017a, 2019a; de la Fuente et al., 2020a,f), academic emotions (de la Fuente et al., 2019a,b) academic confidence and procrastination (Sander and de la Fuente, 2020a,b; de la Fuente et al., 2021c), coping strategies for academic stress (de la Fuente et al., 2017a); levels of engagement-burnout (de la Fuente et al., 2020e), positivity, resilience (de la Fuente et al., 2021d), stress factors and symptoms (de la Fuente et al., 2021a). These results were initially obtained by combining measurements from the Self-Regulation Scale (Pichardo et al., 2014; Garzón-Umerenkova et al., 2017) and the IATLP Scales (de la Fuente and Martínez-Vicente, 2008) and later using the Self- vs. External- Regulation of Learning Inventory (de la Fuente, 2022).
2) In the sphere of Developmental Psychology, this theoretical model enables us to understand the different processes of human development that depend on or are associated with levels of behavioral regulation at each stage of development, the role of regulatory characteristics of the context, and how these interact. Recent evidence has established this relationship by more deeply exploring the role of a regulatory or dysregulatory family context and its effect on learning and achievement (Balaguer et al., 2021), as well as the sometimes dysregulatory role of the social/family context in young-adult university students, in maturational disorders typical of executive dysfunction and emotional dysregulation (de la Fuente et al., 2022).
3) In the sphere of Clinical and Health Psychology, there is also evidence of the degree to which the SR-ER combination can predict variables like procrastination and health (Pachón-Basallo et al., 2021). The scale used in this case is the Self- vs. External- Regulation of Learning Scale (de la Fuente et al., 2020c). SR vs. ER theory has also been applied to analysis and behavioral prevention in the COVID-19 pandemic (de la Fuente et al., 2021e). In the same line as our results, there is documented evidence in relation to the important regulatory role of parents via modeling and the design of the behavioral context (Callejas et al., 2021). Nonetheless, the effects of these cross-diagnostic variables (SR vs. ER) is yet to be analyzed in other areas of the field of psychology:
4) In the area of Social and Organizational Psychology, these assumptions must be tested. The relationship should be established between the proposed SR vs. ER heuristic and specific variables of the social and organizational spheres, such as organizational engagement-burnout, psychological wellbeing in organizations, and levels of performance supported by the organizations themselves.
5) In the area of Traffic Psychology, the ability of the proposed heuristic to explain the behavioral variability of drivers and accident rates should be analyzed. It seems plausible to expect this explanatory ability, given that the “road trip metaphor” (de la Fuente, 2004, based on Pintrich, 1991) is what gave rise to the SR vs. ER theory. The effect of the heuristic combination in determining the level of the behavioral variables associated with driving must be demonstrated.
6) In the field of Moral Psychology, there is also a need to establish the connections between the SR vs. ER heuristic and issues inherent to this field, such as character strengths, spirituality, and others (Villacís et al., 2021). It is necessary to advance in the study of moral behavior (Nucci, 2014), based on the knowledge of the regulatory, personal and contextual factors, in interaction. For this, this heuristic and its instruments are a new opportunity to approach.
Conclusion
Although limited in that most evidence to date has been produced with university-age youths and in an academic context, the consistency of the relationships found encourages us to continue in this line of research. Further evidence in these different fields of Psychology will allow us to affirm with greater assurance the plausibility of the SR vs. ER postulates, especially in differentiating it from the previous theories presented. The results from empirical data that we continue to collect will allow us to conclude the applicability of these postulates to the fields of Educational Psychology, Clinical and Health Psychology, Social Psychology, Traffic Psychology and Moral Psychology.
More than ever, it is time to acknowledge and thank Prof. Albert Bandura for his proposition of the self-regulatory mechanism in human beings. His model fascinated us and has inspired us to take it thus far. These results, in good measure, also belong to him. Thank you, Professor Bandura! RIP.
Author Contributions
JF: initial conceptualization and initial writing. JM-V support for R&D projects. FS, PS, SF, AK, EB, and DK: final reviewer and writing process. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.
Funding
This study was funded by R&D Project PGC2018-094672-B-I00, University of Navarra, Ministry of Education and Science (Spain), and the European Social Fund (EU); R and D Project UAL18- SEJ-DO31-A-FEDER. University of Almería (Spain), and the European Social Fund (EU) (www.inetas.net).
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher's Note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Supplementary Material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.861493/full#supplementary-material
References
- Adams N., Little T. D., Ryan R. M. (2017). Self-determination theory. In M. L. Wehmeyer, K. A. Shogren, T. D. Little and S. J. Lopez (Eds.), Development of Self-Determination Through the Life-Course. Cham, Swirtzerland: Springer. p. 47–54. 10.1007/978-94-024-1042-6_4 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Almukhambetova A., Hernández-Torrano D. (2020). Gifted students' adjustment and underachievement in university: An exploration from the self-determination theory perspective. Gifted Child Quart. 64, 117–131. 10.1177/0016986220905525 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Amate-Romera J., de la Fuente J. (2021). Relationships between test anxiety, self-regulation and strategies for coping with stress, in professional examination candidates. Ann. Psychol. 37, 276–286. 10.6018/analesps.411131 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Balaguer A., Benítez E., de la Fuente J., Osorio A. (2021). Maternal and paternal parenting styles as a whole: validation of the simple form of the Parenting Style Evaluation Scale. Ann. Psychol. 37, 77–87 10.6018/analesps.408171 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Balk Y. A., Englert C. (2020). Recovery self-regulation in sport: Theory, research, and practice. Int. J. Sports Sci. Coach. 15, 273–281. 10.1177/1747954119897528 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Bandura A. (1986). Social Foundations of Thought and Action: A Social Cognitive Theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. [Google Scholar]
- Bandura A. (1987). (María Zaplana, trad.). Teoría Cognitivo-Social del Aprendizaje [Cognitive-Social Learning Theory]. Barcelona: Martínez-Roca [Google Scholar]
- Bandura A. (1991). Social Cognitive Theory of Self-Regulation. Organiz. Behav. Human Decis. Process. 50, 248–287. 10.1016/0749-5978(91)90022-L [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Bandura A. (1997). Self-Efficacy: The Exercise of Control. New York: Freeman [Google Scholar]
- Bandura A. (1999). Moral Disengagement in the Perpetration of Inhumanities. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 3, 193–209. 10.1207/s15327957pspr0303_3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bandura A. (2001). Social cognitive theory: An agentic perspective. Ann. Rev. Psychol. 52, 1–26. 10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bandura A. (2004a). Health promotion by social cognitive means. Health Educ. Behav. 31, 143–164. 10.1177/1090198104263660 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bandura A. (2004b). Social cognitive theory for personal and social change by enabling media. In A. Singhal, M.J. Cody, E.M. Rogers, and M. Sabido (eds.), Entertainment-education and social change: History, research, and practice (pp. 75–96). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. [Google Scholar]
- Bandura A. (2005). The Primacy of Self-Regulation in Health Promotion. Appl. Psychol Int. Rev. 54, 245–254. 10.1111/j.1464-0597.2005.00208.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Bandura A. (2006). Toward a psychology of human agency. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 1, 164–180. 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2006.00011.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Becerra R., Campitelli G. (2013). Emotional reactivity: Critical analysis and proposal of a new scale. Int. J. Appl. Psychol. 3, 161–168. 10.5923/j.ijap.20130306.03 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Bembenutty H., Cleary T. J., Kitsantas A. (2013). Applications of self-regulated learning across diverse disciplines: A tribute to Barry J. Zimmerman. Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing. [Google Scholar]
- Bembenutty H., Hayes A. (2016). The Triumph of Homework Completion: Instructional Approaches Promoting Self-regulation of Learning and Performance among High School Learners. Manuscript submitted for publication. [Google Scholar]
- Bembenutty H., Karabenick S. A. (2004). Inherent association between academic delay of gratification, future time perspective, and self-regulated learning. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 16, 35–57 10.1023/B:EDPR.0000012344.34008.5c [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Bembenutty H., White M. C., Vélez M. R. (2015). Developing Self-regulation of Learning and Teaching Skills among Teacher Candidates. New York, NY: Springer. 10.1007/978-94-017-9950-8 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Berkman E. T., Wilson S. M. (2021). So Useful as a Good Theory? The Practicality Crisis in (Social) Psychological Theory. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 16, 864–74. 10.1177/1745691620969650 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bernardo A., Esteban M., Cervero A., Cerezo R., Herrero F. J. (2019). The influence of self-regulation behaviors on university students' intentions of persistence. Front. Psychol. 10, 2284. 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02284 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blair C., Raver C. C. (2015). School readiness and self-regulation: A developmental psychobiological approach. Ann. Rev. Psychol. 66, 711–731. 10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015221 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boekaerts M., Zeidner M., Pintrich P. R. (1999). Handbook of Self-Regulation. Elsevier. 10.1016/B978-012109890-2/50030-5 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Brown J. M. (1998). Self-regulation and the addictive behaviors. In W. R. Miller and N. Heather (eds.), Applied clinical psychology. Treating Addictive Behaviors. Plenum Press. p. 61–73. 10.1007/978-1-4899-1934-2_5 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Callejas E., Byrne S., Rodrigo M. J. (2021). Parental self-regulation and the promotion of healthy routines in early childhood. J. Child Family Stud. 30, 1791–1802. 10.1007/s10826-021-01981-930725307 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Cassidy S. (2011). Self-regulated learning in higher education: identifying key component processes. Stud. High. Educ. 36, 989–1000. 10.1080/03075079.2010.503269 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Cleary T., Gregory C., Barry C., Zimmerman B. J. (2012). Assessing self-regulation as a cyclical, context-specific phenomenon: overview and analysis of srl microanalytic protocols. Educ. Res. Int. 2012, 428639. 10.1155/2012/428639 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Cleary T., Zimmerman B. (2004). Self-Regulation empowerment program: a school-based program to enhance self-regulated and self-motivated cycles of student learning, Psychol. School. 4, 537–550. 10.1002/pits.10177 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Crandall A., Magnusson M. B, Novilla M. L. B. (2018). Growth in adolescent self-regulation and impact on sexual risk-taking: a curve-of-factors analysis. J. Youth. Adolesc. 47, 793–806 10.1007/s10964-017-0706-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Curren R., Ryan R. M. (2020). Moral self-determination: the nature, existence, and formation of moral motivation. J. Moral Educ. 49, 295–315. 10.1080/03057240.2020.1793744 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- de la Fuente J. (2004). Recent perspective in the study of motivation: Goal orientation theory. Elect. J. Res. Educ. Psychol. 2, 35–62. [Google Scholar]
- de la Fuente J. (2017). Theory of self- vs. externally- regulated learning TM: fundamentals, evidence, and applicability. Front. Psychol. 8, 1675. 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01675 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de la Fuente J. (2021a). Self- vs. External-Regulated Theory. An General Model of Variality of Behavior manuscript pending publication. Pamplona: Univeridad de Navarra. [Google Scholar]
- de la Fuente J. (2021b). A path analysis model of protection and risk factors for university academic stress: analysis and psychoeducational implications for the COVID-19 emergency. Front. Psychol. 12, 562372. 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.562372 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de la Fuente J. (2022). Self- vs. External- Regulation Behavior Inventories (de la Fuente, J., 2022). Madrid: Registration of the intellectual property n° 765-688472 (2022/02/07) [Google Scholar]
- de la Fuente J., Amate J., González-Torres M. C., Artuch R., García-Torrecillas J. M., Fadda S. (2020g). Effects of levels of self-regulation and regulatory teaching on strategies for coping with academic stress in undergraduate students. Front. Psychol. 11, 22. 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00022 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de la Fuente J., Berbén A. B., Zapata L. (2013a). How regulatory teaching impacts university students' perceptions of the teaching-learning process: the role of teacher training. Infanc. Aprendizaje 36, 375–385. 10.1174/021037013807533016 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- de la Fuente J., Cardelle-Elawar M., Peralta F. J., Sánchez M. D., Martínez- Vicente J. M., Zapata L. (2011). Students' factors affecting undergraduates' perceptions of their teaching and learning process within ECTS experience. Front. Psychol. 2, 28. 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00028 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de la Fuente J., Fernández-Cabezas M., Cambil M., Vera M. M., González-Torres M. C., Artuch-Garde R. (2017b). Linear relationship between resilience, learning approaches, and coping strategies to predict achievement in undergraduate students. Front. Psychol. 8, 1039. 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01039 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de la Fuente J., González-Torres M. C., Artuch-Garde R., Vera-Martínez M. M., Martínez-Vicente J. M., Peralta-Sánchez F. J. (2021f). Resilience as a Buffering Variable Between the Big Five Components and Factors and Symptoms of Academic Stress at University. Front. Psychiatry 12, 600240. 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.600240 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de la Fuente J., González-Torres M. C., Aznárez-Sanado M., Martínez-Vicente J. M., Peralta-Sánchez F. J., Vera M. M. (2019a). Implications of unconnected micro, molecular, and molar level research in psychology: the case of executive functions, self-regulation, and external regulation. Front. Psychol. 10, 1919. 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01919 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de la Fuente J., Kauffman D. F., Dempsy M. S., Kauffman Y. (2021e). Analysis and Psychoeducational Implications of the Behavior Factor During the COVID-19 Emergency. Front. Psychol. 12, 613881. 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.613881 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de la Fuente J., Lahortiga-Ramos F., Laspra-Solís C., Maestro-Martín C., Alustiza I., Aubá E., et al. (2020e). A Structural Equation Model of Achievement Emotions, Coping Strategies and Engagement-Burnout in Undergraduate Students: A Possible Underlying Mechanism in Facets of Perfectionism. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 17, 2106. 10.3390/ijerph17062106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de la Fuente J., Malpica-Chavarria E. A., Garzón-Umerenkova A., Pachón-Basallo M. (2021b). Effect of Personal and Contextual Factors of Regulation on Academic Achievement during Adolescence: The Role of Gender and Age. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18, 8944. 10.3390/ijerph18178944 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de la Fuente J. Martínez-Vicente J. M. (2008), Scales for Interactive Assessment of the Teaching-Learning Process (IATLP). Almería: Education Psychology I+D+i, e-Publishing Series. [Google Scholar]
- de la Fuente J., Martínez-Vicente J. M., Pachón-Basallo M., Peralta-Sánchez F. J., Vera-Martínez M. M., Andrés-Romero M. (2022). Differential predictive effect of Self-Regulation Behavior and the combination of Self- vs. External Regulation Behavior on Executive Dysfunctions and Emotion Regulation Difficulties, in University Students. Front. Psychol. 13, 876292. 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.876292 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de la Fuente J., Martínez-Vicente J. M., Peralta-Sánchez F. J., Garzón-Umerenkova A., Vera M. M., Paoloni P. (2019b). Applying the SRL vs. ERL theory to the knowledge of achievement emotions in undergraduate university students. Front. Psychol. 10, 2070. 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02070 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de la Fuente J., Martínez-Vicente J. M., Salmerón J. L., Vera M. M., Cardelle-Elawar M. (2016). Action-emotion style, learning approach and coping strategies, in undergraduate university students. Ann. Psychol. 32, 457–465. 10.6018/analesps.32.2.197991 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- de la Fuente J., Pachón-Basallo M., Santos F. H., Peralta-Sánchez F. J., González-Torres M. C., Artuch-Garde R., et al. (2021a). How Has the COVID-19 Crisis Affected the Academic Stress of University Students? The Role of Teachers and Students. Front. Psychol. 12, 626340. 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.626340 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de la Fuente J., Paoloni P., Kauffman D., Yilmaz Soylu M., Sander P., Zapata L. (2020a). Big five, self-regulation, and coping strategies as predictors of achievement emotions in undergraduate students. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 17, 3602. 10.3390/ijerph17103602 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de la Fuente J., Paoloni P. V., Vera-Martínez M. M., Garzón-Umerenkova A. (2020b). Effect of levels of self-regulation and situational stress on Achievement Emotions in undergraduate students: class, study and testing. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 17, 4293. 10.3390/ijerph17124293 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de la Fuente J., Peralta-Sánchez F. J., Martínez-Vicente J. M., Santos F. H., Fadda S., Gaeta-González M. L. (2020c). Do learning approaches set the stage for emotional well-being in college students? Sustainability 12, 6984. 10.3390/su12176984 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- de la Fuente J., Peralta-Sánchez F. J., Martínez-Vicente J. M. V., Sander P., Garzón-Umerenkova A., Zapata L. (2020d). Effects of self- vs. external regulation on the factors and symptoms of academic stress in undergraduate students. Front. Psychol. 11, 543884. 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01773 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de la Fuente J., Pichardo M. C., Justicia F., García-Berbén A. B. (2008). Learning approaches, self-regulation and achievement in three European universities. Psicothema 20, 705–711. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de la Fuente J., Sander P., Garzón-Umerenkova A., Vera-Martínez M. M., Fadda S., Gaetha M. L. (2021c). Self-Regulation and Regulatory Teaching as Determinants of Academic Behavioral Confidence and Procrastination in Undergraduate Students. Front. Psychol. 12, 602904. 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.602904 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de la Fuente J., Sander P., Kauffman D., Yilmaz Soylu M. (2020f). Differential Effects of Self- vs. External- Regulation on Learning Approaches, Academic Achievement, and Satisfaction in Undergraduate Students. Front. Psychol. 11, 543884. 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.543884 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de la Fuente J., Sander P., Martínez-Vicente J. M., Vera M. M., Garzón A., Fadda S. (2017a). Combined effect of levels in personal self-regulation and regulatory teaching on meta-cognitive, on meta-motivational, and on academic achievement variables in undergraduate students. Front. Psychol. 8, 232. 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00232 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de la Fuente J., Sander P., Putwain D. (2013b). Relationship between undergraduate student confidence, approach to learning and academic performance: the role of gender. J. Psychodid. 18, 375–393. 10.1387/RevPsicodidact.7078 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- de la Fuente J., Santos F. H., Garzón-Umerenkova A., Fadda S., Solinas G., Pignata S. (2021d). Cross-Sectional Study of Resilience, Positivity and Coping Strategies as Predictors of Engagement-Burnout in Undergraduate Students: Implications for Prevention and Treatment in Mental Well-Being. Front. Psychiatry 12, 596453. 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.596453 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de la Fuente J., Zapata L., Martínez-Vicente J. M., Cardelle-Elawar M., Sander P., Justicia F., et al. (2012). Regulatory teaching and self-regulated learning in college students: confirmatory validation study of the IATLP scales. Electronic J. Res. Educ. Psychol. 10, 839–866. 10.25115/ejrep.v10i27.1511 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- de la Fuente J., Zapata L., Martínez-Vicente J. M., Sander P., Cardelle-Elawar M. (2015). The role of personal self-regulation and regulatory teaching to predict motivational-affective variables, achievement, and satisfaction: a structural model. Front. Psychol. 6, 399. 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00399 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deci E., Ryan R. (2000). The “what” and “why” of goal pursuits: human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychol. Inq. 11, 227–268. 10.1207/S15327965PLI1104_01 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Deci E. L., Eghrari H., Patrick B. C., Leone D. R. (1994). Facilitating internalization: The self-determination theory perspective. J. Personality 62, 119–142. 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1994.tb00797.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deci E. L., Ryan R. M. (1985a). The general causality orientations scale: Self-determination in personality. J. Res. Personal. 19, 109–134. 10.1016/0092-6566(85)90023-6 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Deci E. L., Ryan R. M. (1985b). Intrinsic Motivation and Self-Determination in Human Behavior. New York, NY: Plenum Press. 10.1007/978-1-4899-2271-7 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Deci E. L., Ryan R. M. (1985c). The support of autonomy and the control of behavior. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 53, 1024. 10.1037/0022-3514.53.6.1024 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deci E. L., Ryan R. M. (1994). Promoting self-determined education. Scand. J. Educ. Res. 38, 3–14. [Google Scholar]
- Deci E. L., Ryan R. M. (2008). Self-determination theory: A macrotheory of human motivation, development, and health. Canad. Psychol. 49, 182–185. 10.1037/a0012801 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Deci E. L., Ryan R. M. (2012). “Self-determination theory,” in P. A. M. Van Lange, A. W. Kruglanski, y E. T. Higgins (eds.), Handbook of theories of social psychology. Sage Publications Ltd. p. 416–436. 10.4135/9781446249215.n21 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Deci E. L., Ryan R. M. (2016). “Optimizing students' motivation in the era of testing and pressure: a self-determination theory perspective,” in W. C. Liu, J. C. K. Wang and R. M. Ryan (Eds), Building Autonomous Learners: Perspectives From Research and Practice Using Self-Determination Theory. Singapore: Springer. p. 9–29. 10.1007/978-981-287-630-0_2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Deci E. L., Ryan R. M., Williams G. C. (1996). Need satisfaction and the self-regulation of learning. Learn. Indiv. Differ. 8, 165–183. 10.1016/S1041-6080(96)90013-8 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Deci E. L., Spiegel N. H., Ryan R. M., Koestner R., Kauffman M. (1982). Effects of performance standards on teaching styles: Behavior of controlling teachers. J. Educ. Psychol. 74, 852–859. [Google Scholar]
- DiBenedetto M. K., White M. C. (2013). Applying the model of development of self-regulatory competence to mentoring. In H. Bembenutty, T. Cleary, A. Kitsantas, (eds.), Applications of self-regulated learning across diverse disciplines. Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing. p. 445–472. [Google Scholar]
- DiBenedetto M. K., Zimmerman B. J. (2010). Differences in self-regulatory processes among students studying science: A microanalytic investigation. Int. J. Educ. Psychol. Assessm. 5, 2–24. [Google Scholar]
- Frost R. O., Marten P. A. (1990). Perfectionism and evaluative threat. Cogn. Ther. Res. 14, 559–572. [Google Scholar]
- Garzón-Umerenkova A., de la Fuente J., Amate J., Paoloni P. V., Fadda S., Pérez J. F. (2018). A linear empirical model of self-regulation on flourishing, health, procrastination, and achievement, among university students. Front. Psychol. 9, 536. 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00536 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garzón-Umerenkova A., de la Fuente J., Martínez-Vicente J. M., Zapata L., Pichardo M. C., García-Berbén A. B. (2017). Validation of the Spanish Short Self-Regulation Questionnaire (SSSRQ) through Rasch Analysis. Front. Psychol. 8, 276. 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00276 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garzón-Umerenkova A. G., Flores J. G., de la Fuente Arias J. (2020). Rasgos demográficos, académicos y personales asociados a tres tipos de procrastinación en el alumnado universitario. Bordón. Revista de pedagogía. 72, 49–65. 10.13042/Bordon.2020.01.69513 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Guido H. E., Tops M., Koole. S L. (2015). Handbook of Biobehavional Approaches to Self-Regulation. Springer [Google Scholar]
- Hagger M. S., Hamilton K. (2020). General causality orientations in self-determination theory: meta-analysis and test of a process model. Eur. J. Personal. (2021) 35, 710–35. 10.1177/0890207020962330 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hennessy E. A., Johnson B. T., Acabchuk R. L., McCloskey K., Stewart-James J. (2020). Self-regulation mechanisms in health behavior change: a systematic meta-review of meta-analyses, 2006–2017. Health Psychol. Rev. 14, 6–42. 10.1080/17437199.2019.1679654 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Howard J. L., Bureau J. S., Guay F., Chong J. X. Y., Ryan R. M. (2022). Student motivation and associated outcomes: A meta-analysis from self-determination theory. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 16, 1300–23. 10.1177/1745691620966789 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Howard J. L., Gagné M., Bureau J. S. (2017). Testing a continuum structure of self-determined motivation: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 143, 1346–1377. 10.1037/bul0000125 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoyle R. H. (2010). Personality and self-regulation. Handb. Personal Self-Regulat. 1, 18. 10.1002/9781444318111.ch1 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Kopala-Sibley D. C., Zuroff D. C. (2020). The self and depression: Four psychological theories and their potential neural correlates. J. Pers. 88, 14–30. 10.1111/jopy.12456 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Licht B. G., Kistner J. A. (1986). “Motivational problems of learning disabled children: Individual differences and their implications for treatment,” in J. K. Torgesen and B. W. L. Wong (eds.), Psychological and educational perspectives on learning disabilities. Orlando: Academic Press. p. 225–255. [Google Scholar]
- Lombaerts K., Backer F. D., Engels D., Vrije N., Braak J. V., Athanasou J. (2009). Development of the Self-Regulated Learning Teacher Belief Scale. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 24, 79–96 10.1007/BF0317347632196648 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- López-Madrigal C., de la Fuente J., García-Manglano J., Martínez-Vicente J. M., Peralta-Sánchez F. J., Amate-Romera J. (2021). The role of gender and age in the emotional well-being outcomes of young adults. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18, 522. 10.3390/ijerph18020522 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Madigan D. J. (2019). A meta-analysis of perfectionism and academic achievement. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 31, 967–989. 10.1007/s10648-019-09484-232718164 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Martínez-Vicente J. M., de la Fuente J. (2004). La autorregulación del aprendizaje a través el programa Pro and Regula [Self-regulation of Learning through the Pro and Regula Program]. Electronic J. Res. Educ. Psychol. 2, 145–156. [Google Scholar]
- Mithaug D. E. (1993). Self-regulation theory: How optimal adjustment maximizes gain. Praeger Publishers/Greenwood Publishing Group. [Google Scholar]
- Monem R. (2010). “Metacognitive functions, interest, and student engagement in the writing process: A review of the literature,” in M. S. Plakhotnik, S. M. Nielsen, and D. M. Pane (eds.), Proceedings of the Ninth Annual College of Education and GSN Research Conference. Miami: Florida Int. University. p. 64–68. Available online at: http://coeweb.fiu.edu/research_conference/ (accessed December 25, 2021).
- Moohr M. L., Balint-Langel K., Taylor J. C., Rizzo K. L. (2021). Practicing academic independence: self-regulation strategies for students with emotional and behavioral disorders. Beyond Behav. 30, 85–96. 10.1177/10742956211020666 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Moos D. C., Ringdal A. (2012). Self-regulated learning in the classroom: a literature review on the teacher's role. Educ. Res. Int. 2012, 1–15. t 10.1155/2012/42328435022002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Morbée S., Vermote B., Waterschoot J., Dieleman L., Soenens B., Van den Bergh O., et al. (2021). Adherence to COVID-19 measures: The critical role of autonomous motivation on a short- and long-term basis. Motivation Science. 7, 487–496. 10.1037/mot0000250 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ntoumanis N., Ng J.Y., Prestwich A., Quested E., Hancox J.E., Thøgersen-Ntoumani C., et al. (2020). A meta-analysis of self-determination theory-informed intervention studies in the health domain: effects on motivation, health behavior, physical, and psychological health. Health Psychol. Rev. 15, 214–44. 10.1080/17437199.2020.1718529 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nucci L. P. (2014). “The personal and the moral,” in Handbook of moral development. NY: Psychology Press(second edition) p. 538–558. [Google Scholar]
- Núñez J. C., Freire C., Ferradás M. M., Valle A., Xu J. (2020). Perceived parental involvement and student engagement with homework in secondary school: The mediating role of self-handicapping. Current Psychol. 30, 1–2. 10.1007/s12144-021-01791-8 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Pachón-Basallo M., de la Fuente J., Gonzáles-Torres M. C. (2021). Regulation/Non-Regulation/Dys-Regulation of Health Behavior, Psychological Reactance, and Health of University Undergraduate Students. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18, 3793. 10.3390/ijerph18073793 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Panadero E. (2017). A review of self-regulated learning: Six models and four directions for research. Front. Psychol. 8, 422. 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00422 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pekrun R., Hall N. C., Goetz T., Perry R. P. (2014). Boredom and academic achievement: Testing a model of reciprocal causation. J. Educ. Psychol. 106, 696–710, 10.1037/a0036006 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Pervin L. A. (1988). Personalidad: Controversias, problemas e tendencias actuales [Personality: Controversies, problems and current trends]. Psiquiatria y Psicol Humanista 19, 73–98. [Google Scholar]
- Peters E. E., Kitsantas A. (2010). Self-regulation of student epistemic thinking in science: The role of metacognitive prompts. Educ. Psychol. 30, 27–52. 10.1080/01443410903353294 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Pichardo C., Justicia F., de la Fuente J., Martínez-Vicente J. M., Berbén A. B. (2014). Factor structure of the self-regulation questionnaire (SRQ) at Spanish Universities. Spanish J. Psychol. 17, e62. 10.1017/sjp.2014.63 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pintrich P. and Schunk, D (2006). Motivación en contextos educativos. Teoría, investigación y aplicaciones. Madrid: Pearson. [Google Scholar]
- Pintrich P. R. (1991). A Manual for the Use of the Motivated Strategies for Learning Questionnaire (MSLQ). [Google Scholar]
- Pintrich P. R. (2000). The role of goal orientation in self-regulated learning. In M. Boekaerts, P. R. Pintrich, and M. Zeidner, (eds.), Handbook of Self-Regulation. San Diego: Academic Press. p. 452–502. 10.1016/B978-012109890-2/50043-3 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Powers J. P., Moshontz H., Hoyle R. H. (2020). Self-control and affect regulation styles predict anxiety longitudinally in university students. Collabra: Psychol. 6, 1–15. 10.1525/collabra.280 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ramdass D., Zimmerman B. J. (2011). Developing self-regulation skills: The important role of homework. J. advanced academics 22, 194–218. 10.1177/1932202X1102200202 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Reindl M., Tulis M., Dresel M. (2020). Profiles of emotional and motivational self-regulation following errors: Associations with learning. Learn. Indiv. Differ. 77, 101806. 10.1016/j.lindif.2019.101806 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Romer A. L., Hariri A. R., Strauman T. J. (2021). Regulatory focus and the p factor: Evidence for self-regulatory dysfunction as a transdiagnostic feature of general psychopathology. J. Psychiatric Res. 137, 178–185. 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2021.02.051 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ryan R. H., Deci E. L. (2020a). Self-Regulation and the Problem of Human Autonomy: Does Psychology Need Choice, Self-Determination, and Will? J. Personal. 74, 1554–1586. 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2006.00420.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ryan R. M., Deci E. L. (2000b). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. Am. Psychol. 55, 68–78. 10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.68 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ryan R. M., Deci E. L. (2002). An overview of self-determination theory: an organismic dialectical perspective,” in Handbook of Self-Determination Research Rochester. NY: University of Rochester Press. p. 3–33. [Google Scholar]
- Ryan R. M., Deci E. L. (2006). Self-regulation and the problem of human autonomy: Does psychology need choice, self-determination, and will? J. Personal. 74, 1557–1585. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ryan R. M., Deci E. L. (2017a). Self-Determination Theory: Basic Psychological Needs in Motivation, Development, and Wellness. Guilford Publications. 10.1521/978.14625/28806 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ryan R. M., Deci E. L. (2017b). Self-Determination Theory: Basic Psychological Needs in Motivation, Development, and Wellness. New York: Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
- Ryan R. M., Deci E. L. (2020b). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation from a self-determination theory perspective: definitions, theory, practices, and future directions. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 61, 1–11. 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2020.101860 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ryan R. M., Legate N., Niemiec C. P., Deci E. L. (2012). Beyond illusions and defense: Exploring the possibilities and limits of human autonomy and responsibility through self-determination theory. In Shaver, P. R., and Mikulincer, M. (Eds.), Meaning, mortality, and choice: The social psychology of existential concerns. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association p. 215–233. 10.1037/13748-012 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Sander P., de la Fuente J. (2020a). Modelling students' academic confidence, personality and academic emotions. Current Psychol. 22, 1–2. 10.1007/s12144-020-00957-0 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Sander P., de la Fuente J. (2020b). Undergraduate student gender, personality and academic confidence. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 17, 5567. 10.3390/ijerph17155567 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sander P., Medimorec S., Mann R. K., Szymanek L., de la Fuente J. (2021). Adult Involvement in Young People's Education, ALYPE. Middlesbrough: Tesside University. 10.1207/s15326985ep2501_6 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Schunk D. H. (1996). Self-Evaluation and Self-Regulated Learning. Paper presented at the Graduate School and University Center, City University of New York, New York, NY, United States. [Google Scholar]
- Schunk D. H., DiBenedetto M. K. (2016). “Self-efficacy theory in education,” in K. R. Wentzel and D. Miele (eds.), Handbook of motivation at school. New York: Routledge. 2nd ed., p. 34–54. [Google Scholar]
- Schunk D. H., DiBenedetto M. K. (2020a). “Social cognitive theory, self-efficacy, and students with disabilities: Implications for students with learning disabilities, reading disabilities, and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder,” in A. J. Martin, R. A. Sperling, and K. J. Newton (eds.), Handbook of Educ. Psychol. and students with special needs. New York: Routledge. p. 243–261. 10.4324/9781315100654-13 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Schunk D. H., DiBenedetto M. K. (2021). Self-efficacy and human motivation. Adv.motivat. sci. 8, 153–179. 10.1016/bs.adms.2020.10.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Schunk M. K., DiBenedetto M. (2020b). Motivation and social cognitive theory contemporary educational. Psychology 61, 101832. 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2019.10183219550191 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Seligman M. E. P., Peterson C. (2004). Character Strengths and Virtues: A Classification and Handbook. American Psychological Association. [Google Scholar]
- Shum A., Fryer L. K., Cano F., García-Berbén A. B., Pichardo-Martínez M.C. (2021). Nature vs. nurture: learning conceptions and environment as precursors to learning strategy patterns and their outcomes. Higher Education Res. Develop. 10.1080/07294360.2021.1985088 [Epub ahead of print]. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Stöber J. (1998). Worry, problem elaboration and suppression of imagery: The role of concreteness. Behav. Res. Therap. 36, 751–756. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor I. M., Boat R., Murphy S. L. (2020). Integrating theories of self-control and motivation to advance endurance performance. Int. Rev. Sport Exercise Psychol. 13, 1–20. 10.1080/1750984X.2018.1480050 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Usher E. L., Schunk D. H. (2018). “Social cognitive theoretical perspective of self-regulation,” in D. H. Schunk and J. A. Greene (eds.), Handbook of self-regulation of learning and performance. New York: Routledge. 2nd ed., p. 19–35. 10.4324/9781315697048-2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Valikhani A., Mokaberian M., Rahmati L., Moustafa A. A. (2020). Dimensional investigation of individual differences in personality disorder traits based on the three-dimensional model of personality self-regulation. Current Psychol. 1–13. 10.1007/s12144-020-01031-5 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Vallerand R. J. (2021). Reflections on the legacy of self-determination theory. Motivation Science. 10.1037/mot0000227 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Vega D., Torrubia R., Marco-Pallarés J., Soto A., Rodriguez-Fornells A. (2020). Metacognition of daily self-regulation processes and personality traits in borderline personality disorder. J. Affective Disorders 267, 243–250. 10.1016/j.jad.2020.02.033 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Villacís J. L., de la Fuente J., Naval C. (2021). Good Character at College: The Combined Role of Second-Order Character Strength Factors and Phronesis Motivation in Undergraduate Academic Outcomes. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18, 8263. 10.3390/ijerph18168263 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vohs K. D., Baumeister R. F. (2016). Handbook of Self-Regulation: Research, Theory, And Applications. Guilford Publications. [Google Scholar]
- Weiner B. (1993). On sin versus sickness: A theory of perceived responsibility and social motivation. Am. Psychol. 48, 957. 10.1037/0003-066X.48.9.957 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- White M. (2017). Cognitive modeling and self-regulation of learning in instructional settings. Teachers College Record. 119, 1–26. 10.1177/016146811711901304 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- White M. C., Bembenutty H. (2014). “Teachers as culturally proactive agents through cycles of self-regulation,” in Implications of Diversity Toward the Preparation of Teachers for Urban Schools and Communities. Symposium Conducted at the Biannual Department of Secondary Education and Youth Services Research Symposium, Vol. 9, ed S. J. Farenga (Queens, NY). [Google Scholar]
- White M. C., Bembenutty H. (2016). “Transforming classroom practices of teachers and students through training in self-regulation,” in A. Zusho and R. S. Blondie (Chairs), Promoting college and career readiness through self-regulated learning in the classroom. Symposium conducted during the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, Washington, D.C. [Google Scholar]
- White M. C., DiBenedetto M. K. (2015). Self-Regulation and the Common Core: Application to ELA Standards. New York, NY: Routledge. 10.4324/9781315882840 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Wolff W., Hirsch A., Maik H., Bieleke M., Shenhav A. (2021). “Neuroscientific approaches to self-regulatory control in sports,” in Motivation and Self-regulation in Sport and Exercise, eds C. Englert and I. Taylor (New York, NY: Routledge), 149–165. [Google Scholar]
- Wolters C. A., Benzon M. B., Arroyo-Giner C. (2011). “Assessing strategies for the self-regulation of motivation,” in Handbook of self-regulation of learning and performance, eds B. J. Zimmerman, and D. H. Schunk (New York, NY: Routledge; ), 298–312. [Google Scholar]
- Wrosch C., Scheier M. F., Miller G. E., Schulz R., Carver C. S. (2003). Adaptive self-regulation of unattainable goals: Goal disengagement, goal reengagement, and subjective well-being. Personality Soc. Psychol. Bull. 29, 1494–1508. 10.1177/0146167203256921 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zimmerman B. J. (2000). Attaining self-regulation: A social cognitive perspective. In M. Boekaerts, P. R. Pintrich and M. Zeidner (eds.), Handbook of Self-Regulation (pp. 13–39). London: Academic Press. 10.1016/B978-012109890-2/50031-7 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Zimmerman B. J. (2008). Investigating Self-Regulation and Motivation: Historical Background, Methodological Developments, and Future Prospects. 10.3102/0002831207312909 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Zimmerman B. J. (2013). From cognitive modeling to self-regulation: A social cognitive career path. Educ. Psychol. 48, 1–13. 10.1080/00461520.2013.794676 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Zimmerman B. J., Kitsantas A. (2005). “The Hidden Dimension of Personal Competence: Self-Regulated Learning and Practice,” in A. J. Elliot and C. S. Dweck (eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation. Guilford Publications. p. 509–526. [Google Scholar]
- Zimmerman B. J., Labuhn A. S. (2012). “Self-regulation of learning: Process approaches to personal development,” in APA Educ. Psychol. handbook, Vol 1: Theories, constructs, and critical issues. American Psychological Association. P. 399–425. 10.1037/13273-014 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Zimmerman B. J., Martinez-Pons M. (1990). Student differences in self-regulated learning: relating grade, sex, and giftedness to self-efficacy and strategy use. J. Educ. Psychol. 82, 51–59. 10.1037/0022-0663.82.1.51 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Zimmerman B. J., Martinez-Pons M. M. (1986). Development of a structured interview for assessing student use of self-regulated learning strategies. American Educ. Res. J. 23, 614–628. 10.3102/00028312023004614 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Zimmerman B. J., Moylan A., Hudesman J., White N., Flugman B. (2011). Enhancing self-reflection and mathematics achievement of at-risk urban technical college students. Psychol. Test Assessment Model. 53, 141–160. [Google Scholar]
- Zimmerman B. J., Schunk B. (2012). “Motivation: An essential dimension of self-regulated learning,” in D. H. Schunk, B. J. Zimmerman, Motivation and Self-Regulated Learning Theory, Research, and Applications. New York: Imprint Routledge. [Google Scholar]
- Zimmerman B. J., Schunk B. J., DiBenedetto M. K. (2017). “Role of self-efficacy and related beliefs in self-regulation of learning and performance,” in A. Elliot, C. Dweck, and D. Yeager (eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation (2nd ed.) New York: Guilford Press. p. 83–114. [Google Scholar]
- Zimmerman B. J., Schunk D. H. (2001). Self-Regulated Learning and Academic Achievement: Theoretical Perspectives. New York, NY: Routledge. [Google Scholar]
- Zimmerman B. J., Schunk D. H. (2011). Handbook of Self-Regulation of Learning and Performance. Routledge/Taylor and Francis Group. [Google Scholar]
- Zimmerman B. J., Schunk D. H., DiBenedetto M. K. (2015). “A personal agency view of self-regulated learning: The role of goal setting,” in F. Guay, H. Marsh, D. McInerney, and R. G. Craven (eds.), Self-concept, motivation, and identity: Underpinning success with research and practices. Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing. p. 83–114. [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.