Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2022 Aug 3.
Published in final edited form as: Nature. 2021 Oct 6;599(7883):96–101. doi: 10.1038/s41586-021-03956-8

5-HT modulation of a medial septal circuit tunes social memory stability

Xiaoting Wu 1, Wade Morishita 1, Kevin T Beier 2, Boris D Heifets 3, Robert C Malenka 1
PMCID: PMC9348902  NIHMSID: NIHMS1822873  PMID: 34616037

Abstract

Social memory, the ability to recognize and remember familiar conspecifics, is critical for an animal’s survival in its social group1,2. Hippocampal subregions, dorsal CA2 (dCA2)3-5 and ventral CA1 (vCA1)6, and their projection targets6,7, play important roles in social memory. However, the relevant extrahippocampal input regions remain poorly defined. Here, we identify the medial septum (MS) as a dCA2 input region critical for social memory and reveal that 5-HT modulation of the MS bidirectionally controls social memory formation, thereby impacting memory stability. Novel social interactions increase activity in dCA2-projecting MS neurons and induce plasticity at glutamatergic synapses from MS neurons onto dCA2 pyramidal neurons. Activity of dCA2-projecting MS cells is enhanced by the neuromodulator serotonin (5-HT) acting on 5-HT1b receptors. Moreover, optogenetic manipulation of median raphe (MR) 5-HT terminals in the MS bidirectionally regulates social memory stability. This work expands understanding of the neural mechanisms by which social interactions lead to social memory and provides evidence that 5-HT plays a critical role in promoting not only prosocial behaviours8,9, but also social memory, by influencing distinct target structures.


Since the discovery that the hippocampal dCA2 subregion is specific for harbouring social memory3, research on this topic has focused on identifying additional storage sites in the hippocampus5,6,10 and their respective output regions5-7,11. Little is known about the input regions to dCA2 or vCA1, which carry information about social encounters and are required for social memory formation. To identify hippocampal-projecting brain regions, which are specifically involved in social memory, we exposed TRAP2;Ai14 mice after injection of 4-hydroxytamoxifen (4-OHT) to a novel mouse, novel object or an empty behavioural chamber to label neurons activated by these experiences and examined brain regions that are not associated with object memory and project to dCA2 or vCA1 (Extended Data Fig. 1a). Increased numbers of tdTomato-positive cells were found in the MS and nucleus of the diagonal band (NDB) when comparing novel mouse to novel object and control conditions (Extended Data Fig.1b). Interaction with a novel mouse also increased tdTomato-positive cells in dCA2 (Extended Data Fig. 1c, d). To determine if, as previously suggested3,12, neurons in different parts of the septum (LS, MS, NDB) form monosynaptic connections on hippocampal neurons, we used monosynaptic rabies tracing, which generated labelled cells in the MS and NDB, but not LS (Extended Data Fig. 1e, f). Consistent with these and previous results12, anterograde tracing revealed strong innervation of dorsal and ventral hippocampus (Extended Data Fig. 1g, h). Thus, MS and NDB neurons show elevated activity during social encounters and project to dCA2 and vCA1.

Bidirectional modulation of social memory by MS manipulations

The MS and NDB are implicated in hippocampal theta-rhythm13 and arousal14, but little is known about their potential roles in social behaviour. To determine if the MS influences social memory, we expressed an inhibitory designer receptor exclusively activated by designer drugs (DREADD) fused to mCherry (hM4Di) or mCherry alone (mCh) in the MS of mice and administered the DREADD activator clozapine-N-oxide (CNO) or saline prior to a short-term social memory assay (Fig. 1a, b)6,7. Two cohorts of control mice, expressing mCh and receiving CNO or expressing hM4Di and receiving saline, exhibited short-term (10 min interval) social memory as evidenced by their preference for the novel mouse (nm) chamber compared to the chamber containing a familiar mouse (fm) (Fig. 1c, Extended Data Fig. 2a). In contrast, hM4Di mice receiving CNO exhibited no short-term memory of the fm (Fig. 1c, left graph). Calculating a discrimination score for each subject, which enables more direct comparisons between treatments, confirmed the absence of social memory in mice in which MS neuronal activity was inhibited by hM4Di (Fig. 1c, right graph). Baseline sociability assessed during the initial, 3-chamber sociability assay, as well as inanimate object memory, were not affected by hM4Di-mediated inhibition of MS (Extended Data Fig. 2b, c).

Fig. 1∣. Chemogenetic manipulation of the medial septum bidirectionally regulates social memory.

Fig. 1∣

a, Experimental timeline of 3-chamber tests. b, Schematic and representative image of medial septum (MS) injection site. Scale bar, 500 μm. c, d, Duration subjects spent in chamber with familiar mouse (fm) or novel mouse (nm) (left graph) and discrimination scores (right graph) (c: F2, 40=4.666, P=0.0151; mCh: n=19, hM4Di; n=12. d: F2, 28=8.585, P=0.0012; mCh: n=10, hM3Dq: n=11). e, Timeline of TRAP2 experiment. f, Duration in chamber with fm or nm (left) and discrimination scores (right) (t7=2.594, P=0.0357; CNO: n=8, saline: n=10). g, Schematic and hM4Di expression in nucleus of the diagonal band (NDB). Scale bar, 500 μm. h, Duration in chamber with fm or nm (left) and discrimination scores (right) (t17=1.61, P=0.1259; mCh: n=10, hM4Di: n=9). i, Timeline of 5-trial social memory test. j, Duration of direct interaction (F27, 108=2.478, P=0.0005, n=10). (c: hM4Di+CNO, mCh+CNO duration; d, f, h: duration; f: discrimination scores): two-tailed paired Student’s t-test. (c: hM4Di+saline duration): two-tailed Wilcoxon signed rank test, (c, d: discrimination scores): one-way ANOVA with Tukey’s post-hoc test, (h: discrimination scores): two-tailed unpaired Student’s t-test. (j) two-way ANOVA with Tukey’s post-hoc test. N.S.=not significant, *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001. Error bars denote s.e.m.

We next addressed if activation of MS neurons by the excitatory DREADD hM3Dq enhances social memory (Extended Data Fig. 2d). There was no effect of CNO administration in hM3Dq mice prior to the initial social interaction when social memory was assessed at 10 min (Extended Data Fig. 2e). However, this manipulation prolonged the duration of social memory to 2 h in the hM3Dq mice that received CNO but not in the two cohorts of control mice (Fig. 1d). Indeed, hM3Dq activation of MS neurons during the social interaction maintained social memory for 24 hours (Extended Data Fig. 2e) but had no effects on baseline sociability (Extended Data Fig. 2f) or inanimate object memory (Extended Data Fig. 2g). To address if the MS neurons that were activated during the initial social interaction are specifically necessary for social memory formation, we expressed Cre-dependent hM4Di in the MS of TRAP2 mice and exposed these mice to a novel mouse after administering 4-OHT (Extended Data Fig. 2h). Two weeks later, TRAP2 mice that received saline prior to the social interaction exhibited normal social memory, which was blocked when the mice received CNO (Fig. 1e, f). Thus, the MS neurons that are activated by a novel social interaction are necessary for normal social memory formation.

To address if the NDB is also required for social memory, we expressed hM4Di in the NDB but found that CNO administration did not reduce social memory (Fig. 1g, h). As a final, test of the role of the MS in social memory, we performed a 5-trial social memory test3. As expected3, by the 3rd and 4th trials, the control mice spent less time interacting with the now familiar intruder mouse, but on trial 5 the time spent interacting with a novel mouse increased to baseline levels (Fig. 1j, Extended Data Fig. 2i). In contrast, the mice in which MS activity was inhibited (hM4Di + CNO), maintained equal interaction times for all 5 trials (Fig. 1j, Extended Data Fig. 2i).

To address whether MS activity is necessary for memory acquisition and/or memory recall, we performed optogenetic manipulations by expressing the inhibitory halorhodopsin, NpHR3.0 fused to eYFP (NpHR) or eYFP alone in the MS and providing light stimulation at various times during the social memory assay (Fig. 2a). Activation of NpHR throughout the procedure (on) and during acquisition only (acq) disrupted social memory, whereas applying light during the recall (rec) phase had no effect (Fig. 2b). Social memory was also normal when NpHR mice were not exposed to light (Fig. 2b) and in eYFP mice that received light stimulation throughout the procedure (Fig. 2c). Similar to chemogenetic manipulations, optogenetic inhibition of MS did not affect baseline sociability or inanimate object memory (Extended Data Fig. 3a-c).

Fig. 2∣. Inhibition of medial septum to dorsal CA2 projection disrupts social memory.

Fig. 2∣

a, Schematic of experiment. b, c, Duration in chamber with fm or nm (left) and discrimination scores (right) (b: F3,35=5.227, P=0.0044; n=10, recall: n=9, c: tg=0.1045, P=0.919; n=10). d, f, Schematic of CNO infusion. e, g, Duration in chamber (left) and discrimination scores (right) (e: t25=2.522, P=0.0184; mCh: n=15, hM4Di: n=12. g: P=0.4232; mCh: n=9, hM4Di: n=8). h, Timeline of experiment (left). hM4Di expression (right). Scale bar, 500 μm. i, Duration in chamber (left) and discrimination scores (right) (P=0.002; n=15). (b, c, e: duration; c: discrimination score; g, i: mCh duration): two-tailed paired Student’s t-test. (b, i: discrimination score): one-way ANOVA with Tukey’s post-hoc test, (e: discrimination score): two-tailed unpaired Student’s t-test. (g: hM4Di duration): two-tailed Wilcoxon signed rank test, (g: discrimination scores): two-tailed Mann-Whitney test, (i: discrimination scores): Kruskal-Wallis with post-hoc Dunn’s test. N.S.=not significant, *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01. Error bars denote s.e.m.

Regulation of social memory by MS to dCA2 projections

Having established a critical role for the MS in social memory, we examined if MS neuron projections to specific hippocampal subregions are required. We expressed hM4Di-mCh or mCh in the MS and implanted cannula above dCA2 or vCA1 to infuse CNO prior to the sociability assay (Extended Data Fig. 3d). Infusion of CNO into dCA2 of hM4Di mice, but not into dCA2 of mCh mice, blocked social memory (Fig. 2d, e) and had no effects on baseline sociability and inanimate object memory (Extended Data Fig. 3e, f). In contrast, CNO infusion into vCA1 of hM4Di mice had no effect on social memory (Fig. 2f, g).

CNO may have spread to dCA1 and dCA3 and influenced MS projections to these regions. To manipulate activity of MS neurons that specifically synapse on dCA2 pyramidal neurons, we applied an intersectional genetic strategy in Amigo2-Cre mice, a dCA2-specific Cre-driver line3. We injected AAVs expressing Cre-dependent TVA and rabies glycoprotein into dCA2 and an AAV expressing Flp-dependent hM4Di-mCh or mCh into the MS (Fig. 2h). Two weeks later, we injected a rabies virus expressing Flp15 into dCA2 (Fig. 2h). Normal social memory was present in mCh mice administered CNO and in hM4Di mice administered saline but was absent in hM4Di mice administered CNO (Fig. 2i) with no differences between male and female subjects (Extended Data Fig. 4a). DREADD-mediated inhibition of dCA2-projecting MS cells did not influence baseline sociability or inanimate object memory (Extended Data Fig. 4b, c). Because the MS is implicated in contextual memory16 and aversion17,18, which could influence social memory, we tested if inhibition of this subset of MS cells influenced contextual fear conditioning or resulted in conditioned place preference or aversion. Fear conditioning was not affected by CNO administration in these hM4Di-expressing mice (Extended Data Fig. 4d) and had no effect in a conditioned place assay (Extended Data Fig. 4e).

To assess whether dCA2-projecting MS neurons send collaterals elsewhere, we employed a marker to distinguish fibres of passage from axon terminals. We injected CAV-Flp into the dCA2 and Flp-dependent mGFP-synaptophysin-mRuby into the MS (Extended Data Fig. 4f). dCA2-projecting MS cells exhibited collaterals in the vCA1 and a smaller number in the supramammillary nucleus (Extended Data Fig. 4f). However, the MS collaterals innervating vCA1 are unlikely to be important for social memory since inhibiting the MS to vCA1 projection had no effect (Fig. 2g).

Plasticity at MS to dCA2 synapses during social memory formation

The MS contains a heterogeneous population of neurons14,19. To identify the MS cell types that synapse on dCA2 pyramidal neurons, we performed in-situ hybridization to probe for cholinergic (CHAT), glutamatergic (SLC17A6, gene name for vGlut2) and GABAergic (GAD2) markers in MS cells, that were labelled by GFP via monosynaptic rabies tracing in dCA2 of Amigo2-Cre mice (Extended Data Fig. 5a, b). Quantification of GFP-positive cells revealed that ~90% of dCA2-projecting MS cells are CHAT-positive, ~30% are SLC17A6-positive and ~5% are GAD2-positive with ~80% of SLC17A6-positive cells and ~50% of GAD2-positive cells also being positive for CHAT (Extended Data Fig. 5c, d). Immunohistochemistry confirmed that the majority of dCA2-projecting MS neurons express CHAT (~60%) while a smaller proportion (~40%) express calcium/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II (CaMKII), a marker for glutamatergic neurons (Extended Data Fig. 5e)20. To interrogate the functional role of cholinergic and glutamatergic inputs to dCA2 in social memory, we infused into dCA2 a mixture of muscarinic and nicotinic receptor antagonists or the NMDA receptor antagonist D-AP5 (Fig. 3a). Infusion of D-AP5 prevented social memory, which was not influenced by blockade of cholinergic synaptic transmission or control infusion of saline (Fig. 3b). Baseline sociability was not influenced by any of these manipulations (Extended Data Fig. 5f, g) but inanimate object memory was prevented by infusion of the antagonists (Extended Data Fig. 5h), perhaps because of leakage into dCA1.

Fig. 3∣. Glutamatergic inputs from medial septum to dCA2 play a crucial role in social memory formation.

Fig. 3∣

a, Timeline for dCA2 drug infusions. b, Duration in chamber with fm or nm (left) and discrimination scores (right) (F2,36=4.017, P=0.0266; n=13). c, Schematic of ex vivo electrophysiology (ephys). d, Current-clamp recordings from dCA2 neurons. Input-output graph of peak postsynaptic potential (PSP), n=5/1, cells/animals. Scale bars, 10 mV, 50 ms. e, Sample responses to stimulation at different membrane potentials. Scale bars, 20 mV, 50 ms. f, Percentage of cells, where the ChETA (n=20/9) and ChR2 (n=22/6) induced PSCs (n=42/15) were >60% reduced by NBQX (10 μM), picrotoxin (50 μM) or Mecamylamine (5 μM). Representative traces (right: Scale bars, 50 pA, 100 ms). g, Schematic of experiment. h, AMPAR/NMDAR ratios in dCA2 neurons (P=0.0009) of mice which interacted with a novel object (n=29/11, cells/animals) or a novel mouse (n=27/7). Representative traces (above: Scale bars, 100 pA, 100 ms). i, Schematic of in vivo optogenetic LTD induction. j, k, Duration in chambers with fm or nm (left) and discrimination scores (right) (m: t9=2.538, P=0.0318; n=10. n: t9=1.155, P=0.278; n=10). (b: duration; j, k): two-tailed paired Student’s t-test. (b: discrimination score): one-way ANOVA with Tukey’s post-hoc test. (h) two-tailed Mann-Whitney test. N.S.=not significant, *P < 0.05, ***P < 0.001. Error bars denote s.e.m.

To investigate if glutamatergic inputs from the MS elicit synaptic responses in dCA2 pyramidal neurons, we expressed channelrhodopsin2 (ChR2) or channelrhodopsin-E123T accelerated (ChETA) in the MS of Amigo2-Cre mice and Cre-dependent tdTomato in dCA2 (Fig. 3c). Current clamp recordings from visually identified, tdTomato+ CA2 pyramidal neurons in ex vivo slices in response to optogenetic stimulation of MS inputs showed that excitatory postsynaptic potentials (EPSPs) were generated in all cells (n=5). Increasing stimulation intensity increased EPSP amplitude (Fig. 3d) and generated action potentials (Fig. 3e). During voltage clamp recordings, sequential application of the AMPAR antagonist NBQX, the GABA-A receptor antagonist picrotoxin and mecamylamine hydrochloride revealed that ~80% of neurons expressed MS input-evoked postsynaptic currents (PSCs) that were significantly (>60%) reduced by NBQX with <10% of neurons expressing PSCs reduced by picrotoxin or mecamylamine (Fig. 3f; Extended Data Fig. 6a). These results suggest that the major functionally important inputs from MS to dCA2 pyramidal neurons are glutamatergic.

To investigate the role of synaptic strength changes at MS to dCA2 pyramidal cell synapses in social memory, we exposed Amigo2-Cre mice expressing ChR2 in MS and Cre-dependent tdTomato in dCA2 to a novel mouse or novel object and prepared slices immediately after the interaction (Fig. 3g). AMPAR/NMDAR ratios, a surrogate measure for synaptic strength21, in dCA2 neurons were increased after social interaction compared to object interaction (Fig. 3h) while paired-pulse ratios were unchanged (Extended Data Fig. 6b). If an increase in strength at MS to dCA2 pyramidal cell excitatory synapses during novel social interactions is required for social memory, reducing this increase should impair social memory. We first confirmed that low frequency (1 Hz for 5 min) optogenetic stimulation of MS inputs elicited a long-term depression (LTD) at synapses on dCA2 pyramidal neurons, which was not influenced by application of muscarinic, nicotinic and GABA-A receptor antagonists (Extended Data Fig. 6c). Applying the same stimulation protocol (1 Hz/5 min) in vivo to MS inputs in dCA2 (Fig. 3i; Extended Data Fig. 6d) immediately after memory acquisition to reduce the increase in synaptic strength generated by the novel social interaction prevented expression of social memory (Fig. 3j). Similarly, applying low frequency stimulation (1 Hz/10 min) in vivo prior to the initial social interaction also prevented social memory formation (Extended Data Fig. 7a). Importantly, applying the same protocols in control mice expressing eYFP had no effect on social memory (Fig. 3k, Extended Data Fig. 7b) and no effects on baseline sociability or inanimate object memory in either ChR2-or eYFP-expressing mice (Extended Data Fig. 7c-h). These findings suggest that strengthening glutamatergic synapses from MS neurons onto dCA2 pyramidal neurons during a novel social encounter is required for normal social memory.

Bidirectional modulation of social memory by MS 5-HT1b receptors

How might MS neurons be influenced during a novel social interaction? MS neurons express receptors for oxytocin and 5-HT22-24, neuromodulators that play roles in social behaviours8,9. To explore the potential role of oxytocin and 5-HT action in the MS in social memory, we infused oxytocin receptor (OxtR) or 5-HT receptor (5-HTR) antagonists into the MS prior to the social memory assay (Fig. 4a). Social memory was not influenced by infusion of saline or an OxtR antagonist (L-368,899 hydrochloride) but was prevented by infusion of the promiscuous 5-HTR antagonist, methiothepin maleate (Fig. 4b). Infusion of the broad 5-HT2R antagonist methysergide or the 5-HT1aR antagonist NAD-299 did not influence social memory, but infusion of the specific 5-HT1bR antagonist NAS-181 impaired it (Fig. 4b). None of these manipulations had detectable effects on baseline sociability (Extended Data Fig. 8a). Because social memory was impaired by a 5-HT1bR antagonist, we next explored if a 5-HT1b agonist (CP93129) might prolong social memory in a manner similar to increasing MS neuron activity (Fig. 1d). Indeed, while social memory was again absent at 2 hours after the initial social interaction in control mice, MS infusion of CP93129 prior to the social interaction resulted in social memory at 2 hours (Fig. 4c).

Fig. 4∣. Bidirectional modulation of social memory by medial septum 5-HT1b receptors.

Fig. 4∣

a, Schematic and infusion site labelled with ink. Scale bar, 500 μm. b, c, Duration in chamber with fm or nm (left) and discrimination scores (right) (b: F5,78=6.404, P<0.0001; n=14. c: t13=2.602, P=0.0219; n=14). d, Schematic of fibre photometry experiment. e, h, Schematic and GCaMP6f expression. Scale bar, 500 μm. f, g, i, j, Time courses of average GCaMP6f transient z-scores event-locked to direct interactions (left) and quantification of average during interaction (right) (f: F3,16=6.392, P=0.0047; n=5, g: t4=5.103, P=0.007, n=5. i: F2,12=5.523, P=0.0199; n=5. j: t4=3.752, P=0.0199; n=5). k, Schematic of ex vivo electrophysiology set-up. Scale bar, 20 μm. I, Summary time course of IPSCs in response to CP93129 (5 μM) n=15/7 (cells/ animals). Representative traces (above: scale bars, 100 pA, 50 ms). m, Summary time course of EPSCs in response to CP93129 (5 μM) n=9/4. Representative traces (above: scale bars, 100 pA, 10 ms). n, Average PSC (% control) in presence of CP93129 (t22=1.854, P=0.0386; IPSC: n=9, EPSC: n=15). (b: duration; c, g, j): two-tailed paired Student’s t-test. (b: discrimination score; f, i): one-way ANOVA with Tukey’s post-hoc test. (n) one-tailed unpaired Student’s t-test. N.S.=not significant, *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001. Error bars and shading denote s.e.m.

Our findings predict that MS neuron activity increases during a novel social interaction in a 5-HT1bR-dependent manner. To test this hypothesis, we expressed GCaMP6f in MS glutamatergic neurons and recorded MS neuron activity while the subject mouse had sequential interactions with a novel mouse; the same, now familiar mouse; a novel inanimate object and, finally, another novel mouse (Fig. 4d, e). Activity in MS neurons increased robustly during interactions with a novel mouse while interactions with a familiar mouse or a novel inanimate object generated a smaller increase in MS neuron activity (Fig. 4f). Administration of NAS-181 reduced the increase in MS neuron activity during the novel mouse interaction (Fig. 4g), demonstrating the necessity of 5-HT1bRs. Similarly, the activity of MS cells that project to dCA2, captured by expressing CAV-Cre in dCA2 and Cre-dependent GCaMP6f in MS, was significantly higher during interaction with a novel mouse versus a familiar mouse or novel object and was also diminished after administration of NAS-181 (Fig. 4h-j).

To address how 5-HT1bRs may contribute to increasing MS neuron activity during novel social interactions, we performed ex vivo whole-cell recordings from visually identified dCA2-projecting MS neurons that were labelled by expression of CAV-Cre in dCA2 and Cre-dependent EGFP in MS (Fig. 4k). Application of the 5-HT1bR agonist CP93129 (2.5 or 5 μM) generated an inward, depolarizing current in ~60-70% of dCA2-projecting MS neurons (Extended Data Fig. 8b-e). Because 5-HT1bRs are commonly presynaptic and inhibit neurotransmitter release25, we hypothesized that activation of 5-HT1bRs depresses inhibitory synaptic transmission and increases MS neuron activity via “disinhibition”. Consistent with this prediction, CP93129 decreased evoked inhibitory postsynaptic currents (IPSCs) by ~50% (Fig. 4l) and also depressed EPSCs to a smaller degree than IPSCs (Fig. 4m, n). These effects of CP93129 were not due to “rundown” of synaptic currents since EPSCs and IPSCs were stable for 45 min using identical recording conditions (Extended Data Fig. 8f-k). The depression of IPSCs is likely via a presynaptic action as evidenced by a decrease in the frequency of spontaneous IPSCs and no effect on their amplitude (Extended Data Fig. 8l, m).

Bidirectional modulation of social memory by MR 5-HT release in the MS

Our results suggest that social memory formation during a novel social interaction requires 5-HT release in the MS, which increases the activity of dCA2-projecting MS neurons via disinhibition. Consistent with the median raphe (MR) being a source of 5-HT in the MS26, expressing Cre-dependent EGFP in the MR of Sert-Cre mice revealed 5-HTergic axon innervation in the MS (Extended Data Fig. 9a). Using TRIO (tracing the relationship between input and output) monosynaptic rabies virus tracing27, we also observed labelling in the MR (Extended Data Fig. 9b) suggesting that there is direct synaptic input from the MR onto dCA2-projecting MS cells. To test the necessity of MR-mediated 5-HT release in the MS for social memory, we expressed Cre-dependent NpHR3.0-eYFP or eYFP alone in the MR of Sert-Cre mice and placed an optical fibre adjacent to the MS (Fig. 5a). Optogenetic inhibition of MR 5-HT inputs in the MS during the initial social interaction impaired social memory, which was normal in the control eYFP mice and when light was not applied to the NpHR mice (Fig. 5b, c). Baseline sociability and inanimate object memory were unaffected by these manipulations (Extended Data Fig. 9c-f).

Fig. 5∣. Median raphe 5-HT release in the MS regulates social memory.

Fig. 5∣

a, d, Schematic and MR injection site. Scale bar, 500 μm. b, c, e, f, Duration in chamber with fm or nm (left) and discrimination scores (right) (b: t9=2.27, P=0.0493; n=10. c: t9=0.4444, P=0.6672; n=10. e: t10=2.471; P=0.033; n=11. f: t9=0.2896, P=0.7787; n=10). g, Schematic and MS infusion site filled with ink. Scale bar, 500 μm. h, Duration in chamber with fm or nm (left) and discrimination scores (right) (t12=2.202, P=0.048; saline: n=14, CP: n=13). i, Duration in chamber with fo or no (left) and discrimination scores (right) (t13=0.3522, P=0.7304; n=14). Two-tailed paired Student’s t-test except for (e: ChR2 off duration; i: saline duration): two-tailed Wilcoxon signed rank test. N.S.=not significant, *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001. Error bars denote s.e.m.

To test if enhancing 5-HT release in the MS influences the duration of social memory, we expressed ChR2-eYFP or eYFP in MR 5-HT neurons of Sert-Cre mice (Fig. 5d). Activation of 5-HT inputs in the MS during the initial novel social interaction generated social memory lasting 2 hours, which was not present in the control conditions (Fig. 5e, f). Similar to previous results, these manipulations had no effects on baseline sociability (Extended Data Fig. 9g, h). To explore if our findings might have therapeutic implications, we generated mice haploinsufficient for neuroligins 1,2, and 3 (Nlgn123hets)28. Because Nlgn3 knockout or mutation impairs social memory29, but not sociability30, we predicted that Nlgn123hets would exhibit the same behavioural phenotype. Indeed, these mice exhibited normal baseline sociability (Extended Data Fig. 9i) but absent social memory (Fig. 5h). Infusion of CP93129 into the MS rescued this social memory deficit (Fig. 5g, h) but had no effect on inanimate object memory (Fig. 5i) or sociability (Extended Data Fig. 9i).

Discussion

Recent work established that the hippocampal dCA2 subregion plays a specific role in social memory via its projections to vCA13,5, which in turn sends projections to the nucleus accumbens that are also required for social memory6. Here we demonstrate that increased activity in dCA2-projecting glutamatergic MS neurons during the initial social interaction is critical for social memory formation likely because the strength of MS to dCA2 pyramidal neuron synapses increases. Furthermore, we show that dCA2-projecting MS neuron activity during a novel social interaction requires MR-mediated 5-HT release, which influences the stability of social memory via actions on 5-HT1bRs in the MS (Extended Data Fig. 10). Particularly striking were the findings that infusion of a 5-HT1bR agonist into the MS prolongs the duration of social memory and rescues social memory deficits in an autism-associated mouse model. Given the role of neuroligins in synaptic function in CA1 pyramidal neurons31, we surmise that the impairment of social memory in Nlgn123hets may be due to deficient strength and/or plasticity at MS to dCA2 pyramidal neuron synapses and that administration of CP93129 into MS enhances activity in dCA2-projecting MS neurons in a fashion that compensates for their synaptic communication deficit.

Our current model (Extended Data Fig. 10) suggests that 5-HT in the MS primarily works presynaptically to preferentially reduce inhibition of dCA2-projecting MS neurons, perhaps via volume transmission32,33 in a manner similar to the domain-overlap model proposed for dopamine34. However, we cannot rule out a role for 5-HT acting directly on this population of neurons. Importantly, none of our many manipulations of 5-HT action in the MS or MS neuron activity influenced basal sociability, suggesting that the appetitive value of a novel social interaction was not affected. Similar manipulations of 5-HT release in the nucleus accumbens have robust effects on sociability, also via 5-HT1bRs8,9. Furthermore, 5-HT action in other brain structures appears to play a role in aggression and perhaps impulsivity35,36. Thus, our results add to the growing evidence that global conclusions about the functions of neuromodulators such as 5-HT are not warranted. Instead, the function of 5-HT or other neuromodulators need to be considered in the context of the brain region in which they are acting.

5-HT neurons in the MR are heterogeneous and consist of a variety of distinct molecular subtypes37,38, which may orchestrate different behaviours39. Therefore, it remains to be determined which specific subtypes of 5-HT MR neurons are critical for social memory and what role the release of other neurotransmitters from MR neurons, such as glutamate, might play40. It is also important to acknowledge that neuromodulators such as 5-HT do not act in isolation. Both oxytocin41,42 and dopamine29 action in structures other than the MS have been implicated in social memory suggesting that a comprehensive understanding of how social memories form will require simultaneously examining the actions of multiple neuromodulators in a range of brain areas.

Methods

Experimental Subjects

Female and male C57BL/6J (JAX, 664), TRAP243 (gift of L. Luo), TRAP2;Ai14 (gift of L. Luo), B6.Cg-Tg(Amigo2-cre)1Sieg/J (Amigo2-Cre, JAX, 30215)3, Tg(Slc6a4-cre)ET33Gsat (Sert-Cre, JAX, 3836639) and Nlgn123 het mice were used as experimental subjects. A breeding strategy for the transgenic lines was employed, where strictly transgene-carrying male mice were crossed with wild-type female C57BL/6J mice sourced commercially to produce heterozygous offspring. Nlgn123het mice were generated by breeding Nlgn123cKO28 (gift of T. C. Sudhof) male mice with B6.C-Tg(CMV-cre)1Cgn/J (JAX, 6054) females. Mice were weaned at 21 days old and housed with 2-5 mice per cage. Behavioural experiments were performed when mice were 7-14 weeks old. All mice were housed on a 12-hour light/dark cycle with food and water ad libitum and behavioural experiments were conducted during the same circadian period (7:00-19:00). All procedures com plied with the animal care standards set forth by the National Institutes of Health and were approved by the Stanford University Administrative Panel on Laboratory Animal Care and Administrative Panel of Biosafety. No statistical methods were used to predetermine sample size, which was based on previous experience with the variance of the assays. All experiments were analysed without knowledge of the specific manipulation each subject had undergone and performed without knowledge of the identity of the virus that had been injected or the transgene being expressed.

Stereotactic injections and cannula implantation

Mice, 4-7 weeks of age, were anesthetized by intraperitoneal injection with a mixture of ketamine (75 mg/kg body weight) and dexmedetomidine (0.375 mg/kg body weight). Heads were fixed on a Kopf stereotaxic apparatus and holes were drilled bilaterally into the skull except for structures on the midline. For injections into specific brain regions the following bregma coordinates were used: dCA2, −1.6 mm anteroposterior (AP), −1.6 mm mediolateral (ML), 1.7 mm dorsoventral (DV) from dura; MS, 0.65 mm AP, 0 mm ML, 4 mm DV; NDB, 1.25 mm AP, 0 mm ML, 5 mm DV; MR, −4.4 mm AP, 0 mm ML, 4.6 mm DV. Glass cannula filled with virus solution were lowered to the specified depth from the dura and 0.5 – 1.5 μl of virus solution was injected using a microinjection pump (Harvard Apparatus) at a flow rate of 0.1 – 0.25 μl/min. After surgery, atipamezole (10 mg/kg body weight) was administered by intraperitoneal injection.

Adeno-associated viruses (AAV) used for stereotactic injections were purchased from the Stanford Neuroscience Gene Vector and Virus Core and included: AAVDJ-hSyn-EGFP, AAVDJ-hSyn-hM4Di-mCherry, AAVDJ-hSyn-DIO-hM4D(Gi)-mCherry, AAVDJ-hSyn-fDIO-hM4D(Gi)-mCherry, AAVDJ-hSyn-hM3D(Gq)-mCherry, AAVDJ-hSyn-mCherry, AAV8-EF1α-fDIO-mCherry, AAV8-EF1α-NphR3.0-eYFP, AAVDJ-EF1α-DIO-eYFP, AAVDJ-EF1-ChETA-eYFP, AAVDJ-hSyn-ChR2-eYFP, AAVDJ-EF1α-DIO-tdTomato, AAVDJ-CAMKII-GCaMP6f, AAVDJ-EF1α-DIO-GCaMP6f, AAVDJ-EF1α-DIO-NpHR3.0-eYFP, AAVDJ-EF1α-DIO-ChR2-eYFP, AAVDJ-EF1α-DIO-eYFP, AAVDJ-CMV-DIO-EGFP and AAV-DJ-hSyn-FRT-mGFP-2A-Synaptophysin-mRuby44. AAV titers ranged from 1 X 1012 to 2 X 1013 gc/ml. CAV2-Cre45 was purchased from E. Kremer (Institut de Génétique Moléculaire de Montpellier, France). For monosynaptic tracing, synapsin-driven lentiviral NLS-GFP-Cre, AAV-CAG-FLEx-TC, AAV-CAG-FLEx-G and rabies virus were obtained from Janelia Research Campus. The rabies virus expressing Flp (SIR-Flp)15 was generously provided by Kevin Beier (University of California, Irvine). Behavioural experiments involving manipulations of cell bodies were conducted 3-4 weeks following virus injections. The incubation time was 6-8 weeks for experiments involving manipulation of axon terminals.

For optogenetic behavioural experiments, optic fibres were implanted above the MS (coordinates: 0.65 AP, 0 ML, 3.5 DV) and dCA2 (−1.6 AP, −1.6 ML, 1.2 DV). Optical implants were self-manufactured using 1.25 mm diameter multimode ceramic ferrules (ThorLab), 200 μm fibre optic cable with numerical aperture (NA) 0.39 (ThorLabSA) and blue dye epoxy (Fibre Instrument Sales). For securing the optical implants to the skull, miniature screws (thread size 00–90 × 1/16, Antrin Miniature Specialties) and light-cured dental adhesive cement (Geristore A&B paste, DenMat) were applied. For drug infusions, a 26-gauge guide cannula (Plastics One) was first implanted bilaterally or unilaterally for midline structures into the brain region being studied. For dCA2 drug injection, the guide cannula was 0.9 mm in length from the cannula base and the infusers (essentially needles that fit into the guide cannula) for injections were 33-gauge and 2.4 mm in length. The cannulas for implantation above the vCA1 (coordinates: −3.16 AP, 3.1 ML, 4 DV) were 2.8 mm in length and the infusers were 4.3 mm. The cannula implants for MS were 3.2 mm in length and the infusers were 4.7 mm. Based on visual histological analysis and blinded to the subject’s experimental results, a small percentage of subjects (~5% of >300 mice) were excluded from behavioural analysis according to the following criteria: 1) off-target transgene expression (substantial somatic expression of fluorophore outside the region of interest by visual inspection), 2) weak transgene expression or 3) inaccurate implant placement.

Intraperitoneal injection and cannula infusion of drugs

4-hydroxytamoxifen (4-OHT, Sigma H6278-50MG) was delivered via intraperitoneal (IP) injection at 50 mg/kg 45 min prior to behavioural experiments. Clozapine N-oxide (CNO, Tocris Biosciences 4936) was also given via IP injection at a concentration of 10 mg/kg 45 min prior to behavioural assays. For cannula microinjection experiments, 500 pg of CNO was infused in a total volume of 200 nl per side at a speed of 200 nl/min through an injector cannula using a microinfusion pump (Harvard Apparatus). The following drugs were also administered via microinjections at the same speed in a total volume of 400 nl – 2 μl: AP-5 (Tocris Biosciences 0106, 2.4 μg), a mixture of mecamylamine hydrochloride (Tocris Biosciences 2843, 2 μg) and scopolamine hydrobromide (Tocris Biosciences 1414, 3 μg), L-368,899 hydrochloride (OxtR-A, Tocris Biosciences 2641, 0.25 μg), methiothepin mesylate salt (Millipore Sigma M149, 0.2 μg), methysergide maleate (Tocris Biosciences 1064, 0.5 μg), NAD-229 (Tocris Biosciences 3282, 3.5 μg), NAS-181 (Tocris Biosciences 1413, 2 μg), CP93129 dihydrochloride (Tocris Biosciences 1032, 0.5 μg). Injection infusers were removed 2 min after completion of the fusion and mice were returned to their home cages for 30 min before behavioural assays. NAS-181 (10 mg/kg) was administered IP 30 min prior to fibre photometry experiments.

TRAP2 behavioural experiments

Mice were habituated for two days to IP injections (saline, 45 min prior) prior to placement in empty behavioural chambers (30 min, 23 cm length × 23 cm height × 25 cm width). On day three, female TRAP2;Ai14 mice (4-6 weeks old) were injected with 4-OHT (50 mg/kg). 45 min later, mice were divided into three groups. Group 1 was placed in behavioural chambers with a novel female TRAP2;Ai14 mouse. Group 2 was placed in the chambers with an inanimate object (e.g. a Lego toy as shown in Fig. 1a) and group 3 was placed into an empty chamber. After 4 h mice were returned to their home cages and 10 days later perfused with 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA) in PBS. The brains were placed into 4% PFA overnight and then cut on a vibratome into 60 μm slices. Images were acquired with an Olympus automated VS120 slide scanner (10X objective) and overlaid with images from Allen Brain Atlas for blinded manual counting of tdTomato-positive cells in specified brain regions.

Optogenetic stimulation

Mice were habituated for two days in the behavioural chambers with diodes (in off state) connected to their optic fibres. For photostimulation of NpHR3.0, optical implants were connected to a 532 nm laser diode (Shanghai Dream Lasers Technology Co, Ltd) via a FC/PC adaptor and a fibre optic rotary joint (Doric Lenses). For photostimulation of ChR2, ferrules were connected to a 473 nm laser diode (OEM Laser Systems). A Master-8 pulse stimulator (A.M.P.I.) was used to control the laser, which was adjusted to ~8 mW for somatic stimulation and ~15 mW for axon terminal stimulation using a digital power meter console (ThorLabs). To avoid tissue overheating when stimulating NpHR3.0, a light on off cycle of 8 s on and 2 s off was applied. For ChR2 stimulation, 5 ms light pulses at 1 Hz were applied.

Three-chamber behavioural tests

Test mice were habituated to IP injections or to the fibre optic patch cord, depending on the experiment, as described above. They were also habituated to the three-chamber apparatus, which contained two empty upside-down metal grid pencil cups (10 cm in diameter) in the outer two chambers, for 5 min on two consecutive days. The three-chamber apparatus (72 cm length × 23 cm width × 25 cm height) was constructed of 0.635-cm-thick sheets of clear extruded acrylic for walls, and Komatex for floors (white) and barrier walls (black; TAP Plastics), with two outer chambers (28 cm length × 23 cm width) connected by a centre chamber (16 cm length × 23 cm width). The chambers were divided by translucent acrylic walls with 6.5-cm-diameter holes, 1 cm from the chamber floor. These dividers were removed during optogenetic experiments to allow subject mice to move freely with their patch cords attached. Mice placed under the cup, were also habituated to the cup by spending 5 min under the cup on two consecutive days. On test day, subjects were placed in the middle chamber for 1 min, at which time the dividers were lifted, and subjects were allowed to explore the other two chambers freely for 10 min. One chamber contained an inanimate object, and the other chamber contained a novel mouse of the same sex. In the case of male mice, the mouse under the cup was a juvenile (3 – 5 weeks old). This session is referred to as a sociability test and was performed similarly to previously described methods8. The location of novel mice and objects was counterbalanced between sessions. After an interval of 10 min, 2 h or 24 h, subjects were placed back into the middle chamber for 1 min at which time the barriers were removed, and subjects allowed to freely explore for 5 min. During this social memory assay6,7,46 one chamber contained a mouse under a cup from the previous session (familiar mouse) and the other chamber contained a novel mouse under a cup. Again, the location of novel mouse and familiar mouse was counterbalanced between sessions.

For analysis, a video tracking system (BIOBSERVE) was used to automatically track the location of the subject mouse during its free exploration. The sociability discrimination score was calculated as: [(time in novel mouse chamber – time in object chamber)/(time in novel mouse chamber + time in object chamber)]. The social memory discrimination score was calculated as: [(time in novel mouse chamber – time in familiar mouse chamber)/(time in novel mouse chamber + time in familiar mouse chamber)]. The object memory test was conducted in the same manner as the social memory test, with the difference that an object was placed into one chamber and the other chamber remained empty in the first session and the familiarized object was placed in one chamber and a novel object in another chamber in the second session. The same cohort of mice were used for social and object memory tests with the sequence of the two tests counterbalanced within the same cohort.

Female mice were used for the initial experiments illustrated in Figs. 1, 2a-g and Extended Data Figs. 1-3 in order to avoid potential confounds of aggressive behaviour between the subject mice and object mice under the cups. Beginning with the experiments illustrated in Fig. 2h and Extended Data Fig. 4, both male and female mice were used in all behavioural tasks and there was no sex difference between their discrimination scores in the sociability assays or social memory tests in any experimental condition. For all remaining experiments, both male and female subjects were used in approximately equal numbers. Subjects were excluded from the analysis (<2% of total), if they spent the entirety of the assay in only one chamber or if their velocity was 0 for more than 50% of the assay.

Five-trial social memory test

This test was performed as previously described3,47. Female C57BL/6J subjects were individually housed for 7 days prior to behavioural assay. On the day of testing, the subjects were kept in their home cage and presented with a 10-week old novel C57BL/6J female mouse for 4 successive 1-minute trials, interspersed by 10 min intervals. On the final trial, another novel animal was presented. The trials were recorded, and the direct interaction time was scored manually and blinded to the experimental manipulation performed on the subject mouse.

Fear conditioning

Experiments were conducted in operant conditioning chambers (20 cm length × 24 cm width × 18 cm height, Med Associates Inc.) contained within sound-attenuating cubicles. The bottom of the chamber was composed of a metal grid, through which the foot shock was delivered. On day 1, mice received IP injections of CNO (10 mg/kg) 45 min prior to the experiment. Mice were individually placed into the fear conditioning chamber, which was illuminated by white light. After 2 min of free exploration, three foot shocks of 0.35 mA (lasting 2 s) were administered with an interval of 1 min. The subject remained in the chamber for 1 min before being placed in a separate cage. After all mice from one cage underwent fear conditioning, all subjects were returned to their home cage. After 24 h, mice were placed individually into the same chamber and percent time freezing was analysed using video tracking software (BIOBSERVE).

Conditioned place preference

The experimental protocol was similar to that previously described48. On day 1, mice were tested for baseline preference by placing them in an unbiased chamber containing two interchangeable halves of flooring made of two textures. The “grid’ floor was composed of 13 cm acrylic rods (0.15 cm diameter) mounted 0.5 cm apart in acrylic rails (0.5 cm thick, surrounding all sides). The “hole” floor was made out of perforated acrylic sheet with 8 mm round holes, 1 cm apart mounted on acrylic rails. Both floors (23 cm length × 14 cm width) were placed in the bottom of a black box (23 cm length × 23 cm width × 25 cm height). After 15 min of free exploration, mice were returned to their home cage. On day 2, mice were administered saline via IP injection 45 min prior to the conditioning. Each subject explored the black box with either two hole or two grid flooring sections for 15 min. After 4 h mice received IP injection of CNO (15 mg/kg) and were placed 45 min later into the chamber containing two sections of the alternate flooring for 15 min. On day 3, mice were placed into the chamber containing both flooring types and time spent on either type of flooring was scored via video tracking software (BIOBSERVE).

Immunohistochemistry

Mice were perfused with 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA) or 10% formalin, brains were removed and post-fixed overnight at 4°C. For procedures not involving immunohistochemistry (IHC) and in-situ hybridization, 60-75 μm coronal sections were prepared on a vibratome. For IHC, brains were transferred to 30% sucrose for 48 h to cryo-preserve, mounted on the cryostat mounting disk with Tissue-Tek® O.C.T. Compound (Sakura) on the tissue rack at −20°C and sectioned on a cryostat into 40 μm slices. Free-floating sections were processed for immunohistochemistry. After three 10 min washes in PBS on a shaker, the tissue was incubated with blocking solution (5% normal goat serum or donkey serum and 0.3% Triton X-100 in PBS) for 45 min and then incubated in primary antibodies overnight at 4°C on a shaker. Primary antibodies used were: rabbit polyclonal anti-CaMKII 1:200 (Thermo Fisher Scientific, PA5-38239), rabbit polyclonal anti-choline acetyltransferase (ChAT) 1:200 (Millipore Sigma, AB143), chicken anti-GFP 1:1000 (Aves labs, GFP-1020). After three washes of 10 min in PBS, secondary antibodies were added and incubated for 3 h at room temperature (RT) on a shaker. Secondary antibodies used were: goat anti-rabbit Alexa Fluor 546 1:500 (Thermo Fisher Scientific, A11035), donkey anti-chicken Alexa Fluor 488 (Jackson Immuno Research Labs, 703545155). After three more washes the slices were mounted with Fluoromount-G mounting medium (Southern Biotech) onto microscopy slides for visualization at 10X using an Olympus automated VS120 slide scanner (UPLSAPO 10X objective, NA 0.4, Fluorescence light source: X-Cite 120LED light engine, Filter cubes: DAPI, FITC, TRITC, Cy5, Wavelength: 430 – 741 nm, XM10 camera, bit depth of images: 14 bit) or Nikon A1 confocal microscope (CFI Plan Apochromat Lambda 10X objective, NA 0.45, Lasers: LU-N4/N4S 4-laser unit, 405 nm, 488 nm, 561 nm, 640 nm, Filter cubes: 450/50, 525/50, 600/50, 685/70, 700/75, Wavelength: 425 – 738 nm, Detector: A1-DU4-2 4 Detector Unit: 4 Multi-Alkali PMTs, bit depth of images: 12 bit).

In-situ hybridization

The experiment was carried out using the RNAscope® Multiplex Fluorescent v2 kit and was performed according to the ACD Bio manual49. Brains were harvested, fresh frozen on dry ice and stored at −80°C until sectioning on the cryostat. 15 μm slices were prepared and collected directly on Superfrost® plus microscopy slides. Following a 15 min fixation in prechilled 4% PFA, slices were rinsed twice in PBS and dehydrated gradually in higher concentrated ethanol in the order: 50%, 70%, two times 100% ethanol (5 min each). Slides were air-dried for 5 min and a barrier was drawn with a hydrophobic pen around the slices. Slides were then placed into a humidity control tray and incubated with ~ 5 drops of RNAscope hydrogen peroxide each for 10 min. After one wash with distilled water, slices were incubated with Protease IV for 30 min. Following two washes in PBS, slices underwent a hybridization step in a probe mix for 2 h at 40°C in a HyEZ™ Oven, at which time they were washed twice at RT in Wash buffer (50 x RNAscope Wash buffer diluted in distilled water). The following probes were used: RNAscope® Probe-EGFP-C1 (400281), Mm-Gad2-C2 (415071-C2), Mm-Slc17a6-C3 (319171-C3), Mm-Chat-C4 (408731-C4). Probes were warmed up to 40°C in a water bath for 10 min before use. 1 volume of C2, C3 and/or C4 were diluted in 50 volumes of C1. In the first amplification step, slides were incubated with ~6 drops of RNAscope Multiplex FL v2 Amp1 for 30 min at 40°C in the HyEZ™ Oven and subsequently washed twice with Wash buffer at RT. This amplification step is repeated with RNAscope Multiplex FL v2 Amp2 and RNAscope Multiplex FL v2 Amp3, whereby the incubation time in Amp3 was only 15 min. For the development of the horse raddish peroxidase (HRP) signal, slides were incubated with RNAscope Multiplex FL v2 HRP-C1 in the HyEZ™ Oven at 40°C for 15 min and rinsed twice in Wash buffer at RT. Afterwards, slides were incubated in 1:1500 diluted Opal™ 520, 570, 620 or 690 (depending on the probe-dye combination) for 30 min at 40°C and rinsed twice in Wash buffer. This step was repeated with HRP-C2, HRP-C3 or HRP-C4 and different Opal dyes. In the final mounting step, Fluoromount-G mounting medium was dripped onto the slides and cover slips were placed over the slices. Slides were imaged on a Leica SP8 confocal (20X HC PL APO IMM CORR CS2 objective, NA 0.75, Argon laser, 65 mW, Filter cubes: 458, 476, 488, 496, 514 nm, Wavelength: 470 – 670 nm, detector: 5 channel confocal detection: 3 Hybrid-GaAsP detectors for increased light sensitivity, 2 standard fluorescent photomultiplier tubes (PMT), Bit depth of images: 8 bit).

IHC and in-situ quantification

All samples were imaged with the same settings to allow comparison between samples and background subtraction and thresholds were set uniformly for all images. Based on a pixel-intensity threshold, images were binarized and merged with DAPI in ImageJ50. A signal in a given channel that colocalized with DAPI was manually identified as a positive cell. Counting was conducted via visual scanning and with a manual cell counter.

Ex vivo electrophysiology

For recording synaptic responses from dCA2 pyramidal neurons, 4-7 weeks after stereotactic injections of AAVDJ-EF1 ChETA-eYFP or AAVDJ-hSyn-Ch2-eYFP into the MS and AAVDJ-EF1α-DIO-tdTomato into dCA2, Amigo2-Cre mice were sacrificed and brains were placed into sucrose cutting solution containing (in mM): 228 sucrose, 26 NaHCO3, 11 glucose, 2.5 KCl, 1.2 NaH2PO4, 7 MgCl2 and 0.5 CaCl2 and sliced on a Leica vibratome. Coronal MS and hippocampal slices (200-250 μm) were transferred into artificial cerebrospinal fluid (ACSF) containing (in mM): 119 NaCl, 26 NaHCO3, 11 glucose, 2.5 KCl, 1.2 NaH2PO4, 1.3 MgCl2 and 2.5 CaCl2 (osmolarity 289 – 295) for recovery at 32°C for 30 min and then further 60 min of equilibration at RT. To evaluate the accuracy of injections, MS slices were placed in a recording chamber perfused with 28-30°C ACSF equilibrated with 95% O2 and 5% CO2 and visualized with a 40X water-immersion objective on an upright fluorescent microscope (BX51WI; Olympus) equipped with infrared-differential interference contrast video microscopy and epifluorescence (Olympus). If MS neurons expressed eYFP, the hippocampal slices from that animal were then placed in the recording chamber for optical stimulation. Whole-cell voltage-clamp recordings were made with pipettes (3 – 4 MΩ) filled with (in mM): 140 CsMeSO4, 8 NaCl, 10 HEPES, 0.25 EGTA, 2 Mg2ATP, 0.3 Na3GTP, 0.1 spermine, 7 phosphocreatine (pH 7.29–7.36; osmolarity 290 – 295). Series and input resistance were continuously monitored with −4 mV, 70 ms pulse delivered through the recording pipette. Experiments were discarded if the series resistance varied by >15%. In some experiments CsMeSO4 was replaced with an equimolar concentration of KMeSO4 to perform current-clamp recordings or voltage-clamp recordings (for experiments in Extended Data Figure 8b).

Whole cell recordings were made from visually identified tdTomato labelled pyramidal neurons in the CA2 while 473 nm 5 ms light pulses from the Opto Duet Laser system (IkeCool Corporation) were delivered to the whole slice via a 40X water-immersion objective on an Olympus microscope (BX51WI) at 0.1 Hz, thereby stimulating ChETA or ChR2 in MS axons and/or terminals. For voltage-clamp recordings, baseline postsynaptic currents (PSCs) were recorded at −70 mV for 5 min at which time NBQX (10 μM) was added to the ACSF and PSCs recorded until their amplitudes were stable for 5 min. Then mecamylamine hydrochloride (5 μM) and in some cases scopolamine hydrobromide (5 μM) were added and PSCs assayed until stable for 5 min. The holding membrane potential was then adjusted to −50 mV and picrotoxin (50 uM) was applied to assess the contribution of GABA-ARs to the PSCs. In 40% of cells, the order in which drugs were applied was shuffled with no detectable differences in their effects. For calculation of AMPAR/NMDAR ratios, the ACSF contained picrotoxin, mecamylamine hydrocholoride and scopolamine hydrobromide. The peak amplitude of an averaged AMPAR EPSC (n=27-29 consecutive EPSCS measured at −70 mV) was divided by the amplitude of an averaged NMDAR EPSCs (n=27-29 measured at 50 ms after the onset of the EPSC at +40 mV). Paired pulse ratios (PPR) of averaged AMPAR EPSCs (n=17-24) were calculated using a 50 ms interstimulus interval. To assess the strength of MS inputs to CA2 neurons (Figure 3d) the peak amplitude was measured from an average of 10 consecutive EPSPs evoked at each light power. To elicit LTD at MS to dCA2 pyramidal neuron synapses in slices (Extended Data Figure 6c), we applied three hundred 5 ms light pulses at 1 Hz. Summary graphs were generated by averaging individual EPSC measurements in 1 min bins and expressing each point as a percentage of the average of a 5 min baseline EPSC.

MS neurons projecting to dCA2 were labelled with EGFP by injection of CAV-Cre into dCA2 and Cre-dependent EGFP in the MS. Evoked EPSCs and IPSCs were recorded from EGFP-positive MS cells with a bipolar stimulating electrode (fabricated from platinum/iridium wire) placed near the recording pipette at 0.1 and 0.05 Hz, respectively. IPSCs and spontaneous IPSCs (sIPSCs) were recorded at −70 mV in the presence of NBQX (10 μM) and D-AP5 (50 μM). EPSCs were recorded at - 70 mV in the presence of picrotoxin (50 μM). Summary graphs of the effects of CP93129 (Figure 4l, m) and experiments controlling for pipette solution on evoked PSCs stability over time (Extended Data Figure 8f, g) were generated by averaging the raw data in 1 min bins and expressing each point as a percentage of the averaged 3 min control .To increase IPSC signal to noise ratio, MS cells were chloride loaded with a pipette solution containing (in mM): 80 CsCl, 65 CsMeSO4, 8 NaCl, 10 HEPES, 0.25 EGTA, 2 Mg2ATP, 0.3 Na3GTP, 0.1 spermine, 7 phosphocreatine (pH 7.29–7.36; osmolarity 290 – 295). Cumulative probability graphs of sIPSCs were generated from a random sampling of 200 events before and in CP93129. The events for each experimental condition were arranged in ascending order and averaged. The resulting means and s.e.m.s were plotted against their cumulative probability in the data set. Intrinsic cell excitability was assessed with a series of incremental rectangular depolarizing current pulses (20 pA, 500 ms) injected into MS cells while recording in current-clamp. Drug-induced changes in action potential (AP) number were determined by the number of APs evoked in the presence of no applied holding current. In cells where CP93129 increased excitability a graph was constructed plotting AP number vs injected current. Peak currents induced by CP93129 (2 or 5 μM) were defined as those that could be clearly distinguished (>10 pA) as an agonist-induced inward or outward event from baseline. Recordings were made using a MultiClamp 700B amplifier (Molecular Devices), digitized at 10 kHz with the Digidata 1320A or 1440A data acquisition system (Molecular Devices), and analysed using Axograph or custom software written for Igor Pro (Wavemetrics). sIPSCs were analysed with MiniAnalysis (Synaptosoft).

Fibre photometry

Fibre photometry was performed as previously described9. AAVDJ-CaMKII-GCaMP6f or AAVDJ-EF1α-DIO-GCaMP6f was injected into the MS and CAV-Cre was injected into the dCA2. Fibre optic implants (Doric) were inserted and secured above the MS (0.65 AP, 0 ML, 3.5 DV). After 2-3 weeks, mice were habituated to the behavioural set-up for 10 min and tested one day later with simultaneous video and fibre photometry acquisition. On the test day, mice were placed in one chamber of the three-chamber apparatus, which was barricaded to create a small single chamber. Direct interaction with a novel mouse, a familiar mouse and a novel object was monitored for 5 min with an interval of 5 min. The presentation sequence of mouse and object was counterbalanced between subjects. Fibre photometry data was acquired using the Synapse software, which controls an RZ5P lock-in amplifier (Tucker-Davis Technologies). During acquisition GCaMP6f was excited by frequency-modulated 473- and 405-nm light-emitting diodes (Doric), to stimulate Ca2+-dependent and isosbestic emission, respectively. All optical signals were band-pass-filtered with a fluorescence mini cube (Doric), emission was measured with a femtowatt photoreceiver (2151; Newport), and signal was digitized at 6 kHz. MATLAB (MathWorks Inc.) was used for signal processing, including correction of motion artifact and fluorescent bleaching. Signals were debleached by fitting with a mono- or bi-exponential decay function, and the resulting fluorescence trace was z-scored. Videos were manually analysed and the time of every physical contact after approach from a distance of >2 cm between subject mouse and an object or mouse was determined. An average of 7 s non-overlapping epochs of fluorescence was used to construct peristimulus time histograms. The time of contact is defined as time = 0, positioned in between 3 s before and 4 s after contact time. For each subject, 2-7 contacts occurred per session and these were averaged to obtain the z-score graph for that subject. Peak z-scored fluorescence was the maximal z-score between 0 and 4 s.

Statistical methods and reproducibility

The experimenter was blinded to the virus injection the animals had received. All analyses were performed with the experimenter blinded to the manipulation that the subject had received. All data were tested for normality of sample distributions and when violated, non-parametric statistical tests were used. One-way ANOVA with Tukey’s multiple comparison post-hoc test was performed to assess significance for multiple group comparisons and two-way ANOVA with Tukey’s or Sidak’s multiple comparison post hoc test for multiple group comparisons across multiple time points. Two-tailed paired Student’s t-test was used for within group comparison of two treatments and unpaired used for comparison between two groups. When normal distributions were not assumed, Wilcoxon signed rank test was performed for within group comparisons of two treatments, Mann-Whitney for between group comparison, and Kruskal-Wallis with post-hoc Dunn’s test for multiple comparisons. N.S.=not significant. *P < 0.05; **P < 0.01; ***P < 0.001. In all figures, data are shown as mean ± SEM. Fig. 1b: 1 out of 12 mice, Fig. 1g: 1 out of 9 mice, Fig. 2h: 1 out of 15 mice, Fig. 4a: 1 out of 14 mice, Fig. 4e, h: 1 out of 5 mice, Fig. 5a: 1 out of 10 mice, Fig. 5d: 1 out of 11 mice, Fig. 5g: 1 out of 13 mice.

Extended Data

Extended Data Fig. 1∣. Medial septum cells show increased cFos after a social experience.

Extended Data Fig. 1∣

a, Schematic of experimental timeline, 4-OHT was administered via intraperitoneal injection. b, Quantification of tdTomato-positive cells in the median raphe (MR: F2,12=4.681, P=0.0314), paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus (PVH: F2,11=3.446, P=0.0689), nucleus of the diagonal band (NDB: F2,12=4.423, P=0.0364), medial septum (MS: F2,12=13.31, P=0.0009), lateral septum (LS: F2,12=4.14, P=0.0429) in the social (n=7), object (n=5) and control (n=3) conditions. Representative images of MS expressing tdTomato. Scale bar, 200 μm (right). c, Quantification of tdTomato-positive cells in the ventral CA1 (vCA1: F2,8=1.478, P=0.2842) in social (n=5), object (n=3) and control (n=3) conditions. d, Quantification of tdTomato-positive cells in the dorsal CA2 (dCA2: F2,9=5.367, P=0.0292) in social (n=5), object (n=4) and control (n=3) conditions. Representative images of dCA2 expressing tdTomato. Scale bar, 200 μm (right). e, Schematic of monosynaptic tracing experiment. f, Representative images of injection site in the dorsal hippocampus and presynaptic labelling in the MS (left), injection site in the ventral hippocampus and presynaptic labelling in the MS (right). Scale bar, 200 μm, n=3. g, Schematic of anterograde tracing experiment. h, Representative images of injection site in the MS (left) and axon terminals in the dorsal and ventral hippocampus (right). (bd) One-way ANOVA with Tukey’s post-hoc test, N.S.=not significant, *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001 (left). Scale bar, 200 μm, n=3. Error bars denote s.e.m.

Extended Data Fig. 2∣. Chemogenetic manipulation of the medial septum does not affect sociability and object memory.

Extended Data Fig. 2∣

a, Representative traces of subjects during 3-chamber social memory test. b, Duration in chamber with novel object (no) or novel mouse (nm) (left) and discrimination scores (right) (F2,40=0.2875, P=0.7517; mCh: n=19, hM4Di: n=12). c, Duration in chamber with familiar object (fo) or no (left) and discrimination scores (right) (F2,40=0.04521, P=0.9558; mCh: n=19, hM4Di: n=12). d, Schematic and representative image of MS injection site showing hM3Dq expression. n=10. Scale bar, 500 μm. e, Duration in chamber with fm or nm (left) and discrimination scores (right) (F2,27=6.689, P=0.0044, n=10). f, Duration with no or nm (left) and discrimination scores (right) (t9=0.2358, P=0.8189; n=10). g, Duration with familiar object (fo) or no (left) and discrimination scores (right) (F2,27=0.3243, P=0.9681; n=10). h, Schematic and representative image of hM4Di expression in the MS. n=8. Scale bar, 500 μm. i, Individual subjects from main figure 1j. (b, c, e, f, g) duration: two-tailed paired Student’s t-test. (b, c, e, g) discrimination scores: one-way ANOVA with Tukey’s post-hoc test. (f) discrimination scores: two-tailed paired Student’s t-test. N.S.=not significant, *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001. Error bars denote s.e.m.

Extended Data Fig. 3∣. Optogenetic medial septum cell body and chemogenetic terminal inhibition do not affect sociability and object memory.

Extended Data Fig. 3∣

a, Schematic of experimental set-up (left) and representative image of MS injection site showing NpHR expression (right). n=10. Scale bar, 500 μm. b, Duration in chamber with no or nm (left) and discrimination scores (right) (t18=0.9531, P=0.3532; n=10). c, Duration in chamber with fo or no (left) and discrimination scores (right) (t18=0.1348, P=0.8943; n=10). d, Schematic of experimental set-up (left) and representative images of MS injection sites showing hM4Di expression and cannula implant sites (right). n=13. Scale bar, 500 μm. e, Duration in chamber with no or nm (left) and discrimination scores (right) (t26=0.2453, P=0.8081; mCh: n=15, hM4Di n=13). f, Duration in chamber with fo or no (left) and discrimination scores (right) (t22=0.6383, P=0.5298; n=12). All data were assessed by two-tailed unpaired Student’s t-test. N.S.=not significant, *P < 0.05, ***P < 0.001. Error bars denote s.e.m.

Extended Data Fig. 4∣. Inhibition of dCA2-projecting medial septum neurons has no effect on sociability, object memory, contextual fear memory and conditioned place preference.

Extended Data Fig. 4∣

a, Schematic of experimental set-up (left). Duration in chamber with fm or nm (middle) and discrimination scores (right) (hM4Di: P=0.5135; female mCh: n=11, hM4Di n=10, male mCh: n=4, hM4Di: n=5). b, Duration in chamber with no or nm (left) and discrimination scores (right) (F2,42=0.4013, P=0.6721; n=15, hM4Di+saline: n=14). c, Duration with fo or no (left) and discrimination scores (right) (t27=0.1384, P=0.891; mCh: n=15, hM4Di n=14). d, Schematic of experimental set-up (top) and quantification of the percent freezing during shock and recall (bottom left) and fold increase in freezing time (bottom right) (P=0.2671; n=15). e, Schematic of experimental set-up (top), quantification of the percent time spent on each surface after CNO and saline pairing (bottom left) and discrimination scores (bottom right) (t28=0.2619, P=0.7953; n=15). f, Schematic of virus injection, left. Representative images of MS injection site as well as the labelled axon in the dCA2, ventral CA1 (vCA1) and supramammillary nucleus (SUM), right (n = 3, scale bars = 500 μm above and 200 μm below; arrows point to mRuby puncta. (a) two-tailed Mann-Whitney test. (b, c) duration: two-tailed paired Student’s t-test. (b) discrimination score: one-way ANOVA with Tukey’s post-hoc test (c, e) discrimination scores: two-tailed unpaired Student’s t-test. (d) % freezing: Kruskal-Wallis with post-hoc Dunn’s test, fold-increase in freezing: two-tailed Mann-Whitney test. (e) % time on one side: one-way ANOVA with Tukey’s post-hoc test. N.S.=not significant. *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001. Error bars denote s.e.m.

Extended Data Fig. 5∣. dCA2-projecting medial septum cells are primarily CHAT and CaMKII positive.

Extended Data Fig. 5∣

a, Schematic of monosynaptic rabies tracing set-up in Amigo2-Cre mice (left) and representative images of injection site in the dCA2 as well as GFP-positive cells in the MS (right). Scale bar, 200 μm, n=3. b, Representative images of in-situ hybridization. Scale bar, 200 μm (upper left panel), 40 μm. c, Quantification of in-situ hybridization, n=3 subjects. d, Quantification of same in-situ hybridization data for percent of GFP-positive cells colocalizing with CHAT only, SLC17A6 only, GAD2 only or double-positive for SLC17A6 and CHAT, GAD2 and CHAT (right), n=3 subjects. e, Quantification of colocalization with GFP-positive cells via immunohistochemistry (IHC) using either CHAT or CaMKII antibodies (left) and representative images (right, scale bar, 40 μm, n=3). f, Schematic of experimental set-up and representative image of dCA2 infusion site filled with ink. g, Duration in chamber with no or nm (left) and discrimination scores (right) (F2,36=1.702, P=0.1967; n=13). h, Duration in chamber with fo or no (left) and discrimination scores (right) (F2,36=3.259, P=0.05; n=13). (g, h) duration: two-tailed paired Student’s t-test, discrimination scores: one-way ANOVA with Tukey’s post-hoc test. N.S.=not significant, *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001. Error bars denote s.e.m.

Extended Data Fig. 6∣. Medial septum synapses onto dCA2 pyramidal neurons are primarily glutamatergic and can express long-term depression ex vivo.

Extended Data Fig. 6∣

a, Top, pie charts of percentage of cells, where the ChETA (n=20/9) and ChR2 (n=22/6) induced PSCs were >60% reduced by NBQX (10 μM), picrotoxin (50 μM) or Mecamylamine (5 μM). Below, summary time course of ChETA-evoked PSCs from tdTomato-positive dCA2 pyramidal neurons, which were completely blocked (>90%) by bath-application of NBQX (n=13). b, Representative traces of PSCs evoked by paired-pulse MS input activation in slices prepared from Amigo2-Cre mice exposed to a novel object or novel mouse (social). Scale bars, 50 pA, 100 ms. Quantification of paired-pulse ratios for mice (below) interacting with a novel object (n=17/3, cells/animals) or novel mouse (n=24/4) (t39=1.589, P=0.1201), two-tailed unpaired Student’s t-test. N.S.=not significant. c, Representative EPSCs from tdTomato-positive dCA2 pyramidal neurons before and after LTD induction protocol. Scale bars, 25 pA, 100 ms. Summary time-course of EPSCs with and without listed receptor antagonists in bath. With inhibitors: n=8/4, without inhibitors: n=6/3. d, Representative image of MS ChR2 injection site (above) and optical fibre implant site in the dCA2 (below). n=10. Scale bars, 500 μm. Error bars denote s.e.m.

Extended Data Fig. 7∣. Effects of in vivo optogenetic low frequency stimulation (1 Hz for 5 or 10 min) to induce LTD at medial septum to dCA2 synapses on sociability, social memory and object memory.

Extended Data Fig. 7∣

a, b, Duration in chambers with fm or nm (left) and discrimination scores (right), LTD stimulation was performed prior to sociability test, (a: t13=2.898, P=0.0125; off: n=15, 1 Hz: n=14. b: t9=0.9806, P=0.3524; n=10). c, d, Duration in chamber with no or nm, left and discrimination scores (right) in ChR2 and eYFP mice (c: t9=0.3201, P=0.7563; n=10, d: t9=0.6328, P=0.5426; n=10). e, Duration with fo or no (left) and discrimination scores (right) (t9=0.09968, P=0.9228; n=10) in ChR2 mice. f, g, Duration in chamber with no or nm, left and discrimination scores (right) in ChR2 and eYFP mice (f: t13=0.7717, P=0.4540; n=14, g: t9=1.208, P=0.2577; n=10). h, Duration with fo or no (left) and discrimination scores (right) (t13=0.6186, P=0.5469; n=14) in ChR2 mice. Two-tailed paired Student’s t-test was performed on all data. N.S.=not significant, *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001. Error bars denote s.e.m.

Extended Data Fig. 8∣. Effects of CP93129 on dCA2-projecting medial septum neurons.

Extended Data Fig. 8∣

a, Sociability is not affected by MS infusion of indicated drugs. Duration in chamber with no or nm (left) and discrimination scores (right) (F5,78=0.3144, P=0.9029; n=14). b, Pie chart summarizing postsynaptic responses of EGFP-positive cells in CP93129 (2 or 5 μM, n=42). c, Pie chart illustrating action potential (AP) firing changes in CP93129 (left, n=15). Representative traces −/+ CP93129 (right). Vm refers to unclamped resting membrane potential. Scale bars, 25 mV, 100 ms. d, Effects of CP93129 on MS neurons projecting to dCA2. Number of cells per current are indicated in parentheses (t25=3.461, P=0.002; n=26) (NOCHG, no change). e, Quantification of action potential firing as function of current injection for dCA2-projecting MS neurons with increased firing in CP93129 (P=0.001; n=10). f, Summary time course of IPSC responses in AP5 and NBQX with a pipette solution comprising CsMeSO4+CsCl. Representative traces above shown at indicated time points. Scale bars, 100 pA, 50 ms, n=5/2. g, Summary time course of EPSC responses in picrotoxin while recording with a pipette solution comprising CsMeSO4. Representative traces above shown at indicated time points. Scale bars, 50 pA, 10 ms, n=5/3. h, Amplitudes of IPSCs (t4=0.0285, P=0.979, n=5) and EPSCs (t4=0.593, P=0.585, n=5) averaged over the last 5 min of recording as a percentage of the first 3 min of baseline recording. i, j, Corresponding input resistance measurements for cells shown in f and g. k, RN of IPSC (t4=0.058, P=0.957, n=5) and EPSC (t4=−1.409, P=0.232, n=5) recordings averaged over the last 5 min of recording as a percentage of the first 3 min of baseline recording. I, Recordings of spontaneous inhibitory postsynaptic currents (sIPSCs) from EGFP-positive cells before and after bath-application of CP93129 (CP). Cumulative probability plot of sIPSC amplitudes with representative traces (above, scale bars, 15 pA, 15 ms, n=9/6 (cells/ animals)). Bar graph shows effect of CP93129 on mean IPSC amplitude. m, Cumulative probability plot of sIPSC inter-event intervals with representative traces (above, scale bars, 50 pA, 0.5 s, n=9/6). Bar graph shows effect of CP93129 on mean IPSC frequency. (a) duration: two-tailed paired Student’s t-test, discrimination scores: one-way ANOVA with Tukey’s post-hoc test, (d, h, k) two-tailed paired Student’s t-test. (e) Repeated measures two-way ANOVA with Sidak’s multiple comparison post-hoc test. (I, m) two-tailed Wilcoxon signed rank test. N.S.=not significant, *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001. Error bars and shading denote s.e.m.

Extended Data Fig. 9∣. Optogenetic inhibition or excitation of 5-HT inputs in the medial septum does not alter sociability and object memory.

Extended Data Fig. 9∣

Extended Data Fig. 9∣

a, Schematic of virus injection (left) and representative images of the injection site in the median raphe (MR) on the left and the EGFP-positive axons in the MS (right). n = 5. b, Schematic of virus injections to perform TRIO (left) and representative images of the injection site in the MS and the GFP-positive cells in the MR (right). n = 3. Scale bars = 500 μm. c, d, Duration in chamber with no or nm (left) and discrimination scores (right) (c: t9=1.834; P=0.0998; n=10. d: P=0.3750; n=10). e, f, Duration in chamber with fo or no (left) and discrimination scores (right) (e: t9=0.2418, P=0.8143; n=10. f: t8=0.6029, P=0.5632, n=9). g, h, Duration in chamber with no or nm (left) and discrimination scores (right) (g: t10=0.9294, P=0.3746; n=11. h: t9=1.518, P=0.1633; n=10). i, Duration in chamber with no or nm (left) and discrimination scores (right) (t11=1.52, P=0.152; saline: n=12, CP93129: n=14). (d) eYFP on duration and discrimination score: two-tailed Wilcoxon signed rank test. All other data in this figure were analysed by two-tailed paired Student’s t-test. N.S.=not significant, *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001. Error bars denote s.e.m.

Extended Data Fig. 10. Model illustrating 5-HT action on dCA2-projecting MS neurons.

Extended Data Fig. 10.

During a novel social encounter, 5-HT diffuses away from its release sites to bind to 5-HT1bRs on the terminals of MS GABAergic interneurons thereby inhibiting GABA release onto the dCA2-projecting glutamatergic MS neurons. 5-HT also can bind to 5-HT1bRs on presynaptic glutamatergic terminals to inhibit glutamate release but a smaller proportion of glutamatergic inputs express 5-HT1bRs. The net effect of 5-HT is to reduce local inhibition of dCA2-projecting MS neurons to a greater extent than its reduction of excitatory drive, thereby resulting in increased activity in these neurons. The question mark indicates that there may also be a direct effect of 5-HT on dCA2-projecting MS neurons. The inset on the right depicts the circuitry investigated in this study from the median raphe (MR) to the medial septum (MS) to dorsal CA2 (dCA2).

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by philanthropic funds generously donated to the Nancy Pritzker Laboratory at Stanford University. X.W. was supported by a NIH K99 Career Development Award (MH122697). K.T.B. was supported by NIH grant DP2 AG067666. B.D.H. was supported by a NIH K08 Career Development Award (MH110610). We thank B.S. Bentzley for providing assistance with the fear conditioning experiments; P.A. Neumann and S.R. Golf for kindly providing mouse breeding pairs; and members of the Malenka lab for helpful discussions. Extended Data Fig. 10 schematic by Sci Stories, LLC.

Footnotes

Code Availability

Code used for data processing and analysis is available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request. The MATLAB code used for analyses of fibre photometry data is provided as a supplementary file.

Competing interests All protocols used during this study are freely available for non-profit use from the corresponding author upon reasonable request. R.C.M. is on the scientific advisory boards of MapLight Therapeutics and MindMed.

Data availability

The datasets generated and analysed during this study are included in this published article and its supplementary information files. Any additional data generated during and/or analysed during this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

References

  • 1.McGraw LA & Young LJ The prairie vole: an emerging model organism for understanding the social brain. Trends Neurosci 33, 103–109, doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2009.11.006 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Okuyama T Social memory engram in the hippocampus. Neurosci Res 129, 17–23, doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2017.05.007 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Hitti FL & Siegelbaum SA The hippocampal CA2 region is essential for social memory. Nature 508, 88–92, doi: 10.1038/nature13028 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Leroy F, Brann DH, Meira T & Siegelbaum SA Input-Timing-Dependent Plasticity in the Hippocampal CA2 Region and Its Potential Role in Social Memory. Neuron 95, 1089–1102 e1085, doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.07.036 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Meira T et al. A hippocampal circuit linking dorsal CA2 to ventral CA1 critical for social memory dynamics. Nat Commun 9, 4163, doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-06501-w (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Okuyama T, Kitamura T, Roy DS, Itohara S & Tonegawa S Ventral CA1 neurons store social memory. Science 353, 1536–1541, doi: 10.1126/science.aaf7003 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Phillips ML, Robinson HA & Pozzo-Miller L Ventral hippocampal projections to the medial prefrontal cortex regulate social memory. Elife 8, doi: 10.7554/eLife.44182 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Walsh JJ et al. 5-HT release in nucleus accumbens rescues social deficits in mouse autism model. Nature 560, 589–594, doi: 10.1038/s41586-018-0416-4 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Heifets BD et al. Distinct neural mechanisms for the prosocial and rewarding properties of MDMA. Sci Transl Med 11, doi: 10.1126/scitranslmed.aaw6435 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Chiang MC, Huang AJY, Wintzer ME, Ohshima T & McHugh TJ A role for CA3 in social recognition memory. Behav Brain Res 354, 22–30, doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2018.01.019 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Leroy F et al. A circuit from hippocampal CA2 to lateral septum disinhibits social aggression. Nature 564, 213–218, doi: 10.1038/s41586-018-0772-0 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Chandler JP & Crutcher KA The septohippocampal projection in the rat: an electron microscopic horseradish peroxidase study. Neuroscience 10, 685–696 (1983). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Buzsaki G Theta oscillations in the hippocampus. Neuron 33, 325–340, doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00586-x (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Kaifosh P, Lovett-Barron M, Turi GF, Reardon TR & Losonczy A Septo-hippocampal GABAergic signaling across multiple modalities in awake mice. Nat Neurosci 16, 1182–1184, doi: 10.1038/nn.3482 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Ciabatti E, Gonzalez-Rueda A, Mariotti L, Morgese F & Tripodi M Life-Long Genetic and Functional Access to Neural Circuits Using Self-Inactivating Rabies Virus. Cell 170, 382–392 e314, doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2017.06.014 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Sans-Dublanc A et al. Septal GABAergic inputs to CA1 govern contextual memory retrieval. Sci Adv 6, doi: 10.1126/sciadv.aba5003 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Zhang GW et al. Transforming Sensory Cues into Aversive Emotion via Septal-Habenular Pathway. Neuron 99, 1016–1028 e1015, doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.07.023 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Zhang GW et al. A Non-canonical Reticular-Limbic Central Auditory Pathway via Medial Septum Contributes to Fear Conditioning. Neuron 97, 406–417 e404, doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.12.010 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Papouin T, Dunphy JM, Tolman M, Dineley KT & Haydon PG Septal Cholinergic Neuromodulation Tunes the Astrocyte-Dependent Gating of Hippocampal NMDA Receptors to Wakefulness. Neuron 94, 840–854 e847, doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.04.021 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Meng X et al. Manipulations of MeCP2 in glutamatergic neurons highlight their contributions to Rett and other neurological disorders. Elife 5, doi: 10.7554/eLife.14199 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Kauer JA & Malenka RC Synaptic plasticity and addiction. Nat Rev Neurosci 8, 844–858, doi: 10.1038/nrn2234 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Sharma K et al. Sexually dimorphic oxytocin receptor-expressing neurons in the preoptic area of the mouse brain. PLoS One 14, e0219784, doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0219784 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Cloez-Tayarani I et al. Autoradiographic characterization of [3H]-5-HT-moduline binding sites in rodent brain and their relationship to 5-HT1B receptors. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 94, 9899–9904, doi: 10.1073/pnas.94.18.9899 (1997). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Elvander-Tottie E, Eriksson TM, Sandin J & Ogren SO 5-HT(1A) and NMDA receptors interact in the rat medial septum and modulate hippocampal-dependent spatial learning. Hippocampus 19, 1187–1198, doi: 10.1002/hipo.20596 (2009). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Sari Y Serotonin1B receptors: from protein to physiological function and behavior. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 28, 565–582, doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.08.008 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Leranth C & Vertes RP Median raphe serotonergic innervation of medial septum/diagonal band of broca (MSDB) parvalbumin-containing neurons: possible involvement of the MSDB in the desynchronization of the hippocampal EEG. J Comp Neurol 410, 586–598 (1999). [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Schwarz LA et al. Viral-genetic tracing of the input-output organization of a central noradrenaline circuit. Nature 524, 88–92, doi: 10.1038/nature14600 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Jiang M et al. Conditional ablation of neuroligin-1 in CA1 pyramidal neurons blocks LTP by a cell-autonomous NMDA receptor-independent mechanism. Mol Psychiatry 22, 375–383, doi: 10.1038/mp.2016.80 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Bariselli S et al. Role of VTA dopamine neurons and neuroligin 3 in sociability traits related to nonfamiliar conspecific interaction. Nat Commun 9, 3173, doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-05382-3 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Etherton M et al. Autism-linked neuroligin-3 R451C mutation differentially alters hippocampal and cortical synaptic function. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 108, 13764–13769, doi: 10.1073/pnas.1111093108 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Wu X et al. Neuroligin-1 Signaling Controls LTP and NMDA Receptors by Distinct Molecular Pathways. Neuron 102, 621–635 e623, doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2019.02.013 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Bunin MA & Wightman RM Quantitative evaluation of 5-hydroxytryptamine (serotonin) neuronal release and uptake: an investigation of extrasynaptic transmission. J Neurosci 18, 4854–4860 (1998). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Jennings KA A comparison of the subsecond dynamics of neurotransmission of dopamine and serotonin. ACS Chem Neurosci 4, 704–714, doi: 10.1021/cn4000605 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Liu C, Goel P & Kaeser PS Spatial and temporal scales of dopamine transmission. Nat Rev Neurosci, doi: 10.1038/s41583-021-00455-7 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Nelson RJ & Trainor BC Neural mechanisms of aggression. Nat Rev Neurosci 8, 536–546, doi: 10.1038/nrn2174 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Nautiyal KM et al. Distinct Circuits Underlie the Effects of 5-HT1B Receptors on Aggression and Impulsivity. Neuron 86, 813–826, doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.03.041 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Okaty BW et al. Multi-Scale Molecular Deconstruction of the Serotonin Neuron System. Neuron 88, 774–791, doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.10.007 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Jensen P et al. Redefining the serotonergic system by genetic lineage. Nat Neurosci 11, 417–419, doi: 10.1038/nn2050 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Baskin BM, Mai JJ, Dymecki SM & Kantak KM Cocaine reward and memory after chemogenetic inhibition of distinct serotonin neuron subtypes in mice. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 237, 2633–2648, doi: 10.1007/s00213-020-05560-6 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Senft RA, Freret ME, Sturrock N & Dymecki SM Neurochemically and Hodologically Distinct Ascending VGLUT3 versus Serotonin Subsystems Comprise the r2-Pet1 Median Raphe. J Neurosci 41, 2581–2600, doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1667-20.2021 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Ferguson JN et al. Social amnesia in mice lacking the oxytocin gene. Nat Genet 25, 284–288, doi: 10.1038/77040 (2000). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Raam T, McAvoy KM, Besnard A, Veenema AH & Sahay A Hippocampal oxytocin receptors are necessary for discrimination of social stimuli. Nat Commun 8, 2001, doi: 10.1038/s41467-017-02173-0 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Allen WE et al. Thirst-associated preoptic neurons encode an aversive motivational drive. Science 357, 1149–1155, doi: 10.1126/science.aan6747 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Beier KT et al. Circuit Architecture of VTA Dopamine Neurons Revealed by Systematic Input-Output Mapping. Cell 162, 622–634, doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2015.07.015 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Kremer EJ, Boutin S, Chillon M & Danos O Canine adenovirus vectors: an alternative for adenovirus-mediated gene transfer. J Virol 74, 505–512, doi: 10.1128/jvi.74.1.505-512.2000 (2000). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Hung LW et al. Gating of social reward by oxytocin in the ventral tegmental area. Science 357, 1406–1411, doi: 10.1126/science.aan4994 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Ferguson JN, Young LJ & Insel TR The neuroendocrine basis of social recognition. Front Neuroendocrinol 23, 200–224, doi: 10.1006/frne.2002.0229 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Cunningham CL, Gremel CM & Groblewski PA Drug-induced conditioned place preference and aversion in mice. Nat Protoc 1, 1662–1670, doi: 10.1038/nprot.2006.279 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Wang F et al. RNAscope: a novel in situ RNA analysis platform for formalin-fixed, paraffin-embedded tissues. J Mol Diagn 14, 22–29, doi: 10.1016/j.jmoldx.2011.08.002 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Cardozo Pinto DF et al. Characterization of transgenic mouse models targeting neuromodulatory systems reveals organizational principles of the dorsal raphe. Nat Commun 10, 4633, doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-12392-2 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated and analysed during this study are included in this published article and its supplementary information files. Any additional data generated during and/or analysed during this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

RESOURCES