Abstract
Understanding the affective lives of animals has been a long-standing challenge in science. Recent technological progress in infrared thermal imaging has enabled researchers to monitor animals' physiological states in real-time when exposed to ecologically relevant situations, such as feeding in the company of others. During social feeding, an individual's physiological states are likely to vary with the nature of the resource and perceptions of competition. Previous findings in chimpanzees have indicated that events perceived as competitive cause decreases in nasal temperatures, whereas the opposite was observed for cooperative interactions. Here, we tested how food resources and audience structure impacted on how social feeding events were perceived by wild chimpanzees. Overall, we found that nasal temperatures were lower when meat was consumed as compared to figs, consistent with the idea that social feeding on more contested resources is perceived as more dangerous and stressful. Nasal temperatures were significant affected by interactions between food type and audience composition, in particular the number of males, their dominance status, and their social bond status relative to the subject, while no effects for the presence of females were observed. Our findings suggest that male chimpanzees closely monitor and assess their social environment during competitive situations, and that infrared imaging provides an important complement to access psychological processes beyond observable social behaviours.
This article is part of the theme issue ‘Cognition, communication and social bonds in primates’.
Keywords: Pan troglodytes, social cognition, audience effects, skin temperature, social ecology
1. Introduction
How are the affective lives of chimpanzees and other primates shaped by socio-ecological context? In recent decades, much has been learned about what information primates process when navigating their social environment [1], including information on others' physical condition [2,3], rank [4,5], their own as well as others’ social relationships [6–8], and others' knowledge states [9–12]. This understanding has been developed from studies using both systematic observations (e.g. [7]) and experiments (e.g. [3]), typically based on observable behavioural markers, such as gaze (e.g. [10,13]), vocalization (e.g. [9,13–15]) or movement (e.g. [8]).
Often however, primates and other animals do not show observable responses to key social events, despite evidence that events are perceived, processed and often have future behavioural implications [1,16]. Primates continuously and closely monitor their social environment [17]. Affective responses play a crucial role in fostering social relationships (e.g. for emotional book-keeping: [18]; for the neuropeptide theory of social bonding: [19]), and in navigating social challenges, such as aggression or the threat thereof [8]. However, these important affective responses can be challenging to investigate, as they do not always include direct, easily observable, behavioural correlates.
Nevertheless, the role affective processes play in primate social lives has been gradually uncovered using methods such as hormonal profiling (e.g. oxytocin metabolites [20–23], corticosteroids [22,24–27] or pupillary dilatation [28]).
More recently, infrared thermography, a highly versatile technique increasingly used with both captive [16,29–36] and wild primates [37–40], has become part of the methodological toolkit [41]. Thermal imaging is particularly useful as it can offer a near real-time snapshot of the affective life of animals, providing an indirect measure of arousal [33,41]. More precisely, thermal imaging is a contact-free method that can assess the surface temperature of bodies through their wavelength and electromagnetic radiation [41–43]. Recent technological progress has made this technique sensitive to changes in peripheral blood flow [43,44], although other physiological processes, such as metabolic heat production, can also affect skin surface temperature [41]. Changes in blood flow are part of adaptive response mechanisms, measurable in temperature changes in the peripheral body parts, such as the perinasal area [30] or the tail [45]. Previous studies have demonstrated that situations likely to be perceived as stressful by an animal can lead to significant drops in nasal temperatures [30,36,39,44]. The general assumption is that changes in peripheral blood flow reflect changes in activity of the autonomic nervous system and that these changes can be used as a way to assess an animal's ‘internal perspective’ (sensu [1,16]). For example, they have been used to show that chimpanzees respond to relevant social events, such as physical aggression seen in others [30,39], and/or participation in prosocial events, such as mutual grooming [39].
In the current study, we used thermal imaging to evaluate arousal levels in chimpanzees engaged in feeding while in the company of others, with the intention of uncovering whether individuals show otherwise non-observable responses to the presence of others, depending on the type of resource at stake, and the nature of their social relationship to those around them. Social monitoring is a putative function for well-documented audience effects [46,47] in which the presence of others impacts both cognitive and affective functions [48]. Audience effects have received substantial attention, both in the human and non-human animal literature (humans [48,49], primates [50–52], other species [53–55]). In humans, the presence of an audience considerably impacts our cognitive processes, such that the mere presence of others affects our attentional resources [48,56]. Audience effects also occur in non-human animals, and one hypothesis is that audiences which increase the risk of aggression or interference with one's own activities lead to increased social monitoring and arousal [46,47,49].
In chimpanzees, social feeding carries an elevated risk of aggression owing to resource competition [57–61]. However, the perceived threat can vary dramatically from one feeding event to the next, in relation to changes in food patch size, monopolizability and desirability of food items, as well as nearby group members [56,62–64]. A particularly valuable food resource for chimpanzees is meat, usually the result of lengthy and socially coordinated hunting behaviour that can involve a considerable number of individuals jointly attacking a group of monkeys or other prey [65–68]. If successful, the carcass can be monopolized by one individual and defended against competitors, who may use various degrees of coercion to induce sharing [63], although meat may also be shared freely with social partners [7]. As a result, the acquisition and consumption of meat is probably socially more complex and associated with higher levels of competition than other, more commonly consumed and less easily monopolized foods, such as small- and medium-sized figs that can be harvested from large trees [68] and are typically of more moderate energetic value [69].
Chimpanzees, like many social animals, live in groups with individualized relationships, characterized by dominance, kinship, and social bonding [70,71]. As feeding typically takes place in the presence of others, the size and composition of the nearby audience is likely to impact how individuals perceive the social competition associated with a feeding event. In particular, we expect it to be impacted by the social context. For example, trying to retrieve a fruit near a dominant non-bonded group member is probably perceived as more stressful than doing so from near a social partner [7,23,56,63], owing to differences in the risk of aggression. Importantly, these changes in stress response may not be easily directly observed, whether by human observers or conspecifics, from whom it may be advantageous to conceal them [72].
We used infrared thermography to assess how wild chimpanzees' arousal changes during different types of feeding events (meat versus figs), in the presence of male and female group members, and with or without the presence of dominant individuals or social partners. Based on previous research with chimpanzees [30,31,37,39], we focused on temperature differences on the surface of the nose, a body part that reacts with measurable shifts in skin temperature in response to various types of external events [44], including perceived aggression [30,39]. We did not monitor nasal temperature across time (as locomotion is likely to affect skin temperature), but as single independent events. For this reason, we analysed relative differences across variables of interest, namely food type and audience composition. We predicted lower nasal temperatures for larger audiences, particularly when feeding on meat versus figs. More specifically, we predicted lower temperatures for large male audiences, who are not only competitors but also potential aggressors [73]. Similarly, we predicted lower temperatures in the presence of more dominant individuals. We predicted higher temperatures in the presence of social partners, who are less likely to aggress and more likely to support the subject during aggression, providing a potential social buffer in the context of competition. We expected the effects to be larger when feeding on meat as compared to figs. We control for the impact of mating competition as it is a social factor known to affect chimpanzee arousal levels [74,75] (in particular around nutritious food sources [76]).
2. Methods
(a) . Field-site
Data collection were conducted in the Waibira community of the Budongo Central Forest Reserve, in north-western Uganda, a moist, semi-deciduous tropical rain forest covering 428 km2 at an altitude of 1100 m, between 1°35′ and 1°55′ N and 31°08′ and 31°42′ E [77]. Habituation of the Waibira community started in 2011 and most members of the community were well-habituated, including all adult males and all central adult females [78]. At the time of the study, the community was composed of approximately 100 named individuals. Some peripheral adult females and their dependent offspring, as well as some young infants remained unnamed (all are individually recognized and, when seen, interact socially with named individuals as part of the same community), giving an estimated total community size of around 120 individuals.
(b) . Data collection
Data were collected between August 2018 and January 2019 by C.B. and S.M., using focal animal sampling [79] approximately during 07.00 to 17.00 focal follows for approximately 5.5 days a week. Focal animals were 19 adult individuals, nine females (15–37 years old) and 10 males (15–42 years old) (electronic supplementary material, table S1). The focal individuals were chosen because of their tolerance to the presence of observers within 10 m. Every day, one focal individual was chosen amongst the individuals encountered in the forest. No focal individual was followed for two consecutive days, and sampling time was balanced across individuals. Each focal individual was observed on average for approximately 13 h (range 9–15 h). Behavioural data were entered on a portable device (Runbo F1) using a custom built-in interface created with Cybertracker (v. 3.389).
Thermal images were largely taken from focal individuals but, when this was impossible, they were opportunistically taken from other party members. ‘Subjects’ hereafter refers to individuals being photographed. Thermal images were taken using a TESTO thermal imager model 881–2. The camera sensor possesses a spectral range from 8 to 14 µm and a thermal sensitivity of less than 50 mK at 30°C. Emissivity was set at 0.98, a value used for chimpanzee thermal imaging [37,38]. A telephoto lens (9° × 7°/0.5 m) was attached to the camera at all times. Images were taken during feeding events from a distance of approximately 7 m (range 2–15 m) whenever possible to minimize the effect of distance on temperature reading [80], when subjects were feeding either on the ground or in trees (up to a maximum height of 2 m), and only when they were engaged in social feeding behaviour (i.e. feeding in the presence of at least one other subadult or adult individual within 10 m). Subjects were recorded when feeding on meat (Colobus guereza, Cercopithecus mitis, Cephalophus natalensis), or on figs (here, largely on Ficus sur—a tree that produces large volumes of small figs distributed across its branches and trunk and of intermediate value, with respect to its level of sugar compared to other fig species [69]; and, infrequently, on Ficus exasperata).
Subjects had to be stationary for at least 1 min prior to taking images (to avoid the effects of locomotion on nasal temperature [30]), not exposed to direct sunlight, and when there was a direct and unobstructed line of sight. Once all of these conditions were met, we took as many pictures as possible (on average, one every 5 s). For each thermal image, we recorded the following information about adult and subadult individuals present within 10 m: the number of males and females, the number of social partners (defined below) and the dominance distance between the subject and the highest-ranking individual (defined below). Thermal data collection was interrupted as soon as the individual was not engaged in feeding or when the subject moved more than 5 m from the initial location. To control for environmental confounds, humidity (which affects emissivity) and ambient temperature (which affects surface temperature) were recorded every 15 min using a portable digital thermometer and hygrometer (HTC-1 LCD).
(c) . Data analysis: measurement of infrared thermography data
Given that chimpanzee skin temperature can change rapidly (within about 10–20 s [37]) we considered two thermal images as statistically independent if they were separated by greater than 1 min. As the image angle can affect readings [81], we only included images if both eyes of the subject were clearly visible (maximum angle of 45°; figure 1). We extracted temperature measurements from the nasal area, using the TESTO IRSoft analysis software (v. 4.0.5; figure 1). Because air flow entering and exiting the nose may create artefacts in temperature measurements, we manually drew a six-sided diamond-shaped polygon around the nasal area, avoiding the nostril area, and extracted the mean temperature. Intra-rater reliability was performed on n = 89 images (71% of the dataset used for analysis), which showed a high degree of agreement (intraclass correlation coefficient = 0.99).
Figure 1.
Examples of thermal images from a chimpanzee feeding on Ficus sur. The drawing around the nasal area represents the region of interest. (Online version in colour).
(d) . Data analysis: social correlates of thermal images
(i) . Social partners and dyadic composite sociality index
The dyadic composite sociality index (DSI) is calculated as the sum of the duration of time a dyad spent grooming and a dyad spent in proximity, divided by the mean recorded duration of that behaviour, and then divided by the number of behaviours. We computed DSI for all dyads including a focal, based on positive social interactions that occurred between the two individuals. Both adults and subadults were considered as well as same-sex and mixed dyads. The DSI scores were calculated as described in [82], but using only grooming duration (with a distinction made between reciprocal and unidirectional grooming, the former being attributed to both individuals, with duration counted as double) and time spent in proximity (all individuals within 5 m of the focal were considered to be ‘in proximity’). DSI values ranged from 0 to infinity, with dyads with a DSI greater than 1 having a better than average social relationship relative to all recorded social relationships. For each of the subjects, we determined three social partners, corresponding to the three individuals with which the subject exhibited the highest DSI values (among the DSI values > 1) [79], excluding dependent offspring (see the electronic supplementary material, table S2). Number of social partners within 10 m was therefore defined as the number of individuals within 10 m to the subject that were among the top three social partners of the subject.
(ii) . Dominance distance
We computed Elo ratings (dominance ranks) for all individuals based on unidirectional pant-grunts (a submission signal in chimpanzees [83]) collected ad libitum during the study period, using the ‘EloRating’ package (v. 0.46.11 [84,85]). The individual Elo ratings at the end of the study period were used as the dominance rank of the individual throughout the study period and applied to all recorded events. No obvious rank changes were observed during the study period, suggesting that the Elo-scores assigned to each individual were reliable estimates of their relative positions within the dominance hierarchy. We calculated dominance distance as the difference between the Elo rating of the highest-ranking individual within 10 m of the subject and the Elo rating of the subject. Positive values of dominance distance therefore indicate that the subjects were submissive to at least one individual in their audience, whereas negative dominance distance values indicated that the subject outranked all individuals in their audience.
(iii) . Control for mating competition
We included the number of females in oestrus within 10 m of the subject as a proxy for mating competition.
(e) . Statistical analysis: models
Statistical analyses were performed in R (v. 4.1.2 [86]) using R Studio (v. 2021.9.1.372 [87]).
We built general linear mixed models (GLMM) using the ‘lmer’ function of the ‘lme4’ package (v. 1.1.27.1 [88]). Analyses were performed on n = 124 observations from 19 individuals (10 adult males, nine adult females), based on social feeding on ‘meat’ (here: meat of Colobus guereza, Cercopithecus mitis, Cephalophus natalensis) (n = 55 observations) or ‘figs’ (here: Ficus sur, Ficus exasperata) (n = 69 observations).
The dependent variable was nasal temperature, which was transformed to reduce skewness ((datapoint - min(of all datapoints)2) and z-scored. The predictors were: food type (meat or figs); number of males (within 10 m of the subject); number of females (within 10 m of the subject); number of social partners (number of top three social partners of subject within 10 m of the subject) and dominance distance (Elo rating of highest-ranking individual within 10 m of the subject − Elo rating of subject). All predictors were z-scored. We also included three control variables: ambient temperature, expressed in *Celsius; ambient humidity, expressed in % of humidity in the air and number of females in oestrus within 10 m as a proxy for mating competition. All control variables were z-scored.
Colinearity checks were carried out using the function ‘vif’ of the package ‘car’ (v. 3.0.1, [89]). They indicate no colinearity issues (all variance inflation factors (VIFs) value below 1.9). Our sample size was within the range required for analysis using GLMM [90] (sample size ≥ 104 + no. predictors; sample size ≥115 in our case).
The full model comprised interactions between the predictor food type and all other predictors, as well as the two control variables and the identity of the subject as a random factor. We confirmed that the residuals were normally distributed and homogenous by inspecting a scatterplot and quantile-quantile plot of the residuals in function of the fitted values, and that no influential data points were present by excluding data points one at a time from the dataset and checking model estimates.
Before assessing the significance of the predictors, we tested our full model including all interactions against a null model (including only the control variables and random effect) using the function ‘Anova’ of the package ‘car’, to test whether the predictors improved model fit. We also reran the same full model, additionally including a random effect for thermal images taken within a 30 min time period, to ensure the results were robust to temporal auto-correlation. p-values were extracted using the function ‘Anova’ from the package ‘car’ (v. 3.0.1, [89]).
Figures were created using the packages ‘ggplot2’ (v. 3.3.5 [91]) and ‘ggpubr’ (v. 0.4.0 [92]).
3. Results
Our full model was a better fit than the null model (likelihood ratio test: χ29 = 700.98, p < 0.001) and revealed a significant interaction between food type and the number of males within 10 m, the number of social partners within 10 m and the dominance distance to the highest-ranking individual within 10 m. We found no evidence for an effect of the interaction between food type and the number of females within 10 m, nor for an effect of the number of females independently after rerunning the model without the interaction term (table 1; electronic supplementary material, table S3). These results were robust to the inclusion of a random effect term for data points collected within the same 30 min time period (see the electronic supplementary material, table S4). Note also that, as expected, ambient temperature was a strong predictor of nasal temperature. We found no evidence for a significant effect of number of females in oestrus, used as a proxy for mating competition (table 1).
Table 1.
Interaction effect of food type and audience composition on nasal temperature. (The reference level for ‘food type’ is ‘meat’. Significant results are in bold. Interactions are noted with an asterisk.)
term | estimate | s.e. | t-score | p-value |
---|---|---|---|---|
intercept | −4.435 | 2.407 | −1.842 | 0.065 |
food type | 0.227 | 0.222 | 1.019 | 0.308 b |
number of malesa | 0.373 | 0.109 | 3.421 | 0.001b |
number of femalesa | 0.068 | 0.150 | 0.451 | 0.652b |
number of social partnersa | −0.312 | 0.182 | −1.704 | 0.088b |
dominance distancea | 0.118 | 0.119 | 0.993 | 0.321b |
ambient temperaturea | 0.172 | 0.068 | 2.546 | 0.011 |
ambient humiditya | −0.015 | 0.015 | −1.003 | 0.316 |
number of oestrus femalesa | −0.085 | 0.083 | −1.023 | 0.306 |
food type * number of malesa | −0.591 | 0.208 | −2.840 | 0.005 |
food type * number of femalesa | 0.056 | 0.190 | 0.303 | 0.762 |
food type * number of social partnersa | 0.591 | 0.228 | 2.596 | 0.010 |
food type * dominance distancea | −0.380 | 0.171 | −2.221 | 0.026 |
az-scored to allow comparison of model estimates across predictors.
bNote that main effects have limited interpretative value when interactions are significant.
Nasal temperature while feeding on meat was lower for higher numbers of males within 10 m (figure 2a). The opposite pattern was found for figs, with higher nasal temperature for higher numbers of males within 10 m (figure 2a).
Figure 2.
Interaction effect of food type and audience composition on nasal temperature (figs, grey circles; meat, black triangles). The fitted model lines (figs, light grey; meat, dark grey) are presented with 95% confidence bands for the fitted values (95%). (a) Interaction effect between the number of males within 10 m and food type on nasal temperature; (b) interaction effect between the dominance distance to the highest-ranking individual within 10 m (Elo rating of highest-ranking individual within 10 m of the subject − Elo rating of subject) and food type on nasal temperature; and (c) interaction effect between the number of social bond partners presents within 10 m and food type on nasal temperature.
The model also revealed an interaction between food type and dominance distance. When feeding on meat, nasal temperature was lower for higher dominance distances to the highest-ranking individual within 10 m (figure 2b). We found no clear effect of dominance distance when feeding on figs.
Finally, the model showed an interaction between food type and the number of social partners within 10 m. The nasal temperature while feeding on meat was lower for fewer numbers of social bond partners present within 10 m. An opposite effect was observed for figs (figure 2c), although the effect should be treated with caution because of a limited number of data points for a larger number of social partners within 10 m.
Table 1 recapitulates the model results. Note that the values for the main terms are presented but, because of the presence of significant interactions, the interpretation of the individual main effects is limited to their effect, keeping the interacting effect at its average [93].
4. Discussion
In the human literature, audience effects are well documented, such that the mere presence of others is known to affect one's attentional resources and level of arousal [48,49,53,56]. One putative function of such audience effects is the ‘social monitoring’ of others to best prepare oneself for possible intrusion in one's own activities [46,49], including potential aggression. Social feeding, because it carries an elevated risk of aggression, is an ideal context in which to evaluate this hypothesis in chimpanzees [57,94].
To date, animal behaviour researchers were largely limited to the documentation of easily observable behaviours, such as shifts in gaze, or in relative position. Recently, infrared thermal imaging has gained prominence as an important new tool, providing a simple and non-invasive method to assess otherwise hidden physiological responses that can imply real-time cognitive processing of social interactions that would be undetectable with traditional methods [41,44].
We used infrared thermography to study the role of audience effects during feeding events in wild chimpanzees, with the hypothesis that chimpanzees passively monitor the audience when valuable resources are at stake. Our results revealed differences in nasal temperature related to the food resource, the audience size, and the audience composition (dominance and social partners). Specifically, nasal temperature of individuals feeding on meat were lower for contexts marked by higher competition, i.e. both when more males were present within 10 m, and when dominance distances between a lower-ranking subject and the most dominant individual in the party were large. These findings are consistent with elevated levels of stress when surrounded by dominant individuals and feeding on meat, a more easily monopolized and important resource [7,63]. Our results also suggest that social partners may have acted as a buffer (sensu [95]), with higher nasal temperature for individuals with more social partners within 10 m (relative to others with fewer social partners around) when feeding on meat.
This pattern of results is consistent with the findings of reduced temperature in peripheral areas of animals' bodies (such as the nose, or the tail) owing to blood redirection away from areas vulnerable to significant blood loss [45] in social situations likely to be perceived as stressful [30,39,41,44]. These findings support the hypothesis that social feeding carries an elevated risk of physical aggression and stress that is likely to be mitigated by the presence of social partners [96] and the prospect of receiving coalitionary support in case of escalation [8,97,98]. Interestingly, we found the opposite patterns of results for figs, with higher levels of nasal temperature for larger numbers of males present within 10 m, and for lower numbers of social partners present within 10 m. While figs are an important food resource [69,71], the small-sized Ficus sur (which accounted for the majority of observations of feeding on figs in our sample) represent a less competed-for and less monopolizable food resource than meat. In a large community such as Waibira, with an unusually large number of independent males, having more male and high-ranking individuals in a party could perhaps decrease the chance of aggression. The presence of so many males, and therefore of potential allies for whomever is threatened, makes any use of aggression a risky strategy. If this is the case, then when feeding on small-medium sized figs, a much more widely available and less easily monopolized food than meat and thus one less worth competing over, having more males and high-ranking individuals present could represent a lower risk environment than a smaller party with just one or two large dominant individuals present.
The presence of females in oestrous, a characteristic of the environment which is probably conducive to escalated risk of aggression [75], did not impact skin temperature. A possible reason is that there is a trade-off between mating and feeding in terms of time budget, such that feeding may be already reduced when mating opportunities are available (as seen in [99]) and mating opportunities thus had a reduced impact on the limited time left for feeding.
The mere presence of others can affect cognitive, behavioural and emotional responses (see [49] for a review). Several hypotheses have been discussed as to why this is the case. One hypothesis (referred to here as the ‘social monitoring’ hypothesis) is based on the arousal levels generated by the presence and proximity of others [53,100], among which are potential competitors for resources and/or individuals whose behaviour remains uncertain and can represent a physical threat to the subject [46,47], but who can also represent protection from non-social partners and provide social support [21,101]. In chimpanzees, social feeding can be characterized by an elevated risk of aggression [58], which makes monitoring of audience members and their social relationships particularly important. Audience effects are particularly expected for high-value food, such as meat, relative to more spatially dispersed, more abundant and less easily monopolized feeding sources, such as figs. This said, even in the absence of overt physical competition over food (as manifested by, for example, physical aggression or milder harassment, such as persistent begging gestures towards the individual in possession [63,102]), the physiological effects of arousal observed might also reflect an effect of the presence of others [48,53].
While discussion remains over the exact source of body surface temperature shifts [41], some consensus exists over the valid use of infrared thermography to infer shifts in blood flow caused by the activation of the autonomic nervous system [44], with a decrease in nasal temperature when individuals' are exposed to stressful events [30,36,39,44]. Although it is tempting to interpret the observed differences in temperature as reflecting absolute levels of arousal, no time series were collected here, preventing interpretation of our data as indicating shifts in skin temperature relative to a known baseline or changes occurring after a specific stimulus (unlike in e.g. [30,37]). We therefore interpret skin temperature profiles strictly as ‘relative’ differences across various audience sizes and composition, with lower nasal temperature indicating relatively lower blood flow in peripheral regions of the body, a pattern typically associated with the experience of negative emotions [44].
While our findings are strongly suggestive, they remain—like much of the thermal imaging field—preliminary. We would benefit substantially from much larger datasets that allow analysis both within individuals, and between a larger number of individuals with a greater range of social relationships. In future work, longitudinal comparisons may provide a means to explore variation in coping-style across individuals, a fundamental aspect of how other species (including humans) vary in their individual responses to stressful events [103,104]. Future studies could also include complimentary physiological measurements (such as hormonal profiles, e.g. [30]) that could support and enrich our understanding of the effects being observed (e.g. help identify the physiological source of changes in arousal, for example the potential involvement of the ‘hypothalamus–pituitary–adrenal axis, as in [30]).
In summary, our study shows that wild chimpanzees are affected by the presence of audience in stressful situations, such as when feeding over potentially contested resources, and that audience effects in non-human apes include nuanced monitoring of their day-to-day social landscape.
Acknowledgment
We are grateful to the management, staff, field assistants and researchers of the Budongo Conservation Field Station (BCFS) for their support and discussions on the study and to the Royal Zoological Society of Scotland for their long-term funding of the field-site. We thank the Uganda Wildlife Authority and the Uganda National Council for Science and Technology for permission to conduct the study. We also thank Quentin Gallot and Hélène Chotard for their comments on an early draft and Christof Neumann for statistical advice.
Ethics
The study adhered to the ASAB guidelines for the treatment of animals during behavioural studies [105]. A minimum distance of 7 m was maintained between the observer and the focal animal, though habituated chimpanzees would sometimes approach closer. The study was approved by the Uganda Wildlife Authority (UWA/COD/96/5), the Uganda National Council for Science and Technology (NS 637), and the research ethics committees of the Universities of Neuchâtel (38/2019-B) and St Andrews (N. 171).
Data accessibility
Data are available at osf.io/c5pku. Data are also provided in the electronic supplemenary material [106].
Authors' contributions
C.B.: conceptualization, data curation, funding acquisition, investigation, methodology, project administration, validation, visualization, writing—original draft, writing—review and editing; A.S.: conceptualization, formal analysis, investigation, methodology, software, supervision, validation, visualization, writing—original draft, writing—review and editing; C.H.: funding acquisition, methodology, resources, supervision, writing—original draft, writing—review and editing; S.M.: investigation, methodology, resources; D.D.M.: formal analysis, methodology, software, validation, visualization, writing—original draft, writing—review and editing; K.Z.: conceptualization, funding acquisition, methodology, project administration, resources, supervision, writing—review and editing; G.D.: conceptualization, methodology, resources, supervision, visualization, writing—original draft, writing—review and editing.
All authors gave final approval for publication and agreed to be held accountable for the work performed therein.
Conflict of interest declaration
We declare we have no competing interests.
Funding
The study was funded by the Swiss National Science Foundation (project no. 310030_143359 and NCCR Evolving Language to K.Z.), the ‘Fonds des donations’ from the University of Neuchâtel (awarded to C.B.), the European Union's 8th Framework Programme, Horizon 2020, under grant agreement no 802719 (awarded to C.H.), and the Swiss universities and St Leonard College mobility grants (awarded to A.S.). G.D. is indebted to the French government IDEX-ISITE initiative 16-IDEX-0001 (CAP 20-25) for funding.
References
- 1.Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM. 2018. How monkeys see the world: inside the mind of another species. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Benítez ME, Pappano DJ, Beehner JC, Bergman TJ. 2017. Evidence for mutual assessment in a wild primate. Sci. Rep. 7, 2952. ( 10.1038/s41598-017-02903-w) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Kitchen DM, Cheney DL, Engh AL, Fischer J, Moscovice LR, Seyfarth RM. 2013. Male baboon responses to experimental manipulations of loud ‘wahoo calls’: testing an honest signal of fighting ability. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 67, 1825-1835. ( 10.1007/s00265-013-1592-8) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Bergman TJ, Beehner JC, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM. 2003. Hierarchical classification by rank and kinship in baboons. Science 302, 1234-1236. ( 10.1126/science.1087513) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Fischer J, Kitchen DM, Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL. 2004. Baboon loud calls advertise male quality: acoustic features and their relation to rank, age, and exhaustion. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 56, 140-148. ( 10.1007/s00265-003-0739-4) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Bergman TJ, Sheehan MJ. 2013. Social knowledge and signals in primates. Am. J. Primatol. 75, 683-694. ( 10.1002/ajp.22103) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Samuni L, Preis A, Mielke A, Deschner T, Wittig RM, Crockford C. 2018. Social bonds facilitate cooperative resource sharing in wild chimpanzees. Proc. R. Soc. B 285, 20181643. ( 10.1098/rspb.2018.1643) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Wittig RM, Crockford C, Langergraber KE, Zuberbühler K. 2014. Triadic social interactions operate across time: a field experiment with wild chimpanzees. Proc. R. Soc. B 281, 20133155. ( 10.1098/rspb.2013.3155) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Crockford C, Wittig RM, Mundry R, Zuberbühler K. 2012. Wild chimpanzees inform ignorant group members of danger. Curr. Biol. 22, 142-146. ( 10.1016/j.cub.2011.11.053) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Crockford C, Wittig RM, Zuberbühler K. 2017. Vocalizing in chimpanzees is influenced by social-cognitive processes. Sci. Adv. 3, e1701742. ( 10.1126/sciadv.1701742) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Krupenye C, Call J. 2019. Theory of mind in animals: current and future directions. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Cogn. Sci. e1503. ( 10.1002/wcs.1503) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Girard-Buttoz C, Surbeck M, Samuni L, Tkaczynski P, Boesch C, Fruth B, Wittig RM, Hohmann G, Crockford C. 2020. Information transfer efficiency differs in wild chimpanzees and bonobos, but not social cognition. Proc. R. Soc. B 287, 20200523. ( 10.1098/rspb.2020.0523) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Schel AM, Townsend SW, Machanda Z, Zuberbühler K, Slocombe KE. 2013. Chimpanzee alarm call production meets key criteria for intentionality. PLoS ONE 8, e76674. ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0076674) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Slocombe KE, Kaller T, Turman L, Townsend SW, Papworth S, Squibbs P, Zuberbühler K. 2010. Production of food-associated calls in wild male chimpanzees is dependent on the composition of the audience. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 64, 1959-1966. ( 10.1007/s00265-010-1006-0) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Fedurek P, Slocombe KE. 2013. The social function of food-associated calls in male chimpanzees. Am. J. Primatol. 75, 726-739. ( 10.1002/ajp.22122) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Brügger RK, Willems EP, Burkart JM. 2021. Do marmosets understand others' conversations? A thermography approach. Sci. Adv. 7, eabc8790. ( 10.1126/sciadv.abc8790) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Mitani JC, Call J, Kappeler PM, Palombit RA, Silk JB. 2012. The evolution of primate societies. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
- 18.Schino G, Aureli F. 2010. Primate reciprocity and its cognitive requirements. Evol. Anthropol. Issues News Rev. 19, 130-135. ( 10.1002/evan.20270) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Dunbar RIM. 2010. The social role of touch in humans and primates: behavioural function and neurobiological mechanisms. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 34, 260-268. ( 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.07.001) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Crockford C, Deschner T, Ziegler TE, Wittig RM. 2014. Endogenous peripheral oxytocin measures can give insight into the dynamics of social relationships: a review. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 8, 68. ( 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00068) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Samuni L, Preis A, Mundry R, Deschner T, Crockford C, Wittig RM. 2017. Oxytocin reactivity during intergroup conflict in wild chimpanzees. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 114, 268-273. ( 10.1073/pnas.1616812114) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Samuni L, Preis A, Deschner T, Wittig RM, Crockford C. 2019. Cortisol and oxytocin show independent activity during chimpanzee intergroup conflict. Psychoneuroendocrinology 104, 165-173. ( 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2019.02.007) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Wittig RM, Crockford C, Deschner T, Langergraber KE, Ziegler TE, Zuberbühler K. 2014. Food sharing is linked to urinary oxytocin levels and bonding in related and unrelated wild chimpanzees. Proc. R. Soc. B 281, 20133096. ( 10.1098/rspb.2013.3096) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Girard-Buttoz C, et al. 2021. Early maternal loss leads to short- but not long-term effects on diurnal cortisol slopes in wild chimpanzees. eLife 10, e64134. ( 10.7554/eLife.64134) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Abbott DH, et al. 2003. Are subordinates always stressed? a comparative analysis of rank differences in cortisol levels among primates. Horm. Behav. 43, 67-82. ( 10.1016/S0018-506X(02)00037-5) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Rincon AV, Heistermann M, Schülke O, Ostner J. 2020. Testosterone and cortisol are negatively associated with ritualized bonding behavior in male macaques. Psychoneuroendocrinology 120, 104774. ( 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2020.104774) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Tkaczynski PJ, et al. 2020. Patterns of urinary cortisol levels during ontogeny appear population specific rather than species specific in wild chimpanzees and bonobos. J. Hum. Evol. 147, 102869. ( 10.1016/j.jhevol.2020.102869) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Kret ME, Tomonaga M, Matsuzawa T. 2014. Chimpanzees and humans mimic pupil-size of conspecifics. PLoS ONE 9, e104886. ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0104886) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Ermatinger FA, Brügger RK, Burkart JM. 2019. The use of infrared thermography to investigate emotions in common marmosets. Physiol. Behav. 211, 112672. ( 10.1016/j.physbeh.2019.112672) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Kano F, Hirata S, Deschner T, Behringer V, Call J. 2016. Nasal temperature drop in response to a playback of conspecific fights in chimpanzees: a thermo-imaging study. Physiol. Behav. 155, 83-94. ( 10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.11.029) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Sato Y, Kano F, Hirata S. 2018. Cutting-edge infrared thermography as a new tool to explore animal emotions. Jpn J. Anim. Psychol. 68, 1-15. [Google Scholar]
- 32.Chotard H, Ioannou S, Davila-Ross M. 2018. Infrared thermal imaging: positive and negative emotions modify the skin temperatures of monkey and ape faces. Am. J. Primatol. 80, e22863. ( 10.1002/ajp.22863) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Ioannou S, Chotard H, Davila-Ross M. 2015. No strings attached: physiological monitoring of rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) with thermal imaging. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 9, 160. ( 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00160) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Ross SR, Lake BR, Fultz A, Hopper LM. 2021. An evaluation of thermal imaging as a welfare monitoring tool for captive chimpanzees. Primates 62, 919-927. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Kuraoka K, Nakamura K. 2011. The use of nasal skin temperature measurements in studying emotion in macaque monkeys. Physiol. Behav. 102, 347-355. ( 10.1016/j.physbeh.2010.11.029) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Nakayama K, Goto S, Kuraoka K, Nakamura K. 2005. Decrease in nasal temperature of rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) in negative emotional state. Physiol. Behav. 84, 783-790. ( 10.1016/j.physbeh.2005.03.009) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Dezecache G, Zuberbühler K, Davila-Ross M, Dahl CD. 2017. Skin temperature changes in wild chimpanzees upon hearing vocalizations of conspecifics. R. Soc. Open Sci. 4, 160816. ( 10.1098/rsos.160816) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Dezecache G, Wilke C, Richi N, Neumann C, Zuberbühler K. 2017. Skin temperature and reproductive condition in wild female chimpanzees. PeerJ 5, e4116. ( 10.7717/peerj.4116) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.de Vevey M, Bouchard A, Soldati A, Zuberbühler K.. 2022. Thermal imaging reveals audience-dependent effects during cooperation and competition in wild chimpanzees. Sci. Rep. 12, 2972. ( 10.1038/s41598-022-07003-y) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.McFarland R, Barrett L, Fuller A, Hetem RS, Porter WP, Young C, Henzi SP. 2020. Infrared thermography cannot be used to approximate core body temperature in wild primates. Am. J. Primatol. 82, e23204. ( 10.1002/ajp.23204) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Travain T, Valsecchi P. 2021. Infrared thermography in the study of animals’ emotional responses: a critical review. Animals 11, 2510. ( 10.3390/ani11092510) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Speakman JR, Ward S. 1998. Infrared thermography: principles and applications. Zool.-Jena 101, 224-232. [Google Scholar]
- 43.Tattersall GJ. 2016. Infrared thermography: a non-invasive window into thermal physiology. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. A. Mol. Integr. Physiol. 202, 78-98. ( 10.1016/j.cbpa.2016.02.022) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Ioannou S, Gallese V, Merla A. 2014. Thermal infrared imaging in psychophysiology: potentialities and limits. Psychophysiology 51, 951-963. ( 10.1111/psyp.12243) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Vianna DML, Carrive P. 2005. Changes in cutaneous and body temperature during and after conditioned fear to context in the rat. Eur. J. Neurosci. 21, 2505-2512. ( 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04073.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Guerin B. 1983. Social facilitation and social monitoring: a test of three models. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 22, 203-214. ( 10.1111/j.2044-8309.1983.tb00585.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Guerin B, Innes JM. 1982. Social facilitation and social monitoring: a new look at Zajonc's mere presence hypothesis. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 21, 7-18. ( 10.1111/j.2044-8309.1982.tb00506.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Belletier C, Normand A, Huguet P. 2019. Social-facilitation-and-impairment effects: from motivation to cognition and the social brain. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 28, 260-265. ( 10.1177/0963721419829699) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Guerin B, Innes J. 1993. Social facilitation. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 50.Dindo M, Whiten A, de Waal FBM. 2009. Social facilitation of exploratory foraging behavior in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Am. J. Primatol. 71, 419-426. ( 10.1002/ajp.20669) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Galloway AT, Addessi E, Fragaszy DM, Visalberghi E. 2005. Social facilitation of eating familiar food in tufted capuchins (Cebus apella): does it involve behavioral coordination? Int. J. Primatol. 26, 181-189. ( 10.1007/s10764-005-0729-7) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Huguet P, Barbet I, Belletier C, Monteil J-M, Fagot J. 2014. Cognitive control under social influence in baboons. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 143, 2067. ( 10.1037/xge0000026) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Zajonc RB. 1965. Social facilitation. Science 149, 269-274. ( 10.1126/science.149.3681.269) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Halfmann E, Bredehöft J, Häusser JA. 2020. Replicating roaches: a preregistered direct replication of Zajonc, Heingartner, and Herman's (1969) social-facilitation study. Psychol. Sci. 31, 332-337. ( 10.1177/0956797620902101) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Chen SC. 1937. Social modification of the activity of ants in nest-building. Physiol. Zool. 10, 420-436. ( 10.1086/physzool.10.4.30151428) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Roberts SD, Dunbar RIM, Roberts A. 2022. Communicative roots of complex sociality and cognition: neuropsychological mechanisms underpinning the processing of social information. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 95, 51-73. ( 10.1098/rstb.2021.0295.R1) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Houle A, Wrangham RW. 2021. Contest competition for fruit and space among wild chimpanzees in relation to the vertical stratification of metabolizable energy. Anim. Behav. 175, 231-246. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2021.03.003) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Ischer G, Zuberbühler K, Fedurek P. 2020. The relationship between food calling and agonistic behaviour in wild chimpanzees. Behav. Processes 178, 104182. ( 10.1016/j.beproc.2020.104182) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.de Waal FBM. 1989. Food sharing and reciprocal obligations among chimpanzees. J. Hum. Evol. 18, 433-459. ( 10.1016/0047-2484(89)90074-2) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Wittig RM, Boesch C. 2003. Food competition and linear dominance hierarchy among female chimpanzees of the Taï National Park. Int. J. Primatol. 24, 847-867. ( 10.1023/A:1024632923180) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Wittig RM, Boesch C. 2003. ‘Decision-making’ in conflicts of wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): an extension of the Relational Model. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 54, 491-504. ( 10.1007/s00265-003-0654-8) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Koomen R, Herrmann E. 2018. The effects of social context and food abundance on chimpanzee feeding competition. Am. J. Primatol. 80, e22734. ( 10.1002/ajp.22734) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Gilby IC. 2006. Meat sharing among the Gombe chimpanzees: harassment and reciprocal exchange. Anim. Behav. 71, 953-963. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.09.009) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Isabirye-Basuta G. 1988. Food competition among individuals in a free-ranging chimpanzee community in Kibale Forest, Uganda. Behaviour 105, 135-147. ( 10.1163/156853988X00485) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Boesch C. 1994. Cooperative hunting in wild chimpanzees. Anim. Behav. 48, 653-667. ( 10.1006/anbe.1994.1285) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Newton-Fisher NE, Notman H, Reynolds V. 2002. Hunting of mammalian prey by Budongo Forest chimpanzees. Folia Primatol. 73, 281-283. ( 10.1159/000067454) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Stanford CB, Wallis J, Mpongo E, Goodall J. 1994. Hunting decisions in wild chimpanzees. Behaviour 131, 1-18. ( 10.1163/156853994X00181) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Newton-Fisher NE. 1999. The diet of chimpanzees in the Budongo Forest Reserve, Uganda. Afr. J. Ecol. 37, 344-354. ( 10.1046/j.1365-2028.1999.00186.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Reynolds V, Plumptre AJ, Greenham J, Harborne J. 1998. Condensed tannins and sugars in the diet of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) in the Budongo Forest, Uganda. Oecologia 115, 331-336. ( 10.1007/s004420050524) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Goodall J. 1986. The chimpanzees of Gombe: patterns of behavior. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 71.Reynolds V. 2005. The chimpanzees of the Budongo forest: ecology, behaviour and conservation. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 72.Dezecache G, Mercier H, Scott-Phillips TC. 2013. An evolutionary approach to emotional communication. J. Pragmat. 59, 221-233. ( 10.1016/j.pragma.2013.06.007) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Sabbi KH, Thompson ME, Machanda ZP, Otali E, Wrangham RW, Muller MN. 2021. Sex differences in early experience and the development of aggression in wild chimpanzees. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 118, e2017144118. ( 10.1073/pnas.2017144118) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Muller MN, Wrangham RW. 2004. Dominance, aggression and testosterone in wild chimpanzees: a test of the ‘challenge hypothesis'. Anim. Behav. 67, 113-123. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.03.013) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Roberts AI, Roberts SGB. 2015. Gestural communication and mating tactics in wild chimpanzees. PLoS ONE 10, e0139683. ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0139683) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Anderson DP, Nordheim EV, Boesch C. 2006. Environmental factors influencing the seasonality of estrus in chimpanzees. Primates 47, 43-50. ( 10.1007/s10329-005-0143-y) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Eggeling WJ. 1947. Observations on the ecology of the Budongo rain forest, Uganda. J. Ecol. 34, 20-87. ( 10.2307/2256760) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Samuni L, Mundry R, Terkel J, Zuberbühler K, Hobaiter C. 2014. Socially learned habituation to human observers in wild chimpanzees. Anim. Cogn. 17, 997-1005. ( 10.1007/s10071-014-0731-6) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Altmann J. 1974. Observational study of behavior: sampling methods. Behaviour 49, 227-266. ( 10.1163/156853974X00534) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Playà-Montmany N, Tattersall GJ. 2021. Spot size, distance and emissivity errors in field applications of infrared thermography. Methods Ecol. Evol. 12, 828-840. ( 10.1111/2041-210X.13563) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Tabh JKR, Burness G, Wearing OH, Tattersall GJ, Mastromonaco GF. 2021. Infrared thermography as a technique to measure physiological stress in birds: body region and image angle matter. Physiol. Rep. 9, e14865. ( 10.14814/phy2.14865) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.De Moor D, Roos C, Ostner J, Schülke O. 2020. Female Assamese macaques bias their affiliation to paternal and maternal kin. Behav. Ecol. 31, 493-507. ( 10.1093/beheco/arz213) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Fedurek P, Tkaczynski PJ, Hobaiter C, Zuberbühler K, Wittig RM, Crockford C. 2021. The function of chimpanzee greeting calls is modulated by their acoustic variation. Anim. Behav. 174, 279-289. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2021.02.002) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Neumann C, Kulik L. 2020. EloRating: animal dominance hierarchies by Elo rating. See https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=EloRating.
- 85.Neumann C, Duboscq J, Dubuc C, Ginting A, Irwan AM, Agil M, Widdig A, Engelhardt A. 2011. Assessing dominance hierarchies: validation and advantages of progressive evaluation with Elo-rating. Anim. Behav. 82, 911-921. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.07.016) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 86.R Core Team. 2019. R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. [Google Scholar]
- 87.R Studio Team. 2019. RStudio: integrated development environment for R. Boston, MA: R Studio Inc. [Google Scholar]
- 88.Bates D, Mächler M, Bolker B, Walker S. 2015. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J. Stat. Softw. 67, 1-48. ( 10.18637/jss.v067.i01) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Fox J, Weisberg S, Price B, Adler D, Bates D, Baud-Bovy G, Bolker B. 2019. car: Companion to Applied Regression. See https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/car/index/html.
- 90.Field A. 2013. Discovering statistics using IBM SPSS statistics. Los Angeles, CA: SAGE Publications. [Google Scholar]
- 91.Wickham H. 2009. Elegant graphics for data analysis. Media 35, 10-1007. [Google Scholar]
- 92.Kassambara A. In press. ggplot2 based publication ready plots. See https://rpkgs.datanovia.com/ggpubr/index.html.
- 93.Underwood AJ. 1997. Experiments in ecology: their logical design and interpretation using analysis of variance. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 94.Muller MN, Mitani JC. 2005. Conflict and cooperation in wild chimpanzees. Adv. Study Behav. 35, 275-331. ( 10.1016/S0065-3454(05)35007-8) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Kikusui T, Winslow JT, Mori Y. 2006. Social buffering: relief from stress and anxiety. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 361, 2215-2228. ( 10.1098/rstb.2006.1941) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Wittig RM, Crockford C, Weltring A, Langergraber KE, Deschner T, Zuberbühler K. 2016. Social support reduces stress hormone levels in wild chimpanzees across stressful events and everyday affiliations. Nat. Commun. 7, 1-8. ( 10.1038/ncomms13361) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Gilby IC, Brent LJ, Wroblewski EE, Rudicell RS, Hahn BH, Goodall J, Pusey AE. 2013. Fitness benefits of coalitionary aggression in male chimpanzees. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 67, 373-381. ( 10.1007/s00265-012-1457-6) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Newton-Fisher NE. 2006. Female coalitions against male aggression in wild chimpanzees of the Budongo Forest. Int. J. Primatol. 27, 1589-1599. ( 10.1007/s10764-006-9087-3) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Georgiev AV, Russell AF, Emery Thompson M, Otali E, Muller MN, Wrangham RW. 2014. The foraging costs of mating effort in male chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii). Int. J. Primatol. 35, 725-745. ( 10.1007/s10764-014-9788-y) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Ioannou S, Morris P, Mercer H, Baker M, Gallese V, Reddy V. 2014. Proximity and gaze influences facial temperature: a thermal infrared imaging study. Front. Psychol. 5, 845. ( 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00845) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Samuni L, Crockford C, Wittig RM. 2021. Group-level cooperation in chimpanzees is shaped by strong social ties. Nat. Commun. 12, 1-10. ( 10.1038/s41467-020-20709-9) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Hobaiter C, Leavens DA, Byrne RW. 2014. Deictic gesturing in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)? Some possible cases. J. Comp. Psychol. 128, 82. ( 10.1037/a0033757) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Sapolsky RM. 2004. Why zebras don't get ulcers: the acclaimed guide to stress, stress-related diseases, and coping. New York, NY: Holt Paperbacks. [Google Scholar]
- 104.Beasley M, Thompson T, Davidson J. 2003. Resilience in response to life stress: the effects of coping style and cognitive hardiness. Personal. Individ. Differ. 34, 77-95. ( 10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00027-2) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 105.No authorship indicated. 2018. Guidelines for the treatment of animals in behavioural research and teaching. Anim. Behav. 135, i-x. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2017.10.001) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Barrault C, Soldati A, Hobaiter C, Mugisha S, De Moor D, Zuberbühler K, Dezecache G. 2022. Data from: Thermal imaging reveals social monitoring during social feeding in wild chimpanzees. Figshare. ( 10.6084/m9.figshare.c.6060627) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Citations
- Barrault C, Soldati A, Hobaiter C, Mugisha S, De Moor D, Zuberbühler K, Dezecache G. 2022. Data from: Thermal imaging reveals social monitoring during social feeding in wild chimpanzees. Figshare. ( 10.6084/m9.figshare.c.6060627) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
Data Availability Statement
Data are available at osf.io/c5pku. Data are also provided in the electronic supplemenary material [106].