Abstract
To navigate towards a food source, animals frequently combine odor cues about source identity with wind direction cues about source location. Where and how these two cues are integrated to support navigation is unclear. Here we describe a pathway to the Drosophila fan-shaped body that encodes attractive odor and promotes upwind navigation. We show that neurons throughout this pathway encode odor, but not wind direction. Using connectomics, we identify fan-shaped body local neurons called h∆C that receive input from this odor pathway and a previously described wind pathway. We show that h∆C neurons exhibit odor-gated, wind direction-tuned activity, that sparse activation of h∆C neurons promotes navigation in a reproducible direction, and that h∆C activity is required for persistent upwind orientation during odor. Based on connectome data, we develop a computational model showing how h∆C activity can promote navigation towards a goal such as an upwind odor source. Our results suggest that odor and wind cues are processed by separate pathways and integrated within the fan-shaped body to support goal-directed navigation.
Subject terms: Navigation, Neural circuits, Olfactory system, Computational neuroscience
Flies navigate to food sources by combining odour and wind-direction cues. This study identifies pathways to the fan-shaped body that encode these signals, and demonstrates how local neurons integrate odour- and wind information to guide navigation.
Introduction
Searching for a resource such as food requires the integration of multiple sensory cues. In natural environments, food odors are often transported by wind, forming turbulent plumes1,2. Within these plumes, instantaneous odor concentration is often a poor cue to source direction3–5. Thus, many organisms have evolved a strategy of using odor information to gate upwind (or upstream) movement to locate the source of an attractive odor6–10. This strategy complements those observed in odor gradients, where odor increases drive straighter trajectories, while odor decreases drive re-orientation (e.g.11). Navigation towards potential food sources thus requires integration of directional information about the prevailing wind (often derived from mechanosensation), with information about the identity and quality of odors carried on that wind9,10. Where and how these two types of information are integrated to support navigation towards an odor source is not clear.
In the insect brain, several conserved central neuropils have been implicated in olfactory food search and navigation (Fig. 1A). The mushroom body (MB) and lateral horn (LH) have been implicated in learned and innate olfactory processing, respectively12–14. Subsets of MB and LH output neurons (MBONs and LHONs) promote approach and avoidance behavior15–18. A number of putative mechanosensory inputs to the MB have been identified19 and wind intensity signals have been observed in certain MB compartments20. The LH also receives input from mechanosensory centers in a discrete ventral region17,19. However, it is not known whether the MB and LH represent wind direction signals, as well as odor identity and value signals, to support navigation.
In contrast, the fan-shaped body (FB), a part of the Drosophila navigation center called the central complex (CX), has been recently shown to encode wind direction21. Columnar inputs to the fan-shaped body (FB), known as PFNs, represent wind direction as a set of orthogonal basis vectors, and receive input from the lateral accessory lobe (LAL) via LNa neurons21. Wind direction signals are strongest in PFNs targeting ventral layers of the FB (PFNa, p, and m in the hemibrain connectome22). PFNs targeting more dorsal layers (PFNd and v) encode both optic flow23 and self-motion during walking24 in a similar vector format. PFNs of all types show little sensitivity to odor stimuli21 suggesting that this pathway mostly encodes flow and self-motion information independent of odor.
A distinct set of FB inputs, known as FB tangential cells, are anatomically downstream of the MB25,26 however, most of these have not been functionally characterized. To date, few olfactory inputs to the FB have been described27. Large lesions to the FB disrupt visual navigation28 and activation of subsets of FB neurons can produce oriented locomotor behaviors in cockroaches29. Recent theoretical and experimental work suggests that FB circuitry is optimized for encoding vectors and specifying navigational goals23,24,30–32 but experimental evidence for goal encoding in the FB is still sparse. Numerous studies have explored the role of the FB in path integration31,33 visual navigation34 and landmark-guided long-distance dispersal35–37. However, few studies have investigated the role of this region in olfactory navigation38.
Here we used an optogenetic wind-navigation paradigm, together with calcium imaging, connectomic analysis, and computational modeling to ask how the MB/LH and FB work together to promote olfactory navigation behavior. We show that a subset of attraction-promoting MB and LH neurons evoke upwind movement when activated. However, calcium imaging indicates that these neurons do not strongly encode wind direction, suggesting that integration of odor and wind information occurs elsewhere. We next performed a large behavioral screen of FB inputs, finding that several groups of FB tangential inputs, but not columnar PFNs, drive upwind movement. Imaging revealed that these neurons also encode odor but not wind direction. Finally, we identify a specific type of FB local neuron, called h∆C, that receives input from both wind-sensitive PFNs and from odor-sensitive FB tangential cells. We show that these neurons encode an odor-gated wind direction-tuned signal, and promote navigation in a reproducible direction when sparsely activated in a fly-specific pattern. Silencing h∆C neurons impairs the ability of flies to maintain upwind orientation throughout an odor stimulus. Based on motifs from the fly connectome, we develop a computational model showing how different patterns of activity in h∆C neurons can promote navigation either upwind (under natural odor and wind activation) or in a reproducible arbitrary direction (during sparse optogenetic activation). Taken together, our data support an emerging model of the FB, in which spatial direction cues and non-spatial context cues enter this region through distinct anatomical pathways, and are integrated by local neurons to specify goal-directed navigation behaviors.
Results
An optogenetic paradigm to investigate the neural circuit basis of upwind navigation
To investigate the neural circuit basis of wind-guided olfactory navigation, we developed an optogenetic activation paradigm. We modified a set of miniature wind tunnels (Fig. 1B10) to present temporally controlled red light stimuli as walking flies navigated in a laminar wind flow. To validate this assay, we asked whether optogenetic activation of olfactory receptor neurons (ORNs) with Chrimson could produce behavioral phenotypes similar to those observed with an attractive odor, apple cider vinegar (vinegar). We found that broad activation of ORNs using either the orco, or the orco and IR8a co-receptor promoters together39,40 resulted in robust navigation behaviors similar to those observed with vinegar (Fig. 1C, D, Fig. S1A). In response to either odor or light, flies ran upwind, generating an increase in upwind velocity. Following odor or light OFF, flies initiated a local search, characterized by increased curvature. Neither behavior was observed in the absence of the orco-GAL4 or orco/IR8a-GAL4 driver, or when we expressed Chrimson under an empty-GAL4 or empty split-GAL4 driver (Fig. 1E, Fig. S1B). Silencing both orco and IR8a-positive ORNs using tetanus toxin abolished both upwind and search responses to odor (Fig. 1F, G). Thus, optogenetic activation can substitute for odor in producing both upwind orientation and OFF search, and ORNs are required for these behavioral responses to odor.
Vinegar activates a subset of both orco+ and IR8a+ glomeruli41. Although the behavioral phenotypes evoked by vinegar and by optogenetic activation of ORNs were similar, they exhibited some subtle differences. Vinegar produced a stronger upwind response than optogenetic activation of orco+ ORNs in the same flies (Fig. S1A). However, the OFF search behavior evoked by optogenetic activation orco-GAL4, or of orco/IR8a-GAL4, was more robust than that evoked by vinegar (Fig. 1C, D, Fig. S1A). Moreover, activation of orco/IR8a+ ORNs in the absence of wind produced OFF search without upwind orientation (Fig. S1C). These results indicate that upwind orientation can be evoked independently of OFF search, and suggest that these two behaviors are driven by distinct but overlapping populations of olfactory glomeruli. Activation of single ORN types known to be activated by vinegar41 did not generate significant upwind orientation or OFF search (Fig. S1D). In addition, silencing of orco+ or IR8a+ ORNs alone did not abolish upwind orientation, but did reduce OFF search (Fig. 1G, Fig. S1E). These data indicate that groups of ORNs must be activated together to promote upwind orientation and that substantial silencing of most olfactory neurons is required to abolish upwind movement in response to vinegar.
A subset of LHONs and MBONs drive wind navigation and encode a non-directional odor signal
We next asked whether activation of central neurons in the LH and MB could similarly produce wind navigation phenotypes. We activated several groups of LHONs and MBONs that were previously shown to produce attraction or aversion in quadrant preference assays15,17. We found that several of these neuron groups drove robust upwind movement when activated (Fig, 2A, B, Fig. S2A). Neurons promoting upwind movement included the cholinergic LHON cluster AD1b2 (labeled by LH1396, LH1538, and LH1539 (Fig. 2A), and the cholinergic MBON lines MB052B (labeling MBONs 15–19), MB077B (labeling MBON12), and MB082C (labeling MBONs 13 and 14, Fig. 2B). AD1b2 drivers and MB052B also elicited significant increases in OFF curvature when activated (Fig. 2A, B), while activating individual MBONs within MB052B (MBONs 15–19) did not drive significant upwind movement (Fig. S2B). Silencing single MBON or LHON lines that drove navigation phenotypes did not abolish upwind movement in response to odor (Fig. S2C) consistent with models suggesting that odor valence is encoded by population output at the level of the MB16 and with our findings at the periphery that very broad silencing is required to eliminate behavioral responses to vinegar.
We also identified MBONs that produced other navigational phenotypes. For example, the glutamatergic (inhibitory) MBON line MB434B (labeling MBONs 5 and 6), which was previously shown to produce aversion15 generated downwind movement in our paradigm (Fig. 2C). Moreover, two MBON lines produced straightening (reduced curvature) in our paradigm (Fig. 2D) but no change in movement relative to wind (Fig. 2D, Fig. S2A): the GABAergic line MB112C (labeling MBON 11), which evoked attraction in quadrant assays, and the glutamatergic line MB011B (labeling MBONs 1,3,4), which evoked aversion15. Overall, these results indicate that LH/MB outputs can drive coordinated suites of locomotor behavior that promote attraction or aversion in different environments. Several LHONs and MBONs redundantly drive upwind movement, key to attraction in windy environments, while other MBONs drive straightening, which promotes attraction in odor gradients11 or in response to familiar visual stimuli42.
Are the LHONs and MBONs that drive upwind movement sensitive to wind direction, or do they encode a non-directional odor signal that is integrated with wind direction downstream? To answer this question, we used calcium imaging to measure responses to calibrated wind and odor stimuli delivered from five different directions (Fig. S2D). Across all four upwind-promoting lines (MB052B, LH1396, MB077B, and MB082C), we observed responses to vinegar, but no tuning for wind direction (Fig. 2E, S2E, ANOVA: F(4,35) = 0.35, p = 0.8408, F(4,40) = 0.3, p = 0.8794, F(4,25) = 0.2, p = 0.9989, F(4,35) = 0.67, p = 0.6166). We separately used electrophysiology to show that the α’3 compartment of the MB, which was previously shown to respond to airflow20 does not encode wind direction (Fig. S2F). To rigorously test whether MB/LH responses carry wind direction information, we generated tree classifiers (see Methods) and asked them to decode if wind was presented from the left, right, or center relative to the fly (Fig. 2F) based on responses during the odor period. We found that classifiers trained on MB/LH responses performed no better than shuffled controls (Fig. 2F). In contrast, classifiers trained on the responses of wind pathway neurons (PFNa, or the difference between left and right LNa neurons)21 performed significantly better than shuffled controls. We trained a second set of classifiers to discriminate between odor and wind ON (Fig. 2G). In this case, MB052B and MB082C performed significantly better than control, while neither wind pathway neuron showed significant discrimination. All neurons significantly discriminated odor from baseline (Fig. S2G). We also applied our wind direction classifiers to other phases of the response, such as wind ON and OFF (Fig. S2H). LH1396, but no other neuron group or phase, showed some discrimination at wind ON. This was largely due to responses that were stronger in front of the fly than at the sides. Taken together, these analyses support the idea that MB/LH neurons that promote upwind orientation largely encode odor presence independent of wind direction.
Multiple tangential FB inputs promote upwind orientation and respond to odor
The FB is anatomically downstream of the MB and LH25,26 and has previously been shown to encode wind direction. We, therefore, asked whether inputs to the FB are likewise capable of driving movement relative to wind direction. We first confirmed that the FB is anatomically downstream of our neurons of interest by performing anterograde trans-synaptic tracing43 on two of our lines that drove upwind movement (MB052B and LH1396); we observed signal in the dorsal layers of the FB in both cases (Fig. 3A).
To ask whether the FB plays a role in wind-guided navigation, we performed an activation screen of ~40 lines labeling FB input neurons, including dorsal and ventral FB tangential inputs, and columnar PFNs, as well as additional CX neurons (Fig. 3B, Fig. S3). We performed this screen using genetically blind flies (see Methods) and in the presence of teashirt-Gal8044 to reduce potential Chrimson expression in the ventral-nerve cord (VNC) (see Methods). We found that 4 lines labeling FB tangential inputs, but no lines labeling PFNs, generated significant movement upwind (Fig. 3B, S4A). Two dorsal FB input lines that were previously shown to promote sleep (23E10 and 84C1045) did not produce any wind-oriented movement in our assay, although we did observe a decrease in groundspeed in 23E10. FB tangential lines driving upwind phenotypes targeted both dorsal and ventral layers of the FB. We attempted to refine these lines by making split-Gal4 drivers from combinations of these hemidrivers, but only one of these, labeling a set of ventral FB tangential inputs, also drove an upwind phenotype (Fig. 3C). Most split-Gal4 drivers labeled only a very small number of neurons.
In addition to the lines identified through our screen, we identified the neuron FB5AB using the connectome (Fig. 3D, E). This single pair of neurons stood out as the only FB input that receives at least one direct synaptic input from each of the MBON lines with wind navigation phenotypes (Fig. 3D). In addition, FB5AB receives the largest number of di-synaptic LH inputs from vinegar-responsive glomeruli of any FB input neuron (Fig. 3E, see Methods). We identified a GAL4 line that labels FB5AB neurons (21D07). As 21D07 labels some neurons in the antennal lobe, a primary olfactory area, we used the cell class-lineage intersection (CLIN46) technique to limit the expression of Chrimson to neurons in the FB (see Methods). High intensity light activation of this driver, which weakly but specifically labels FB5AB, also drove upwind orientation (Figs. 3C, 4A, B).
Overall, upwind velocity responses to FB tangential input stimulation were more persistent than those evoked by optogenetic activation in the MB and LH, continuing to promote upwind displacement after light OFF, rather than evoking search behavior (Figs. 3F, 4A, S4A). We characterized the neurotransmitter phenotypes of each of the hits from our screen and found that all were cholinergic, and thus excitatory (Fig, S4B). As in the MB and LH, silencing of individual FB input lines that drove upwind movement was not sufficient to block upwind movement in response to odor (Fig. S4C). Together these results support the hypothesis that patterns of population activity in FB tangential inputs can promote upwind movement.
We next sought to characterize the sensory responses of upwind-promoting FB tangential inputs (Fig. 4A, B) by performing calcium imaging in response to wind and odor from different directions (Fig. 4C, S4D). We observed responses to vinegar in all but one line (45D04). No FB tangential line showed significant directional tuning (ANOVA: 21D07: F(4,40) = 2.14, p = 0.0938, 65C03: F(4,30) = 0.68, p = 0.6096, 12D12: F(4,25) = 0.13, p = 0.971, vFB split: F(4,25) = 0.64, p = 0.6358) nor were tree classifiers trained on their odor responses able to distinguish between odor delivered from the left, right or center (Fig. 4D, S4E). In contrast, tree classifiers trained to distinguish odor ON from wind ON performed better than shuffled controls for 2 of 4 lines (vFB split and FB5AB, Fig. 4D), while all performed better than shuffled control when trained to distinguish odor from baseline (Fig. S4F). The largest odor responses were observed in FB5AB (21D07), although these (but not other FB tangential line responses) decayed over trials (Fig. 4E). In the line 65C03, we observed odor responses only from the dorsal layers of the FB, while in the line 12D12, we observed odor responses only from the ventral layers of the FB. Together these data identify a population of olfactory FB tangential inputs, targeting multiple layers of the FB, that respond to attractive odor and promote upwind movement.
h∆C neurons encode a wind direction signal that is modulated ON by odor
Our functional imaging data suggest that, like the upwind-promoting neurons in the MB/LH, FB tangential inputs are not directionally tuned for wind. In contrast, PFNa and LNa encode wind direction, but not the presence of vinegar (Fig. 2F). Together these data suggest that columnar and tangential inputs to the FB encode directional and non-directional information, respectively. We therefore hypothesized that FB local neurons, which receive input from both columnar and tangential inputs, might integrate these two types of information.
To identify FB local neurons that integrate odor and wind direction signals, we used the hemibrain connectome22 to search for neurons that receive input both from FB5AB and from the most wind-sensitive PFNs in our previous survey (PFNa, p, and m21). This analysis revealed a population of 20 h∆C neurons that tile the vertical columnar structure of the FB (Fig. 5A31). Each h∆C neuron receives input from both left and right-preferring wind-sensitive PFNs at its ventral dendrites, and from FB5AB at its dorsal axons, where excitatory olfactory input might gate synaptic output through axo-axonic connections (Fig. 5B, C).
To ask whether h∆C neurons respond to wind and odor signals, we identified a GAL4 line—VT062617—that appears to label h∆C neurons (although it may also label other local neuron types), and performed calcium imaging from the FB while presenting odor and wind from different directions. Stains revealed these neurons to be cholinergic (Fig. S5A). We observed calcium responses in the dorsal FB, where h∆C neurons form output tufts (Fig. 5D, S5B). In several examples, we observed odor responses that were strongest for a particular direction of wind (Fig. 5E). h∆C responses were strongest to odorized wind from +45° and −45° (Fig. 5F), although the relationship between wind direction and peak response was not consistent across flies (Fig. 5E). h∆C responses were typically localized to a few nearby columns (Fig. 5G), and we observed no consistent relationship between column location and preferred wind direction when pooling data across flies (Fig. S5C). Thus, the wind direction representation in h∆C neurons appears to be unique to each fly, similar to what has been observed for heading representations in compass neurons47, and distinct from the wind representation in PFNs, which is uniformly tuned to 45° ipsilateral21.
We next examined h∆C responses as a function of stimulus phase. Across flies and columns, the strongest and most consistent responses occurred during the odor, although weaker responses also occurred at wind ON, odor OFF, and wind OFF (Fig. 5H). To examine whether wind tuning differed across these phases, we selected the maximally responsive column for each fly, and aligned responses to the peak direction of that response, then plotted responses to other phases of the stimulus relative to that wind direction. Responses in other stimulus phases tended to be in the same direction or nearby directions to the odor response (Fig. 5I). Taken together, our results suggest that h∆C neurons exhibit a bump of activity that depends on wind direction, but whose columnar position is unique for each fly. This bump is activated most strongly during odor, although it can also appear more weakly during other phases of the stimulus.
h∆C neurons promote diverse navigation phenotypes and contribute to persistent upwind walking
Do h∆C neurons contribute to navigation behavior? To address this question, we activated hΔC neurons optogenetically using various spatial activation patterns, and examined the resulting behavioral trajectories (Fig. 6). Because our imaging data suggest that h∆C neurons show a bump of activity during odor and wind stimulation, we first asked what behavior was produced by sparsely activating h∆C neurons. We used SPARC2-I48 to activate a random ~15% of h∆C neurons in each fly while they walked in the presence of laminar wind. Activation of sparse subsets of hΔC neurons (Fig, 6A) caused many flies to re-orient and then walk in a specific reproducible direction (Fig. 6B, C). Each fly walked in a distinct direction, suggesting that this was specified by the particular pattern of h∆C neurons activated in that fly (Fig. 6A, B). Directions were biased towards the long axis of our arena, but equally distributed up- and downwind (Fig. 6C). Directional walking was not due to the arena walls, as fly behavior near the walls was excluded from our analysis (see Methods). To assess the significance of this directional walking, we compared the strength of orientation in h∆C > SPARC flies to empty-GAL4 > SPARC flies (Fig. 6C, D, S6A). Empty-Gal4>SPARC flies showed significantly weaker orientation behavior (Fig. 6D). To determine whether the pattern of h∆C activation was related to walking direction, we dissected and stained each h∆C > SPARC fly. However, we observed no relationship between the anatomy of the activated columns, and the direction or strength of oriented walking (Fig S6A-E). We interpret these results to suggest that sparse activation in h∆C neurons can produce a reliable heading, and that this heading is encoded in fly-specific (not fixed) coordinates.
In addition, we asked whether broad activation of h∆C neurons could drive a behavioral phenotype. For these experiments, we generated a split-GAL4 line selectively labeling FB local neurons within VT062617, and expressed Chrimson throughout this population (Fig. 6E). Activation of this line caused unstable reorientation behaviors. At medium light levels (26 µW/mm2), fly walking was interrupted by frequent left and right turns, while at higher light levels (34 µW/mm2), flies displayed tight turns that were clockwise, counterclockwise, or alternating between clockwise and counterclockwise (Fig. 6F). Across flies, these behaviors were captured by an increase in curvature with no change in upwind velocity (Fig. 6G). Similar increases in curvature were obtained using a variety of drivers for h∆C neurons (Fig. S6F). Therefore, uniform activation of hΔC neurons causes unstable reorientation behaviors.
Finally we asked whether h∆C activity is required for upwind orientation. In preliminary experiments, we found constitutive silencing of h∆C neurons to be lethal. We therefore used the light-activated chloride channel GtACR49 to acutely silence h∆C neurons during odor presentation. We compared the effects of acutely silencing h∆C neurons to silencing of olfactory receptor neurons using orco,IR8a and FB5AB. Consistent with our constitutive silencing results (Fig. 1F, G), we found that acute silencing of olfactory receptor neurons impaired upwind orientation throughout the odor period (Fig. 6H). In contrast, silencing of FB5AB had no effect on upwind orientation, as we observed for other FB tangential inputs (Fig. 6H, Fig. S4C). In flies with h∆C neurons silenced, we observed normal upwind orientation early in the odor period, however trajectories then deviated significantly from upwind later in the odor period (Fig. 6H, I). We conclude that h∆C activity is required for persistent upwind fixation throughout the odor stimulus.
A computational model of h∆C neurons’ contribution to navigation behavior
Our data suggest that h∆C activity contributes to persistent upwind orientation during odor and that sparse activation of h∆C neurons can promote navigation in a reproducible direction. To understand how different patterns of h∆C neuron activation can evoke diverse behavioral phenotypes, we developed a computational model. The elements of our model are all based on neurons and connection motifs (direct or indirect) found in the hemibrain connectome (Fig. 7A22,31), although we imagine that additional neurons and connections may play a role in natural olfactory navigation behavior.
We first asked whether odor-gated wind direction information in h∆C neurons could be used to promote upwind movement. Although the nature of the wind representation in h∆C neurons is not entirely clear from our imaging experiments, our results are broadly consistent with a bump of activity that is tuned to wind-direction in a fly-specific manner, likely reflecting the fly-specific coordinates of the compass in each animal47. Thus, we imagine that h∆C neurons might show a bump of activity whose location reflects the allocentric wind direction (Fig. 7B), as has previously been described for traveling direction signals in other FB local neurons23,24. Alternatively, h∆C might show a bump of activity related to wind direction only in the frontal hemisphere, which could be computed based on the known frontally tuned responses of wind-sensitive PFNs (Fig. S7A, B). In either case, in our model, this bump becomes active when the fly encounters odor, due to gating input from FB5AB and other FB tangential neurons, although it could, in principle, also be activated at other times.
Activity in h∆C neurons is then translated into locomotor commands by a set of previously described output neurons: PFL3 and PFL2 (Fig. 7C, see Methods). PFL3 neurons project unilaterally to the right or left LAL and are thought to drive turning31–34,50 while PFL2 neurons project bilaterally to the right and left LAL and are hypothesized to modulate forward walking speed31. PFL neurons compare the representation arriving from h∆C neurons (a bump of activity flipped by 180° due to hΔC neurons projecting to columns halfway across the FB) with a shifted representation of the fly’s current heading arriving from compass neurons31,33. The heading representation in PFL3 neurons is shifted by −90° for right PFL3s and +90° for left PFL3s. This allows PFL3 neurons to determine whether a left or right turn will bring the fly in line with the heading specified by the h∆C bump. For example, wind to the right of the fly will generate a wind bump that overlaps more with the right PFL3 heading bump than the left (Fig. 7C). This system will cause the fly to turn until its heading bump aligns with the h∆C wind bump. In parallel, PFL2 neurons cause the fly to accelerate when the heading from h∆C overlaps with the compass heading representation. This heading representation is shifted by 180°, so it will overlap the wind bump when the wind is directly in front of the fly. Thus, PFL2 neurons drive the fly to walk faster when it is pointed upwind.
We added one additional element to our model that has not been previously described. This is a second set of mutually inhibitory FB local neurons (light blue in Fig. 7C) that receive input from h∆C neurons and also send information to the PFL output system. This motif is not observed in h∆C neurons but reciprocal connections between opposing columns can be seen in several other FB local neuron types downstream of h∆C, such as h∆A, h∆G, h∆H, and h∆M, which all send strong projections to PFL3. As we show below, this motif is required to account for the unstable reorientation behavior we observe during broad activation of h∆C neurons.
We first asked whether this simplified FB model could promote upwind movement when the h∆C wind bump is activated (Fig. 7D, E). We simulated the circuit described above using rate-based neurons (see Methods), and simulated odor input by turning on the bump of wind activity in hΔC neurons. As predicted, the odor-gated wind bump in h∆C neurons generated stable upwind orientation that was robust to allocentric wind direction and initial heading (Fig. 7E). Flies also oriented upwind using a frontal wind representation in h∆C, although this was less reliable than using an allocentric representation (Fig. S7A-D). We did not observe local search after odor OFF in our simulations, consistent with our finding that FB tangential inputs do not drive this behavior.
We next asked whether the same model could recapitulate the directional walking we observed with sparse random h∆C activation. To simulate the sparse activation experiment, we replaced the hΔC wind bump with random activation of 15% of the h∆C population in a fly-specific pattern (Fig. 7F). Similar to our behavioral results, simulated flies reoriented and then walked in an arbitrary but reproducible direction, where the direction was stable for each fly. This result occurred because the random optogenetic input provided stable non-uniform input to the PFL3 neurons, and the maximum of this input (i.e., the FB columns opposite to the highest density of ‘active’ hΔC neurons) establishes a stable direction. These directions were distributed in 360° space (Fig. 7G) because our simulation lacked arena constraints. Oriented walking was significantly reduced when we simulated empty-GAL4 > SPARC controls by omitting h∆C activity from our simulation (Fig. 7G).
Finally we asked whether our model could reproduce the unstable reorientation we observed with broad h∆C activation (Fig. 7H). To simulate these experiments, we uniformly activated hΔC neurons at two different intensities, corresponding to the two light levels in our experiment. Like our behavioral results, low activation generated walking interrupted with left and right turns, while high activation generated tight turns that were clockwise, counterclockwise, or alternating between clockwise and counterclockwise. Consistent with our experiments, simulated flies displayed elevated curvature that depended on the light level (Fig. 7I). The unstable reorientation behaviors in our simulations stemmed from oscillations within the mutual inhibition layer of the circuit; in the absence of this layer, uniform activation did not alter turning statistics (Fig. S7E–G). Taken together, our experiments and modeling suggest that sensory representations in FB local neurons such as h∆C can provide a goal heading that promotes stable navigation towards an environmental target such as an upwind odor source.
Discussion
Distinct direction and context pathways for olfactory navigation
Wind-guided olfactory navigation is an ancient and conserved behavior used by many organisms to locate odor sources in turbulent environments. Despite large differences in the types of odors they seek, and in the physics of odor dispersal, very similar behaviors have been observed in pheromone-tracking moths51 food-seeking crustaceans8 and ants seeking both food and their nest52. Thus, this behavior is required in many animals for survival and reproduction, and likely mediated by conserved neural circuits.
In a previous study, we identified inputs to the FB that encode wind direction21. Neurons throughout this pathway, including both LNa neurons and PFNs, show activity that is strongly modulated by wind direction, but weakly modulated by odor. Other LN and PFN neurons have been shown to strongly encode optic flow direction23,33 another cue that signals self-motion and wind direction—particularly in flight. Taken together, current data thus suggest that the columnar pathway to the FB encodes information about self-movement and environmental flow useful for recognizing where the fly is relative to its environment.
In contrast, the present study identifies an olfactory pathway to the FB that encodes odor information largely independent of the wind direction from which it is delivered. We found that neurons in both the MB (a center for associative learning) and the LH (a center for innate olfactory and multi-modal processing) are capable of driving upwind movement. However, very few of these upwind-promoting neurons exhibited wind-direction tuning, particularly during the odor period. Thus, the outputs of these regions likely represent odor identity or value signals, as has been suggested by several recent models42,53. We further characterized a large population of olfactory tangential inputs to the FB that likewise promote upwind movement and encode odor independent of wind direction. At least one of these neurons is anatomically downstream of upwind-promoting MB and LH neurons. These data are consistent with previous studies showing that other FB tangential inputs encode non-spatial variables such as sleep state45 and food choice54. Together, these data suggest that the pathway running from the MB/LH to the FB encodes non-spatial contextual information.
The segregation of direction and context inputs to the FB that we observe here shows some similarities to the organization of visual circuits in primates and of navigation circuits in rodents. In primate vision, processing of object location and object identity are famously thought to occur in distinct pathways55. In the hippocampus, inputs from the medial entorhinal cortex encode spatial cues, while anatomically distinct inputs from the lateral entorhinal cortex encode non-spatial context cues, including odors56,57. Thus, a segregation between the computation of spatial/directional information and context/identity information may be a general feature of central neural processing in both vertebrate and invertebrate brains.
What might be the advantage of this type of organization (Fig. 8)? One hypothesis is that it allows learning about non-spatial cues to be generalized to different spatial contexts. For example, during food-guided search, innate information about which odors signal palatable food must be integrated with learned odor associations13,14 as well as with internal state variables such as hunger54,58. However, the fly may wish to generalize information learned while walking to search in flight, as has been demonstrated in bees59. In walking flies, wind direction can be computed directly from mechanosensors in the antennae60,61. In flight however, wind only transiently activates mechanosensors62 but then displaces the fly as a whole, leading to an optic flow signal opposed to the fly’s direction of movement9,63. As different PFNs carry airflow and optic flow signals in a similar format21,23 this circuit may be well-poised to compute wind direction in both walking and flying flies. Separating the computation of stimulus value, in the MB/LH and tangential FB inputs, from the computation of wind direction, in PFNs, may allow flies to generalize stimulus associations learned in one context (walking) to another (flight).
Specification of a goal direction by FB local neurons
How does the nervous system represent goals for navigation? When sensory cues are continuously available, no explicit goal representation may be required, and movement towards a goal such as food may be accomplished through chains of sensory-motor reflexes9–11. In contrast, when sensory cues are unavailable, such as during navigation towards a remembered shelter, the nervous system must build and store a representation of the goal location33. Navigation towards an odor source presents an interesting intermediate case. Food sources generally produce strong sensory cues that can be used to drive chained reflexes. However, the wind direction and odor cues produced by turbulent plumes are fluctuating, variable, and uncertain, meaning that memory and internal estimates of source position can make an important contribution to effective navigation64–66. Understanding how nervous systems represent and use internal variables during this task may help us to design robust search algorithms for noisy environments.
Here we present evidence that h∆C neurons participate in specifying a goal heading during olfactory navigation. Silencing of h∆C neurons impairs persistent upwind orientation during odor, suggesting that the activity of these neurons is required to maintain upwind fixation. h∆C neurons receive input from an odor-tuned, upwind-promoting pathway through the MB/LH to FB tangential neurons. Our imaging results are consistent with a model in which h∆C neurons represent wind direction (likely through inputs from wind-sensitive PFNs) and modulate the strength of this representation during odor (perhaps through axo-axonic gating input from FB tangential neurons). Thus, h∆C neurons are well-poised to construct an internal representation of the goal direction during olfactory navigation. However, understanding the precise nature of wind, odor, and goal representations in h∆C neurons will require future imaging of population activity during ongoing navigation.
We also show here that sparse random activation of h∆C neurons can evoke locomotion in a reproducible direction, consistent with the idea that h∆C activity patterns can specify a goal heading. We were unable to determine from our data whether h∆C activation specifies a distance (and thus a vector) as well as a direction, although many trajectories from the same fly were of similar length. Curiously, we found that h∆C activity had to be non-uniform in order to evoke directional walking, but need not be organized as a bump of activity—any asymmetry in the response across columns was sufficient. Although we produced this activity through sparse optogenetic stimulation, similar asymmetries could be generated and stored as patterns of asymmetric synaptic input from FB tangential neurons onto FB local neurons, or as asymmetric synaptic weights between interconnected FB local neurons. Because the Drosophila FB contains at least 27 local neuron types targeted by ~145 tangential neuron types31 this structure could provide a reasonably large capacity for storing diverse direction or vector memories.
To understand how h∆C activity might specify a goal heading, either upwind or in an arbitrary reproducible direction, we developed a computational model. This model is based on motifs found in the Drosophila connectome, such as the phase shifts between compass neurons and PFL neurons31,33 and mutual connections found in certain local neuron populations. However, the connection schemes used in our model are simplified compared to the real circuit. In our model, the architecture of the FB is used to compare a goal heading in h∆C neurons with the current heading represented in compass neurons, and then drive turning to minimize discrepancies between these directions. This computation is implemented by the convergence of hΔC input and compass neuron input onto PFL output neurons that control steering and forward velocity. Our steering model is similar to several recent models proposed for path integration and visual landmark-guided navigation31–33. However, our model differs in the proposed location and nature of the goal representation. In previous models, goals were stored in PFN activity33 or in plastic synapses between compass neurons and PFLs32. Here the goal is instead stored in the dynamic activity pattern of a population of local neurons. One advantage of our model is that different local neurons can be rapidly switched on and off through tangential input, allowing the fly to rapidly update its goals depending on behavioral demands. A further advantage is the large number of local neurons and tangential inputs, which as noted above provide a substrate for learning, storing, and releasing multiple goal memories.
Although our results implicate h∆C neurons in the integration of odor and wind cues and the specification of a target walking direction, flies with h∆C neurons silenced were still able to initially turn upwind in response to odor, suggesting that other neurons play a role in this initial upwind turn. These might be additional FB local neuron types—we identified multiple FB tangential inputs that drive upwind movement and not all of these target h∆C. They may also include neurons that bypass the central complex. In addition to targeting the FB, MBONs also make direct connections to the lateral accessory lobe (LAL) a region implicated in motor control and steering, including odor-evoked steering behavior in moths25,26,50,67. As the LAL also receives wind direction input68 it could form a second site of wind and odor integration. In this study, we found that activation of FB neurons produced more persistent wind orientation phenotypes than activation of MB/ LH output neurons. Thus, parallel pathways from olfactory centers to motor centers could regulate behavior on different timescales. FB representations of odor and wind might also allow a fly to adopt courses at particular angles to the wind, or to generate an internal estimate of the direction of the odor source. Determining how activity in these different pathways shapes behavior on different timescales, and in different spatial contexts, will provide additional insight into the organization of central circuits for navigation.
Methods
Our research complies with all relevant ethical regulations. Because only invertebrate organisms were used in this study, no protocol approval was required.
Fly stocks
A complete table of fly genotypes and ages for each figure panel is given in Table S1. Information on parental stocks is given in Table S2. All experimental flies (except trans-tango flies) were raised at 25 °C on a standard cornmeal-agar medium, with a 12 h light-dark cycle. For optogenetic activation, experiments were run in genetically blind male norpA hemizygotes. All other flies were female. We previously showed no difference in olfactory behavior of male versus female flies in our assay10. For calcium imaging, we used older flies (5–21 days) to maximize indicator expression. For electrophysiology, we used younger flies (2–3 days) to minimize glial ensheathing. Trans-tango flies were raised at 18 °C and aged until they were 10–20 days old following recommended protocols43.
Behavioral experiments
Behavioral experiments were performed in a miniature wind tunnel setup modified from ref. 10 (plans and code at https://github.com/nagellab/AlvarezSalvado_ElementaryTransformations). Flies walked in a shallow acrylic arena with constant laminar airflow at ~12 cm/s and were tracked using IR LEDS (850 nm, Environmental Lights) and a camera (Basler acA1920-155um). For odor experiments, a 10 s pulse of 1% apple cider vinegar (Pastorelli) was introduced through solenoid valves (LHDA1233115HA, Lee Company) immediately below the arena. Three 70 s trial conditions (wind plus odor, wind alone, and no wind), were randomly interleaved with odor starting at 30 s. For Chrimson experiments, we used red LEDs (626 nm, SuperBrightLEDS) at an intensity of 26 µW/mm2 (626 nm) for most experiments, and 34 µW/mm2 for high light level and CLIN experiments, all with ambient lighting, except for SPARC experiment which were run in the dark. Four trial conditions (wind plus light pulse, light pulse alone, wind alone, blank) were randomly interleaved. For GtACR experiments, we used blue LEDs (470 nm, Environmental Lights) at an intensity of 58 µW/mm2 (530 nm), and ran experiments in the dark. Four trial conditions (wind plus light pulse, wind plus odor pulse, wind plus odor and light, wind only) were randomly interleaved.
Two to seven days before the experiment, flies were collected and housed in time-shifted boxes. All flies were run between subjective ZT1–ZT4 and starved for ~24 h before the experiment. Fly food for optogenetic experiments was supplemented with 50 µL all-trans retinal (35 mM stock: Sigma, R2500, dissolved in ethanol, stored at −20 °C) mixed into ~1 teaspoon of hydrated potato flakes. Flies were briefly anaesthetized with ice for ~1 min before loading and allowed 5–10 min to recover before the experiment began.
We tracked X,Y coordinates and orientation in real time at 50 Hz using custom Labview code then analyzed data offline using Matlab (Labview code at https://github.com/nagellab/AlvarezSalvado_ElementaryTransformations:, Matlab code at https://github.com/nagellab/Mathesonetal2022). We low-pass filtered raw data at 2.5 Hz using a 2-pole Butterworth filter and removed trials where the fly moved <25 mm, and flies that moved on <5 trials for a condition. Odor trials were aligned to the time of actual odor encounter based on PID measurements. To compute behavioral parameters as a function of time, we omitted time periods when the fly moved at <1 mm/s. Groundspeed was calculated as the distance between adjacent X,Y samples divided by the frame interval (20 ms). Upwind velocity was the difference in Y-coordinates divided by the frame interval. Angular velocity was the absolute value of the difference in unwrapped orientation divided by the frame interval. Curvature was the angular velocity divided by the groundspeed. Probability of movement (pmove) was the probability of groundspeed being above 1 mm/s. For quantifications, we compared average parameter values for each fly to a baseline period (10–25 s after trial onset). For place preference baseline was 25–30 s (immediately before stimulus ON).
We used the following time windows for analysis: pmove: 0–5 s from stimulus ON, upwind velocity (upwind): 0–5 s from stimulus ON for periphery, LH, MB, 0–10 s from stimulus ON for FB, OFF upwind velocity (upwindoff): 0–2 s after stimulus OFF, groundspeed: 2s–5s from stimulus ON, OFF groundspeed: 0–2 s after stimulus OFF, angular velocity (angv): 2s–5s from stimulus ON, OFF angular velocity (angvoff): 0–2 s after stimulus OFF, ON angular velocity (angvon): 0–1 s from stimulus ON, curvature: 2s–5s from stimulus ON, OFF curvature (curvatureoff): 0–2 s after stimulus OFF, ON curvature (curvature on): 0–1 s from stimulus ON, place preference (placepref): 7.5 s from stimulus ON to 2.5 s after stimulus OFF. For display purposes we do not depict significance values in tables (Fig. 3B, C, Fig. S3) for upwind velocity for small magnitude increases (<1 mm/s), or for curvature (ON and OFF) if angular velocity is <50 deg/s and groundspeed is <4 mm/s. In these cases, curvature increase was due mostly to a drop in groundspeed and trajectories did not exhibit characteristics of search. To measure upwind displacement (Fig. 3F) we computed displacement relative to position at stimulus OFF for each fly and show the mean across flies. For orientation histograms (Figs. 6H, 7F, S6B) we plotted the histogram of orientations that each fly adopted while moving >1 mm/s. We compared the probability of adopting an orientation ±10˚ from upwind between odor and odor+light conditions using a student’s t-test.
For SPARC experiments, we analyzed the orientation index and preferred direction using data from 2–6 s from stimulus ON. We excluded all data points where the fly was within 3 mm of the arena walls as well as points where they fly moved at <1 mm/s. The orientation index was computed by taking the SVD of the orientation histogram, projecting the data onto the two largest principal components (PCs), and then calculating the ratio of the standard deviations along these two dimensions. Histograms that show more pronounced orientation exhibit higher ratios (see Fig. S6A). Preferred direction was the direction along the first PC with the highest value. Flies with fewer than 15 trials or fewer than 2000 orientation data points were excluded from analysis. We obtained anatomy for 51 h∆C > SPARC flies. We quantified anatomy by counting cell body numbers near the FB. To compute the expression vector (Fig. S6B), we drew an ROI around the dorsal FB layer containing h∆C output tufts, split this ROI into 12 columns, converted normalized expression across these columns into a 360° polar plot, then converted the polar plot into a vector.
Calcium imaging
For calcium imaging, we anaesthetized flies on ice and mounted them in a holder (https://ptweir.github.io/flyHolder/) using UV glue. We stabilized the head, proboscis, and body, removed the two front legs, and dissected the cuticle over the back of the head, removing trachea, airsacs, and muscles that covered the brain. The brain was bathed in extracellular saline (103 mM NaCl, 3 mM KCl, 5 mM TES, 8 mM trehalose dihydrate, 10 mM glucose, 26 mM NaHCO3, 1 mM NaH2PO4H20, 1.5 mM CaCl22H2O, and 4 mM MgCl26H2O, pH 7.1–7.4, osmolarity 270–274 mOsm) bubbled with carbogen (5% CO2, 95% O2) during dissection and imaging.
2-photon imaging was performed using a pulsed infrared laser (Mai Tai DeepSea, Spectraphysics) at 920 nm with a Bergamo II microscope (Thorlabs), using a 20× water immersion objective (Olympus XLUMPLFLN 20×) and ThorImage 3.0 software. Power at the sample was 13–66 mW. Emitted photons were spectrally separated using red (tdTOM, 607/70 nm) and green (GCamp6f, 525/50 nm) bandpass filters and detected by GaAsP PMTs. Imaging area ranged from 47 × 47 µM to 122 × 74 µM and was identified using the tdTOM signal under epifluorescence. Images were acquired at ~5.0 frames per second. We excluded flies where we were unable to obtain 5 trials of each stimulus direction, 2 65C03 flies which showed rhythmic spike-like activity and did not respond to any phase of our stimulus, and one VT062617 fly in which no columns responded >2STD above baseline.
Wind and odor stimuli were delivered using a custom five-direction manifold61 (MB052B, LH1396, 65C03-GAL4, 21D07, vFB split, and VT062617) or rotary union apparatus21 (MB077B, MB082C, and 12D12). Charcoal-filtered air was passed through a flowmeter (Cole-Parmer) to direct air and odor at the fly from one of five directions using either proportional valves (manifold, EV-05-0905; Clippard Instruments) or solenoid valves (rotary union, LHDA1233115HA, Lee Company). Odor (apple cider vinegar, 10%) was diluted in distilled water on the day of the experiment. Airflow was ~25 cm/s for manifold experiments and 40 cm/s for rotary union experiments and maintained at a constant level throughout the experiment. Trials consisted of 5–10 s without stimuli, 10 s of wind alone, 10 s of odorized wind, 10 s of wind alone followed by 8–10 s of no stimulus. Odor/wind direction was randomized in blocks of five trials, with five blocks per fly (25 total trials). Imaging position, z-plane, gain, and power levels were adjusted as needed between blocks.
To extract time courses from calcium imaging data, we first used the CalmAn package69 (Matlab) to implement the NoRMCorre rigid motion correction algorithm70. Motion correction was performed on the red (tdTOM) time series, then applied to the green (GCaMP6f) times series. ROIs were drawn by hand in ImageJ on a maximum intensity projection of the first trial of the tdTom time series, then manually adjusted for drift between trials. We targeted the following regions in each line. LH1396: dendritic processes in the LH; MB052B, MB082C and MB077B: putative axonal processes in the protocerebrum; FB5AB, 65C03, 12D12, and vFB split: FB layer innervated by each line. For h∆C imaging (VT062617) imaging, we drew ROIs across eight putative columns of the FB based on glomerular structure in the tdTom signal. ROIs were imported into MATLAB using ReadImageJROI71. ∆F/F was computed by dividing the green (GCaMP6f) time series by the average fluorescence of the baseline period (first 5 s of the trial, excluding the first sample due to shutter lag). Traces were resampled to 5 Hz if frame rate varied between experiments to compute means. Supplemental heat maps were normalized to maximum response across all trials within an individual fly.
Electrophysiology
For whole cell patch clamp recordings, flies were mounted in a holder with hot wax and dissected. We used collagenase (5% in extracellular saline, Worthington Biochemical Corporation Collagenase Type 4) to remove the sheath over the brain. We visualized cell bodies using a 40× objective (Olympus, LUMPLFLN40XW), an LED source (Cairn Research MONOLED), and a filter (U-N19002 AT-GFP/F LP C164404), and cleaned them using extracellular saline and light positive pressure.
Glass pipettes (6–10 µOhms, World Precision Instruments 1B150F-3) were pulled using a Sutter P-1000 puller and filled with intracellular solution (140 mM KOH, 140 mM aspartic acid, 10 mM HEPES, 1 mM EGTA, 1 mM KCl, 4 mM MgATP, 0.5 mM Na3GFP, and 13 mM biocytin hydrazide). Current and voltage signals were amplified using either an A-M systems Model 2400 amplifier with Brownlee Precision 410 preamplifier or a Molecular Devices Multiclamp 700B and digitized at 10 kz. We recorded from a total of 12 MBONs, 6 on each side that met our criteria of access:input ratio great than 5:1. Resting membrane potential ranged from −34.6 mV to −25.2 mV (mean −30.8 mV).
Stimuli for electrophysiology were delivered using simple olfactometer comprising a charcoal filter, flowmeter, and Teflon tube (4 mm OD, 2.5 mm ID) positioned <1 mm from the fly head on the right side. Solenoid valves (Lee company, LFAA1201610H) switched between humidified and odorized air. Odor pulses shown (10% apple cider vinegar) are 10 s long. Total airflow was 1.3 L/min. Membrane potential was extracted in Matlab by applying a 2.5 Hz low-pass Butterworth filter to raw voltage signals. Responses during the first 4 s of the odor period were compared to 2 s of baseline during wind only.
Immunohistochemistry
Dissected brains were fixed for 15 min in 4% paraformaldehyde (in 1× PBS), washed 3× in PBS and stored at 4 °C until staining (within 2 weeks). To stain, fixed brains were incubated in 5% normal goat serum in PBST (1× PBS with 0.2% Triton-X) for 20–60 min, incubated overnight at room temperature in primary antibody (Table 1), washed 3× in PBST, incubated overnight in secondary antibody (Table 1), washed 3× in PBST then stored in PBS at 4 °C until imaging. Mounted brains were covered in vectashield (Vector Labs H-1000), sealed with coverslips, and imaged using a 20× objective (Zeiss W Plan-Apochromat 20×/1.0 DIC CG 0.17 M27 75 mm) on a Zeiss LSM 800 confocal microscope at 1–1.25 μM depth resolution. Images presented are maximum z-projections over relevant depths.
Table 1.
Antibody | Source | Dilution | Identifier |
---|---|---|---|
Chicken anti-GFP | Fisher Scientific | 1:50 | RRID:AB_1074893 |
Mouse anti-nc82 | DSHB | 1:50 | RRID:AB_2314866 |
Rabbit anti-DsRed | Clontech | 1:500 | 632496 |
Rabbit anti-GABA | Sigma | 1:100 | RRID:AB_477652 |
Alexa488-conjugated goat anti-chicken | Fisher Scientific | 1:250 | RRID:AB_2534096 |
Alexa633-conjugated goat anti-mouse | Fisher Scientific | 1:250 | RRID:AB_2535719 |
Alexa568-conjugated goat anti-rabbit | Fisher Scientific | 1:250 | RRID:AB_2315774 |
Alexa568-conjugated streptavidin | Fisher Scientific | 1:1000 | RRID:AB_2576217 |
We dissected, stained, and imaged the following number of brains to determine expression patterns: Fig. 3a: MB052B > transtango N = 2, LH1396 > transtango N = 3, Fig. 4A: FB5AB (21D07||CLIN) > Chrimson N = 3, 65C03 > Chrimson N = 1, 12D12 > Chrimson N = 2, vFB split>Chrimson N = 1. Figure 6A: h∆C > SPARC N = 51, Fig. 6E: h∆C split>Chrimson N = 1, Fig. S4B: 21D07||ChAT N = 2, 21D07 GABA N = 2, 65C03||ChAT N = 1, 65C03||GAD1 N = 1, 12D12||ChAT N = 1, 12D12||GAD1 N = 1, vFBsplit||ChAT N = 3 Fig. S5A: VT062617||ChAT N = 2, VT062617||GAD1 N = 1. For Chrimson experiments, we dissected females from the same cross as experimental males.
Connectomic analysis
Data from the hemibrain connectome22 were obtained from neuprint explorer (https://neuprint.janelia.org, hemibrain:v1.1) and analyzed using MATLAB and Python (see Code availability). For the analysis shown in Fig. 5B, the location of FB5AB synapses onto h∆C was determined by their x-position, with no filtering or constraints. For the analysis in Fig. 4B, we counted all FB tangential neurons two synapses downstream of the following projection neurons: VM7d_adPN, VM7v_adPN, DM1_lPN, DM4_adPN, DM4_vPN, VA2_adPN, DP1l_adPN, DP1l_vPN, DL2d_adPN, DL2d_vPN, DL2v_adPN, DC4_adPN, DC4_vPN, DP1m_adPN, DP1m_vPN in which the intermediate neurons contained LH (they were lateral horn neurons) and where synaptic weights exceeded a weight of 3.
Modeling
The FB steering circuit model implemented in this study contains three types of inputs—heading, wind, and odor; two intermediate layers corresponding to hΔC and mutual inhibition local neurons; and an output layer corresponding to left PFL3, right PFL3, and PFL2 neurons, which modulate angular velocity and walking speed. Heading inputs go directly to PFL3 and PFL2, while odor and wind inputs are integrated by hΔC neurons, processed by the mutual inhibition layer, and then integrated by PFL3 and PFL2. Angular velocity depends on the relative activity of left and right PFL3, while speed depends on the activity of PFL2. All modeling was performed in Matlab (see Code availability). Model parameters are given in Table 2. All dynamic systems were computationally approximated using Euler’s method of integration with a time step of 0.05 s.
Table 2.
Parameter | Value | Units | Role | Source |
---|---|---|---|---|
phase_shiftleft_PFL3 | −90 | deg | Anatomical shift of left PFL3 neurons entering FB | 31 |
phase_shiftright_PFL3 | 90 | deg | Anatomical shift of right PFL3 neurons entering FB | 31 |
phase_shiftPFL2 | 180 | deg | Anatomical shift of PFL2 neurons entering FB | 31 |
phase_shiftleft_PFN | 45 | deg | Anatomical shift of left PFN neurons entering FB | 31 |
phase_shiftright_PFN | −45 | deg | Anatomical shift of right PFN neurons entering FB | 31 |
peakleft | −45 | deg | Peak wind direction tuning of left PFNs | 21 |
peakright | 45 | deg | Peak wind direction tuning of right PFNs | 21 |
optogenetic_power | Low: 0.75, High: 2.3 | Activity | Activation of hΔC neurons by optogenetic stimulus | Optimized to match behavior |
khΔC | 0.1 | – | Slope of hΔC activation function | 72 |
θhΔC | 0.7 | Input | Threshold of hΔC activation function | Optimized to match behavior |
taun | 10 | ms | Time constant for exponentially filtered white noise | 72 |
σn | 0.03 | Activity | Standard deviation of exponentially filtered white noise | 72 |
Wii | I <-> i+4: 1.1 Otherwise: 0 | – | Synaptic strength between mutually inhibited neurons | 72 |
g | 0.5 | – | Adaptation weight for mutual inhibition neurons | 72 |
tauu | 1 | ms | Time constant for mutual inhibition neurons | 72 |
taua | 100 | ms | Time constant for slow feedback adaptation | 72 |
kmi | 0.1 | – | Slope of mutual inhibition activation function | 72 |
θmi | −0.15 | Input | Threshold of mutual inhibition activation function | Adjusted from72 |
taupfl | 1 | ms | Time constant for PFL3 and PFL2 neurons | 72 |
kpfl | 0.1 | – | Slope of PFL activation function | 72 |
θpfl | 1.7 | Input | Threshold of PFL activation function | Optimized to match behavior |
λ | 0.0003 | Rate | Baseline turn rate for 20000 Hz simulated data | 10 |
σturn | 20 | deg/s | Standard deviation of angular velocity distribution | 10 |
m1 | 0.03 | deg/(s*activity) | Constant coupling PFL3 activity to angular velocity at 20,000 Hz | Optimized to match behavior |
vbase | 6 | mm/s | Baseline groundspeed | 10 |
m2 | 0.25 | mm/(s*activity) | Constant coupling PFL2 activity to groundspeed | Optimized to match behavior |
Note: many parameters were manually fit to match simulation data with behavioral trajectories. While not explored in this study, many of these parameters (e.g., activation thresholds) are flexible and can be adjusted together to keep the steering circuit functioning across a range of sinusoid amplitudes.
Input layer: heading
Heading input to PFL3 and PFL2 was simulated by phase-shifting heading-tuned one-cycle sinusoids:
1 |
where FBlocation refers to the spatial location within the FB (0° = left FB, 360° = right FB), heading refers to the heading direction of the fly, and phase_shift refers to the anatomical shift introduced by PFL neurons entering the FB. The phase shift parameters were taken directly from the connectome31 and were defined as the following: phase_shiftleft_PFL3 = −90°, phase_shiftright_PFL3 = +90°, phase_shiftPFL2 = +180.
Input layer: wind
Wind-tuned bumps of activity in hΔC neurons were simulated using one-cycle sinusoids that depended on the encoding strategy. For allocentric representation (Fig. 7), the bump of activity in hΔC neurons follows the allocentric direction of wind and was defined by the following:
2 |
where wind_direction refers to the allocentric wind direction. For frontal representation (Fig. S7) hΔC wind activity was constructed by phase shifting and summing the activity of a left and right population of wind-sensitive PFNs21, each displaying a heading-tuned bump scaled by wind direction:
3 |
where phase_shift refers to the anatomical shift introduced by PFN neurons entering the FB, and scale refers to the wind-tuned scaling of bump amplitude. The phase shifts differed for the two populations of PFNs and were defined as: phase_shiftleft = +45°, phase_shiftright = −4531 The scaling corresponded to the wind tuning of the PFNs, which differed between the left and right PFN populations21
4 |
where wind_directionegocentric is the egocentric wind direction and peak is the egocentric wind direction that maximally activates the PFN cell type (peakleft = −45°, peakright = +45°).
Intermediate layer 1: hΔC neurons
The total activity of hΔC neurons was calculated as the sum of wind input and optogenetic input:
5 |
where wind_gain is a binary variable set to 1 only if odor is present and optogenetic_input is a vector of sparse or broad optogenetic patterns. Sparse optogenetic patterns were generated by creating a 20-element vector, with each element stochastically set to 0 or 2.3 at a 15% probability. Broad optogenetic patterns were generated by creating a 20-element vector with every value set to the same value (low light = 0.75, high light = 2.3). These values were chosen to best match simulated trajectories with real behavior.
The output of the hΔC population was calculated by phase-shifting the hΔC activity by 180°. To simplify the pathways connecting hΔC neurons with mutual inhibition circuits, PFL3 neurons, and PFL2 neurons, which all involve at least one additional cell type, the output of hΔC neurons was transformed from 20-neuron space into 8-column space and then thresholded using a sigmoidal activation function:
6 |
where ShΔC is the hΔC sigmoid activation function with slope khΔC and threshold θhΔC, WhΔC is the feedforward transformation matrix, is the 180° phase shift function, and n is exponentially filtered white noise. The matrix used for the space transformation was inspired by the connectivity between local neurons in the fan-shaped body. To construct this matrix, each neuron’s output was set as the size of one column, such that there were 20 overlapping column-sized outputs. The matrix element for columni and neuronj was then set as the amount of overlap between columni and the output of neuronj, and the total input to columni from all neurons was normalized to equal 1. The exact structure of WhΔC was not critical, as long as the matrix was normalized and maintained the approximate location of wind sinusoids in FB space. The filtered white noise was defined by the following equation72
7 |
where n is the noise variable, taun is the noise time constant, is the noise standard deviation, and is white noise with zero mean and unit variance.
Intermediate layer 2: mutual inhibition local neurons
hΔC columnar output was relayed into an inhibitory recurrent network, consisting of eight inhibitory neurons with mutual connections between neurons located in opposite columns. Neurons in this network also contained a slow adaptation parameter, a, that curtailed input when neural activity was high. This system was described using the following differential equations:
8 |
9 |
Where u is an 8-element vector corresponding to the activity of each inhibitory neuron, a is an 8-element vector corresponding to the adaptation of each inhibitory neuron, Wii is the recurrent weight matrix connecting the inhibitory neurons, is the adaptation weight, hΔCoutput is the columnar output of the hΔC neurons, tauu is the time constant for neural activity, taua is the time constant for adaptation, and Smi is the mutual inhibition sigmoid activation function with slope kmi and threshold θmi. The equations and parameters used to model the mutual inhibition circuit were taken from ref. 72.
The output of the mutual inhibition layer was calculated by phase-shifting u by 180°. This output was subtracted from the hΔC output to create the total local output sent to PFL3 and PFL2.
Output layer: PFL2 and PFL3 neurons
The activity of left PFL3, right PFL3, and PFL2 were calculated by summing the excitatory output of hΔC neurons, inhibitory output of the mutual inhibition layer, and phase-shifted excitatory output of the heading system, and then converting this input into activity using the following dynamic system:
10 |
where taupfl is the time constant for PFL neurons and Spfl is the PFL sigmoid activation function with slope kpfl and threshold θpfl.
Output layer: navigation modulation
Turning was calculated by comparing the activity of right PFL3 neurons and left PFL3 neurons and adding a noise term:
11 |
where m1 is the coupling constant between PFL3 activity and turning, is a binary Poisson variable with λ rate, and G are angular velocity values drawn from a Gaussian distribution with zero mean and turn variance. The random turn equations and parameters were taken from ref. 10 while the coupling constant was selected to best match behavioral trajectories.
Groundspeed was calculated by summing the activity of PFL2 neurons according to the following equation:
12 |
where vbase is the baseline speed and m2 is the coupling constant between PFL2 activity and speed. The baseline speed was taken from ref. 10 while the coupling constant was selected to best match behavioral trajectories.
Output layer: simulated trajectories
The model simulates heading and groundspeed over time in response to wind/odor or optogenetic stimuli. These values were converted into movement in x and y using:
13 |
where is the time step between data points, is the current groundspeed, and is the current heading.
Other
The sigmoid activation function used for hΔC neurons, the mutual inhibition circuit, and PFL neurons was the following.
14 |
where x is the input, θ is the activation threshold, and k is the activation slope.
Quantification and statistical analysis
Behavior
Based on previous behavior data10 we assumed these data were not normally distributed and applied non-parametric statistics. We used the two-sided Wilcoxon signed rank test (MATLAB signrank) to compare the average baseline value of each parameter to the average of the parameter over a window of interest. Between genotype comparisons (genetic silencing experiments) were made between baseline subtracted parameter values using the two-sided Mann–Whitney U test (MATLAB ranksum). For behavioral time courses we display standard error around the mean. For summary plots we present standard deviation around the mean. Bonferroni corrections for multiple comparisons were applied based on the number of genotypes tested labeling similar neuron types (i.e. 6 in Fig. 1D, 21 for all dorsal FB inputs in Fig. 3B, C) or on the number of comparisons made to control in genetic silencing experiments (3 in Fig. 1F).
Imaging and physiology
We used parametric tests for calcium imaging and electrophysiological data. When testing for significant differences in odor responses across directions we averaged individual trials across flies, and performed a one-way ANOVA across pooled trials across flies (MATLAB anova1). When assessing differences between wind ON responses and odor responses we calculated mean wind and odor responses in 10 s windows following ON. We compared wind and odor periods using a two-tailed paired student’s t-test (MATLAB ttest, Fig. S4E, Fig. S5A). Similarly, we performed two-tailed paired student’s t-test when comparing wind and odor responses in MBON electrophysiology experiments and a two-tailed unpaired student’s t-test (MATLAB ttest2) when comparing odor responses between ipsilateral and contralateral MBONs. To assess the decay of fluorescence response to odor over time (Fig. 4D), we computed the average response to the odor presentation period across flies for five trial blocks, including one trial of each direction. We averaged the response across all flies imaged, and normalized by the mean response in trial block one. Shaded regions depict standard error across flies around the mean.
Classification training
We trained classifiers to assess if an observer could correctly identify whether wind was coming from the left, right or front relative to the fly based on calcium imaging data from select neuron groups. We pooled the data for 45 and 90 degrees left and right for this analysis. We trained classification tree models on calcium response data during odor ON (5 s, Figs. 2F, 4D) and during wind ON and OFF (5 s, Supplementary Figs. 2H, 4E). Model were trained using two predictors: the fly identity, and the Z-score of the calcium response during the specified window. We took the Z-score of the response to standardize the data between electrophysiology firing rates (PFNa) and calcium responses (all other neurons). As we recorded both left and right LNa neurons simultaneously we asked how well the difference between the activity of the two performed. We trained classification trees (Matlab’s fitctree) to identify wind direction and tested model performance using 10-fold cross validation error (Matlab’s KfoldLoss). We compared this error to classification trees that were trained on the same data with shuffled labels, with 50 random iterations of shuffled labels.
We also designed classifier to assess if an observer could distinguish between wind ON activity and odor ON activity. We used the same parameters in the direction classifier but pooled all data across all wind directions. We performed the same analysis to assess if an observer could distinguish between baseline activity and odor ON activity in Supplementary Figs. 2F and 4F.
Reporting summary
Further information on research design is available in the Nature Research Reporting Summary linked to this article.
Supplementary information
Acknowledgements
The authors would like to thank Marc Gershow, Karla Kaun, Tom Clandinin, Calude Desplan, Michael Dickinson, Michael Reiser, Matthew Clark, Matthieu Louis, Richard Mann, Gilad Barnea, Rachel Wilson, Tzumin Lee, Janelia Flylight, and the Bloomington and Vienna Drosophila Resource Centers for fly stocks. We thank Michael Long, Elizabeth Hong, Floris van Breugel, David Schoppik, Gaby Maimon, and members of the Nagel and Schoppik labs for helpful input on the manuscript. We thank Haim Schoppik for help with data archiving. This work was supported by R01DC017979, RF1NS127129, NSF Ideaslab (IOS-1555933) and NSF Neuronex grants (NSF 2014217), as well as a McKnight Scholar Award to K.I.N. M.H.S. was supported by an NSF CAREER award IOS-204720. T.A.C. was supported by a Dean’s Dissertation Fellowship from NYU. A.M.M. was supported by a Diversity Supplement to R01DC017979 and a McKnight Pecot Fellowship.
Author contributions
K.I.N., A.M.M.M., and A.J.L. conceived the project. A.M.M.M. performed the majority of behavioral, imaging, electrophysiology, and anatomy experiments, and generated new genetic stocks. A.J.L. performed SPARC experiments with A.M.M., performed connectomic analysis, and developed the computational model with input from K.I.N. and A.M.M.M. A.M.L. contributed to behavioral experiments. T.A.C. performed a subset of electrophysiology experiments. M.H.S. provided the genetic strategy to isolate CX neurons. K.I.N., A.M.M.M., and A.J.L. wrote the paper with input from the other authors.
Peer review
Peer review information
Nature Communications thanks Stanley Heinze, Barbara Webb and the other anonymous reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work. Peer review reports are available.
Data availability
Behavior and imaging data generated in this study have been deposited on Zenodo at https://zenodo.org/record/6863832#.Ytlati-B3UJ, DOI: 10.5281/zenodo.6863832. The connectomic data used in this study (hemibrain version 1.1) are available on neuprint https://neuprint.janelia.org/. Source data are provided with this paper. Any additional information required to reanalyze the data reported in this paper is available from the Lead Contact upon request.
Code availability
All original code is available on Github at https://github.com/nagellab/Mathesonetal2022. Model code is available on Zenodo at https://zenodo.org/record/6762105#.YtlbYi-B3UJ, 10.5281/zenodo.6762105.
Materials availability
No new transgenes were created for this study. Transgenic stocks are available on request from the Lead Contact.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41467-022-32247-7.
References
- 1.Murlis J, Elkinton JS, Carde RT. Odor plumes and how insects use them. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 1992;37:505–532. doi: 10.1146/annurev.en.37.010192.002445. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Cardé RT, Willis MA. Navigational strategies used by insects to find distant, wind-borne sources of odor. J. Chem. Ecol. 2008;34:854–866. doi: 10.1007/s10886-008-9484-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Crimaldi JP, Koseff JR. High-resolution measurements of the spatial and temporal scalar structure of a turbulent plume. Exp. Fluids. 2001;31:90–102. doi: 10.1007/s003480000263. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Webster DR, Weissburg MJ. Chemosensory guidance cues in a turbulent chemical odor plume. Limnol. Oceanogr. 2001;46:1034–1047. doi: 10.4319/lo.2001.46.5.1034. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Celani A, Villermaux E, Vergassola M. Odor landscapes in turbulent environments. Phys. Rev. X. 2014;4:041015. [Google Scholar]
- 6.David CT, Kennedy JS, Ludlow AR. Finding of a sex pheromone source by gypsy moths released in the field. Nature. 1983;303:804–806. doi: 10.1038/303804a0. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Wolf H, Wehner R. Pinpointing food sources: olfactory and anemotactic orientation in desert ants, Cataglyphis fortis. J. Exp. Biol. 2000;203:857–868. doi: 10.1242/jeb.203.5.857. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Page JL, Dickman BD, Webster DR, Weissburg MJ. Getting ahead: context-dependent responses to odorant filaments drive along-stream progress during odor tracking in blue crabs. J. Exp. Biol. 2011;214:1498–1512. doi: 10.1242/jeb.049312. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.van Breugel F, Dickinson MH. Plume-tracking behavior of flying Drosophila emerges from a set of distinct sensory-motor reflexes. Curr. Biol. 2014;24:274–286. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.12.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Álvarez-Salvado E, et al. Elementary sensory-motor transformations underlying olfactory navigation in walking fruit-flies. Elife. 2018;7:e37815. doi: 10.7554/eLife.37815. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Schulze A, et al. Dynamical feature extraction at the sensory periphery guides chemotaxis. Elife. 2015;4:e06694. doi: 10.7554/eLife.06694. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.De Belle JS, Heisenberg M. Associative odor learning in Drosophila abolished by chemical ablation of mushroom bodies. Science. 1994;263:692–695. doi: 10.1126/science.8303280. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Hige T, Aso Y, Modi MN, Rubin GM, Turner GC. Heterosynaptic plasticity underlies aversive olfactory learning in Drosophila. Neuron. 2015;88:985–998. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.11.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Schlegel P, et al. Information flow, cell types and stereotypy in a full olfactory connectome. Elife. 2021;10:e66018. doi: 10.7554/eLife.66018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Aso Y, et al. Mushroom body output neurons encode valence and guide memory-based action selection in Drosophila. Elife. 2014;3:e04580. doi: 10.7554/eLife.04580. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Owald D, et al. Activity of defined mushroom body output neurons underlies learned olfactory behavior in Drosophila. Neuron. 2015;86:417–427. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.03.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Dolan MJ, et al. Neurogenetic dissection of the Drosophila lateral horn reveals major outputs, diverse behavioural functions, and interactions with the mushroom body. Elife. 2019;8:e43079. doi: 10.7554/eLife.43079. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Huoviala, P. et al. Neural circuit basis of aversive odour processing in Drosophila from sensory input to descending output. Preprint at BioRxiv10.1101/394403 (2020).
- 19.Bates AS, et al. Complete connectomic reconstruction of olfactory projection neurons in the fly brain. Curr. Biol. 2020;30:3183–3199. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2020.06.042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Mamiya A, Beshel J, Xu C, Zhong Y. Neural representations of airflow in Drosophila mushroom body. PLoS ONE. 2008;3:e4063. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0004063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Currier TA, Matheson AM, Nagel KI. Encoding and control of orientation to airflow by a set of Drosophila fan-shaped body neurons. Elife. 2020;9:e61510. doi: 10.7554/eLife.61510. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Scheffer LK, et al. A connectome and analysis of the adult Drosophila central brain. Elife. 2020;9:e57443. doi: 10.7554/eLife.57443. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Lyu C, Abbott LF, Maimon G. Building an allocentric travelling direction signal via vector computation. Nature. 2022;601:92–97. doi: 10.1038/s41586-021-04067-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Lu J, et al. Transforming representations of movement from body-to world-centric space. Nature. 2022;601:98–104. doi: 10.1038/s41586-021-04191-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Li F, et al. The connectome of the adult Drosophila mushroom body provides insights into function. Elife. 2020;9:e62576. doi: 10.7554/eLife.62576. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Scaplen KM, et al. Transsynaptic mapping of Drosophila mushroom body output neurons. Elife. 2021;10:e63379. doi: 10.7554/eLife.63379. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Homberg U. Interneurones of the central complex in the bee brain (Apis mellifera, L.) J. Insect Physiol. 1985;31:251–264. doi: 10.1016/0022-1910(85)90127-1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Strauss R, Heisenberg M. A higher control center of locomotor behavior in the Drosophila brain. J. Neurosci. 1993;13:1852–1861. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.13-05-01852.1993. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Martin JP, Guo P, Mu L, Harley CM, Ritzmann RE. Central-complex control of movement in the freely walking cockroach. Curr. Biol. 2015;25:2795–2803. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.09.044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Le Moël F, Stone T, Lihoreau M, Wystrach A, Webb B. The central complex as a potential substrate for vector based navigation. Front. Psychol. 2019;10:690. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00690. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Hulse BK, et al. A connectome of the Drosophila central complex reveals network motifs suitable for flexible navigation and context-dependent action selection. Elife. 2021;10:e66039. doi: 10.7554/eLife.66039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Goulard R, Buehlmann C, Niven JE, Graham P, Webb B. A unified mechanism for innate and learned visual landmark guidance in the insect central complex. PLoS Comput. Biol. 2021;17:e1009383. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1009383. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Stone T, et al. An anatomically constrained model for path integration in the bee brain. Curr. Biol. 2017;27:3069–3085. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.08.052. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Sun X, Yue S, Mangan M. A decentralised neural model explaining optimal integration of navigational strategies in insects. Elife. 2020;9:e54026. doi: 10.7554/eLife.54026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Dacke M, et al. Multimodal cue integration in the dung beetle compass. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2019;116:14248–14253. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1904308116. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Honkanen A, Adden A, da Silva Freitas J, Heinze S. The insect central complex and the neural basis of navigational strategies. J. Exp. Biol. 2019;222:jeb188854. doi: 10.1242/jeb.188854. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Leitch KJ, Ponce FV, Dickson WB, van Breugel F, Dickinson MH. The long-distance flight behavior of Drosophila supports an agent-based model for wind-assisted dispersal in insects. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2021;118:e2013342118. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2013342118. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Sun X, Yue S, Mangan M. How the insect central complex could coordinate multimodal navigation. Elife. 2021;10:e73077. doi: 10.7554/eLife.73077. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Larsson MC, et al. Or83b encodes a broadly expressed odorant receptor essential for Drosophila olfaction. Neuron. 2004;43:703–714. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.08.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Silbering AF, et al. Complementary function and integrated wiring of the evolutionarily distinct Drosophila olfactory subsystems. J. Neurosci. 2011;31:13357–13375. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2360-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Jung SH, Hueston C, Bhandawat V. Odor-identity dependent motor programs underlie behavioral responses to odors. Elife. 2015;4:e11092. doi: 10.7554/eLife.11092. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Ardin P, Peng F, Mangan M, Lagogiannis K, Webb B. Using an insect mushroom body circuit to encode route memory in complex natural environments. PLoS Comput. Biol. 2016;12:e1004683. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004683. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Talay M, et al. Transsynaptic mapping of second-order taste neurons in flies by trans-Tango. Neuron. 2017;96:783–795. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.10.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Clyne JD, Miesenböck G. Sex-specific control and tuning of the pattern generator for courtship song in Drosophila. Cell. 2008;133:354–363. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2008.01.050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Donlea JM, Pimentel D, Miesenböck G. Neuronal machinery of sleep homeostasis in Drosophila. Neuron. 2014;81:860–872. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.12.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Ren Q, Awasaki T, Huang YF, Liu Z, Lee T. Cell class-lineage analysis reveals sexually dimorphic lineage compositions in the Drosophila brain. Curr. Biol. 2016;26:2583–2593. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.07.086. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Seelig JD, Jayaraman V. Neural dynamics for landmark orientation and angular path integration. Nature. 2015;521:186–191. doi: 10.1038/nature14446. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Isaacman-Beck J, et al. SPARC enables genetic manipulation of precise proportions of cells. Nat. Neurosci. 2020;23:1168–1175. doi: 10.1038/s41593-020-0668-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Mohammad F, et al. Optogenetic inhibition of behavior with anion channelrhodopsins. Nat. Methods. 2017;14:271–274. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.4148. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Rayshubskiy, A. et al. Neural circuit mechanisms for steering control in walking Drosophila. Preprint at bioRxiv10.1101/2020.04.04.024703 (2020).
- 51.Kennedy JS, Marsh D. Pheromone-regulated anemotaxis in flying moths. Science. 1974;184:999–1001. doi: 10.1126/science.184.4140.999. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Buehlmann C, Hansson BS, Knaden M. Path integration controls nest-plume following in desert ants. Curr. Biol. 2012;22:645–649. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.02.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Cognigni P, Felsenberg J, Waddell S. Do the right thing: neural network mechanisms of memory formation, expression and update in Drosophila. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2018;49:51–58. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2017.12.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Sareen PF, McCurdy LY, Nitabach MN. A neuronal ensemble encoding adaptive choice during sensory conflict in Drosophila. Nat. Commun. 2021;12:1–13. doi: 10.1038/s41467-021-24423-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Mishkin M, Ungerleider LG, Macko KA. Object vision and spatial vision: two cortical pathways. Trends Neurosci. 1983;6:414–417. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(83)90190-X. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Hargreaves EL, Rao G, Lee I, Knierim JJ. Major dissociation between medial and lateral entorhinal input to dorsal hippocampus. Science. 2005;308:1792–1794. doi: 10.1126/science.1110449. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Leitner FC, et al. Spatially segregated feedforward and feedback neurons support differential odor processing in the lateral entorhinal cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 2016;19:935–944. doi: 10.1038/nn.4303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Sayin S, et al. A neural circuit arbitrates between persistence and withdrawal in hungry. Drosoph. Neuron. 2019;104:544–558. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2019.07.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Chaffiol A, Laloi D, Pham-Delègue MH. Prior classical olfactory conditioning improves odour-cued flight orientation of honey bees in a wind tunnel. J. Exp. Biol. 2005;208:3731–3737. doi: 10.1242/jeb.01796. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Bell WJ, Kramer E. Search and anemotactic orientation of cockroaches. J. Insect Physiol. 1979;25:631–640. doi: 10.1016/0022-1910(79)90112-4. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Suver MP, et al. Encoding of wind direction by central neurons in Drosophila. Neuron. 2019;102:828–842. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2019.03.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Fuller SB, Straw AD, Peek MY, Murray RM, Dickinson MH. Flying Drosophila stabilize their vision-based velocity controller by sensing wind with their antennae. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2014;111:E1182–E1191. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1323529111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Kennedy, J. S. The visual responses of flying mosquitoes. Proc. Zoological Soc. London109, No. Pt. 4 (1940).
- 64.Vergassola M, Villermaux E, Shraiman BI. ‘Infotaxis’ as a strategy for searching without gradients. Nature. 2007;445:406–409. doi: 10.1038/nature05464. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Masson JB. Olfactory searches with limited space perception. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2013;110:11261–11266. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1221091110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Grünbaum D, Willis MA. Spatial memory-based behaviors for locating sources of odor plumes. Mov. Ecol. 2015;3:1–21. doi: 10.1186/s40462-015-0037-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Namiki S, Kanzaki R. Comparative neuroanatomy of the lateral accessory lobe in the insect brain. Front. Physiol. 2016;7:244. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2016.00244. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Okubo TS, Patella P, D’Alessandro I, Wilson RI. A neural network for wind-guided compass navigation. Neuron. 2020;107:924–940. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2020.06.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Giovannucci A, et al. CaImAn an open source tool for scalable calcium imaging data analysis. eLife. 2019;8:e38173. doi: 10.7554/eLife.38173. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Pnevmatikakis EA, Giovannucci A. NoRMCorre: an online algorithm for piecewise rigid motion correction of calcium imaging data. J. Neurosci. Methods. 2017;291:83–94. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2017.07.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Muir DR, Kampa BM. FocusStack and StimServer: a new open source MATLAB toolchain for visual stimulation and analysis of two-photon calcium neuronal imaging data. Front. Neuroinformatics. 2014;8:85. doi: 10.3389/fninf.2014.00085. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Shpiro A, Curtu R, Rinzel J, Rubin N. Dynamical characteristics common to neuronal competition models. J. Neurophysiol. 2007;97:462–473. doi: 10.1152/jn.00604.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
Behavior and imaging data generated in this study have been deposited on Zenodo at https://zenodo.org/record/6863832#.Ytlati-B3UJ, DOI: 10.5281/zenodo.6863832. The connectomic data used in this study (hemibrain version 1.1) are available on neuprint https://neuprint.janelia.org/. Source data are provided with this paper. Any additional information required to reanalyze the data reported in this paper is available from the Lead Contact upon request.
All original code is available on Github at https://github.com/nagellab/Mathesonetal2022. Model code is available on Zenodo at https://zenodo.org/record/6762105#.YtlbYi-B3UJ, 10.5281/zenodo.6762105.
No new transgenes were created for this study. Transgenic stocks are available on request from the Lead Contact.