Skip to main content
JAMA Network logoLink to JAMA Network
. 2022 Aug 12;3(8):e222570. doi: 10.1001/jamahealthforum.2022.2570

Association of Direct-to-Consumer Advertising of Prescription Drugs With Consumer Health-Related Intentions and Beliefs Among Individuals at Risk of Cardiovascular Disease

Matthew D Eisenberg 1,, Yashaswini Singh 1, Neeraj Sood 2
PMCID: PMC9375162  PMID: 36200632

This cross-sectional study provides evidence on the association of direct-to-consumer-advertising of prescription drugs with consumer health-related intentions and beliefs among individuals at risk of cardiovascular disease.

Key Points

Question

What is the association of direct-to-consumer advertising of prescription drugs with consumer intentions and beliefs related to prescription drugs and health-related behaviors?

Findings

In this cross-sectional study of 2874 individuals at risk of cardiovascular disease, participants in the treatment groups viewed 5 minutes of prescription drug advertisements while those in the control group viewed advertisements for other consumer products. Participants in the treatment groups were more likely to report favorable perceptions of pharmaceutical manufacturers and medication-related behavior intentions, such as switching medications, though there were no statistically significant differences in behavior intentions for diet and exercise between the treatment and control groups.

Meaning

Brief exposure to prescription drug advertisements has a large and positive association with medication-related demand intentions with no offsetting negative spillovers on lifestyle-related intentions.

Abstract

Importance

Consumers in the US are exposed to unprecedented high levels of direct-to-consumer advertising (DTCA) for prescription drugs, yet there is limited evidence regarding their effect on health-related intentions and beliefs.

Objective

To provide evidence on the association of DTCA for prescription drugs with consumer health-related intentions and beliefs.

Design, Setting, and Participants

This cross-sectional study recruited participants from a nationally representative sample of individuals at high risk of cardiovascular disease. Participants were randomly assigned into 1 of 3 study arms: (1) exposure to DTCA for heart disease medications (treatment 1 [n = 926]), (2) exposure to DTCA for heart disease medications with price disclosure (treatment 2 [n = 921]), (3) and exposure to nonpharmaceutical advertising (control group [n = 902]). Each study arm viewed five 1-minute video advertisements, totaling 5 minutes of advertising exposure. The 2 treatment arms viewed pharmaceutical advertising videos for 4 heart disease medications, and the control arm viewed nonpharmaceutical advertising videos. Participants then completed a survey questionnaire to measure medication- and lifestyle-related intentions and health-related beliefs and perceptions.

Exposures

Direct-to-consumer advertising for heart disease medications (treatment 1), DTCA for heart disease medications with price disclosure (treatment 2), and nonpharmaceutical advertising (control group).

Main Outcomes and Measures

The primary outcomes included ordinal measures of medication- and lifestyle-related intentions, health-related beliefs, and brand perceptions.

Results

Among the 2874 included participants (mean [SD] age, 53.8 [7.1] years; 1549 [54%] male) χ2 tests confirmed that there were no statistically significant differences in baseline demographic characteristics across study arms. There was a positive association between DTCA and medication-related behavioral intentions, including intention to switch medication (marginal effect [ME] = 0.004; P = .002) and engage in information-seeking behaviors (ME = 0.02; P = .01). There was no evidence that pharmaceutical DTCA discouraged use of nonpharmacological lifestyle interventions that can help manage heart disease (eg, diet and exercise), and DTCA exposure also had a positive association with consumers’ favorable perceptions of pharmaceutical manufacturers (competence: ME = 0.03; P = .01; innovative: ME = 0.03; P = .008). There was no evidence for differential associations of price disclosures in DTCA.

Conclusions and Relevance

In this cross-sectional study, results showed that brief exposure to pharmaceutical DTCA had a large and positive association with medication-related demand intentions with no offsetting negative spillovers on lifestyle-related intentions. Lack of associations with price disclosure in DTCA suggests that policy makers should consider alternative strategies to promote value-based decision-making for prescription drugs.

Introduction

Consumers in the US are exposed to unprecedented levels of pharmaceutical direct-to-consumer advertising (DTCA), with more than $18 billion spent in 2016 through 2018.1 Proponents argue that DTCA provides information to consumers, promotes medication adherence, and encourages healthy behaviors. Critics argue that DTCA leads consumers to choose brand-name drugs over lower-cost generics, discourages use of nonpharmacologic treatments, and promotes inappropriate drug prescribing behavior. While DTCA has the potential to affect consumer behavior, the causal link between advertising and behavior change contains many steps. Prior work has identified associations between DTCA and rising prescription drug utilization using observational data2,3,4,5,6 and experimental settings.7 Patient preferences play only one role in the process because prescribers have a considerable degree of influence to direct, modify, or nullify the effect of DTCA on consumer behavior, which also can be nuanced across prescribers.7

In response to rising prescription drug prices8,9 and DTCA,1 the US Department of Health and Human Services attempted regulation10 that would require drug manufacturers to include prices in all television advertisements.11 More recent bipartisan efforts have attempted to legislate price disclosure requirements in DTCA.12 Price disclosure might discourage consumers from seeking treatment with high advertised prices. Alternatively, disclosure may be ineffective if consumers cannot evaluate prescription drug prices or if they view price as a signal of quality. Preliminary research suggests that price disclosure can influence consumer responses,13 but the implications of price disclosure on a broad range of behavioral outcomes is unclear.

In this cross-sectional study with a survey, we aim to fill these knowledge gaps by examining the association of DTCA and price disclosure in DTCA with medication- and lifestyle-related intentions, health-related beliefs, and brand perceptions among individuals at risk of heart disease. We focused on those with or at risk of cardiovascular disease, including coronary artery disease, stroke, heart failure, and hypertension.14 Cardiovascular disease is highly prevalent in the US and has several pharmacological treatments15 and nonpharmacological lifestyle interventions available to patients.16 We hypothesized that exposure to DTCA for heart products would increase the likelihood of medication adherence, promote conversations with clinicians, and decrease the likelihood of diet and exercise intentions. We hypothesized that there would be no or small associations with price disclosure.8,9,10 As efforts to regulate DTCA continue in Congress, this study will help manufactures, policy makers, and consumers understand the behavioral consequences of DTCA, as well as if price disclosure promotes competitive pressures that could lead to lower prices.

Methods

Data and Procedures

Study participants were recruited from Ipsos Public Affairs (hereafter, Ipsos) KnowledgePanel, a national probability-based household panel. The target population consisted of individuals who were at high risk of cardiovascular disease. Recruited participants were English-speaking US residents aged 40 to 64 years who were diagnosed with high cholesterol or currently a cigarette smoker, or overweight or obese (ie, body mass index >25 [calculated as weight in kilograms divided by height in meters squared]). Randomization occurred at the level of the individual into 1 of 3 study arms: exposure to DTCA (treatment 1), exposure to DTCA with prices (treatment 2), and nonpharmaceutical advertising (control group).

Participants in each study arm viewed five 1-minute video advertisements, totaling 5 minutes of advertising exposure. The treatment 1 arm viewed DTCA videos for 4 heart disease medications (Brilinta [AstraZeneca], Entresto [Novartis], Repatha [Amgen], and Xarelto [Bayer]). The advertisements viewed by the treatment 2 arm were identical to the advertisements viewed by the treatment 1 arm, except that treatment 2 arm’s advertising videos were edited to disclose the net prices received by the manufacturer as reported in the SSR Health database.17 The control arm viewed nonpharmaceutical advertisements, such as for-consumer electronics and web-based services. The advertisements watched by each study arm were not artificially created by the researchers; instead, they represented advertisements that have been aired on television.

After viewing the advertisements, participants completed a survey questionnaire to measure medication- and lifestyle-related intentions. In addition, participants were asked about their health-related beliefs and brand perceptions. Participants also answered demographic questions and received $5 as compensation for participation. The survey instrument is provided in the eAppendix in the Supplement.

The survey was fielded over a 15-day period from July 21, 2021, to August 4, 2021. Statistical analysis was performed from September 2021 to November 2021. The survey was fielded to 4933 respondents, of which 3026 respondents started and completed the survey, yielding a response rate of 61%. Of the 3026 respondents who completed the survey, 152 respondents did not meet the survey inclusion criteria. The final analytic sample included 2874 respondents across the control arm (n = 952), treatment 1 arm (n = 964), and treatment 2 arm (n = 958).

This study received institutional review board approval from the University of Southern California. All participants provided written informed consent via electronic form. This study follows the Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) reporting guideline18 and the American Association for Public Opinion Research reporting guideline for survey studies.19

Measures

To ask about medication-related intentions, we focused on the current pharmacological treatment guidelines that support the use of antihypertensives and statins for prevention of heart disease.20,21 Specifically, we relied on prior research to develop survey questions regarding medication-related outcomes that included ordinal measures (ranging from 1 [highly unlikely] to 5 [highly likely]) on the likelihood of switching medication, asking a physician about advertised medication, asking an insurer about advertised medication, searching for medication online, or taking medication as directed.13,22,23

To ask about lifestyle-related intentions, we focused on nonpharmacological lifestyle interventions that have been recommended to improve outcomes in individuals at high risk of cardiovascular diseases.24,25,26 Specifically, lifestyle-related outcomes included ordinal measures (ranging from 1 [highly unlikely] to 5 [highly likely]) on the likelihood of being more physically active and eating healthier food.

To ask about brand perceptions, we relied on the marketing literature to identify measures that represent consumers’ beliefs about brands as intentional agents.27 Consumer beliefs about brands, such as brand competence, innovativeness, and trustworthiness, can guide consumers’ demand intentions (eg, intention to search for medication online), as well as actual behaviors (eg, asking a physician about advertised medication). We included ordinal measures (ranging from 1 [always disagree] to 5 [always agree]) on individual perceptions of pharmaceutical manufacturers as being competent, innovative, and trustworthy.

Finally, to ask about health-related beliefs, we relied on the Determinants of Lifestyle Behavior Questionnaire, a survey instrument that has been shown to be valid for measuring determinants of lifestyle behavioral change in adults at high risk of cardiovascular diseases.28 Questions on health-related beliefs were categorized into 2 categories: (1) questions that ask about perceived importance of physical activity/dietary behavior and (2) questions that ask about perceived difficulty of physical activity/dietary behavior. The perceived importance measure included ordinal measures (ranging from 1 [always disagree] to 5 [always agree]) on beliefs related to physical activity and dietary behavior (eg, “Eating healthier food is pleasant,” “Eating healthier food is important,” “Eating healthier food is easy”). The perceived difficulty measure included ordinal measures (ranging from 1 [always disagree] to 5 [always agree]) on beliefs related to behavior-specific situations (eg, “I am able to eat healthier food on average,” “I find it difficult to eat healthier food on average,” “I find it difficult to eat healthier food when I am busy,” “My family and friends think I should eat healthier food”). We also asked questions on medication-related beliefs, including perceived seriousness of heart disease and perceived effectiveness of heart disease medication.

Statistical Analysis

Baseline characteristics and outcomes of interest were compared for the treatment and control groups using χ2 tests. We estimated the association between DTCA exposure and outcomes of interest using ordered logit regression models.

We conducted 3 sets of regression models. First, we estimated the association of being randomized to a treatment group (ie, either treatment 1 or treatment 2) compared with those who were randomized to the control group. We separately also analyzed those who were randomized to receive treatment 1 (DTCA without price disclosure) compared with the control group and those who were randomized to receive treatment 2 (DTCA with price disclosure) compared with the control group. The key independent variable was an indicator variable for random assignment to a treatment group. All regressions included self-reported demographic characteristics, including participant age, sex, race, household income, and indicators for which of the inclusion criteria the respondent met (ie, high cholesterol, current smoker, overweight or obese).

To assess the magnitude of DTCA associations, we calculated marginal effects (MEs) of treatment holding all other variables at their mean. The MEs can be interpreted as the difference in probability of an outcome between treatment and control arms.

It is possible that the study participants were not representative of individuals at risk for cardiovascular disease because they were much more likely to spend time taking paid surveys. While recent work suggests that Ipsos has lower bias and superior data quality when compared with other surveys,29 to gain confidence in the present results, we reestimated all models with the Ipsos-generated survey weights to make the sample comparable with the Current Population Survey.

Given the large number of outcomes, we also estimated Romano-Wolf P values by estimating a familywise error rate or the probability of making any type I error.30 Stata, version 16.1 (StataCorp) was used for all analyses.

Results

Overall, observable characteristics were similar across the treatment and control groups (Table 1). The χ2 tests demonstrated no statistically significant differences on age, gender, race, household income, education, or census region (eTable 1 in the Supplement).

Table 1. Balance in Baseline Covariates Across Treatment and Control Groupsa.

Characteristic No. (%) P valueb
Total (n = 2874) Control (n = 952) Treatment (n = 1922)
Respondent screening, mean (SD)
High cholesterol 0.52 (0.50) 0.52 (0.50) 0.52 (0.50) .80
Current smoker 0.16 (0.36) 0.16 (0.36) 0.16 (0.37) .82
BMI overweight 0.91 (0.28) 0.91 (0.29) 0.91 (0.28) .76
Age, y
30-44 443 (15) 134 (14) 309 (16) .11
45-59 1675 (58) 547 (57) 1128 (59)
≥60 756 (26) 271 (28) 485 (25)
Gender
Female 1325 (46) 459 (48) 866 (45) .11
Male 1549 (54) 493 (52) 1056 (55)
Race
American Indian or Alaska Native 26 (1) 10 (1) 16 (1) .99
Asian 82 (3) 26 (3) 56 (3)
Black or African American 290 (10) 99 (10) 191 (10)
Native Hawaiian or Pacific Islander 3 (0) 1 (0) 2 (0)
White 2379 (83) 784 (82) 1595 (83)
≥2 Races 94 (3) 32 (3) 62 (3)
Household income, $
<10 000 73 (3) 28 (3) 45 (2) .12
10 000-24 999 218 (8) 57 (6) 161 (8)
25 000-49 999 427 (15) 135 (14) 292 (15)
50 000-74 999 439 (15) 157 (16) 282 (15)
75 000-99 999 424 (15) 129 (14) 295 (15)
100 000-149 999 635 (22) 216 (23) 419 (22)
≥150 000 658 (23) 230 (24) 428 (22)
Education
No high school diploma or GED 146 (5) 46 (5) 100 (5) .59
High school graduate or the equivalent 773 (27) 271 (28) 502 (26)
Some college or associate’s degree 941 (33) 303 (32) 638 (33)
Bachelor’s degree or higher 1014 (35) 332 (35) 682 (35)
US region
Northeast 538 (19) 179 (19) 359 (19) .66
Midwest 640 (22) 211 (22) 429 (22)
South 1063 (37) 364 (38) 699 (36)
West 633 (22) 198 (21) 435 (23)

Abbreviations: BMI, body mass index; GED, general education development.

a

Respondent-level demographic characteristics at baseline are reported for respondents randomized into the treatment 1 arm (exposure to direct-to-consumer advertising) and the treatment 2 arm (exposure to direct-to-consumer advertising with prices), as well as the control arm (exposure to nonpharmaceutical advertising).

b

P values reported result from χ2 tests.

Figure 1 illustrates the direct associations of DTCA exposure with medication-related intentions. Respondents in the treatment group reported stronger intentions to search for medications online or to switch medications. For example, respondents in the treatment groups were more likely to report that they were “very likely” to switch medication (ME = 0.004; P = .002) and less likely to report that they were “very unlikely” to switch medications (ME = −0.05; P = .03). Similarly, respondents in the treatment groups were more likely to report they were “very likely” to search for information about the medication online (ME = 0.02; P = .01) and less likely to report that they were “very unlikely” to search for information about the medication online (ME = −0.06; P = .01). There was no statistically significant association for the likelihood of taking medication as directed and asking a physician or insurer about the advertised medication.

Figure 1. Marginal Effect of Treatment Exposure on Consumer Medication-Related Intentions.

Figure 1.

Presented are marginal effects estimated from ordered logit regressions of each outcome measure on treatment assignment, controlling for respondent age, sex, race, household income, and indicators for which of the inclusion criteria the respondent met (ie, high cholesterol, current smoker, or overweight or obese). The ordered logit regressions estimated the effects of being randomized to either treatment group (ie, either treatment 1 or treatment 2) compared with those who were randomized to the control group. Outcome measures are medication-related outcomes that include ordinal measures (ranging from 1 [highly unlikely] to 5 [highly likely]) on the likelihood of switching medication, asking a physician about advertised medication, asking an insurer about advertised medication, searching for medication online, or taking medication as directed.

eFigure 1 in the Supplement displays the indirect associations of DTCA exposure with behavioral intentions (ie, intention to be more physically active and eat healthier food). There were no statistically significant associations of DTCA on these behavioral intentions.

Figure 2 illustrates the association between DTCA exposure and perceptions of pharmaceutical manufacturers. Respondents in the treatment groups were more likely to hold beliefs that pharmaceutical firms were competent and innovative. For example, respondents in the treatment groups were likely to “always agree” with the statement that pharmaceutical firms were competent (ME = 0.03; P = .01) and innovative (ME = 0.03; P = .008). There were no statistically significant differences between the treatment and control groups in perceptions about trustworthiness of pharmaceutical manufacturers. Similarly, there were no differences between the treatment and control groups in perceptions about the competence, innovativeness, and trustworthiness of nonpharmaceutical industries (eFigure 4 in the Supplement).

Figure 2. Marginal Effect of Treatment on Consumer Brand Perceptions.

Figure 2.

Presented are marginal effects estimated from ordered logit regressions of each outcome measure on treatment assignment, controlling for respondent age, sex, race, household income, and indicators for which of the inclusion criteria the respondent met (ie, high cholesterol, current smoker, or overweight or obese). The ordered logit regressions estimated the effects of being randomized to either treatment group (ie, either treatment 1 or treatment 2) compared with those who were randomized to the control group). Outcome measures represent consumer beliefs about pharmaceutical manufacturers and include ordinal measures (ranging from 1 [always disagree] to 5 [always agree]) on perceptions of pharmaceutical manufacturers as being competent, innovative, and trustworthy.

Figure 3 illustrates the associations between DTCA exposure and health-related beliefs across 2 categories: (1) medication and (2) physical activity and diet. Figure 3A shows results for medication-related beliefs, defined as perceived effectiveness of medication and perceived seriousness of heart disease. Respondents exposed to pharmaceutical DTCA were more likely to hold beliefs that medication is an effective treatment for heart disease and that heart disease is a serious condition. In particular, respondents in the treatment groups were statistically significantly more likely to “always agree” with the statement that medication is an effective treatment for heart disease (ME = 0.04; P = .003) and the statement that heart disease is serious (ME = 0.04; P = .02).

Figure 3. Marginal Effect of Treatment on Consumer Health-Related Beliefs.

Figure 3.

Presented are marginal effects estimated from ordered logit regressions of each outcome measure on treatment assignment, controlling for respondent age, sex, race, household income, and indicators for which of the inclusion criteria the respondent met (ie, high cholesterol, current smoker, or overweight or obese). The ordered logit regressions estimated the effects of being randomized to either treatment group (ie, either treatment 1 or treatment 2) compared with those who were randomized to the control group. Outcome measures include questions on health-related beliefs related to medication, physical activity, and diet. Medication-related questions include ordinal measures (ranging from 1 [always disagree] to 5 [always agree]) on perceived seriousness of heart disease and perceived effectiveness of heart disease medication (A). Measures related to the perceived importance of physical activity and diet include ordinal measures (ranging from 1 [always disagree] to 5 [always agree]) on beliefs related to physical activity and dietary behavior (eg, “Eating healthier food is important,” “Physical activity is important”) (B).

Figure 3B shows the associations between DTCA exposure and physical activity– and diet-related beliefs, defined as the perceived importance of physical activity and healthy eating. Respondents exposed to DTCA were more likely to have favorable perceptions of the importance of physical activity and healthy eating, with no changes to perceived ability to engage in these activities. In particular, respondents exposed to DTCA were more likely to report that they “always agree” with the statement that it is important to be physically active (ME = 0.05, P = .01) and eat healthily (ME = 0.08; P < .001). We found similar results for perceived pleasantness of physical activity and healthy eating (eFigures 4 and 5 in the Supplement). There were no differences around respondents’ perceived ability to engage in physical activity or healthy eating (eFigure 5 in the Supplement).

There were no statistically significant differences across the 2 treatment arms that did and did not receive price disclosure for outcomes related to medication-related intentions (eFigure 2 in the Supplement), indirect behavioral intentions (eFigure 3 in the Supplement), perceptions of pharmaceutical manufacturers (eFigure 4 in the Supplement), or health-related beliefs (eFigure 5 in the Supplement). eFigure 6 in the Supplement shows similar results when reestimating models with Ipsos-generated survey weights. eTable 6 in the Supplement summarizes Romano-Wolf P values for multiple testing. While these adjustments limit the ability to claim statistical significance in the present outcomes, many outcomes have been tested, and this adjustment leans on the conservative side. Finally, eFigure 7 in the Supplement presents regression results without demographic controls, which are consistent with descriptive unadjusted distributions for all outcomes (eTables 2, 3, 4, and 5 in the Supplement).

Discussion

This cross-sectional study with a survey found that brief exposure to pharmaceutical DTCA has positive, if nuanced, associations around medication-related demand intentions. Direct-to-consumer advertising was associated with increased searching for information but no increased likelihood of physician conversations. This is unsurprising and confirms prior research examining the associations of DTCA in observational settings.2,3,4,5,6 These advertisements are commercial in nature, and their goal is to increase the sales of the advertised drug. Most shifts in intentions came from the “very unlikely” category, suggesting that consumers may be more likely to shift away from previously held negative beliefs than positive ones.31

We found mixed evidence around diet and exercise. On one hand, we found that exposure to DTCA was not associated with intention to engage in exercise or eat a healthy diet. On the other hand, we found that DTCA was associated with more favorable beliefs about diet and exercise. These results were smaller in magnitude and with mixed statistical significance when compared with the results around medication. Overall, the results suggest that exposure to DTCA for prescription drugs is unlikely to have an adverse effect on diet and exercise. Prior research has found that DTCA generally emphasizes drug promotion over general health education or healthy behaviors.32 However, a substantial proportion of DTCA, including advertisements used in this study, highlights healthy lifestyle behaviors. Prior research using observational data has found a similarly mixed picture of the associations of DTCA for prescription drugs with health-related behaviors.5 It is also possible that DTCA has a societal-level cumulative effect on these beliefs because those in the treatment and control groups have been exposed to DTCA for several decades. While we found no association of a single exposure to DTCA, this does not mean that DTCA has not affected this population.

We found no statistically significant differences across the treatment arms that did and did not receive price disclosure, in contrast with prior results from Garrett et al13 examining the influence of DTCA price disclosure on consumer decision-making.13 While the present study focused on the most commonly advertised products for heart disease, regardless of cost, Garrett et al focused on a fictitious diabetes drug with a very high price ($15 500 per month). The difference in results suggests that price disclosure may be more likely to shift consumer thinking when it comes to very high-priced drugs and that the information would be less salient for more moderately priced drugs. These results are in line with the broader literature on price transparency in health care, which suggests that public information does not shift demand in appreciable ways.33,34,35,36 The bipartisan Drug-price Transparency for Competition Act is currently pending before Congress.12 The present results suggest that consumers are not strongly influenced by price disclosure for moderately priced drugs, and policy makers may wish to consider alternative strategies for promoting competition in this space.

Limitations

This study has limitations. First, this was an online survey in which study participants viewed advertisements in one sitting. In reality, consumers are exposed to DTCA throughout a variety of media (scrolling through a smartphone, watching television/streaming services, and banner/video advertisements on the web). Despite this artificial setting, the present results have internal validity (owing to the randomized nature of the survey) and a case for generalizability because we recruited a large sample (n = 2874) that was nationally representative of the US population with or at risk for heart disease. Second, the study was unable to address actual demand for prescription drugs and instead had to focus on beliefs and intentions. This may be particularly worrisome in the health care context, where the link between behavioral intentions and eventual behavior change can be weak.37 Third, the study focused on only the short-term behavioral intentions even though advertising is known to have both an instantaneous (flow) and long-term accumulation and depreciation (stock) effect. Future research should focus on the long-term effects of advertising in a real-world, randomized setting. Fourth, the study lacks a true control group because pharmaceutical DTCA has been ubiquitous in the US for several decades. Fifth, the analysis only examined DTCA relative to advertisements for nonhealth consumer products. The results may be explained by priming effects, and we might have seen similar effects if the treatment group was exposed to advertisements for healthy foods. Lastly, we focused only on 5 advertisements for a single product category (heart disease), limiting generalizability to other conditions.

Conclusion

Results of this cross-sectional study with a survey show that brief exposure to pharmaceutical DTCA has a large and positive association on medication-related demand intentions with no offsetting negative spillovers on lifestyle-related intentions. These results suggest that sustained expansion of DTCA in the US can have important demand effects on consumer behavior. This study indicates that the threat that DTCA may generate welfare-reducing lifestyle changes is not supported by the data. Moreover, the present results suggest DTCA might influence consumer views on policies related to pharmaceutical manufacturers. Lack of associations with price disclosure in DTCA suggests that policy makers should consider alternative strategies to promote value-based decision-making for prescription drugs.

Supplement.

eAppendix. Survey Instrument

eTable 1. Balance in Baseline Covariates Across Treatment Groups

eFigure 1. Marginal Effect of Treatment Exposure on Indirect Behavioral Intentions

eFigure 2. Marginal Effect of Treatment Exposure on Medication-Related Intentions, By Treatment Arm

eFigure 3. Marginal Effect of Treatment Exposure on Indirect Behavioral Intentions, By Treatment Arm

eFigure 4. Marginal Effect of Treatment on Brand Perceptions

eFigure 5. Marginal Effect of Treatment on Health-Related Beliefs, By Treatment Arm

eFigure 6. Marginal Effect of Treatment on Medication-Related Intentions, Indirect Behavioral Intentions, Brand Perceptions, and Health-Related Beliefs (weighted regressions)

eFigure 7. Marginal Effect of Treatment on Medication-Related Intentions, Indirect Behavioral Intentions, Brand Perceptions, and Health-Related Beliefs (without demographic controls)

eTable 2. Descriptive Statistics for Medication-Related Intentions, By Treatment Arm

eTable 3. Descriptive Statistics for Indirect Behavioral Intentions, By Treatment Arm

eTable 4. Descriptive Statistics for Brand Perceptions, By Treatment Arm

eTable 5. Descriptive Statistics for Health-Related Beliefs, By Treatment Arm

eTable 6. Unadjusted and Romano-Wolf P values After Multiplicity Adjustment

References

  • 1.Prescription drugs: Medicare spending on drugs with direct-to-consumer advertising. US Government Accountability Office . May 18, 2021. Accessed July 18, 2022. https://www.gao.gov/products/gao-21-380
  • 2.Eisenberg MD, Avery RJ, Cantor JH. Vitamin panacea: is advertising fueling demand for products with uncertain scientific benefit? J Health Econ. 2017;55:30-44. doi: 10.1016/j.jhealeco.2017.06.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Alpert A, Lakdawalla D, Sood N. Prescription drug advertising and drug utilization: the role of Medicare Part D. National Bureau of Economic Research working paper 21714. November 2015. Accessed July 18, 2022. https://www.nber.org/papers/w21714
  • 4.Shapiro BT. Positive spillovers and free riding in advertising of prescription pharmaceuticals: the case of antidepressants. J Polit Econ. 2018;126(1):381-437. doi: 10.1086/695475 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Niederdeppe J, Avery RJ, Kellogg MD, Mathios A. Mixed messages, mixed outcomes: exposure to direct-to-consumer advertising for statin drugs is associated with more frequent visits to fast food restaurants and exercise. Health Commun. 2017;32(7):845-856. doi: 10.1080/10410236.2016.1177903 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Avery RJ, Eisenberg MD, Simon KI. The impact of direct-to-consumer television and magazine advertising on antidepressant use. J Health Econ. 2012;31(5):705-718. doi: 10.1016/j.jhealeco.2012.05.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Gallo KP, Comer JS, Barlow DH, Clarke RN, Antony MM. Direct-to-consumer marketing of psychological treatments: a randomized controlled trial. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2015;83(5):994-998. doi: 10.1037/a0039470 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Wineinger NE, Zhang Y, Topol EJ. Trends in prices of popular brand-name prescription drugs in the United States. JAMA Netw Open. 2019;2(5):e194791. doi: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2019.4791 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Dusetzina SB, Bach PB. Prescription drugs—list price, net price, and the rebate caught in the middle. JAMA. 2019;321(16):1563-1564. doi: 10.1001/jama.2019.2445 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Dabbous M, François C, Chachoua L, Toumi M. President Trump’s prescription to reduce drug prices: from the campaign trail to American Patients First. J Mark Access Health Policy. 2019;7(1):1579597. doi: 10.1080/20016689.2019.1579597 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Gardner J. In win for pharma, Trump TV drug price rule struck down. BioPharma Dive . July 9, 2019. Accessed July 18, 2022. https://www.biopharmadive.com/news/in-win-for-pharma-trump-tv-drug-price-rule-struck-down/558376/
  • 12.Durbin, Grassley, King introduce bill to require price transparency in prescription drug advertisements. News release. United States Senator of Illinois Dick Durbin website . June 24, 2021. Accessed July 18, 2022. https://www.durbin.senate.gov/newsroom/press-releases/durbin-grassley-king-introduce-bill-to-require-price-transparency-in-prescription-drug-advertisements
  • 13.Garrett JB, Tayler WB, Bai G, Socal MP, Trujillo AJ, Anderson GF. Consumer responses to price disclosure in direct-to-consumer pharmaceutical advertising. JAMA Intern Med. 2019;179(3):435-437. doi: 10.1001/jamainternmed.2018.5976 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Heart disease facts. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Updated July 15, 2022. Accessed July 18, 2022. https://www.cdc.gov/heartdisease/facts.htm
  • 15.Medications used to treat heart failure. American Heart Association . Updated May 31, 2017. Accessed July 18, 2022. https://www.heart.org/en/health-topics/heart-failure/treatment-options-for-heart-failure/medications-used-to-treat-heart-failure
  • 16.Krist AH, Davidson KW, Mangione CM, et al. ; US Preventive Services Task Force . Behavioral counseling interventions to promote a healthy diet and physical activity for cardiovascular disease prevention in adults with cardiovascular risk factors: US Preventive Services Task Force recommendation statement. JAMA. 2020;324(20):2069-2075. doi: 10.1001/jama.2020.21749 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.SSR health . Accessed July 18, 2022. https://www.ssrhealth.com/
  • 18.von Elm E, Altman DG, Egger M, Pocock SJ, Gøtzsche PC, Vandenbroucke JP; STROBE Initiative . The Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) statement: guidelines for reporting observational studies. Bull World Health Organ. 2007;85(11):867-872. doi: 10.2471/BLT.07.045120 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Standard definitions: final dispositions of case codes and outcome rates for surveys. American Association for Public Opinion Research . Revised 2016. Accessed July 18, 2022. https://www.aapor.org/aapor_main/media/publications/standard-definitions20169theditionfinal.pdf
  • 20.Brophy JM, Joseph L, Rouleau JL. Beta-blockers in congestive heart failure: a Bayesian meta-analysis. Ann Intern Med. 2001;134(7):550-560. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-134-7-200104030-00008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Zhu J, Chen N, Zhou M, et al. Calcium channel blockers versus other classes of drugs for hypertension. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2021;10(10):CD003654. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Ahmed A. Quality and outcomes of heart failure care in older adults: role of multidisciplinary disease-management programs. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2002;50(9):1590-1593. doi: 10.1046/j.1532-5415.2002.50418.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.McAlister FA, Lawson FM, Teo KK, Armstrong PW. A systematic review of randomized trials of disease management programs in heart failure. Am J Med. 2001;110(5):378-384. doi: 10.1016/S0002-9343(00)00743-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Anderson L, Thompson DR, Oldridge N, et al. Exercise-based cardiac rehabilitation for coronary heart disease. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2016;(1):CD001800. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Caterson ID, Finer N, Coutinho W, et al. ; SCOUT Investigators . Maintained intentional weight loss reduces cardiovascular outcomes: results from the Sibutramine Cardiovascular OUTcomes (SCOUT) trial. Diabetes Obes Metab. 2012;14(6):523-530. doi: 10.1111/j.1463-1326.2011.01554.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Seimon RV, Espinoza D, Ivers L, et al. Changes in body weight and blood pressure: paradoxical outcome events in overweight and obese subjects with cardiovascular disease. Int J Obes (Lond). 2014;38(9):1165-1171. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2014.2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Kolbl Ž, Arslanagic-Kalajdzic M, Diamantopoulos A. Stereotyping global brands: is warmth more important than competence? J Bus Res. 2019;104:614-621. doi: 10.1016/j.jbusres.2018.12.060 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Lakerveld J, Bot SD, Chinapaw MJ, Knol DL, de Vet HC, Nijpels G. Measuring pathways towards a healthier lifestyle in the Hoorn prevention study: the determinants of lifestyle behavior questionnaire (DLBQ). Patient Educ Couns. 2011;85(2):e53-e58. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2011.01.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Bradley VC, Kuriwaki S, Isakov M, Sejdinovic D, Meng XL, Flaxman S. Unrepresentative big surveys significantly overestimated US vaccine uptake. Nature. 2021;600(7890):695-700. doi: 10.1038/s41586-021-04198-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Clarke D, Romano JP, Wolf M. The Romano–Wolf multiple-hypothesis correction in Stata. Stata J. 2020;20(4):812-843. doi: 10.1177/1536867X20976314 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Kardes FR, Posavac SS, Cronley ML. Consumer inference: a review of processes, bases, and judgment contexts. J Consum Psychol. 2004;14(3):230-256. doi: 10.1207/s15327663jcp1403_6 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Bell RA, Wilkes MS, Kravitz RL. The educational value of consumer-targeted prescription drug print advertising. J Fam Pract. 2000;49(12):1092-1098. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Sinaiko AD, Rosenthal MB. Increased price transparency in health care—challenges and potential effects. N Engl J Med. 2011;364(10):891-894. doi: 10.1056/NEJMp1100041 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Epstein D, Mason A. Costs and prices for inpatient care in England: mirror twins or distant cousins? Health Care Manag Sci. 2006;9(3):233-242. doi: 10.1007/s10729-006-9090-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Sinaiko AD, Rosenthal MB. Examining a health care price transparency tool: who uses it, and how they shop for care. Health Aff (Millwood). 2016;35(4):662-670. doi: 10.1377/hlthaff.2015.0746 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Lieber EM. Does it pay to know prices in health care? Am Econ J Econ Policy. 2017;9(1):154-179. doi: 10.1257/pol.20150124 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Faries MD. Why we don’t “just do it”: understanding the intention-behavior gap in lifestyle medicine. Am J Lifestyle Med. 2016;10(5):322-329. doi: 10.1177/1559827616638017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplement.

eAppendix. Survey Instrument

eTable 1. Balance in Baseline Covariates Across Treatment Groups

eFigure 1. Marginal Effect of Treatment Exposure on Indirect Behavioral Intentions

eFigure 2. Marginal Effect of Treatment Exposure on Medication-Related Intentions, By Treatment Arm

eFigure 3. Marginal Effect of Treatment Exposure on Indirect Behavioral Intentions, By Treatment Arm

eFigure 4. Marginal Effect of Treatment on Brand Perceptions

eFigure 5. Marginal Effect of Treatment on Health-Related Beliefs, By Treatment Arm

eFigure 6. Marginal Effect of Treatment on Medication-Related Intentions, Indirect Behavioral Intentions, Brand Perceptions, and Health-Related Beliefs (weighted regressions)

eFigure 7. Marginal Effect of Treatment on Medication-Related Intentions, Indirect Behavioral Intentions, Brand Perceptions, and Health-Related Beliefs (without demographic controls)

eTable 2. Descriptive Statistics for Medication-Related Intentions, By Treatment Arm

eTable 3. Descriptive Statistics for Indirect Behavioral Intentions, By Treatment Arm

eTable 4. Descriptive Statistics for Brand Perceptions, By Treatment Arm

eTable 5. Descriptive Statistics for Health-Related Beliefs, By Treatment Arm

eTable 6. Unadjusted and Romano-Wolf P values After Multiplicity Adjustment


Articles from JAMA Health Forum are provided here courtesy of American Medical Association

RESOURCES