Skip to main content
African Health Sciences logoLink to African Health Sciences
. 2022 Mar;22(1):367–376. doi: 10.4314/ahs.v22i1.45

Effects of hypohydration and fluid balance in athletes' cognitive performance: a systematic review

Adiele Dube 1, Chantell Gouws 1, Gerrit Breukelman 1
PMCID: PMC9382508  PMID: 36032481

Abstract

Background

The effects of progressive body fluid loss on athletic and cognitive performance are known to result from exposure to environmental heat stress, morphologic factors, and limited fluid replenishment. Athletes need to restore lost body water. However, athletes may fail to maintain euhydration during exercise. This systematic review investigated hypohydration and fluid balance effects on an athlete's cognitive function.

Methods

The PubMed, Sports Discuss, and Ebsco databases were searched for studies reporting on hypohydration, fluid balance and heat on cognitive performance in sport. Multiple phrases including hydration, dehydration, fluid balance, mood, cognition, vigilance, decision making, and brain were explored. Participants in the studies did either receive fluid or did not receive fluid during exercise.

Results

Twenty-four trials (n=493 participants) from 24 articles met the inclusion criteria. Significant hypohydration, >2% body mass loss was reported consistently in 16 publications. Five articles where hypohydration was associated with heat stress and limited fluid intake (3–5% body mass loss) impaired cognitive performance. Mood disturbance, fatigue, and ratings of perceived exertion constantly complemented hypohydration impairment on cognition.

Conclusion

Findings show that hypohydration impairs cognitive performance and mood at higher levels of 3–5% body mass loss. However, sport-specific cognitive protocols of accessing hypohydration and fluid balance in individual and team sports remain equivocal.

Keywords: Hypohydration, cognition, mood, fluid replenishment

Introduction

Mega sporting events will continue to take place in diverse hot geographical environment across the globe as they have been in the past and in the present. As always, hypohydration can be expected in these events. The events include the Olympic Games: Beijing 2008; Rio 2016, Tokyo 2021; World Athletics Championships Doha 2019, and Federation International Football Association World Championships Qatar 2022 1,2. With such exposure to hot environments, athletes mainly in prolonged vigorous exercise, racket and intermittent team sports experience significant and exceeding >2% body fluid loss due to thermoregulation 2,3. Inadequate and/or no fluid loss replacement can cause endurance capacity impairment associatd with physiological and cognitive function alterations4,5. Indeed excessive dehydration impacts are major cause of concern to athletic trainers and sports medical staff.

Dehydration and hypohydration deleterious effects on athletic performance and cognition have been widely researched1,3,5. It is well known that a 2% body mass loss can impair endurance performance in humid/hot environments6,7. There has been limited research on the impact of hypohydration on athlete's cognitive performance and mood during individual and intermittent team sport8,9. Literature has supported that dehydration may impair cognitive performance10- 12 and functional task13. However, it is known that rehydration may cause minimal or no effect on athletic, cognition and immunological performance if the outcome to be assessed is insensitive to the modest (up to 2% of body weight) fluid losses1, 5, 8. Severe dehydration may disturb, aggravated fatigue, dizziness, confusion and often severe cases lead to delirium, coma and death1419. Various studies have demonstrated that heat-stress and exercise-induced dehydration did not alter cognitive performance4, 11, 20, 21. However, inconsistent conclusions exist in the current literature4, 5, 8, 12. Some studies have demonstrated discrepancies in literature may be due to task complexity, test duration, magnitude of heat stress, test combined11, 13.

Prolonged exercising in hot, humid environments with inadequate fluid replenishment may increase core body temperature (hyperthermia) to ∼4°C provoking athlete's mental status that worsen in moderately and untrained athletes4, 22, 23. Despite that elite acclimated athletes may physiologically negotiate hyperthermic conditions, athletic trainers, sports scientists and sports medical staffs tirelessly work to uncover cooling techniques to curb core body temperature, delayed onset peripheral and central fatigue4, 2326. Thus, researchers have investigated dehydration, hypohydration and fluid ingestion aspects and their subsequent athletic performance effects2,8 remains unclear. To date, no papers have reviewed and collectively discussed these aspects to equip professionals better understand impact on individual and team sport performances. Therefore, the aim of this systematic review was to summarise to summarise the literature assessing impact on hypohydration and fluid balance in relation to cognitive function in semi-professional to elite athletes exercising in humid, hot environments.

Methods

The study protocol was devised following the specifications outlined in the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-analysis (PRISMA) Statement27.

Literature Search strategy

Relevant research studies on dehydration and hypohydration effects on cognitive function when training in hot, humid environments identified on electronic database from 2005 until May 2020. Available literature before 2005 focused mainly on athletic performance of elite athletes. For the purpose of the current review focus was on cognitive function and mood of semi-professional compared to elite athletes. The database include: PubMed/Medline, Sports Discuss, and Ebsco. Keywords and terms search were hydration, athletic, exercise, mood, attention, vigilance, decision making, reaction time, sweat loss, individual/team sport, ad libitum, water, fluid (eg. administration, consumption, ingestion, intake, replacement, replenishment), hydration (de-, eu-, hypo-, re-), each combined with cognitive/cognition (aspects, function), brain were explored. Searches were restricted to full-length peer-reviewed published articles in the English language. Unpublished experimental observations, published abstracts records that contain irrelevant terms (children, elderly, patient, disease, rat/mouse) were excluded. Twenty four original research studies involving fluid balance (water, flavoured water/ ad libitum water/ sodium chloride solution/ sports drink) not controlled by investigators, and sweat rate in athletes were included. These studies measured impact of hypohydration on cognitive performance. Little attention was paid to effects of physical/athletic performance considering that the data was available and up to date.

Inclusion

All research studies with fulfilling the following criteria were eligible for inclusion;

  1. All relevant South Africa and international studies.

  2. Studies with standardized dehydration protocol.

  3. Focussed on male or female humans with no underlying medical conditions (≥ 13 years of age).

  4. Fluid consumption was done in limited time ≤ 4 hours between dehydration protocol and subsequent performance test.

  5. A cognitive function and athletic performance outcome measured.

Exclusion

  1. Unpublished experimental observations, published abstracts records that contain irrelevant terms (elderly, patient, disease, and rat/mouse) were excluded.

  2. Experimental designs without cognitive performance outcome measured.

Data Extraction

All published scientific research peer-reviewed articles meeting the inclusion criteria were extracted and considered for the following characteristics; participant, exercise and hydration protocol, change in body mass, study findings, and study limitations. Research studies that contained more than one intervention and eligible for inclusion tested cognitive performance under two different conditions were treated as separate trials16. These trials derived from one study are denoted and cited by letters (a-b). Where necessary information was not given, the author considered it as unavailable.

Fluid balance terminology

An athlete is responsible for maintaining normal hydration status (euhydration) for optimal body performance. Armstrong28 suggests that change in body fluid balance is a resultant of baseline mass value compared to the individual body mass. Hydration status can be explained using terms such as; euhydration (normal baseline body water content), hypohydration (excessive body water deficit) and dehydration (progressive body water loss from normal baseline to hypohydration).

Search results

567 articles were potentially relevant. After the exclusion of duplicates, articles published before 2005, articles focusing on athletic performance, ageing, diseases and children below 13 years and review of full-text versions, 24 articles were selected for review as shown in Fig 1 and Table 1 below.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

PRISMA Flow Chart of study selection process

Table 1.

Summary of research studies evaluating effects hydration levels on cognitive performance

Citation Participants Protocol Hydration loss
levels (% Δ body
mass
Fluid Type Key Findings Limitations
Physiological and
subjective
measures
Cognitive
domains
assessed
Hyperthermia and
Hypohydration effects on
cognition
Goodman
et al. (29)
N=15
12M,
3 Gender not revealed
Military Defence Force
21–34yrs

USA
Crossover designed, 90min self-
paced military march in

standardised military attire in the
heat with a 20kg backpack
Fluid restriction and or prescribed
fluid intake throughout the
exercise.
Cognitive test battery
Envir. Conditions:
39.5–41.8°C, 28–42% RH
2.28 (HYP trial- no

fluid)
0.53 (EUY trial –
Ad libitum fluid)
Ad libitum
water
HYP:
↑Core body temp.
↑HR,
↑RPE, ↑Thirst.

EUY:
No significant
difference in core
body temp,
perceived thirst
Information
processing,
memory,
impulsivity,
attention, and
concentration,
response time
domains
HYP:
↓working memory
↓response times
↓attention task
↓depression
↓accuracy

No significant effect on
immediate or delayed
memory, accuracy, and
response speed.
Participants not blinded
to hydration status
Wittbrodt
et al. (30)
N=13
7M, 6F
19–28yrs
Healthy recreationally
active adults

USA
2-week; Counterbalanced 150min
trial (intermittent exercise
protocol):
Three experimental; no exercise
heat stress (CON), exercise heat
seat stress with fluid replacement
(EUY), exercise heat stress with
dehydration (DEHY), exercise
heat stress without fluid
replacement (HYP)
Visuomotor Pacing Task (VMPT)
Envir. Conditions:
EUY, HYD, HYP : 45°C, 15%
RH
CON: 22 °C, 30% RH
3.1 DEHY, HYP
(3.3 men
3.1 women)

0.2 EUY

0.0. CON
EUY, DEHY:
water
equivalent to
sweat

HYP:
No fluid, only
mouth rinse
once per hour
↑HR, ↑Rectal temp.,
RPE, Thirst
Visuomotor
functioning,
Accuracy,
reaction time
EUY, DEHY:
Visuomotor performance
impaired

A significant effect on
processing accuracy, and
reaction time
Participants not blinded
to hydration status
MacLeod
et al. (1)
N=8F
19–22yrs
Healthy unacclimatized
elite hockey players

UK
4 experimental sessions:
50min Hockey intermittent
treadmill protocol
with prescribed fluid intake to
replace sweat loss; ad libitum
water intake, or no fluid
Cognitive testing after treadmill
protocol
Envir. conditions:
Hot; 33.2–33.4°C, 58–60% RH
Moderate; 13–19°C, 51–55% RH
HYP:
∼2 no fluid

EUY:
∼ 0.0
No difference in ad
water intake on
moderate temp.
Ad libitum
water
↑RPE ↑Thirst (HYP)
prior to treadmill
protocol

No significant effect
on HR and Temp
(body core)
Process speed,
working
memory,
perceptive
discrimination,
visual scanning/
processing speed
HYP:
↑Psychomotor function,
visual scanning/ process
speed
EUY;
↑ working memory
Participants not blinded
to hydration status
Piil et al. (13) N=139
139M
30–32yrs
Recreationally active

Cyprus, Denmark,
Greece, Spain

(Compiled in Greece)
Laboratory experiment: (EUY,
DEHY)
Occupational study (urine
sampling), 8M for laboratory
experiment in an environmental
chamber with fluid replacement
Motor-cognitive test battery pre-
& post-
Envir conditions: Manufacturing;
29°C, 25% H
Agriculture; 29 °C, 55% RH
Police officers; 27 °C, 50% RH
Tourism; 30 °C, 55% RH
Construction; 26 °C, 54% RH
Environmental chamber; 40 °C,
25% RH
∼ 2.0 (no fluid)

0.0 (fluid
replacement)
Water ↑RPE, ↑Core body
temp., ↑Thermal
comfort,
↑Thirst
↑HR
Process speed,
working visual
scanning/
processing speed
No significant effect on
cognitive domains
Participants unaware of
the researcher's
hypothesis and naive to
the purpose of the studies
Van den Heuvel
et al. (31)
N=17
17M
25yrs
Healthy, non-smoking

Australia
Three Passive thermal-hydration
protocol (water immersion) with
states and then clamped using
controlled, isotonic fluid
administration.
Unique immersion protocol
establishment in the first trial and
replicated in subsequent trials
averaging 185min (137–242min)
Envir. Conditions:
Temperate; 34–35 °C,
Warm water; 40–41 °C
3 and 5 (HYP)

0.0 (EUY trial)
sodium
chloride
NaCI+
↑HR, Thermal state,
Core body temp., in
HYP at 3% and 5%
Visual
perception,
working memory
↑Decision process modified
↓Depression
↓Discriminative ability
(hyperthermia)

No significant effect visual
and working memory
following 3–5% dehydration
Participants not blinded
to hydration status
Gamage et al.
(32)
N=30
30M
22yrs,
elite cricketers

UK
Fluid restriction(4ml/kg/h) or
fluid provision (12–15 ml/kg/h)
during 2h of standardised cricket
training

Envir Conditions:
Outdoors: 27.2–32.8 -°C89, R66H,
∼2mph wind speed
3.7 fluid restriction
trial
0.9 fluid provision
trial
Not reported Not reported Process speed,
working
memory,
perceptive
discrimination,
visual scanning/
processing speed
Not reported Participants not blinded
to hydration status

Fluid type unknown

No validity or reliability
testing of sport (cricket)
skill
Wittbrodt et al.
(33)
N=12
12M
Recreational active

USA
Vigorous exercise intensity for

50mins

Fluid assimilation time >50min

Envir Conditions:
Ambient temp 32°C, 65 RH
1.5 Water ↑ HR ↑altered skin
temp.
↑ Thirst,
↑ fatigue
Process speed,
working
memory,
perceptive
discrimination,
visual scanning/
processing speed
No effect Participants not blinded
to hydration status

Exercise intensity not
mentioned
Wilson et al. (34) N=8
8M
Licensed jockeys

UK
Exercise for 45 minutes

Fluid assimilation time ∼35min
1.8 Water Not reported Response
inhibition
No effect Participants not blinded
to hydration status
Owen et al. (35) N=13
13M
22 yr olds, soccer semi-
professional
players

UK
LIST protocol (90mims) with
prescribed fluid intake to replace
89 sweat loss; ad libitum, water
intake, or no fluid
LSST and LLSPT performed
after LIST protocol

Envir Conditions:
19.4°C, 59.4 RH
0.3 (water intake)

1.1 (ad libitum
water)

2.5 (no fluid)
Ad libitum
water
↑ RPE (no fluid than
water intake)

↑HR (no fluid than
water intake and ad
libitum water)
Process speed,
working
memory,
perceptive
discrimination,
visual
scanning/processing speed
No effect Participants were not
blinded to hydration
status
MacLeod
et al. (3)
N=8
8F
22yr olds, Elite field
hockey players

UK
2-day experiment
Day 1: Baseline hockey skill
measurement
Passive heat stress (39.9 °C, 73
RH) → controlled fluid intake to
induce HYPO or EUH

Day 2: 60 min-hockey imitated
and designed intermittent
treadmill protocol
Hockey skills test in a
gymnasium

Envir Conditions:
Treadmill protocol; 33.3 °C, 59
RH
Gym 16.3 °C 22.2 °C,
∼ 2 (HYP trial)

Day 2:
∼ 0 (EUY trial)

No difference in
fluid intake
Replacement fluid
loss (88 vs 80) %
Ad libitum
water
↑RPE and ↑Thirst
(HYPO) prior to
treadmill protocol

No significant effect
on HR and Temp
(body core)
Process speed,
working
memory,
perceptive
discrimination,
visual scanning/
processing speed
↓ decision making time (skills
test) ∼7 slower (HYP vs
EUY) prior to treadmill
protocol

No significant effect on
decision making time post
treadmill protocol
Protocol, not field sportspecific
but intermittent
treadmill protocol

Use of Day 1 passive
heat stress for Day 2
trials may be invalid

Participants not blinded
to hydration status
Hoffman et al.
(36)
N=10

10F
21 yr
division 1 college
Basketball player
USA
40 min live scrimmage exercise
Quick board lower body reaction
agility, Dynavision D2- visual
reaction time – all performed
prior and post live scrimmage

Envir Conditions:
Indoors 22.6°C, 50.9 RH
2.3 no fluid)

Not availed (water
intake)
Water No significant effect
on HR and player
load
Psychomotor
function/process
speed, visual
scanning/processing speed
No significant effect on visual
reaction time
Participants not blinded
to hydration status

No trial report for Δ
body mass during water
intake

Cognitive tests not
validated prior
Brandenburg &
Gaetz (37)
N=12

12F
24yr Basketball Elite
players

Canada
A descriptive study covering 2
international indoor matches

Envir Conditions:
22.5 – 23.5 °C
44–50 RH
1st match
-2.1 to +5

2nd match
-2 to +0.1
Diluted ad
libitum and
water
according to
individual
taste
↑ HR Process speed,
working
memory,
perceptive
discrimination,
visual
scanning/processing speed
No significant effect on field
goal percentage

Adverse relation (goal vs
body mass loss in the 2nd
match
Carbohydrate has the
confounding potential
effect on Goal percentage

No controlled trial (EUY)
Ely et al. (38) N=32
32M
Healthy and non-heat
3-week experiment
EUY and HYP trials,
3h work-rest cycle,
4 Sodium
chloride
(NaCI) + water
HYP (no fluid
replacement)
Psychomotor
function/process
No significant effect on mood
and cognition
Carbohydrate ingestion
may have confounding
Carvalho et al.
(39)
N=12
12M
14–15yr Basketball
national team players

Portugal
90 min training session
HYP trials
Basketball drills before and after
training
Envir Conditions:
Indoors; 21.9–26.0 -°C 5,4 4.18 .3
RH
2.5 (no fluid)
1.1 fluid intake)
Ad libitum
water
HYP trial:
↑RPE in
Process speed,
working
memory,
perceptive
discrimination,
visual
scanning/processing speed
Not availed Participants not blinded
to hydration status
EUY (control) trial not
available
Basketball drill not
validated prior
Ali et al. (40) N=10
10F
Soccer Premier division
players

New Zealand
90min LIST protocol with fluid
intake (15ml/kg) or without
LSPT performed before, during,
and after LIST
Envir Conditions
Not availed
2.2 (HYP)

1.0 (EUY)
Water HYP trial:
↑RPE, core
temperature, HR,
blood lactate
Processing speed,
perceptive
discrimination,
visual scanning
No significant effect;
perceived activation and (dis-
pleasure)
Participants not blinded
to hydration status

EUY (control) trial not
available
Giano et al. (17) N=24
24M
Physically fit

USA
3-day laboratory experiment.
DEHY + Diuretic
DEHY + Placebo
EUY + Placebo
Envir Conditions:
26.1–27.9 °C, 54
Wind speed
1.59 Water HYP trial:
↑RPE, core temp,
HR
Process speed,
working
memory,
perceptive
discrimination,
visual scanning/
processing speed
↑Processing speed and
working memory

↓Fatigue
Participants were not
blinded to hydration
status
Bandelow et al.
(21)
N=20

20M
University soccer
players

UK
Cognitive battery tests:
Sternberg test
Corsi block test, Finger tapping
test
Visual sensitivity test
Trials before, at half-time, after
the match
HYP
Envir Conditions:
34°C, 62 -65 RH
2.5 Ad libitum
water

Sports drink
Not reported Process speed,
working
memory,
perceptive
discrimination,
visual scanning/
processing speed
↓working memory (HYP)
No significant in fine motor
speed, working memory,
reaction time
EUY trial not available
(control)

No sport-specific
cognitive tests
D'Anci et al. (16)
a
N=31

16M; 15F
University lacrosse and
rowing athletes

USA
Study 1:
HYP trial,
EUY trial
Coach-run, hard natural practice
Cognitive test battery post-practice

Envir Conditions;
RH not stated
Assimilating time 60–70min
2.0 (HYP)

0.1 (EUY)
Water HYP trial:
↑Thirst, ↑POMS:
tension, anger,
fatigue, depression
↓ vigor
Vigilance
attention, shortterm
memory,
simple and
choice reaction,
map planning,
visual perception,
mathematical
addition,
mood
HYP:
↑Processing speed
↓Vigilance, depression (3–4%)
No effect on spatial memory,
reaction time, map planning,
mathematical addition
Participants were not
blinded to hydration
status
D'Anci et al. (16)
b
N=24

12M; 12F
University lacrosse,
rowing, and American
football athletes

USA
Study 2:
HYP trial,
EUY trial
Coach-run, hard natural practice
Cognitive test battery postpractice
Envir Conditions
RH not stated
Assimilating time 60–75min
1.7 (HYP)

+0.1 (EUY)
Water HYP trial:
↑Thirst, ↑POMS:
tension, anger,
fatigue, depression
↓ vigor
Vigilance
attention, short-term
memory,
simple and
choice reaction,
map planning,
visual perception,
mathematical
addition,
mood
No effect on short-term and
spatial memory, reaction
time, map planning,
mathematical addition
No sport-specific
cognitive tests
administered
Adam et al. (20) N=8
8M
Active soldiers (6)

USA
Heat exposure for 300 min
Envir Conditions:
20 °C, 50% RH
Wind speed 1 to 2.2 m/s
3.0 No fluid ↑Thirst, thermal
discomfort ↑altered
skin temp.
↑fatigue
↑HR
Processing speed,
working
memory,
perceptive
discrimination,
vigilance, visual
scanning
No significant effect on
cognitive domains
EUY trial not available
(control)
Baker et al. (41) N=11
11M
17–28yr male
competitive basketball
players

USA
Experimental: 3hr interval
walking in heat chamber; HYP
trials, EUY trials,
80 min stimulated match
Attention variables test: baseline,
post chamber, post-match
Envir Conditions:
40 °C, 20% RH (heat chamber),
room temp. (indoor match)
HYP:
1%, 2%, 3%, 4%

EUY: 0
No fluid

Flavoured
water
HYP trial (1–4%):
↑lightheaded,
overheat, fatigue

No effect of core
body temp.
Attention
variables,
perceptive
discrimination,
vigilance, visual
scanning
HYP trial (1–4%):
↑ commission and omission
errors
↓response time (6–8%)
Participants were not
blinded to hydration
status
Rationale of induced heat
stress to attention
variables test before a
basketball match
unrealistic
Edwards et al.
(42)
N=11
11M
moderately active
soccer players

New Zealand
90 min exercise:
45 min cycling,
45 min soccer match
(80 fluid loss replacement)
Post-match mental concentration
test (number identification)

Envir Conditions:
24- 25 -°C 5,5 4 R7H (cycling),
19- 21 -°C57, 4R6H (soccer
match)
0.7 (fluid intake)

2.1 (mouth rinse)

2.4 (no fluid)
Water mouth
rinse

No fluid
↑ HR ↑thermal
discomfort ↑altered
skin temp.
↑ Thirst,
↑ fatigue
Processing speed,
visual scanning
No significant effect on
mental concentration
Participants were not
blinded to hydration
status

The rationale of cycling
before a match in soccer
is doubtful
Serwah & Marino
(43)
N=8
8M
25yrs
Healthy volunteers

Australia
90min discontinuous fixedintensity
exercise:
3 experimental conditions (full
fluid replacement, half fluid
replacement, no fluid
Own bicycle mounted on the
electromagnetically braked cycle
trainer

Envir. Conditions:
31.3°C, 62.1–64.5% RH
Wind speed 2m/s
2.0 (full fluid)

1.0 (half fluid)

1.7 (no fluid)
Water No fluid:
↑HR
↑Skin temp.,
↑Thirst

No effect of core
body temp. in full
and half fluid
conditions
Processing speed,
working
memory,
perceptive
discrimination,
vigilance, visual
scanning
No significant effect on
cognitive domains
Participants were not
blinded to hydration
status

No sport can employ a
discontinuous
fixed-intensity nature of
exercise protocol
Szinnai et al (44) N=17
8M
7F
25–33yrs
Health non-smoking
volunteers

Switzerland
Experimental done in random
order EXP; CON
Female: Pre and post menstrual
Men:
Cognitive function test
Envir. Conditions:
Cognitive tests: 22°C
1.75 CON

3.26 DEHY
Mineral water No fluid:
↑HR
↑Fatigue
↑Thirst
↓Alertness

No significant effect
in the control group
Processing speed,
working
memory,
perceptive
discrimination,
vigilance, visual
scanning,
reaction time
No significant effect on
cognitive domains in
moderate dehydration
Participants were not
blinded to hydration
status

EUY- euhydration, HYD-Hydrated, DEHY- dehydration, HYP-hypohydration, CON- control, Envir. Conditions- environmental conditions, RH. relative humidity, HR- heart rate, N- Number, M- male, F-female, LIST- Loughborough intermittent shuttle test, LSPT-Loughborough soccer passing test, RPE-rate of perceived exertion, Temp- temperature, ↑- increase, ↓- decrease

Discussion

This systematic review aimed to summarise literature assessing the impact of hypohydration and fluid balance on cognitive function in semi-professional to elite athletes exercising in humid, hot environments. The discussion considered the risk factors posed by an increase in sweat loss to ≥2% body mass loss. Major causes of hypohydration were discussed as environmental factors, exercise intensity, and/ or limited fluid replacement in relation to the brain and cognitive performance. Effects on cognitive performance and mood in the studies included in this review considered individual and team sports with training or competition duration of more than 1 hour 8, 23, 26. Although hypohydration risk levels may vary in different sports, the review takes the notion that individual risk factors among athletes may be altered between low- and high-level categories depending on humidity, timing day/season and intensity level, hydrating behaviours, social and cultural considerations.

Fluid balance and the Brain

The brain, a complex active part of the human body is known for its high metabolism. It accounts for ∼15% of resting cardiac output and a relatively higher total body aerobic metabolism of ∼20%45, 46. To maintain its high metabolism, the brain depends solely on adequate circulation of oxygen, metabolic substrates, and metabolic by-products elimination5. Heat stress, hyperthermia, and dehydration are known physiological stressors to alter cerebral circulation and metabolism. Hypohydration was found to mediate brain function reduction by reducing cerebral blood flow and brain cell volume, hence increasing blood-brain permeability8.

Exercise stimulus causes adjustments to Cerebral Blood Flow (CBF). A study by Kety and Schmidt47 showed that CBF could not be altered during the athletic rest-to-exercise transition. Recent temporal resolution methods showed a ∼20% CBF rise due to endurance and moderate exercise intensities4649. Indeed, CBF is subdued with high exercise intensities and significantly surpass rest levels due to exhaustion50, 51.

Progressive dehydration during individual and/ or intermittent team sports without concomitant hyperthermia increases CBF52. However, when the athlete is resting, a 1.5°C increase in body core temperature causes a ∼15% CBF reduction53. It should be noted that both dehydration and hyperthermia changes CBF mechanisms in different exercises5, intensities, and environments. Dehydration ≥3% body mass loss during endurance exercise in a hot, humid environment reduces CBF due to cerebrovascular instability and cardiovascular drift5456. In contrast, CBF reduction is attenuated when there is equilibrium between body fluid lost through sweating and fluid replenishment during exercise5, 54, 57.

Heat-induced stress, hyperthermia, and dehydration effects on CBF are associated with prolonged aerobic exercise5. Previous studies reveal that CBF reduction is worsened during acute-intensity exercise in hot and humid environments23, 30, 51, 58, compared to cold and temperate environments. Similarly, elite athletes' dehydration levels during training or competition in hot environments are compensable despite reduced CBF and work rate than their untrained counterparts. When athletes maintain euhydration status, the mechanisms and dynamics of CBF tend to normalise51. Therefore, endurance exercising in a hot and humid environment provokes dehydration, and hyperthermia enhancing cerebrovascular strain with CBF decline.

Hypohydration and Cognition

Excessive dehydration (hypohydration) effects on cognitive performances have been widely researched across different ages and populations of varied physical fitness. Scientific evidence shows inconsistent findings. Some studies revealed that hypohydration does not affect cognition20, 42, 59 others showed a reduction in cognitive function among military, athletes, young healthy adults, and the elderly16, 17, 29 - 31, 41, 60. Despite evidence of fluid intake benefits on cognitive function observed, literature lacks a clear indication of better treatment efficacy on specific cognitive domains4, 60. Cheuvront & Kenefick7 indicated a lack of clear mechanism by which hypovolemia or hyperosmolality cause cognitive impairment. Studies, however, consistently report hypohydration effects on brain function through 1–4% body mass loss reported in cognitive performance literature4, 23, 30. Prolonged exercise in hot environments without fluid replacement elevates core body temperature thereby creating a cognitive burden21. The symptoms of hypohydration including thirst and negative mood states have an equal effect on accomplishing cognitive tasks and consequently impair function7, 8. Cognitive trials conducted in less than 5 minutes after dehydration protocol ended found that ≤ 2.8% body mass loss induced through fluid deprivation had no impact on cognitive-motor performance13, 40, 4244. Although many studies did not clearly show the time from the end of dehydration protocol to commencement of the cognitive tests, a significant raise in ratings of thirst, concentration, and ratings of perceived effort was found13, 16, 42. In all the above trials, the long-lasting effects of physiological stressors employed may obstruct fluid intake influencing cognitive performance8, 60.

Fluid replenishment attenuates Total Mood Disturbance in 3 of the 5 trials where mood was measured16, 23, 29, 31. Considering that mood effects and cognition were independent, it should be noted that the above three findings were objective compared to the subjective60. However, if not clearly stated, self-reported mood questionnaires are subjective and consider mood effects as dependent variables. It is certain that the influence of fluid replenishment on cognitive function and mood needs further research23, 60.

It should be noted that rehydration may have no or little effect on cognitive function in cases where outcome measured is not receptive to the modest fluid loss effects60. The amount of fluid ingested, and the time when the fluid was administered has varied physiological responses. These may be confounded in response to dehydration protocol (control and intervention trials) which could have implications on cognitive and athletic performance.

Conclusion

Considering that, most of the studies measured up to 2.7% body mass loss, the impact of hypohydration and fluid balance on cognitive performance in individual and team sports remains equivocal. In all the studies involved, measures of cognitive function altered include processing speed, vigilance, and reaction time for working memory. It is important to note that visuomotor reaction, mental concentration, and visual scanning and/ perception were not significantly affected by fluid balance and hypohydration. This inconsistency should inform the need to consider objectivity, subjectivity, validity, reliability, and sensitivity of cognitive function assessment tools for the athletic population. The current review serves to draw attention to areas for future research.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  • 1.MacLeod H, Cooper S, Bandelow S, Malcolm R, Sunderland C. Effects of heat stress and dehydration on cognitive function in elite female hockey players. Sport Sci Med and Rehab. 2018:10–12. doi: 10.1186/s13102-018-0101-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Rodríguez MÁ, Piedra JV, Sánchez-Fernández M, Del Valle M, Crespo I, Olmedillas H. A Matter of Degrees: A Systematic Review of the Ergogenic Effect of Pre-Cooling in Highly Trained Athletes. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2020;17(8):2952. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17082952. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.MacLeod H, Sunderland C. Previous-day hypohydration impairs skill performance in elite female field hockey players. Scand and J Med Sci Sports. 2012;22(3):430–438. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0838.2010.01230.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Belval LN, Hosokawa Y, Casa DJ, Adams WM, Armstrong LE, Baker LB, Burke L, Cheuvront S, Chiampas G, González-Alonso J, Huggins RA, Kavouras SA, Lee EC, McDermott BP, Miller K, Schlader Z, Sims S, Stearns RL, Troyanos C, Wingo J. Practical Hydration Solutions for Sports. Nutrients. 2019;11(7):1550. doi: 10.3390/nu11071550. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Trangmar SJ, González-Alonso J. Heat, Hydration and the Human Brain, Heart and Skeletal Muscles. Sp Med. 2019;49(Supp 1):S69–S85. doi: 10.1007/s40279-018-1033-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Sawka MN, Burke LM, Eichner ER, Maughan RJ, Montain SJ, Stachenfeld NS. American College of Sports Medicine position stand. Exercise and fluid replacement. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2007;39(2):377–390. doi: 10.1249/mss.0b013e31802ca597. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Cheuvront SN, Kenefick RW. Dehydration: physiology, assessment, and performance effects. Compr Physiol. 2014;4(1):257–285. doi: 10.1002/cphy.c130017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Nuccio RP, Barnes KA, Carter JM, Baker LB. Fluid Balance in Team Sport Athletes and the Effect of Hypohydration on Cognitive, Technical, and Physical Performance. Sports Med. 2017;47(10):1951–1982. doi: 10.1007/s40279-017-0738-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Zhang N, Du SM, Zhang JF, Ma GS. Effects of dehydration and rehydration on cognitive performance and mood among male college students in Changzhou, China: A self-controlled trial. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2019;16:1891. doi: 10.3390/ijerph16111891. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Ely BR, Lovering AT, Horowitz M, Minson CT. Heat acclimation and cross tolerance to hypoxia. Temp. 2014;1(2):107–114. doi: 10.4161/temp.29800. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Taylor L, Watkins SL, Marshall H, Dascombe BJ, Foster J. The Impact of Different Environmental Conditions on Cognitive Function: A Focused Review. Front Physiol. 2015;6:372. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2015.00372. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Trinies V, Chard AN, Mateo T, Freeman MC. Effects of Water Provision and Hydration on Cognitive Function among Primary-School Pupils in Zambia: A Randomized Trial. PLoS One. 2016;11(3):e0150071. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0150071. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Piil JF, Lundbye-Jensen J, Trangmar SJ, Nybo L. Performance in complex motor tasks deteriorates in hyperthermic humans. Temperature. 2017:1–9. doi: 10.1080/23328940.2017.1368877. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Cian C, Barraud PA, Melin B, Raphel C. Effects of fluid ingestion on cognitive function after heat stress or exercise-induced dehydration. Int J Psychophysiol. 2001;42:243–251. doi: 10.1016/s0167-8760(01)00142-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Lieberman HR, Bathalon GP, Falco CM, Kramer FM, Niro P. Severe decrements in cognition function and mood induced by sleep loss, heat, dehydration, and under-nutrition during simulated combat. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57:422–429. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.11.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.D'Anci KE, Vibhakar A, Kanter JH, Mahoney CR, Taylor HA. Voluntary dehydration and cognitive performance in trained college athletes. Percept Mot Skills. 2009;109(1):251–269. doi: 10.2466/PMS.109.1.251-269. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Ganio MS, Armstrong LE, Casa DJ, McDermott BP, Lee EC, Yamamoto LM, Marzano S, Lopez RM, Jimenez L, Le Bellego L, Chevillotte E, Lieberman HR. Mild dehydration impairs cognitive performance and mood of men. Br J Nutr. 2011;106(10):1535–1543. doi: 10.1017/S0007114511002005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Armstrong LE, Ganio MS, Casa DJ, Lee EC, McDermott BP, Klau JF, Jimenez L, Le Bellego L, Chevillotte E, Lieberman HR. Mild dehydration affects mood in healthy young women. J Nutr. 2012;142(2):382–388. doi: 10.3945/jn.111.142000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Masento NA, Golightly M, Field DT, Butler LT, van Reekum CM. Effects of hydration status on cognitive performance and mood. Br J Nutr. 2014;111:1841–1852. doi: 10.1017/S0007114513004455. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Adam GE, Carter R, 3rd, Cheuvront SN, Merullo DJ, Castellani JW, Lieberman HR, Sawka MN. Hydration effects on cognitive performance during military tasks in temperate and cold environments. Physiol Behav. 2008;93(4–5):748–756. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.11.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Bandelow S, Maughan R, Shirreffs S, Ozgünen K, Kurdak S, Ersöz G, Binnet M, Dvorak J. The effects of exercise, heat, cooling and rehydration strategies on cognitive function in football players. Scand J Med Sci Sports. 2010;20(S3):148–160. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0838.2010.01220.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Nybo L. Hyperthermia and fatigue. J Appl Physiol. 2008;104:871–878. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00910.2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Barnes K, Baker L. Hydration and team sport cognitive function, technical skill and physical performance. Sp Sci Exch. 2021;210:1–5. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Wegmann M, Faude O, Poppendieck W, Hecksteden A, Fröhlich M, Meyer T. Pre-cooling and sports performance: A meta-analytical review. Sports Med. 2012;42:545–564. doi: 10.2165/11630550-000000000-00000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Watson P, Head K, Pitiot A, Morris P, Maughan RJ. Effect of exercise and heat-induced hypohydration on brain volume. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2010;42(12):2197–2204. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0b013e3181e39788. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Douzi W, Dupuy O, Theurot D, Smolander J, Dugué B. Per-cooling (Using cooling systems during physical exercise) enhances physical and cognitive performances in hot environments. A narrative review. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2020;17:1031. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17031031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Panic N, Leoncini E, de Belvis G, Ricciardi W, Boccia S. Evaluation of the endorsement of the preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analysis (PRISMA) statement on the quality of published systematic review and meta-analyses. PLoS One. 2013;8:e83138. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0083138. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Armstrong LE. Assessing hydration status: the elusive gold standard. J Am Coll Nutr. 2007;26(5 Suppl):575S–584S. doi: 10.1080/07315724.2007.10719661. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Goodman SP, Moreland AT, Marino F. Maintaining euhydration preserves cognitive performance, but is not superior to hypohydration. J Cogn Enhanc. 2019;3:338–348. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Wittbrodt MT, Sawka MN, Mizelle JC, Wheaton LA, Millard-Stafford ML. Exercise-heat stress with and without water replacement alters brain structures and impairs visuomotor performance. Physiol Rep. 2018;6(16):e13805. doi: 10.14814/phy2.13805. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Van den Heuvel AMJ, Haberley BJ, Hoyle DJR, Taylor NAS, Croft RJ. The independent influences of heat strain and dehydration upon cognition. Eur J Appl Physiol. 2017;117(5):1025–1037. doi: 10.1007/s00421-017-3592-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Gamage JP, De Silva AP, Nalliah AK, Galloway SD. Effects of Dehydration on Cricket Specific Skill Performance in Hot and Humid Conditions. Int J Sport Nutr Exerc Metab. 2016;26(6):531–541. doi: 10.1123/ijsnem.2016-0015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Wittbrodt MT, Millard-Stafford M, Sherman RA, Cheatham CC. Fluid replacement attenuates physiological strain resulting from mild hypohydration without impacting cognitive performance. Int J Sport Nutr Exe. 2015;25(5):439–447. doi: 10.1123/ijsnem.2014-0173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Wilson G, Hawken MB, Poole I, Sparks A, Bennett S, Drust B, Morton J, Close GL. Rapid weight-loss impairs simulated riding performance and strength in jockeys: implications for making-weight. J Sports Sci. 2014;32(4):383–391. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2013.825732. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Owen JA, Kehoe SJ, Oliver SJ. Influence of fluid intake on soccer performance in a temperate environment. J Sports Sci. 2013;31(1):1–10. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2012.720701. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Hoffman JR, Stavsky H, Falk B. The effect of water restriction on anaerobic power and vertical jumping height in basketball players. Int J Sports Med. 1995;16(4):214–218. doi: 10.1055/s-2007-972994. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Brandenburg JP, Gaetz M. Fluid balance of elite female basketball players before and during game play. Int J Sport Nutr Exerc Metab. 2012;22(5):347–352. doi: 10.1123/ijsnem.22.5.347. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Ely BR, Sollanek KJ, Cheuvront SN, Lieberman HR, Kenefick RW. Hypohydration and acute thermal stress affect mood state but not cognition or dynamic postural balance. Eur J Appl Physiol. 2013;113(4):1027–1034. doi: 10.1007/s00421-012-2506-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Carvalho P, Oliveira B, Barros R, Padrão P, Moreira P, Teixeira VH. Impact of fluid restriction and ad libitum water intake or an 8% carbohydrate-electrolyte beverage on skill performance of elite adolescent basketball players. Int J Sport Nutr Exerc Metab. 2011;21(3):214–221. doi: 10.1123/ijsnem.21.3.214. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Ali A, Gardiner R, Foskett A, Gant N. Fluid balance, thermoregulation and sprint and passing skill performance in female soccer players. Scand J Med Sci Sports. 2011;21(3):437–445. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0838.2009.01055.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Baker LB, Conroy DE, Kenney WL. Dehydration impairs vigilance-related attention in male basketball players. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2007;39(6):976–983. doi: 10.1097/mss.0b013e3180471ff2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Edwards AM, Mann ME, Marfell-Jones MJ, Rankin DM, Noakes TD, Shillington DP. Influence of moderate dehydration on soccer performance: physiological responses to 45 min of outdoor match-play and the immediate subsequent performance of sport-specific and mental concentration tests. Br J Sports Med. 2007;41(6):385–391. doi: 10.1136/bjsm.2006.033860. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Serwah N, Marino FE. The combined effects of hydration and exercise heat stress on choice reaction time. J Sci Med Sport. 2006;9(1–2):157–164. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2006.03.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Szinnai G, Schachinger H, Arnaud MJ, Linder L, Keller U. Effect of water deprivation on cognitive-motor performance in healthy men and women. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. 2005;289(1):R275–R280. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00501.2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Lassen NA. Normal average value of cerebral blood flow in younger adults is 50 ml/100 g/min. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. 1985;5:347–349. doi: 10.1038/jcbfm.1985.48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Madsen PL, Holm S, Herning M, Lassen NA. Average blood flow and oxygen uptake in the human brain during resting wakefulness: a critical appraisal of the Kety-Schmidt technique. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. 1993;13:646–655. doi: 10.1038/jcbfm.1993.83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Kety SS, Schmidt CF. Measurement of cerebral blood flow and cerebral oxygen consumption in man. Fed Proc. 1946;5:264. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Nybo L, Nielsen B. Middle cerebral artery blood velocity is reduced with hyperthermia during prolonged exercise in humans. J Physiol. 2001;534:279–286. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.2001.t01-1-00279.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Sato K, Ogoh S, Hirasawa A, Oue A, Sadamoto T. The distribution of blood flow in the carotid and vertebral arteries during dynamic exercise in humans. J Physiol. 2011;589:2847–2856. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2010.204461. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Moraine JJ, Lamotte M, Berré J, Niset G, Leduc A, Naeijel R. Relationship of middle cerebral artery blood flow velocity to intensity during dynamic exercise in normal subjects. Eur J Appl Physiol Occup Physiol. 1993;67:35–38. doi: 10.1007/BF00377701. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Trangmar SJ, Chiesa ST, Kalsi KK, Secher NH, González-Alonso J. Whole body hyperthermia, but not skin hyperthermia, accelerates brain and locomotor limb circulatory strain and impairs exercise capacity in humans. Physiol Rep. 2017;5:e13108. doi: 10.14814/phy2.13108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Fan JL, Cotter JD, Lucas RAI, Thomas K, Wilson L, Ainslie PN. Human cardiorespiratory and cerebrovascular function during severe passive hyperthermia: effects of mild hypohydration. J Appl Physiol. 2008;105:433–445. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00010.2008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Ogoh S, Sato K, Okazaki K, Miyamoto T, Hirasawa A, Morimoto K, Shibasaki M. Blood flow distribution during heat stress: cerebral and systemic blood flow. J Cereb Blood Flow Meta. 2013;33:1915–1920. doi: 10.1038/jcbfm.2013.149. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Kempton MJ, Ettinger U, Foster R, Williams SC, Calvert GA, Hampshire A, Zelaya FO, O'Gorman RL, McMorris T, Owen AM, Smith MS. Dehydration affects brain structure and function in healthy adolescents. Hum Brain Mapp. 2011;32(1):71–79. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Périard JD, Racinais S. Heat stress exacerbates the reduction in middle cerebral artery blood velocity during prolonged self-paced exercise. Scand J Med Sci Sport. 2015;25:135–144. doi: 10.1111/sms.12379. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Watanabe K, Stöhr EJ, Akiyama K, Watanabe S, González-Alonso J. Dehydration reduces stroke volume and cardiac output during exercise because of impaired cardiac filling and venous return, not left ventricular function. Physiol Rep. 2020;8:e1443310. doi: 10.14814/phy2.14433. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Arnaoutis G, Kavouras SA, Stratakis N, Likka M, Mitrakou A, Papamichael C, Sidossis LS, Stamatelopoulos K. The effect of hypohydration on endothelial function in young healthy adults. Eur J Nutr. 2017;56:1211–1217. doi: 10.1007/s00394-016-1170-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Trangmar SJ, Chiesa ST, Stock CG, Kalsi KK, Secher NH, González-Alonso J. Dehydration affects cerebral blood flow but not its metabolic rate for oxygen during maximal exercise in trained humans. J Physiol. 2014;592:3143–3160. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2014.272104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.McGregor SJ, Nicholas CW, Lakomy HK, Williams C. The influence of intermittent high-intensity shuttle running and fluid ingestion on the performance of a soccer skill. J Sports Sci. 1999;17(11):895–903. doi: 10.1080/026404199365452. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.McCartney D, Desbrow B, Irwin C. The Effect of Fluid Intake Following Dehydration on Subsequent Athletic and Cognitive Performance: A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis. Sports Med Open. 2017;3(1):13. doi: 10.1186/s40798-017-0079-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from African Health Sciences are provided here courtesy of Makerere University Medical School

RESOURCES