Abstract
Linguistic racism shapes the psychological antecedents of code switching and its consequences for Black people and other people of color. We highlight mentalizing as an antecedent of code switching. We posit that stereotype threat arises in contexts where racism is salient, prompting scrutiny of others’ mental states (i.e., mentalizing) when making choices about linguistic self-presentation. Additionally, we posit that sustained appraisals of stereotype threat add cognitive load and reinforce self-protective code switching. We highlight potential consequences of linguistic racism for Black people and other people of color, including reduced opportunities for authentic self-presentation, increased emotional effort, and stress. Finally, we outline paths forward for research and practice: (1) recognizing the heterogeneity of language and thereby reducing linguistic racism, (2) implementing changes that promote racially affirming environments that reduce demands for self-protective code switching, and (3) adapting and creating scalable psychometric tools to measure linguistic choices and linguistic racism.
Keywords: Code switching, Mentalizing, Stereotype threat, Self-presentation, Linguistic racism
Introduction
Consider the following vignette: Nia is excited about her first week in graduate school.
She wants to do her best work, grow as a scientist, and make new friends and colleagues. However, she’s also very aware that she is the only Black student in her program. Nia feels comfortable with herself and the way that she speaks but understands that she might be perceived differently by others in her program, depending on how she communicates (e.g., her words, grammar, tone, mannerisms). One day, she decides to attend the first-year happy hour at a popular student bar nearby in hopes of getting to know her cohort better. Her arrival to the bar goes smoothly—members of her cohort say hello and she sits down with a group. But then, she gets a call from her sister. Normally, Nia would pick up immediately and greet her sister enthusiastically. Instead, she answers in a hushed voice, “Can I call you back later?”, concerned that her usual greeting would be seen as inappropriate in this new context. Her sister asks, “You don’t sound like yourself. Is everything ok?”. Feeling unsure about whether she can be herself and be embraced as a scholar by others in her program is unsettling and distracts Nia from fully participating in her cohort’s discussion of their first-year projects.
Nia’s situation is not unique. Graduate students from all racial backgrounds may face concerns about how they are perceived when first interacting with a new group of people.
However, in this context, Nia faces the additional risk of her speech practices being marked (or assessed by others) as improper or non-standard by listeners because of linguistic racism, a form of discrimination targeting the cultural and linguistic practices of Black people and other people of color (Baker-Bell, 2020; Dovchin, 2020). If a listener holds bias against Black people and/or their linguistic practices, this may color how they perceive Nia’s speech, regardless of the manner in which Nia is actually speaking (Rosa, 2019; Rosa & Flores, 2017). In other words, Nia could speak using linguistic practices that would go completely unmarked were they used by a White speaker and still be interpreted as speaking improperly (Samy Alim, 2007; Flores & Rosa, 2015). Although White graduate students can and do face concerns about how their speech is perceived, it is likely that their speech practices will go unmarked1 (i.e., will be perceived as standard/normative) by listeners, thereby inoculating them against linguistic racism as it manifests itself for Black people and other people of color (Cutler, 2003). In short, though Nia’s situation is not unique, she faces a particular risk above and beyond the challenges that come with interacting with a new group of people. These risks are largely due to the thoughts and beliefs surrounding race and language located in the hegemonic modes of perceptions listeners are socialized into (Flores & Rosa, 2015). The material consequences that follow carry risk for Black and other people of color in terms of career opportunities and feelings of authenticity (McCluney et al., 2019) within a cultural context that centers whiteness.
Objective
In this review, we explicitly link a raciolinguistic perspective that examines the ongoing social and historically constructed relationship between language and race in the minds of perceivers (Flores & Rosa, 2015), to psychological literature on mentalizing, stereotype threat, stress, and emotion. In making these links, we highlight some psychological mechanisms that may be involved in the linguistic choices people make, and the consequences of these linguistic choices for Black people and other people of color, due to the impact of linguistic racism. We acknowledge that there are many different antecedents to code switching, which may include processes common to many individuals’ linguistic decisions, such as mentalizing, as well as added vigilance (McCluney et al., 2019) demanded for Black people and other people of color in contexts where group identity is salient (Giles & Johnson, 1987) and specifically in racist environments (Hall et al., 2012). At the structural level, we argue for the need to implement changes that promote racially affirming environments that allow people to employ their full linguistic repertoire in social interactions and reduce external demands for self-alterations during interactions that do not feel authentic to the speaker’s sense of self. In service of these goals, we also seek to (1) adapt and create more scalable psychometric tools to measure linguistic choices and linguistic racism and (2) measure the antecedents and consequences of these choices.
What is Code Switching?
Code switching is described in a variety of different ways across disciplines. Within sociolinguistics, code switching describes the act of alternating between languages, or dialects of the same language (Gumperz, 1977). Its practice at the individual level, and at the level of a community, aids in establishing social boundaries during interactions and between group members (Heller, 1988; Myers-Scotton, 1995). In the field of education, code switching has been conceptualized as a tool to teach students to distinguish between dialects in appropriate scenarios (Wheeler et al., 2004). Within psychology, the definition of the practice expands.
Psychologists describe code switching not only as a linguistic practice, but also as an impression management strategy that incorporates adjusting aspects of one’s appearance, behavior, or even expressions in different contexts (McCluney et al., 2019).
Related constructs to code switching within psychology include cross-cultural code switching (Molinsky, 2007), cultural frame switching (Benet-Martínez et al., 2002), shifting (Jones & Shorter-Gooden, 2009), and passing (DeJordy, 2008; Newheiser & Barreto, 2014). All of these constructs encompass the idea of altering aspects of oneself (e.g., speech, behavior, mannerisms) in service of conforming to context-specific standards of appropriateness (Flores & Rosa, 2015).
This review calls attention to two foundational ideas related to the definition of code switching. The first is a raciolinguistic perspective (expanded below), which argues that linguistic heterogeneity and switching between modes of communication is a neutral practice in itself and one that nearly everyone engages in during social interactions (Flores & Rosa, 2015; Rosa & Flores, 2017). This perspective highlights that heterogeneity in speech practices becomes marked as divergent when employed by groups that already face marginalization or othering.
The second idea is that although adapting communication styles in different contexts is normal and universal, for some groups, these adaptations do not have the same psychological antecedents and consequences. For instance, Black Americans and other marginalized groups who experience structural inequality (viz., linguistic racism) may face conditions that precipitate demands for them to shift (Jones & Shorter-Gooden, 2009), particularly away from their authentic selves (which varies across people and contexts) towards dominating2 cultural norms.
A Raciolinguistic Perspective
In this review, we put code switching research into conversation with the recently developed raciolinguistic perspective that has emerged in sociolinguistics (Rosa & Flores, 2017). At the core of a raciolinguistic perspective is a translingual orientation, which points to the inherent heterogeneity of all language practices (Guerra, 2016). Race is a key dimension that is used to determine what counts as “switching.” In this way, the linguistic heterogeneity of communities of color come to be marked as “switching” in ways that the linguistic heterogeneity of White populations often are not—even when engaged in seemingly similar linguistic practices (Rosa & Trivedi, 2020).3 This bundling of race and language practices often serves to frame the language practices traditionally associated with communities of color, such as African American English (AAE), as inappropriate for school and professional settings (Baker-Bell, 2020) even while White speakers often use similar linguistic practices (e.g., Dupree & Fiske, 2019) to frame themselves as cool, hip, or down to earth (Cutler, 2003). In short, while code switching has historically been conceptualized as an objective linguistic phenomenon, a raciolinguistic perspective brings attention to the ways race is integral to the ideological production of what is and is not perceived as code switching within any particular context. In this review, we do not presuppose that code switching is an objective linguistic phenomenon but rather seek to focus our attention on the role of race and, in particular, anti-Blackness, in marking the heterogeneity in the linguistic practices of Black people and other people of color as fundamentally different from the unmarked heterogeneity of the White norm and the consequences of this on the psychological well-being of Black people and other people of color. We argue that understanding how people make choices about their language practices and how those choices are perceived is key for creating more racially affirming environments.
As we espouse the raciolinguistic perspective, which is in line with the notion that everyone makes strategic choices about self-presentation and language use, we also highlight the ways that linguistic racism marks certain language choices as divergent. For this reason, many Black people and other people of color make strategic decisions about how to linguistically present themselves across different contexts (e.g., work, school, home) that weigh risks and benefits that are less salient or not present for other groups (McCluney et al., 2019). For some Black Americans, as with other groups, making strategic language decisions can be a way to index certain aspects of their cultural identity through the way that they speak (Myers, 2020). For others, however, these decisions, which may or may not feel consistent with a person’s authentic sense of self, constitute a form of impression management to navigate racist environments where the threat of linguistic racism is salient (Hall et al., 2012; Jones & Shorter-Gooden, 2009). Given the ongoing impact of structural racism in the USA (Bailey et al., 2017; McGee, 2020), and linguistic racism in particular, it is crucial to study how decisions about how to linguistically present oneself to others map onto individual and structural level experiences of racial inequity (Alim et al., 2016). It is also key to understand how this relates to a person’s experience of well-being, authenticity, cognitive load, and stress.
Code Switching and Mentalizing
In line with the idea that all people make choices about their linguistic practices during communication, we introduce mentalizing as an antecedent to decisions about how to linguistically present oneself. We propose that most people may engage in mentalizing when making choices about how to use language.
Mentalizing is a cognitive process by which a person infers and reasons about the thoughts, feelings, and/or intentions of another person (Apperly, 2010; Frith & Frith, 2003; Malle, 2020; Saxe et al., 2004); mentalizing has been associated with the development of successful communication in children, (Fan et al., 2015; Hale & Tager-Flusberg, 2005) as well as successful persuasion and negotiation in adults (Baek et al., 2018; Galinsky et al., 2008).
Although the relationship between code switching and mentalizing has so far received little attention in affective science, recent theoretical and empirical work has shed some light on the relevance of mentalizing to decisions about how to linguistically present oneself.
We draw from the Interactive Mentalizing Theory (Wu et al., 2020) in proposing that mentalizing is recruited on an ongoing basis in order to simultaneously manage one’s own impressions in the eyes of others while also figuring out the thoughts, feelings, and intentions of one’s interaction partner. This theory describes how real-world social interactions involve a dynamic interplay between first-order mentalizing (inferences about the thoughts and feelings of others) and second-order mentalizing (inferences about how others are inferring one’s own thoughts and feelings; Wu et al., 2020). In other words, thinking about others’ thoughts is intimately tied to how we perceive the way others are thinking about us.
Following this logic, we suggest that mentalizing processes are likely recruited at different stages of social interactions. Prior to an interaction, a speaker may mentalize in an attempt to form an impression of whether or not the listener is likely to perceive their speech practices favorably or unfavorably. If the speaker presumes that the listener will perceive their speech unfavorably, they may make an a priori decision to alter their ways of speaking in order to align with the communication goals and needs of the listener. So, to revisit the case of Nia, uncertainty about the motives of her new peers would likely trigger additional mentalizing in an effort to get an accurate read on their past and present thoughts, feelings, and intentions, in order to determine how she should engage linguistically.
As discussed in the next section, this process may be recruited more frequently before and during conversations by people whose speech practices are likely to be marked, due to concerns about linguistic racism and confirming stereotypes associated with one’s own group (Richeson & Shelton, 2012). These concerns about confirming a stereotype, coupled with uncertainty about how to present oneself linguistically to avoid discrimination, constitute additional cognitive effort that is expended by Black people and people of color.
Consequences of Linguistic Racism
For Black people and other people of color, beyond universal human motivations to be accepted and connected to others (Leary, 2010; Panksepp, 2004; Ryan & Deci, 2000), attempting to adhere to dominating cultural norms to avoid racial discrimination carries added pressure linked to perseverance as well as academic and professional success (Baugh, 2010; Jones & Shorter-Gooden, 2009; Schmaling et al., 2017). As in our introductory vignette about Nia, we contend that above and beyond general risks associated with navigating everyday scenarios, Black people and other people of color may have an awareness that their speech can be perceived as inappropriate in certain contexts (e.g., Baker-Bell, 2020). If the context prompts concerns about how their speech is perceived (e.g., by colleagues, clients, supervisors, family, or peers), they may begin to mentalize in an active attempt to understand others’ thoughts or feelings. Considering other people’s thoughts and feelings may also raise concerns about confirming negative stereotypes about one’s group (i.e., stereotype threat). The person’s appraisal of the likely costs and benefits of adapting their linguistic expressions then motivates their eventual decision to engage in code switching or discontinue code switching.
Chronic decisions about how to linguistically present oneself to others in order to avoid discrimination (DeJordy, 2008; Newheiser & Barreto, 2014) may then (1) diminish the congruence between a person’s perceived authentic self and outward presentation, (2) require additional cognitive effort, and (3) result in added stress and negative health effects. Here, we focus on these psychological consequences for Black people and other people of color who face discrimination, due to linguistic racism, particularly in contexts where there are demands for presentations of self that deviate away from a person’s authentic self.
Opportunities for Self-Verification and Social Belonging
The ability to effectively affiliate oneself with a group has a variety of positive outcomes (Leary, 2010; Panksepp, 2004; Ryan & Deci, 2000). Membership in social groups can improve self-esteem (Jetten et al., 2015), increase levels of social support that may help buffer the negative effects of stress (Cohen, 2004), and strengthen social ties that may provide valuable advantages for emotional, physical, and mental health (Sbarra & Coan, 2018; Seeman, 1996; Umberson & Karas Montez, 2010). For example, in a study with an intervention to increase perceptions of social belonging for African American students, students reported higher levels of overall health and academic performance (Walton & Cohen, 2011). Perceptions of language are one way in which people determine ingroup and outgroup status (Baugh, 1996; Doss & Gross, 1994) and therefore facilitate discrimination or acceptance.
For these reasons, in describing the consequences of linguistic racism in affect, cognition, and behavior, we draw from DeJordy’s (2008) description of “passing” in noting that the potential dissonance created by the gap between the “me” that is experienced by others and the “I” that encapsulates a person’s subjective understanding of themselves might be modulated by the extent to which a person is code switching away from a self that they experience as true and authentic (Mead & Morris, 1935). In other words, code switching may increase or decrease the distance between a person’s actual self and their presented self. For example, a job applicant may code switch as a means of downplaying negative associations with her racial identity or may code switch as a means of fitting in with a specific peer group. Downplaying one’s racial identity in resumes has been linked to a greater likelihood of getting hired, even in companies that explicitly value diversity (Kang et al., 2016).
So, if a person frequently code switches away from their actual self (e.g., towards a dominating norm), this could decrease their opportunity for others to see them as they see themselves (i.e., self-verification; DeJordy, 2008). Increased demand on individuals to self-alter during interactions may also require additional cognitive and emotional labor (McCluney et al., 2019) and constitute negative consequences arising from pressures to switch away from one’s genuine self (Boulton, 2016).
Stereotype Threat and Cognitive Load
An individual experiences stereotype threat if they believe they are at risk of confirming an unfavorable societal stereotype about a group to which they belong (Spencer et al., 2016; Steele & Aronson, 1995). We posit that stereotype threat is salient in a number of contexts including broadly within academia (Spencer et al., 2016) and is one of several appraisals that underscores motivations to adapt one's speech in an attempt to subvert unfavorable stereotypes about one’s group.
Black students and other students of color may strategically adapt their speech practices in an attempt to avoid negative perceptions (Baker-Bell, 2020). This self-conscious regulation of speech, which is triggered by appraisals of one’s perceptions, may consume mental resources. Further strain on these resources may result from online mentalizing about whether or not one’s impressions are successfully being managed.
Additional empirical work linking stereotype threat and code switching comes from qualitative work (Boulton, 2016). In a qualitative study on stereotype threat and code switching in advertising interns, Boulton (2016) highlights tensions experienced by Black advertising interns. On the one hand, Black interns often (but not always) felt pressure to adapt their speech practices among White peers in order to be embraced as professionals and individuals. On the other hand, when speaking with Black peers, there were sometimes judgments that they were “too white” or pretentious (Durkee et al., 2019). Accusations of “acting White” can have negative consequences for the mental health and well-being of people of color (Durkee & Williams, 2015). Future research that creates links between stereotype threat and marked forms of speech would be fruitful in helping to lessen tensions often experienced by speakers.
Although frequently studied in the context of test performance (Pennington et al., 2016; Steele, 1998; Steele & Aronson, 1995), stereotype threat is also known to sabotage interracial interactions (see Richeson & Shelton, 2012). Black participants who were primed to be concerned about White racial prejudice (vs. not primed to have this concern) showed more ingratiating behavior towards White interaction partners (Richeson & Shelton, 2012). Although Black participants’ behavior had the effect of making their White partners feel more positively (see also Shelton, 2003), this came at the expense of Black participants feeling more negatively about the interaction (Shelton et al., 2005). Although this work indicates that stereotype threat can motivate people of color to accommodate White interaction partners to avoid prejudice, more quantitative affective science on how such prejudicial attitudes manifest and are involved in interracial interactions in the context of language is needed.
The pressures that arise from stereotype threat also consume working memory capacity as individuals experiencing threats monitor for signs that they are confirming a stereotype (Forbes & Leitner, 2014; Schmader et al., 2008). Combined with a fear of failure, the motivation to disconfirm negative stereotypes (e.g., Kray et al., 2001; Nussbaum & Steele, 2011) constitutes an additional pressure to succeed. Such pressure can sabotage performance by bringing conscious attention to well-learned skills that are best performed without conscious effort (Beilock et al., 2006). Stereotype threat can also trigger a physiological stress response (Mendes & Jamieson, 2011; Schmader et al., 2008) that may be adaptive in the short term, particularly for relatively easy tasks; however, this stress response results in poorer performance on more difficult tasks (Ben-Zeev et al., 2005; O’Brien & Crandall, 2003). In the long term, chronic stereotype threat can also lead to disengagement in jobs (Hewlin, 2009; Slepian & Jacoby-Senghor, 2020; Walton et al., 2015) and career paths (Boulton, 2016; Cheryan et al., 2017; Freeman, 2020) through a reduced sense of belonging and can have health consequences as described below.
Health Consequences and Stress
While people may adapt the way they communicate in different contexts to achieve broad social goals as outlined above and to traverse hostile environments (Hall et al., 2012; McGee, 2020), recurrent incidences of discrimination (e.g., microaggressions) linked to negative perceptions of Black people and people of color (viz., linguistic racism) within these environments may negatively impact psychological well-being (Anderson & Stevenson, 2019). For example, individuals who experience frequent microaggressions can incur increased levels of stress (Smith et al., 2011), in part due to the need for more active vigilance to social cues (McCluney et al., 2019). In addition, past or present experiences of racial discrimination that lead to adverse mental health outcomes (Williams & Mohammed, 2013; Williams et al., 1997)4 including stress may be internalized at a physiological level (Hill et al., 2017).
Indeed, although there may be occupational benefits to code switching as outlined above (e.g., social belonging in professional contexts, career benefits), these benefits come at a cost to Black people in the context of work (McCluney et al., 2019). Black people and people of color may experience greater emotional exhaustion and career burnout through the added burden of decisions related to code switching to avoid discrimination (Hewlin, 2009). These are just a few examples of the potential material consequences of linguistic racism for Black people and people of color as they navigate structural racism throughout their careers.
Future Directions
At the structural level, we argue for the need to implement changes within the field of affective science, and the academy more broadly, that promote racially affirming environments that reduce demands for self-alterations (in written and verbal) interactions. One way affective scientists can do this is by considering a raciolinguistic perspective within their work and outside of it. By recognizing the utility, diversity, and heterogeneity inherent in the communicative practices that people employ in general, and thereby embracing the range of language practices employed by themselves and others, affective scientists can enrich the ways they perceive one another and communicate both within and outside of the context of academic work and science. Yet, it is also key to recognize how and why speech practices of some people of color, including those that are native to non-Western Educated Industrialized Rich Democratic countries (non-WEIRD; Henrich et al., 2010), are marked as divergent and to document and counteract the negative consequences of linguistic racism. Specifically, we encourage affective scientists to consider revisiting policies and practices that structurally disadvantage the speech practices of these populations, likely to be marked as divergent (e.g., curbing comments about “formal academic language” within the peer review and job interview process). Understanding the way linguistic heterogeneity of people of color often is marked can perhaps broaden and expand the ways affective scientists determine which ways of speaking and writing are perceived as “academic” or standard and therefore appropriate.
In service of these goals, future work is also needed to adapt and create more scalable psychometric tools to measure linguistic choices and linguistic racism with a goal to interrogate biases associated with perceptions of speech and understand more about how linguistic racism impacts the psychological well-being of people of color in the USA and beyond, for example, asking “In what situations do the speech practices of people of color and non-WEIRD populations get marked as divergent?” and “how can we better understand impacts associated with having one’s speech practices marked as divergent for individual speakers?” Future research testing these links could support the development of a formal theoretical model of linguistic racism as one antecedent of code switching and longer-term negative health consequences tied to belonging, performance, and well-being. With this foundation, we look forward to future work that will test interventions to buffer negative consequences and raise awareness of these consequences for Black people, people of color, and non-WEIRD populations more generally.
Additional Information
Funding
The authors acknowledge the support from the NIH/National Cancer Institute Grant 1R01CA180015-01 (PI: Falk) and from the Army Research Office under MURI contract W911NF-18–1-0244 (PI: Falk).
Data Availability
Data sharing not applicable to this article as no datasets were generated or analyzed during the current study.
Ethical Approval
N/A
Conflict of Interest
On behalf of all authors, the corresponding author states that there is no conflict of interest.
Informed Consent
N/A
Disclaimer
The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not represent the official views of the funding agencies.
Footnotes
Note: The ideas we lay out here also apply to other minoritized individuals along various intersecting dimensions including class, gender, and sexuality. The focus of this piece is on race and linguistic racism, but parallel ideas apply to other identities.
Flores and Rosa (2015) transition from the use of the term minority to minoritized and use the term racialized when writing about students and their linguistic practices in the U.S. education system to highlight the fact that people actively socially construct these distinctions. In line with that logic, we use the phrase dominating instead of dominant here when describing cultural norms used in different spaces to bring to attention the active and ongoing processes underlying how these norms remain predominant.
Research about code switching in relationship to Black Americans has traditionally been examined in the context of language and specifically bidialectalism (Standard English: SE and African American English: AAE; e.g., Sledd, 1969; Young & Barrett, 2018). This research includes research about linguistic profiling, a form of bias commonly directed towards Black people and people of color (Baugh, 2003, 2010). As a consequence of linguistic profiling, these populations are often subjected to negative stereotypes that they are less credible or informed because of perceptions of their speech practices (Baugh, 2003).
Many Black Americans are perceived as code switching between Standard English (SE) and African American English (AAE), two commonly referenced dialects of English (e.g., Pullum, 1999). In 1979, AAE gained public recognition as a dialect of English within the USA in the Martin Luther King Elementary School Children vs. Ann Arbor School District court case (Labov, 1982; Yellin, 1980). Further discussions about the use of AAE gained traction within the field of Education in 1996 when the Oakland Unified School District passed a resolution recognizing AAE as a legitimate form of speech that could be used within schools during language instruction, prompting an uproar by community members (Wolfram, 1998). Oakland residents were outraged largely due to negative perceptions of AAE, which have been explored in research about language practices traditionally associated with Black people (e.g., Baugh, 2003; Doss & Gross, 1994).
An in-depth treatment of the multiple contributors to racial trauma, and adverse health outcomes, is beyond the scope of this paper. However, while we focus on linguistic racism and code switching here, we note that it is one process in a broader cultural context.
Contributor Information
Darin G. Johnson, Email: darin.johnson@asc.upenn.edu
Emily B. Falk, Email: emily.falk@asc.upenn.edu
References
- Alim, H. S., Rickford, J. R., & Ball, A. F. (2016). Introducing raciolinguistics. Raciolinguistics: how language shapes our ideas about race, 1–30.
- Anderson RE, Stevenson HC. RECASTing racial stress and trauma: Theorizing the healing potential of racial socialization in families. The American Psychologist. 2019;74(1):63–75. doi: 10.1037/amp0000392. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Apperly I. Mindreaders: The cognitive basis of “theory of mind”. Psychology Press; 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Baek EC, Baek EC, Falk EB. Persuasion and influence: What makes a successful persuader? Current Opinion in Psychology. 2018;24:53–57. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.05.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bailey ZD, Krieger N, Agénor M, Graves J, Linos N, Bassett MT. Structural racism and health inequities in the USA: Evidence and interventions. The Lancet. 2017;389(10077):1453–1463. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(17)30569-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baker-Bell A. Dismantling anti-black linguistic racism in English language arts classrooms: Toward an anti-racist black language pedagogy. Theory into Practice. 2020;59(1):8–21. [Google Scholar]
- Baugh, J. (1996). Perceptions within a variable paradigm: Black and White racial detection and identification based on speech. Focus on the USA, 169–182.
- Baugh J. Linguistic profiling. Black Linguistics: Language, Society, and Politics in Africa and the Americas. 2003;1(1):155–168. [Google Scholar]
- Baugh J. Black street speech: Its history, structure, and survival. University of Texas Press; 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Beilock SL, Jellison WA, Rydell RJ, McConnell AR, Carr TH. On the causal mechanisms of stereotype threat: Can skills that don’t rely heavily on working memory still be threatened? Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin. 2006;32(8):1059–1071. doi: 10.1177/0146167206288489. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Benet-Martínez V, Leu J, Lee F, Morris MW. Negotiating biculturalism: Cultural frame switching in biculturals with oppositional versus compatible cultural identities. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 2002;33(5):492–516. [Google Scholar]
- Ben-Zeev T, Fein S, Inzlicht M. Arousal and stereotype threat. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 2005;41(2):174–181. [Google Scholar]
- Boulton C. Black identities inside advertising: Race inequality, code switching, and stereotype threat. Howard Journal of Communications. 2016;27(2):130–144. [Google Scholar]
- Cheryan S, Ziegler SA, Montoya AK, Jiang L. Why are some STEM fields more gender balanced than others? Psychological Bulletin. 2017;143(1):1–35. doi: 10.1037/bul0000052. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen S. Social relationships and health. The American Psychologist. 2004;59(8):676–684. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.59.8.676. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cutler C. “Keepin’ it real”: White hip-hoppers’ discourses of language, race, and authenticity. Journal of Linguistic Anthropology. 2003;13(2):211–233. [Google Scholar]
- DeJordy R. Just passing through: Stigma, passing, and identity decoupling in the work place. Group & Organization Management. 2008;33(5):504–531. [Google Scholar]
- Doss RC, Gross AM. The effects of black english and code-switching on intraracial perceptions. The Journal of Black Psychology. 1994;20(3):282–293. [Google Scholar]
- Dovchin S. Introduction to special issue: Linguistic racism. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism. 2020;23(7):773–777. [Google Scholar]
- Dupree CH, Fiske ST. Self-presentation in interracial settings: The competence downshift by White liberals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2019;117(3):579–604. doi: 10.1037/pspi0000166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Durkee MI, Gazley ER, Hope EC, Keels M. Cultural invalidations: Deconstructing the “acting White” phenomenon among Black and Latinx college students. Cultural Diversity & Ethnic Minority Psychology. 2019;25(4):451–460. doi: 10.1037/cdp0000288. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Durkee MI, Williams JL. Accusations of acting White: Links to Black students’ racial identity and mental health. The Journal of Black Psychology. 2015;41(1):26–48. [Google Scholar]
- Fan SP, Liberman Z, Keysar B, Kinzler KD. The exposure advantage: Early exposure to a multilingual environment promotes effective communication. Psychological Science. 2015;26(7):1090–1097. doi: 10.1177/0956797615574699. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Flores N, Rosa J. Undoing appropriateness: Raciolinguistic ideologies and language diversity in education. Harvard Educational Review. 2015;85(2):149–171. [Google Scholar]
- Forbes CE, Leitner JB. Stereotype threat engenders neural attentional bias toward negative feedback to undermine performance. Biological Psychology. 2014;102:98–107. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.07.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freeman JB. Measuring and resolving LGBTQ Disparities in STEM. Policy Insights from the Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 2020;7(2):141–148. [Google Scholar]
- Frith U, Frith CD. Development and neurophysiology of mentalizing. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B, Biological Sciences. 2003;358(1431):459–473. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2002.1218. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Galinsky AD, Magee JC, Gruenfeld DH, Whitson JA, Liljenquist KA. Power reduces the press of the situation: Implications for creativity, conformity, and dissonance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2008;95(6):1450–1466. doi: 10.1037/a0012633. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Giles, H., & Johnson, P. (1987). Ethnolinguistic identity theory: A social psychological approach to language maintenance. International Journal of the Sociology of Language, 1987(68). 10.1515/ijsl.1987.68.69
- Guerra JC. Cultivating a rhetorical sensibility in the translingual writing classroom. College English. 2016;78(3):228–233. [Google Scholar]
- Gumperz JJ. The sociolinguistic significance of conversational code-switching. RELC Journal. 1977;8(2):1–34. [Google Scholar]
- Hale CM, Tager-Flusberg H. Social Communication in Children with Autism: the Relationship between Theory of Mind and Discourse Development. Autism: the International Journal of Research and Practice. 2005;9(2):157–178. doi: 10.1177/1362361305051395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hall JC, Camille Hall J, Everett JE, Hamilton-Mason J. Black women talk about workplace stress and how they cope. Journal of Black Studies. 2012;43(2):207–226. doi: 10.1177/0021934711413272. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heller, M. (1988). Codeswitching: Anthropological and sociolinguistic perspectives. Walter de Gruyter.
- Henrich J, Heine SJ, Norenzayan A. Most people are not WEIRD. Nature. 2010;466(7302):29–29. doi: 10.1038/466029a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hewlin PF. Wearing the cloak: Antecedents and consequences of creating facades of conformity. The Journal of Applied Psychology. 2009;94(3):727–741. doi: 10.1037/a0015228. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hill LK, Hoggard LS, Richmond AS, Gray DL, Williams DP, Thayer JF. Examining the association between perceived discrimination and heart rate variability in African Americans. Cultural Diversity & Ethnic Minority Psychology. 2017;23(1):5–14. doi: 10.1037/cdp0000076. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jetten J, Branscombe NR, Alexander Haslam S, Haslam C, Cruwys T, Jones JM, Cui L, Dingle G, Liu J, Murphy S, Thai A, Walter Z, Zhang A. Having a lot of a good thing: Multiple important group memberships as a source of self-esteem. PLoS ONE. 2015;10(5):e0124609. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0124609. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jones, M. C., & Shorter-Gooden, K. (2009). Shifting: The double lives of Black women in America. Harper Collins.
- Kang SK, DeCelles KA, Tilcsik A, Jun S. Whitened Résumés: Race and self-presentation in the labor market. Administrative Science Quarterly. 2016;61(3):469–502. [Google Scholar]
- Kray LJ, Thompson L, Galinsky A. Battle of the sexes: Gender stereotype confirmation and reactance in negotiations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2001;80(6):942–958. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Labov W. Objectivity and commitment in linguistic science: The case of the Black English Trial in Ann Arbor. Language in Society. 1982;11(2):165–201. [Google Scholar]
- Leary, M. R. (2010). Affiliation, acceptance, and belonging: The pursuit of interpersonal connection. In S. T. Fiske (Ed.), Handbook of social psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 864–897). John Wiley & Sons, Inc., xiv.
- Malle, B. F. (2020). The tree of social cognition: Hierarchically organized capacities ofmentalizing. http://research.clps.brown.edu/soccogsci/publications/Pubs/Malle_inpress-b.pdf
- McCluney, C. L., Robotham, K., Lee, S., Smith, R., & Durkee, M. (2019). The costs of code-switching (p. 15). Harvard Business Review.
- McGee, E. O. (2020). Interrogating structural racism in STEM Higher Education. Educational Researcher. 0013189X20972718.
- Mead GH, Morris CW. Mind, self and society From the standpoint of a social behaviorist. The Journal of Philosophy. 1935;32(6):162. [Google Scholar]
- Mendes, W. B., & Jamieson, J. (2011). Embodied stereotype threat exploring brain and body mechanisms underlying performance impairments. Stereotype Threat Theory, Process, and Application, 52–68. 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199732449.003.0004
- Molinsky A. Cross-cultural code-switching: The psychological challenges of adapting behavior in foreign cultural interactions. AMRO. 2007;32(2):622–640. [Google Scholar]
- Myers-Scotton, C. (1995). Social motivations for codeswitching: Evidence from Africa. Clarendon Press.
- Myers, T. K. (2020). Can you hear me now? An autoethnographic analysis of code-switching. Cultural Studies ↔ Critical Methodologies, 20(2), 113–123.
- Newheiser A-K, Barreto M. Hidden costs of hiding stigma: Ironic interpersonal consequences of concealing a stigmatized identity in social interactions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 2014;52:58–70. [Google Scholar]
- Nussbaum A, Steele C. Confronting stereotype threat: The motivation to disprove and avoid confirming stereotypes. PsycEXTRA Dataset. 2011 doi: 10.1037/e683152011-501. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- O’Brien LT, Crandall CS. Stereotype threat and arousal: Effects on women’s math performance. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin. 2003;29(6):782–789. doi: 10.1177/0146167203029006010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Panksepp, J. (2004). Affective neuroscience: The foundations of human and animal emotions. Oxford University Press.
- Pennington CR, Heim D, Levy AR, Larkin DT. Twenty years of stereotype Threat research: A review of psychological mediators. PLoS ONE. 2016;11(1):e0146487. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0146487. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pullum, G. K. (1999). African American vernacular English is not standard English with mistakes. The Workings of Language: From Prescriptions to Perspectives, 59–66.
- Richeson, J. A., & Shelton, J. N. (2012). Stereotype threat in interracial interactions. In M. Inzlicht (Ed.), Stereotype threat: Theory, process, and application (Vol. 320, pp. 231–245). Oxford University Press, xv.
- Rosa J. Looking Like a language, sounding like a race. Oxford University Press; 2019. [Google Scholar]
- Rosa J, Flores N. Unsettling race and language: Toward a raciolinguistic perspective. Language in Society. 2017;46(5):621–647. [Google Scholar]
- Rosa, J., & Trivedi, S. (2020). Language and race/ethnicity. In The international encyclopedia of linguistic anthropology (pp. 1–10). Wiley. 10.1002/9781118786093.iela0275
- Ryan RM, Deci EL. Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. The American Psychologist. 2000;55(1):68–78. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.55.1.68. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- SamyAlim H. Critical hip-hop language pedagogies: Combat, consciousness, and the cultural politics of communication. Journal of Language, Identity & Education. 2007;6(2):161–176. [Google Scholar]
- Saxe R, Carey S, Kanwisher N. Understanding other minds: Linking developmental psychology and functional neuroimaging. Annual Review of Psychology. 2004;55:87–124. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.142044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sbarra DA, Coan JA. Relationships and health: The critical role of affective science. Emotion Review: Journal of the International Society for Research on Emotion. 2018;10(1):40–54. [Google Scholar]
- Schmader T, Johns M, Forbes C. An integrated process model of stereotype threat effects on performance. Psychological Review. 2008;115(2):336–356. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.115.2.336. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmaling, K. B., Blume, A. W., Engstrom, M. R., Paulos, R., & De Fina, S. (2017). The leaky educational pipeline for racial/ethnic minorities. In A. M. Czopp (Ed.), Social issues in living color: Challenges and solutions from the perspective of ethnic minority psychology: Societaland global issues (Vol. 2, pp. 103–122). Praeger/ABC-CLIO, ix.
- Seeman TE. Social ties and health: The benefits of social integration. Annals of Epidemiology. 1996;6(5):442–451. doi: 10.1016/s1047-2797(96)00095-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shelton JN. Interpersonal concerns in social encounters between majority and minority group members. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations: GPIR. 2003;6(2):171–185. [Google Scholar]
- Shelton JN, Richeson JA, Salvatore J, Trawalter S. Ironic effects of racial bias during interracial interactions. Psychological Science. 2005;16(5):397–402. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01547.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sledd J. Bi-dialectalism: The linguistics of White supremacy. The English Journal. 1969;58(9):1307–1329. [Google Scholar]
- Slepian, M. L., & Jacoby-Senghor, D. S. (2020). Identity threats in everyday life: Distinguishing belonging from inclusion. In Social psychological and personality science (p. 194855061989500). 10.1177/1948550619895008
- Smith WA, Hung M, Franklin JD. Racial battle fatigue and the MisEducation of Black men: Racial microaggressions, societal problems, and environmental stress. The Journal of Negro Education. 2011;80(1):63–82. [Google Scholar]
- Spencer SJ, Logel C, Davies PG. Stereotype threat. Annual Review of Psychology. 2016;67:415–437. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-073115-103235. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steele CM. Stereotyping and its threat are real. The American Psychologist. 1998;53(6):680–681. [Google Scholar]
- Steele CM, Aronson J. Stereotype threat and the intellectual test performance of African Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1995;69(5):797–811. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.69.5.797. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Umberson, D., & Karas Montez, J. (2010). Social relationships and health: A flashpoint for health policy. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 51(1_suppl), S54–S66. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Vorauer JD, Turpie CA. Disruptive effects of vigilance on dominant group members’ treatment of outgroup members: Choking versus shining under pressure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2004;87(3):384–399. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.87.3.384. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walton GM, Cohen GL. A brief social-belonging intervention improves academic and health outcomes of minority students. Science. 2011;331(6023):1447–1451. doi: 10.1126/science.1198364. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walton GM, Murphy MC, Ryan AM. stereotype threat in organizations: Implications for equity and performance. Annual Review of Organizational Psychology and Organizational Behavior. 2015;2(1):523–550. [Google Scholar]
- Wheeler RS, Swords R, Carpenter M. Codeswitching: Tools of language and culture transform the dialectally diverse classroom. Language Arts. 2004;81(6):470–480. [Google Scholar]
- Williams, D. R., & Mohammed, S. A. (2013). Racism and health I: Pathways and scientific evidence. The American Behavioral Scientist, 57(8). 10.1177/0002764213487340 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Williams DR, Yu Y, Jackson JS, Anderson NB. Racial differences in physical and mental health. Journal of Health Psychology. 1997;2(3):335–351. doi: 10.1177/135910539700200305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolfram W. Language ideology and dialect: Understanding the Oakland Ebonics Controversy. Journal of English Linguistics. 1998;26(2):108–121. [Google Scholar]
- Wu H, Liu X, Hagan CC, Mobbs D. Mentalizing during social InterAction: A four component model. Cortex; a Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior. 2020;126:242–252. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2019.12.031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yellin D. The Black English Controversy: Implications from the Ann Arbor Case. Journal of Reading. 1980;24(2):150–154. [Google Scholar]
- Young, V. A., & Barrett, R. (2018). Other people’s English: Code-meshing, code-switching, and African American literacy. Parlor Press LLC.
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
Data sharing not applicable to this article as no datasets were generated or analyzed during the current study.