Skip to main content
Oxford University Press - PMC COVID-19 Collection logoLink to Oxford University Press - PMC COVID-19 Collection
. 2022 Jan 20;37(1):1–6. doi: 10.1093/her/cyab042

Pilot study of peer modeling with psychological inoculation to promote coronavirus vaccination

A G Ramirez 1,*, C Despres 2, P Chalela 3, J Weis 4, P Sukumaran 5, E Munoz 6, A L McAlister 7
PMCID: PMC9383212  PMID: 35048119

Abstract

Promoting coronavirus vaccination is deterred by misinformation, ranging from elaborate conspiracy theories about sinister purposes to exaggeration of side effects, largely promulgated by social media. In this pilot study, we tested the effects of different messages on actions leading to vaccination. Two theory-based advertisements were produced for Facebook, which provided video testimonials from peer role models recommending vaccination and its benefits while providing psychological inoculation through the models’ acknowledging misinformation, rejecting it and receiving the vaccine. These ads were paid to appear on Facebook users’ feeds in rural counties in South Texas, along with a generic vaccine promotion ad from the CDC without peer models or psychological inoculation. Ad viewers could click a link to ‘find a vaccine near you’; these responses served as the outcome variable for assessing experimental effects. Ads featuring peer modeling with psychological inoculation yielded a significantly higher rate of positive responses than CDC ads (30.5 versus14.9/1000 people reached in English and 49.7 versus 31.5/1000 in Spanish; P < 0.001 for both English and Spanish rate comparisons). This provides useful pilot data supporting the hypothesis that theory-based communication, i.e. peer modeling with psychological inoculation, may be more effective than more traditional forms of advertising for promoting coronavirus vaccination.

Introduction

Vaccine hesitancy is as old as vaccination [1] and is considered by the World Health Organization as one of the top 10 threats to global health impacting polio, measles and HPV vaccination, among others [2–4]. It includes a delay in acceptance, or declining immunizations despite the availability of adequate services [5], and is higher for populations experiencing health disparities. There is evidence of disparities in the acceptance and uptake of vaccinations among adults from racial and ethnic minority populations, including vaccines for influenza and HPV. Recent research on influenza vaccination uptake shows a pattern of racial and ethnic minorities being less likely to receive the vaccine, with socioeconomic and clinician/health care system factors that decrease access and impair provider–patient relationships playing a role [6]. The reasons for hesitancy are varied, but may include concerns about perceived safety, skepticism about the trustworthiness of the source(s) of vaccination recommendations, misinformation about conspiracies by powerful interests to use vaccination as a cover for malevolent objectives, considering immunization a low priority, perceived low risk of illness, limited knowledge about the disease or the benefits of vaccination, limited health literacy, difficulty accessing services, clinician bias, cost or personal, cultural or religious beliefs discouraging vaccination [6–9].

Some common misbelief spreading through social media include conspiracy theories and claims that the COVID-19 vaccine contains a microchip that will be used to track individuals, has the virus in it and will be used to eliminate parts of the population, causes cancer, will alter people’s DNA and even cause vaccinated persons’ skin to shed. In addition to conspiracy theories and wild exaggerations of potential side effects, religious beliefs are also keeping some Hispanics from getting vaccinated, including believing that God will cure COVID-19 and that the only cure needed is the church. Social media groups (Facebook, YouTube, etc.) share memes supporting these beliefs that are shared among users’ own networks. In addition, religious antivaccination websites promoting hesitancy claim that the vaccine represents loyalty to God’s enemies, as part of a satanic ritual that will confer the ‘mark of the beast.’ Vaccine misinformation has spread easily in many Hispanic populations due to lack of reliable information in Spanish and poorly translated information, combined with little government outreach [10]. The coronavirus pandemic is the first in history in which technology and social media are being used on a massive scale to keep people safe, informed, productive and connected [11]. At the same time, the same technology is enabling and amplifying the dissemination of misinformation that is polarizing public debate on COVID-19 related topics, increasing stigmatization of those who are vaccinated and thus producing poor adherence to public health recommendations and jeopardizing the country’s ability to control the pandemic, in particular vaccination efforts [11, 12]. We, and others, have identified many sources of misinformation that increase vaccine hesitancy generally [12] and more specifically among Hispanics in South Texas. These are mainly social media and most particularly Facebook and YouTube, where many video presentations can be found providing various reasons why people should not allow themselves to be vaccinated against COVID-19.

Our team has many years of experience using ‘peer modeling’ as a method of communication to influence beliefs, attitudes, intentions and behavior change. Peer modeling is grounded in Albert Bandura’s social cognitive theory, which asserts that the main way humans learn is by observing others around them [13, 14]. We have pioneered the concept of ‘behavioral journalism’, i.e. using real stories about real people to present information that can be imitated by those who read or observe them [15–19]. Beginning with a ‘reality TV’ program in Finland in which real people were followed over time as they improved diet, lost weight, controlled blood pressure and achieved sustained cessation of smoking [17], we have employed this technique in many studies [20–23] and the empirical evidence for the effectiveness of what McAlister [16] termed ‘behavioral journalism’ in a 2017 publication [18]. The most substantial body of research supporting this method for communicating about infectious diseases was in the AIDS Community Demonstration Studies, where peer modeling was conveyed creatively through many different modalities (e.g. four-panel cartoons, tabloid print stories with copious illustrations, erotic images with brief vignettes on condom wrappers about learning to enjoy condom use in anonymous anal sex) [24–26].

Research on persuasion and resistance to persuasion in situations where audiences are exposed to strong arguments against a recommendation has identified a communication technique known as ‘psychological inoculation [27–29],’ which was first used to prepare soldiers who might be captured to resist indoctrination into opposing political ideologies [30]. In psychological inoculation, existing or anticipated persuasive arguments against a desired belief are explicitly presented and refuted before or after people are exposed to misinformation [31–33]. Prior research has shown that inoculating messages from trusted sources can prevent belief in new conspiracy theories, increase vaccine intentions and activate protective responses, such as critical thinking when exposed to future COVID-19 misinformation [8, 32–36]. In a recent study, participants identified doctors are the most trusted source of COVID-19 information, even for those who believed conspiracy theories, who trusted doctors’ information more than social media or other sources where misinformation proliferates [33, 34, 37, 38]. Healthcare providers, as well as trusted community leaders, and academic/community organizations, have a powerful opportunity to educate the community about the importance of COVID-19 vaccination and to inoculate them against current misinformation and beliefs while promoting vaccine uptake [33, 34]. But we believe that peer modeling, in which the sources of messages are also members of their intended audiences, will be the most effective strategy for psychological inoculation communication, and this formed the theoretical basis for the communication strategy in the pilot work we report here.

Methods

Through existing social networks, the research team of Salud America!, a Latino health equity program based at UT Health San Antonio, Texas, identified and interviewed two Hispanic peer role models who reported initial misgivings about vaccination before deciding to be vaccinated. The video interviews with the peer role models were used to create digital stories and brief bilingual videos around how the peer models overcame misinformation, turned initial fear into confidence, got the vaccine, reconnected with family and are helping end the pandemic. We conducted a pilot test via Facebook advertising to compare the video peer model stories to a CDC social media vaccine graphic ad to test the hypothesis that our peer model videos with psychological inoculation would prompt more viewers to click or tap on a link to find a vaccine near them via the CDC’s vaccinefinder.org (which has English and Spanish versions). See Fig 1. CDC is the official provider of accurate COVID-19 vaccine-related information. We tested Facebook ads for two peer model video stories, Jesus Larralde from South Texas and Rosa Herrera of Minnesota [39, 40]. The peer model videos featured playable 2 min videos that showed Rosa and Jesus answering questions on why they were hesitant, why they changed their minds and got the shot and what getting the shot has helped them do now. Both peer models were initially hesitant about COVID vaccination, and they expressed knowledge about conspiracy theories and misbeliefs about side effects that provided psychological inoculation with explicit recognition of widely heard arguments against getting the vaccination, but decided to reject those erroneous beliefs and accept vaccination.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1.

Screenshots of pilot study ads.

In Rosa’s Spanish-language video (English link https://youtu.be/pZKAvBdElUE), she talked about how she became reluctant to obtain vaccination when she read on Facebook that the COVID-19 vaccine would inject her with a microchip to track her but then learned that was not true by watching a webinar with public health experts who explained the vaccine; and how she decided, despite misgivings about misinformation, to get the shot to make sure her daughters, who had gotten COVID-19, did not get sick again from her and to be able to visit family who live in Mexico. ‘I’m able to see my grandkids and my kids. It gives you more freedom,’ Rosa said. ‘If you don’t do it for yourself, do it for your family.’ In Jesus’ English-language video (https://youtu.be/1qIFRAxPiqE), he talked about fearing dangerous vaccine side effects after hearing misinformation on Facebook exaggerating the risk of harmful side effects and then saw his wife and sisters get their shots with no ill effects and got his shot after his doctor recommended it because of his underlying high blood pressure and diabetes. He explains that he experienced very minor side effects, and how he is looking forward to family gatherings and barbecues. ‘[The vaccine shot] was really quick … less than 15 minutes. Didn’t seem to have any side effects. I was pretty much fine,’ Jesus said. ‘We need to stop spreading this thing around. If we don’t take our shots, [getting back to family is] not going to happen.’ We tested these two peer model video ads on Facebook in comparison with CDC social media vaccine graphics (one in English and one in Spanish). The CDC ad showed a container of the COVID-19 vaccine, with a tagline of ‘When it’s your turn, get vaccinated,’ in English and ‘Cuando sea tu turno, vacúnate,’ in Spanish. All of the ads − both the peer models and the CDC graphic − featured a call to action to click to ‘find a vaccine near you,’ which linked directly to the CDC’s vaccinefinder.org either in English or Spanish.

For this Facebook advertising pilot test, we spent a total of $1995.26 to place the CDC’s generic graphic in Spanish and English and the peer role model videos in two rural counties with a high proportion of Hispanic residents near San Antonio, Texas. Funds were distributed evenly to purchase placement of the Jesus Larralde peer model video in English in Medina County, Texas, and also the CDC’s generic vaccination graphic in Spanish there. We also purchased placement for the CDC’s generic vaccination graphic in English and the Rosa Herrera peer model video in Spanish in Atascosa County, Texas. This quasi-experimental design was necessary because when two similar ads are placed on Facebook in a particular location, the platform’s algorithms automatically purchase more placements of ads that yield higher response rates, which would not provide a good comparison for research purposes. Due to limited funds for this pilot work, we only studied placements of one theory-based ad in English in a single county and one in Spanish in another county. These two rural counties were chosen based on similar population size, high proportion of Spanish speaking residents and physical proximity to Bexar County.

Results and discussion

Our approximately $2000 expenditure yielded a total of 125 287 impressions (exposures) in the two counties, 26 564 for the theory-based ad with Jesus Larralde and 32 636 for Rosa Herrera and 31 354 and 34 733 for the respective CDC ads in Spanish and English. We first calculated the cost in ad placement expense that was needed to obtain the desired response. Both theory-based ads achieved lower cost per click to find a vaccine ($2.66 per click for Jesus in English and $3.14 per click for Rosa in Spanish), compared to the CDC generic ads ($4.03 in English and $5.43 in Spanish).

Next, for significance testing, we calculated and compared the number of positive responses divided by the number of ‘people reached’ with the different ads to obtain the rate per 1000 exposed Facebook users who responded by taking action toward obtaining vaccination. For English ads these were 14.9/1000 for the CDC ad and 30.5/1000 for the theory-based ad (P < 0.001, Fisher exact test). Regarding Spanish ads, the corresponding rates were 31.5/1000 for the CDC ad and 49.7/1000 for our theory-based ad (P < 0.001, Fisher exact test). Our peer modeling with psychological doubled the effects seen with a conventional CDC ad in English and yielded a 58% higher response rate in Spanish.

This pilot study has some limitations. Only one CDC ad was used for comparison purposes against two peer-modeling video ads, and the outcome variable was only tapping or clicking on the CDC’s vaccinefinder.org link, not actually receiving vaccination. In addition, the study did not assess vaccine hesitancy among people clicking on the link. It is possible that the higher response rates were obtained only because the theory-based ads were videos featuring people, rather than mainly graphic information. Only two peer models were presented, and it is possible that they had unique persuasive characteristics that other models may not. Lastly, the quasi-experimental design and the lack of randomization may limit the study’s ability to conclude a causal association between an intervention and the outcome. Nevertheless, these data demonstrate the feasibility and potential to achieve a useful impact of carefully tailored bilingual peer role model stories with psychological inoculation against misinformation, in promoting vaccine confidence and persuading reluctant persons to obtain vaccination. The peer-modeling ads outperformed a CDC control ad that included only a sentence encouraging vaccination and featuring a vaccine container. Further, more rigorous research is clearly warranted. We believe that a high volume of tailored peer modeling communication can overcome vaccine hesitancy and enhance efforts to help the USA achieve the level of herd immunity needed to fully contain COVID-19 [41]. To study this more rigorously and comprehensively, we have very recently submitted a research proposal to the National Institutes of Health in which rural and urban counties in South Texas will be the units of randomization and observation and recorded vaccination rates reported by the Texas Department of State Health Services will be the primary outcome measured. With more extensive research like this, we expect to find that theory-based communication, in the form of peer modeling with psychological inoculation, is a highly effective way to promote coronavirus vaccination in a communication environment filled with misinformation that deters many from following the advice of public health leaders and promptly obtaining recommended vaccination.

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank the research team of the Institute for Health Promotion Research at UT Health San Antonio, the research team of the Salud America! program supported by the Robert Wood Johnson Foundation and the Mays Cancer Center for their support in conducting this pilot test.

Contributor Information

A G Ramirez, Department of Population Health Sciences, Institute for Health Promotion Research, University of Texas Health Science Center at San Antonio, 7411 John Smith Drive, Suite 10.00, San Antonio, TX 78229, USA.

C Despres, Department of Population Health Sciences, Institute for Health Promotion Research, University of Texas Health Science Center at San Antonio, 7411 John Smith Drive, Suite 10.00, San Antonio, TX 78229, USA.

P Chalela, Department of Population Health Sciences, Institute for Health Promotion Research, University of Texas Health Science Center at San Antonio, 7411 John Smith Drive, Suite 10.00, San Antonio, TX 78229, USA.

J Weis, Department of Population Health Sciences, Institute for Health Promotion Research, University of Texas Health Science Center at San Antonio, 7411 John Smith Drive, Suite 10.00, San Antonio, TX 78229, USA.

P Sukumaran, Department of Population Health Sciences, Institute for Health Promotion Research, University of Texas Health Science Center at San Antonio, 7411 John Smith Drive, Suite 10.00, San Antonio, TX 78229, USA.

E Munoz, Department of Population Health Sciences, Institute for Health Promotion Research, University of Texas Health Science Center at San Antonio, 7411 John Smith Drive, Suite 10.00, San Antonio, TX 78229, USA.

A L McAlister, School of Public Health, University of Texas Health Science Center at Houston, Austin Campus, 1616 Guadalupe, Suite 6.300, Austin, TX 78701, USA.

Conflict of interest statement

None declared.

References

  • 1.Larson HJ. Stuck: How Vaccine Rumors Start–and Why They Don’t Go Away. Oxford, New York: Oxford University Press, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Rosselli  R, Martini  M, Bragazzi  NL. The old and the new: vaccine hesitancy in the era of the Web 2.0. Challenges and opportunities. J Prev Med Hyg  2016; 57: E47–50. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. World Health Organization . Ten Threats to Global Health in 2019. Available at: https://www.who.int/news-room/spotlight/ten-threats-to-global-health-in-2019. Accessed: 15  June  2021.
  • 4.UNICEF Middle East and North Africa. Vaccine Misinformation Management Field Guide. Available at: https://www.unicef.org/mena/reports/vaccine-misinformation-management-field-guide. Accessed: 15  June  2021.
  • 5. Jarrett  C, Wilson  R, O’Leary  M  et al.  Strategies for addressing vaccine hesitancy – a systematic review. Vaccine  2015; 33: 4180–90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Webb Hooper  M, Nápoles  AM, Pérez-Stable  EJ. No populations left behind: vaccine hesitancy and equitable diffusion of effective COVID-19 vaccines. J Gen Intern Med  2021; 36: 2130–3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Fisher  KA, Bloomstone  SJ, Walder  J  et al.  Attitudes toward a potential SARS-CoV-2 vaccine: a survey of U.S. adults. Ann Intern Med  2020; 173: 964–73. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Jolley  D, Douglas  KM. Prevention is better than cure: addressing anti-vaccine conspiracy theories. J Appl Soc Psychol  2017; 47: 459–69. [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Salmon  DA, Dudley  MZ, Glanz  JM  et al.  Vaccine hesitancy: causes, consequences, and a call to action. Vaccine  2015; 33: D66–71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Marcos  JG and CM . COVID-19 Crisis: Vaccine Conspiracy Theories, Hoaxes in Spanish Targeting Hispanic Community Breed Fear, Hesitancy. Available at: https://www.usatoday.com/story/tech/2021/03/16/facebook-whatsapp-covid-vaccine-misinformation-spanish-hispanic-hesitancy/4711556001/. Accessed: 15  June  2021.
  • 11. World Health Organization. Managing the COVID-19 Infodemic: Promoting Healthy Behaviours and Mitigating the Harm from Misinformation and Disinformation. Available at: https://www.who.int/news/item/23-09-2020-managing-the-covid-19-infodemic-promoting-healthy-behaviours-and-mitigating-the-harm-from-misinformation-and-disinformation. Accessed: 8  April  2021.
  • 12. Broniatowski  DA, Jamison  AM, Qi  S  et al.  Weaponized health communication: Twitter bots and Russian trolls amplify the vaccine debate. Am J Public Health  2018; 108: 1378–84. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Bandura  A. Health promotion from the perspective of social cognitive theory. Psychol Health  1998; 13: 623–49. [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Bandura  A. Health promotion by social cognitive means. Health Educ Behav  2004; 31: 143–64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. McAlister  A. Behavioral journalism: beyond the marketing model for health communication. Am J Health Promot  1995; 9: 417–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. McAlister  AL, Perry  CL, Parcel  GS. How Individuals, Environments, and Health Behaviors Interact: Social Cognitive Theory. Health Behavior and Health Education: Theory, Research, and Practice  4th edn. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass, 2008, 169–88. [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Puska  P, McAlister  A, Niemensivu  H  et al.  A television format for national health promotion: Finland’s “Keys to Health. Public Health Rep  1987; 102: 263–9. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Communication . Behavioral Journalism in Health and Risk Messaging. Available at: https://oxfordre.com/communication/view/10.1093/acrefore/9780190228613.001.0001/acrefore-9780190228613-e-326. Accessed: 15  June  2021.
  • 19. Ramirez  AG, Gallion  KJ, Despres  C  et al.  Advocacy, efficacy, and engagement in an online network for Latino childhood obesity prevention. Health Promot Pract  2015; 16: 878–84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Ramirez  AG, Villarreal  R, McAlister  A  et al.  Advancing the role of participatory communication in the diffusion of cancer screening among Hispanics. J Health Commun  1999; 4: 31–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Ramirez  AG, McAlister  A, Gallion  KJ  et al.  Community level cancer control in a Texas barrio: part I—Theoretical basis, implementation, and process evaluation. J Natl Cancer Inst Monogr  1995; 18: 117–22. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Ramirez  AG, Baldwin  S, Adeigbe  RT  et al.  SaludToday: curating Latino health information for a new generation. J Commun Healthc  2016; 9: 60–9. [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Ramirez  A, Chalela  P, Gallion  K  et al.  Diffusion acceleration: a model for behavior change and social mobilization. In: Huff  R, Kline  M, Peterson  D (eds). Health Promotion in Multicultural Populations. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications, 2015, 381–401. [Google Scholar]
  • 24. McAlister  A, Johnson  W, Guenther-Grey  C  et al.  Behavioral journalism for HIV prevention: community newsletters influence risk-related attitudes and behavior. J Mass Commun Q  2000; 77: 143–59. [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Fishbein  M, Guenther-Grey  C, Johnson  W  et al. Using a theory-based community intervention to reduce AIDS risk behaviors: the CDC’s AIDS Community Demonstration Projects. In: Goldberg ME, Fishbein M, Middlestadt SE (eds). Social Marketing: Theoretical and Practical Perspectives. New York, NY: Routledge, Taylor & Francis Group, 1997, 123–46. [Google Scholar]
  • 26. O’Reilly  KR, Higgins  DL. AIDS Community Demonstration Projects for HIV prevention among hard-to-reach groups. Public Health Rep  1991; 106: 714–20. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Pfeffer  J, Zorbach  T, Carley  KM. Understanding online firestorms: negative word-of-mouth dynamics in social media networks. J Mark Commun  2014; 20: 117–28. [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Richards  AS, Banas  JA. Inoculating against reactance to persuasive health messages. Health Commun  2015; 30: 451–60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Roozenbeek J, van der Linden S, Nygren T. Prebunking interventions based on the psychological theory of “inoculation” can reduce susceptibility to misinformation across cultures. HKS Misinfo Rev 2020. Available at: https://www.repository.cam.ac.uk/handle/1810/301772. Accessed: 15  June  2021.
  • 30. McGuire  WJ, Papageorgis  D. The relative efficacy of various types of prior belief-defense in producing immunity against persuasion. J Abnorm Soc Psychol  1961; 62: 327–37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Lazer  D, Ognyanova  K, Baum  M  et al.  The COVID States Project: A 50-State COVID-19 Survey. Report #43: COVID-19 Vaccine Rates and Attitudes among Americans. OSF Preprints, 2021, 1–32. doi: 10.31219/osf.io/rnw8z [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Klofstad  CA, Uscinski  JE, Connolly  JM  et al.  What drives people to believe in Zika conspiracy theories?  Palgrave Commun  2019; 5: 1–8. [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Earnshaw  VA, Katz  IT. Educate, amplify, and focus to address COVID-19 misinformation. JAMA Health Forum  2020; 1: e200460. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Earnshaw  VA, Eaton  LA, Kalichman  SC  et al.  COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs, health behaviors, and policy support. Transl Behav Med  2020; 10: 850–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Compton  J, Jackson  B, Dimmock  JA. Persuading others to avoid persuasion: inoculation theory and resistant health attitudes. Front Psychol  2016; 7: 122. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. van der Linden  S, Roozenbeek  J, Compton  J. Inoculating against fake news about COVID-19. Front Psychol  2020; 11: 566790. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Jin F, Wang W, Zhao L et al.  Misinformation Propagation in the Age of Twitter. Available at: https://www.computer.org/csdl/magazine/co/2014/12/mco2014120090/13rRUxYIMYR. Accessed 15  June  2021.
  • 38. Finney Rutten  LJ, Zhu  X, Leppin  AL  et al.  Evidence-based strategies for clinical organizations to address COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy. Mayo Clin Proc  2021; 96: 699–707. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Weis  J. Jesus Larralde: Get the Vaccine So We Can Return to Normal! Available at: https://salud-america.org/jesus-larralde-get-the-vaccine-so-we-can-return-to-normal/. Accessed: 16  June  2021.
  • 40. Weis  J. Rosa Herrera: Get the Vaccine for Your Family! Available at: https://salud-america.org/rosa-herrera-get-the-vaccine-for-your-family/. Accessed: 16  June  2021.
  • 41. Despres  C, Aguilar  R, McAlister  A  et al.  Communication for awareness and action on inequitable impacts of COVID-19 on Latinos. Health Promot Pract  2020; 21: 859–61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Health Education Research are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press

RESOURCES