Abstract
Objective:
Educational environments that are structured by race perpetuate poor mental health for Black adolescents. This empirical relationship is pronounced when it is examined through Racial Battle Fatigue theory, which provides a framework that links educational environments and poor psychological health of Black students. School police have major effect on Black adolescents educational and health experiences and trajectories. The purpose of this person-centered study was to assess the risk of Black students’ depressive symptoms who were stopped by school police, saw other students stopped by school police, or experienced school discipline.
Method:
Black student youths from the Fragile Families Study Year 15 wave (N=1,601) was used to conduct a latent class analysis to identify subgroups of school policing (i.e., being stopped by school police, seeing other students stopped by school police) and school discipline on the distal outcome of depressive symptoms. Covariates included demographic and school attachment.
Results:
We identified 4 distinct Black student subgroups: (1) unscathed [no school policing or discipline reported]; (2) school disciplined; (3) combined school policing; and (4) school policed [arrested by police]. Each subgroup had an incremental increase in mean depressive symptom scores. Compared to the unscathed subgroup each subgroup also had lower school attachment.
Conclusion:
This study disrupts the notion that education environments are a social determinant of health and a great equalizer. This study critically exposes how educational institutions, complicit with school policing, are associated with racism-related mental health conditions of Black youths.
Keywords: Black adolescent mental health, latent class analysis, racial battle fatigue, school policing, school discipline
Introduction
The literature concerning the relationship between educational environments and mental health outcomes of Black adolescents is still emerging. Empirical studies find that educational environments with disproportionate numbers of White students are related to the increased depressive symptoms of Black students; as the number of White students increases, so do the levels of depressive symptoms for Black students.1,2 Further, these depressive symptoms, borne of inequitable educational environments, persist into adulthood.2 Indeed, racially arduous experiences perpetuated by educational institutions negatively affect the mental health of Black youths, but the literature is scant. Hurst and colleagues3 provide a review that posits the adverse mental health effects of school policing towards Black students. These authors discuss the history of over-policing enforcement tactics against Black communities. Furthermore, these scholars identify an association between the legacy of egregious state-sanctioned policing against Black communities and psychological health outcomes such as fear, paranoia, depressive symptoms, and Racial Battle Fatigue of Black students. Racial Battle Fatigue (RBF) is the psychological and physiological responses in Black people when coping with institutional and structural racism replete in educational environments.4 Their contribution establishes a framework for this burgeoning catalog of research, as their paper intends to spur empirical work regarding the relationship between school policing and the mental health outcomes of Black people. Our study builds on the seminal work of Hurst and her colleagues3 by identifying the mental health profiles of Black adolescent high school students stopped by school police and received school discipline.
“Zero-Tolerance” education policies began in the 1980s and 1990s, which propelled increases in school discipline and school policing in campuses across U.S. schools.5–7 “Zero-tolerance policies refer to policies that require school officials to apply predetermined consequences, such as suspension or expulsion, regardless of the situational context, mitigating circumstances, or the seriousness of the offense.”8 Among these zero-tolerance, ‘get tough’ policies was the Gun Free Schools Act of 1994.9–12 These policies were adapted from the criminal justice system which called for swift and harsh punishment, usually suspension or expulsion, for students with no track record of infractions.13 Before zero-tolerance discipline policies of the 1990s, exclusionary school discipline was a last resort for egregious cases like drug or gun possession or physical violence.6,13–15 A result of these harsh discipline practices of the 1980s and 1990s, children of color are suspended or expelled more than their White counterparts for the same offenses.15–18
In addition to increases in school discipline rates, zero-tolerance policies also increased the presence of school police in U.S schools. From the 1950s to the 1990s, the number of school police rose to 17,000.19 An exact amount is unknown, but a recent study estimates that over 20,000 school police officers in the U.S. as of 2015.20 Even though zero-tolerance policies and school police presence was used to ensure student safety,21,22 this goal was largely not achieved.6,23 Police presence in schools leads to two types of enforcement scenarios. The first scenario is where school police are the enforcers of mundane discipline issues that educators addressed in yesteryear.24 The second discipline scenario is where school police are co-enforcers with educators and are present when educators interrogate students.25 This passive co-enforcement scenario allows school police to gather self-incriminating information from students, bypassing their constitutional Miranda rights (for review, see U.S. Supreme Court case Miranda v. Arizona, 1966).25 Both of these enforcement tactics are associated with racial inequities in education,5,6 which increases interaction with the juvenile just system, pushing Black students into the “school-to-prison pipeline” (STPP).6
In aligning studies on the effects of educational inequities, the depressive symptoms of Black adolescents are high when the racial composition of the student body becomes increasingly White.1,2 Applying the framework provided by Hurst and colleagues3 suggests that the educational inequities propelled by school policing are associated with poor mental health outcomes of Black students. Therefore, being stopped by school police may increase the odds of depressive symptoms of Black students, which is consistent with the tenets of RBF.26
Theoretical Framework
Underpinning this research study is RBF theory which undergirds the relationship between educational environments, replete with racism, and racism-related health outcomes of Black students. Further, this theory explains the psychological and physiological responses in Black people when coping with institutional and structural racism commonly found in educational environments.4 The application of RBF theory raises understanding of how educational environments place Black students at risk of harmful psychological and physiological health outcomes.27 According to RBF, Black students report psychological and physiological health problems like frustration, stress, rumination, hopelessness, anxiety, or fear.4,26,27 Specifically, RBF suggests that racism-related stress manifests in certain mental health symptoms such as: stress, frustration, shock, anger, disappointment, resentment, anxiety, helplessness, hopelessness, and fear.4 Our study applies RBF theory to understand better the relationship between school policing and depressive symptoms of Black students. Thus, disrupting dominant narratives about the mental health of Black students places the onus of education and health on historically white institutions and instead of Black communities.
Purpose
Indeed, more anti-racist research is needed to center the educational and health experiences of Black adolescents as they navigate educational settings, which are historically structured by race and racism. This paper seeks to identify the nexus between school policing, school discipline, and depressive symptoms of Black adolescents. We hypothesize that there would be nuanced subgroup differences between Black adolescent high school students and levels of depressive symptoms by reported school policing (i.e., whether they were and/or saw another student on campus being stopped by school police) and school discipline. We further hypothesize that these person-centered profiles would vary by gender.
Method
Materials
To identify high school student subgroups of school policing and discipline and depressive symptoms, we used latent class analysis (LCA), a person-centered analysis on the Fragile Families dataset. Data for the present study came from Year 15 from the Fragile Families and Child Wellbeing Study (FFCWS), a large, population-based cohort study beginning in 1998 and is presently ongoing. The national random sample is from 77 large U.S. cities with populations over 200,000 people in 1994, between 1998 and 2000.28,29 This study followed a cohort of new parents and their children, providing unparalleled data and information about conditions and contexts surrounding unwed partners and families. Data was collected from 4,700 births (3,600 non-marital and 1,100) in 75 different hospitals in various U.S. cities.28 Notably, this study is not without flaw; the intent of the FFCWS was to understand unwed couples and targeting communities of color for this information stems from a deficit-based perspective about these communities.
Latent Variable Indicators
School policing had two categories. The first was the student report of being “stopped by police at school”, categorized as yes or no. The second school policed variable was if student had “ever seen someone stopped by police in your school?”, which was categorized as yes or no. School discipline was based on student report of “ever been suspended or expelled in past 2 years?” and was categorized as yes or no. Sex/gender of adolescents was categorized as male and female.
Distal continuous outcome.
Depressive symptoms were scored using a 4-point scale [strongly disagree=0, somewhat disagree=1, somewhat agree=2, and strongly agree=3] using five questions selected by the Fragile Families et al.30 from the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression (CES-D) scale based on participant feelings in the past week: (1) you felt that you could not shake off the blues, even with help from your family and your friends; (2) you felt sad; (3) you were happy; (4) you felt life was not worth living; and (5) you felt depressed. The CES-D “you were happy” item is positively worded as such was reverse coded.
Covariates
There were three demographic covariates that indicated whether mothers were married or unmarried, level of maternal income (up to $14,999, $15,000 to $34,999, and $35,000 or more), and maternal high school education (or equivalent). There were four school-level covariates, where the last was considered an indicator of perceived discrimination:1 (1) “I feel like I am part of my school”; (2) “I feel close to people at my school”; (3) “I am happy in school”; and (4) “Teachers in school treat the students with respect”. With a Likert scale, we also assessed “How likely [students were] to graduate from college?”. This last covariate was included to indicate to whether punitive control strategies may influence long-term academic goals and attitudes.
Data Analysis Plan
We first conducted a descriptive analysis to report the characteristics of the Black adolescent student study sample. We used LCA to identify and characterize school policed subgroups with conditional probabilities of observed school policing and discipline indicators using depressive scores as the distal continuous outcome. A comparative approach was used to select the best LCA model for interpretation. This approach compares multiple models (i.e., 1-class to 4-class solution) using the following model fit criteria: Bayesian information criterion, (BIC), sample size adjusted (SSA) BIC, Lo-Mendell-Rubin (LMR) adjusted likelihood-ratio-test (LRT), parametric bootstrapped (PB) LRT, and entropy. Entropy provides an index of reliability for separation of classes or subgroups.31 Once the final model was selected a multinomial logistic regression was used to examine the role of covariates in order to assess between subgroup differences. All analyses were performed in Mplus 8.4 (Muthén & Muthén).
Results
Sample Descriptives
The majority of the Black student sample had not been ever stopped by police at school (77.6%) and had never been disciplined at school (64.5%). The sample had almost equally seen others stopped by police at school. The sample was also equally male and female. The depressive symptom mean score was 2.92.
Latent Class Model
The four-class solution was selected for interpretation based on the criteria seen in Table 2. Classes or subgroups were named based on participant reports of school discipline and policing. Classes were ordered by CES-D depressive mean scores.
Table 2.
Bayesian information criterion (BIC) | Sample Size Adjusted-BIC (SSA-BIC) | Entropy | Lo-Mendell-Rubin (LMR) adjusted likelihood-ratio-test (LMR LRT) | p-value | Parametric Bootstrapped (PB-LRT) | p-value | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
|
|||||||
1-Class Solution | 15030.3 | 15011.2 | - | - | - | - | - |
2-Class Solution | 14662.9 | 14624.7 | 0.85 | 402.6 | 0.000 | 411.7 | 0.000 |
3-Class Solution | 14630.4 | 14573.2 | 0.57 | 75.0 | 0.003 | 76.7 | 0.000 |
4-Class Solution | 14651.3 | 14575.1 | 0.82 | 5.5 | 0.326 | 8.7 | 1.000 |
As seen in Table 3, Class 1 had the lowest depressive mean score and had almost equal probabilities to be male or female. Class 1 was categorized as the Not School Policed nor Disciplined subgroup as this profile had the highest conditional probability (95%) of never having been stopped by the police at school. Students in this subgroup had exclusively never been disciplined at school.
Table 3.
Class 1 | Class 2 | Class 3 | Class 4 | |
---|---|---|---|---|
|
||||
Not School Policed nor Disciplined | School Disciplined | Observed School Policing and/or Policed or Disciplined | School Policed | |
|
||||
828 | 188 | 292 | 293 | |
51.7% | 11.7% | 18.2% | 18.3% | |
|
||||
Have you ever been stopped by police at school? | ||||
No | 0.95 | 0.92 | 0.66 | 0.67 |
Yes | 0.06 | 0.09 | 0.34 | 0.33 |
Have you ever seen others stopped by police at school? | ||||
No | 0.67 | 1.00 | 0.00 | 0.43 |
Yes | 0.33 | 0.00 | 1.00 | 0.57 |
Have you ever been disciplined at school? | ||||
No | 1.00 | 0.00 | 0.36 | 0.58 |
Yes | 0.00 | 1.00 | 0.65 | 0.42 |
Sex/Gender | ||||
Male | 0.46 | 0.60 | 0.62 | 0.40 |
Female | 0.54 | 0.40 | 0.39 | 0.60 |
Depressive Symptoms | ||||
Mean | 1.54 | 2.11 | 2.16 | 7.75 |
Class 2 had an intermediate-low depressive mean score compared to all classes. Students had a higher conditional probability to be male in this subgroup. Students in this subgroup were categorized as School Disciplined due to having been exclusively school disciplined. The school disciplined also had the second highest conditional probability of never been stopped by police at school, as well as exclusively never seen others stopped by the police at school.
Class 3 had an intermediate-high mean depressive score compared to all classes. Students had a higher conditional probability to be male in this subgroup. Students in the Class 3 subgroup were categorized as Observed School Policing and/or Policed or Disciplined due to having been had the highest conditional probability of having been stopped by police at school, as well as exclusively seen others stopped by the police at school.
Class 4 had the highest depressive mean score and the highest conditional probability to be female. This student subgroup was categorized as School Policed due to having the second highest conditional probability of being stopped by the police at school.
The school disciplined subgroup (Class 2) had 48% lower odds of having a married mother and 61% students reporting “I am happy in school” compared to unscathed subgroup (Class 1). The combined school policed subgroup (Class 3) was found to have 60% lower odds of students reporting “I am happy in school” and 73% lower odds for students reporting “teachers in school treat the students with respect” compared to unscathed subgroup (Class 1). The school policed subgroup (Class 4) had 76% lower odds of feeling “I feel like I am part of my school,” 69% lower odds of students reporting “I am happy to be at my school,” and 74% lower odds for students reporting “teachers in school treat the students with respect” compared to unscathed subgroup (Class 1). See Table 4 for all covariates.
Table 4.
Class 2 |
Class 3 |
Class 4 |
||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
95% CI |
95% CI |
95% CI |
||||||||||
OR | Lower | Upper | p-value | OR | Lower | Upper | p-value | OR | Lower | Upper | p-value | |
|
||||||||||||
Income | 0.69 | 0.44 | 1.09 | 0.11 | 1.20 | 0.77 | 1.86 | 0.41 | 0.89 | 0.58 | 1.38 | 0.61 |
Married mother | 0.52 | 0.28 | 0.98 | 0.04 | 0.67 | 0.37 | 1.22 | 0.19 | 0.66 | 0.38 | 1.17 | 0.16 |
US-born mother | 0.74 | 0.28 | 1.99 | 0.55 | 2.15 | 0.55 | 8.42 | 0.27 | 2.36 | 0.60 | 9.18 | 0.22 |
Mother with high school/GED | 0.86 | 0.56 | 1.32 | 0.49 | 1.46 | 0.95 | 2.24 | 0.08 | 1.14 | 0.76 | 1.72 | 0.53 |
“I feel like I am part of my school” | 0.51 | 0.21 | 1.24 | 0.14 | 0.46 | 0.19 | 1.12 | 0.09 | 0.24 | 0.11 | 0.52 | 0.00 |
“I feel close to people at my school” | 0.74 | 0.39 | 1.40 | 0.36 | 0.80 | 0.40 | 1.57 | 0.51 | 0.60 | 0.33 | 1.09 | 0.10 |
“I am happy in school” | 0.39 | 0.20 | 0.78 | 0.01 | 0.40 | 0.19 | 0.86 | 0.02 | 0.31 | 0.16 | 0.59 | 0.00 |
“Teachers in school treat the students with respect” | 0.52 | 0.22 | 1.24 | 0.14 | 0.27 | 0.10 | 0.68 | 0.01 | 0.26 | 0.11 | 0.60 | 0.00 |
Felt likely to graduate and go to college | 3.27 | 0.25 | 43.09 | 0.37 | 0.76 | 0.18 | 3.23 | 0.71 | 0.48 | 0.13 | 1.74 | 0.27 |
Discussion
The purpose of the current study was to identify the depressive symptom profiles of Black high school students who experienced school policing, i.e., were stopped by school police or who observed other students being stopped by school police, as well the mental health profiles of Black high school students who experienced school discipline. Students who reported these two types of school policing had higher odds of experiencing depressive scores when compared to students that did not experience school policing or school discipline. If we uncritically assume that education is healthy, we erase essential experiences of Black students; centering the educational experiences of Black students should propel scholars to investigate countering narratives about the education-health relationship.
Our findings also revealed that all student subgroups, when compared to the unscathed students, were less likely to feel happy at school. The intermediate-high depressive score subgroup of the combined school policed in addition to not feeling happy at school also felt that teachers did not treat students with respect. The highest depressive mean score group of the school policed subgroup were found to have lower odds of feeling happy in school and felt teachers in school treated students with respect, in addition to feeling like they were part of their school. The school policed subgroup had an increased probability of being female. We gain further understanding of these mental health outcomes when making the symptomatic link to RBF, as well as “vicarious racism,” which includes students of color observing racism.32
Although this study advances the literature, it has limitations. First, this study employs a cross-sectional design which does not allow for causal inference found in longitudinal studies. Furthermore, we recommend the need for prospective studies to ascertain and better identify causal inference. The latter studies would inform if health observation changes latent classes over time; therefore, future studies should replicate these findings using longitudinal analyses. Second, the two measures that observe being stopped by school police are binary and may provide simple information; more nuanced, comprehensive instrumentation on school policing is needed such as what educators were present, educator-police interaction, the number of referrals, time of day, the context of an altercation, geographic area of school and so on. Third, there are potential underlying mechanisms underpinning the analysis, such as institutional, systemic, and interpersonal racism—even the “mis-education” of Black people, where U.S. education is used as an apparatus to both assimilate and oppress Black students.33
Despite the above limitations, this investigation has strengths that advance the extant literature on both RBF theory and the attributable, person-centered health profiles of Black students who are compelled to interact with school police. First, this study is nationally representative and sampled from major cities in the U.S. The second strength is that the Fragile Families Study includes both education and health measures, which allows for comprehensive, interdisciplinary health and education research. Finally, the ability of this data set to lend information to structure the education-health relationship by race and racism allows us to establish a counternarrative that exposes the deficits of the educational enterprise, rather than perpetuates the dominant deficit-based narrative of Black adolescents’ education-borne mental health conditions.
Future directions will need to focus on longitudinal data collection and analyses using both person-centered and variable-centered approaches. Not only do we need to understand if certain mental health observations change latent classes over time, but we also need to know the long-term mental health impacts of school policing, and other forms of social control strategies sanctioned by the educational enterprise, against Black people. As such, we can better inform educational and health policy towards the education, literacy, and well-being of Black youth and their communities. It will help researchers and practitioners understand whether the educational enterprise is a social determinant of health or whether it perpetuates the disenfranchisement from historically white institutions. The authors call on researchers, clinicians, and policymakers to create prospective studies or, retro-fit existing studies with the intent to better conceptualize, understand, and investigate how the relationship between the educational enterprise and health outcomes are structured by race and racism. Furthermore, the authors encourage child and adolescent psychiatrists to consider social systems, social forces, or institutions when identifying the potential causes of depressive symptoms in Black children and adolescents. Specifically, when psychiatrists examine and treat depressive symptoms of Black youth and the role that school policing has on the etiology and comorbidity of depression. We also encourage child and adolescent psychiatrists to implement, and participate in, community-based interventions to help remedy the mental health conditions that present challenges to Black adolescents.
This catalog of research cannot be ahistorical; empirical research must go above and beyond couching studies with lists of events or problems. Scholars need to couch research in historiography, for this method reflects on the histories of power and oppression. Studies often include a lacuna of properly understanding outcomes of the 1954 Supreme Court decision, Brown v. Board of Education, which has been less impactful to the educational quality of Black communities.34 Finding ways to examine the quality of education and how culturally responsive and inclusive education is, with nationally representative data, is paramount to changing the narrative of Black students. Finally, the history of redlining and “de jure segregation,”35 as well as “resegregation”36 must be considered when investigating the education-health relationship.
Table 1.
n | % | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
|
||||
Have you ever been stopped by police at school? | ||||
No | 384 | 77.6 | ||
Yes | 111 | 22.4 | ||
Have you ever seen others stopped by police at school? | ||||
No | 802 | 50.3 | ||
Yes | 792 | 49.7 | ||
Have you ever been disciplined at school? | ||||
No | 1021 | 64.5 | ||
Yes | 563 | 35.5 | ||
Sex/Gender | ||||
Male | 803 | 50.2 | ||
Female | 798 | 49.8 | ||
|
||||
M | SE | |||
|
||||
Depressive symptom scores (R: 0, 15) | 2.92 | 0.07 |
Acknowledgments
The effort of Dr. Montiel Ishino was supported by the Division of Intramural Research, National Institute on Minority Health and Health Disparities, National Institutes of Health. The content of this work is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily reflect the views of the National Institutes of Health.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
The research was performed with permission from University of South Carolina’s Institutional Review Board.
Dr. Montiel Ishino served as the statistical expert for this research.
Disclosure: Drs. Platt and Montiel Ishino and Mr. Perryman have reported no biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest.
Contributor Information
Collin Perryman, University of South Carolina, Columbia.
C. Spencer Platt, University of South Carolina, Columbia.
Francisco Montiel Ishino, National Institute on Minority Health and Health Disparities, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, Maryland.
References
- 1.Walsemann KM, Bell BA, Maitra D. The intersection of school racial composition and student race/ethnicity on adolescent depressive and somatic symptoms. Soc Sci Med. 2011;72(11):1873–1883. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2011.03.033 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Walsemann KM, Bell BA, Goosby BJ. Effect of School Racial Composition on Trajectories of Depressive Symptoms from Adolescence Through Early Adulthood. Race Soc Probl. 2011;3(3):131–145. doi: 10.1007/s12552-011-9053-3 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Hurst AN, Bailey ML, Krueger NT, Garba R, Cokley K. The psychological impact of policing on African American students. Weissinger SE, Mack DA. In: Law Enforcement in the Age of Black Lives Matter. Lexington Books, 2018: 53–73 [Google Scholar]
- 4.Smith WA. Toward An understanding of misandric microaggressions and racial battle fatigue among African Americans in historically white institutions. State African Am Male. Published online 2010:265–277. [Google Scholar]
- 5.Skiba RJ, Eckes SE, Brown K. African American disproportionality in school discipline: The divide between best evidence and legal remedy. New York Law Sch Law Rev. 2009;54:1071–1112. http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=a9h&AN=50250474&site=ehost-live [Google Scholar]
- 6.Gottfredson DC, Crosse S, Tang Z, et al. Effects of school resource officers on school crime and responses to school crime. Criminol Public Policy. 2020;19(3):905–940. doi: 10.1111/1745-9133.12512 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Addington LA. Behavioral Scientist as a Policy Response to Columbine. Am Behav Sci. 2009;52(10):1426–1446. doi: 10.1177/0002764209332556 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Nance J Dismantling the School-to-Prison Pipeline: Tools for Change. Univ Florida Levin Coll Law Res Pap. 2016;313(16-2). [Google Scholar]
- 9.Ann Payne A, Welch K. Modeling The Effects Of Racial Threat On Punitive And Restorative School Discipline Practices. Criminology. 2010;48(4):1019–1062. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-9125.2010.00211.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Hirschfield PJ. Preparing for prison?: The criminalization of school discipline in the USA. Theor Criminol. 2008;12(1):79–101. doi: 10.1177/1362480607085795 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Morris EW, Perry BL. Girls Behaving Badly? Race, Gender, and Subjective Evaluation in the Discipline of African American Girls. Sociol Educ. 2017;90(2):127–148. doi: 10.1177/0038040717694876 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Welch K, Payne AA. Exclusionary school punishment: The effect of racial threat on expulsion and suspension. Youth Violence Juv Justice. 2012;10(2):155–171. doi: 10.1177/1541204011423766 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Skiba RJ, Peterson R. The Dark Side of Zero Tolerance: Can Punishment Lead to Safe Schools? Phi Delta Kappan. 1999;80(5):372–X. [Google Scholar]
- 14.Simson D. Exclusion, punishment, racism and our schools: A Critical Race Theory perspective on school discipline. UCLA Law Rev. 2014;61(2):506–563. [Google Scholar]
- 15.Peguero AA, Shekarkhar Z, Popp AM, Koo DJ. Punishing the Children of Immigrants: Race, Ethnicity, Generational Status, Student Misbehavior, and School Discipline. J Immigr Refug Stud. 2015;13(2):200–220. doi: 10.1080/15562948.2014.951136 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Skiba RJ, Losen DJ. Turning the Page on School Discipline. Am Educ. 2016;39(4):4–12. https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/EJ1086522.pdf [Google Scholar]
- 17.George JA. Sterotype and School Pushout: Race, Gender, and Discipline Disparities. Ark Law Rev. 2015;65(101):101–129. [Google Scholar]
- 18.Morris EW, Perry BL. The punishment gap: School suspension and racial disparities in achievement. Soc Probl. 2016;63(1):68–86. doi: 10.1093/socpro/spv026 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Mckenna JM, Pollock JM. Law Enforcement Officers in Schools: An Analysis of Ethical Issues. Crim Justice Ethics. 2014;33(3):163–184. doi: 10.1080/0731129X.2014.982974 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Bleakley P, Bleakley C. School Resource Officers, ‘Zero Tolerance’ and the Enforcement of Compliance in the American Education System. Interchang A Q J Educ. 2018;49(2):247–261. doi: 10.1007/s10780-018-9326-5 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Devlin DN, Gottfredson DC. The Roles of Police Officers in Schools: Effects on the Recording and Reporting of Crime. Youth Violence Juv Justice. 2018;16(2):208–223. doi: 10.1177/1541204016680405 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Fisher BW, Devlin DN. School Crime and the Patterns of Roles of School Resource Officers: Evidence From a National Longitudinal Study. Crime Delinq. Published online 2019. doi: 10.1177/0011128719875702 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Na C, Gottfredson DC. Police Officers in Schools: Effects on School Crime and the Processing of Offending Behaviors. Justice Q. 2013;30(4):619–650. doi: 10.1080/07418825.2011.615754 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Beger R Expansion of Police Power in Public Schools and the Vanishing Rights of Students. Soc Justice. 2002;29(1):119–130. [Google Scholar]
- 25.Nance JP. Students, police, and the school-to-prison pipeline. Washingt Univ Law Rev. 2016;93(4):919–987. [Google Scholar]
- 26.Husband M Racial battle fatigue and the Black student affairs professional in the era of #BlackLivesMatter. Vermont Connect. 2016;37:91–98. [Google Scholar]
- 27.Smith WA, Yosso TJ, Solorzano DG. Racial primes and black misandry on historically white campuses: Toward critical race accountability in educational administration. Educ Adm Q. 2007;43(5):559–585. doi: 10.1177/0013161X07307793 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Reichman NE, Teitler JO, Garfinkel I, McLanahan SS. Fragile Families: Sample and Design. Child Youth Serv Rev. 2001;23(4-5):303–326. doi: 10.1016/S0190-7409(01)00141-4 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Waldfogel J, Craigie TA, Brooks-Gunn J. Fragile families and child wellbeing. Futur Child. 2010;20(2):87–112. doi: 10.1353/foc.2010.0002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Perreira KM, Deeb-Sossa N, Harris KM, Bollen K. What are we measuring? An evaluation of the CES-D across race/ethnicity and immigrant generation. Soc Forces. 2005;83(4):1567–1602. doi: 10.1353/sof.2005.0077 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Nylund KL, Asparouhov T, Muthén BO. Deciding on the number of classes in latent class analysis and growth mixture modeling: A Monte Carlo simulation study. Struct Equ Model. 2007;14(4):535–569. doi: 10.1080/10705510701575396 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Truong KA, Museus SD, McGuire KM. Vicarious racism: a qualitative analysis of experiences with secondhand racism in graduate education. Int J Qual Stud Educ. 2016;29(2):224–247. doi: 10.1080/09518398.2015.1023234 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Woodson CG. The Mis-Education of the Negro. The Associated Publishers. 1933. [Google Scholar]
- 34.Bell DA. Unintended Lessons in Brown v. Board of Education, The. NYL Sch Law Rev. 2005;483(4):1053–1067. [Google Scholar]
- 35.Rothstein R The Color of Law. Liven ght Publishing Corporation. 2017. [Google Scholar]
- 36.Ayscue JB, University of California LACRP/ PDC. Settle for Segregation or Strive for Diversity? A Defining Moment for Maryland’s Public Schools.; 2013. [Google Scholar]