Skip to main content
American Journal of Epidemiology logoLink to American Journal of Epidemiology
. 2021 Oct 6;191(9):1532–1539. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwab246

Environmental Influences on Sleep in the California Teachers Study Cohort

Charlie Zhong , Travis Longcore, Jennifer Benbow, Nadia T Chung, Khang Chau, Sophia S Wang, James V Lacey Jr, Meredith Franklin
PMCID: PMC9437820  PMID: 34613370

Abstract

Only two-thirds of Americans meet the recommended 7 hours of sleep nightly. Insufficient sleep and circadian disruption have been associated with adverse health outcomes, including diabetes and cardiovascular disease. Several environmental disruptors of sleep have been reported, such as artificial light at night (ALAN) and noise. These studies tended to evaluate exposures individually. We evaluated several spatially derived environmental exposures (ALAN, noise, green space, and air pollution) and self-reported sleep outcomes obtained in 2012–2015 in a large cohort of 51,562 women in the California Teachers Study. Odds ratios (ORs) and 95% confidence intervals (CIs) were calculated for sleep duration and latency. After adjusting for age, race/ethnicity, chronotype, use of sleep medication, and self-reported trouble sleeping, ALAN (per 5 millicandela (mcd)/m2 luminance, OR = 1.13, 95% CI: 1.07, 1.20) and air pollution (per 5 μg/m3 PM2.5, OR = 1.06, 95% CI: 1.04, 1.09) were associated with shorter sleep duration (<7 hours), and noise was associated with longer latency (>15 minutes) (per 10 decibels, OR = 1.05, 95% CI: 1.01, 1.10). Green space was associated with increased duration (per 0.1 units, OR = 0.41, 95% CI: 0.28, 0.60) and decreased latency (per 0.1 units, OR = 0.55, 95% CI: 0.39, 0.78). Further research is necessary to understand how these and other exposures (e.g., diet) perturb an individuals’ inherited sleep patterns and contribute to downstream health outcomes.

Keywords: air pollution, artificial light at night, circadian rhythm, cohort study, epidemiology, green space, noise, sleep disruption

Abbreviations

ALAN

artificial light at night

CI

confidence interval

CTS

California Teachers Study

dB

decibel

EVI

enhanced vegetation index

mcd

millicandela

OR

odds ratio

PM2.5

particulate matter with an aerodynamic diameter ≤2.5 μm

Editor’s note: An invited commentary on this article appears on page 1540, and the authors’ response appears on page 1544.

Disruptions in sleep have been associated with increased risk of several health conditions, including cardiovascular disease, diabetes, and cancer (1). In 2015, the American Academy of Sleep Medicine and Sleep Research Society published a joint consensus statement recommending that adults get at least 7 hours of sleep each night (2); however, 35% of American adults fall short of this recommendation (3).

Light is one of the main environmental factors affecting the sleep-wake cycle. Photosensitive cells in our retina respond to light and trigger the suprachiasmatic nucleus in the hypothalamus. The suprachiasmatic nucleus then modulates body temperature and regulates levels of cortisol and melatonin in response to the amount of light we perceive. The reduction of exposure to light at night triggers the increase in melatonin, leading to the feeling of sleepiness. Exposure to bright lights before sleep can delay the release of melatonin, therefore increasing sleep latency and disrupting circadian rhythm (4). While the direct stimuli of light indoors prior to sleep has been shown to delay sleep onset and reduce sleep quality (5), so has artificial light at night (ALAN) from the outdoor environment (6, 7). ALAN has been increasing several percent yearly over the past decades due to increasing human development and urbanization and may be playing a role in disrupting circadian rhythm (8, 9).

In addition to ALAN, another large component to sleep disruption is noise. In 2011, the WHO published a report on the “burden of disease from environmental noise,” estimating almost a million disability-adjusted life-years of sleep lost to noise (10). Like ALAN, noise may contribute to a delay in sleep latency and has been associated with nocturnal awakenings. As we progress into the later and lighter stages of sleep, external noise stimuli contribute to the possibility of waking earlier (11). In addition to noise, our increasing urbanization has also disrupted green space and increased air pollution; these additional environmental factors may be linked to sleep disruption. Higher levels of green space have been directly and indirectly associated with improved sleep outcomes. The beneficial associations with green space were seen not only in studies that assessed use of green space (gardening or walking), but were also present in studies that only assessed residential surroundings (12). It is not clear the exact mechanisms of green space on health, but it is believed that mimicking the natural environment helps improve mental health through the reduction of stress and pain (13). Last, air pollution has been associated with poorer sleep (14), potentially due to inflammation and irritation of breathing airways and not limited to individuals with asthma and sleep apnea (15, 16).

The implication of such disruptions extends beyond sleep to other detrimental health outcomes. Mounting evidence of a link between breast cancer and circadian disruption due to night-shift work led the World Health Organization’s International Agency for Research on Cancer to classify night-shift work as a probable human carcinogen (17). We previously reported associations of ALAN with breast cancer and lymphoma in our cohort (18, 19). Air pollution has been associated with a host of adverse health outcomes, including but not limited to cancer risk, cardiovascular disease risk, and impairment of lung function and growth (20). After adjusting for near-roadway air pollution, noise pollution has also been associated with increased morbidity and mortality. A 2018 meta-analysis found 7% increased risk of diabetes mellitus per 5-decibel (dB) increase in road traffic noise (21), while a 2021 meta-analysis found no increased risk of death from road traffic noise but did see increased risk from aircraft noise (22). Finally, higher levels of green space are also associated with improvements in mental health and reduced mortality (23), with 2 studies reporting up to a 10% reduction in mortality for those living in greener areas (24, 25).

Many studies of environmental sleep disruption examined a single environmental exposure at a time. In this work, we assessed the association of several geospatially derived environmental exposures concurrently on self-reported sleep outcomes in the California Teachers Study (CTS), a large cohort of women residing in California.

METHODS

Study population

The CTS is a large cohort of 133,477 women recruited in 1995–1996 from the California State Teachers’ Retirement System. Women returned a baseline questionnaire and have since participated in ongoing follow-up activities. Additional details of the cohort have been previously published (26). A total of 56,114 women responded to a follow-up questionnaire, conducted from 2012–2015, that included self-reported questions of sleep history. Of the respondents, 53,426 answered all questions regarding their sleep habits. We were able to geocode exposure data for 51,562 participants who were residents in the state of California in 2014, when our environmental exposures were assessed.

Environmental exposures

We assessed 2 primary environmental factors that may contribute to sleep disruption, outdoor artificial light at night and environmental noise, as well as 2 secondary exposures, green space and air pollution. We used the New World Atlas of Artificial Night Sky Brightness as our measure of ALAN (27). The World Atlas provides a global 750-m gridded measure of estimated light at zenith (the sky directly overhead) in millicandela (mcd) per meters squared based on the Visible Infrared Imaging Radiometer Suite satellite and thousands of handheld sky quality measurements. This measurement has shown to be a better estimate of ground-level light exposure than use of satellite data alone (28), and its higher resolution makes it superior to older, coarser measurements and less likely to be highly correlated with air and noise pollution (29).

Environmental noise was assessed with the National Park Service sound map (30). The sound model used to produce the map uses collected data coupled with geospatial prediction to calculate the L50 (sound level exceeded half the time) sound pressure–level metric, which is a weighted measure that considers how the human ear perceives sound. The median L50 sound level in dB is produced over a 270-m grid representing natural sound (e.g., wind and animals) as well as for sound from human sources (e.g., traffic). The model provides data on longitude, latitude, weather, land cover, distance to bodies of water/streams, distance to roads, and air traffic, and it was validated with thousands of measurements taken from 479 sites across the contiguous United States.

To assess green space, we used the enhanced vegetation index (EVI). EVI is derived from the normalized difference vegetation index, which is a calculation of the ratio of near infrared to visible light observed by the Moderate Resolution Imaging Spectroradiometer instrument onboard NASA’s Terra and Aqua Satellites. The EVI improves upon the normalized difference vegetation index by incorporating blue light to correct for atmospheric conditions and dense vegetation (31). EVI is unitless ranging from −1 to 1; where −1 represents completely barren land and 1 represents areas of high-density vegetation. The 2014 annual, cloud-free, average EVI was calculated in 250-m grids.

Last, air pollution was assessed utilizing an ensemble-based machine-learning model developed by Di et al. (32). The model augmented the GEOS-Chem chemical transport model output with land-use variables including elevation, road density, the Environmental Protection Agency’s National Emissions Inventory, population density, urbanization, normalized difference vegetation index, weather patterns, and Moderate Resolution Imaging Spectroradiometer satellite–derived aerosol optical depth. The model estimates concentrations of particulate matter with an aerodynamic diameter ≤2.5 μm (PM2.5) in a 1-km grid. Participants were assigned the average level of PM2.5 for 2014.

Additional covariates on the built environment were obtained from 2015 5-year average American Community Survey data at the census-tract level (33). We estimated tract-level socioeconomic status by creating a z score of homeowner percentage, college education percentage, home value, and income. We also assessed whether participants resided in a rural or urban location based on the United States Department of Agriculture 2010 Rural-Urban Commuting Area Codes (34).

Outcomes

Sleep outcomes were ascertained by self-reported questionnaire between 2012–2015. Chronotype was assessed with a single question based on the Horne-Ostberg Morningness-Eveningness Questionnaire (35). While the full Horne-Ostberg Morningness-Eveningness Questionnaire contains 19 questions, it has been shown that a single question predicts overall chronotype nearly as well as the full questionnaire (36). An adapted version of the Pittsburg Sleep Quality Index was used to ask participants about their sleep habits over the past month, including duration, latency, trouble sleeping, and any use of sleep-aid medication (37). Sleep duration is the amount of time slept each night and sleep latency is the time it takes for an individual to transition from wakefulness to sleep. We dichotomized our main outcomes, sleep duration (≥7 hours vs. <7 hours) and sleep latency (≤15 minutes vs. >15 minutes), based on the recommendations and consensus of the American Academy of Sleep Medicine, Sleep Research Society, and National Sleep Foundation (2, 38); This and all past CTS questionnaires are available online at https://www.calteachersstudy.org/.

Statistical methods

Pearson correlation coefficients were calculated between the light-at-night, noise, green space, and air pollution exposures. We evaluated the association of the exposures with sleep duration and sleep latency using logistic regression, calculating age-adjusted odds ratios (ORs) and 95% confidence intervals (CIs). In multipollutant models, we also adjusted for race and chronotype. All analyses were conducted in SAS, version 9.4 (SAS Institute, Inc., Cary, North Carolina).

RESULTS

The average age of participants who responded to questionnaire 5 was 66.9 years (range, 39.6–106.8), and a majority were non-Hispanic White (88%) (Table 1). Most women reported having an early or more-early-than-late chronotype (59%). The most common length of sleep was 7 hours (42%), followed by 8 or more hours (31%). Only 2% of women reported getting less than 5 hours of sleep. Most women reported low sleep latency, 47% <15 minutes and 36% 15–30 minutes. Only 22% reported not having any trouble sleeping in the past month, with 20% reporting having trouble more than 3 times/week; however, only 30% reported use of any sleep medication. The vast majority (92%) of participants resided in an urban setting (Rural-Urban Commuting Area Codes 1–3).

Table 1.

Self-Reported Sleep Outcomes and Built Environment (n = 51,562), California Teachers Study Cohort, California, United States, 2012–2015

Characteristic No. %
Age, yearsa 66.9 (10.9) (39.6, 106.8)
Race/ethnicity
 Non-Hispanic White 45,351 88
 Otherb 6,211 12
Light at night, mcd/m2a 2.81 (2.09) (0.01, 13.1)
Noise, dBa 47.37 (4.07) (24.9, 59.1)
Green spacea 0.21 (0.06) (−0.51, 0.59)
PM2.5 air pollution, μg/m3a 10.43 (4.04) (1.71, 34.2)
Chronotype
 Early 22,131 43
 More early than late 7,970 15
 Neither 6,969 14
 More late than early 6,795 13
 Late 7,697 15
Sleep duration, hours
 ≥8 16,167 31
 7 21,748 42
 5–6 12,417 24
 <5 1,230 2
Sleep latency, minutes
 <15 24,359 47
 16–30 18,776 36
 31–60 6,494 13
 >60 1,933 4
Sleep medication use, times per week
 Not in past month 36,158 70
 <1 5,567 11
 1–2 3,020 6
 ≥3 6,817 13
Sleep trouble, times per week
 Not in past month 11,506 22
 <1 17,066 33
 1–2 12,864 25
 ≥3 10,126 20

Abbreviations: dB, decibel; mcd, millicandela; PM2.5, particulate matter with an aerodynamic diameter ≤2.5 μm.

a Values are expressed as mean (standard deviation) and range.

b Other races/ethnicities included Black, Hispanic, Native American, Asian/Pacific Islander, and other/mixed.

The average ALAN among the sleep respondents was 2.81 (standard deviation, 2.09) mcd/m2 and ranged from a minimum of almost no ALAN to 13.1 mcd/m2. The average sound level was 47.37 (standard deviation, 4.07) dB and ranged from 24.9 to 59.1 dB. Participants resided in moderately green areas with an average EVI of 0.21 (standard deviation, 0.06), ranging from −0.51 to 0.59. The average PM2.5 was 10.43 (standard deviation, 4.04) μg/m3 and ranged from 1.71 to 34.2 μg/m3. The distributions of these environmental factors across the state of California are available in Web Figure 1 (available at https://doi.org/10.1093/aje/kwab246).

ALAN was somewhat correlated with air pollution (r = 0.40), and green space was negatively correlated with ALAN (r = −0.46) and air pollution (r = −0.22) (Web Table 1). There was a modest correlation between self-reported sleep disturbance with latency (r = 0.46) and duration (r = 0.41) (Web Table 2).

In the single pollutant models, for each 5-mcd/m2 increase in light at night, there was a 1.35 (95% CI: 1.29, 1.41) times greater risk of getting less than 7 hours of sleep (Table 2). A 0.1-unit increase in EVI was associated with 0.16 (95% CI: 0.11, 0.22) decreased risk of sleeping less than 7 hours, while each 5-μg/m3 increase in PM2.5 was associated with a 1.13 (95% CI: 1.10, 1.16) times greater risk of less sleep. A 10-dB increase in sound was associated with 1.05 (95% CI: 1.01, 1.10) increased risk of longer sleep latency (taking >15 minutes to fall asleep). A 0.1-unit increase in green space was associated with improved sleep latency (OR = 0.57, 95% CI: 0.43, 0.76), and a 5-μg/m3 increase in PM2.5 was associated with longer sleep latency (OR = 1.04, 95% CI: 1.01, 1.06). Compared with those that reported early chronotype, those that reported the late chronotype had worse sleep duration (OR = 1.22, 95% CI: 1.15, 1.29). The more intermediate chronotypes, however, appeared to have a 15% lower risk of having a shorter sleep duration. There was a consistent, increasing risk of longer sleep latency with later chronotypes (P for trend < 0.001).

Table 2.

Characteristics and Environmental Exposures Associated With Sleep (n = 51,562), California Teachers Study Cohort, California, United States, 2012–2015

Sleep Duration <7 Hours Sleep Latency >15 Minutes
Characteristic Age-Adjusted OR 95% CI Age-Adjusted OR 95% CI
Light at night, 5 mcd/m2 1.35 1.29, 1.41 1.02 0.98, 1.07
Noise, 10 dB 1.03 0.98, 1.08 1.05 1.01, 1.10
Green space, 0.1 EVI units 0.16 0.11, 0.22 0.57 0.43, 0.76
PM2.5 air pollution, 5 μg/m3 1.13 1.10, 1.16 1.04 1.01, 1.06
Chronotype
 Early 1.00 Referent 1.00 Referent
 More early 0.79 0.74, 0.84 1.13 1.07, 1.19
 Neither 0.87 0.82, 0.93 1.24 1.17, 1.31
 More late 0.84 0.79, 0.90 1.48 1.40, 1.56
 Late 1.22 1.15, 1.29 1.65 1.56, 1.74
Race/ethnicity, non-Hispanic White vs. other 2.08 1.96, 2.19 1.15 1.09, 1.21
Self-reported use of sleep medication, never vs. ever 1.55 1.59, 1.62 2.82 2.71, 2.94
Self-reported trouble sleeping, never vs. ever 2.79 2.64, 2.96 4.25 4.06, 4.46

Abbreviations: CI, confidence interval; dB, decibel; EVI, enhanced vegetation index; mcd, millicandela; OR, odds ratio; PM2.5, particulate matter with an aerodynamic diameter ≤2.5 μm.

In the multipollutant model, adjusting for age, race, chronotype, and other self-reported sleep factors, each 5-mcd/m2 increase in light at night was associated with a 1.13 (95% CI: 1.07, 1.20) increased risk of getting less than 7 hours of sleep (Table 3). A 10 dB-increase in sound was not associated with sleep duration but was associated with a 1.05 (95% CI: 1.01, 1.10) increased risk of longer sleep latency (taking >15 minutes to fall asleep). Increasing EVI was associated with decreased risk of poor sleep latency (OR = 0.55, 95% CI: 0.39, 0.78). Increased air pollution was associated with lower sleep duration (OR = 1.06, 95% CI: 1.04, 1.09) but not sleep latency (OR = 1.02, 85% CI: 1.00, 1.05). Results were similar when stratified to only urban participants (Web Table 3). When we considered only the natural sound model, green space was still associated with better sleep latency (OR = 0.59, 95% CI: 0.41, 0.84), but the association with natural sound intensity and sleep latency was not significant (OR = 1.09, 95% CI: 0.99, 1.20, Web Table 4). Adjusting for our neighborhood-level z score of socioeconomic status did not alter the associations of the environmental exposures with sleep duration but did attenuate the associations with sleep latency and were no longer statistically significant (Web Table 5).

Table 3.

Multipollutant Model of Environmental Exposures and Sleep (n = 51,562), California Teachers Study Cohort, California, United States, 2012–2015

Environmental Exposure Sleep Duration <7 Hours a Sleep Latency >15 Minutes b
OR 95% CI OR 95% CI
Light at night, 5 mcd/m2 1.13 1.07, 1.20 0.97 0.91, 1.02
Noise, 10 dB 1.02 0.97, 1.08 1.05 1.01, 1.10
Green space, 0.1 EVI units 0.41 0.28, 0.60 0.55 0.39, 0.78
PM2.5 air pollution, 5 μg/m3 1.06 1.04, 1.09 1.02 1.00, 1.05

Abbreviations: CI, confidence interval; dB, decibel; EVI, enhanced vegetation index; mcd, millicandela; OR, odds ratio; PM2.5, particulate matter with an aerodynamic diameter ≤2.5 μm.

a Adjusted for age, race, chronotype, use of sleep medication, trouble sleeping, and sleep latency.

b Adjusted for age, race, chronotype, use of sleep medication, trouble sleeping, and sleep duration.

DISCUSSION

Self-reported chronotype tended to fall into the earlier (59% “early” or “more early than late”) categories in our cohort. Those reporting the late chronotype had shorter sleep duration and increased sleep latency. Studies of shift-work show disruptions in sleep for individuals with later chronotypes who have earlier shift schedules (39), a pattern that would be typical in our cohort of school educators.

Light exposure meeting certain thresholds suppresses melatonin, thereby delaying the onset of sleep (6, 7, 40, 41); however, we did not see an association between ALAN and sleep latency. We did see an association with lower sleep duration and increased ALAN. Data on sleep timing were not collected for the cohort, so we were unable to assess the association between the environment and when participants went to sleep. Beyond the associations seen with sleep disruption and disease risk (7), studies have also shown an association with ALAN and tumor progression (42, 43). In contrast to ALAN, noise pollution was not significantly associated with sleep duration but was associated with sleep latency. This is consistent with several studies that have shown an association with sound and increase in sleep latency (4446).

Green space was significantly associated with longer sleep duration and shorter sleep latency. While we have no data on participant use of green space, in a review by Shin et al. (12), the associations of green space with improved sleep outcomes were shown to not only be limited to studies in which participant use of green space was assessed, but they were seen in studies that only evaluated study participant surroundings. Green space is believed to help improve mood and mental health as well as mitigate noise pollution, air pollution, and ALAN, leading to reduced morbidity and mortality (13, 23, 47, 48). Some studies have reported potential gene-environment interaction, indicating some individuals may have differential benefit from residing in green spaces (25, 49). We saw an inverse association with EVI and ALAN (r = −0.46) and air pollution (r = −0.22). The associations of green space do not seem to be limited entirely to the surrounding environment, as studies have shown that viewing simulated imagery of green space can also improve both mood and sleep (5052). While we did control for self-reported trouble sleeping, this may not fully capture psychosocial stressors of the built environment (53) that can contribute to poor sleep (54). Results were consistent when restricted to participants residing in urban Rural-Urban Commuting Area Codes (Web Table 3). When we adjusted for other features of the built environment, with census tract–level socioeconomic status as a proxy, the associations between exposures and sleep latency were similar but no longer significant (Web Table 5). However, the size of the census tracts, as they are based on population, compared with the resolution of other data sources, may obscure real patterns. A recent study by McIsaac et al. (29) demonstrated the potential to misclassify exposure as spatial resolution decreased.

Air pollution was significantly associated with reduced sleep duration and was marginally significant for increased sleep latency. There was a modest inverse correlation between air pollution and green space, and the association with air pollution was attenuated in the multipollutant model while the association of sound on sleep was not. Air pollution’s detrimental association with sleep may not be limited to individuals with asthma or other respiratory conditions that are exacerbated by poor air quality (14, 15). Gene-environment interactions have also been observed for light at night (55) and air pollution (56). Interestingly, there was no correlation between sound levels and any of the other exposures. Unlike ALAN and air pollution, which are largely human-made, the noise model includes ambient sound (nature) as well as human-made sound. When we evaluated natural sound only, it was no longer significantly associated with sleep latency (per 5 dB, OR = 1.09, 95% CI: 0.99, 1.20; Web Table 4), indicating that the association may be attributed more heavily to anthropogenic sound. In the multipollutant model, associations with sleep duration were attenuated substantially but less so for sleep latency. These factors likely point to shared attributes of the built environment that may work synergistically to disrupt sleep.

Strengths of this study include a large sample size, comprising 50,000 California teachers and school administrators. Our continued follow-up provided high-quality residential address history of participants and allowed us to assess multiple environmental exposures. Our measure of ALAN, the New World Atlas of Artificial Night Sky Brightness, better captures ground-level exposure to light at night than raw values of upward radiance recorded by satellites (28). Additionally, our air pollution model provides a highly accurate and validated measure of PM2.5 at a 1-km resolution compared with use of sparsely located air-quality monitoring stations (32).

Study limitations included the self-reported nature of the sleep variables. We also did not have information on the timing of sleep, nor could we assess a direct correlation between indoor exposure to light and exposure to outdoor light. One small study did not see a correlation between outdoor light and indoor light exposure in the bedroom (57); however, light exposure and disruptions to sleep have been found to persist for hours (40, 41). In a recent Dutch study of children, individual-level light exposure showed the influence of outdoor light on indoor light during the darkest time period with a correlation of 0.31 (58), even when nearly all bedrooms had blackout curtains. In a survey of lighting designers using their own light meters, Miller and Kinzey (59) reported measurements in a number of different contexts within homes. At windows without drapes, a maximum of 20 lux was reported, with a mean of 5 lux and median of 0.5 lux. We also did not have information on any use of handheld devices before bedtime. The US Parks noise model provided daytime levels incorporating ambient sound, unlike many noise models that are largely focused on traffic and not as accurate on smaller roadways in less dense urban areas (60). Similar to ALAN, it may be the exposure prior to sleep and waking that are most associated with sleep disruption, and not necessarily continued exposure during the nighttime (44, 45).

In summary, higher levels of light at night and air pollution were associated with less sleep duration, and higher sound levels were associated increased sleep latency. There was a significant positive association with sleep duration and latency from green space. In the multipollutant model, the detrimental association of light at night and air pollution with sleep duration were attenuated, but this was not so for sleep latency. As our understanding of sleep improves, additional studies need to be done to understand the mechanisms by which the environment affects sleep.

Supplementary Material

Web_Material_kwab246

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

Author affiliations: Department of Population and Public Health Sciences, Keck School of Medicine, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, California, United States (Charlie Zhong, Khang Chau, Meredith Franklin); Institute of the Environment and Sustainability, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, California, United States (Travis Longcore); Division of Health Analytics, Department of Computational and Quantitative Medicine, Beckman Research Institute of the City of Hope National Medical Center, Duarte, California, United States (Jennifer Benbow, Sophia S. Wang, James V. Lacey Jr); and Children’s Oncology Group, Monrovia, California, United States (Nadia T. Chung).

Research reported in this publication was supported by the National Institute of Environmental Health Sciences of the National Institutes of Health (award number T32ES013678). The California Teachers Study and the research reported in this publication were supported by the National Cancer Institute of the National Institutes of Health (award numbers U01-CA199277, P30-CA033572, P30-CA023100, UM1-CA164917, and R01-CA077398).

The data underlying this article will be shared on reasonable request to the corresponding author.

We thank the California Teachers Study Steering Committee, which is responsible for the formation and maintenance of the study within which this research was conducted. A full list of California Teachers Study team members is available at https://www.calteachersstudy.org/team.

The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and the opinions, findings, and conclusions expressed herein are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official views of the State of California, Department of Public Health, the National Cancer Institute, the National Institutes of Health, the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention or their contractors and subcontractors, or the Regents of the University of California, or any of its programs.

Conflict of interest: none declared.

REFERENCES

  • 1. Jike M, Itani O, Watanabe N, et al. Long sleep duration and health outcomes: a systematic review, meta-analysis and meta-regression. Sleep Med Rev. 2018;39:25–36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Consensus Conference Panel, Watson NF, Badr MS, et al. Joint Consensus Statement of the American Academy of Sleep Medicine and Sleep Research Society on the recommended amount of sleep for a healthy adult: methodology and discussion. Sleep. 2015;38(8):1161–1183. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Kant AK, Graubard BI. Association of self-reported sleep duration with eating behaviors of American adults: NHANES 2005–2010. Am J Clin Nutr. 2014;100(3):938–947. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Tähkämö L, Partonen T, Pesonen A-K. Systematic review of light exposure impact on human circadian rhythm. Chronobiol Int. 2019;36(2):151–170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Dautovich ND, Schreiber DR, Imel JL, et al. A systematic review of the amount and timing of light in association with objective and subjective sleep outcomes in community-dwelling adults. Sleep Health. 2019;5(1):31–48. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Ohayon MM, Milesi C. Artificial outdoor nighttime lights associate with altered sleep behavior in the American general population. Sleep. 2016;39(6):1311–1320. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Touitou Y, Reinberg A, Touitou D. Association between light at night, melatonin secretion, sleep deprivation, and the internal clock: health impacts and mechanisms of circadian disruption. Life Sci. 2017;173:94–106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Kyba CCM, Kuester T, Sánchez de Miguel A, et al. Artificially lit surface of Earth at night increasing in radiance and extent. Sci Adv. 2017;3(11):e1701528. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Hölker F, Moss T, Griefahn B, et al. The dark side of light: a transdisciplinary research agenda for light pollution policy. Ecol Soc. 2010;15(4):13. [Google Scholar]
  • 10. World Health Organization . Burden of disease from environmental noise: quantification of healthy life years lost in Europe. Copenhagen, Denmark: World Health Organization. Regional Office for Europe; 2011. https://www.euro.who.int/__data/assets/pdf_file/0008/136466/e94888.pdf. Accessed October 1, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Pirrera S, De Valck E, Cluydts R. Nocturnal road traffic noise: a review on its assessment and consequences on sleep and health. Environ Int. 2010;36(5):492–498. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Shin JC, Parab KV, An R, et al. Greenspace exposure and sleep: a systematic review. Environ Res. 2020;182:109081. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Stanhope J, Breed MF, Weinstein P. Exposure to greenspaces could reduce the high global burden of pain. Environ Res. 2020;187:109641. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Liu J, Wu T, Liu Q, et al. Air pollution exposure and adverse sleep health across the life course: a systematic review. Environ Pollut. 2020;262:114263. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Mutlu GM, Peker Y. Air pollution, asthma, and sleep apnea: new epidemiological links? Ann Am Thorac Soc. 2019;16(3):307–308. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Zanobetti A, Redline S, Schwartz J, et al. Associations of PM10 with sleep and sleep-disordered breathing in adults from seven U.S. urban areas. Am J Respir Crit Care Med. 2010;182(6):819–825. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. IARC Monographs Vol 124 group . Carcinogenicity of night shift work. Lancet Oncol. 2019;20(8):1058–1059. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Hurley S, Goldberg D, Nelson D, et al. Light at night and breast cancer risk among California teachers. Epidemiology. 2014;25(5):697–706. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Zhong C, Franklin M, Wiemels J, et al. Outdoor artificial light at night and risk of non-Hodgkin lymphoma among women in the California Teachers Study cohort. Cancer Epidemiol. 2020;69:101811. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Brunekreef B, Holgate ST. Air pollution and health. Lancet. 2002;360(9341):1233–1242. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Zare Sakhvidi MJ, Zare Sakhvidi F, Mehrparvar AH, et al. Association between noise exposure and diabetes: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Environ Res. 2018;166:647–657. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Cai Y, Ramakrishnan R, Rahimi K. Long-term exposure to traffic noise and mortality: a systematic review and meta-analysis of epidemiological evidence between 2000 and 2020. Environ Pollut. 2021;269:116222. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. James P, Banay RF, Hart JE, et al. A review of the health benefits of greenness. Curr Epidemiol Rep. 2015;2(2):131–142. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Vienneau D, Hoogh K, Faeh D, et al. More than clean air and tranquillity: residential green is independently associated with decreasing mortality. Environ Int. 2017;108:176–184. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Liu L, Zhu A, Shu C, et al. Gene-environment interaction of FOXO and residential greenness on mortality among older adults. Rejuvenation Res. 2021;24(1):49–61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Bernstein L, Allen M, Anton-Culver H, et al. High breast cancer incidence rates among California teachers: results from the California Teachers Study (United States). Cancer Causes Control. 2002;13(7):625–635. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Falchi F, Cinzano P, Duriscoe D, et al. The new world atlas of artificial night sky brightness. Sci Adv. 2016;2(6):e1600377. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Simons AL, Yin X, Longcore T. High correlation but high scale-dependent variance between satellite measured night lights and terrestrial exposure. Environ Res Commun. 2020;2(2):021006. [Google Scholar]
  • 29. McIsaac MA, Sanders E, Kuester T, et al. The impact of image resolution on power, bias, and confounding: a simulation study of ambient light at night exposure. Environ Epidemiol. 2021;5(2):e145. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Mennitt D, Sherrill K, Fristrup K. A geospatial model of ambient sound pressure levels in the contiguous United States. J Acoust Soc Am. 2014;135(5):2746–2764. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Liu HQ, Huete A. A feedback based modification of the NDVI to minimize canopy background and atmospheric noise. IEEE Trans Geosci Remote Sens. 1995;33(2):457–465. [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Di Q, Amini H, Shi L, et al. An ensemble-based model of PM2.5 concentration across the contiguous United States with high spatiotemporal resolution. Environ Int. 2019;130:104909. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. U.S. Census Bureau . Summary File. American Community Survey Office; 2015. https://www2.census.gov/programs-surveys/acs/summary_file/2015/data/5_year_seq_by_state/California/Tracts_Block_Groups_Only/. Accessed August 7, 2020.
  • 34. US Department of Agriculture Economic Research Service . Rural-urban continuum codes; 2010. https://www.ers.usda.gov/webdocs/DataFiles/53241/ruca2010revised.xlsx?v=4906.5. Accessed October, 1 2020.
  • 35. Horne JA, Ostberg O. A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningness-eveningness in human circadian rhythms. Int J Chronobiol. 1976;4(2):97–110. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Roenneberg T, Kuehnle T, Juda M, et al. Epidemiology of the human circadian clock. Sleep Med Rev. 2007;11(6):429–438. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Hurley S, Goldberg D, Von Behren J, et al. Sleep deficiency and breast cancer risk among postmenopausal women in the California Teachers Study (CTS). Cancer Causes Control. 2020;31(12):1115–1128. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Ohayon M, Wickwire EM, Hirshkowitz M, et al. National Sleep Foundation's sleep quality recommendations: first report. Sleep Health. 2017;3(1):6–19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Juda M, Vetter C, Roenneberg T. Chronotype modulates sleep duration, sleep quality, and social jet lag in shift-workers. J Biol Rhythms. 2013;28(2):141–151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Gooley JJ, Chamberlain K, Smith KA, et al. Exposure to room light before bedtime suppresses melatonin onset and shortens melatonin duration in humans. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2011;96(3):E463–E472. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Akacem LD, Wright KP Jr, LeBourgeois MK. Sensitivity of the circadian system to evening bright light in preschool-age children. Physiol Rep. 2018;6(5):e13617. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Zubidat AE, Fares B, Fares F, et al. Artificial light at night of different spectral compositions differentially affects tumor growth in mice: interaction with melatonin and epigenetic pathways. Cancer Control. 2018;25(1):1073274818812908. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Blask DE, Dauchy RT, Dauchy EM, et al. Light exposure at night disrupts host/cancer circadian regulatory dynamics: impact on the Warburg effect, lipid signaling and tumor growth prevention. PLoS One. 2014;9(8):e102776. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Röösli M, Brink M, Rudzik F, et al. Associations of various nighttime noise exposure indicators with objective sleep efficiency and self-reported sleep quality: a field study. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2019;16(20):3790. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Evandt J, Oftedal B, Hjertager Krog N, et al. A population-based study on nighttime road traffic noise and insomnia. Sleep. 2017;40(2):e102776. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Mayne S, Morales K, Williamson AA, et al. 0390 Associations of the neighborhood built environment with adolescent sleep outcomes [abstract]. Sleep. 2020;43(suppl 1):A149–A150. [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Kondo MC, Fluehr JM, McKeon T, et al. Urban green space and its impact on human health. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2018;15(3):445. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Franklin M, Yin X, McConnell R, et al. Association of the built environment with childhood psychosocial stress. JAMA Netw Open. 2020;3(10):e2017634. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Ji JS, Zhu A, Shu C, et al. APOE genotype status on the effect of residential greenness on cognitive function and mortality: a cohort study. The Lancet. 2019;394:S73. [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Meidenbauer KL, Stenfors CUD, Bratman GN, et al. The affective benefits of nature exposure: what's nature got to do with it? J Environ Psychol. 2020;72:101498. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51. Anderson A, Mayer M, Fellows A, et al. Relaxation with immersive natural scenes presented using virtual reality. Aerosp Med Human Perform. 2017;88(6):520–526. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52. Yüksel D, Goldstone A, Prouty D, et al. 0916 The use of immersive virtual reality and slow breathing to enhance relaxation and sleep in adolescents [abstract]. Sleep. 2020;43(suppl 1):A348. [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Robinette JW, Charles ST, Almeida DM, et al. Neighborhood features and physiological risk: An examination of allostatic load. Health Place. 2016;41:110–118. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54. Chen X, Redline S, Shields AE, et al. Associations of allostatic load with sleep apnea, insomnia, short sleep duration, and other sleep disturbances: findings from the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey 2005 to 2008. Ann Epidemiol. 2014;24(8):612–619. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55. Haim A, Zubidat AE. Artificial light at night: melatonin as a mediator between the environment and epigenome. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2015;370(1667):20140121. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56. Favé MJ, Lamaze FC, Soave D, et al. Gene-by-environment interactions in urban populations modulate risk phenotypes. Nat Commun. 2018;9(1):827. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57. Rea MS, Brons JA, Figueiro MG. Measurements of light at night (LAN) for a sample of female school teachers. Chronobiol Int. 2011;28(8):673–680. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58. Huss A, Wel L, Bogaards L, et al. Shedding some light in the dark—a comparison of personal measurements with satellite-based estimates of exposure to light at night among children in the Netherlands. Environ Health Perspect. 2019;127(6):067001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59. Miller NJ, Kinzey BR. Home nighttime light exposures: how much are we really getting? International Association of Lighting Designers; 2018. https://www.iald.org/News/In-the-News/Home-Nighttime-Light-Exposures-How-Much-Are-We-Re. Accessed March 5, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  • 60. Yin X, Fallah-Shorshani M, McConnell R, et al. Predicting fine spatial scale traffic noise using Mobile measurements and machine learning. Environ Sci Technol. 2020;54(20):12860–12869. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Web_Material_kwab246

Articles from American Journal of Epidemiology are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press

RESOURCES