Skip to main content
Frontiers in Psychology logoLink to Frontiers in Psychology
. 2022 Aug 22;13:977381. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.977381

How negative anthropomorphic message framing and nostalgia enhance pro-environmental behaviors during the COVID-19 pandemic in China: An SEM-NCA approach

Shuai Zhou 1,2,*, Yibo Wang 1
PMCID: PMC9441880  PMID: 36072047

Abstract

Although extensive research has been conducted on promoting pro-environmental behaviors among consumers, little is known about whether and how negative anthropomorphic message framing (NAMF) and nostalgia affect pro-environmental behavior. To provide a framework for explaining pro-environmental behavior, this study integrates protection motivation theory, the stimulus-organism-response model, and message framing. To create the model of the influences on pro-environmental behavior, NAMF was employed as the external stimulus; the sense of environmental responsibility, environmental empathy, perceived threat, and perceived vulnerability as the psychological and cognitive response factors; pro-environmental behavior as the final decision of consumers; and nostalgia as the moderating variable. An online questionnaire was distributed and 380 usable questionnaires were collected using convenience sampling and analyzed using two complementary approaches: partial least squares structural equation modeling (PLS-SEM) and necessary condition analysis (NCA). PLS-SEM results showed that pro-environmental behavior was significantly affected by NAMF (β = 0.313, t-value = 5.583), environmental responsibility (β = 0.207, t-value = 3.994), and perceived threats (β = 0.252, t-value = 4.889). Meanwhile, an increase in nostalgia increased the effect of NAMF and environmental responsibility on pro-environmental behavior. The NCA results revealed that NAMF (d = 0.108, p < 0.001) and perceived threat (d = 0.209, p < 0.001) were key factors of pro-environmental behavior. In addition, for high level of pro-environmental behavior (>80%), NAMF (12.1%) and perceived threat (39.6%) are required. Finally, we offer several suggestions based on the results of our empirical research. For example, marketing and service offerings should be tailored to the needs of masses with different nostalgic tendencies to enhance their pro-environmental behaviors.

Keywords: pro-environmental behavior, environmental responsibility, environmental empathy, perceived threat, perceived vulnerability, nostalgia, SOR model, protection motivation theory

Introduction

As social transformation accelerates and consumerism becomes more prevalent, environmental concerns have become more prominent (Panda et al., 2020; Busby, 2021). An increasing number of urban residents are realizing that worsening environmental issues are connected to their own lives and their behavior contributes to urban environmental problems (do Paço et al., 2013; Nekmahmud and Fekete-Farkas, 2020; Patak et al., 2021). A recent study indicated that unreasonable consumption habits and consumption patterns contribute significantly to environmental degradation (Sheng et al., 2019). A consumer behavior strategy that encourages consumers to abandon the consumption style of wastefulness and actively practice green consumption that respects nature and pursues health is a practical necessity for promoting sustainable economic and social development and a pressing theoretical issue in consumer behavior. Therefore, to solve urban environmental problems, a correct understanding of the pro-environmental behavior of urban residents is required (Lindenberg and Steg, 2007; Zhang et al., 2020; Xi and Wang, 2022), as well as providing urban residents full credit for their contribution to environmental protection. In general, pro-environmental behavior is an action that is taken by humans to protect the environment and prevent its degradation (Stern, 2000).

Scholars have investigated the mechanisms of pro-environmental behavior based on different theoretical foundations and diverse fields. Among them, the most representative theories are the theory of planned behavior (TPB) (De Groot and Steg, 2007; De Leeuw et al., 2015; Carfora et al., 2017; Yang et al., 2020; Alzubaidi et al., 2021) and the norm-activation model (Onwezen et al., 2013; Han et al., 2015; van der Werff and Steg, 2015; Gao et al., 2022). Studies using these theories have demonstrated the importance of rational cognitive factors in the development of pro-environmental behaviors. As a result, rational cognitive factors, such as environmental concerns and subjective norms, have been continuously incorporated into the system of factors influencing pro-environmental behavior. Nevertheless, several empirical studies have indicated that logical cognitive elements have no appreciable influence on pro-environmental behavior (Steg, 2005; Nisbet et al., 2009; Miliute-Plepiene et al., 2016; Liang et al., 2019; Kao and Du, 2020). These findings question the role of rational cognitive factors in pro-environmental behavior, leading scholars to shift the focus from rational variables to the role and mechanisms of emotional factors.

Emotions are closely associated with consumer behavior (Westbrook, 1987; Arnold and Reynolds, 2009; Troiville et al., 2019). However, there have been few empirical studies on the relationship between emotions and pro-environmental actions (Vining, 1987; Bissing-Olson et al., 2016), especially in the context of COVID-19. COVID-19 brought the world to a standstill, which is a testament to the frailty of globalization (El-Said and Aziz, 2022). The lockdowns due to COVID-19 have been linked to psychological distress, such as loneliness, unhappiness, depression, and anxiety (Zhou et al., 2021). Those experiencing negative emotional states such as loneliness and depression are prone to nostalgia (Wildschut et al., 2006). Unfortunately, the link between nostalgia and pro-environmental behavior is not well understood (Zhang et al., 2021). Studies have shown that nostalgia can assist in making socially beneficial decisions and achieving social goals. Compared to non-nostalgic charity appeals, nostalgic appeals are more likely to increase charitable giving, volunteering, and helping behavior (Merchant et al., 2011; Zhou et al., 2012). However, it is not clear whether nostalgia influences people's pro-environmental behavior, which is a subset of pro-social behavior at different levels. Moreover, little is known from past research about the moderators of the relationship between natural anthropomorphism and pro-environmental outcomes (Williams et al., 2021a). To fill these research gaps, this study was the first, to our knowledge, to use nostalgia as a moderator in a framework investigating the factors influencing pro-environmental behavior.

Nevertheless, encouraging people to connect emotionally with nature is not easy. Scholars have begun to analyze how anthropomorphism could be applied to establish a positive emotional connection with the environment. However, as anthropomorphism is embodied in different forms, its impact on participation in environmental behavior also differs (White et al., 2011). Few studies have explored the relationship between anthropomorphism and pro-environmental behavior during the COVID-19 pandemic (Williams et al., 2021a), let alone different forms of anthropomorphism. Previous scholars have divided message frames into positive and negative information frames (White et al., 2011), which are conditional phenomena and rarely work in isolation (Karpinska-Krakowiak et al., 2020). Meanwhile, the process of pro-environmental behaviors is difficult to explain in studies based on the TPB because external stimulus factors are not considered (Yue et al., 2020). Based on the above analysis, an integrated model (Figure 1) based on stimulus-organism-response (SOR), protection motivation, and message frame theories was built to explain the mechanisms influencing the pro-environmental behavior of residents during the COVID-19 pandemic.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Theoretical framework.

Protection motivation theory provides an alternative theoretical perspective for explaining environmental behavior (Bockarjova and Steg, 2014). It employs a broader range of predictor variables than the TPB, the norm activation model, and the value-belief-norm theory, enhancing our understanding of attitudes and behaviors driving pro-environmentalism, which can be used to encourage pro-environmental actions to mitigate environmental threats (Bockarjova and Steg, 2014). Mehrabian and Russell (1974) developed the SOR model of early psychology by introducing the concept of the “organism” to emphasize that the external environment influences individual behavior through changes in psychological perceptions. According to this model, various situational elements (stimuli) lead to cognitive and emotional psychological changes in the recipient (organism), which in turn facilitate behavioral responses. The message framework refers to the concept that the same message content, presented in different ways, will be understood differently by the recipient. Messages can be described in terms of negative or positive information frames. Negative information frames are descriptions of information that cause losses or negative effects to people. In our study, we combined negative framing and anthropomorphism as the external stimulus of SOR theory, which few studies have done before (Karpinska-Krakowiak et al., 2020), to investigate how the negative form of anthropomorpism affects pro-environmental behavior. In addition, we selected two core variables (perceived threat and perceived vulnerability) in protection motivation theory and environmental empathy and responsibility as consumers' cognitive and emotional responses to stimuli of negative framing and anthropomorphism. We then employed the variable of pro-environmental behavior as the final decision of the consumers. This is a pioneering study on the relationship between perceived threat, perceived vulnerability, environmental empathy, and environmental responsibility.

Overall, our work offers theoretical and applied insights. First, the more comprehensive model can provide better insight into the multifaceted motivations of individuals (Mehrabian and Russell, 1974). Second, numerous studies have examined the role of nostalgia in promoting prosocial behavior (Zhou et al., 2008, 2012; Routledge et al., 2011; Stephan et al., 2014; Juhl et al., 2020). Although pro-environmental behavior is a form of pro-social behavior (Ito and Li, 2019; Sun et al., 2021; Zelenski and Desrochers, 2021), scholars have not paid sufficient attention to the relationship between nostalgia and pro-environmental behavior (Zhang et al., 2021). To our knowledge, this is the first pro-environmental behavior study to consider nostalgia as a moderator variable in a research model. We investigated whether different levels of nostalgia affect consumer behavior regarding environmental issues. Third, this study investigates the underlying mechanisms and develops a comprehensive framework for understanding how negative anthropomorphic message framing (NAMF) influences pro-environmental behavior. Finally, most previous studies employed structural equation modeling (SEM; CB-SEM and partial least squares (PLS)-SEM), which is useful for elucidating the net effects of variables, but they do not address the complexity of causal relationships that exist asymmetrically in the real world (Tho, 2019). Therefore, PLS-SEM and necessary condition analysis (NCA) were both used in this study to identify and validate the variables' sufficient and necessary conditions.

Theoretical background and hypotheses development

Anthropomorphism, message frame, and pro-environmental behavior

Anthropomorphism refers to the application of human physical traits, motives, and emotional inclinations to non-human objects (Mota-Rojas et al., 2021). Anthropomorphism has a broad variety of marketing uses and studies have examined how anthropomorphism influences individuals' behavior. Anthropomorphism has a positive effect on customer preferences, engagement, and purchase intentions (Kim et al., 2016; Schroll et al., 2018). A study of anthropomorphism and its relationship to nature demonstrated a link between anthropomorphism and environmental concerns (Borovik and Pensini, 2022). Minimizing social distance and fostering social connections is critical for assisting people in engaging in pro-environmental behaviors (Schultz, 2002). Research has shown that anthropomorphizing nature can enhance people's sense of connection to it and encourage sustainabile behaviors (Tam, 2015b; Williams et al., 2021a,b). Thus, anthropomorphism may encourage prosocial behavior and increase a person's desire to purchase green products. Indeed, those who anthropomorphize nature are more likely to engage in social connection (Tam et al., 2013) and empathy (Tam, 2013b, 2015a) with the environment.

The message frame effect, also known as the ornamental effect, refers to the various ways in which the same information can be presented to elicit various interpretations by the information's receiver (Tversky and Kahneman, 1985). A message frame can be positive or negative. While message frames are crucial for consumer behavioral choices (Levin et al., 1988), no scholarly consensus exists over which information frameworks are more successful in enhancing pro-environmental behavior. Message framing is a conditional phenomenon and seldom operates in isolation (Olsen et al., 2014; Amatulli et al., 2019). Therefore, it is unclear whether anthropomorphism, when combined with negative message framing, impacts consumers' environmental behaviors and the possible mechanism of this impact. To address this, we used a multi-theoretical framework and two complementary approaches, PLS-SEM and NCA, to investigate the impact of NAMF on pro-environmental behavior.

Hypotheses development

NAMF

Message frameworks are crucial for consumers' behavioral choices (Levin et al., 1988). Negative message frames are used to describe messages that result in losses or negative consequences for people (Krishnamurthy et al., 2001; Gerend and Cullen, 2008) and trigger the recipient's expected feelings of shame and guilt (Amatulli et al., 2019). A negative framing emphasizing negative environmental implications increased customer desire to utilize environmentally friendly biofuels more than access-based or hybrid-based frameworks (Moon et al., 2016). Anthropomorphism relates environmental things to humans, increasing the similarities between individuals and environmental topics, making the perceived psychological distance smaller (Chen et al., 2020). According to interpretive-level theory, a closer psychological distance tends to elicit a lower level of consumer interpretation and enables people to feel how nature is being violated (Zhang, 2014).

It is possible to trigger active behaviors by activating adaptive biases, including anthropomorphism and an innate tendency to respond to negative contexts (Haselton and Nettle, 2006; Haselton et al., 2015). Positive and negative messages increase the vividness of the information and elicit a stronger emotional response (Karpinska-Krakowiak et al., 2020; Kusmanoff et al., 2020). Negative target messages may be more effective than positive target messages because they generate negative emotions (Witte, 1992; Chang and Wu, 2015), such as fear, anger, and vulnerability, which are perceived as a threat by the individual. According to The Khoa et al. (2021), who developed a framework for negative anthropomorphic messages, consumers may be persuaded to maintain social distancing using anthropomorphic persuasion, such as by replacing the word coronavirus with “the deadly Mr. COVID-19” or showing terrifying images of the virus.

Anthropomorphism is intrinsically tied to human sociocognitive development (Epley et al., 2007). If people care about the environment, their emotions and perceptions must be aligned with human emotions and perceptions (Clayton et al., 2011). As a general rule, people are more likely to accept the behavior of others who are similar to themselves (Bandura, 2001) and may require some degree of anthropomorphism to achieve identification and empathy for non-human subjects (Tam et al., 2013; Prguda and Neumann, 2014). It has been demonstrated that when humans anthropomorphize nature, their sense of connection to it improves (Tam et al., 2013), generating a strong natural empathy (Tam, 2013b, 2015a) which increases conservation behavior (Tam et al., 2013; Liu et al., 2019). Therefore, we propose the following hypotheses:

  • H1a. Negative anthropomorphic message framing has a positive influence on perceived vulnerability.

  • H1b. Negative anthropomorphic message framing has a positive influence on environmental empathy.

  • H1c. Negative anthropomorphic message framing has a positive influence on perceived threats.

  • H1d. Negative anthropomorphic message framing has a positive influence on pro-environmental behavior.

Perceived vulnerability and perceived threats

Perceived vulnerability refers to how people feel in the face of adverse circumstances. Empathy refers to the ability of a person to feel, understand, and even share the feelings of others (Escalas and Stern, 2003; Marjanovic et al., 2012; Rohani et al., 2018). A behavioral immune system more sensitive to environmental threats or changes is found in more vulnerable individuals (Tam, 2013a). Consequently, these individuals feel compelled to invest more effort into protecting the environment to reduce their chances of exposure to environmental hazards (Jiang et al., 2021). As vulnerability is recognized as a condition of impairment, more responsibility must be extended to those who are especially vulnerable (Gross and McGoey, 2015). People who are aware of the vulnerability of the environment feel responsible for it and choose to make environmentally friendly purchases (Channa et al., 2022). Related research has found that people feel more responsible for helping those they perceive to be as vulnerable (Back and Lips, 1998; Chasteen and Madey, 2003; Moche and Västfjäll, 2021). When environmental subjects are anthropomorphized, people will treat the environment in the same way as they treat people, encouraging people to actively engage in pro-environmental behavior. Previous research has examined how farmers' adaptation decisions are influenced by psychological factors, such as perceived vulnerability and capacity (Wang Y. et al., 2019). They found that perceived vulnerability influenced Chinese farmers' intention to adopt environmental practices. We conclude that individuals feel a sense of environmental responsibility and urge to protect the earth when they see a crying planet.

Perceived threat was developed based on a social psychology study of fear. It is a significant component affecting environmental behavior and is related to the degree to which people perceive damage from the external environment. According to protective motivation theory, people confront risks using two cognitive processes: threat assessment and reaction evaluation. In reaction to dangers, people employ protective behaviors depending on their threat assessment. Danger intensity is viewed as a trigger for protective behavior and a belief about the threat's outcome, and individuals behave positively when they believe the threat is controllable and unmanageable. Due to the anthropomorphization of environmental issues, individuals may see the controllability of the environment and engage in pro-environmental acts. Using a large sample of 27 nations, Oreg and Katz-Gerro (2006) illustrated that environmental concerns, perceived threats, and behavioral restrictions may all impact willingness to sacrifice and a variety of pro-environmental behaviors. In particular, perceived threats might reinforce pro-environmental behavior when pro-environmental standards become dominant (Fritsche et al., 2010). Environmental problems are more likely to be perceived as significant by individuals with a higher perception of environmental threats than those with a lower perception, motivating them to take part in pro-environmental behaviors (O'Connor et al., 1999). In addition, concern for nature is linked to an individual's underlying values and belief systems (Stern et al., 1995), meaning that they develop a feeling of empathy for nature when they feel a threat due to environmental problems (Berenguer, 2010). In summary, we propose the following hypotheses:

  • H2a. Perceived vulnerability positively influences environmental empathy.

  • H2b. Perceived vulnerability positively influences environmental responsibility.

  • H2c. Perceived vulnerability positively influences pro-environmental behavior.

  • H3a. Perceived threat positively influences environmental empathy.

  • H3b. Perceived threat positively influences environmental responsibility.

  • H3c. Perceived threat positively influences pro-environmental behavior.

Environmental empathy

Empathy for nature is the emotional experience of comprehending and sharing the natural world. It encompasses induced and character empathy (Tam, 2013b; Tam et al., 2013). According to the TPB, a person's emotions are critical in anticipating their behavioral intentions (Ajzen, 1991). Emotional reactions to environmental concerns and problems are conceptualized as persistent sentiments toward nature, especially when triggered by environmental challenges and crises (Liu and Lin, 2015). Empathy for creatures impacted by environmental difficulties has been shown to generate significant environmental attitudes and behaviors (Schultz P., 2000). In addition, the model of altruistic emotions also implies that the more empathy one has for nature, the more probable it is that one would sense nature's wellbeing, which may result in moral worries regarding nature conservation behavior (Batson, 2014). In summary, empathy for distressed natural beings may inspire concern for both the individual and the natural world and instill a sense of environmental responsibility in consumers. Thus, we propose the following hypothesis:

  • H4. Environmental empathy has a positive influence on environmental Responsibility.

Environmental responsibility

Environmental responsibility refers to an individual's sense of obligation to take action to address environmental issues or avoid the degradation of environmental quality and is the primary factor determining environmental behavior (Yue et al., 2020). According to the norm activation and value-belief-norm theories, environmental behavior implementation is contingent on the activation of individual norms that result in a sense of responsibility to adopt environmental behavior (Stern, 2000; Nordlund and Garvill, 2003). Individuals perceive environmental responsibility as a response to moral responsibility, which plays a key role in amplifying intrinsic motivation and reinforcing their willingness to act. Research has shown that by incorporating environmental responsibility into theoretical models of planned behavior, environmental behavioral intentions can be predicted more accurately (Hines et al., 1987; Achchuthan et al., 2017; Rezaei et al., 2019). Thus, we propose the following hypothesis:

  • H5. Environmental responsibility has a positive influence on pro-environmental behavior.

Nostalgia

Nostalgia is a complex, positive, self-relevant emotion that occurs when people reminisce about their past (Sedikides et al., 2015). Human behavior is heavily influenced by emotion (Vitell et al., 2013) and nostalgia has become an increasingly important psychological resource that enhances the sense of meaning in existence and promotes prosocial behavior among individuals. Nostalgia is positively correlated with prosocial behaviors (Lasaleta et al., 2014; Stephan et al., 2014). In addition, participants who viewed nostalgic advertisements were more willing to make charitable donations than those who viewed non-nostalgic advertisements and were more likely to volunteer and donate to charitable organizations (Zhou et al., 2012). Pro-environmental behavior is an important expression of pro-social behavior (Ramus and Killmer, 2007). However, few studies have examined how nostalgia affects consumers' pro-environmental behaviors. One study used structural equation modeling to examine how nostalgia affects tourists' pro-environmental behavior through subjective attitudes, perceived behavioral control, subjective norms, and meaning in life (Wu et al., 2020). Two studies revealed that personal nostalgia increases the likelihood of retaining and reusing products to protect nature (Wang et al., 2020; Zhang et al., 2021). Thus, we propose the following hypotheses:

  • H6a. Nostalgia positively moderates the relationship between perceived vulnerability and pro-environmental behavior.

  • H6b. Nostalgia positively moderates the relationship between environmental responsibility and pro-environmental behavior.

  • H6c. Nostalgia positively moderates the relationship between perceived threats and pro-environmental behavior.

  • H6d. Nostalgia positively moderates the relationship between negative anthropomorphic message framing and pro-environmental behavior.

Methodology

The hypotheses of this study were investigated using PLS-SEM and NCA, with PLS-SEM assessing the adequacy of the antecedent conditions and NCA and analyzing the necessity of the antecedent conditions (Farmaki et al., 2022; McLeay et al., 2022; Sharma et al., 2022a). In combination with NCA, PLS-SEM facilitates the assessment of a model's necessary logic (Dul, 2016; Richter et al., 2020). The combined research method of SEM and NCA may assist in establishing whether a determinant has an average effect and/or a necessary effect on the outcome, providing a thorough understanding of the causal relationships between variables (Lee and Jeong, 2021). In this study, individual indicator weights, along with measurement errors, were estimated using PLS-SEM and then utilized to calculate composite scores for the defined latent variables (Hair et al., 2017). Next, the scores from this process are utilized in the NCA (Richter et al., 2020).

PLS-SEM requires a relatively small sample size, does not require sample data to follow a normal distribution, and can handle both reflective and formative indicators, making it more suitable for exploratory studies (Hair et al., 2019). Moreover, it can handle more complex models (Chin, 2010), making it suitable for this study. Smart PLS 3.3.9 was employed for data analysis. In recent years, NCA has emerged as a method for analyzing the necessary conditions to facilitate a particular outcome and is used to complement more traditional regression methods that analyze sufficiency conditions (Dul et al., 2020). The presence or absence of antecedent conditions directly impacts an outcome and NCA allows for a quantitative analysis of the level of antecedent conditions required to reach an outcome variable (Dul, 2016). In our study, NCA techniques were used to complement the SEM approach, which is well suited to adequacy analysis and includes the analysis of effect size and bottleneck conditions.

Survey development and data collection procedure

Before conducting formal research, an experiment was conducted to test the validity of our manipulation of the negative frame. An experiment was conducted with 60 students at a university in central China which displayed two anthropomorphic images (Appendix 5): a positive anthropomorphic image of a smiling Earth and a negative anthropomorphic image of a tearful Earth. Each student was asked to answer four questions of positive and negative frames: “Do you think the campaign of protecting the Earth has a positive (negative) meaning” and “Do you think participating in protecting the Earth initiative will benefit you in any way?” (1 = strongly disagree; 5 = strongly agree). The results showed that the Cronbach's α of the positive frame was 0.741 and 0.828 for the negative frame, indicating that the questionnaire is credible. In addition, after reversing the results of the negative anthropomorphic message, we found a significant difference between the negative and positive frames [M negative frame = 4.333, M positive frame = 2.283, F(119) = 193.164, p < 0.001], demonstrating the success of our manipulation of the negative and positive frames. The subjects produced positive and negative emotions when they saw smiling earth and crying earth, respectively.

Next, to explore the research hypothesis, a survey was conducted for 1 week on WJX, an online survey platform that offers specialized and reliable services (Wang D. et al., 2019). We clarified the purpose of the study and emphasized the anonymity and confidentiality of the participants. In total, 412 electronic questionnaires were received and 32 responses were deleted after filtering for straight lining (Sharma et al., 2022b) and missing data. The demographic data of the respondents are presented in Table 1.

Table 1.

Subjects' demographics.

Measure Item Frequency Percent Cumulative percent
Gender Male 193 50.8 50.8
Female 187 49.2 100
Age Below 20 9 2.4 2.4
21–30 175 46.1 48.4
31–40 126 33.2 81.6
41–50 36 9.5 91.1
50 and over 34 8.9 100
Edu Junior high school or below 19 5 5
Senior high school 46 12.1 17.1
Junior college or university 188 49.5 66.6
Master's degree or PhD 127 33.4 100
Income/month (RMB) Less than 3,500 188 49.5 49.5
3,500–6,000 101 26.6 76.1
6,001–8,000 52 13.7 89.7
8,001–9,999 22 5.8 95.5
Over 10,000 17 4.5 100
Total 380 100

Measurement procedure

The variables in this study were assessed using a variety of items derived from existing scales in previous research. The items in this research were measured using a five-point Likert scale (1 = greatly inaccurate, 5 = highly accurate). The questionnaire consisted of three main sections. In the first part, we presented a picture of an anthropomorphic and weeping Earth (Appendix 5), which was the material used in the experiment before the formal study to evoke negative emotions among the participants. In the second part, respondents' demographic information was provided. Based on existing measurement scales validated in previous research, in the third part, we presented seven variables: NAMF (Aggarwal and McGill, 2007), perceived vulnerability (Workman et al., 2008; Shafiei and Maleksaeidi, 2020), perceived threat (Liang and Xue, 2009), environmental empathy (Tam, 2013b), environmental responsibility (Stone et al., 1995), pro-environmental behavior (Stern, 2000), and nostalgia (Hepper et al., 2012).

Results

Common method bias, social desirability bias, and non-response bias

To test the questionnaire's validity and quality, three types of bias were analyzed: common method bias, social desirability bias, and non-response bias. To reduce the impact of common method bias, the English scales derived from foreign literature were back-translated and repeatedly compared and corrected, thus reducing the possibility of errors caused by ambiguity in the language. In addition, Harman's single-factor analysis was performed and seven variables were used to ensure that the collected data did not have common method bias. In this study, a single factor was constructed by combining these seven factors. The results indicated that the newly formed factor explains 31.448% of the variation, which is less than the 40% requirement (Babin et al., 2016). Hence, the data collected did not indicate a concern regarding common method bias.

Social desirability bias and non-response bias are common problems in survey research using convenient sampling methods. We found no significant differences between data collected in the pre- and post-periods of this study for either demographic or latent variables, demonstrating that non-response bias was not a significant problem in our study (Armstrong and Overton, 1977). Regarding social desirability bias, first, the survey was completely anonymous so that respondents could express their innermost thoughts, and we made it clear at the outset that their personal information would not be compromised to avoid social desirability bias (Konrad and Linnehan, 1995; Wei, 2021). Second, according to previous research (Park et al., 2019), all scales in our study were derived from high-quality studies to mitigate the social desirability bias. Third, regarding pro-environmental behavior, previous studies have indicated that the social desirability bias does not play a significant role (Kaiser, 1998; Milfont, 2009). Finally, in light of the data description of the measurement items (Appendix 2), there was no significant deviation in the values of the variables, indicating that social desirability bias may not pose a problem in this study (Sethi and Sethi, 2009).

Assessment of the measurement model

To determine the convergent and discriminant validity of the measurement model, we used conventional criteria. First, average variance extracted (AVE) scores (>0.5) and outer loadings (>0.708) were used to test convergent validity (Hair et al., 2013). The findings in Table 2 show that all items were above the acceptable value, suggesting that this study attained convergent validity. Second, according to Hair et al. (2021), Cronbach's alpha, composite reliability (CR), and rho_A were used to assess the reliability of the measures. The three values were all above 0.7, the accepted cutoff point, demonstrating the reliability of the scale (Table 2).

Table 2.

Reliability and convergent validity.

Construct Item Loading VIF Cronbach's alpha rho_A CR AVE
EE EE1 0.884 2.345 0.874 0.900 0.913 0.726
EE2 0.878 2.567
EE3 0.889 2.699
EE4 0.748 1.650
ER ER1 0.917 2.445 0.818 0.832 0.892 0.735
ER2 0.829 1.772
ER3 0.822 1.800
NAMF NAMF1 0.865 2.003 0.793 0.816 0.866 0.618
NAMF2 0.820 1.747
NAMF3 0.743 1.588
NAMF4 0.707 1.567
NO NO1 0.715 1.941 0.879 0.946 0.906 0.661
NO2 0.753 2.255
NO3 0.899 3.012
NO4 0.881 2.462
NO5 0.802 1.926
PEB PEB1 0.827 2.142 0.882 0.885 0.911 0.630
PEB2 0.823 2.445
PEB3 0.743 2.091
PEB4 0.804 2.169
PEB5 0.783 2.087
PEB6 0.780 1.925
PT PT1 0.879 1.927 0.807 0.813 0.886 0.722
PT2 0.813 1.580
PT3 0.855 1.874
PV PV1 0.871 2.746 0.889 0.890 0.923 0.750
PV2 0.860 2.703
PV3 0.864 2.698
PV4 0.867 2.719

NAMF, negative anthropomorphic message framing; PV, perceived vulnerability; PT, perceived threat; EE, environmental empathy; ER, environmental responsibility; PEB, pro-environmental behavior; NO, nostalgia.

In addition, the Fornell-Larcker criterion (Fornell and Larcker, 1981) and the heterotrait-monotrait correlation ratios (Henseler et al., 2015) were used to test the discriminant validity of the model. It is essential to achieve discriminant validity of the construct because it differentiates constructs and measures distinct concepts (Fornell and Larcker, 1981). The Fornell-Larcker criteria are based on the standardized outer loadings of each construct and the correlation between each latent variable and all other constructs. The square root of the extracted average variance must be larger than the component correlation coefficient (Table 3). The HTMT refers to the ratio of correlations between variables to correlations within variables, and an HTMT value greater than HTMT 0.85 (Henseler et al., 2015), or HTMT 0.90 (Gold et al., 2001) indicates a problem with discriminant validity. As shown in Table 4, the HTMT test resulted in values that met the HTMT 0.85 and HTMT 0.90 requirements, showing that the measurement model was determined to have sufficient discriminant validity. The results of the multicollinearity indicator with the variance inflation factor (VIF) are presented in Table 2. VIF values of less than three were considered ideal (Sarstedt et al., 2019). Our fit validity test indicated that the SRMR was 0.063, which is less than the recommended maximum of 0.08 (Hair et al., 2021). This indicated a good fit for the model. We also used goodness of fit (GOF) to evaluate the model fit of our study (Tenenhaus et al., 2005). Quality assessment of the research framework was performed as follows.

Table 3.

Fornell and larcker.

EE ER NAMF NO PEB PT PV
EE 0.852
ER 0.538 0.857
NAMF 0.504 0.387 0.786
NO 0.086 0.092 0.248 0.813
PEB 0.356 0.468 0.614 0.274 0.794
PT 0.545 0.460 0.578 0.116 0.573 0.850
PV 0.382 0.455 0.382 −0.027 0.234 0.307 0.866

The diagonal numbers (bold values) are the square root of each variable AVE.

Table 4.

HTMT.

EE ER NAMF NO PEB PT PV
EE
ER 0.617
NAMF 0.592 0.472
NO 0.098 0.123 0.248
PEB 0.400 0.545 0.724 0.268
PT 0.635 0.565 0.715 0.143 0.677
PV 0.417 0.533 0.445 0.065 0.266 0.363
GOF=AVE¯ x R2¯=0.692 x 0.351=0.493

Considering the above results, the GOF was 0.493, which satisfies the 0.40 cut-off requirements for a substantial impact size (Wetzels et al., 2009). Compared with recent studies that used SRMR (Petrescu-Mag et al., 2021, 2022) and GOF (Asghar et al., 2021; Kopaei et al., 2021; Rastegari Kopaei et al., 2021) values to test the model fit, the model fit measurements of both SRMR and GOF in our study showed that our model is appropriate.

Assessment of the structural model

Collinearity problems in the dataset may lead to distortion of the regression results. VIF was used to assess collinearity. To avoid collinearity problems, the VIF values should not exceed three (Hair et al., 2019). According to Table 5, the VIF values of all variables (1.187–1.728) fulfilled this criterion, indicating that collinearity was not a concern. In total of 5000 subsamples were utilized in a bootstrap technique to determine the significance of the route coefficients. The structural model is illustrated in Figure 2 and Table 5 shows that NAMF has a significant positive effect on perceived vulnerability (β = 0.382, t-value = 8.096), environmental empathy (β = 0.227, t-value = 4.287), perceived threat (β = 0.578, t-value = 15.961), and pro-environmental behavior (β = 0.313, t-value = 5.583). Thus, H1a, H1b, H1c, and H1d were supported. Perceived vulnerability was found to significantly increase environmental empathy (β = 0.186, t-value = 3.871) and environmental responsibility (β = 0.578, t-value = 15.961) but did not significantly affect pro-environmental behavior (β = −0.068, t-value = 1.575). Therefore, H2a and H2b are supported and H2c is rejected. We also found that perceived threat had a positive effect on environmental empathy (β = 0.356, t-value = 7.435), environmental responsibility (β = 0.199, t-value = 3.721), and pro-environmental behavior (β = 0.252, t-value = 4.889), supporting H3a, H3b, and H3c. Additionally, environmental empathy demonstrated a significant positive effect on environmental responsibility (β = 0.356, t-value = 7.435), while environmental responsibility also had a significant effect on pro-environmental behavior (β = 0.207, t-value = 3.994), thus supporting H4 and H5.

Table 5.

Assessment of structural model.

Hypothesis and path Coefficiet Std T values P values f2 VIF Result
ER -> PEB 0.207 0.052 3.994 *** 0.073 1.484 Supported
NAMF -> PEB 0.313 0.056 5.583 *** 0.165 1.728 Supported
NO -> PEB 0.116 0.037 3.152 0.002 0.030 1.089 Supported
PT -> PEB 0.252 0.052 4.889 *** 0.083 1.669 Supported
PV -> PEB −0.068 0.043 1.575 0.115 0.013 1.374 Not Supported
PEB; R2 = 0.497; Q2 predict = 0.309
NAMF -> EE 0.227 0.053 4.287 *** 0.052 1.615 Supported
PT -> EE 0.356 0.048 7.435 *** 0.134 1.523 Supported
PV -> EE 0.186 0.048 3.871 *** 0.047 1.187 Supported
EE; R2 = 0.375; Q2 predict=0.266
EE -> ER 0.328 0.049 6.617 *** 0.115 1.533 Supported
PT -> ER 0.199 0.053 3.721 *** 0.045 1.446 Supported
PV -> ER 0.268 0.048 5.631 *** 0.099 1.190 Supported
ER; R2 = 0.385; Q2 predict = 0.280
NAMF -> PT 0.578 0.036 15.961 *** 0.503 Supported
PT; R2 = 0.333; Q2 predict = 0.237
NAMF -> PV 0.382 0.047 8.096 *** 0.171 Supported
PV; R2 = 0.144; Q2 predict = 0.106
PV*NO -> PEB −0.009 0.044 0.212 0.832 Not Supported
NAMF*NO -> PEB 0.151 0.059 2.571 0.010 Supported
PT*NO -> PEB −0.028 0.045 0.626 0.531 Not Supported
ER*NO -> PEB 0.128 0.053 2.427 0.015 Supported
*

p < 0.05,

**

p < 0.01, and

***

p < 0.001.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Inner model results. ***p-value < 0.001, **p-value < 0.01, and *p-value < 0.05.

In addition, using the Smart-PLS algorithm function, structural model assessments were used to calculate the coefficient of determination (R2), which evaluates the explained variance. Four endogenous constructs were used: pro-environmental behavior, environmental empathy, environmental responsibility, perceived threat, and perceived vulnerability. The results in Table 5 reveal that the R2 values for the five endogenous constructs were 0.497, 0.375, 0.385, 0.333, and 0.144, respectively. Simultaneously, Stone-Geisser Q2 was calculated to assess the predictive value of endogenous variables. The findings in Table 5 show that the Q2 values for pro-environmental behavior (0.309), environmental empathy (0.266), environmental responsibility (0.280), perceived threat (0.237), and perceived vulnerability (0.106) were all above zero (Shmueli et al., 2016). To analyze the statistical power of our data, we calculated the effect size using f2. An f2 value less than or equal to 0.02 indicates a weak small relationship. If the value of the independent variable is larger than 0.15 and less than 0.35, then the relationship's moderate or medium strength may be assessed. If the independent variable had an effect greater than 0.35, the relationship was considered strong (Hair et al., 2019). As shown in Table 5, NAMF, perceived threat, and environmental empathy had the largest f2 values for pro-environmental behavior, environmental empathy, and environmental responsibility, respectively. The f2 values from NAMF to perceived threat and perceived vulnerability were 0.503, 0.171.

An approach based on product indicators was used in Smart PLS to assess moderating effects (Henseler and Fassott, 2010). The results of the moderating effect tests are presented in Table 5. This study highlights the significant effects of the interaction between NAMF and nostalgia (NAMF*nostalgia) on pro-environmental behavior (β = 0.151, t-value = 2.571) and the interaction between environmental responsibility and nostalgia (environmental responsibility*nostalgia) on pro-environmental behavior (β = 0.128, t-value = 2.427), thereby supporting H6b and H6d. Figures 3, 4 show the positive moderating effects of NAMF and environmental responsibility on pro-environmental behavior. Unexpectedly, H6a and H6c were not supported.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Moderating effect (ER*NO-PEB). ER, environmental responsibility; PEB, pro-environmental behavior; NO, nostalgia.

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Moderating effect (NAMF*NO-PEB). NAMF, negative anthropomorphic message framing; PEB, pro-environmental behavior; NO, nostalgia.

NCA result

NCA involves analyzing and interpreting necessary condition hypotheses using three components: a scatterplot, an effect size, and a bottleneck table (Toth et al., 2019). Instead of assessing average correlations, an NCA analysis displays regions in the scatter plots of dependent and independent variables, which might indicate the existence of a required condition (Dul, 2016; Richter et al., 2020). This study employed the NCA package in the R environment to perform NCA analysis (Dul, 2021). NCA was conducted using the latent variable scores obtained by PLS-SEM as a starting point (Richter et al., 2020). We attempted to determine whether NAMF, environmental empathy, environmental responsibility, nostalgia, perceived threat, and perceived vulnerability were necessary conditions for pro-environmental behavior. Figure 5 shows the scatter plots for all relevant relations.

Figure 5.

Figure 5

Scatter plots of necessary condition analysis.

We then used the CE-FDH line to avoid further linear assumptions, which is a non-decreasing step function derived from the scatterplot between the predictors and outcomes (Dul, 2016, 2021). In addition to having discrete data that covered a relatively narrow range and exhibited a limited number of levels, the CE-FDH ceiling line was justified (Dul, 2021). In addition, the parameters of the CE-FDH line from the NCA findings were employed for interpretation, as this line is acceptable for Likert-scale survey data (Vis and Dul, 2018). Thus, we were able to identify the extent to which each independent variable attribute limits overall pro-environmental behavior by separating the space containing observations from that containing no observations (Sukhov et al., 2022). Accordingly, the results in Table 6 and Figure 6 meaningfully (d ≥ 0.1) and significantly (p < 0.05) reveal that NAMF (d = 0.108, p < 0.001) and perceived threat (d = 0.209, p < 0.001) are necessary conditions for generating pro-environmental behavior. However, other variables had a small impact size and did not have a substantial influence on pro-environmental conduct (Dul, 2016).

Table 6.

Necessary condition analysis result (method: CE-FDH).

Construct Ceiling zone Scope Effect size (d) p values Conditional inefficiency (%) Outcome inefficiency (%)
EE 0.659 1.360 0.043 0.289 81.381 75.006
ER 1.483 15.643 0.095 *** 26.575 66.328
NAMF 1.912 17.631 0.108 *** 49.988 71.033
NO 0.992 15.922 0.062 ** 44.701 79.026
PT 3.782 18.066 0.209 *** 50 40.648
PV 0.518 16.451 0.031 0.356 87.352 75.006

0 < d <0.15 small effect size; 0.1 ≤ d <0.3 medium effect size; 0.3 ≤ d <0.5 large effect size; d ≥ 0.5 very large effect size.

*

p < 0.05,

**

p < 0.01, and

***

p < 0.001.

Figure 6.

Figure 6

Histograms with bottleneck values.

Finally, bottleneck tables were used to examine the ceiling line from a unique viewpoint, demonstrating which criteria and what level must be satisfied to attain the desired output level. Specifically, the bottleneck table of the NCA (Table 7) presents the threshold (critical) values for NAMF, environmental empathy, environmental responsibility, nostalgia, perceived threat, and perceived vulnerability to achieve the desired level of pro-environmental behavior value. The level of perceived threat should not fall below 6.9% to maintain a medium level of pro-environmental behavior (>40%). Similarly, to achieve a high level of pro-environmental behavior (80%), at least 5.6% of environmental empathy, 16.5% of environmental responsibility, 12.1% of NAMF, 39.6% of perceived threat, and 4.4% of perceived vulnerability must occur.

Table 7.

Bottleneck Table (CE-FDH).

PEB EE ER NAMF NO PT PV
0 NN NN NN NN NN NN
10 NN NN NN NN NN NN
20 NN NN NN NN NN NN
30 NN NN NN NN NN NN
40 NN NN NN NN 6.9 NN
50 NN NN NN NN 15.1 NN
60 NN NN NN NN 23.3 NN
70 NN NN NN NN 31.4 NN
80 5.6 16.5 12.1 NN 39.6 4.4
90 14.1 41.9 30.8 20.6 47.8 12.8
100 22.6 67.3 49.4 50.6 56 21.2

Discussion

Theoretical contributions

This study integrated protection motivation theory, the SOR framework, and message framing theory at the same time, combining NAMF, environmental empathy, environmental responsibility, perceived threat, perceived vulnerability, and nostalgia into the model to evaluate how these variables affect pro-environmental behavior. The previous classification of anthropomorphic research was mainly based on the differentiation of social relationships (Karpinska-Krakowiak et al., 2020). This study integrated the framing effect and anthropomorphism, distinguished between positive and negative anthropomorphic message frames, and discussed them from the perspective of a negative message frame, enriching the theory of environmental behavior research and broadening its application. It is vital to understand people's behavioral mechanisms to motivate them to make sustainable decisions. Moreover, most previous studies used structural equation modeling and ordinary least squares regression techniques to analyze the factors that influence pro-environmental behavior. We used both sufficiency and necessity logic in research on pro-environmental behavior. PLS-SEM and NCA analyses revealed that the predictors of pro-environmental behavior were statistically significant. In addition, each predictor had varying degrees of necessity and net effects.

Discussion of results

According to the PLS-SEM results, NAMF had a positive effect on environmental empathy and pro-environmental behavior. These results are in line with previous research (Tam et al., 2013; Chang and Wu, 2015; Moon et al., 2016; Amatulli et al., 2019; Liu et al., 2019; Tam, 2019; Karpinska-Krakowiak et al., 2020; Williams et al., 2021a; Borovik and Pensini, 2022). When anthropomorphism is coupled with negative information, empathy is triggered (Slovic, 2010) and researchers have found that when negative information is paired with anthropomorphic cues, they are more effective in inspiring green intentions (Karpinska-Krakowiak et al., 2020). Additionally, it was significantly associated with perceived threats and vulnerability. Interestingly, of the five direct relationships examined, NAMF was the strongest significant predictor of pro-environmental behavior. Consumers who receive negative anthropomorphic stimuli feel distressed and sympathetic toward the environment. They are usually in a negative mood, making them more sensitive to environmental issues and more determined to take steps to improve them. In addition, empathy for the environment leads to greater environmental responsibility, consistent with previous studies (Schultz P. W., 2000; Berenguer, 2007; Luchs and Mooradian, 2012; Stevenson et al., 2014). Higher levels of connection with nature are associated with increased prosocial behaviors and more sympathetic care for the biosphere and its inhabitants (Shelton and Rogers, 1981; Berenguer, 2007). Increased emotional empathy for nature increases a person's likelihood of acting properly; as a result, their love for nature is likely to grow (Boeve-de Pauw and Van Petegem, 2011). Empathy for nature is seen as a precondition for environmentally responsible behavior and has been linked to good environmental attitudes and knowledge (Chawla, 1998). Moreover, according to previous research, empathy is malleable and can be cultivated for future generations, which can lead to a variety of pro-environmental outcomes, such as greater engagement with environmental issues and pro-environmental behavioral intentions (Batson et al., 2003). Furthermore, perceived threat and perceived vulnerability were found to have a significant relationship with environmental empathy and environmental responsibility, respectively, relationships that have not been explored. In addition, perceived threat has a substantial influence on pro-environmental behavior, which is consistent with earlier results demonstrating the relationship between people's views of the threat posed by climate change and their desire to participate in pro-environmental activities (Grothmann and Patt, 2005; Spence et al., 2011; Bockarjova and Steg, 2014; Rainear and Christensen, 2017; Lim and Moon, 2020). Unexpectedly, the relationship between perceived vulnerability and pro-environmental factors was not significant, which is inconsistent with previous studies (Keshavarz and Karami, 2016; Bubeck et al., 2018) but in line with the research of Shafiei and Maleksaeidi (2020). In other words, higher perceived vulnerability did not affect individuals' perceptions of pro-environmental behaviors in this study. Not surprisingly, environmental responsibility positively influenced pro-environmental behavior, corroborating previous findings (Kaiser and Shimoda, 1999; Dermody et al., 2018; Han et al., 2020; Janmaimool and Khajohnmanee, 2020; Ahmed et al., 2021). Because environmental behavior benefits nature, other people, and society, it is likely to entail some cost to the actor, such as time, money, or energy. When a person is faced with a choice between personal and social interests, the internalized sense of environmental responsibility will be reawakened, leading the individual to engage in environmental behavior. We believe that consumers with a high sense of environmental responsibility will recognize their relationship with the environment as a duty, consider their obligation to solve environmental problems, and be more likely to adopt pro-environmental behaviors than consumers with a low sense of environmental responsibility. Finally, nostalgia was found to be a significant moderator and predictor of pro-environmental behavior. This implies that nostalgia augments the effects of environmental responsibility and negative anthropomorphic messages on pro-environmental behavior. It appears that nostalgia motivates pro-environmentalists (Rees et al., 2015). In general, the results of this study are consistent with those found in the literature regarding the effects of nostalgia on pro-environmental behavior. For example, Wu et al. (2020) added nostalgia to the framework of the TPB and found that nostalgia had a significant effect on pro-environmental behaviors through meaning in life and other core variables. Similarly, Zhang et al. (2021) focused on recycling behavior, which is a type of pro-environmental behavior, and found that nostalgia enhanced the intention to perform more environmentally friendly activities.

The NCA results revealed that NAMF (d = 0.108, p < 0.001) and perceived threat (d = 0.209, p < 0.001) were key factors of pro-environmental behavior. In addition, to receive a high level of pro-environmental behavior (>80%), NAMF (12.1%) and perceived threat (39.6%) are required, which means that marketers will not receive additional benefits above this level. Overall, incorporating the results of both PLS-SEM and NCA, we can conclude that NAMF and perceived threat are both necessary factors of pro-environmental behavior. Overall, the combined use of PLS-SEM and NCA provided more comprehensive knowledge of the sufficient and required antecedent factors for pro-environmental behavior.

Managerial implications

The results of our study had several managerial implications: first, differentiated marketing strategies should be implemented for marketers. The findings of this study confirm the significant moderating effects of nostalgia on the relational structures of negative anthropomorphic message frames, perceived threats, and pro-environmental behavior, which marketers can use to develop differentiated management decisions. Individuals with a strong sense of nostalgia are more likely to trigger strong pro-environmental behaviors in their scheduled life when they are at the same level of environmental responsibility or perceive the same level of negative anthropomorphic external stimuli, whereas individuals with low levels of nostalgia have the exact opposite. Nostalgia marketing is a popular marketing method that uses nostalgic elements to encourage people to actively engage in pro-environmental behaviors; it is also a marketing method that is currently popular and well received by consumers. Therefore, marketing and services need to be targeted toward the masses with different nostalgic tendencies. For instance, marketers can identify individuals with low nostalgic tendencies and create a comprehensive nostalgic atmosphere by appealing to visual, auditory, and olfactory senses through social media green advertising and other methods to trigger individuals' nostalgic emotions through different sensory stimulations to further enhance their nostalgic memories. While promoting the pro-environmental behavior of individuals with a strong nostalgia tendency, we can increase nostalgia propaganda and dedicate more high-quality green nostalgia marketing content through precision social media marketing.

In addition, consumers should be aware of their environmental responsibilities. The government can use media, such as television and the internet, to disseminate general environmental knowledge to consumers and provide environmental training. This will enable them to understand the dichotomy between humans and the environment and trigger their emotional response to environmental issues. Furthermore, the government can also focus on environmental issues that directly affect consumers, such as water pollution, haze, and inform on them in a variety of ways, to make everyone aware that environmental issues need to be addressed, thus enhancing consumers' sense of urgency to protect the environment and inspiring a sense of responsibility. Finally, some consumers are not as informed or concerned about the environment as others (Kronrod et al., 2012). Therefore, persuading consumers to adopt environmentally friendly behaviors is never easy and often requires a high investment of time and money with modest returns. In the future, governments and marketers may combine negative message framing with anthropomorphic messages in green advertising or marketing campaigns, especially in China. Confucian culture heavily influences Chinese values and behavioral patterns and China is a country with a high-power distance where everyone subconsciously seeks their place within society. Centralistic governance and values have led Chinese consumers to attribute environmental problems to the government (De Silva et al., 2021). Thus, anthropomorphing nature can be empathetic and negative message framing makes consumers feel that what they are doing is important and urgent, which would greatly enhance the persuasive power of green advertising and marketing campaigns.

Limitations and future research directions

Although this study contributed to the theoretical, methodological, and managerial domains, it had several limitations. First, as part of the research, we divided the message frames into positive and negative aspects; however, to focus this study, we only utilized negative message frames with anthropomorphism and examined the mechanisms that influenced pro-environmental behavior. Future research should focus on investigating the similarities and differences between negative and positive anthropomorphic message frames and whether positive anthropomorphism also evokes empathy for nature, environmental responsibility, perceived threat, and perceived vulnerability, which in turn affects consumers' pro-environmental behavior.

Second, we used the survey method, self-reported data, and a correlational design with inherent limitations that impose measurement error issues, social desirability bias, and a lack of causal conditions (Sharma et al., 2022a). Multiple research approaches should be combined in future studies to produce highly robust results. In addition, the research conclusion is one-sided as it only examines a sample of Chinese consumers and does not compare and analyze the different characteristics of different consumer groups in the process of green consumption under multicultural conditions. Future research should explore the path that motivates pro-environmental behavior by including samples from different cultural groups.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Author contributions

SZ: conceptualization, methodology, data curation, writing—original draft, figure production, writing—review, and editing. YW: figure production, writing—review, and editing. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.977381/full#supplementary-material

References

  1. Achchuthan S., Umanakenan R., Kajenthiran K. (2017). Comprehensive model for understanding and enhancing pro-environmental purchase behavior: towards a conceptual framework. J. Bus. Stud. 4, 1–24. 10.4038/jbs.v4i1.9 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  2. Aggarwal P., McGill A. L. (2007). Is that car smiling at me? Schema congruity as a basis for evaluating anthropomorphized products. J. Consum. Res. 34, 468–479. 10.1086/518544 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  3. Ahmed U., Umrani W. A., Yousaf A., Siddiqui M. A., Pahi M. H. (2021). Developing faithful stewardship for environment through green HRM. Int. J. Contemp. Hospital. Manage. 33, 3115–3133. 10.1108/IJCHM-09-2020-1066 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  4. Ajzen I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 50, 179–211. 10.1016/0749-5978(91)90020-T [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  5. Alzubaidi H., Slade E. L., Dwivedi Y. K. (2021). Examining antecedents of consumers' pro-environmental behaviours: TPB extended with materialism and innovativeness. J. Bus. Res. 122, 685–699. 10.1016/j.jbusres.2020.01.017 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  6. Amatulli C., De Angelis M., Peluso A. M., Soscia I., Guido G. (2019). The effect of negative message framing on green consumption: an investigation of the role of shame. J. Bus. Ethics 157, 1111–1132. 10.1007/s10551-017-3644-x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  7. Armstrong J. S., Overton T. S. (1977). Estimating nonresponse bias in mail surveys. J. Market. Res. 14, 396–402. 10.1177/002224377701400320 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  8. Arnold M. J., Reynolds K. E. (2009). Affect and retail shopping behavior: Understanding the role of mood regulation and regulatory focus. J. Retail. 85, 308–320. 10.1016/j.jretai.2009.05.004 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  9. Asghar M. Z., Iqbal A., Seitamaa-Hakkarainen P., Barbera E. (2021). Breaching learners' social distancing through social media during the COVID-19 Pandemic. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18, 11012. 10.3390/ijerph182111012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Babin B. J., Griffin M., Hair J. F. (2016). Heresies and sacred cows in scholarly marketing publications. J. Bus. Res. 69, 3133–8. 10.1016/j.jbusres.2015.12.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  11. Back S., Lips H. M. (1998). Child sexual abuse: Victim age, victim gender, and observer gender as factors contributing to attributions of responsibility. Child Abuse Neglect. 22, 1239–1252. 10.1016/S0145-2134(98)00098-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Bandura A. (2001). Social cognitive theory of mass communication. Media Psychol. 3, 265–299. 10.1207/S1532785XMEP0303_03 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  13. Batson C. D. (2014). The Altruism Question: Toward a Social-Psychological Answer. New York, NY: Psychology Press. 10.4324/9781315808048 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  14. Batson C. D., Lishner D. A., Carpenter A., Dulin L., Harjusola-Webb S., Stocks E. L., et al. (2003). As you would have them do unto you”: does imagining yourself in the other's place stimulate moral action? Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 29, 1190–1201. 10.1177/0146167203254600 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Berenguer J. (2007). The effect of empathy in proenvironmental attitudes and behaviors. Environ. Behav. 39, 269–283. 10.1177/0013916506292937 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  16. Berenguer J. (2010). The effect of empathy in environmental moral reasoning. Environ. Behav. 42, 110–134. 10.1177/0013916508325892 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  17. Bissing-Olson M. J., Fielding K. S., Iyer A. (2016). Experiences of pride, not guilt, predict pro-environmental behavior when pro-environmental descriptive norms are more positive. J. Environ. Psychol. 45, 145–153. 10.1016/j.jenvp.2016.01.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  18. Bockarjova M., Steg L. (2014). Can protection motivation theory predict pro-environmental behavior? explaining the adoption of electric vehicles in the Netherlands. Global Environ. Change 28, 276–288. 10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2014.06.010 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  19. Boeve-de Pauw J., Van Petegem P. (2011). The effect of Flemish eco-schools on student environmental knowledge, attitudes, and affect. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 33, 1513–1538. 10.1080/09500693.2010.540725 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  20. Borovik K., Pensini P. (2022). Be good to your mother (Earth): the relationship between anthropomorphising nature, financial insecurity, and support for pro-environmental policies in the context of the coronavirus pandemic. Current Res. Ecol. Soc. Psychol. 3, 100039. 10.1016/j.cresp.2022.100039 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Bubeck P., Wouter Botzen W., Laudan J., Aerts J. C., Thieken A. H. (2018). Insights into flood-coping appraisals of protection motivation theory: empirical evidence from Germany and France. Risk Anal. 38, 1239–1257. 10.1111/risa.12938 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Busby J. W. (2021). Beyond internal conflict: the emergent practice of climate security. J. Peace Res. 58, 186–194. 10.1177/0022343320971019 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  23. Carfora V., Caso D., Sparks P., Conner M. (2017). Moderating effects of pro-environmental self-identity on pro-environmental intentions and behaviour: a multi-behaviour study. J. Environ. Psychol. 53, 92–99. 10.1016/j.jenvp.2017.07.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  24. Chang M.-C., Wu C.-C. (2015). The effect of message framing on pro-environmental behavior intentions. Br. Food J. 117, 339–357. 10.1108/BFJ-09-2013-024729207544 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  25. Channa N. A., Tariq B., Samo A. H., Ghumro N. H., Qureshi N. A. (2022). Predicting consumers' intentions to purchase eco-friendly athletic wear in a moderated model of individual green values and gender. Int J Sports Market Sponsorship. 23, 410–436. 10.1108/IJSMS-12-2020-0215 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  26. Chasteen A. L., Madey S. F. (2003). Belief in a just world and the perceived injustice of dying young or old. Omega J. Death Dying 47, 313–326. 10.2190/W7H7-TE9E-1FWN-B8XD [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  27. Chawla L. (1998). Significant life experiences revisited: a review of research on sources of environmental sensitivity. J. Environ. Educ. 29, 11–21. 10.1080/00958969809599114 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  28. Chen F., Chen R. P., Yang L. (2020). When sadness comes alive, will it be less painful? The effects of anthropomorphic thinking on sadness regulation and consumption. J. Consumer Psychol. 30, 277–295. 10.1002/jcpy.1137 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  29. Chin W. W. (2010). How to write up and report PLS analyses, in Handbook of Partial Least Squares (Berlin: Springer; ), 655–690. 10.1007/978-3-540-32827-8_29 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  30. Clayton S., Fraser J., Burgess C. (2011). The role of zoos in fostering environmental identity. Ecopsychology 3, 87–96. 10.1089/eco.2010.0079 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  31. De Groot J., Steg L. (2007). General beliefs and the theory of planned behavior: the role of environmental concerns in the TPB. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 37, 1817–1836. 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2007.00239.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  32. De Leeuw A., Valois P., Ajzen I., Schmidt P. (2015). Using the theory of planned behavior to identify key beliefs underlying pro-environmental behavior in high-school students: Implications for educational interventions. J. Environ. Psychol. 42, 128–138. 10.1016/j.jenvp.2015.03.005 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  33. De Silva M., Wang P., Kuah A. T. (2021). Why wouldn't green appeal drive purchase intention? Moderation effects of consumption values in the UK and China. J. Bus. Res. 122, 713–724. 10.1016/j.jbusres.2020.01.016 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  34. Dermody J., Koenig-Lewis N., Zhao A. L., Hanmer-Lloyd S. (2018). Appraising the influence of pro-environmental self-identity on sustainable consumption buying and curtailment in emerging markets: Evidence from China and Poland. J. Bus. Res. 86, 333–343. 10.1016/j.jbusres.2017.09.041 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  35. do Paço A., Alves H., Shiel C., Filho W. L. (2013). Development of a green consumer behaviour model. Int. J. Consum. Stud. 37, 414–421. 10.1111/ijcs.12009 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  36. Dul J. (2016). Necessary condition analysis (NCA) logic and methodology of “necessary but not sufficient” causality. Organ. Res. Methods 19, 10–52. 10.1177/1094428115584005 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  37. Dul J. (2021). Necessary Condition Analysis (NCA) with R (Version 3.1. 0): A Quick Start Guide. [Google Scholar]
  38. Dul J., Van der Laan E., Kuik R. (2020). A statistical significance test for necessary condition analysis. Organ. Res. Methods 23, 385–395. 10.1177/1094428118795272 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  39. El-Said O., Aziz H. (2022). Virtual tours a means to an end: an analysis of virtual tours' role in tourism recovery post COVID-19. J. Travel Res. 61, 528–548. 10.1177/0047287521997567 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  40. Epley N., Waytz A., Cacioppo J. T. (2007). On seeing human: a three-factor theory of anthropomorphism. Psychol. Rev. 114, 864. 10.1037/0033-295X.114.4.864 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Escalas J. E., Stern B. B. (2003). Sympathy and empathy: emotional responses to advertising dramas. J. Consum. Res. 29, 566–578. 10.1086/346251 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  42. Farmaki A., Pappas N., Kvasova O., Stergiou D. P. (2022). Hotel CSR and job satisfaction: a chaordic perspective. Tourism Manage. 91, 104526. 10.1016/j.tourman.2022.104526 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  43. Fornell C., Larcker D. F. (1981). Structural Equation Models With Unobservable Variables and Measurement Error: Algebra and Statistics. Los Angeles, CA: Sage Publications. 10.2307/3150980 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  44. Fritsche I., Jonas E., Kayser D. N., Koranyi N. (2010). Existential threat and compliance with pro-environmental norms. J. Environ. Psychol. 30, 67–79. 10.1016/j.jenvp.2009.08.007 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  45. Gao Y., Zhao Z., Ma Y., Li Y. (2022). A rational-affective-moral factor model for determining tourists' pro-environmental behaviour. Current Issues Tourism. 25, 1–19. 10.1080/13683500.2022.2078687 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  46. Gerend M. A., Cullen M. (2008). Effects of message framing and temporal context on college student drinking behavior. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 44, 1167–1173. 10.1016/j.jesp.2008.02.00726363446 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  47. Gold A. H., Malhotra A., Segars A. H. (2001). Knowledge management: An organizational capabilities perspective. J. Manage. Inform. Syst. 18, 185–214. 10.1080/07421222.2001.11045669 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  48. Gross M., McGoey L. (2015). Routledge International Handbook of Ignorance Studies. London: Routledge. 10.4324/9781315867762 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  49. Grothmann T., Patt A. (2005). Adaptive capacity and human cognition: the process of individual adaptation to climate change. Global Environ. Change 15, 199–213. 10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2005.01.002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  50. Hair J. F., Hult G. T. M., Ringle C. M., Sarstedt M. (2021). A Primer on Partial Least Squares Structural Equation Modeling (PLS-SEM). Berlin: Sage publications. 10.1007/978-3-030-80519-7 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  51. Hair J. F., Ringle C. M., Sarstedt M. (2013). Partial least squares structural equation modeling: Rigorous applications, better results and higher acceptance. Long Range Plann. 46, 1–12. 10.1016/j.lrp.2013.01.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  52. Hair J. F., Risher J. J., Sarstedt M., Ringle C. M. (2019). When to use and how to report the results of PLS-SEM. Eur. Bus. Rev. 31, 2–24. 10.1108/EBR-11-2018-0203 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  53. Hair J. F., Sarstedt M., Ringle C. M., Gudergan S. P. (2017). Advanced Issues in Partial Least Squares Structural Equation Modeling. London: Sage publications. [Google Scholar]
  54. Han H., Chua B.-L., Hyun S. S. (2020). Eliciting customers' waste reduction and water saving behaviors at a hotel. Int. J. Hospital. Manage. 87, 102386. 10.1016/j.ijhm.2019.102386 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  55. Han H., Hwang J., Kim J., Jung H. (2015). Guests' pro-environmental decision-making process: Broadening the norm activation framework in a lodging context. Int. J. Hospital. Manage. 47, 96–107. 10.1016/j.ijhm.2015.03.013 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  56. Haselton M. G., Nettle D. (2006). The paranoid optimist: an integrative evolutionary model of cognitive biases. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 10, 47–66. 10.1207/s15327957pspr1001_3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Haselton M. G., Nettle D., Murray D. R. (2015). The evolution of cognitive bias, in The Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology, Chapter 41, 1–20. 10.1002/9781119125563.evpsych24125855820 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  58. Henseler J., Fassott G. (2010). Testing moderating effects in PLS path models: An illustration of available procedures, in Handbook of Partial Least Squares (Berlin: Springer; ), p. 713–735. 10.1007/978-3-540-32827-8_31 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  59. Henseler J., Ringle C. M., Sarstedt M. (2015). A new criterion for assessing discriminant validity in variance-based structural equation modeling. J. Acad. Market. Sci. 43, 115–135. 10.1007/s11747-014-0403-8 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  60. Hepper E. G., Ritchie T. D., Sedikides C., Wildschut T. (2012). Odyssey's end: lay conceptions of nostalgia reflect its original Homeric meaning. Emotion 12, 102. 10.1037/a0025167 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Hines J. M., Hungerford H. R., Tomera A. N. (1987). Analysis and synthesis of research on responsible environmental behavior: a meta-analysis. J. Environ. Educ. 18, 1–8. 10.1080/00958964.1987.994348231184251 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  62. Ito K., Li L. M. W. (2019). Holism and pro-environmental commitment: an examination on the mediating roles of affective and cognitive determinants. Pers. Individ. Dif. 149, 160–166. 10.1016/j.paid.2019.05.055 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  63. Janmaimool P., Khajohnmanee S. (2020). Enhancing university students' global citizenship, public mindedness, and moral quotient for promoting sense of environmental responsibility and pro-environmental behaviours. Environ. Develop. Sustain. 22, 957–970. 10.1007/s10668-018-0228-6 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  64. Jiang D., Li M., Wu H., Liu S. (2021). Learning from COVID-19: infectious disease vulnerability promotes pro-environmental behaviors. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18, 8687. 10.3390/ijerph18168687 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Juhl J., Wildschut T., Sedikides C., Diebel T., Cheung W. Y., Vingerhoets A. J. (2020). Nostalgia proneness and empathy: generality, underlying mechanism, and implications for prosocial behavior. J. Pers. 88, 485–500. 10.1111/jopy.12505 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Kaiser F. G. (1998). A general measure of ecological behavior 1. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 28, 395–422. 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1998.tb01712.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  67. Kaiser F. G., Shimoda T. A. (1999). Responsibility as a predictor of ecological behaviour. J. Environ. Psychol. 19, 243–253. 10.1006/jevp.1998.91236706793 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  68. Kao T.-F., Du Y.-Z. (2020). A study on the influence of green advertising design and environmental emotion on advertising effect. J. Clean. Prod. 242, 118294. 10.1016/j.jclepro.2019.118294 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  69. Karpinska-Krakowiak M., Skowron L., Ivanov L. (2020). “I will start saving natural resources, only when you show me the planet as a person in danger”: the effects of message framing and anthropomorphism on pro-environmental behaviors that are viewed as effortful. Sustainability 12, 5524. 10.3390/su12145524 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  70. Keshavarz M., Karami E. (2016). Farmers' pro-environmental behavior under drought: application of protection motivation theory. J. Arid Environ. 127, 128–136. 10.1016/j.jaridenv.2015.11.010 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  71. Kim S., Chen R. P., Zhang K. (2016). Anthropomorphized helpers undermine autonomy and enjoyment in computer games. J. Consum. Res. 43, 282–302. 10.1093/jcr/ucw016 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  72. Konrad A. M., Linnehan F. (1995). Formalized HRM structures: coordinating equal employment opportunity or concealing organizational practices? Acad. Manage. J. 38, 787–820. 10.5465/256746 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  73. Kopaei H. R., Nooripoor M., Karami A., Ertz M. (2021). Modeling consumer home composting intentions for sustainable municipal organic waste management in Iran. AIMS Environ. Sci. 8, 1–17. 10.3934/environsci.2021001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  74. Krishnamurthy P., Carter P., Blair E. (2001). Attribute framing and goal framing effects in health decisions. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 85, 382–399. 10.1006/obhd.2001.2962 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Kronrod A., Grinstein A., Wathieu L. (2012). Go green! Should environmental messages be so assertive? J. Market. 76, 95–102. 10.1509/jm.10.0416 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  76. Kusmanoff A. M., Fidler F., Gordon A., Garrard G. E., Bekessy S. A. (2020). Five lessons to guide more effective biodiversity conservation message framing. Conserv. Biol. 34, 1131–1141. 10.1111/cobi.13482 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Lasaleta J. D., Sedikides C., Vohs K. D. (2014). Nostalgia weakens the desire for money. J. Consum. Res. 41, 713–729. 10.1086/677227 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  78. Lee W., Jeong C. (2021). Distinctive roles of tourist eudaimonic and hedonic experiences on satisfaction and place attachment: Combined use of SEM and necessary condition analysis. J. Hospital. Tourism Manage. 47, 58–71. 10.1016/j.jhtm.2021.02.012 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  79. Levin I. P., Schnittjer S. K., Thee S. L. (1988). Information framing effects in social and personal decisions. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 24, 520–529. 10.1016/0022-1031(88)90050-917535469 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  80. Liang D., Hou C., Jo M.-S., Sarigöll,ü E. (2019). Pollution avoidance and green purchase: the role of moral emotions. J. Clean. Prod. 210, 1301–1310. 10.1016/j.jclepro.2018.11.103 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  81. Liang H., Xue Y. (2009). Avoidance of information technology threats: a theoretical perspective. MIS Q. 33, 71–90. 10.2307/20650279 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  82. Lim J. Y., Moon K.-K. (2020). Perceived environmental threats and pro-environmental behaviors: investigating the role of political participation using a South Korean survey. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 17, 3244. 10.3390/ijerph17093244 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Lindenberg S., Steg L. (2007). Normative, gain and hedonic goal frames guiding environmental behavior. J. Soc. Issues 63, 117. 10.1111/j.1540-4560.2007.00499.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  84. Liu S.-C., Lin H.-,s. (2015). Exploring undergraduate students' mental models of the environment: are they related to environmental affect and behavior? J. Environ. Educ. 46, 23–40. 10.1080/00958964.2014.953021 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  85. Liu T., Geng L., Ye L., Zhou K. (2019). “Mother Nature” enhances connectedness to nature and pro-environmental behavior. J. Environ. Psychol. 61, 37–45. 10.1016/j.jenvp.2018.12.003 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  86. Luchs M. G., Mooradian T. A. (2012). Sex, personality, and sustainable consumer behaviour: elucidating the gender effect. J. Consumer Policy 35, 127–144. 10.1007/s10603-011-9179-0 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  87. Marjanovic Z., Struthers C. W., Greenglass E. R. (2012). Who helps natural-disaster victims? Assessment of trait and situational predictors. Anal. Soc. Issues Public Policy 12, 245–267. 10.1111/j.1530-2415.2011.01262.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  88. McLeay F., Olya H., Liu H., Jayawardhena C., Dennis C. (2022). A multi-analytical approach to studying customers motivations to use innovative totally autonomous vehicles. Technol. Forecast. Soc. Change 174, 121252. 10.1016/j.techfore.2021.121252 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  89. Mehrabian A., Russell J. A. (1974). An Approach to Environmental Psychology. Cambridge: The MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
  90. Merchant A., Ford J. B., Rose G. (2011). How personal nostalgia influences giving to charity. J. Bus. Res. 64, 610–616. 10.1016/j.jbusres.2010.06.013 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  91. Milfont T. L. (2009). The effects of social desirability on self-reported environmental attitudes and ecological behaviour. Environmentalist 29, 263–269. 10.1007/s10669-008-9192-2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  92. Miliute-Plepiene J., Hage O., Plepys A., Reipas A. (2016). What motivates households recycling behaviour in recycling schemes of different maturity? Lessons from Lithuania and Sweden. Resour. Conserv. Recycl. 113, 40–52. 10.1016/j.resconrec.2016.05.008 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  93. Moche H., Västfjäll D. (2021). Helping the child or the adult? Systematically testing the identifiable victim effect for child and adult victims. Soc. Influence 16, 78–92. 10.1080/15534510.2021.1995482 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  94. Moon S., Bergey P. K., Bove L. L., Robinson S. (2016). Message framing and individual traits in adopting innovative, sustainable products (ISPs): Evidence from biofuel adoption. J. Bus. Res. 69, 3553–3560. 10.1016/j.jbusres.2016.01.029 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  95. Mota-Rojas D., Mariti C., Zdeinert A., Riggio G., Mora-Medina P., del Mar Reyes A., et al. (2021). Anthropomorphism and its adverse effects on the distress and welfare of companion animals. Animals 11, 3263. 10.3390/ani11113263 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Nekmahmud M., Fekete-Farkas M. (2020). Why Not Green Marketing? Determinates of consumers' intention to green purchase decision in a new developing nation. Sustainability 12, 7880. 10.3390/su12197880 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  97. Nisbet E. K., Zelenski J. M., Murphy S. A. (2009). The nature relatedness scale: linking individuals' connection with nature to environmental concern and behavior. Environ. Behav. 41, 715–740. 10.1177/0013916508318748 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  98. Nordlund A. M., Garvill J. (2003). Effects of values, problem awareness, and personal norm on willingness to reduce personal car use. J. Environ. Psychol. 23, 339–347. 10.1016/S0272-4944(03)00037-9 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  99. O'Connor R. E., Bard R. J., Fisher A. (1999). Risk perceptions, general environmental beliefs, and willingness to address climate change. Risk Anal. 19, 461–471. 10.1111/j.1539-6924.1999.tb00421.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  100. Olsen M. C., Slotegraaf R. J., Chandukala S. R. (2014). Green claims and message frames: how green new products change brand attitude. J. Mark. 78, 119–137. 10.1509/jm.13.0387 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  101. Onwezen M. C., Antonides G., Bartels J. (2013). The norm activation model: an exploration of the functions of anticipated pride and guilt in pro-environmental behaviour. J. Econ. Psychol. 39, 141–153. 10.1016/j.joep.2013.07.005 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  102. Oreg S., Katz-Gerro T. (2006). Predicting proenvironmental behavior cross-nationally: Values, the theory of planned behavior, and value-belief-norm theory. Environ. Behav. 38, 462–483. 10.1177/0013916505286012 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  103. Panda T. K., Kumar A., Jakhar S., Luthra S., Garza-Reyes J. A., Kazancoglu I., et al. (2020). Social and environmental sustainability model on consumers' altruism, green purchase intention, green brand loyalty and evangelism. J. Clean. Prod. 243, 118575. 10.1016/j.jclepro.2019.118575 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  104. Park S., Martina R., Smolka K. (2019). Working Passionately Does Not Always Pay Off: The Negative Moderating Role of Passion on the Relationship Between Deliberate Practice and Venture Performance, in The Anatomy of Entrepreneurial Decisions (Berlin: Springer; ), 173–195. 10.1007/978-3-030-19685-1_8 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  105. Patak M., Branska L., Pecinova Z. (2021). Consumer intention to purchase green consumer chemicals. Sustainability 13, 7992. 10.3390/su13147992 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  106. Petrescu-Mag R. M., Rastegari Kopaei H., Petrescu D. C. (2021). What drives landowners to resist selling their land? insights from ethical capitalism and landowners' Perceptions. Land 10, 312. 10.3390/land10030312 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  107. Petrescu-Mag R. M., Rastegari Kopaei H., Petrescu D. C. (2022). Consumers' acceptance of the first novel insect food approved in the European union: predictors of yellow mealworm chips consumption. Food Sci. Nutr. 10, 846–862. 10.1002/fsn3.2716 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Prguda E., Neumann D. L. (2014). Inter-human and animal-directed empathy: a test for evolutionary biases in empathetic responding. Behav. Processes 108, 80–86. 10.1016/j.beproc.2014.09.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Rainear A. M., Christensen J. L. (2017). Protection motivation theory as an explanatory framework for proenvironmental behavioral intentions. Commun. Res. Rep. 34, 239–248. 10.1080/08824096.2017.1286472 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  110. Ramus C. A., Killmer A. B. (2007). Corporate greening through prosocial extrarole behaviours–a conceptual framework for employee motivation. Bus. Strategy Environ. 16, 554–570. 10.1002/bse.504 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  111. Rastegari Kopaei H., Nooripoor M., Karami A., Petrescu-Mag R. M., Petrescu D. C. (2021). Drivers of residents' home composting intention: integrating the theory of planned behavior, the norm activation model, and the moderating role of composting knowledge. Sustainability 13, 6826. 10.3390/su13126826 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  112. Rees J. H., Klug S., Bamberg S. (2015). Guilty conscience: motivating pro-environmental behavior by inducing negative moral emotions. Clim. Change 130, 439–452. 10.1007/s10584-014-1278-x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  113. Rezaei R., Safa L., Damalas C. A., Ganjkhanloo M. M. (2019). Drivers of farmers' intention to use integrated pest management: integrating theory of planned behavior and norm activation model. J. Environ. Manage. 236, 328–339. 10.1016/j.jenvman.2019.01.097 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Richter N. F., Schubring S., Hauff S., Ringle C. M., Sarstedt M. (2020). When predictors of outcomes are necessary: guidelines for the combined use of PLS-SEM and NCA. Ind. Manage. Data Syst. 120, 2243–2267. 10.1108/IMDS-11-2019-0638 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  115. Rohani C., Kesbakhi M. S., Mohtashami J. (2018). Clinical empathy with cancer patients: a content analysis of oncology nurses' perception. Patient Prefer. Adherence 12, 1089. 10.2147/PPA.S156441 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Routledge C., Arndt J., Wildschut T., Sedikides C., Hart C. M., Juhl J., et al. (2011). The past makes the present meaningful: nostalgia as an existential resource. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 101, 638. 10.1037/a0024292 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Sarstedt M., Hair J. F., Jr, Cheah J.-H., Becker J.-M., Ringle C. M. (2019). How to specify, estimate, and validate higher-order constructs in PLS-SEM. Aust. Market. J. 27, 197–211. 10.1016/j.ausmj.2019.05.003 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  118. Schroll R., Schnurr B., Grewal D. (2018). Humanizing products with handwritten typefaces. J. Consum. Res. 45, 648–672. 10.1093/jcr/ucy014 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  119. Schultz P. (2000). Empathizing with nature: the effects of perspective taking on concern for environmental issues. J. Soc. Issues 56, 391–406. [Google Scholar]
  120. Schultz P. (2002). Inclusion with nature: the psychology of human-nature relations, in Psychology of sustainable development. Springer, 61–78. 10.1007/978-1-4615-0995-0_4 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  121. Schultz P. W. (2000). New environmental theories: empathizing with nature: the effects ofPerspective taking on concern for environmental issues. J. Soc. Issues 56, 391–406. 10.1111/0022-4537.00174 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  122. Sedikides C., Wildschut T., Routledge C., Arndt J., Hepper E. G., Zhou X. (2015). To nostalgize: mixing memory with affect and desire, in Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Amsterdam: Elsevier; ), 189–273. 10.1016/bs.aesp.2014.10.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  123. Sethi R., Sethi A. (2009). Can quality-oriented firms develop innovative new products? J. Product Innovat. Manage. 26, 206–221. 10.1111/j.1540-5885.2009.00346.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  124. Shafiei A., Maleksaeidi H. (2020). Pro-environmental behavior of university students: application of protection motivation theory. Global Ecol. Conserv. 22, e00908. 10.1016/j.gecco.2020.e00908 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  125. Sharma A., Dwivedi R., Mariani M. M., Islam T. (2022a). Investigating the effect of advertising irritation on digital advertising effectiveness: a moderated mediation model. Technol. Forecast. Soc. Change 180, 121731. 10.1016/j.techfore.2022.121731 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  126. Sharma A., Fadahunsi A., Abbas H., Pathak V. K. (2022b). A multi-analytic approach to predict social media marketing influence on consumer purchase intention. J. Indian Bus. Res. 14, 125–149. 10.1108/JIBR-08-2021-0313 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  127. Shelton M. L., Rogers R. W. (1981). Fear-arousing and empathy-arousing appeals to help: the pathos of persuasion. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 11, 366–378. 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1981.tb00829.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  128. Sheng G., Xie F., Gong S., Pan H. (2019). The role of cultural values in green purchasing intention: empirical evidence from Chinese consumers. Int. J. Consum. Stud. 43, 315–326. 10.1111/ijcs.12513 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  129. Shmueli G., Ray S., Estrada J. M. V., Chatla S. B. (2016). The elephant in the room: predictive performance of PLS models. J. Bus. Res. 69, 4552–4564. 10.1016/j.jbusres.2016.03.049 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  130. Slovic P. (2010). If I look at the mass I will never act: psychic numbing and genocide, in Emotions and Risky Technologies. Springer, 37–59. 10.1007/978-90-481-8647-1_3 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  131. Spence A., Poortinga W., Butler C., Pidgeon N. F. (2011). Perceptions of climate change and willingness to save energy related to flood experience. Nat. Clim. Chang. 1, 46–49. 10.1038/nclimate1059 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  132. Steg L. (2005). Car use: lust and must. Instrumental, symbolic and affective motives for car use. Transp. Res. Part A Policy Practice 39, 147–162. 10.1016/j.tra.2004.07.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  133. Stephan E., Wildschut T., Sedikides C., Zhou X., He W., Routledge C., et al. (2014). The mnemonic mover: nostalgia regulates avoidance and approach motivation. Emotion 14, 545. 10.1037/a0035673 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  134. Stern P. (2000). Toward a coherent theory of environmentally significant behavior. J. Soc. Issues 56, 407–424. 10.1111/0022-4537.00175 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  135. Stern P. C., Dietz T., Guagnano G. A. (1995). The new ecological paradigm in social-psychological context. Environ. Behav. 27, 723–743. 10.1177/001391659527600135393503 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  136. Stevenson K. T., Peterson M. N., Carrier S. J., Strnad R. L., Bondell H. D., Kirby-Hathaway T., et al. (2014). Role of significant life experiences in building environmental knowledge and behavior among middle school students. J. Environ. Educ. 45, 163–177. 10.1080/00958964.2014.901935 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  137. Stone G., Barnes J. H., Montgomery C. (1995). Ecoscale: a scale for the measurement of environmentally responsible consumers. Psychol. Market. 12, 595–612. 10.1002/mar.4220120704 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  138. Sukhov A., Olsson L. E., Friman M. (2022). Necessary and sufficient conditions for attractive public Transport: Combined use of PLS-SEM and NCA. Transp. Res. Part A Policy Practice 158, 239–250. 10.1016/j.tra.2022.03.012 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  139. Sun X., Su W., Guo X., Tian Z. (2021). The impact of awe induced by COVID-19 pandemic on green consumption behavior in China. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18, 543. 10.3390/ijerph18020543 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  140. Tam K.-P. (2013a). Concepts and measures related to connection to nature: similarities and differences. J. Environ. Psychol. 34, 64–78. 10.1016/j.jenvp.2013.01.004 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  141. Tam K.-P. (2013b). Dispositional empathy with nature. J. Environ. Psychol. 35, 92–104. 10.1016/j.jenvp.2013.05.004 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  142. Tam K.-P. (2015a). Mind attribution to nature and proenvironmental behavior. Ecopsychology 7, 87–95. 10.1089/eco.2014.0054 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  143. Tam K.-P. (2019). Anthropomorphism of nature, environmental guilt, and pro-environmental behavior. Sustainability 11, 5430. 10.3390/su11195430 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  144. Tam K.-P., Lee S.-L., Chao M. M. (2013). Saving Mr. Nature: anthropomorphism enhances connectedness to and protectiveness toward nature. J. Exp. Social Psychol. 49, 514–521. 10.1016/j.jesp.2013.02.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  145. Tam K. P. (2015b). Are anthropomorphic persuasive appeals effective? The role of the recipient's motivations. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 54, 187–200. 10.1111/bjso.12076 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  146. Tenenhaus M., Vinzi V., Chatelin Y., Lauro C. (2005). Partial least squares. Comput. Stat. Data Anal. 48, 159–205. 10.1016/j.csda.2004.03.005 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  147. The Khoa D., Wang C.-Y., Guchait P. (2021). Using regulatory focus to encourage physical distancing in services: When fear helps to deal with Mr. Deadly COVID-19. Serv. Ind. J. 41, 32–57. 10.1080/02642069.2020.1831477 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  148. Tho N. D. (2019). Strategic orientations and firm innovativeness: a necessary condition analysis. Baltic J. Manage. 14, 427–442. 10.1108/BJM-07-2018-0280 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  149. Toth Z., Dul J., Li C. (2019). Necessary condition analysis in tourism research. Ann. Tourism Res. 79. 10.1016/j.annals.2019.102821 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  150. Troiville J., Hair J. F., Cliquet G. (2019). Definition, conceptualization and measurement of consumer-based retailer brand equity. J. Retail. Consum. Serv. 50, 73–84. 10.1016/j.jretconser.2019.04.022 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  151. Tversky A., Kahneman D. (1985). The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice, in Behavioral Decision Making (Berlin: Springer; ), 25–41. 10.1007/978-1-4613-2391-4_2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  152. van der Werff E., Steg L. (2015). One model to predict them all: predicting energy behaviours with the norm activation model. Energy Res. Soc. Sci. 6, 8–14. 10.1016/j.erss.2014.11.002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  153. Vining J. (1987). Environmental decisions: The interaction of emotions, information, and decision context. J. Environ. Psychol. 7, 13–30. 10.1016/S0272-4944(87)80042-7 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  154. Vis B., Dul J. (2018). Analyzing relationships of necessity not just in kind but also in degree: complementing fsQCA with NCA. Sociol. Methods Res. 47, 872–899. 10.1177/0049124115626179 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  155. Vitell S. J., King R. A., Singh J. J. (2013). A special emphasis and look at the emotional side of ethical decision-making. AMS Rev. 3, 74–85. 10.1007/s13162-013-0042-0 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  156. Wang D., Jia Y., Hou Z.-J., Xu H., Zhang H., Guo X.-L. (2019). A test of psychology of working theory among Chinese urban workers: examining predictors and outcomes of decent work. J. Vocat. Behav. 115, 103325. 10.1016/j.jvb.2019.103325 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  157. Wang Y., Liang J., Yang J., Ma X., Li X., Wu J., et al. (2019). Analysis of the environmental behavior of farmers for non-point source pollution control and management: An integration of the theory of planned behavior and the protection motivation theory. J. Environ. Manage. 237, 15–23. 10.1016/j.jenvman.2019.02.070 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  158. Wang Y., Tian H., Sarigöllü E., Xu W. (2020). Nostalgia prompts sustainable product disposal. J. Consumer Behav. 19, 570–580. 10.1002/cb.1832 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  159. Wei J. (2021). The impacts of perceived risk and negative emotions on the service recovery effect for online travel agencies: the moderating role of corporate reputation. Front. Psychol. 12, 685351. 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.685351 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  160. Westbrook R. A. (1987). Product/consumption-based affective responses and postpurchase processes. J. Market. Res. 24, 258–270. 10.1177/002224378702400302 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  161. Wetzels M., Odekerken-Schröder G., Van Oppen C. (2009). Using PLS path modeling for assessing hierarchical construct models: guidelines and empirical illustration. MIS Q. 177–195. 10.2307/20650284 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  162. White K., MacDonnell R., Dahl D. W. (2011). It's the mind-set that matters: the role of construal level and message framing in influencing consumer efficacy and conservation behaviors. J. Market. Res. 48, 472–485. 10.1509/jmkr.48.3.472 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  163. Wildschut T., Sedikides C., Arndt J., Routledge C. (2006). Nostalgia: content, triggers, functions. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 91, 975. 10.1037/0022-3514.91.5.975 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  164. Williams M. O., Whitmarsh L., Chríost D. M. G. (2021a). The association between anthropomorphism of nature and pro-environmental variables: a systematic review. Biol. Conserv. 255, 109022. 10.1016/j.biocon.2021.109022 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  165. Williams M. O., Whitmarsh L., Haddock G., Mac Giolla Chríost D. (2021b). A grounded theory of pro-nature behaviour: from moral concern to sustained action. Sustainability 13, 8944. 10.3390/su13168944 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  166. Witte K. (1992). Putting the fear back into fear appeals: The extended parallel process model. Commun. Monographs 59, 329–349. 10.1080/0363775920937627623330860 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  167. Workman M., Bommer W. H., Straub D. (2008). Security lapses and the omission of information security measures: a threat control model and empirical test. Comput. Human Behav. 24, 2799–2816. 10.1016/j.chb.2008.04.005 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  168. Wu Z., Chen Y., Geng L., Zhou L., Zhou K. (2020). Greening in nostalgia? How nostalgic traveling enhances tourists' proenvironmental behaviour. Sustain. Develop. 28, 634–645. 10.1002/sd.2014 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  169. Xi J., Wang X. (2022). Development of landscape architecture design students' pro-environmental awareness by project-based learning. Sustainability 14, 2164. 10.3390/su14042164 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  170. Yang X., Chen L., Wei L., Su Q. (2020). Personal and media factors related to citizens' pro-environmental behavioral intention against haze in China: a moderating analysis of TPB. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 17, 2314. 10.3390/ijerph17072314 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  171. Yue B., Sheng G., She S., Xu J. (2020). Impact of consumer environmental responsibility on green consumption behavior in China: the role of environmental concern and price sensitivity. Sustainability 12, 2074. 10.3390/su12052074 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  172. Zelenski J. M., Desrochers J. E. (2021). Can positive and self-transcendent emotions promote pro-environmental behavior? Current Opin. Psychol. 42, 31–35. 10.1016/j.copsyc.2021.02.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  173. Zhang J., Yu Z., Zhao B., Sun R., Vejre H. (2020). Links between green space and public health: a bibliometric review of global research trends and future prospects from 1901 to 2019. Environ. Res. Lett. 15, 063001. 10.1088/1748-9326/ab7f64 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  174. Zhang L. (2014). How effective are your CSR messages? The moderating role of processing fluency and construal level. Int. J. Hospital. Manage. 41, 56–62. 10.1016/j.ijhm.2014.04.005 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  175. Zhang X., Gong X., Jiang J. (2021). Dump or recycle? Nostalgia and consumer recycling behavior. J. Bus. Res. 132, 594–603. 10.1016/j.jbusres.2020.11.033 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  176. Zhou X., Sedikides C., Mo T., Li W., Hong E. K., Wildschut T. (2021). The restorative power of nostalgia: Thwarting loneliness by raising happiness during the COVID-19 pandemic. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 13, 803–815. 10.1177/19485506211041830 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  177. Zhou X., Sedikides C., Wildschut T., Gao D.-G. (2008). Counteracting loneliness: on the restorative function of nostalgia. Psychol. Sci. 19, 1023–1029. 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02194.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  178. Zhou X., Wildschut T., Sedikides C., Shi K., Feng C. (2012). Nostalgia: the gift that keeps on giving. J. Consum. Res. 39, 39–50. 10.1086/662199 [DOI] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.


Articles from Frontiers in Psychology are provided here courtesy of Frontiers Media SA

RESOURCES