Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2023 Sep 1.
Published in final edited form as: Am J Psychiatry. 2022 Jun 22;179(9):661–672. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.21090911

Persistent Dissociation and its Neural Correlates Uniquely Predict Worse Outcomes after Trauma Exposure

Lauren A M Lebois 1,2,*, Nathaniel G Harnett 1,2, Sanne J H van Rooij 3, Timothy D Ely 3, Tanja Jovanovic 4, Steven E Bruce 5, Stacey L House 6, Caitlin Ravichandran 1,2,60, Nathalie M Dumornay 1, Katherine E Finegold 1, Sarah B Hill 7, Julia B Merker 1, Karlye A Phillips 8,9, Francesca L Beaudoin 10, Xinming An 11, Thomas C Neylan 12, Gari D Clifford 13,14, Sarah D Linnstaedt 11, Laura T Germine 15,16,2, Scott L Rauch 15,17,2, John P Haran 18, Alan B Storrow 19, Christopher Lewandowski 20, Paul I Musey Jr 21, Phyllis L Hendry 22, Sophia Sheikh 22, Christopher W Jones 23, Brittany E Punches 24,25, Robert A Swor 26, Meghan E McGrath 27, Lauren A Hudak 28, Jose L Pascual 29,30, Mark J Seamon 31,30, Elizabeth M Datner 32,33, Anna M Chang 34, Claire Pearson 35, Robert M Domeier 36, Niels K Rathlev 37, Brian J O’Neil 35, Paulina Sergot 38, Leon D Sanchez 39,40, Mark W Miller 41,42, Robert H Pietrzak 43,44, Jutta Joormann 45, Deanna M Barch 46, Diego A Pizzagalli 1,2, John F Sheridan 47,48, Jordan W Smoller 49,50, Beatriz Luna 51, Steven E Harte 52,53, James M Elliott 54,55,56, Ronald C Kessler 57, Karestan C Koenen 58, Samuel A McLean 59,11, Jennifer S Stevens 3, Kerry J Ressler 1,2
PMCID: PMC9444876  NIHMSID: NIHMS1794352  PMID: 35730162

Abstract

Objective:

Dissociation, a disruption or discontinuity in psychological functioning, is often linked with worse psychiatric symptoms; however, the prognostic value of dissociation after trauma is inconsistent. Determining whether trauma-related dissociation is uniquely predictive of later outcomes would enable early identification of at-risk trauma populations. We completed the largest prospective, longitudinal, biomarker study of persistent dissociation to date to determine its predictive capacity for adverse psychiatric outcomes following acute trauma.

Methods:

All data were part of the Freeze 2 data release from the AURORA study. Individuals provided self-report data about persistent derealization (N=1,464), a severe type of dissociation, and completed a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) emotion reactivity task and resting-state scan 2-weeks post-trauma (N=145). We collected 3-month follow-up reports of posttraumatic stress, depression, pain, anxiety symptoms and functional impairment.

Results:

Derealization was associated with increased ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) activation in the emotion reactivity task and decreased resting-state vmPFC connectivity with cerebellum and orbitofrontal cortex. Furthermore, in separate analyses, brain-based and self-report measures of persistent derealization at 2-weeks predicted worse 3-month posttraumatic stress symptoms, distinct from childhood maltreatment and current posttraumatic stress symptoms.

Conclusions:

Our work suggests persistent derealization is both an early psychological and biological marker of worse later psychiatric outcomes. The neural correlates of trauma-related dissociation may serve as potential targets for treatment engagement to prevent PTSD. These results underscore dissociation assessment as crucial following trauma exposure to identify at-risk individuals and highlight an unmet clinical need for tailored early interventions.


Trauma-related pathological dissociation is a disruption or discontinuity in the typical integration of a person’s psychological functioning in the aftermath of a traumatic event (1). It encompasses a range of symptoms including derealization – the focus of this study because it is a symptom that is, in particular, tied to impairment and disease severity (2, 3). Derealization is a feeling of detachment from people, places, or objects in one’s environment (4). When experiencing derealization, individuals sometimes report feeling foggy or like they are in a movie or a dream. By providing some psychological distance from the traumatic experience, this type of dissociation may help people cope during experiences of overwhelming threat and in the aftermath of trauma (5).

Unfortunately, persistent dissociative coping responses come at a high cost for both the individual and society, as dissociative symptoms are often linked to a more severe psychiatric course and protracted treatment across disorders (6, 7). Furthermore, dissociation is often associated with worse symptoms of posttraumatic stress, depression, anxiety, pain, and greater functional impairment (710).

Still, evidence for the efficacy of leveraging dissociative symptoms to predict post-trauma psychiatric course is inconsistent. These inconsistencies are likely due to two interwoven issues: the duration of dissociation being measured (peritraumatic vs. persistent), and whether dissociation functions as an independent predictor of posttraumatic stress–that is, independent from variables like trauma history and current psychiatric symptoms. For example, peritraumatic dissociation that occurs during or in the immediate aftermath of a traumatic event predicts ongoing post-trauma stress, anxiety, depression, and pain, but this prediction is often removed or diminished when accounting for other variables (11, 12). Alternatively, a growing body of both cross-sectional and prospective work demonstrates persistent dissociation that lasts beyond the peritraumatic period is a much better indicator of long-term posttraumatic stress symptom severity compared to peritraumatic dissociation (13). Initial evidence also suggests persistent dissociation appears to maintain its predictive capacity for posttraumatic stress even when accounting for other variables such as trauma type and current PTSD symptoms (14). Notably, researchers have not yet tested whether persistent dissociation prospectively predicts post-trauma anxiety, depression, pain, and functional impairment while also accounting for other variables like trauma history and current posttraumatic stress symptoms. Determining whether persistent dissociation is predictive of later outcomes would be enormously beneficial by enabling early identification of at-risk trauma populations and those in need of targeted, early mental health intervention.

Given the potential predictive capacity of self-reported dissociation for various psychiatric outcomes and impairment, its biological correlates may also provide added value in identifying individuals at risk of worse outcomes. However, the biology of trauma-related dissociation has itself received limited attention, and its predictive capacity has not yet been studied. Work that has examined the biology of trauma-related dissociation demonstrates patterns of excessive corticolimbic inhibition for depersonalization/derealization in trauma spectrum disorders, suggesting this kind of dissociation is a type of emotion and arousal over-regulation (15, 16). In emotionally provocative contexts, individuals with high levels of dissociation exhibit hyperactivity in cortical regions related to emotion regulation (e.g., ventromedial prefrontal cortex, vmPFC) and hypoactivity in limbic regions related to salience detection and the visceral experience of one’s body (e.g., amygdala, insula). Hyperactivity in medial prefrontal cortex, in particular, demonstrates the most prominent association with depersonalization/derealization symptoms of dissociation (17). Like self-report studies, longitudinal neurobiological studies of dissociation are limited, and none have used the neurobiology of dissociation to predict later outcomes.

To address these critical gaps, we conducted the largest prospective, longitudinal biomarker study of dissociation to date focused on a specific type of dissociation: persistent derealization. We focused specifically on derealization because recent evidence suggests this subtype of dissociation may, in particular, be associated with symptom and impairment severity in trauma spectrum disorders (2, 3). In a sample of adults who recently experienced trauma, we tested the unique predictive capacity of both self-report and neurobiological measures of persistent derealization.

Approximately 2-weeks after the traumatic event, participants reported on symptoms of persistent derealization, and a subset completed a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) scan including an emotion reactivity task (passive viewing of fearful faces) and a resting-state scan. Based on prior data, while viewing fearful faces, we expected derealization would be related to hyperactivity in vmPFC and hypoactivity in amygdala and insula – potentially reflecting overmodulation of affective responses. Additionally, given the strength of medial PFC findings with dissociation in prior work, we hypothesized both self-report derealization and vmPFC activity during the fearful faces task would predict 3-month follow-up reports of posttraumatic stress, depression, pain, anxiety symptoms and functional impairment, even when accounting for a history of childhood maltreatment and 2-week levels of posttraumatic stress symptoms.

Methods

Participants

Participants were a convenience sample recruited as part of a multisite study on adverse posttraumatic psychiatric outcomes from twenty-two different emergency departments across the United States (18). To be eligible for the study, participants had to be ages 18–75, fluent in English, alert and oriented at the time of enrollment, and possess an iOS or Android-compatible smartphone. Exclusion criteria included self-inflicted injury, occupational injury, prisoner status, individuals pregnant or breastfeeding, individuals in an ongoing domestic violence situation, and those with more severe physical injury. Additional exclusion criteria for the MRI session were contraindications to MRI (e.g., metal implants). MRI scans were completed approximately 2-weeks after the emergency department visit at one of four sites: McLean Hospital, Emory University, Temple University or Wayne State University. All data reported here were part of the Freeze 2 data release from the AURORA study which includes data collection from September 23, 2016, to July 31, 2019. All participants provided written informed consent. All data were collected in accordance with ethical guidelines pertaining to the use of human participants and procedures were approved by each data collection site’s Institutional Review Board.

Measures

Participants completed surveys on their own at home or over the phone with an experimenter’s assistance. We measured history of childhood maltreatment using an abbreviated version of the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (19), and persistent derealization severity at 2-weeks post-trauma using a 2-item abbreviated version of the Brief Dissociative Experiences Scale (DES-B) (20). We measured posttraumatic stress symptom severity at 2-weeks and 3-months post-trauma using the PTSD checklist for DSM-5 (PCL-5) (21). At 3-months post-trauma, we also measured depression and anxiety symptom severity, pain extent, and functional impairment in work, family, and social life using the PROMIS Depression short form (22), an abbreviated version of the PROMIS Anxiety Bank (22), the Numeric Pain Rating Scale, and the Sheehan Disability Scale (23), respectively. Higher scores on all measures indicated more severe symptoms or impairment (see Supplement for further details).

Magnetic Resonance Imaging

Detailed information on sequence and task parameters, and preprocessing steps is discussed in the Supplement. Briefly, brain scans were completed approximately 2-weeks after the emergency department visit using Siemens 3 Tesla MRI systems with an MPRAGE T1-weighted image for structural scans and an EP2d-BOLD sequence for functional scans. The functional scans occurred in the following order: a 9.2 min resting-state scan, a 4.9 min emotion reactivity task, a 9.8 min go/no-go inhibition task (not reported here), and a 9.7 min monetary reward task (not reported here). The emotion reactivity task was designed to probe reactivity to social threat cues by having participants passively view fearful and neutral facial expressions (24). All data were preprocessed using a standardized pipeline via the FMRIPREP 1.2.2 software package.

We derived our vmPFC and insula ROI from previous dissociation-related results in motor vehicle accident samples completing an emotionally provocative task (25). Specifically, we used a 5mm sphere placed in left vmPFC (MNI coordinates −12, 50, 4) and right anterior insula (MNI coordinates 38, 20, 0). We derived our amygdala ROI from previous work using the fearful faces task in a traumatized civilian sample (24). Specifically, we used a 5mm sphere placed in right amygdala (MNI coordinates 20, 0, −16). We extracted the average value of the ROIs from the first-level fearful>neutral face contrast in the emotion reactivity task. We also used the vmPFC ROI to extract the average value from subject-level resting-state data. We correlated the vmPFC ROI values with the whole brain voxel time courses for each subject, and computed Fisher Z transformations to generate subject-level connectivity maps.

Group-Level Statistical Analyses

Reported p-values are two-tailed. All hypothesis tests and confidence intervals based on linear regression models were calculated using type HC3 robust standard errors in SPSS version 28, which do not require assumptions of normality or constant variance for validity (26).

Neural Correlates of Derealization.

To test whether self-report persistent derealization was associated with ROI activity in vmPFC, amygdala, and insula during the emotion reactivity task we conducted three separate linear regressions controlling for MRI scanner (McLean Hospital, Emory University, Temple University or Wayne State University), sex assigned at birth (male, female), age, childhood maltreatment and 2-week posttraumatic stress symptoms. We applied a Hommel multiple testing correction (27) using R version 3.5.3 to account for our choice of three ROIs with derealization; corresponding p-values are designated “corrected” in the text of the results. No multiple testing correction was applied to results for control covariates. To test whether vmPFC connectivity was associated with derealization, we entered subject-level seed-based connectivity maps into a linear model using the AFNI 20.0.24 3dMVM program (28). Only results that survived multiple comparison correction are reported (see Supplement for further details).

Derealization self-report and 3-month outcomes.

We conducted Pearson correlation analyses to test associations between 2-week derealization and 3-month symptom outcomes. We then completed a series of multiple linear regression analyses to test whether derealization self-report data predicted 3-month symptom outcomes when controlling for sex, age, childhood maltreatment and 2-week posttraumatic stress symptoms. We applied a Hommel multiple testing correction (27) to account for our choice of five clinical associations of interest with derealization. No multiple testing correction was applied to results for control covariates.

vmPFC and 3-month outcomes.

We conducted Pearson correlation analyses to test associations between vmPFC activity and 3-month symptom outcomes. To test whether vmPFC activity during the emotion reactivity task also predicted 3-month outcomes that were found to be significant in the self-report sample, we then completed a series of linear regressions when controlling for MRI scanner, sex, age, childhood maltreatment and 2-week posttraumatic stress symptoms. We conducted 2 separate regressions (one for each significant 3-month outcome in the self-report sample). We applied a Hommel multiple testing correction (27) to account for our choice of two clinical associations of interest with vmPFC activity. No multiple testing correction was applied to results for control covariates.

Results

Sample Characteristics

Participants were 1,464 adults who presented in the emergency department within 72 hours after a trauma exposure. A subset of these individuals also completed an MRI scan approximately 2-weeks after their emergency department visit (N = 145). See Supplement for details on quality control exclusion. Participant demographics and acute trauma exposure types are summarized in Table 1. Approximately 55% of self-report participants (N = 798) and 50% of MRI participants (N = 72) endorsed some level of persistent derealization at 2-weeks. See the Supplement for details on demographic variables associated with derealization.

Table 1.

Demographics and Acute Trauma Exposure Type

Self-report Sample
(n=1464)
MRI Sample
(n=145)
n % n %
Sex assigned at birth
Female 954 65.2 97 66.9
Male 510 34.8 48 33.1
Race/Ethnicity
Hispanic/Latino 174 11.9 26 17.9
White 472 32.2 54 37.2
Black/African American 765 52.3 59 40.7
Asian, Hawaiian, American Indian, Other 49 3.3 6 4.1
missing 4 0.3 0 0
Education
Less than high school 171 11.7 5 3.4
High school diploma / GED 369 25.2 37 25.5
Some college 412 28.1 50 34.5
Associate 213 14.5 14 9.7
Bachelors 192 13.1 25 17.2
Masters 79 5.4 11 7.6
Professional / Doctorate 22 1.5 2 1.3
missing 6 .4 0 0
Yearly Family Income
<$19,000 497 33.9 37 25.5
19,001–35,000 434 29.6 43 29.7
35,001–50,000 194 13.3 25 17.2
50,001–75,000 114 7.8 13 9.0
75,001–100,000 84 5.7 10 6.9
> 100,000 88 6.0 16 11.0
missing 53 3.6 1 0.7
Trauma type
Motor vehicle collision 1138 77.7 107 73.8
Physical assault 139 9.5 16 11.0
Sexual assault 12 0.8 2 1.4
Fall >10’ 16 1.1 1 0.7
Incident causing traumatic stress exposure to many people 5 0.3 1 0.7
Non-motorized collision (e.g., bike, skateboard) 17 1.2 5 3.4
Fall < 10’ 48 3.3 4 2.8
Burns 7 0.5 1 0.7
Animal-related 29 2.0 3 2.1
Other 53 3.6 5 3.4
         
M SD M SD
Age 35.39 12.94 34.66 12.62

Neural Correlates of Persistent Derealization

Emotion Reactivity Task Activity.

A linear regression controlling for MRI scanner, sex, age, childhood maltreatment and 2-week posttraumatic stress symptoms revealed higher levels of derealization were associated with greater vmPFC ROI activity during fearful > neutral face conditions, B = .03, SE = .01, t = 2.60, pcorrected = .030, (ηp2 =.03); Figure 1a, b). This relationship was robust to MRI scanner effects (see supplement). A second and third linear regression tested the same model, but now predicting amygdala and insula ROI activity during fearful > neutral face conditions, respectively. The relationship between derealization and amygdala, B = −.01, SE = .03, t = −.35, pcorrected = .730, (ηp2 = .00), and insula activity, B = −.01, SE = .02, t = .36, pcorrected = .730, (ηp2 = .00), in these models was not significant. Finally, we completed an exploratory whole-brain voxelwise correlation analysis with persistent derealization in the fearful > neutral contrast controlling for the same covariates as those in the ROI analyses; however, no activity survived multiple comparison correction.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Neural Correlates of Derealization. (a) The red 5mm sphere represents the Hopper et al. (2007) left ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) region of interest used in the emotion reactivity (fearful faces) task and resting-state connectivity analysis. (b) vmPFC activity during the emotion reactivity task was positively correlated with 2-week self-report derealization severity, r(143) = .18, p = .030 (c) Connectivity between left vmPFC and right cerebellar lobule VIIIa (orange) was negatively correlated with 2-week derealization scores, r(143) = −.26, p = .001 (d) Connectivity between left vmPFC and right orbitofrontal cortex (yellow) was negatively correlated with 2-week derealization scores, r(143) = −.27, p < .001. Scatterplots and correlation values are for zero-order correlations. Scatterplot dots represent individual participants scores for activity/connectivity and derealization severity. Lines represent the linear line of best fit. Shaded error bars represent +/− 1 standard error of the mean. vmPFC = ventromedial prefrontal cortex. OFC = orbitofrontal cortex.

Resting-state Connectivity.

To further understand how the vmPFC ROI was related to derealization, we completed a whole-brain resting-state seed-based connectivity analysis with the same vmPFC ROI (25) as the seed. When controlling for MRI scanner, sex, age, childhood trauma, and 2-week posttraumatic stress symptom severity, derealization was associated with decreased connectivity between vmPFC and two regions: the right lobule VIIIa in the cerebellum, cluster volume = 92, Peak t = −4.14, MNI coordinates for the peak t = 26, −63, −57 (Figure 1c), and an area in right orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), Brodmann area 47, cluster volume = 74, Peak t = −4.11, MNI coordinates for the peak t = 26, 26, −15 (Figure 1d). In post-hoc analyses, this connectivity was not linked to levels of activity in vmPFC during the emotional reactivity task (see supplement).

Do Self-report and Neural Correlates of Persistent Derealization Predict 3-Month Outcomes?

Self-report Derealization.

Higher 2-week derealization was associated with higher levels of 3-month PTSD, anxiety, depression, pain extent, and impairment (Table 2). A series of multiple linear regressions revealed that 2-week derealization predicted 3-month PTSD (pcorrected=.040) and depression symptoms (pcorrected=.020) even when accounting for sex, age, childhood maltreatment and 2-week PTSD symptoms (Table 3).

Table 2.

Correlations with 3-Month Outcomes

2-week Derealization Self-report and 3-Month outcomes
Mean (SD) 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6.

1. Derealization 2-weeks 1.73 (2.12) -
2. PTSD 3-Months 25.13 (19.13) .53** -
3. Pain Extent 3-Months 7.54 (6.50) .32** .55** -
4. Impairment 3-Months 11.87 (9.37) .29** .66** .52** -
5. Anxiety 3-Months 6.63 (4.70) .42** .83** .45** .62** -
6. Depression 3-Months 53.34 (11.09) .42** .67** .31** .48** .63** -

Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex Activity and 3-Month outcomes
Mean (SD) 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6.

1. vmPFC Fear > Neutral −.01 (.23) -
2. PTSD 3-Months 23.40 (16.86) .23* -
3. Pain Extent 3-Months 7.10 (6.08) .14 .63** -
4. Impairment 3-Months 11.93 (10.06) .05 .65** .57** -
5. Anxiety 3-Months 6.46 (4.58) .11 .84** .52** .64** -
6. Depression 3-Months 53.31 (10.66) .19* .78** .49** .62** .82** -
**

p<.001,

*

p<.05, vmPFC = ventromedial prefrontal cortex

Table 3.

Derealization Self-report Predicting 3-Month Outcomes

B Robust SE t p η2p Adj. R2 Model without Derealization Adj. R2 F df p
PTSD 3-Months
Overall Model 519 .515 222.71 5, 1022 <.001
 Sex .47 .89 .52 .601 .00
 Age .09 .03 3.25 .001 .01
 Childhood Maltreatment .13 .05 2.64 .009 .01
 PTSD 2-weeks .62 .03 19.86 <.001 .28
 Derealization 2-weeks .82 .32 2.58 .010* .01

Pain Extent 3-Months
Overall Model .223 .220 58.69 5, 1000 <.001
 Sex −.77 .39 .92 .355 .00
 Age .36 .01 5.79 <.001 .03
 Childhood Maltreatment .08 .02 3.68 <.001 .01
 PTSD 2-weeks .11 .01 8.06 <.001 .06
 Derealization 2-weeks .27 .13 2.08 .038^ .00

Impairment 3-Months
Overall Model .246 .246 68.59 5, 1029 <.001
 Sex .75 .54 1.39 .165 .00
 Age .10 .02 5.22 <.001 .03
 Childhood Maltreatment .11 .03 3.92 <.001 .02
 PTSD 2-weeks .23 .02 11.97 <.001 .12
 Derealization 2-weeks −.22 .17 −1.28 .202 .00

Anxiety 3-Months
Overall Model .372 .373 125.64 5, 1046 <.001
 Sex .25 .24 1.06 .291 .00
 Age .02 .01 2.32 .020 .01
 Childhood Maltreatment .04 .01 3.01 .003 .01
 PTSD 2-weeks .14 .01 16.16 <.001 .20
 Derealization 2-weeks .05 .08 .59 .557 .00

Depression 3-Months
Overall Model .394 .389 138.32 5, 1050 <.001
 Sex −.77 .58 −1.33 .183 .01
 Age .02 .02 1.20 .231 .00
 Childhood Maltreatment .14 .03 4.87 <.001 .02
 PTSD 2-weeks .29 .02 14.36 <.001 .16
 Derealization 2-weeks .56 .19 2.88 .004* .01

Note. P-values reported in the table are uncorrected values, derealization p-values corrected for multiple comparison are reported in the text.

*

= derealization p-value that survived multiple comparison correction;

^

= derealization p-value that did not survive multiple comparison correction. B = unstandardized estimates. η2p = partial eta squared. Adj R2 = adjusted R2 value

vmPFC Activity.

More vmPFC activity in the fear > neutral face contrast during the emotion reactivity task was associated with higher 3-month PTSD and depression (Table 2). Given self-report derealization predicted 3-month PTSD and depression symptoms, we wanted to test if vmPFC would also predict these outcomes. A series of multiple linear regressions revealed that 2-week vmPFC activity predicted 3-month PTSD symptoms (pcorrected=.022) even when accounting for MRI scanner, sex, age, childhood maltreatment and 2-week posttraumatic stress symptoms (Table 4).

Table 4.

Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex Activity in Fear > Neutral Face Contrast Predicting 3-Month Outcomes

B Robust SE t p η2p Adj. R2 Model without Derealization Adj. R2 F df p
PTSD 3-Months
 Overall Model 390 .366 10.34 8, 109 <.001
  MRI Scanner
   1 vs. 4 4.93 3.31 1.49 .139 .02
   2 vs. 4 1.53 5.89 .26 .795 .00
   3 vs. 4 1.16 3.21 .36 .719 .00
  Sex −2.26 2.82 −.80 .425 .01
  Age .01 .11 .07 .943 .00
  Childhood Maltreatment <.001 .14 0.003 .998 .00
  PTSD 2-weeks .61 .09 7.10 <.001 .32
  vmPFC Activity 12.55 4.88 2.57 .011* .06

Depression 3-Months
 Overall Model .298 .288 7.20 8, 109 <.001
  MRI Scanner
   1 vs. 4 .09 2.45 .04 .971 .00
   2 vs. 4 .12 5.00 .02 .981 .00
   3 vs. 4 −1.13 2.10 −.54 .593 .00
  Sex .19 1.93 .10 .920 .00
  Age −.01 .07 −.15 .882 .00
  Childhood Maltreatment .11 .11 1.06 .290 .01
  PTSD 2-Weeks .32 .07 4.93 <.001 .18
  vmPFC Activity 5.80 2.28 1.77 .080 .03

Note. P-values reported in the table are uncorrected values, vmPFC activity p-values corrected for multiple comparison are reported in the text.

*

= vmPFC activity p-value that survived multiple comparison correction. B = unstandardized estimates. η2p = partial eta squared. Adj R2 = adjusted R2 value. vmPFC = ventromedial prefrontal cortex

The prior results predict dimensional symptom outcomes. We also completed a post-hoc set of analyses to predict PTSD and depression diagnosis using self-report derealization and vmPFC activity (see supplement). Models with 2-week derealization alone as a predictor had adequate discrimination for both depression and PTSD diagnosis at three months. However, improvement in overall discrimination and in sensitivity and specificity were minimal when 2-week derealization score was combined with other clinical predictors. Discrimination for vmPFC activity alone was below acceptable levels for both depression and PTSD diagnosis, and addition of vmPFC to other clinical predictors only modestly improved overall discrimination, sensitivity, and specificity.

Discussion

Despite foundational work on the psychological and neural basis of dissociation, the field has yet to definitively determine whether dissociation is a unique predictor of worse post-trauma psychiatric course. We examined whether the psychological and neural correlates of a specific type of dissociation, derealization, predicted worse psychiatric symptoms in the largest prospective, longitudinal biomarker study of dissociation to date. Here, we demonstrated that self-report and the neural manifestations of persistent (2-week) derealization both predicted 3-month PTSD symptom severity, distinct from a history of childhood maltreatment history and 2-week posttraumatic stress symptoms. This suggests that persistent derealization, following acute trauma, is an early sign of individuals who will likely have worse mental health as time goes on.

In terms of the underlying neural markers, we found that persistent derealization was associated with increased vmPFC activity during an emotion reactivity task when controlling for the severity of childhood maltreatment and current posttraumatic stress symptoms. This finding partially replicates prior work demonstrating a pattern of increased medial PFC activity in the dissociative subtype of PTSD and dissociative identity disorder (15, 16), especially for consciously perceived fearful facial stimuli (29). The vmPFC is consistently involved in emotion regulation, and specifically emotion regulation that is automatic and outside awareness or conscious control (30). This neural activity in part, may underlie emotion over-regulation and the experience of feeling detached from surroundings in the aftermath of traumatic events.

Interestingly, we did not find an association between derealization and amygdala or insula activation in the emotion reactivity task. Models of dissociation implicate corticolimbic inhibition and decreased amygdala/insula activity in emotionally arousing contexts when individuals are feeling dissociative (15, 16). However, a recent systematic review found the direction and strength of amygdala and insula findings in relation to dissociation to be mixed (17). The relationship between dissociation and activity in these structures may be highly dependent on the task context and type of dissociation being studied. More work is necessary to understand the nuances of these potential neural markers of dissociation.

Moreover, we found that at rest, derealization was associated with decreased resting-state connectivity between vmPFC and the posterior lobe of the cerebellum (lobule VIIIa). This region of the cerebellum is largely involved in sensorimotor function and includes a secondary somatotopic representation of the human body (31). Lobule VIII typically demonstrates functional connectivity with anterior prefrontal cortex and primary motor cortex (32). Recent work on the dissociative subtype of PTSD found decreased connectivity in other sensorimotor regions of the cerebellum and cortical regions involved in multisensory integration (33). Contemporary theories of cerebellar function hypothesize that the cerebellum is a master regulator across sensorimotor, cognitive, and affective domains, helping to maintain allostasis (34). Given this, we speculate that decreased synchronization between vmPFC and this region of the cerebellum may contribute to perceptual and affective distortions experienced during derealization (e.g., feelings that surroundings are fading away, unreal, strange). Taken together, the cerebellum is emerging as a key region that has been understudied in trauma-related dissociation.

Additionally, we observed an association between derealization and decreased resting-state connectivity between vmPFC and OFC. Both regions have recently been identified as potential biomarkers of pathological dissociation, though their functional connectivity remains understudied in relation to dissociation (17). The OFC receives a wide array of sensory and visceral input and may serve as a major integration area for this information (35). It has also been implicated in decision making, emotional processing and assigning a reward value and valence to experience (35). It has also been linked to individual differences in anxiety through its role in amygdala regulation (36, 37). The vmPFC is a hub of the default network involved in emotion processing and self-related thinking (38). Typically, OFC has strong connections with cingulate regions, and vmPFC receives inputs from both OFC and cingulate regions (39). This communication is thought to facilitate an assessment of a stimulus’ value and can influence goal directed action (39). Moreover, altered communication between OFC and cingulate regions can impact the output of all medial PFC regions (39). Given this, we hypothesize that decreased synchronization between OFC and vmPFC may also be connected to the increased vmPFC activity during derealization. Furthermore, we hypothesize these altered networks may also contribute to perceptual and affective distortions experienced during derealization (e.g., feelings that familiar surroundings are unreal, unfamiliar, strange, puzzling).

Concerning the relationship between persistent derealization and later symptoms, we found that more severe persistent derealization was associated with increased PTSD, anxiety, and depression symptom severity, pain extent, and functional impairment 3 months after trauma exposure. However, when accounting for sex assigned at birth, age, childhood maltreatment and concurrent posttraumatic stress symptom severity, persistent derealization only predicted later PTSD and depression symptoms. PTSD and depression often co-occur in the aftermath of trauma (40). There is both evidence that co-occurring PTSD and depression may represent a more general traumatic stress factor, and in contrast, that these disorders can be separate constructs with unique predictors and outcomes (41). It is yet unclear whether derealization predicts PTSD and depression as separate entities or whether it predicts a more general traumatic stress reaction in this population, which we hope can be further addressed in future studies. Nonetheless, these results suggest that knowing someone’s levels of derealization in the weeks following trauma exposure will help predict their future depression and PTSD symptoms, distinct from knowing their sex, age, childhood maltreatment history and current posttraumatic stress symptoms. This highlights the importance of assessing for dissociation in the aftermath of traumatic events to identify individuals who are at risk of a worse psychiatric course.

Similarly, brain activity associated with persistent derealization was related to more severe PTSD and depression symptoms later on (i.e., increased vmPFC activity in an emotionally provocative context). When accounting for MRI scanner, sex, age, childhood maltreatment and concurrent posttraumatic stress symptom severity, vmPFC activity still significantly predicted future PTSD symptoms. This suggests that after acute trauma, increased vmPFC activity during an emotionally provocative task may be a signal of worse PTSD symptoms to come.

This result is consistent with other recent findings implicating vmPFC as a central node in prolonged posttraumatic psychopathology. For example, less vmPFC activation during an emotionally provocative task before a difficult combat training was linked to more PTSD symptoms after training (42). A “Goldilocks” level of vmPFC activity during emotionally charged contexts may exist (43). This optimal level of activity may be representative of successful emotion regulation that protects against future PTSD symptoms. In contrast, either an excess or a deficiency in this activity may lead to worse PTSD later on. Alternatively, the vmPFC may be comprised of subregions, some of which are associated with persistent dissociative symptoms (and later worse PTSD) while others are associated with successful emotion regulation (and fewer PTSD symptoms later on).

Several limitations constrain the interpretations of our findings. We have demonstrated that the neural correlates and self-report measures of derealization predict later psychiatric outcomes; however, we have not tested whether they cause these outcomes directly. Moreover, it could be that some derealization preceded the specific traumatic event that brought individuals to the emergency department in this study. Future work is needed to test the causal relationship between trauma-related persistent dissociation and later psychiatric outcomes. Pathological dissociation is a multidimensional construct encompassing a wide range of experiences; however, we focused solely on derealization given its link to impairment severity (2, 3). The AURORA study prioritized measuring many posttraumatic sequalae, which limited the depth of assessment into any one construct. Therefore, our findings may not be generalizable to other types of dissociation. Also, our measure of persistent derealization was limited to a self-report 2-weeks from the acute trauma. Future work may wish to explore dissociation that persists beyond 2-weeks with clinician-administered measures. In addition, the requirement of smartphone ownership may limit the generalizability of this sample. Moreover, we excluded individuals in ongoing domestic violence situations and those with self-inflicted injuries. Interpersonal trauma is associated with more dissociative symptoms and self-harm (44). Additionally, dissociative disorder diagnosis is strongly associated with measures of self-harm, suicidality, and numbers of suicide attempts (45). Therefore, our findings may suffer from a restriction of range in level of participant acuity. Specifically, the prevalence of derealization may have been even higher if individuals in domestic violence situations and those with self-inflicted injuries were eligible for enrollment. We recommend future work include these individuals to both better understand the full range of dissociative symptomatology and to also better aid those with more severe pathology down the line.

Conclusions and Clinical Implications

In the largest prospective, longitudinal biomarker study of dissociation to date, our work suggests persistent derealization is both a psychological and biological marker of worse later psychiatric outcomes. Additionally, we have replicated previous neurobiological work and introduced novel findings demarcating the neural correlates of trauma-related dissociation. Importantly, the activity and connectivity of these regions may serve as potential neural targets for treatment engagement to prevent PTSD.

Prior work suggests that dissociation may undermine seeking treatment for PTSD, highlighting that there may be a need to screen for and treat individuals with prominent dissociative symptoms differently (7). Our work lends urgency to these claims, as persistent derealization is associated with more severe depression, anxiety, pain and posttraumatic stress and greater functional impairment at 3 months following trauma. Clinicians may wish to screen for dissociation in the weeks following trauma, and perhaps in other contexts (e.g., primary care) to identify individuals potentially at risk of a chronic, more severe psychiatric course. Ultimately, we hope this would, in turn, address a presently unmet clinical need for early intervention.

Supplementary Material

supplement

Acknowledgments:

The investigators wish to thank the trauma survivors participating in the AURORA Study. Their time and effort during a challenging period of their lives make our efforts to improve recovery for future trauma survivors possible. We would also like to thank the research staff at McLean Hospital, Emory University, Temple University, and Wayne State University for their efforts and aid. This project was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health K01 MH118467, K00 MH119603, and U01 MH110925US, the US Army Medical Research and Material Command, One Mind, and The Mayday Fund. Data and/or research tools used in the preparation of this manuscript were obtained from the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) Data Archive (NDA). NDA is a collaborative informatics system created by the National Institutes of Health to provide a national resource to support and accelerate research in mental health. Dataset identifier: NIMH Data Archive Digital Object Identifier 10.15154/1521347. This manuscript reflects the views of the authors and may not reflect the opinions or views of any of the funders. Support for title page creation and format was provided by AuthorArranger, a tool developed at the National Cancer Institute.

Footnotes

Competing Interests Statement:

LAML reports unpaid membership on the Scientific Committee for the International Society for the Study of Trauma and Dissociation (ISSTD), grant support from the National Institute of Mental Health, K01 MH118467, and spousal IP payments from Vanderbilt University for technology licensed to Acadia Pharmaceuticals unrelated to the present work. ISSTD and NIMH were not involved in the analysis or preparation of the manuscript. NGH reports grant support from the National Institute of Mental Health, K00 MH119603. In the last three years GDC has received research funding from the NSF, NIH and LifeBell AI, and unrestricted donations from AliveCor, Amazon Research, the Center for Discovery, the Gordon and Betty Moore Foundation, MathWorks, Microsoft Research, the Gates Foundation, Google, One Mind Foundation, and Samsung Research. GDC has financial interest in AliveCor and receives unrestricted funding from the company. He also is the CTO of MindChild Medical and CSO of LifeBell AI and has ownership in both companies. These relationships are unconnected to the current work. LTG receives an honorarium as a member of the Scientific Advisory Board for Sage Bionetworks as well as funding from the NIH. SLR reports grants from NIH during the conduct of the study; personal fees from SOBP (Society of Biological Psychiatry) paid role as secretary, other from Oxford University Press royalties, other from APP (American Psychiatric Publishing Inc.) royalties, other from VA (Veterans Administration) per diem for oversight committee, and other from Community Psychiatry paid board service, including equity outside the submitted work; and Leadership roles on Board or Council for SOBP, ADAA (Anxiety and Depression Association of America), and NNDC (National Network of Depression Centers). SS has received funding from the Florida Medical Malpractice Joint Underwriter’s Association Dr. Alvin E. Smith Safety of Healthcare Services Grant; Allergan Foundation; the NIH/NIA-funded Jacksonville Aging Studies Center (JAX-ASCENT; R33AG05654); and the Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration (1H79TI083101–01); and the Florida Blue Foundation. CWJ reports no direct conflicts related to this paper. He has been an investigator on studies funded by Hologic Inc, Janssen, AstraZeneca, and Vapotherm, for which his department has received research funding. JJ receives consulting payments from Janssen Pharmaceuticals. Over the past 3 years, DAP has received consulting fees from BlackThorn Therapeutics, Boehringer Ingelheim, Compass Pathway, Concert Pharmaceuticals, Engrail Therapeutics, Otsuka Pharmaceuticals, and Takeda Pharmaceuticals; one honorarium from Alkermes, and research funding from NIMH, Dana Foundation, Brain and Behavior Research Foundation, and Millennium Pharmaceuticals. In addition, he has received stock options from BlackThorn Therapeutics. JWS is PI of a collaborative study of the genetics of depression and bipolar disorder sponsored by 23andMe for which 23andMe provides analysis time as in-kind support but no payments. JME reports support from the National Institutes of Health (NIH) through Grant Numbers R01HD079076 & R03HD094577: Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health & Human Development; National Center for Medical Rehabilitation Research. He also reports funding from and New South Wales Health, Spinal Cord Injury Award (2020–2025) and consulting fees (< $15,000 per annum) from Orofacial Therapeutics, LLC. In the past 3 years, RCK was a consultant for Datastat, Inc., Holmusk, RallyPoint Networks, Inc., and Sage Pharmaceuticals. He has stock options in Mirah, PYM, and Roga Sciences. All other authors have no conflicts of interest to disclose. KJR serves on advisory boards or has performed scientific consultation for Takeda, Janssen, Bioxcel and Verily, and he has received sponsored research support from Takeda, Alkermes, Alto Neuroscience, and Brainsway. He receives funding from NIH and the Brain and Behavior Research Fund.

References

  • 1.Spiegel D, Loewenstein RJ, Lewis-Fernández R, et al. : Dissociative disorders in DSM-5. Depress Anxiety 2011; 28:824–852 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Boyd JE, Protopopescu A, O’Connor C, et al. : Dissociative symptoms mediate the relation between PTSD symptoms and functional impairment in a sample of military members, veterans, and first responders with PTSD. Eur J Psychotraumatol 2018; 9:1463794. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Sar V, Alioğlu F, Akyuz G: Depersonalization and derealization in self-report and clinical interview: The spectrum of borderline personality disorder, dissociative disorders, and healthy controls. J Trauma Dissociation 2017; 18:490–506 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5®) American Psychiatric Pub, 2013 [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Putnam FW: Pierre Janet and modern views of dissociation. J Trauma Stress 1989; 2:413–429 [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Mansfield AJ, Kaufman JS, Marshall SW, et al. : Deployment and the use of mental health services among US Army wives. N Engl J Med 2010; 362:101–109 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Stein DJ, Koenen KC, Friedman MJ, et al. : Dissociation in posttraumatic stress disorder: evidence from the world mental health surveys. Biol Psychiatry 2013; 73:302–312 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Soffer-Dudek N: Dissociation and dissociative mechanisms in panic disorder, obsessive–compulsive disorder, and depression: A review and heuristic framework. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice 2014; 1:243–270 [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Duckworth MP, Iezzi T, Archibald Y, et al. : Dissociation and Posttraumatic Stress Symptoms in Patients With Chronic Pain. International Journal of Rehabilitation and Health 2000; 5:129–139 [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Boyd JE, O’Connor C, Protopopescu A, et al. : The contributions of emotion regulation difficulties and dissociative symptoms to functional impairment among civilian inpatients with posttraumatic stress symptoms. Psychol Trauma 2020; 12:739–749 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Kessler RC, Ressler KJ, House SL, et al. : Socio-demographic and trauma-related predictors of PTSD within 8 weeks of a motor vehicle collision in the AURORA study. Mol Psychiatry 2020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 12.van der Velden PG, Wittmann L: The independent predictive value of peritraumatic dissociation for PTSD symptomatology after type I trauma: a systematic review of prospective studies. Clin Psychol Rev 2008; 28:1009–1020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Briere J, Scott C, Weathers F: Peritraumatic and persistent dissociation in the presumed etiology of PTSD. Am J Psychiatry 2005; 162:2295–2301 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Werner KB, Griffin MG: Peritraumatic and persistent dissociation as predictors of PTSD symptoms in a female cohort. J Trauma Stress 2012; 25:401–407 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Lanius RA, Vermetten E, Loewenstein RJ, et al. : Emotion Modulation in PTSD: Clinical and Neurobiological Evidence for a Dissociative Subtype [Internet]. American Journal of Psychiatry 2010; 167:640–647. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Reinders AATS, Willemsen ATM, den Boer JA, et al. : Opposite brain emotion-regulation patterns in identity states of dissociative identity disorder: a PET study and neurobiological model. Psychiatry Res 2014; 223:236–243 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Roydeva MI, Reinders AATS: Biomarkers of Pathological Dissociation: A Systematic Review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2020; 123:120–202 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.McLean SA, Ressler K, Koenen KC, et al. : The AURORA Study: a longitudinal, multimodal library of brain biology and function after traumatic stress exposure. Mol Psychiatry 2020; 25:283–296 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Bernstein DP, Fink L, Handelsman L, et al. : Childhood Trauma Questionnaire. The American Journal of Psychiatry Assessment of family violence: A handbook for researchers and practitioners
  • 20.Dalenberg C, Carlson E: Severity of Dissociative Symptoms-Adult (Brief Dissociative Experiences Scale [DES-B]-Modified) American Psychiatric Association; 2010; [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Weathers FW, Litz BT, Keane TM, et al. : The ptsd checklist for dsm-5 (pcl-5). Scale available from the National Center for PTSD at www.ptsd.va.gov 2013.
  • 22.Pilkonis PA, Choi SW, Reise SP, et al. : Item Banks for Measuring Emotional Distress From the Patient-Reported Outcomes Measurement Information System (PROMIS®): Depression, Anxiety, and Anger. Assessment 2011; 18:263–283 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Leon AC, Olfson M, Portera L, et al. : Assessing psychiatric impairment in primary care with the Sheehan Disability Scale. Int J Psychiatry Med 1997; 27:93–105 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Stevens JS, Jovanovic T, Fani N, et al. : Disrupted amygdala-prefrontal functional connectivity in civilian women with posttraumatic stress disorder. J Psychiatr Res 2013; 47:1469–1478 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Hopper JW, Frewen PA, Van der Kolk BA, et al. : Neural correlates of reexperiencing, avoidance, and dissociation in PTSD: Symptom dimensions and emotion dysregulation in responses to script-driven trauma imagery. J Trauma Stress 2007; 20:713–725 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Long JS, Ervin LH: Using Heteroscedasticity Consistent Standard Errors in the Linear Regression Model. Am Stat 2000; 54:217–224 [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Hommel G: A stagewise rejective multiple test procedure based on a modified Bonferroni test. Biometrika 1988; 75:383–386 [cited 2021 Sep 14] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Chen G, Adleman NE, Saad ZS, et al. : Applications of multivariate modeling to neuroimaging group analysis: a comprehensive alternative to univariate general linear model. Neuroimage 2014; 99:571–588 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Felmingham K, Kemp AH, Williams L, et al. : Dissociative responses to conscious and non-conscious fear impact underlying brain function in post-traumatic stress disorder. Psychol Med 2008; 38:1771–1780 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Etkin A, Büchel C, Gross JJ: The neural bases of emotion regulation. Nat Rev Neurosci 2015; 16:693–700 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Stoodley CJ, Schmahmann JD: Evidence for topographic organization in the cerebellum of motor control versus cognitive and affective processing. Cortex 2010; 46:831–844 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Krienen FM, Buckner RL: Segregated fronto-cerebellar circuits revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity. Cereb Cortex 2009; 19:2485–2497 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Rabellino D, Densmore M, Théberge J, et al. : The cerebellum after trauma: Resting‐state functional connectivity of the cerebellum in posttraumatic stress disorder and its dissociative subtype. Hum Brain Mapp 2018; [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 34.Schmahmann JD, Weilburg JB, Sherman JC: The neuropsychiatry of the cerebellum—insights from the clinic. Cerebellum 2007; 6:254–267 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Kringelbach ML: The human orbitofrontal cortex: linking reward to hedonic experience. Nat Rev Neurosci 2005; 6:691–702 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Kalin NH: Mechanisms underlying the early risk to develop anxiety and depression: A translational approach. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 2017; 27:543–553 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Kenwood MM, Kalin NH, Barbas H: The prefrontal cortex, pathological anxiety, and anxiety disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology 2022; 47:260–275 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Raichle ME: The brain’s default mode network. Annu Rev Neurosci 2015; 38:433–447 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Rolls ET: The cingulate cortex and limbic systems for action, emotion, and memory. Handb Clin Neurol 2019; 166:23–37 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Kessler RC, Sonnega A, Bromet E, et al. : Posttraumatic stress disorder in the National Comorbidity Survey. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1995; 52:1048–1060 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.O’Donnell ML, Creamer M, Pattison P: Posttraumatic Stress Disorder and Depression Following Trauma: Understanding Comorbidity [Internet]. American Journal of Psychiatry 2004; 161:1390–1396. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Gilam G, Lin T, Fruchter E, et al. : Neural indicators of interpersonal anger as cause and consequence of combat training stress symptoms. Psychol Med 2017; 47:1561–1572 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Roeckner AR, Oliver KI, Lebois LAM, et al. : Neural contributors to trauma resilience: A review of longitudinal neuroimaging studies. Trans psychiatry 2021; [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 44.van der Kolk BA, Pelcovitz D, Roth S, et al. : Dissociation, somatization, and affect dysregulation: The complexity of adaption to trauma. Am J Psychiatry 1996; 153:83–93 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Foote B, Smolin Y, Neft DI, et al. : Dissociative disorders and suicidality in psychiatric outpatients. J Nerv Ment Dis 2008; 196:29–36 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

supplement

RESOURCES