Skip to main content
Scientific Reports logoLink to Scientific Reports
. 2022 Sep 8;12:15220. doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-19279-1

Carotenoids improve the development of cerebral cortical networks in formula-fed infant macaques

Oscar Miranda-Dominguez 1,2,, Julian S B Ramirez 3, A J Mitchell 4,8, Anders Perrone 1,2, Eric Earl 9, Sam Carpenter 4, Eric Feczko 1,2, Alice Graham 4, Sookyoung Jeon 5,10, Neal J Cohen 6, Laurie Renner 8, Martha Neuringer 8, Matthew J Kuchan 7, John W Erdman Jr 5, Damien Fair 1,2
PMCID: PMC9458723  PMID: 36076053

Abstract

Nutrition during the first years of life has a significant impact on brain development. This study characterized differences in brain maturation from birth to 6 months of life in infant macaques fed formulas differing in content of lutein, β-carotene, and other carotenoids using Magnetic Resonance Imaging to measure functional connectivity. We observed differences in functional connectivity based on the interaction of diet, age and brain networks. Post hoc analysis revealed significant diet-specific differences between insular-opercular and somatomotor networks at 2 months of age, dorsal attention and somatomotor at 4 months of age, and within somatomotor and between somatomotor-visual and auditory-dorsal attention networks at 6 months of age. Overall, we found a larger divergence in connectivity from the breastfeeding group in infant macaques fed formula containing no supplemental carotenoids in comparison to those fed formula supplemented with carotenoids. These findings suggest that carotenoid formula supplementation influences functional brain development.

Subject terms: Neuroscience, Cognitive neuroscience, Computational neuroscience, Development of the nervous system

Introduction

The human brain dramatically increases in size and complexity during the first 2 years of life1. One of the developmental processes central to this phenomenon is an extraordinary increase in synaptic density25. Increased synaptic density combined with selective synaptic pruning6,7 underpin the changes in connectivity within and between brain networks. Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) can be used to measure correlated patterns of brain activity across brain regions. This technique can also be used to characterize functional networks, sets of brain regions with correlated patterns of activity, that are dedicated to tasks such as motor, visual, attention and executive control processes8. In this way, fMRI can be used to measure functional maturation of networks in the developing brain.

Breastfed human infants experience more rapid overall brain structural maturation than formula fed infants911 which can be detected by assessing myelin microstructure12. Advantages in both brain structural maturation and cognitive performance positively relate to the extent of breastfeeding9,11. Thus, there is a structural basis for the improved cognitive status broadly reported in breastfed infants1317.

The composition of breastmilk differs in a multitude of ways from that of infant formula and may positively impact brain development. For example, lutein and β-carotene are dietary carotenoids routinely found in human milk1820 and lutein is also found in human and macaque breast milk1820, but their concentration in human’s breast milk varies depending on maternal diet18,19. Because mammals cannot synthesize carotenoids, formula-fed human and macaque infants have generally lower plasma and tissue lutein levels than breast-fed infants2022. Lutein is selectively concentrated in the macula23, a retinal structure uniquely found in humans and higher primates and, importantly, it is the predominant carotenoid in the brain of both human24 and macaque infants20 despite not being the most common carotenoid in either the diet or breast milk. In contrast, β-carotene is generally found at higher levels in diet but is found less consistently in brain tissue of human and macaque infants and at dramatically lower levels compared to lutein20,24. Like lutein, β-carotene is a strong antioxidant, but unlike lutein, it can be converted to vitamin A25. Although long term supplementation of adult humans with β-carotene provided benefits in verbal memory and cognition global score26, its role in brain development has not been reported.

Lutein intake is associated with positive outcomes in brain health. During pregnancy, maternal lutein consumption leads to offspring with higher scores of verbal intelligence and behavioral regulation in mid-childhood27, suggesting an impact on the developing fetal brain. In addition, higher breast milk concentrations of lutein and choline are associated with better recognition memory in 5 month old infants28. In 8–10 year old children, macular lutein concentrations are positively correlated with cognition29. In randomized supplementation studies, dietary lutein improved measures of visuomotor processing speed in younger adults30,31. In older adults, lutein remains the predominant carotenoid in the brain32,33 and higher blood levels are associated with improved cognitive status24,3340. Together, these findings indicate that lutein may have an important role in supporting brain development and enhanced cognitive functioning.

Unsupplemented infant formulas contain very low levels of lutein and β-carotene and most formulas are not supplemented to match the higher levels found in human milk20. A reasonable supplemented concentration in infant formula is approximately 230 nmol/L lutein which is the 4th quartile value found in breastmilk collected from women consuming a healthy level of green leafy vegetables and fruit41. Therefore, we supplemented an infant formula with lutein as part of a carotenoid blend that also contain β-carotene, wherein the lutein level matched those driven by a ‘healthy diet’ in human breastmilk and assessed brain development. Given the recent evidence on the potential role of lutein in cognition, it is important to investigate the role of lutein in brain development under controlled conditions.

Nonhuman primates are uniquely suitable to study the impact of lutein on brain development. In contrast to laboratory animals such as rodents and pigs, they accumulate lutein in macular and brain tissue in a manner similar to humans. In addition, they have cerebral cortical systems that have close counterparts in the human brain, such as those supporting visual and somatosensory processing but also higher order processes such as cognition4244. We have previously shown that supplementation with lutein, β-carotene, and lycopene lead to different brain bioaccumulation across the brain with the strongest effect for lutein and only in the cerebellum for β-carotene20. Hence, in the current study, we hypothesize that a supplemental dietary carotenoid blend including lutein and β-carotene would improve brain maturation not only in brain networks related to visual processing but in those supporting cognitive processes in humans. To test this hypothesis we examined brain maturation using functional connectivity via connectotyping45 in infant macaques fed formulas differing in carotenoid concentrations. We also studied a reference group of infant macaques fed with breast milk and maternal diet. Typical brain development in this study is defined by the maturation pattern exhibited by the reference group.

Methods

Animal and diets

Other results from this cohort of infant monkeys have previously been reported20,46,47. All procedures were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of Oregon Health & Science University and carried out in accordance with the Society for Neuroscience Policies on the Use of Animals and Humans in Neuroscience Research, as well as the National Institutes of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals and the ARRIVE (Animal Research: Reporting of In Vivo Experiments) Guidelines. The health of all infant monkeys was continuously monitored by veterinary staff.

On the day after birth, 24 rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) of the Indian-origin subspecies were randomized to 1 of 3 diet groups: a reference mixed-diet (MD) group that received a combination of breast milk and maternal diet: Fiber-Balanced Monkey Diet 5000 (LabDiet Inc., St. Louis, MO, USA) (n = 8), or one of two infant formulas (Table 1). The control formula contained no supplemental carotenoids but was supplemented with synthetic vitamin E (all rac-α tocopherol (αT)) and docosahexaenoic acid (DHA) (UF; n = 8). The UF carotenoid content was (in nmol/L,) 38.6, 2.3, 21.5, and undetected for lutein, zeaxanthin, β-carotene, and lycopene, respectively. In contrast, the supplemented formula was supplemented with carotenoids including lutein, zeaxanthin, β-carotene, and lycopene (237, 19.0, 74.2, and 338 nmol/L, respectively), natural vitamin E (RRR- αT) and DHA (Similac Advance with OptiGRO, Abbott Nutrition, Columbus, OH) (SF; n = 8). There were no differences in the concentration of total αT among groups; αT’s concentration was assessed biweekly in plasma during the study and in different brain areas at the end of the study. Carotenoids concentrations, in contrast, exhibited large differences among groups46. A more complete composition of each diet was previously provided20,46.

Table 1.

Concentration of carotenoids, a-tocopherol and docosahexaenoic acid in infant formulas fed to infant rhesus macaques.

Supplement Supplemented formula (SF) Unsupplemented formula (UF)
Lutein (nmol/L) 237 38.6
Zeaxanthin (nmol/L) 19.0 2.3
β-carotene (nmol/L) 74.2 21.5
Lycopene (nmol/L) 338 Undetected
α-Tocopherol (μmol/L) 20.9 29.7
α-Tocopherol (IU/L) 12.2 12.8
Docosahexaenoic acid (nmol/L) 18.9 18.9

Formula-fed infants were nursery reared by bottle from 1 day after birth according to an established protocol20 and received only their assigned infant formula for the 6-mo study period. In contrast, MD infants were housed with their dam and received a mixture of breast milk and their mother’s diet, Fiber Balanced Monkey Diet 5000 (LabDiet) plus a variety of fresh fruits and vegetables. As required, MD infants were not restricted from eating the maternal diet. The infants generally began ingesting small amounts of monkey diet at approximately 2 months of age then progressively increased their daily intake to approximately 75–90 g of monkey diet and 60 mL breast milk at 6 months of age46. Infant formulas were labeled with a numerical code by Abbott Nutrition, to which all study staff were blinded until analyses were complete. Randomization was stratified by gender and birth weight as previously described20, ending up with 4 females and 3–4 males per group. There were no differences in days of age at sacrifice, final body weight, rate of weight gain or formula intake among the 3 dietary groups20.

One UF monkey died before 2 months of age. Additionally, scans from 2 UF infants did not pass quality checks due to imperfect delineation of gray and white matter or insufficient data for functional connectivity analysis. As the longitudinal analysis required data from each time point, the final sample size was 21 monkeys with 8, 8 and 5 participants in the MD, SF and UF groups, respectively.

Longitudinal fMRI data were collected at 2, 4 and 6 months of age. The dietary intervention period ended when the infants were 6 months of age, at which time the infant macaques were euthanized under pentobarbital anesthesia by a veterinarian as recommended by the Panel on Euthanasia of the American Veterinary Medical Association. Samples from nine brain areas (prefrontal cortex, occipital cortex, superior temporal cortex, striatum, cerebellum, motor cortex, isolated frontal gray matter, frontal white matter, and hippocampus) were dissected to estimate brain concentration of lutein and β-carotene20.

Imaging acquisition and protocol

Animals were sedated with 5 mg/kg of ketamine and intubated, and anesthesia was maintained with 1% isoflurane vaporized in 100% oxygen. Vital signs were continuously monitored, and homeostasis was maintained. Four T1-weighted (TR = 2500 ms, TE = 3.86 ms; 0.5 mm iso-voxels, 128 slices, FOV = 108 × 128 mm) and one T2-weighted (TR = 10640 ms, TE1/TE2 = 11/95 ms; echo train length = 8, slice thickness = 1 mm, in-plane nominal resolution: 0.5 × 0.5 mm, sampling matrix: 256 × 256, and 60 slices) structural images were acquired for registration. We collected thirty minutes of blood-oxygen-level dependent (BOLD) contrast imaging data using a gradient echo-planar imaging (EPI) sequence (TR = 2070 ms, TE = 25 ms, FA = 90o, 1.5 mm iso-voxels, 32 slices with interleaved acquisition, FOV 96 × 96 mm). A field map scan data (TR = 450 ms, TE = 5.19 ms/7.65 ms, FA = 60°, 1.25 × 1.25 × 2 mm voxels, 40 slices, FOV) was also acquired to correct for image distortions in the BOLD signal.

Data was processed using surface-based registration following the standards and steps proposed by the Human Connectome Project (HCP)48, with macaque specific modifications including the use of FSL4951 and FreeSurfer5254. To improve image registration, we incorporated tools from the Advanced Normalization Tools (ANTs)55 version 1.9 (http://stnava.github.io/ANTs/) in combination with in-house established tools. Details of this implementation have been described previously by our group56. Data were visually inspected to ensure that they were processed and registered properly. We used only BOLD data from low head-movement frames57. Resulting BOLD data from each grayordinate of the cortex were parcellated into 82 regions of interest (ROIs) using a predefined macaque cortical parcellation schema58 which can be grouped into 7 functional networks59 (Fig. 1A). See additional details in Supplementary Materials.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Functional networks and group’s average connectivity matrix. (A) Bezgin’s parcellation schema58, color-coded by functional network. Color-coded legend also indicates the number of ROIs per network. (B) Mean connectivity matrix (n = 63, 3 timepoints from 8 MD, 8 SF and 5 UF macaques) grouped by functional networks.

Functional connectivity

Functional connectivity was calculated using connectotyping45 with the parcellated data58. Supplementary Fig. 1 shows the mean connectivity values per group and age. The result was a directed connectivity matrix for each scan with 6642 connections (6642=82×82-1). We symmetrized each matrix (S=(M+M)/2, where M is a connectivity matrix, M is the transpose of the matrix M, and S is the resulting symmetrized matrix) and the resulting matrices had 3321 unique connectivity values. As shown in Fig. 1, connections were assigned to the unique network pair defined by the network each ROI belongs to (Auditory-Auditory, Auditory-Default, …, n = 28)58. Count of connections per network pair are reported in the Supplementary Table 1.

Statistical analysis

We assessed differences in functional connectivity for the factors group (MD, SF and UF), age (2, 4 and 6 mo), functional network pair (n = 28, Auditory-Auditory, Auditory-Default, … See list of all the network pairs in Supplementary Table 1) and their interactions using a 3-way repeated measures ANOVA test in MATLAB. First, a linear mixed effects model was fitted to predict functional connectivity values as a function of group using age and functional network pair as within-subject factors. Next, corrected values were grouped based on between- and within-subject factors, and then a post hoc ANOVA test was used to characterize differences among factors. Mauchly’s test of sphericity was used to test for differences in variance among the groups being compared, and p-values were adjusted accordingly using the correction factor epsilon. Epsilon-adjusted p-values were corrected for multiple comparisons using the Tukey–Kramer method, and 0.05 was used as threshold for significance6062.

As a secondary analysis, we determined if the concentrations of lutein and β-carotene in the brain were associated with functional connectivity using multivariate statistics60,6264. Lycopene did not accumulate in brain despite being included in the diet of supplemented animals. To minimize the risk of overfitting we used regularization via partial least squares regression (PLSR)65 to fit models to predict brain carotenoid concentration as a function of functional connectivity, one model for each functional network pair. Significance of the associations was determined by comparing out-of-sample performance using hold-cross association versus null data (N = 4000).

Because carotenoid concentrations20 were highly correlated among brain areas we calculated two global indices, one for lutein and another for β-carotene using principal component analysis (PCA). As the first component explained most of the observed variance on each case, we used the first component as a predicted variable. PCA scores were normalized using a logarithmic transformation (Boxcox transformation)66, where the logarithmic base was optimized by gradient descent. Associations between lutein and β-carotene indices (PCA scores) and functional connectivity were assessed using imaging data from the last scan (i.e. at 6 mo). We did not attempt to determine causality but rather significant associations. For this reason, it did not matter which variable (carotenoid concentration or functional connectivity) was used as dependent or independent variable.

Model accuracy was estimated by calculating the mean absolute error of the predictions. Performance of the models versus chance was compared by training models using null data for each functional network pair. Models trained with permuted data (4000 times) were also tested with out-of-sample un-permuted data. Significance was determined based on the comparison of the distribution of the mean absolute errors against predicted null-hypothesis data. This comparison was quantified using Cohen’s effect size. Here we report results when predictions had at least a medium effect size (Cohen’s d > 0.5). See additional details in Supplementary Materials.

Results

Group differences in functional connectivity

Functional connectivity was significantly different among functional network pairs (repeated measures ANOVA test, F=46.0,p<<0.01), and for the simultaneous 3-way interaction of the factors: functional network pair, diet, and age (repeated measures ANOVA test, F=1.5,p=0.002), as shown in Table 2. No significant main effects were found for diet or age or any other interaction although the interaction between networks and age was close to the threshold for significance (repeated measures ANOVA test, F=1.3,p=0.08).

Table 2.

Repeated measures ANOVA for the effects of group, age, functional network pair and their interactions.

Effect Sum of squares Degrees of freedom Mean squares F p-value
Group 0.048 2 0.024 1.0 0.39
Age 0.008 2 0.004 0.3 0.76
Networks 8.629 27 0.320 46.0 <<1e−6
Group and age 0.037 4 0.009 0.6 0.64
Networks and group 0.251 54 0.005 0.7 0.97
Networks and age 0.404 54 0.007 1.3 0.08
Networks and group and age 0.916 108 0.008 1.5 0.002

Significant values are given in bold.

Following up on the significant 3-way interaction effect, post-hoc analyses revealed significant differences at the 3 ages explored in this study (2, 4, and 6 months), as shown in Fig. 2 and summarized in Table 3. At 2 months of age, group differences were found only for the Insular Opercular—Somatomotor network pair, as shown in Fig. 2A. The MD group had increased negative connectivity compared to the SF group (difference=0.013,p=0.011, corrected for multiple comparisons), and a trend for increased negative connectivity compared to the UF group (difference=0.011,p=0.067, corrected), whereas the two formula groups were similar (see Supplementary Table 2 for all the paired comparisons). It is important to note that the large difference between the MD and UF groups is not significant due to the large variability of the data (difference=0.016,p=0.121, corrected), as reported in Supplementary Table 2.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Functional network pairs with significant differences in functional connectivity for the interaction of group (MD, SF and UF) and age (2 m, 4 m, 6 m). Circles represent the mean functional connectivity and the bar indicates the interquartile range. Each plot indicates the p-value for each individual repeated measures ANOVA test. Each panel also includes a highly inflated projection of the brain cortex with left-lateral and left-medial views indicating the location of the corresponding functional network pairs.

Table 3.

Post-hoc analysis for the 3-way interaction of diet, age and networks using functional connectivity.

Functional network pair Age Subgroup 1 Subgroup 2 Comparison
Diet Mean fconn Std. error Diet Mean fconn Std. error Group 1–group 2 StdErr p value
InO and SoM 2m MD − 0.018 0.003 SF − 0.005 0.003 − 0.013 0.004 0.011
InO and SoM 2m MD − 0.018 0.003 UF − 0.007 0.003 − 0.011 0.004 0.067
DoA and SoM 4m MD 0.003 0.002 SF − 0.009 0.002 0.012 0.003 0.005
DoA and SoM 4m MD 0.003 0.002 UF − 0.006 0.003 0.009 0.004 0.053
SoM and SoM 6m UF − 0.014 0.005 MD 0.008 0.004 − 0.021 0.006 0.005
SoM and SoM 6m UF − 0.014 0.005 SF 0.005 0.004 − 0.019 0.006 0.011
SoM and Vis 6m UF − 0.025 0.003 SF − 0.011 0.002 − 0.014 0.004 0.005
SoM and Vis 6m UF − 0.025 0.003 MD − 0.014 0.002 − 0.011 0.004 0.021
Aud and DoA 6m MD − 0.015 0.003 UF − 0.002 0.004 − 0.013 0.005 0.038
Aud and Lmb 6m UF − 0.011 0.004 MD 0.001 0.003 − 0.012 0.005 0.072

Each subgroup corresponds to the 3-way interaction of diet, age and networks shown in Fig. 2. Subgroups are sorted by age, then by functional network pair with the lowest p-value. Subgroup 1 has always the largest absolute value of functional connectivity compared to Subgroup 2. p-values < 0.05 highlighted in bold font.

m, age in months; MD, mixed-fed; UF, un-supplemented formula; SF, supplemented formula. InO, Insular-Opercular network; SoM, Somatomotor network; DoA, Dorsal Attention network; Vis, Visual network; Aud, Auditory network; Lmb, Limbic network.

We also found group differences at 4 months of age in the Dorsal Attention and Somatomotor functional network pair (Fig. 2B). The MD group had positive functional connectivity while the formula groups had negative connectivity, with the largest difference between the MD and SF group (difference = 0.012, p = 0.005, corrected). The difference between the MD and UF was just at the threshold for significance (difference = 0.009, p = 0.053, corrected). No significant differences were found between the two formula groups. Multiple significant group differences were found at 6 months of age. Within the Somatomotor network (Fig. 2C), the UF was the only group with negative connectivity and was significantly different compared to both the MD group (difference = 0.021, p = 0.005, corrected) and the SF group (difference = 0.019, p = 0.011, corrected), whereas values for the SF group were similar to the MD group (Supplementary Table 2 reports all the comparisons). Similarly, in the Somatomotor—Visual network pair (Fig. 2D), the UF group had more negative connectivity compared to both the MD group (difference = 0.011, p = 0.021) and the SF group (difference = 0.014, p = 0.021, corrected) at 6 months of age, whereas the SF group did not differ from the MD group. For the Auditory—Dorsal Attention network pair at 6 months (Fig. 2E), the UF group had null connectivity (meanfunctionalconnectivity=-0.002,standarderror=0.004) and was significantly different from the MD group that had negative connectivity (difference = 0.013, p = 0.038, corrected). We found no other differences.

Finally, in the Auditory and Limbic network pair (Fig. 2F), the UF trended to negative connectivity compared to the MD group (difference = 0.012, p = 0.072, corrected), with no differences among all the other groups.

Composite index of lutein and  β-carotene concentrations

Lutein concentrations in the nine brain areas studied (prefrontal cortex, occipital cortex, superior temporal cortex, striatum, cerebellum, motor cortex, isolated frontal gray matter, frontal white matter, and hippocampus) were highly correlated. The same was true of β-carotene in the nine brain areas. The median Pearson correlation value (interquartile range) was 0.98 (0.97–0.99) for lutein and 0.56 (0.32–0.67) for β-carotene, as shown in Fig. 3A,E. Therefore, for each carotenoid we calculated a global concentration index using PCA. The first PCA component accounted for 97.9% and 58.2% of the observed variance for lutein and β-carotene, respectively (Fig. 3C,G). The first component was also highly correlated with the concentration of the corresponding carotenoid for each brain area (from 0.980 to 0.997 for lutein and 0.31 to 0.90 for β-carotene, as also shown in Fig. 3B,F), suggesting that the first PCA component can be used effectively as a surrogate for carotenoid concentration. In preparation for PLSR models, PCA first components were normalized using a logarithmic transformation, as shown in Fig. 3D,H.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Carotenoid concentrations and composite index. (A) Correlation of lutein concentrations across brain areas with color-coded numerical scale and a companion insert displaying histograms with the concentration of lutein in each brain area, color-coded by group: MD, mixed-diet (green); UF, unsupplemented formula (blue); SF, supplemented formula (orange). (B) Correlation of the first PCA component with the concentration of lutein for each brain area using the same color-coded scale used in (A). (C) Explained variance for each component after orthogonalization of lutein’s concentration across brain areas using PCA. (D) PCA’s first component row-scores (y-axis) and transformed scores using a logarithmic (BoxCox) transformation. Similar data for β-carotene is shown in (EH).

Associations between functional connectivity and brain concentration of carotenoids

Associations between functional connectivity and carotenoids' concentration were estimated using the data closest in time of tissue collection, i.e., using the brain scan acquired at 6 mo. We found that functional connectivity was significantly associated with brain concentration of lutein and β-carotene, as accounted by PLSR models. For lutein, only models trained with connectivity data for the Auditory and Limbic network pair predicted out-of-sample data (i.e., normalized first PCA of lutein’s concentration) with an effect size > 0.5 (Cohen’s d = 0.72, predicting out-of-sample scores versus predicting null data). In other words, the mean absolute error between the predicted and the composite index of actual concentrations was significantly smaller than the randomized permuted data (null-hypothesis). Predicted values and the first PLSR component were also highly correlated (R=0.84), as shown in Fig. 4A. For β-carotene, three functional network pairs predicted out-of-sample data with an effect size > 0.5. Models trained with connectivity data from the Auditory and Insular-Opercular network pair predicted out-of-sample composite index values with a large effect size (Cohen’s d = 0.84), as shown in Fig. 4B. Models trained with data from the Auditory and Dorsal Attention and Auditory and Limbic pairs predicted out-of-sample outcome with a medium effect size (Cohen’s d = 0.71 and 0.61, respectively), as shown in Fig. 4C,D. Predicted values and the first PLSR components were also highly correlated (0.82, 0.84 and 0.81 for Aud and InO, Aud and DoA, and Aud and Lmb, respectively), as also shown in Fig. 4B–D.

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Associations between functional connectivity and carotenoid brain concentration. (A) Lutein brain concentration (first PLSR component) was associated with functional connectivity between the Auditory and Limbic networks (see Fig. 1 for network color codes). (BD. β-carotene brain concentration (first PLSR component) was associated with functional connectivity between (B). Auditory and Insular-Opercular; (C) Auditory and Dorsal Attention; and (D) Auditory and Limbic networks. Column 1 indicates the location of each functional network pair (see Fig. 1 for color codes) in a highly inflated projection of the brain cortex with left-lateral and left-medial views. Column 2 shows the distribution of the out-of-sample mean absolute errors (n = 1330, yellow), against null-hypothesis data (n = 4000, black). Column 3 is a scatter plot displaying, for each subject, the predicted outcome and the first PLSR component, color-coded by group: MD, breastfed (green); UF, un-supplemented formula (blue); SF, supplemented formula (orange).

Discussion

We aimed to identify differences in brain maturation using functional connectivity in a well-controlled diet intervention protocol using a highly translational macaque model. We have previously reported that this intervention led to differential concentrations of lutein on different brain areas. β-carotene, in contrast, only showed a difference across groups in the cerebellum. This follow-up study aimed to identify differences in brain maturation patterns among groups. Our observations generally indicated that by 6 months of age the UF formula resulted in a larger divergence from the pattern of functional connectivity observed in the MD group, a naturally fed group of macaque infants. The UF formula was not supplemented with lutein/carotenoids so that carotenoid intakes were markedly lower than that of the other groups, as indicated in Table 1. In contrast, the SF formula was supplemented with lutein and other carotenoids, and its 6-month pattern of functional connectivity was more like that of the MD infants. The functional connectivity patterns of the formula groups were different for the somatomotor and the somatomotor-visual networks implicating carotenoid supplementation in the development of those networks. In addition, the SF group had a maturation pattern similar to the pattern observed for the MD group in the auditory and dorsal attention network pair and the auditory and limbic network pair, which was not the case for the UF group.

Interestingly, the UF and SF groups showed similar patterns at 2- and 4-months. The SF group was different from the MD group at 2 months of age for insular opercular-somatomotor connectivity; however, this difference disappeared at 4 and 6 months. At 4 months of age, the MD group had positive connectivity in the dorsal attention-somatomotor network pair, whereas both formula groups had negative connectivity, and were not different from each other. Overall, we found that supplementation may take some time to take effect, since by 6-mo of age the SF group showed more similar patterns to the MD group, differing from the UF group. This finding indicated supplementation of formula with carotenoids influenced the development of these brain networks. Previous results from these monkeys20 implicate lutein as being an important carotenoid in brain maturation since its concentration in the infant’s brains was dramatically higher than those of the other carotenoids. To understand the relevance of our network observations, we compared the formula-fed groups to the MD group through post hoc comparisons.

It is challenging to disentangle the combined effect of nutrition and rearing conditions on brain maturation when comparing laboratory reared formula-fed infants to infants that were breastfed by their dam. Differences in rearing of non-human primates is known to have significant impact on social behavior, social cognition and affective behavior, as well as brain development6769. With this in mind, we conducted a post hoc analysis to assess the directional context of the differences detected on network maturation between the formula fed groups. The post hoc analysis revealed a distinct pattern for both the somatomotor and the somatomotor-visual network pairs at 6 months of age. In both cases, the UF group was significantly different from the MD group, but the SF group was not. The post hoc analysis also revealed additional, less distinct 6-month connectivity effects in the auditory-dorsal attention and auditory-limbic network pairs. In both cases, connectivity in UF infants was different from that in the MD group. Notably, for those two functional pairs the only significant difference in connectivity values were for the UF versus MD groups. Taken together, these findings suggest that lack of supplementation led to a divergent maturation pattern in comparison to the MD group. These observations generally indicated that by 6 months of age the UF formula resulted in a larger divergence from the pattern of functional connectivity observed in the MD group in the somatomotor, somotomotor and visual, auditory and dorsal attention and auditory and limbic systems. We consider this to be an important observation since the SF formula was supplemented with nutrient compounds not added to most commercially-marketed infant formulas; most are not supplemented with lutein or the other carotenoids. While unsupplemented formulas have some baseline carotenoid content, such concentration levels might not be sufficient.

Non-human primates offer a unique translational opportunity to study the impact of nutrition on brain development. They, like humans, deposit lutein in the brain and retina20. In addition, non-human primates have high cognitive capacity, exhibit social behavior and have cerebral cortical areas and networks similar to those found in the human brain4244. This allows the controlled study of early dietary interventions on functional cortical organization later in life. For example, a prior study in macaques showed that dietary omega-3 fatty acids may increase functional connectivity in early visual pathways and may have an impact on large-scale functional network organization, including the dorsal attention, and cingulo-opercular networks70. Similarly, here we found differences between the UF and the MD group within the Auditory and Dorsal Attention networks.

Our observations of connectivity differences within the somatomotor network pairs are consistent with human research which supports positive effects of lutein on the performance of tasks requiring visual-motor and vestibular-motor coordination. Renzi et al.71 found that macular pigment optical density, a measure of lutein and zeaxanthin concentration, was positively correlated with reaction time and balance in adults. These processes are supported by the integration of the motor cortex with visual stimuli (visual cortex) and vestibular function (insular cortex). Motor cortex lutein concentration is implicated as a causative factor by these findings since macular pigment optical density is highly correlated with lutein concentrations in the motor cortex32,35,72. Additionally, in human infants, the development of motor skills was associated with transitions from disperse to focal brain connectivity patterns at the ages of 6 to 12 months73. Our findings suggest that a diet lacking lutein might lead to atypical maturation patterns of functional connectivity of the somatomotor network with other networks. This may be important for the development of a variety of motor cortex dependent behavioral responses beyond simple components of movements, including defensive movements, reaching and hand-to-mouth movements, as reported in primate research74. Additional research is necessary to test this possibility.

Previous research revealed that concentrations of lutein across nine brain areas in these infants were significantly increased by carotenoid supplementation, but the MD group had several-fold higher concentrations compared to the SF group20. Levels of β-carotene did not differ between the formula groups and were only slightly higher in some regions in the MD group20. Here, we quantified associations of network connectivity patterns with a global concentration index for each carotenoid. Brain levels of both lutein and β-carotene were positively correlated with connectivity in the auditory-limbic network pair, while β-carotene was correlated with connectivity of the auditory network with the insular-opercular and dorsal-attention networks, both key networks involved in cognitive function. It is not known why these carotenoids would have selective effects on the connectivity of the auditory cortex with both the limbic system and higher-order cortical regions, but this effect calls for further study. In this regard, it is relevant to mention that there are reports linking impaired connectivity between the auditory and limbic networks with tinnitus. Tinnitus is the sensation of hearing noises without a real external auditory signal and might be related to oxidative stress. One of the hypotheses supported by neuroimaging findings7577 is that tinnitus emerges after the inability of the limbic system to suppress a “tinnitus” signal caused by degeneration of the auditory cortex78. While our findings cannot in any way suggest a causal mechanism between tinnitus and carotene supplementation, they do indicate the networks whose functional connectivity is highly correlated with carotene concentration might be more susceptible to oxidative stress caused by carotene deficiencies.

Functional MRI offers a unique opportunity to non-invasively characterize functional brain development and can also be used to bridge findings across species since some human-defined brain systems are preserved in non-human primates and even rodents79,80. This modality utilizes the blood-oxygenation-level-dependent (BOLD) signal, which measures changes in the magnetic properties of oxygenated and de-oxygenated hemoglobin in the brain’s blood81. This signal combines non-linearly many complex physiological processes. However, if there is a local change in neural activity, that brain area will also exhibit a change in signal intensity that can be detected by the MRI scanner, even if the participant performs no activity when the data is acquired82. Patterns of brain connectivity might support behavioral differences related to higher order processes8 such as executive function, learning and memory or self-regulation83,84, to name a few. In this study we found significant differences in connectivity patterns for the 3-way interaction of diet, age, and networks. As described before, those changes landed not only in the visual cortex but also in systems that in humans support cognition. Since the concentration of carotenoids was predicted by connectivity patterns in those systems, we believe the observed differences in maturation patterns are directly related to differences in diet. It is important to mention, however, that this study did not aim to characterize behavioral differences related to differences in diet.

Functional MRI, however, has inherent challenges driven by both signal to noise ratio, head-movement and the need to properly align scans from different subjects. We used validated methods to overcome these methodological challenges and the relatively small number of infant macaques used in this study. Specifically, we used surface-based registration85 and connectotyping45. Surface-based registration can improve brain alignment, the signal-to-noise ratio and cut out confounding signal from white matter, cerebrospinal fluid, and bone. Additionally, it helps to avoid overlap in areas of cortical folding, where regions containing different functional information may be close in Euclidian distance in volume space, but further away from another on the cortical sheet. In addition, by using connectotyping we can more accurately estimate patterns of brain connectivity using limited data45,86.

This study has several limitations, including the inability to isolate the contributions of individual carotenoids and the relatively short duration of the study. We acknowledge that the ideal design would have required more than three groups in order to test the individual effects and their interactions. Given the challenges of a large per group sample in this longitudinal macaque study, we decided to combine carotenoid/lutein supplementation and different sources of α-tocopherol. As previously reported46, there were no differences in the brain concentration of total α-tocopherol among groups for the nine brain regions assessed46. In contrast, brain lutein concentration was dramatically higher than that of β-carotene in the MD and SF groups, but not the UF group20 and no lycopene was found in any of the nine brain regions tested20. Thus, we believe that higher brain lutein is the most likely driving force behind the observed changes in brain maturation. However, potential interactions between lutein, β-carotene and source of α-tocopherol cannot be ruled out. It is of note that the α-tocopherol natural and synthetic stereoisomer profiles in the brains of the MD and SF infants were markedly different, while the UF group was intermediate42. This pattern suggests that α-tocopherol source was not important in the network pairs described here. Another limitation corresponds to the small sample sizes inherent to most nonhuman primate developmental studies. However, the differences between the formula groups are clear and not diminished by the sample size constraint. While, in general, large samples are required to derive strong conclusions about brain behavior associations87, there are alternatives that are as effective as having large samples, including the design of hypothesis-driven interventions, the use of longitudinal data and using methods to improve the signal-to-noise ratio of the variables of interest88. This study used a well-designed intervention based on diet primarily. We also used longitudinal data acquired at 2, 4 and 6 months of age and we used several procedures to maximize the data signal to noise ratio, including surface-based registration, connectotyping and a rigorous QC based on structural and functional data, where we excluded data from scans with imperfect delineation of gray and white matter or insufficient data for functional connectivity analysis. In addition, we used a stringent repeated measures ANOVA to characterize any potential difference among the variables of interest. This positive finding was followed up by post-hoc stats to characterize the cases driving the differences. However, we acknowledge that while data was acquired in a critical developmental period (2–6 months of age) where the most dramatic changes in brain structure and function occur, a larger sample size would improve the detection of smaller effects. Despite these limitations, repeated measures ANOVA approach was able to identify significant differences in functional connectivity within specific brain networks.

Previous reports in these infants indicated that white and gray matter development measured by fractional anisotropy was greater in the MD group compared with both formula groups47. Here, we found that network connectivity of Somatomotor, Somatomotor-Visual, Auditory-Dorsal Attention, and Auditory-Limbic networks in the SF group more closely resembled the MD group when compared to the UF group. Results of this longitudinal study suggest that supplementation may take some time to take effect, since by 6-mo of age the SF group showed more similar patterns to the MD group, differing from the UF group. These findings implicate a role for lutein, and possibly β-carotene, in the development of these network pairs. Support for this conclusion was provided by associations between brain concentrations of lutein and β-carotene and connectivity between the auditory network and networks subserving emotion regulation and cognitive function. In conclusion, these findings suggest that variation in carotenoid intake are related to aspects of functional brain development. Importantly, a formula with lower carotenoid levels resulted in the largest divergence from breastfeeding in functional connectivity.

Supplementary Information

Acknowledgements

We thank Amy Moore for their help proofreading the different versions of the manuscript.

Author contributions

O.M-D., J.R., A.J.M., A.P., E.E., S.C., E.F., A.G., S.J., N.C., L.R., M.N., M.J.K., J.W.E.J., D.F. Conception of the work; D.F., O.M-D., M.N., M.J.K., J.W.E. Design of the work; D.F., O.M-D., M.J.K., J.W.E., D.F., N.C., M.N. The acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data; O.M-D., J.R., A.J.M., A.P., E.E., S.C., E.F., A.G., S.J., N.C., L.R., M.N., M.J.K., J.W.E., D.F. The creation of new software used in the work: O.M-D., J.R., A.J.M., A.P., E.E., S.C., E.F. Have drafted the work or substantively revised it: O.M-D., J.R., A.J.M., A.P., E.E., S.C., E.F., A.G., S.J., N.C., L.R., M.N., M.J.K., J.W.E., D.F. All authors have approved the submitted version (and any substantially modified version that involves the author's contribution to the study) and have agreed both to be personally accountable for the author's own contributions and to ensure that questions related to the accuracy or integrity of any part of the work, even ones in which the author was not personally involved, are appropriately investigated, resolved, and the resolution documented in the literature.

Funding

This work was supported by Abbott Laboratories through the Center for Nutrition, Learning, and Memory at the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign (JWE, MN), NIH grant P51OD011092 (Oregon National Primate Research Center Core grant), the Tartar Trust Fellowship (Miranda-Dominguez), OHSU Parkinson Center of Oregon Pilot Grant GBNEU0408A (Miranda-Dominguez).

Data availability

The datasets generated during and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Competing interests

OM-D, JR, AJM, AP, EE, SC, EF, AG declare no potential conflict of interest. MJK is employed by Abbott Laboratories.

Footnotes

Publisher's note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41598-022-19279-1.

References

  • 1.Dobbing J, Sands J. Quantitative growth and development of human brain. Arch. Dis. Child. 1973;48:757–767. doi: 10.1136/adc.48.10.757. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Chu D, Huttenlocher PR, Levin DN, Towle VL. Reorganization of the hand somatosensory cortex following perinatal unilateral brain injury. Neuropediatrics. 2000;31:63–69. doi: 10.1055/s-2000-7475. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Huttenlocher PR. Synaptic density in human frontal cortex - developmental changes and effects of aging. Brain Res. 1979;163:195–205. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(79)90349-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Huttenlocher PR, Dabholkar AS. Regional differences in synaptogenesis in human cerebral cortex. J. Comp. Neurol. 1997;387:167–178. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19971020)387:2&#x0003c;167::AID-CNE1&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-Z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Glantz LA, Gilmore JH, Hamer RM, Lieberman JA, Jarskog LF. Synaptophysin and postsynaptic density protein 95 in the human prefrontal cortex from mid-gestation into early adulthood. Neuroscience. 2007;149:582–591. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2007.06.036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Huttenlocher PR, de Courten C, Garey LJ, Van der Loos H. Synaptogenesis in human visual cortex–evidence for synapse elimination during normal development. Neurosci. Lett. 1982;33:247–252. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(82)90379-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Dubois J, Kostovic I, Judas M. Development of structural and functional connectivity. Brain Map. 2015;2:423–437. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-397025-1.00360-2. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Fox MD, Raichle ME. Spontaneous fluctuations in brain activity observed with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2007;8:700–711. doi: 10.1038/nrn2201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Isaacs EB, et al. Impact of breast milk on intelligence quotient, brain size, and white matter development. Pediatr. Res. 2010;67:357–362. doi: 10.1203/PDR.0b013e3181d026da. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Hallowell SG, Spatz DL. The relationship of brain development and breastfeeding in the late-preterm infant. J. Pediatr. Nurs. 2012;27:154–162. doi: 10.1016/j.pedn.2010.12.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Kafouri S, et al. Breastfeeding and brain structure in adolescence. Int. J. Epidemiol. 2013;42:150–159. doi: 10.1093/ije/dys172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Deoni SCL, et al. Breastfeeding and early white matter development: A cross-sectional study. Neuroimage. 2013;82:77–86. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.090. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Quigley MA, et al. Breastfeeding is associated with improved child cognitive development: A population-based cohort study. J. Pediatr. 2012;160:25–32. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2011.06.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Boucher O, et al. Association between breastfeeding duration and cognitive development, autistic traits and ADHD symptoms: A multicenter study in Spain. Pediatr. Res. 2017;81:434–442. doi: 10.1038/pr.2016.238. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Lenehan SM, et al. The impact of short-term predominate breastfeeding on cognitive outcome at 5 years. Acta Paediatr. 2020;109:982–988. doi: 10.1111/apa.15014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Kim KM, Choi J-W. Associations between breastfeeding and cognitive function in children from early childhood to school age: A prospective birth cohort study. Int. Breastfeed. J. 2020;15:83. doi: 10.1186/s13006-020-00326-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Kim J. Relations among maternal employment, depressive symptoms, breastfeeding duration, and body mass index trajectories in early childhood. J. Korean Soc. Matern. Child Heal. 2020;24:75–84. doi: 10.21896/jksmch.2020.24.2.75. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Canfield LM, et al. Multinational study of major breast milk carotenoids of healthy mothers. Eur. J. Nutr. 2003;42:133–141. doi: 10.1007/s00394-003-0403-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Erick M. Breast milk is conditionally perfect. Med. Hypotheses. 2018;111:82–89. doi: 10.1016/j.mehy.2017.12.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Jeon S, et al. Lutein is differentially deposited across brain regions following formula or breast feeding of infant rhesus macaques. J. Nutr. 2018;148:31–39. doi: 10.1093/jn/nxx023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Bettler J, Zimmer JP, Neuringer M, DeRusso PA. Serum lutein concentrations in healthy term infants fed human milk or infant formula with lutein. Eur. J. Nutr. 2010;49:45–51. doi: 10.1007/s00394-009-0047-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Mackey AD, et al. Plasma carotenoid concentrations of infants are increased by feeding a milk-based infant formula supplemented with carotenoids. J. Sci. Food Agric. 2013;93:1945–1952. doi: 10.1002/jsfa.5996. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Landrum JT, Bone RA. Lutein, zeaxanthin, and the macular pigment. Arch. Biochem. Biophys. 2001;385:28–40. doi: 10.1006/abbi.2000.2171. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Vishwanathan R, Kuchan MJ, Sen S, Johnson EJ. Lutein and preterm infants with decreased concentrations of brain carotenoids. J. Pediatr. Gastroenterol. Nutr. 2014;59:659–665. doi: 10.1097/MPG.0000000000000389. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Eggersdorfer M, Wyss A. Carotenoids in human nutrition and health. Arch. Biochem. Biophys. 2018;652:18–26. doi: 10.1016/j.abb.2018.06.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Grodstein F, Kang JH, Glynn RJ, Cook NR, Gaziano JM. A randomized trial of beta carotene supplementation and cognitive function in men: The Physicians’ Health Study II. Arch. Intern. Med. 2007;167:2184–2190. doi: 10.1001/archinte.167.20.2184. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Mahmassani HA, et al. Maternal intake of lutein and zeaxanthin during pregnancy is positively associated with offspring verbal intelligence and behavior regulation in mid-childhood in the project viva cohort. J. Nutr. 2021 doi: 10.1093/jn/nxaa348. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Cheatham CL, Sheppard KW. Synergistic effects of human milk nutrients in the support of infant recognition memory: An observational study. Nutrients. 2015;7:9079–9095. doi: 10.3390/nu7115452. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Walk AM, et al. From neuro-pigments to neural efficiency: The relationship between retinal carotenoids and behavioral and neuroelectric indices of cognitive control in childhood. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2017;118:1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2017.05.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Renzi LM, Hammond BR. The relation between the macular carotenoids, lutein and zeaxanthin, and temporal vision. Ophthalmic Physiol. Opt. 2010;30:351–357. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-1313.2010.00720.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Bovier ER, Renzi LM, Hammond BR. A double-blind, placebo-controlled study on the effects of lutein and zeaxanthin on neural processing speed and efficiency. PLoS ONE. 2014;9:e108178. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0108178. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Craft NE, Haitema TB, Garnett KM, Fitch KA, Dorey CK. Carotenoid, tocopherol, and retinol concentrations in elderly human brain. J. Nutr. Health Aging. 2004;8:156–162. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Johnson EJ, et al. Relationship between serum and brain carotenoids, α-tocopherol, and retinol concentrations and cognitive performance in the oldest old from the Georgia Centenarian Study. J. Aging Res. 2013;2013:951786. doi: 10.1155/2013/951786. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Feeney J, et al. Low macular pigment optical density is associated with lower cognitive performance in a large, population-based sample of older adults. Neurobiol. Aging. 2013;34:2449–2456. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2013.05.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Johnson EJ. A possible role for lutein and zeaxanthin in cognitive function in the elderly. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2012;96:1161S–S1165. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.112.034611. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Renzi LM, Dengler MJ, Puente A, Miller LS, Hammond BR. Relationships between macular pigment optical density and cognitive function in unimpaired and mildly cognitively impaired older adults. Neurobiol. Aging. 2014;35:1695–1699. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2013.12.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Johnson EJ, et al. Cognitive findings of an exploratory trial of docosahexaenoic acid and lutein supplementation in older women. Nutr. Neurosci. 2008;11:75–83. doi: 10.1179/147683008X301450. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Power R, et al. Supplemental retinal carotenoids enhance memory in healthy individuals with low levels of macular pigment in a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled clinical trial. J. Alzheimers. Dis. 2018;61:947–961. doi: 10.3233/JAD-170713. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Zamroziewicz MK, et al. Parahippocampal cortex mediates the relationship between lutein and crystallized intelligence in healthy, older adults. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2016;8:1–9. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2016.00297. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Lindbergh CA, et al. Relationship of lutein and zeaxanthin levels to neurocognitive functioning: An fMRI study of older adults. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2017;23:11–22. doi: 10.1017/S1355617716000850. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Xu X, Zhao X, Berde Y, Low YL, Kuchan MJ. Milk and plasma lutein and zeaxanthin concentrations in Chinese breast-feeding mother-infant dyads with healthy maternal fruit and vegetable intake*. J. Am. Coll. Nutr. 2019;38:179–184. doi: 10.1080/07315724.2018.1490934. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Van Essen DC, Drury HA. Structural and functional analyses of human cerebral cortex using a surface-based atlas. J. Neurosci. 1997;17:7079–7102. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-18-07079.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Bullmore E, Sporns O. Complex brain networks: Graph theoretical analysis of structural and functional systems. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2009;10:186. doi: 10.1038/nrn2575. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Markov NT, et al. A weighted and directed interareal connectivity matrix for macaque cerebral cortex. Cereb. Cortex. 2014;24:17–36. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs270. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Miranda-Dominguez O, et al. Connectotyping: Model based fingerprinting of the functional connectome. PLoS ONE. 2014;9:e111048. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0111048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Kuchan MJ, et al. Infant rhesus macaque brain α-tocopherol stereoisomer profile is differentially impacted by the source of α-tocopherol in infant formula. J. Nutr. 2020;150:2305–2313. doi: 10.1093/jn/nxaa174. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Liu Z, et al. The effects of breastfeeding versus formula-feeding on cerebral cortex maturation in infant rhesus macaques. Neuroimage. 2019;184:372–385. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.09.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Glasser MF, et al. The minimal preprocessing pipelines for the Human Connectome Project. Neuroimage. 2013;80:105–124. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Smith SM, et al. Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. Neuroimage. 2004;23:S208–S219. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Jenkinson M, Beckmann CF, Behrens TEJ, Woolrich MW, Smith SM. FSL. Neuroimage. 2012;62:782–790. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Woolrich MW, et al. Bayesian analysis of neuroimaging data in FSL. Neuroimage. 2009;45:S173–S186. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.10.055. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Dale AM, Fischl B, Sereno MI. Cortical surface-based analysis. I. Segmentation and surface reconstruction. Neuroimage. 1999;9:179–194. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1998.0395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Desikan RS, et al. An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. Neuroimage. 2006;31:968–980. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Fischl B, Dale AM. Measuring the thickness of the human cerebral cortex from magnetic resonance images. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2000;97:11050–11055. doi: 10.1073/pnas.200033797. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Avants BB, et al. A reproducible evaluation of ANTs similarity metric performance in brain image registration. Neuroimage. 2011;54:2033–2044. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.09.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Ramirez JSB, et al. Maternal interleukin-6 is associated with macaque offspring amygdala development and behavior. Cereb. Cortex. 2019 doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhz188. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Power JD, Barnes KA, Snyder AZ, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. Spurious but systematic correlations in functional connectivity MRI networks arise from subject motion. Neuroimage. 2012;59:2142–2154. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Bezgin G, Vakorin VA, van Opstal AJ, McIntosh AR, Bakker R. Hundreds of brain maps in one atlas: Registering coordinate-independent primate neuro-anatomical data to a standard brain. Neuroimage. 2012;62:67–76. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.04.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Grayson DS, et al. The rhesus monkey connectome predicts disrupted functional networks resulting from pharmacogenetic inactivation of the amygdala. Neuron. 2016;91:453–466. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.06.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Rudolph MD, et al. Maternal IL-6 during pregnancy can be estimated from newborn brain connectivity and predicts future working memory in offspring. Nat. Neurosci. 2018;21:765–772. doi: 10.1038/s41593-018-0128-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Kovacs-Balint Z, et al. Early developmental trajectories of functional connectivity along the visual pathways in rhesus monkeys. Cereb. Cortex. 2019;29:3514–3526. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhy222. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Miranda-Domínguez Ó, et al. Lateralized connectivity between globus pallidus and motor cortex is associated with freezing of gait in Parkinson’s disease. Neuroscience. 2020;443:44–58. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2020.06.036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Rudolph MD, et al. At risk of being risky: The relationship between “brain age” under emotional states and risk preference. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2017;24:93–106. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2017.01.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Silva-Batista C, et al. Cortical thickness as predictor of response to exercise in people with Parkinson’s disease. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2021;42:139–153. doi: 10.1002/hbm.25211. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Rosipal, R., Kr, N., Krämer, N., Kr, N. & Krämer, N. Overview and recent advances in partial least squares. Int. Stat. Optim. Perspect. Work. Subspace, Latent Struct. Featur. Sel. 34–51 (2005).
  • 66.Montgomery DC. Design and Analysis of Experiments. 6. Wiley; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Bogart SL, Bennett AJ, Schapiro SJ, Reamer LA, Hopkins WD. Different early rearing experiences have long-term effects on cortical organization in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) Dev. Sci. 2014;17:161–174. doi: 10.1111/desc.12106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Corcoran CA, et al. Long-term effects of differential early rearing in rhesus macaques: Behavioral reactivity in adulthood. Dev. Psychobiol. 2012;54:546–555. doi: 10.1002/dev.20613. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Sánchez MM, Hearn EF, Do D, Rilling JK, Herndon JG. Differential rearing affects corpus callosum size and cognitive function of rhesus monkeys. Brain Res. 1998;812:38–49. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(98)00857-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Grayson DS, Kroenke CD, Neuringer M, Fair DA. Dietary omega-3 fatty acids modulate large-scale systems organization in the rhesus macaque brain. J. Neurosci. 2014;34:2065–2074. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3038-13.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Renzi LM, Bovier ER, Hammond BR. A role for the macular carotenoids in visual motor response. Nutr. Neurosci. 2013;16:262–268. doi: 10.1179/1476830513Y.0000000054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Vishwanathan, R., Neuringer, M., Schalch, W. & Johnson, E. Lutein (L) and zeaxanthin (Z) levels in retina are related to levels in the brain. FASEB J.25, (2011).
  • 73.Nishiyori R, Bisconti S, Meehan SK, Ulrich BD. Developmental changes in motor cortex activity as infants develop functional motor skills. Dev. Psychobiol. 2016;58:773–783. doi: 10.1002/dev.21418. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Graziano M. The organization of behavioral repertoire in motor cortex. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2006;29:105–134. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.29.051605.112924. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Seydell-Greenwald A, et al. Functional MRI evidence for a role of ventral prefrontal cortex in tinnitus. Brain Res. 2012;1485:22–39. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2012.08.052. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Leaver AM, et al. Dysregulation of limbic and auditory networks in tinnitus. Neuron. 2011;69:33–43. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.12.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Hinkley LB, Mizuiri D, Hong O, Nagarajan SS, Cheung SW. Increased striatal functional connectivity with auditory cortex in tinnitus. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2015;9:568. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00568. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Rauschecker JP, Leaver AM, Mühlau M. Tuning out the noise: Limbic-auditory interactions in tinnitus. Neuron. 2010;66:819–826. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.04.032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Miranda-Dominguez O, et al. Bridging the gap between the human and macaque connectome: A quantitative comparison of global interspecies structure-function relationships and network topology. J. Neurosci. 2014;34:5552–5563. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4229-13.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Stafford JM, et al. Large-scale topology and the default mode network in the mouse connectome. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2014;111:18745–18750. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1404346111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Biswal B, Yetkin FZ, Haughton VM, Hyde JS. Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar MRI. Magn. Reson. Med. 1995;34:537–541. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910340409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Biswal BB, Van Kylen J, Hyde JS. Simultaneous assessment of flow and BOLD signals in resting-state functional connectivity maps [see comments] NMR Biomed. 1997;10:165–170. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1099-1492(199706/08)10:4/5&#x0003c;165::AID-NBM454&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Cui Z, et al. Individual variation in functional topography of association networks in youth. Neuron. 2020;106:340–353.e8. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2020.01.029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Luna B. Developmental changes in cognitive control through adolescence. Adv. Child Dev. Behav. 2009;37:233–278. doi: 10.1016/S0065-2407(09)03706-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Van Essen DC. Surface-based approaches to spatial localization and registration in primate cerebral cortex. Neuroimage. 2004;23(Suppl 1):S97–107. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Miranda-Dominguez O, et al. Heritability of the human connectome: A connectotyping study. Netw. Neurosci. 2018;2:175–199. doi: 10.1162/netn_a_00029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Marek S, et al. Reproducible brain-wide association studies require thousands of individuals. Nature. 2022 doi: 10.1038/s41586-022-04492-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Gratton C, Nelson SM, Gordon EM. Brain-behavior correlations: Two paths toward reliability. Neuron. 2022;110:1446–1449. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2022.04.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated during and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.


Articles from Scientific Reports are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES