Skip to main content
PLOS Biology logoLink to PLOS Biology
. 2022 Sep 14;20(9):e3001798. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.3001798

A prosthesis utilizing natural vestibular encoding strategies improves sensorimotor performance in monkeys

Kantapon Pum Wiboonsaksakul 1,2, Dale C Roberts 1, Charles C Della Santina 3, Kathleen E Cullen 1,2,3,4,*
Editor: Peter Thier5
PMCID: PMC9473632  PMID: 36103550

Abstract

Sensory pathways provide complex and multifaceted information to the brain. Recent advances have created new opportunities for applying our understanding of the brain to sensory prothesis development. Yet complex sensor physiology, limited numbers of electrodes, and nonspecific stimulation have proven to be a challenge for many sensory systems. In contrast, the vestibular system is uniquely suited for prosthesis development. Its peripheral anatomy allows site-specific stimulation of 3 separate sensory organs that encode distinct directions of head motion. Accordingly, here, we investigated whether implementing natural encoding strategies improves vestibular prosthesis performance. The eye movements produced by the vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR), which plays an essential role in maintaining visual stability, were measured to quantify performance. Overall, implementing the natural tuning dynamics of vestibular afferents produced more temporally accurate VOR eye movements. Exploration of the parameter space further revealed that more dynamic tunings were not beneficial due to saturation and unnatural phase advances. Trends were comparable for stimulation encoding virtual versus physical head rotations, with gains enhanced in the latter case. Finally, using computational methods, we found that the same simple model explained the eye movements evoked by sinusoidal and transient stimulation and that a stimulation efficacy substantially less than 100% could account for our results. Taken together, our results establish that prosthesis encodings that incorporate naturalistic afferent dynamics and account for activation efficacy are well suited for restoration of gaze stability. More generally, these results emphasize the benefits of leveraging the brain’s endogenous coding strategies in prosthesis development to improve functional outcomes.


Recent advances have created new opportunities for applying our understanding of the brain to sensory prothesis development. This study in macaques shows that by implementing natural coding strategies, the precision of vestibular prosthesis performance can be enhanced.

Introduction

Even the simplest sensorimotor transformations require precise neural dynamics. For example, the visuomotor transformations required to produce voluntary saccadic and smooth pursuit eye movements must precisely account for the biomechanical properties of the extraocular muscles and orbital connective tissue (reviewed in [1,2]). Recent developments of sensory prostheses have focused on mimicking the response dynamics of peripheral sensors/afferent systems to improve the functional outcomes of retinal [3] and cochlear [4,5] implants, as well as prostheses aimed at restoring tactile sensation [69]. However, complex sensor physiology, limited number of electrodes, and nonspecific stimulation have proven to be a challenge to the development of biomimetic prostheses in these sensory systems.

The vestibular system detects our head motion relative to space. Rotational head motion is sensed by the 3 semicircular canals and linear acceleration is sensed by the 2 otolith organs. In turn, this information is used to generate essential stabilizing reflexes and complex motor synergies that control gaze and posture, in addition to providing us with our subjective sense of motion and orientation. There are many reasons why the vestibular system is uniquely well suited for the development of neural prostheses for the restoration of sensory function. First, the 3 axes of head rotation are encoded by 3 separate semicircular canals, each innervated by a distinct nerve bundle. This sensor structure allows for targeted stimulation of each of the associated orthogonal rotational axes. Second, vestibular afferent response dynamics have been extensively studied and are well understood ([1013]; also reviewed in [14]). Third, the central pathways mediating essential vestibular reflexes, such as the vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR), which serves to stabilize gaze during head motion, are simple—comprising 3-neuron circuits in their most direct forms (reviewed in [14]). As a result, the vestibulo-motor transformations generating these rapid stereotyped behavioral responses can be objectively quantified and utilized as a direct measure of prosthesis performance [1517]. Despite these advantages, whether leveraging the brain’s endogenous coding strategies can improve vestibular prosthesis performance surprisingly remains unknown.

To address this question, here, we directly tested whether implementing the natural response dynamics of vestibular afferents improves prosthesis performance in a nonhuman primate model. We integrated these response dynamics into the mapping between head motion and pulse rate delivered by a vestibular prosthesis targeting the ampullary nerve innervating the horizontal semicircular canal [1619]. Quantification of the resulting eye movements produced by the VOR provided a direct measure of prosthesis performance. Overall, we found that biomimetic mappings that accounted for the brain’s endogenous dynamic coding strategies produced more temporally accurate VOR eye movements than those that did not. Further exploration of the parameter space (i.e., the variables that control the mapping of head movement into stimulation rate) revealed that incorporating even more extreme tunings than those naturally displayed by vestibular afferents provided unnatural phase advances, as well as undesirable nonlinear gain saturation. Trends were comparable for stimulation encoding virtual versus physical head rotations, with gains enhanced in the latter case. Using computational methods, we then demonstrated that the same model could explain the eye movements evoked by both sinusoidal and transient stimulation and that a stimulation efficacy substantially less than 100% could account for our results—i.e., each stimulation pulse does not always evoke an afferent action potential. Thus, taken together, our results show that mappings incorporating naturalistic afferent dynamics and stimulation efficacy improve the restoration of gaze stability. More broadly, these results also underscore the benefits of leveraging the brain’s natural coding strategies to improve functional outcome of sensory prostheses.

Results

Natural encoding strategies improve sensorimotor performance

Vestibular afferents are classified as either regular or irregular based on differences in their resting discharge variability as well as morphology (Fig 1A). While both afferent types show similar response dynamics, namely an increase in gain and phase lead as a function of head motion frequency, irregular afferents exhibit steeper increases of both as a function of frequency, as compared to their regular counterparts (reviewed in [14]). Accordingly, we first implemented 2 biomimetic mappings that accounted for this difference in the natural response dynamics of vestibular afferents, namely a “regular mapping” (Fig 1B, blue) and an “irregular mapping” (Fig 1B, red) in reference to the afferent type that they mimic. We then compared the performance of these 2 biomimetic mappings with that of the conventionally used mapping (e.g., [15,20,21]), which we termed the “static mapping” (Fig 1B, gray). Notably, this latter mapping is characterized by flat gain and no phase lead across the same frequency range, as opposed to the high-pass tuning that is actually demonstrated by vestibular afferents. At low head rotation frequencies, the stimulation rates produced by the 3 mappings were similar, as can be seen in the overlap of the traces in Fig 1C. In contrast, at high frequencies, the stimulation rates of both regular and irregular mappings produced greater depth of modulation and increased phase leads due to their high-pass tuning (Fig 1D).

Fig 1. Biomimetic afferent response dynamics are implemented in the vestibular prosthesis mapping between head motion and stimulation rate.

Fig 1

(A) Schematic of the VOR pathway. Both types of vestibular afferents (regular and irregular) convey head movement information to the vestibular nuclei, which, in turn, stabilize the gaze via the VOR. (B) Schematic of how the prosthesis converts head movements into pulsatile stimulation. The mapping (black box) consists of the linear transfer functions, which mimic the response dynamics of regular (blue) and irregular (red) afferents or represent the conventional mapping with static gain and phase lead (gray), in cascade with the sigmoidal nonlinearity, which limits the firing rate to be above zero and below the maximum rate of 500 Hz. (C) Example of the stimulation rate, modulated around the baseline rate of 150 pps, in response to sinusoidal head movements at 1 Hz. At this frequency, the stimulation rates are similar for all mappings. Vertical line denotes the peak of head movement for phase comparison. (D) Example of the stimulation rate in response to sinusoidal head movements at 10 Hz. Both regular and irregular mappings display greater depth of modulation in firing and bigger phase lead than the static mapping, which remains in-phase and shows the same depth of modulation as in (C).

To establish how implementation of the above mappings impacted performance, we measured and compared the VOR eye movements evoked by the vestibular prosthesis. Two monkeys with total bilateral vestibular loss were implanted with prosthetic electrodes targeted to the ampullary nerve innervating the horizontal semicircular canal. Frequency-modulated pulsatile stimulation was then delivered in darkness to the head-restrained monkeys. These pulses encode virtual head rotations spanning the natural range (0.2 to 20 Hz) (see Materials and methods). Examples of the resultant VOR eye movement evoked in 1 monkey are shown in Fig 2A. To quantify the evoked VOR responses for each of the 3 mappings, we computed the gain and phase at each frequency (Fig 2B and 2C). Gains were normalized to facilitate comparison across animals (see Fig 2B top insets and also S1 Fig for corresponding curves prior to normalization). Notably, while prosthesis stimulation produced comparable VOR gains and phases across all mappings at lower frequencies (0.2 to 1 Hz), this was not the case for stimulation at higher frequencies (e.g., 5 to 20 Hz). Instead, the VOR gain decreased as a function of frequency for the static mapping (negative log-linear slope, linear regression, p < 0.001 for both monkeys; Fig 2B, bottom inset), while it increased as a function of frequency for both afferent-like mappings (positive log-linear slope, linear regression, p < 0.001 for both monkeys and mappings; Fig 2B, bottom inset). Correspondingly, the VOR phase increasingly lagged the head motion at high frequencies (5 to 20 Hz) for the static mapping while it remained more compensatory for the regular and irregular mappings (i.e., significantly closer to zero degrees, p < 0.001 for both monkeys and mappings, Bonferroni corrected). To directly compare the phase compensation across mappings, we then computed mean phase deviations from zero (the mean absolute value) for each mapping over the 5 to 20 Hz stimulation. On average, the static mapping yielded a significantly higher phase deviation than both regular and irregular mappings (p < 0.001 for both monkeys and mappings, Bonferroni corrected; Fig 2C, inset). In addition, there was no significant difference in the gain or phase responses of early versus late cycles in either monkey, indicating that no adaption occurred over time (p > 0.05, Bonferroni corrected). Taken together, these results show that implementing afferent-like high-pass tuning in the prosthesis mapping overall resulted in more robust high-frequency VOR gains and more compensatory phase behavior.

Fig 2. Afferent-like high-pass dynamics helped maintain robust VOR gains and minimize VOR phase lags at high head rotation frequencies.

Fig 2

(A) Example traces of the evoked eye movements during 1, 5, and 10 Hz virtual sinusoidal rotations, normalized to the peak evoked response (i.e., response evoked by the irregular mapping at 20 Hz). Note that the vestibular prosthesis stimulation induced contralaterally versus ipsilaterally directed VOR eye movements in response to increasing versus decreasing stimulation rate relative to the baseline, respectively. Dashed line indicated the simulated head velocity, scaled to facilitate phase response comparison. (B) The normalized VOR gains across natural frequency range (0.2–20 Hz), using the same normalization reference as in (A). The shaded areas around each trace indicate ± SEM. Light green shading indicates the higher frequencies (5–20 Hz) where the differences between each mapping are more pronounced. The superimposed dashed lines are the log-linear fit over the high frequency range. (Top inset) The VOR gain prior to normalization. (Bottom inset) The log-linear slope of the fit. (C) The phase response of the VOR. (Inset) The mean absolute phase at the high frequencies for each mapping. Red, blue, and gray refer to the irregular, regular, and static mappings, respectively. Data underlying this figure can be found at https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.6338639.

Probing parameter space further establishes the advantages of leveraging the brain’s natural sensory coding strategy

Our above findings led to the question—would a mapping with even greater high-pass tuning further improve VOR performance? Importantly, testing this hypothesis would be impossible with natural stimulation. However, because we control exactly how the prosthesis encodes head motion, here, we could activate the vestibular afferents in ways natural head motion cannot. To test this possibility, we implemented a novel mapping characterized by a steeper gain and phase lead increase than the natural irregular mapping—which we termed the “super high-pass” mapping (Fig 3A, yellow; see Materials and methods). Examples of the evoked VOR are shown in Fig 3B. Quantification of the gain and phase of the evoked VOR responses are shown in Fig 3C and 3D. Compared to either of our 2 natural (i.e., regular and irregular) afferent mappings, the super high-pass mapping evoked significantly larger VOR responses at high frequencies (Fig 3C; p < 0.05 for both monkeys, Bonferroni corrected, 10 to 20 Hz; gains prior to normalization are shown in S2 Fig). Overall, the extreme high-pass nature of this mapping resulted in an approximately 4- and 14-fold increase in gain for 20 versus 0.2 Hz stimulation, for Monkeys Y and G, respectively. Notably, such a steep gain increase as function of frequency differs markedly from the VOR gain in healthy animals, which is flat across the same frequency range [22,23]. Further, the super high-pass mapping overcompensated for the VOR pathway delay, such that the resulting VOR unnaturally led the stimulus at all frequencies except for 20 Hz (p < 0.01 for both monkeys, Bonferroni corrected; Fig 3D, yellow).

Fig 3. VOR performance is directly impacted by the response dynamics of the prosthesis mapping and the biomimetic mappings (i.e., regular, mixed, and irregular) resulted in the most accurate timing.

Fig 3

(A) The transfer functions of the additional mappings to explore the high-pass tuning parameter space: the super high-pass mapping (yellow), which shows an even higher increase in gain and phase lead than normal irregular afferents do, and the mixed mapping (purple), which was designed to represent the combined responses of regular and irregular afferents. (B) Example traces of the normalized evoked eye movements in 1 monkey at 5 and 10 Hz, again normalized to the maximum velocity evoked by the irregular mapping as in Fig 2. Results from Fig 2 are also plotted in thinner line for comparison. Dashed line indicated the simulated head velocity, scaled to facilitate phase response comparison. The vertical black lines indicate the maximum of the virtual head movement stimulus for comparison with the evoked eye velocity. The black arrows denote the peaks of the response from the super high-pass mapping, which showed larger phase lead than the other mappings. (C and D) Similar plots to (B) and (C) of Fig 2, respectively, with the addition of the data from the 2 new mappings. (E) Quantification of the VOR responses after doubling the overall mapping gains (i.e., 2× mapping gains; see Materials and methods). Top row shows the amplitude ratio of the evoked eye velocity of Monkey Y (left) and Monkey G (right) after doubling mapping gains for each of our 5 conditions (i.e., the velocity evoked by the 2× mapping/the velocity evoked by the original 1× mapping). The red arrows point to the ratio from super high-pass mapping at 20 Hz, which is significantly lower than other mappings and indicative of response saturation. Bottom row shows the change in phase after doubling the overall mapping gains. For both monkeys, the changes are generally not significant, with some significant but small (<13 deg) changes (p < 0.05, Bonferroni corrected). Data underlying this figure can be found at https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.6338639.

We then further explored the mapping parameter space to account for the fact that central vestibular neurons that generate the VOR actually receive a mixed input from both afferent types [2427]. Thus, we tested an additional “mixed” mapping (Fig 3A, purple), with the response dynamics designed to be partway between those of regular and irregular afferents (see Materials and methods). This mapping produced both robust VOR responses and relatively compensatory phase as compared to the static mapping across the tested frequency range (purple curves, Fig 3C and 3D). Specifically, as expected, quantification of the evoked eye movements revealed VOR response dynamics that roughly fell between those of the regular and irregular mappings (compare purple to red and blue curves). Taken together, our results so far show that VOR performance is directly impacted by the level of high-pass tuning in the prosthesis mapping and that biomimetic mappings (regular, mixed, and irregular) yielded better phase compensation than unphysiological mappings (static and super high-pass).

Finally, in healthy animals and human participants, the gain of the VOR is typically close to unity over the frequency range tested here (0.2 to 20 Hz) [22,23]. In contrast, the gain of the responses evoked by our natural afferent mappings above were generally substantially less for both monkeys. Thus, we next asked whether we could obtain higher response gains while maintaining response phase. To do this, we doubled the overall mapping gains without altering the mapping dynamics (see Materials and methods) and analyzed the evoked VOR response (S3 Fig) as described above. Consistent with our prediction, quantification of the gain at each frequency revealed a corresponding increase approaching 2× for the regular, irregular, mixed, and static mappings, except for the super high-pass mapping at 20 Hz, where the response for the doubled gain mapping was significantly lower than 2× in both animals (Fig 3E, top, red arrows; p < 0.01 for both monkeys, Bonferroni corrected). This gain decrease is indicative of response saturation—a point which is further discussed below. Additionally, response phases remained mostly unchanged across all frequencies as compared to the original gain condition (compare Figs S3 to 3D). We quantified this observation by calculating the change in phase at each frequency (Fig 3E, bottom). In general, changes in phase were relatively minor with any significant changes (p < 0.05, Bonferroni corrected) less than 13 deg. Together, these findings establish that we could, in general, obtain higher VOR gains by increasing the overall mapping gains without altering the VOR phase response.

So far, we have focused on the prosthesis-evoked VOR eye movements during virtual head rotations in darkness to isolate the contribution of the vestibular system. We next investigated whether VOR gain was enhanced when prosthetic stimulation patterns were evoked by actual physical motion rather than virtual head rotations (see Materials and methods). Overall, we observed similar trends for the physical and virtual stimulation conditions (compare Figs S4 and 3) in darkness across mappings. We also investigated whether VOR gain was enhanced when prosthetic stimulation patterns were evoked by physical rotations in the light (see Materials and methods) and again found similar trends (compare Figs S5 and 3). Interestingly, higher gains were observed in the physical rotation conditions versus virtual rotation condition mainly for frequencies >5 Hz range for Monkey G (p < 0.05, Bonferroni corrected, in darkness and light) and at 10 Hz for Monkey Y in light (p < 0.05, Bonferroni corrected). Thus, together these results indicate that biomimetic mappings likewise improved VOR performance during physical head rotations, while also showing gain enhancement at high frequencies. We further consider the implications of this finding in the discussion.

Improvements in performance generalize to more behaviorally relevant sensory stimuli

In everyday life, we generate transient gaze-orienting head movements that are more complex than a single frequency [28,29]. Accordingly, we next analyzed the VOR evoked by stimulation encoding a head movement representative of monkey’s orienting gaze behavior (i.e., [30]; Fig 4A, dashed lines). First, we compared the amplitude of the VOR evoked across mappings. As expected from our above findings using sinusoidal stimulation, VOR gains increased systematically for mappings with greater and greater high-pass tuning (Fig 4B). Next, to quantify VOR timing accuracy, we estimated the time difference between the peak of the encoded head velocity and that of the evoked eye velocity. The biomimetic regular and mixed mappings evoked a peak VOR response that was well aligned with peak head velocity. (Fig 4C, no significant lead for Monkey Y, and only a small but significant lead of 4.1 ± 0.8 ms in Monkey G for the mixed mapping, p < 0.01, Bonferroni corrected). In contrast, the VOR evoked by the static mapping was significantly delayed relative to the head velocity profile (p < 0.01, Bonferroni corrected, lags of 12 ± 0.7 ms and 12 ± 1.4 for Monkeys G and Y, respectively), while that evoked by irregular and super high-pass mappings actually led the stimulus (p < 0.05, Bonferroni corrected, Monkey G: lead of 7.1 ± 0.8 ms and 14.3 ± 0.9 ms for irregular and super-irregular mappings, respectively, Monkey Y: lead of 16.9 ± 1 ms for super-irregular mapping, no significance for irregular mapping). Finally, we computed the onset latencies of the VOR evoked across mappings and found that, while the latencies were shortest (approximately 7 to 11 ms) for our 2 highest-pass mappings, this difference did not reach significance (Fig 4D). Comparable results were also found for off-direction transient stimulation (S6 Fig). Finally, for completeness, we compared responses evoked by stimulation encoding physical versus virtual motion (compare Figs S7 to 4). As was shown above for sinusoidal stimuli, similar trends were observed across mappings, and, in general, gains were significantly enhanced for physical motion in both animals (p < 0.05, Bonferroni corrected).

Fig 4. Afferent-like response dynamics improve timing accuracy of transient VOR.

Fig 4

(A) Traces of the evoked eye movements for Monkey G during virtual transient head movements (on direction), again normalized to the maximum velocity evoked by the irregular mapping as in Figs 2 and 3. Dashed lines indicate the inverted head velocity. Dotted vertical line indicates the start of the head movement. Solid vertical line denotes the peak of the inverted head movement. Short colored vertical lines indicated the estimated latency. Arrows show the peak of the eye movement response. (B, C, and D) Quantification of the traces in A for normalized gain [using the same normalization reference as in (A)], time difference between eye and head velocity peaks, and onset latency, respectively. Results from Monkey G and Monkey Y are plotted on the left and on the right, respectively. Error bars indicate the SEM. Data underlying this figure can be found at https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.6338639.

Overall, our results using both sinusoidal and transient stimuli revealed parallel trends. Thus, we next asked whether the same model could account for the eye movements evoked by both types of stimulation. To test this proposal, we first fit a simple linear model (Fig 5A; see Materials and methods) to the input–output relationship between sinusoidal prosthetic stimulation and eye movements recorded in our study above (Fig 3). We then used this model to predict the transient VOR responses described above. Fig 5B shows example model fits with the regular (top) and irregular (bottom) mappings for Monkey Y. We found that a model based on VOR eye movement responses to sinusoidal prosthetic stimulation well-predicted VOR responses to transient prosthetic stimulation (mean VAF = 0.91 and 0.90 in the on-direction for Monkeys Y and G, respectively; see S8 Fig for all VAF values). Notably, the model tended to slightly underpredict the evoked VOR in the on-direction (eye movements away from the side of the implant; Fig 5B, left) while overpredicting the evoked VOR in the off-direction (eye movements toward the side of the implant; Fig 5B right). This asymmetry results from unilateral stimulation and is similar to that observed in animals with unilateral vestibular deficits (see, for example, [16,31]).

Fig 5. Eye movement responses to virtual transient stimulation can be predicted by those to sinusoidal stimulation.

Fig 5

(A) The schematic of the model that estimates the VOR response dynamics of each monkey using a simple linear transfer function (see Materials and methods). (B) The model prediction (black line) of the evoked eye movements during transient head movements in the on (left column) and off (right column) directions for the regular (top row) and the irregular (bottom row) mappings.

Conventional rate mappings artificially limit the prosthesis performance

To account for the fact that the firing rates of afferents saturate at 350 to 400 spikes/s [11,32], prior applications of vestibular prostheses in patients [15,20] and nonhuman primates [16,33] have generally programmed a static sigmoidal nonlinearity into the mapping of rotational head velocity to pulse rate. Here, we programmed a similar sigmoidal nonlinearity with a slightly higher saturation rate (500 pps) into our prosthesis mappings (see Materials and methods; also, Figs 1B and 6A). We speculated that this programmed nonlinearity was responsible for the reduction in VOR gain (saturation) observed above for our super high-pass 2× gain mapping at 20 Hz (Fig 3E, red arrows). To confirm this proposal, we plotted the post-sigmoid (linear–nonlinear cascade) pulse rate versus the pre-sigmoid (linear only) pulse rate. As expected, this particular mapping was the only one to deviate from the unity line (S9A Fig, red arrow). Thus, the programmed static sigmoidal nonlinearity could account for, at least partially, the saturation observed with this mapping.

Fig 6. Conventional approach in programming the static nonlinearity artificially constrained the dynamic range of prostheses to an unnaturally narrow band.

Fig 6

(A) Schematic of the VOR generation in a monkey implanted with the prosthesis. (B) The maximum evoked eye velocity plotted as a function of the maximum stimulation rate. The relationship remained linear even at the 400–500 pps stimulation range, as indicated by the yellow shaded areas. (C) The schematic of the model that could predict the results in (B). The model combines the adjustable stimulation efficacy with the control systems-based model of the VOR pathway. In addition, this model accounted for the low-pass behavior of the oculomotor premotor pathway (blue box) responsible for the decrease in slope observed with increasing modulation frequency in (B). (D) Plots of the actual eye velocity and the predicted eye velocity from the model in (C) using 100% stimulation efficacy (top) and the lower stimulation efficacy that best fit the data (bottom). The dashed line is the unity line. (Inset) Schematic showing that 500 pps maximum stimulation rate only translated to 140 sp/s afferent firing rate at 28% efficacy. Data underlying this figure can be found at https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.6338639.

Because we set the maximum pulse rate higher than the normal saturation range, namely to 500 pps, we next investigated whether the ability to reach such high post-sigmoid pulse rate resulted in afferent saturation and further contributed to the VOR saturation. To do this, we pooled the experimental data across all mappings and plotted the maximum evoked eye velocity as a function of the maximum stimulation rate for each sinusoidal frequency, as well as for the transient condition (Fig 6B). Surprisingly, we found that the observed relationship was approximately linear across the entire stimulation range, suggesting that our mappings did not result in afferent saturation (R squared = 0.99 and 0.91 for Monkey G and 0.96 and 0.79 for Monkey Y, respectively, for 20 Hz and transient stimulations, both of which had the stimulation extended into the 400 to 500 pps range; yellow shaded areas of Fig 6B).

At first glance, this observed linearity was unexpected given that afferents saturate at 350 to 400 sp/s. However, the conventional inclusion of a static sigmoidal nonlinearity explicitly mimicking afferent saturation inherently assumes that the efficacy between electrical stimulation and vestibular afferent firing is 1:1 (i.e., 100%). Based on our experimental findings, we speculated that the actual stimulation efficacy was significantly lower than 1:1. To test this proposal, we asked whether we could explain the relationship between the stimulation rate and the evoked eye movements using a standard control systems-based model of the VOR (see Materials and methods; Fig 6C, blue box) with stimulation efficacy <100% (Fig 6C, red box). Indeed, our model best predicted the observed eye velocity when the value of the stimulation efficacy was markedly less than 1:1 (i.e., 28% and 4.5% for Monkeys G and Y, respectively; Fig 6D). Notably, our prediction of approximately 28% efficacy in Monkey G corresponded well to the approximately 27% efficacy showed by a prior study in which single afferents were directly recorded during stimulation with the same prosthesis design [18]. Furthermore, it is likely that the prosthesis stimulation induced a decrease in the synaptic efficacy of afferent-vestibular nuclei neuron transmission, as was also established in this same prior study. Accordingly, to account for this effect, we included 17% central pathway depression in the VOR model (S9B Fig). Simulation of this adjusted model yielded slightly higher estimates of stimulation efficacy (34% and 5% for Monkeys G and Y, respectively; S9C Fig). If our approximately 28% to 34% stimulation efficacy estimate is correct, then our maximum stimulation rate of 500 pps would have translated into only approximately 140 to 170 sp/s of afferent firing (Fig 6D, inset), which could explain the observed linearity. We hypothesize that the even lower efficacy estimated for Monkey Y was likely due to less optimal electrode placement. Taken together, our results suggest that prior studies that programmed the maximum stimulation rate to be exactly the afferent saturation rate utilized only a fraction of the available firing rate range and therefore artificially constrained the dynamic range of prostheses to an unnaturally narrow band.

Discussion

Implementing natural encoding strategies optimizes vestibular prosthesis performance

Our central finding is that by applying our understanding of the brain’s natural coding strategies, we can improve vestibular prosthesis performance. Specifically, incorporating the natural dynamics of vestibular afferents into the mapping of head motion to pulsatile stimulation produced more temporally accurate VOR eye movements. Further exploration of the parameter space revealed that the mapping incorporating more extreme high-pass tuning than is naturally displayed by vestibular afferents produces an unnatural phase advance, whereas the static mapping without dynamics produces VOR eye movements that substantially lag stimulation. Trends were comparable for virtual and actual head rotations, with gains enhanced in the latter case. Using computational methods, we further demonstrate that the same model could explain the eye movements evoked by sinusoidal and transient prosthetic stimulation and that a stimulation efficacy substantially less than 100% could account for our results. Taken together, our results underscore the benefits of leveraging knowledge of endogenous afferent dynamics and activation efficacy to ensure vestibular sensorimotor accuracy, which could directly translate to better functional outcomes in patients.

Neurophysiologically derived mappings better compensate for neural pathways delays

During everyday activities, the vestibular input experienced by humans and monkeys has significant power up to 20 Hz [28,29]. Over this frequency range, vestibular afferents show strong high-pass tuning that produces a 2-fold increase in response modulation at 20 versus 0.5 Hz. Further, while afferents modulate in phase with rotational head velocity at the lower end of this frequency range (<0.5 Hz), they also demonstrate increasing phase leads as a function of frequency [1012]. In this context, it is interesting that while the tuning of vestibular afferents has been well characterized, it had not been incorporated in design of vestibular prosthetic devices. Notably, mappings used currently in clinical trials directly map instantaneous angular head velocity to a specific pulse rate—equivalent to the static mapping in our present study (e.g., [15,20,21]). Likewise, static mappings have largely been used in prior studies in primate animal models [16,3436]. As expected, our static mapping generated VOR responses consistent with the results of these prior studies, which were characterized by poor phase compensation. Finally, it is noteworthy that prior work by Merfeld and colleagues employed a dynamic mapping between angular head velocity to a specific pulse rate [33,3739]. However, this mapping only approximated afferent dynamics over a very low frequency range (<0.1 Hz) and thus performed in a comparable manner to the static mapping over most of the natural frequency range (i.e., 0.1 Hz to 20 Hz).

In contrast to the poor phase compensation displayed by both the static mapping in this study and other prior studies, our biomimetic mappings exhibited much more natural phase behavior. In healthy primates, the VOR is compensatory across the natural frequency range with eye velocity nearly in phase with head velocity, as well as gains close to 1 even at 20 Hz [22,23]. The prevailing view is that this compensatory performance is achieved because the high-pass tuning of vestibular afferents account for the finite approximately 5 ms delay of VOR pathway (i.e., synaptic, neural, and muscle activation times from afferents to extraocular muscles), as well as the response dynamics of the VOR pathway [22,40]. Our present results using virtual stimulation provide direct support for this view and emphasize the critical role of neural response dynamics in ensuring accurate sensorimotor transformations. A further advantage of our virtual stimulation condition was that it isolated the vestibular system’s contribution to the VOR. Interestingly, we likewise found similar trends across mappings when the same patterned prosthetic stimulation was delivered during actual physical rotation. Importantly, VOR response gains were enhanced in this latter condition, such that the gain increase was more than the sum of the gains during physical rotation with no stimulation or just stimulation alone. We speculate that this enhanced response was due to the presence of additional congruent extravestibular signals (i.e., proprioception, tactile, and visual) augmenting neuronal responses to modulated prosthetic stimulation and/or a heightened arousal state during the actual motion stimulation.

Dual coding strategies support 2 functional streams

Central neurons in the vestibular nuclei that generate the VOR receive direct monosynaptic inputs from both types of vestibular afferents, but inputs from regular afferents predominate [2427]. Here, we correspondingly found that mappings that mimicked regular afferent dynamics or a mix of regular/irregular afferents dynamics evoked VOR eye movements with temporal properties that best matched the encoded head motion stimulus (Fig 4A). In contrast, as noted above, our static mappings did not compensate for VOR neural circuitry delay and/or oculomotor plant dynamics, resulting in large phase lags. Moreover, the VOR eye movements evoked by our more dynamic irregular and super high-pass mappings displayed unnatural advances in phase and timing relative to the applied sinusoidal and transient stimulation, respectively.

In this context, there is evidence that the relative contributions of regular versus irregular afferents to the central pathways mediating the vestibulo-spinal reflex versus the VOR are optimized to match the dynamic requirement of each pathway [25,26,41]. As noted above, regular afferent dynamics are well suited to offset the VOR pathway delays and the biomechanics of the oculomotor plant. On the other hand, the larger gains and phase leads of the irregular afferents are required to drive robust postural responses since vestibulo-spinal pathways must account for the higher inertia of the body relative to the eye [27,42]. Furthermore, irregular afferents likely also make important contributions to ascending pathways that convey vestibular information to cortical brain circuits mediating self-motion perception ([43]; also reviewed in [14]). Interestingly, a recent imaging study in humans reported that these cortical circuits overlap with those mediating postural control [44]. Thus, we speculate that the biomimetic irregular mapping might be better suited for postural control and self-motion perception, whereas the biomimetic regular and mixed mappings produced the most temporally accurate VOR eye movements as was observed in our present study.

The trade-off between response amplitude and coding range also constrains neuroprostheses

The timing characteristics of the VOR evoked by our regular and mixed mappings produced temporally accurate responses, yet the VOR evoked by these mappings was not fully compensatory (i.e., gain less than 1). To better understand why this was the case, we probed the parameter space in 2 different dimensions. First, we tested whether implementing a mapping with dynamics that were even more high pass than those of irregular afferents improved gain performance. This “super high-pass” mapping produced relatively higher VOR gains that in 1 monkey actually approached unity at the highest frequencies (S2 Fig). However, it is important to note that due to the steepness of the gain curve, this was not the case at lower frequencies (compare 0.2 to 1 Hz and 2 to 20 Hz; S2 Fig). Second, we tested how implementing a higher mapping gain overall affects the evoked VOR. In general, we found that doubling the gain of any of our mappings produced roughly 2-fold larger VOR eye movement responses with similar phase behavior as the original mappings (Fig 3E). However, we also found that for our most dynamic mapping (i.e., super high-pass), the gain of the evoked VOR saturated and then began to decrease starting at approximately 10 Hz.

Overall, the above analysis of the parameter space highlights the trade-off problem inherent in mapping head motion to stimulation rate. Improvements in VOR gain can be accomplished by increasing the overall mapping gain, yet this will decrease the dynamic range that the prothesis can encode before cutoff and saturation. This trade-off problem is further exacerbated by artificial constraints from the common approach in programming the static nonlinearity used in prior studies, both in humans and animal models [15,16,20,33]. Setting the maximum stimulation rate to be exactly the afferent saturation rate inherently assumes that the relationship between prosthetic stimulation and afferent firing is 1:1 (100% stimulation efficacy). However, our present modeling results suggest that the efficacy was actually substantially less, consistent with our prior experimental results [18]. In this prior study, the experimentally measured afferent stimulation efficacy was only approximately 27%, for monkey with comparable VOR eye movement gain as Monkey G (see Fig 3 of [18])—a value similar to that estimated here (28%). We thus speculate that the generally low prosthesis-evoked VOR gains reported in the literature (human participants [15,21]; monkeys [16,34]) are, at least in part, due to artificially limiting the dynamic range of stimulation relative to the firing rate of the afferents. We propose that the use of mappings, which actually account for the efficacy of afferent activation (Fig 6D), would lead to improved VOR gains, while maintaining the dynamic range required to represent the natural range of head movements experienced in everyday life [28,29,45]. In parallel to the implementation of such mappings, innovative methods based on direct current stimulation could become a potential alternative and/or complementary approach for increasing stimulation efficacy [4648].

Finally, we note that at least 2 additional factors likely combine to limit prothesis performance. First, our prior single unit recording studies in the vestibular nuclei have shown that prosthetic pulsatile stimulation rapidly reduces the efficacy of the afferent-central neuron synapse, which, in turn, reduces the gain of the prosthesis-evoked VOR eye movement [18]. As discussed in this previous report, the pulsatile stimulation produced by the prosthesis appears to induce long-term depression at this synapse due to the synchrony evoked across the vestibular afferent population—such synchrony is not present during natural head motion. Second, following bilateral loss, it is an open question how the central pathways adapt to process vestibular signals, because the vestibular contribution cannot be measured in the animals without a prosthesis. On one hand, it is possible that central mechanisms could increase the synaptic weighting of the afferent input as has been observed following unilateral peripheral loss [49,50]. On the other hand, it is possible that, in the case of bilateral peripheral loss, adaptive central mechanisms instead function to suppress the contribution from any residual vestibular afferent nerve input. Future experiments focused on distinguishing between these 2 possibilities can serve to inform the development of improved prothesis-based approaches to restore vestibular function.

Vestibular implant users will likely benefit from incorporating natural encoding strategies

Vestibular prostheses are the only restorative treatment for patients with bilateral vestibular deficits who cannot compensate centrally via rehabilitation exercises. However, the benefits of vestibular implantation must be weighed against the cost and risks of surgery [20,51] in an analysis that depends directly on the prosthesis functional performance. Here, we found that biomimetic mappings resulted in the same trends in functional improvement in both monkeys. However, we also observed substantially lower gains in 1 monkey. Comparable variability in evoked VOR gains has likewise been reported across subjects in prior human [15,21] and monkey [16,34] studies. One factor that could contribute to such gain variability is differences in the precise placement of the electrodes targeting the ampullae [52]. Indeed, optimization of electrode placement is a focus of ongoing research [53,54]. In this context, our finding that biomimetic mappings enhance performance despite gain differences across subjects directly advocates for incorporating such coding strategies in clinical practice. Furthermore, the implementation of such mappings does not increase cost or risk from additional surgery or hardware changes.

Advances in vestibular prosthesis can inform the development of other sensory prostheses

Our present findings serve to extend a growing body of literature showing improved functional performance from biomimetic sensory prosthesis designs (e.g., enhanced speech intelligibility for cochlear implants [5]; improved manual dexterity for limb prostheses that include tactile feedback [5557]). Notably, our present findings show that accounting for the specific afferent input to a well-defined sensorimotor pathway can optimize functional performance (i.e., VOR). As discussed above, VOR pathways receive inputs from 2 afferent classes (regular and irregular), with the input from the former being stronger (reviewed in [14]). Correspondingly, our stimulation mappings that best matched this afferent input produced the most temporally accurate VOR. In contrast, we postulate mappings that predominately account for the dynamics of irregular afferents may be best suited to restore other vestibular functions including posture and self-motion perception. More generally, studies in other sensory systems have likewise shown heterogeneity in the afferent inputs mediating different behavioral and perceptual outcomes. For example, in the somatosensory system, slow adapting mechanoreceptors detect sustained force and are thought to better aid in adjusting grip force, while fast adapting receptors detect changes in force and are thought to contribute to the perception of vibration (reviewed in [58]). We suggest that the development of prostheses that can account for such pathway specific heterogeneities will be essential to improving functional outcomes across sensory systems.

Materials and methods

Study design

The goal of this study was to evaluate whether biomimetic coding strategies, when implemented in the mapping between head motion and stimulation rate, improve prosthesis performance, as measured by the VOR accuracy. Testing was done in 2 macaque monkeys with bilateral vestibular loss and a vestibular prosthesis comparable to that currently implanted in human patients in an ongoing clinical trial [15,20]. We first compared the VOR evoked by 2 mappings mimicking the natural response dynamics of the 2 types of vestibular afferents to that evoked by the standard conventional mapping currently used clinically. Specifically, semicircular canal afferents demonstrate “high-pass dynamics” characterized by an increase in gain and phase lead as a function of head motion frequency (see Fig 1B, blue and red), which contrasts with the flat gain and lack of phase lead characterizing the conventional mapping (see Fig 1B, gray). Additionally, we explored the parameter space by testing mappings with dynamics between those of these 2 afferent types as well as a mapping with an even greater level of high-pass tuning than vestibular afferents. Data analyses were done in a blind manner, i.e., without knowing which mapping was used for each dataset. The number of repetitions for each paradigm and exclusion criteria are described in detail under “Data analysis” below.

Surgical procedures

Two macaque monkeys (Monkey Y, male, 8 kg; Monkey G, female, 9 kg) were used for all experimental paradigms. All housing, surgical, and experimental protocols were approved by the Johns Hopkins Animal Care and Use Committee. Animals were housed on a 12-h light/dark cycle with daily enrichment. Throughout the study, animals were monitored in consultation with the clinical veterinarian staff to ensure physical and emotional well-being. Euthanasia procedure follows the best practices recommended by the clinical veterinarian staff and the ACUC. Both monkeys were initially fitted with a head implant for head fixation and scleral coils for eye position recording (detailed methods in [16,18]). In brief, isoflurane was used to maintain surgical level of anesthesia (2% to 3% initially then 0.8% to 1.5% throughout the procedure). A stainless steel post was affixed to the skull with stainless steel screws and dental acrylic. A 15- to 16-mm coil, made of 3 turns of Teflon-coated stainless steel wire, was sutured to the scleral beneath the conjunctiva of the eye.

Both monkeys were also implanted with vestibular prothesis electrodes targeting each ampulla (detailed methods in [16]; right ear for Monkey Y and left ear for Monkey G). Monkey Y was implanted with its canals intact, while Monkey G had received gentamicin treatment via bilateral intratympanic injection. A mastoidectomy was performed under sterile conditions to allow access to the labyrinth. Two small holes were made at the junction of the ampullae of the superior and horizontal semicircular canals and a forked electrode array was inserted to target the 2 canals. A hole was also made in the thin segment of the posterior semicircular canal near its junction with the ampulla and a single-tine electrode array was inserted. Additionally, reference electrodes were inserted into the common crus of the labyrinth and in extracranial musculature. To stabilize the electrodes, pieces of fascia and bone were inserted around each array. The electrode leads were run under the periosteum and secured to the headcap with dental acrylic. The animals recovered for 2 weeks before any experiments were performed.

Each monkey had bilateral vestibular loss confirmed by VOR gain <0.1 at 2 Hz in both directions of rotation. This was because vestibular implantation disrupted the canal function, causing vestibular hypofunction in the implanted side. In addition, both monkeys also had deficits in their contralateral ear: Monkey Y had nonfunctioning old prosthesis electrodes in the left ear and Monkey G had bilateral gentamicin treatment via intratympanic injection and, subsequently, a labyrinthectomy in the right ear due to insufficient reduction of the VOR gain contributed by the right ear. Current experiments were done 10 years after implantation for Monkey G and 2 years after implantation for Monkey Y. Both monkeys were use in other studies with the same implant. Stimulation was only delivered during experiments and not in home cages.

Mapping angular head velocity into stimulation rates

The vestibular prosthesis (adapted from [16]) consisted of a gyroscope module that senses angular rotation, a processing module that converts head angular velocity into stimulation rate according to a head-velocity-to-pulse-rate mapping and sends a trigger to an external current source (AM Systems), which delivers a charge-balanced biphasic pulse (200 μS/phase, cathodic first) to the canal electrodes. The stimulation current was set at the maximum level for each monkey: 170 μA for Monkey G (80% of the facial threshold) and 250 μA for Monkey Y (the safe charge limit based on the electrode size). In this study, we modified the mapping function to consist of a cascade between a linear transfer function and a fixed cutoff nonlinearity, which has been shown to accurately describe vestibular afferent behaviors [45].

For the linear part, we assumed that the linear firing rate r(t) as a function of head movement velocity stimulus VH(t) is given by: r(t) = (H * VH)(t) + r0, where the asterisk denotes convolution and r0 denotes the baseline firing rate (chosen to be 150 Hz). The Fourier transform of H(t), i.e., the transfer function, is in the form:

H(f)=kS(S+1T1)(S+1TC)(S+1T2), (Eq 1)

where S = i2πf. In particular, we used constants similar to those used in [45] to estimate the transfer functions of the regular and irregular afferents. For regular afferents: k = 5.056 pps/dps, T1 = 0.0175 s, T2 = 0.0027 s, and Tc = 5.7 s. For irregular afferents: k = 38.889 pps/dps, T1 = 0.03 s, T2 = 0.0006 s, and Tc = 5.7 s. In addition, we introduced 2 new transfer functions. One is called the “super high-pass” transfer function, which has a higher level of high-pass behavior than irregular afferents in nature. The transfer function is in the form given above but with k = 76.76 and T1 = 0.06, which were doubled their normal values for irregular afferents. The other is called the “mixed” transfer function, which behaves like a mix between regular and irregular afferents. The transfer function is given by:

Hmixed(f)=n(Hregular+Hirregular)/2, (Eq 2)

where n is a constant (1.5923) used to match the gain at 0.5 Hz to that used by [16] (i.e., 0.78 pps/dps) without changing the phase relationship. The static mapping has no high-pass response dynamics with a flat gain of 0.78 pps/dps and zero phase lead.

For the fixed cutoff nonlinearity, a sigmoidal function was used similar to [16,45] and is given by the form:

rfinal(t)=c3/(1+ec1(rlinear(t)c2))), (Eq 3)

where rlinear is the linear firing rate from the transfer functions and rfinal is the actual final firing rate. Other constants were chosen such that the upper limit of the firing rate was 500 pps and the gain in the middle region (centered around the baseline firing rate of 150 pps) of the sigmoid is 1: c1 = 4/c3, c2 = 150 + log(c3/150 − 1)/c1, and c3 = 500.

For the double mapping gain conditions, we multiplied each gain constant k by 2, while keeping other constants the same for the regular, irregular, and super high-pass mappings. For the mixed mapping, we multiplied n (in Eq 2) by 2 while keeping other constants the same: n = 3.1846. For the static mapping, the flat gain was doubled to 1.56 pps/dps.

Data acquisition

Monkeys sat comfortably in a primate chair with head fixation in darkness while prosthesis pulses were delivered. Scleral search coil technique was used to measure horizontal and vertical eye position. Eye data were digitized at 1 kHz (Blackrock Microsystems). Trigger pulses indicating the start of stimulation paradigms were concurrently digitized at 30 kHz and later synchronized with the eye data. Room light was turned on between stimulation paradigms to maintain alertness and reduce darkness visual adaptation. Juice rewards were intermittently given to maintain alertness.

Stimulation conditions

For the “virtual head motion” condition, the prosthetic pulses were delivered as if the monkeys’ heads were moving, but the monkeys were actually stationary in the chair. For the “physical head motion” condition, comparable prosthetic pulses were also delivered but evoked by actual physical head motion that was detected by the gyroscope module of the prosthesis during applied whole-body rotation. Two types of stimuli were used: Sinusoidal motion. Sinusoidal rotations with 50 deg/s at 7 frequencies spanning the naturalistic range (0.2, 0.5, 1, 2, 5, 10, 20 Hz) were used. Each presentation consisted of 10 sinusoidal cycles and at least 2 presentations were recorded for each experimental condition. Due to motion platform system limitations, we only delivered the actual motion for the “physical head motion” condition up to 10 Hz. Transient motion. Brief, transient motions (around 150 ms long and 200 deg/s peak velocity) in both directions were used to mimic natural head turns. These velocity profiles were then converted to the stimulation pulse rate via the different mappings as described above (see section “Mapping angular head velocity into stimulation rates”). At least 20 presentations were recorded for each experimental condition.

Data analysis

All analyses were performed using MATLAB 2019b (Mathworks). Digitized (1 kHz) eye position data were low-passed filtered at 125 Hz (51st degree, Hamming window, filtfilt function in MATLAB). Eye velocity data were then calculated by differentiating the eye position data. Saccades were detected using thresholding and removed from the dataset. Slow-phase eye velocity data were then further low-pass filtered at 50 Hz (51st degree, Hamming window, filtfilt function in MATLAB).

Sinusoidal data

A dynamic linear regression technique was used to find the gain, offset, and the shift (phase) of the entire cycle of VOR response. For 0.2 and 0.5 Hz, only cycles with at least 40% slow phase were included and at least 3 cycles were included in the analysis. For higher frequencies, only cycles with 60% slow phase were included and at least 10 cycles were included in the analysis. The log-linear slope of the gains at high frequencies (5 to 20 Hz) was estimated using linear regression after transforming the frequencies to the logarithmic scale. The gain ratio was calculated by dividing each ×2 gain data point by the mean ×1 gain. The standard error of change in phase was nonparametrically estimated using bootstrap methods with 2,000 iterations. VOR responses from the “physical head motion” condition were quantified from difference between the mapped stimulation condition and baseline only condition to compute the component of the VOR evoked by the prosthesis.

Transient data

Only trials with no saccade 50 ms prior and after the stimulus were included. At least 4 trials were included in the analysis. The response latency was estimated by fitting a linear fit to the 10 data points before and after the eye movements crossed the 2 SD thresholds. The intersection between the fitted line and the x-axis was then used as the start of the response. The gain of the response was calculated by dividing the maximum eye velocity by the amplitude of the head movement. The peak timing is the time point where the eye movement reached its peak velocity. Transient VOR responses from the “physical head motion” condition were quantified as described above for sinusoidal data.

Modeling

Input–output modeling

A linear transfer function approximating the gain and phase response was obtained for each monkey using the invfreqs function in Matlab. To account for the fixed pathway delay, the phase response was shifted back by 6 ms before the transfer function estimation and the simulated output of the model was then shifted forward 6 ms. The transfer function with 1 zero and 1 pole worked well for Monkey Y and the transfer function with 1 zero and 2 poles worked well for Monkey G. The model fit was assessed using variance accounted for (VAF):

VAF=1Var(yy^)Var(y), (Eq 4)

where y is the actual data and y^ is the model estimation.

Pathway modeling

Overall, a linear systems–based VOR pathway model [1,59] was used to predict the eye velocity from the afferent firing rate:

EV(f)=TVN(f)TNI(f)TPlant(f)AFR(f) (Eq 5)

where EV(f) is the Fourier transform of the eye velocity, AFR(f) is the Fourier transform of the afferent firing rate from the earlier part of the model. The other terms are the transfer functions of different parts of the VOR pathway:

VestibularnucleiTVN(f)=gVORTVOR(sTVOR+1)(sTc+1)Tc (Eq 6)
NeuralintegratorTNI(f)=Te1+1s (Eq 7)
PlantTPlant(f)=sesτ(sTe1+1)(sTe2+1) (Eq 8)

where s=2πif,i=1. gVOR is the VOR gain, which was set to yield perfect compensation at 2 Hz rotation in normal monkeys. Tc = 5.7 s is the time constant of canal afferent. TVOR = 16 s is the VOR time constant. τ = 0.006 s is the pathway delay. These constants were taken from previous literature [1]. Te1 and Te2 are time constants describing the neural integrator and plant dynamics; Te1 cancels out when Eqs 7 and 8 are combined, whereas Te2 was set to 0.008 and 0.025 for Monkey Y and Monkey G, respectively, to account for the low-pass dynamics of their oculomotor plants (i.e., see different slopes in Fig 6B).

To compute the input afferent firing rate to this model for each rotation movement condition/prosthesis mapping, the prosthesis pulse rate was generated using the linear nonlinear cascade as described above. To account for the high-pass behavior seen in the prosthesis-evoked VOR response at low frequencies (Fig 2B, gray; see also [15,16,34]), an extra high-pass filter was applied to the afferent firing rate. For each animal, the filters were estimated from the gray traces in Fig 2B (cutoff frequency of 3.5 Hz and 0.2 Hz for Monkey G and Monkey Y, respectively). In our modeling of VOR eye movements, we first assumed a 1:1 relationship between pulse rate and afferent firing rate (i.e., setting the stimulation efficacy to 100%), which overestimated the eye velocity. We next adjusted the stimulation efficacy to estimate the best fit to our data across conditions in each animal. Finally, to account for the 17% reduction in prosthesis-evoked eye movements that occurs due to the reduction of central VOR pathway efficacy [18], we created another model that incorporated this 17% reduction.

Statistical analysis

All data were presented as mean ± SEM. MATLAB 2019b was used to conduct statistical analysis. Normality assumption of each dataset was assessed using Lilliefors test prior to other statistical analyses. For normally distributed datasets, two-tailed t test was used. For all other datasets, nonparametric Wilcoxon signed rank test was used. Statistical significance was determined at p < 0.05 for all tests.

Supporting information

S1 Fig. VOR data of each monkey prior to normalization.

(A) VOR gains prior to normalization across natural frequency range (0.2–20 Hz). Note that the figure shows the same data as Fig 2B but the gains are displayed on the absolute scale (not normalized). (B) Example VOR traces prior to normalization. Dashed lines indicate inverted, virtual head velocity. The shaded areas indicate SEM. Red, blue, and gray refer to the irregular, regular, and static mappings, respectively. Data underlying this figure can be found at https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.6338639.

(TIF)

S2 Fig. VOR data of each monkey prior to normalization with 2 additional mappings.

(A) VOR gains prior to normalization across natural frequency range (0.2–20 Hz). Note that the figure shows the same data as Fig 3C but the gains are displayed on the absolute scale (not normalized). (B) Example VOR traces prior to normalization. Dashed lines indicate the inverted, virtual head velocity. The shaded areas indicate SEM. Yellow, red, purple, blue, and gray refer to the super high-pass, irregular, mixed, regular, and static mappings, respectively. Data underlying this figure can be found at https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.6338639.

(TIF)

S3 Fig. VOR gain and phase plots for the X2 condition.

(A) Normalized VOR gains across natural frequency range (0.2–20 Hz), using the same normalization reference as Fig 2. Note the reduction in gain at 20 Hz due to saturation. (B) The phase response of the VOR. Note the similarity to the phase response in the X1 condition in Fig 3D. The shaded area indicated the SEM. Yellow, red, purple, blue, and gray refer to the super high-pass, irregular, mixed, regular, and static mappings, respectively. Data underlying this figure can be found at https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.6338639.

(TIF)

S4 Fig. VOR data during physical sinusoidal rotation in the dark.

(A) VOR gains across the frequency range (0.2–10 Hz). (B) The phase response of the VOR. (C) Example VOR traces. Dashed lines indicate inverted head velocity. There was a significant increase in the VOR gain mainly over the 5–10 Hz range for Monkey G (p < 0.05, Bonferroni corrected) but not for Monkey Y. There was also a significant increase in phase lead mainly over 5–10 Hz range for both monkeys (p < 0.05, Bonferroni corrected). There was no significant difference in the gain and phase responses for early and late cycles in both monkeys. Yellow, red, purple, blue, and gray refer to the super high-pass, irregular, mixed, regular, and static mappings, respectively. Data underlying this figure can be found at https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.6338639.

(TIF)

S5 Fig. VOR data during physical sinusoidal rotation in the light.

(A) VOR gains across the frequency range (0.2–10 Hz). (B) The phase response of the VOR. (C) Example VOR traces. Dashed line indicated the inverted head velocity. There was a significant increase in the VOR gain mainly over the 5–10 Hz range for Monkey G (p < 0.05, Bonferroni corrected) and at 10 Hz for Monkey Y (p < 0.05, Bonferroni corrected). There was also a significant increase in phase lead mainly over 5–10 Hz range for both monkeys (p < 0.05, Bonferroni corrected). There was no significant difference in the gain and phase responses for early and late cycles in both monkeys. Yellow, red, purple, blue, and gray refer to the super high-pass, irregular, mixed, regular, and static mappings, respectively. Data underlying this figure can be found at https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.6338639.

(TIF)

S6 Fig. Evoked VOR during virtual transient head movements in the off-direction.

(A) Traces of the evoked eye movements for Monkey G during transient head movements (off-direction), normalized as in Fig 4. Dashed lines indicate the inverted head velocity. Dotted vertical line indicates the start of the head movements. Solid vertical line denotes the peak of the head movements. Short colored vertical lines indicated the estimated onset of the evoked eye movement. (B, C, and D) Quantification of the traces in (A) for normalized gain [using the same normalization reference as in Fig 4], time difference between eye and head velocity peaks, and onset latency, respectively. Results from Monkey G and Monkey Y are plotted on the left and on the right, respectively. Error bars indicate the SEM. Yellow, red, purple, blue, and gray refer to the super high-pass, irregular, mixed, regular, and static mappings, respectively. Similar to the on-direction results, the biomimetic regular and mixed mappings evoked a peak VOR response that was well aligned with the peak head velocity (not significantly different from 0 ms except for Monkey G mixed mapping, which showed a significant but small lead of 12.3 ± 1.7 ms, p < 0.01, Bonferroni corrected). In contrast, the static mapping resulted in the VOR peak with a significant delay (p < 0.001 for Monkey G, not significant for Monkey Y, Bonferroni corrected), while the VOR peaks evoked by irregular and super high-pass mappings actually led the stimulus (p < 0.05 for Monkey G, not significant for Monkey Y). Data underlying this figure can be found at https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.6338639.

(TIF)

S7 Fig. Evoked VOR during physical transient head movements in the on-direction.

(A) Traces of the evoked eye movements for Monkey G during physical transient head movements (on-direction), normalized as in Fig 4. Dashed lines indicate the inverted head velocity, scaled to facilitate timing comparison. Dotted vertical line indicates the start of the head movements. Solid vertical line denotes the peak of the head movements. Short vertical lines indicated the estimated onset of the evoked eye movement. (B, C, and D) Quantification of the traces in (A) for normalized gain, time difference between eye and head velocity peaks, and onset latency, respectively. Results from Monkey G and Monkey Y are plotted on the left and on the right, respectively. Error bars indicate the SEM. Yellow, red, purple, blue, and gray refer to the super high-pass, irregular, mixed, regular, and static mappings, respectively. Data underlying this figure can be found at https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.6338639.

(TIF)

S8 Fig. VAFs of the model prediction of virtual transient eye velocity using the virtual sinusoidal data.

The VAFs are plotted for each trial of the head movements in the on (top row) and off (bottom rows) directions for Monkey Y (left column) and Monkey G (right column). Error bars indicate the SEM. Data underlying this figure can be found at https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.6338639.

(TIF)

S9 Fig. Conventional approach in programming the static nonlinearity artificially constrained the dynamic range of prostheses to an unnaturally narrow band.

(A) Post-sigmoid stimulation rate plotted as a function of pre-sigmoid stimulation rate. The red arrow points to the data point from the super high-pass 2× gain mapping at 20 Hz, which showed large deviation from linearity. (B) Similar to the model in Fig 6C but with the addition of the CNS adaptation (17%; [18]). (C) Plots of the actual eye velocity and the predicted eye velocity from the model in (B) using 100% stimulation efficacy (top) and the lower stimulation efficacy that best fit the data (bottom). The dashed line is the unity line. Data underlying this figure can be found at https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.6338639.

(TIF)

Acknowledgments

We thank V. Chang, M. Christman, O. Leavitt, R. Mildren, T. Niebur, O. Stanley, E. Tourney, R. Wei, and O. Zobeiri for critically reading the manuscript.

Data Availability

All data associated with this study are present in the paper or the Supplementary Materials. The dataset is available from Zenodo database: https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.6338639.

Funding Statement

This work was supported by National Institutes of Health grant R01DC002390 (to KEC and CCDS) and grant R01DC018061 (to KEC). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Robinson DA. The use of control systems analysis in the neurophysiology of eye movements. Annu Rev Neurosci. 1981;4:463–503. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.04.030181.002335 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Lisberger SG. Visual Guidance of Smooth Pursuit Eye Movements. Annu Rev Vis Sci. 2015;1:447–68. doi: 10.1146/annurev-vision-082114-035349 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Nirenberg S, Pandarinath C. Retinal prosthetic strategy with the capacity to restore normal vision. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2012;109:15012–7. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1207035109 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Lopez-Poveda EA, Eustaquio-Martín A, Stohl JS, Wolford RD, Schatzer R, Gorospe JM, et al. Intelligibility in speech maskers with a binaural cochlear implant sound coding strategy inspired by the contralateral medial olivocochlear reflex. Hear Res. 2017;348:134–7. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2017.02.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Fumero MJ, Eustaquio-Martín A, Gorospe JM, Polo López R, Gutiérrez Revilla MA, Lassaletta L, et al. A state-of-the-art implementation of a binaural cochlear-implant sound coding strategy inspired by the medial olivocochlear reflex. Hear Res. 2021;409:108320. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2021.108320 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Saal HP, Bensmaia SJ. Biomimetic approaches to bionic touch through a peripheral nerve interface. Neuropsychologia. 2015;79:344–53. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.06.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Bensmaia SJ, Tyler DJ, Micera S. Restoration of sensory information via bionic hands. Nat Biomed Eng. 2020. doi: 10.1038/s41551-020-00630-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Kumaravelu K, Tomlinson T, Callier T, Sombeck J, Bensmaia SJ, Miller LE, et al. A comprehensive model-based framework for optimal design of biomimetic patterns of electrical stimulation for prosthetic sensation. J Neural Eng. 2020;17:046045. doi: 10.1088/1741-2552/abacd8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Iskarous MM, Thakor NV. E-Skins: Biomimetic Sensing and Encoding for Upper Limb Prostheses. Proc IEEE. 2019;107:2052–64. doi: 10.1109/JPROC.2019.2939369 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Ramachandran R, Lisberger SG. Transformation of vestibular signals into motor commands in the vestibuloocular reflex pathways of monkeys. J Neurophysiol. 2006;96:1061–74. doi: 10.1152/jn.00281.2006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Sadeghi SG, Minor LB, Cullen KE. Response of vestibular-nerve afferents to active and passive rotations under normal conditions and after unilateral labyrinthectomy. J Neurophysiol. 2007;97:1503–14. doi: 10.1152/jn.00829.2006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Jamali M, Chacron MJ, Cullen KE. Self-motion evokes precise spike timing in the primate vestibular system. Nat Commun. 2016;7:1–14. doi: 10.1038/ncomms13229 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Jamali M, Carriot J, Chacron MJ, Cullen KE. Coding strategies in the otolith system differ for translational head motion vs. static orientation relative to gravity. Gold JI, Laurens J, editors. Elife. 2019;8:e45573. doi: 10.7554/eLife.45573 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Cullen KE. Vestibular processing during natural self-motion: implications for perception and action. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2019;20:346–63. doi: 10.1038/s41583-019-0153-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Boutros PJ, Schoo DP, Rahman MA, Valentin NS, Chow MR, Ayiotis AI, et al. Continuous vestibular implant stimulation partially restores eye-stabilizing reflexes. JCI Insight 2019;4. doi: 10.1172/jci.insight.128397 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Dai C, Fridman GY, Davidovics NS, Chiang B, Ahn JH, Della Santina CC. Restoration of 3D vestibular sensation in rhesus monkeys using a multichannel vestibular prosthesis. Hear Res. 2011;281:74–83. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2011.08.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Mitchell DE, Dai C, Rahman MA, Ahn JH, Della Santina CC, Cullen KE. Head Movements Evoked in Alert Rhesus Monkey by Vestibular Prosthesis Stimulation: Implications for Postural and Gaze Stabilization. PLoS ONE. 2013;8:1–12. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0078767 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Mitchell DE, Della Santina CC, Cullen KE. Plasticity within non-cerebellar pathways rapidly shapes motor performance in vivo. Nat Commun. 2016;7:1–13. doi: 10.1038/ncomms11238 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.DiE M, Della Santina CC, Cullen KE. Plasticity within excitatory and inhibitory pathways of the vestibulo-spinal circuitry guides changes in motor performance. Sci Rep. 2017;7:1–15. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-00956-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Chow MR, Ayiotis AI, Schoo DP, Gimmon Y, Lane KE, Morris BJ, et al. Posture, Gait, Quality of Life, and Hearing with a Vestibular Implant. N Engl J Med. 2021;384:521–32. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa2020457 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Fornos AP, Guinand N, Van De Berg R, Stokroos R, Micera S, Kingma H, et al. Artificial balance: Restoration of the vestibulo-ocular reflex in humans with a prototype vestibular neuroprosthesis. Front Neurol. 2014:66. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2014.00066 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Huterer M, Cullen KE. Vestibuloocular reflex dynamics during high-frequency and high-acceleration rotations of the head on body in Rhesus monkey. J Neurophysiol. 2002;88:13–28. doi: 10.1152/jn.2002.88.1.13 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Ramachandran R, Lisberger SG. Normal performance and expression of learning in the vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR) at high frequencies. J Neurophysiol. 2005;93:2028–38. doi: 10.1152/jn.00832.2004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Highstein SM, Goldberg JM, Moschovakis AK, Fernandez C. Inputs from regularly and irregularly discharging vestibular nerve afferents to secondary neurons in the vestibular nuclei of the squirrel monkey. II. Correlation with output pathways of secondary neurons. J Neurophysiol. 1987;58:719–38. doi: 10.1152/jn.1987.58.4.719 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Boyle R, Goldberg JM, Highstein SM. Inputs from regularly and irregularly discharging vestibular nerve afferents to secondary neurons in squirrel monkey vestibular nuclei. III. Correlation with vestibulospinal and vestibuloocular output pathways. J Neurophysiol. 1992;68:471–84. doi: 10.1152/jn.1992.68.2.471 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Goldberg JM, Highstein SM, Moschovakis AK, Fernandez C. Inputs from regularly and irregularly discharging vestibular nerve afferents to secondary neurons in the vestibular nuclei of the squirrel monkey. I An electrophysiological analysis. J Neurophysiol. 1987;58:700–18. doi: 10.1152/jn.1987.58.4.700 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Minor LB, Goldberg JM. Vestibular-nerve inputs to the vestibulo-ocular reflex: A functional-ablation study in the squirrel monkey. J Neurosci. 1991;11:1636–48. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.11-06-01636.1991 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Carriot J, Jamali M, Chacron MJ, Cullen KE. Statistics of the vestibular input experienced during natural self-motion: Implications for neural processing. J Neurosci. 2014;34:8347–57. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0692-14.2014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Carriot J, Jamali M, Chacron MJ, Cullen KE. The statistics of the vestibular input experienced during natural self-motion differ between rodents and primates. J Physiol. 2017;595:2751–66. doi: 10.1113/JP273734 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Brooks JX, Carriot J, Cullen KE. Learning to expect the unexpected: Rapid updating in primate cerebellum during voluntary self-motion. Nat Neurosci. 2015;18:1310–7. doi: 10.1038/nn.4077 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Sadeghi SG, Minor LB, Cullen KE. Dynamics of the horizontal vestibuloocular reflex after unilateral labyrinthectomy: Response to high frequency, high acceleration, and high velocity rotations. Exp Brain Res. 2006;175:471–84. doi: 10.1007/s00221-006-0567-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Goldberg JM, Fernandez C. Physiology of peripheral neurons innervating semicircular canals of the squirrel monkey. I. Resting discharge and response to constant angular accelerations. J Neurophysiol. 1971;34:635–60. doi: 10.1152/jn.1971.34.4.635 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Lewis RF, Haburcakova C, Gong W, Makary C, Merfeld DM. Vestibuloocular reflex adaptation investigated with chronic motion-modulated electrical stimulation of semicircular canal afferents. J Neurophysiol. 2010;103:1066–79. doi: 10.1152/jn.00241.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Boutros PJ, Valentin NS, Hageman KN, Dai C, Roberts DC, Della Santina CC. Nonhuman primate vestibuloocular reflex responses to prosthetic vestibular stimulation are robust to pulse timing errors caused by temporal discretization. J Neurophysiol. 2019;121:2256–66. doi: 10.1152/jn.00887.2018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Saginaw MA. Eye movement studies with a vestibular prosthesis. Massachusetts Institute of Dent Tech. 2010. Available from: https://dspace.mit.edu/handle/1721.1/57680.
  • 36.Phillips JO, Ling L, Nowack AL, Phillips CM, Nie K, Rubinstein JT. The dynamics of prosthetically elicited vestibulo-ocular reflex function across frequency and context in the rhesus monkey. Front Neurosci. 2018:88. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00088 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Gong W, Merfeld DM. Prototype Neural Semicircular Canal Prosthesis using Patterned Electrical Stimulation. Ann Biomed Eng. 2000;28:572–81. doi: 10.1114/1.293 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Gong W, Merfeld DM. System design and performance of a unilateral horizontal semicircular canal prosthesis. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng. 2002;49:175–81. doi: 10.1109/10.979358 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Karmali F, Haburcakova C, Gong W, Della Santina CC, Merfeld DM, Lewis RF. An Implanted Vestibular Prosthesis Improves Spatial Orientation in Animals with Severe Vestibular Damage. J Neurosci. 2021;41: 3879 LP–88. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2204-20.2021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Hullar TE, Minor LB. High-frequency dynamics of regularly discharging canal afferents provide a linear signal for angular vestibuloocular reflexes. J Neurophysiol. 1999;82:2000–5. doi: 10.1152/jn.1999.82.4.2000 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Boyle R, Goldberg JM, Highstein SM. Vestibular Nerve Inputs to Vestibulospinal and Vestibulo-ocular Neurons of the Squirrel Monkey. The Head-Neck Sensory Motor System. New York: Oxford University Press; 2012. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195068207.003.0040 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Peterson EH. Are there parallel channels in the vestibular nerve? News Physiol Sci. 1998;13:194–201. doi: 10.1152/physiologyonline.1998.13.4.194 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Hadi Z, Pondeca Y, Calzolari E, Mahmud M, Chepisheva M, Smith RM, et al. The human brain networks mediating the vestibular sensation of self-motion. bioRxiv. 2022; 2021.12.03.471139. doi: 10.1101/2021.12.03.471139 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Calzolari E, Chepisheva M, Smith RM, Mahmud M, Hellyer PJ, Tahtis V, et al. Vestibular agnosia in traumatic brain injury and its link to imbalance. Brain. 2021;144:128–43. doi: 10.1093/brain/awaa386 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Schneider AD, Jamali M, Carriot J, Chacron MJ, Cullen KE. The increased sensitivity of irregular peripheral canal and otolith vestibular afferents optimizes their encoding of natural stimuli. J Neurosci. 2015;35:5522–36. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3841-14.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Aplin FP, Fridman GY. Implantable Direct Current Neural Modulation: Theory, Feasibility, and Efficacy. Front Neurosci. 2019. Available from: https://www.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fnins.2019.00379. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Fridman GY, Della SCC. Safe Direct Current Stimulation to Expand Capabilities of Neural Prostheses. IEEE Trans Neural Syst Rehabil Eng. 2013;21:319–28. doi: 10.1109/TNSRE.2013.2245423 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Steinhardt CR, Fridman GY. Direct current effects on afferent and hair cell to elicit natural firing patterns. iScience. 2021;24: 102205. doi: 10.1016/j.isci.2021.102205 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Sadeghi SG, Minor LB, Cullen KE. Neural Correlates of Motor Learning in the Vestibulo-Ocular Reflex: Dynamic Regulation of Multimodal Integration in the Macaque Vestibular System. J Neurosci. 2010;30:10158 LP–68. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1368-10.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Sadeghi SG, Minor LB, Cullen KE. Multimodal Integration After Unilateral Labyrinthine Lesion: Single Vestibular Nuclei Neuron Responses and Implications for Postural Compensation. J Neurophysiol. 2010;105:661–73. doi: 10.1152/jn.00788.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Sun DQ, Ward BK, Semenov YR, Carey JP, Della Santina CC. Bilateral Vestibular Deficiency: Quality of Life and Economic Implications. JAMA Otolaryngol Neck Surg. 2014;140:527–34. doi: 10.1001/jamaoto.2014.490 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Hedjoudje A, Hayden R, Dai C, Ahn J, Rahman M, Risi F, et al. Virtual Rhesus Labyrinth Model Predicts Responses to Electrical Stimulation Delivered by a Vestibular Prosthesis. J Assoc Res Otolaryngol. 2019;20:313–39. doi: 10.1007/s10162-019-00725-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Hedjoudje A, Schoo DP, Ward BK, Carey JP, Della Santina CC, Pearl M. Vestibular Implant Imaging. Am J Neuroradiol. 2021;42:370 LP–6. doi: 10.3174/ajnr.A6991 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Stultiens JJA, Postma AA, Guinand N, Pérez Fornos A, Kingma H, van de Berg R. Vestibular Implantation and the Feasibility of Fluoroscopy-Guided Electrode Insertion. Otolaryngol Clin North Am. 2020;53: 115–26. doi: 10.1016/j.otc.2019.09.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.George JA, Kluger DT, Davis TS, Wendelken SM, Okorokova EV, He Q, et al. Biomimetic sensory feedback through peripheral nerve stimulation improves dexterous use of a bionic hand. Sci Robot. 2019;4:eaax2352. doi: 10.1126/scirobotics.aax2352 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Osborn LE, Dragomir A, Betthauser JL, Hunt CL, Nguyen HH, Kaliki RR, et al. Prosthesis with neuromorphic multilayered e-dermis perceives touch and pain. Sci Robot. 2018;3:eaat3818. doi: 10.1126/scirobotics.aat3818 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Valle G, Mazzoni A, Iberite F, D’Anna E, Strauss I, Granata G, et al. Biomimetic Intraneural Sensory Feedback Enhances Sensation Naturalness, Tactile Sensitivity, and Manual Dexterity in a Bidirectional Prosthesis. Neuron. 2018;100:37–45.e7. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.08.033 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Abraira VE, Ginty DD. The Sensory Neurons of Touch. Neuron. 2013;79:618–39. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.07.051 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Glasauer S. Current models of the ocular motor system. Dev Ophthalmol. 2007:158–74. doi: 10.1159/000100485 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Gabriel Gasque

4 Mar 2022

Dear Dr Cullen,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript entitled "Restoring vestibular afferent dynamics enhances sensorimotor performance: implications for vestibular prostheses" for consideration as a Research Article by PLOS Biology.

Your manuscript has now been evaluated by the PLOS Biology editorial staff, as well as by an academic editor with relevant expertise, and I am writing to let you know that we would like to send your submission out for external peer review.

However, before we can send your manuscript to reviewers, we need you to complete your submission by providing the metadata that is required for full assessment. To this end, please login to Editorial Manager where you will find the paper in the 'Submissions Needing Revisions' folder on your homepage. Please click 'Revise Submission' from the Action Links and complete all additional questions in the submission questionnaire.

Once your full submission is complete, your paper will undergo a series of checks in preparation for peer review. Once your manuscript has passed the checks it will be sent out for review. To provide the metadata for your submission, please Login to Editorial Manager (https://www.editorialmanager.com/pbiology) within two working days, i.e. by Mar 06 2022 11:59PM.

If your manuscript has been previously reviewed at another journal, PLOS Biology is willing to work with those reviews in order to avoid re-starting the process. Submission of the previous reviews is entirely optional and our ability to use them effectively will depend on the willingness of the previous journal to confirm the content of the reports and share the reviewer identities. Please note that we reserve the right to invite additional reviewers if we consider that additional/independent reviewers are needed, although we aim to avoid this as far as possible. In our experience, working with previous reviews does save time.

If you would like to send previous reviewer reports to us, please email me at ggasque@plos.org to let me know, including the name of the previous journal and the manuscript ID the study was given, as well as attaching a point-by-point response to reviewers that details how you have or plan to address the reviewers' concerns.

During the process of completing your manuscript submission, you will be invited to opt-in to posting your pre-review manuscript as a bioRxiv preprint. Visit http://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/s/preprints for full details. If you consent to posting your current manuscript as a preprint, please upload a single Preprint PDF.

Given the disruptions resulting from the ongoing COVID-19 pandemic, please expect some delays in the editorial process. We apologise in advance for any inconvenience caused and will do our best to minimize impact as far as possible.

Feel free to email us at plosbiology@plos.org if you have any queries relating to your submission.

Kind regards,

Gabriel

Gabriel Gasque

Senior Editor

PLOS Biology

ggasque@plos.org

Decision Letter 1

Kris Dickson

15 Apr 2022

Dear Dr Cullen,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript "Restoring vestibular afferent dynamics enhances sensorimotor performance: implications for vestibular prostheses" for consideration as a Research Article at PLOS Biology. Your manuscript has been evaluated by the PLOS Biology editors, an Academic Editor with relevant expertise, and by several independent reviewers.

In light of the reviews (below), we will not be able to accept the current version of the manuscript, but we would welcome re-submission of a much-revised version that takes into account all of the reviewers' comments. In considering the reviewer feedback, we particularly note that all of the Reviewers expressed concerns with the low VOR-evoked gain you observe and Reviewer 1 raised some additional concerns with your virtual head motion design. The Academic Editor agreed that a particular concern is the low gain of the prothesis based VOR and that this is further complicated by the gain difference you see between the 2 animals. They also suggested that a potential explanation for the low gains, might, in fact, be related to the study reliance on direct stimulation rather than stimulation reflecting natural head movements.

In any revision we would therefore be particularly looking to see how you address the concerns related to gain levels and head motion. Along these lines, if possible, we ask that you add date on prosthetic stimulation patterns reflecting natural head movements. If you do not have such data and the experimental animals are no longer available for further assessment, we would be looking for a convincing argument as to why a consideration of natural head movements is not needed to appreciate that relevance of "nature-like" stimulation patterns. This is particularly important given the potential usage of your findings for prostheses design and would be a consideration for any final decision on your study. Additionally, we would expect to see a substantial revamping of the study presentation in any revision to PLOS Biology to ensure your study is accessible to our broad scientific readership. This should also include some expansion to the discussion to further touch on the broader implications of your work beyond the vestibular system and how this fits in with evidence from other domains – something you only touch on very briefly at the moment.

Please note that we cannot make any decision about publication until we have seen the revised manuscript and your response to the reviewers' comments. Your revised manuscript is also likely to be sent for further evaluation by the reviewers.

We expect to receive your revised manuscript within 3 months.

Please email us (plosbiology@plos.org) if you have any questions or concerns, or would like to request an extension. At this stage, your manuscript remains formally under active consideration at our journal; please notify us by email if you do not intend to submit a revision so that we may end consideration of the manuscript at PLOS Biology.

**IMPORTANT - SUBMITTING YOUR REVISION**

Your revisions should address the specific points made by each reviewer. Please submit the following files along with your revised manuscript:

1. A 'Response to Reviewers' file - this should detail your responses to the editorial requests, present a point-by-point response to all of the reviewers' comments, and indicate the changes made to the manuscript.

*NOTE: In your point by point response to the reviewers, please provide the full context of each review. Do not selectively quote paragraphs or sentences to reply to. The entire set of reviewer comments should be present in full and each specific point should be responded to individually, point by point.

You should also cite any additional relevant literature that has been published since the original submission and mention any additional citations in your response.

2. In addition to a clean copy of the manuscript, please also upload a 'track-changes' version of your manuscript that specifies the edits made. This should be uploaded as a "Related" file type.

*Re-submission Checklist*

When you are ready to resubmit your revised manuscript, please refer to this re-submission checklist: https://plos.io/Biology_Checklist

To submit a revised version of your manuscript, please go to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pbiology/ and log in as an Author. Click the link labelled 'Submissions Needing Revision' where you will find your submission record.

Please make sure to read the following important policies and guidelines while preparing your revision:

*Published Peer Review*

Please note while forming your response, if your article is accepted, you may have the opportunity to make the peer review history publicly available. The record will include editor decision letters (with reviews) and your responses to reviewer comments. If eligible, we will contact you to opt in or out. Please see here for more details:

https://blogs.plos.org/plos/2019/05/plos-journals-now-open-for-published-peer-review/

*PLOS Data Policy*

Please note that as a condition of publication PLOS' data policy (http://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/s/data-availability) requires that you make available all data used to draw the conclusions arrived at in your manuscript. If you have not already done so, you must include any data used in your manuscript either in appropriate repositories, within the body of the manuscript, or as supporting information (N.B. this includes any numerical values that were used to generate graphs, histograms etc.). For an example see here: http://www.plosbiology.org/article/info%3Adoi%2F10.1371%2Fjournal.pbio.1001908#s5

*Blot and Gel Data Policy*

We require the original, uncropped and minimally adjusted images supporting all blot and gel results reported in an article's figures or Supporting Information files. We will require these files before a manuscript can be accepted so please prepare them now, if you have not already uploaded them. Please carefully read our guidelines for how to prepare and upload this data: https://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/s/figures#loc-blot-and-gel-reporting-requirements

*Protocols deposition*

To enhance the reproducibility of your results, we recommend that if applicable you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io, where a protocol can be assigned its own identifier (DOI) such that it can be cited independently in the future. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols

Thank you again for your submission to our journal. We hope that our editorial process has been constructive thus far, and we welcome your feedback at any time. Please don't hesitate to contact us if you have any questions or comments.

Sincerely,

Kris

Kris Dickson

Neurosciences Senior Editor/Section Manager

PLOS Biology

kdickson@plos.org

*****************************************************

REVIEWS:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

Reviewer #3: No

Reviewer #1: This is an extremely interesting and provocative paper. It is well and clearly written and the figures are superb. That is this manuscript certainly deserves to be published. I have only one major remark concerning the choice of recording the VOR in head fixed monkeys. The rest of my comments, apart from typos, concerned the "pedagogic" aspects of the introduction and result sections. This paper deserves a large audience and particularly it should be understandable by ENT doctors, being its topic. However, the fact is that some paragraphs, and concepts, which are highlighted below, may be difficult to understand by readers (including the ENT) not too familiar with the vestibular physiology and its physiopathology. These passages should be corrected to ameliorate their comprehension.

More precisely:

I would suggest a more detailed title: Restoring vestibular afferent dynamics in Macaque monkeys enhances sensorimotor performance: implications for vestibular prostheses in Human

Introduction

The authors state that their protocols used "virtual head motion "in the two monkeys with complete vestibular loss. That is, the prosthesis pulses were delivered as if the monkeys' heads were moving, but they were actually stationary in the chair. Why this option was chosen? It means that the afferent information encoded by the spindles of the neck muscles did not occur during these virtual head motion, which could have impacted the VOR since proprioceptive information converge with the vestibular information in the vestibular nuclei and reticular formation. That would plead to test the stimulations during real head movements. Why it was not done? Please comment that point for the reader.

The vestibular system detects our head motion relative to space. In turn, this information is used to generate essential stabilizing reflexes and to provide us with our subjective sense of motion and orientation.

I would suggest a more detailed sentence to help the uninformed reader: . In turn, this information is used to generate essential stabilizing reflexes and complex motor synergies to control gaze and posture and to provide us with our subjective sense of motion and orientation.

First, the three axes of head rotation are encoded by three separate sensory organs (i.e., the semicircular canals),

I understand that sentence, but again it may be confusing for the uninformed reader not to mention the otoliths sensors in one way or the other at that stage.

To address this question, here we directly tested whether implementing the natural response

dynamics of vestibular afferents improves prosthesis performance.

Please add that this was done in a primate model

parameter space

Please define that concept

Using computational methods, we then demonstrated that the same model could account for the eye movements evoked by both sinusoidal and transient stimulation, and that the efficacy between stimulation pulses and afferent firing is substantially less than 1:1 as often assumed.

This point is important and the description is too sketchy to be easily understood by an average reader at that stage of the manuscript. If I get it well, do the authors mean that it is often assume that a single pulse of stimulation would trigger each time one (or several?) action potentials? If it is the case how a computational model can test that hypothesis and why an experiment approach was not used also.

Methods

by two mappings mimicking the natural high-pass response dynamics of the two types of vestibular afferents

A more detailed explanation of the terms "natural high-pass response dynamics" is required for the reader unfamiliar with the vestibular neurophysiology and possibly a figure to illustrate that point also.

All surgical and experimental protocols were approved by the Johns Hopkins Animal Care and Use Committee.

Date of approval, reference of the demand and as stated by PNAS

Animal Research (involving vertebrate

animals, embryos or tissues)

Provide the name of the Institutional Animal

Care and Use Committee (IACUC) or other

relevant ethics board that reviewed the

study protocol, and indicate whether they

approved this research or granted a formal

waiver of ethical approval

*

Include an approval number if one was

obtained

*

If the study involved non-human primates,

add additional details about animal welfare

and steps taken to ameliorate suffering

The vestibular function, the way it is written for the two monkey is confusing and need to be revised. Monkey Y was implanted with canal intact in the right ear. In addition, it had non-functioning old prosthesis electrodes in the left ear. Reading that description, this monkey should have an intact vestibular function. Why it ended up with a model of bilateral vestibular loss? Monkey G had received gentamicin treatment via bilateral intratympanic injection. Presumably this one should have a bilateral vestibular loss as a result of the injection. In that condition, why subsequently, a labyrinthectomy in the right ear was performed and why on one side only?

Typo problems in my text : 170 uA and 250 uA. Presumably micro Amp ?

digitized at 30 kHz and later synced: what mean syned? Synchronized

Reviewer #2: In this study, the authors investigate how to optimize vestibular prostheses (that restore the sense of rotation by stimulating the vestibular organs in the inner ear). They demonstrate that incorporating in the prosthesis a transfer function that mimics the vestibular afferents' dynamics improves the temporal accuracy of behavioural responses in two rhesus monkeys. This study is an important step in the development of neuroprotheses, and also an impressive technical achievement.

The main conclusion of the study (regarding the dynamics of the response) is solidly supported.

However, one limitation of vestibular prostheses is that their response gains are very low, and it is the case in this study. The authors propose that this is due to a poor stimulation efficiency between the prosthesis and the afferent, but this conclusion isn't be well supported, and should be reconsidered.

Major remarks:

The author observe that the gain of the VOR evoked by the prosthesis is 28% in one animal and 4.5% in the other. To explain this, they propose that the stimulation efficiency (i.e. the frequency of the discharge of the vestibular afferent relative to the pulse rate of the prosthesis) is 28% or 4.5% respectively. This can explain the observed data, indeed. However, the authors don't consider any alternative hypothesis. One possible alternative is that central VOR pathways readapt following the vestibular lesion. Another alternative is that the prosthesis evokes synchronous firing that is less efficient at driving VOR. None of these hypothesis can be discounted based on the author's data. Therefore, the authors haven't demonstrated that stimulation efficiency is less than 1:1, and this conclusion should be removed from the abstract, introduction, etc.

Note that this point has major implications for the development of vestibular prosthesis. If the low gain of the VOR is indeed attributable to a low stimulation efficiency, then the implication is that developing better stimulation methods could eventually restore normal VOR. On the other hand, if it is due to central adaptation (including possibly physiological changes in the vestibular nuclei), then this adaptation is the bottleneck.

Figure 6 and S6: the gain of the response is much higher in animal G (28%) compared to Y (4.5%). The author show animal G in the main figure 6 and Y in the supplementary figure S2: this is not a very balanced way to present data. It feels like the authors emphasize animal G and, in layman language, 'hide the bad monkey' in the supplementary figure. Given how the figure is organized, there is all the space necessary to show both animals in Fig. 6, and no justification not to do so.

Figure 2A: superimposing the 'normalize eye velocity' to an 'inverted head velocity' is incorrect. First, the head is not actually moving so it would be proper to refer to a 'simulated' head velocity. Second, head velocity has a unit of degrees per second, whereas the normalized eye velocity is unitless. Third, and fundamentally, the authors are giving the impression that the VOR is approximately compensatory at 10Hz, which is not the case (Fig. S1). Merging Fig. S1 and Fig. 2 would be a more balanced way to present the results.

Minor remarks:

Figure 3: when comparing Fig. 3A and Fig. 3C, it appears that the normalized gain of the VOR resembles a lot the gain of the afferent's transfer functions in Fig. 3A. In other words, in seems that the gain of the VOR compared to stimulation amplitude is constant. Given this, wouldn't it be sensible to program a transfer function with a flat gain, which would presumably induce a VOR with a constant gain along frequencies?

Methods: could you give more details about the animal's history? How much time elapsed between the implantations and the recordings shown here? Was the prosthetics continuously active in their home cage (or used on a regular basis)? Were they used in other studies? With the same implants?

Reviewer #3: This is a excellent piece of scientific work in a challenging area. I have no major criticisms.

The absolute gain responses are relatively low as shown in the supplementary information. I think at least an insert of the absolute gains should be shown in one of the main figures to give an idea of the current state of play of vestibular prostheses.

Did the investigators at any point, give real inertial stimuli in the light with the vestibular prosthesis turned on? If the vestibular prosthesis is only ever switched on in the dark to generate virtual eye movements, then there is no opportunity for the system to calibrate (and adapt) itself to the artificial input, irrespective of how well the investigators attempt to emulate natural stimuli.

Hence, have the authors assesed sinusoidal VOR in the light with prothesis on and off? If so, does the VOR gain improve over time?

In this regard, the authors mention that "the vestibular system is uniquely suited for prosthesis development" with which I agree. But there is little mention in the introduction about the development of the prosthesis being a clinically useful device for patients. In that regard, the authors should mention that human patients generally adapt well to bilateral vestibular loss with exercise so the benefits of a vestibular prosthesis on top of vestibular exercises would need to be balanced by the risks of the procedure/device. This risk-benefit curve would tilt one way or the other depending upon the relative efficacy and safety of the prosthesis. Such an argument would be useful in explaining the need for the current animal-based work (to improve efficacy and lower risks).

Has there been any finite element modelling to assess how the e-fields are affected by changing the stimulus parameters, which in turn may influence the impact upon local and not so local neural structures. I am not saying this needs to be done, simply it would be interesting to know if this has been done.

The assumptions and coefficients on pg 14 could only be easily understood by researchers who performed the studies that were referenced, but would otherwise require me to dig into the original papers. Is there anyway to justify the various parameter selections a little more without over-inflating the manuscript?

Page 16 - "Pathway modeling. Overall, a linear systems based VOR pathway model (Glasauer, 2007;Robinson, 1981) was used to predict the eye velocity from the afferent firing rate:" Does this statement assume that the afferent firing rate is 1:1 with stimulus frequency (as presumably you are not measuring the afferent firing rate - see next comment).

But, I think an important finding is the implication of a 28% (or 4.5% for monkey Y) stimulus efficacy. If this is an important barrior for clinical implementation, can the authors comment about the potential to surmount this barrier?

For the reported P values, can the mean and the statistic be consistently reported. Also, when relevant, correct the P value for multiple comparisons.

Decision Letter 2

Kris Dickson

2 Aug 2022

Dear Dr Cullen,

Thank you for your patience while we considered your revised manuscript "Restoring vestibular afferent dynamics enhances sensorimotor performance in Macaque monkeys: implications for vestibular prosthesis use in humans" for publication as a Research Article at PLOS Biology. This revised version of your manuscript has been evaluated by the PLOS Biology editors, the Academic Editor and the original reviewers.

Based on the reviews and feedback from our Academic Editor, we are likely to accept this manuscript for publication, provided you satisfactorily address the minor discussion comments from Reviewer 3, and the data and other policy-related requests at the bottom of this email.

Additionally, in order to ensure your study is as accessible as possible to our broad audience, we would like to suggest a slight title change to this work. We'd suggest: "A prosthesis utilizing natural vestibular encoding strategies improves sensorimotor performance in monkeys".

As you address the various journal-related items, please take this last chance to review your reference list to ensure that it is complete and correct. If you have cited papers that have been retracted, please include the rationale for doing so in the manuscript text, or remove these references and replace them with relevant current references. Any changes to the reference list should be mentioned in the cover letter that accompanies your revised manuscript.

We expect to receive your revised manuscript within two weeks.

To submit your revision, please go to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pbiology/ and log in as an Author. Click the link labelled 'Submissions Needing Revision' to find your submission record. Your revised submission must include the following:

- a cover letter that should detail your responses to any editorial requests, if applicable, and whether changes have been made to the reference list

- a Response to Reviewers file that provides a detailed response to the reviewers' comments (if applicable)

- a track-changes file indicating any changes that you have made to the manuscript.

NOTE: If Supporting Information files are included with your article, note that these are not copyedited and will be published as they are submitted. Please ensure that these files are legible and of high quality (at least 300 dpi) in an easily accessible file format. For this reason, please be aware that any references listed in an SI file will not be indexed. For more information, see our Supporting Information guidelines:

https://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/s/supporting-information

*Published Peer Review History*

Please note that you may have the opportunity to make the peer review history publicly available. The record will include editor decision letters (with reviews) and your responses to reviewer comments. If eligible, we will contact you to opt in or out. Please see here for more details:

https://blogs.plos.org/plos/2019/05/plos-journals-now-open-for-published-peer-review/

*Press*

Should you, your institution's press office or the journal office choose to press release your paper, please ensure you have opted out of Early Article Posting on the submission form. We ask that you notify us as soon as possible if you or your institution is planning to press release the article.

*Protocols deposition*

To enhance the reproducibility of your results, we recommend that if applicable you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io, where a protocol can be assigned its own identifier (DOI) such that it can be cited independently in the future. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols

Please do not hesitate to contact me should you have any questions.

Sincerely,

Kris

Kris Dickson, Ph.D. (she/her)

Neurosciences Senior Editor/Section Manager,

kdickson@plos.org,

PLOS Biology

------------------------------------------------------------------------

ETHICS STATEMENT:

Thank you for the information provided on the work you've performed in NHPs. When resubmitting the final version, please double check that this information fully complies with our policies here:

https://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/s/animal-research#loc-non-human-primates

In particular, non-human primate studies must be performed in accordance with the recommendations of the Weatherall report “The use of non-human primates in research”. Manuscripts describing research involving non-human primates must include details of animal welfare, including information about housing, feeding, and environmental enrichment, and steps taken to minimize suffering, including use of anesthesia and method of sacrifice if appropriate.

------------------------------------------------------------------------

DATA POLICY:

You may be aware of the PLOS Data Policy, which requires that all data be made available without restriction: http://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/s/data-availability. For more information, please also see this editorial: http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1001797

Note that we do not require all raw data. Rather, we ask that all individual quantitative observations that underlie the data summarized in the figures and results of your paper be made available in one of the following forms:

1) Supplementary files (e.g., excel). Please ensure that all data files are uploaded as 'Supporting Information' and are invariably referred to (in the manuscript, figure legends, and the Description field when uploading your files) using the following format verbatim: S1 Data, S2 Data, etc. Multiple panels of a single or even several figures can be included as multiple sheets in one excel file that is saved using exactly the following convention: S1_Data.xlsx (using an underscore).

2) Deposition in a publicly available repository. Please also provide the accession code or a reviewer link so that we may view your data before publication.

Regardless of the method selected, please ensure that you provide the individual numerical values that underlie the summary data displayed in the following figure graphs/heat maps as they are essential for readers to assess your analysis and to reproduce it: Fig2, Fig3, Fig4, Fig6BD, Supplemental Figures (all): 1-9

(Also note that main Fig 4 currently has panel C above panel B which is a bit confusing)

NOTE: the numerical data provided should include all replicates AND the way in which the plotted mean and errors were derived (it should not present only the mean/average values).

Please also ensure that FIGURE LEGENDS in your manuscript include information on where the underlying data can be found, and ensure your supplemental data file/s has a legend. (This step is often forgotten!!!)

Please ensure that your Data Statement in the submission system accurately describes where your data can be found.

------------------------------------------------------------------------

DATA NOT SHOWN?

- Please note that per journal policy, we do not allow the mention of "data not shown", "personal communication", "manuscript in preparation" or other references to data that is not publicly available or contained within this manuscript. Please double check your submission and either remove mention of any such data or provide figures presenting the results and the data underlying the figure(s).

------------------------------------------------------------------------

Reviewer remarks:

Reviewer #1: The reviewer thanks the authors to have taken into account all his suggestions and in particular to have include new data on prosthetic stimulation patterns evoked by actual physical head movements.

Reviewer #2: The authors did a very nice job at revising an already impressive manuscript. I particularly appreciate the thoughtfulness with which they responded to my remarks, and I have no further comments to make.

Reviewer #3: I am satisfied with the author's replies and changes in the manuscript in response to my comments. The changes further strengthen the paper.

I have two small comments about the Discussion.

The paragraph in Discussion Ln 447 is important as it highlights how different stimulus settings may suite different vestibular mechanisms, e.g. VOR vs. perception vs. postural control, important for future optimisation of the prosthesis - as they note in Ln 456 - "Thus, we speculate that the biomimetic irregular mapping might be better suited for postural control and self-motion perception, whereas the biomimetic regular and mixed mappings produced the most temporally accurate VOR eye movements as was observed in our present study.", hence....

- the authors should make a note in their manuscript that in humans, cortical networks mediating vestibular perception and postural control actually overlap in the right temporal cortex in the inferior longitudinal fasciculus (Calzolari et al., Brain DOI: 10.1093/brain/awaa386). It follows that in line with their comments, future prosthesis optimisation should involve perceptual and postural measures.

- In Ln455 they should qualify the contribution of ascending pathways to perception a little more precisely. These pathways specifically convey the vestibular signals to cortical brain circuits and it is these brain circuits that are more closely engaged in the processes that mediate vestibular perception (add reference doi: Hadi et al. BioRxiv https://doi.org/10.1101/2021.12.03.471139). This seems a little pedantic since what the authors wrote was not wrong, but the suggested change seems clearer to my mind.

Decision Letter 3

Kris Dickson

16 Aug 2022

Dear Dr Cullen,

Thank you for the submission of your revised Research Article "A prosthesis utilizing natural vestibular encoding strategies improves sensorimotor performance in monkeys" for publication in PLOS Biology. On behalf of my colleagues and the Academic Editor, Peter Thier, I am pleased to say that we can in principle accept your manuscript for publication, provided you address any remaining formatting and reporting issues. These will be detailed in an email you should receive within 2-3 business days from our colleagues in the journal operations team; no action is required from you until then. Please note that we will not be able to formally accept your manuscript and schedule it for publication until you have completed any requested changes.

When you are making any of these requested changes, we'd also encourage you to upload a spreadsheet-based form of the underlying data for your study (e.g Excel-based). While the MATLAB files you've deposited in Zenodo will be helpful to researchers in the field, not all of our interested readership will necessarily have the ability to open and access the .mat file format.

Please take a minute to log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pbiology/, click the "Update My Information" link at the top of the page, and update your user information to ensure an efficient production process.

PRESS

We frequently collaborate with press offices. If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper at this point, to enable them to help maximize its impact. If the press office is planning to promote your findings, we would be grateful if they could coordinate with biologypress@plos.org. If you have previously opted in to the early version process, we ask that you notify us immediately of any press plans so that we may opt out on your behalf.

We also ask that you take this opportunity to read our Embargo Policy regarding the discussion, promotion and media coverage of work that is yet to be published by PLOS. As your manuscript is not yet published, it is bound by the conditions of our Embargo Policy. Please be aware that this policy is in place both to ensure that any press coverage of your article is fully substantiated and to provide a direct link between such coverage and the published work. For full details of our Embargo Policy, please visit http://www.plos.org/about/media-inquiries/embargo-policy/.

Thank you again for choosing PLOS Biology for publication and supporting Open Access publishing. We look forward to publishing your study. 

Sincerely, 

Kris

Kris Dickson, Ph.D. (she/her)

Neurosciences Senior Editor/Section Manager

PLOS Biology

kdickson@plos.org

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Fig. VOR data of each monkey prior to normalization.

    (A) VOR gains prior to normalization across natural frequency range (0.2–20 Hz). Note that the figure shows the same data as Fig 2B but the gains are displayed on the absolute scale (not normalized). (B) Example VOR traces prior to normalization. Dashed lines indicate inverted, virtual head velocity. The shaded areas indicate SEM. Red, blue, and gray refer to the irregular, regular, and static mappings, respectively. Data underlying this figure can be found at https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.6338639.

    (TIF)

    S2 Fig. VOR data of each monkey prior to normalization with 2 additional mappings.

    (A) VOR gains prior to normalization across natural frequency range (0.2–20 Hz). Note that the figure shows the same data as Fig 3C but the gains are displayed on the absolute scale (not normalized). (B) Example VOR traces prior to normalization. Dashed lines indicate the inverted, virtual head velocity. The shaded areas indicate SEM. Yellow, red, purple, blue, and gray refer to the super high-pass, irregular, mixed, regular, and static mappings, respectively. Data underlying this figure can be found at https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.6338639.

    (TIF)

    S3 Fig. VOR gain and phase plots for the X2 condition.

    (A) Normalized VOR gains across natural frequency range (0.2–20 Hz), using the same normalization reference as Fig 2. Note the reduction in gain at 20 Hz due to saturation. (B) The phase response of the VOR. Note the similarity to the phase response in the X1 condition in Fig 3D. The shaded area indicated the SEM. Yellow, red, purple, blue, and gray refer to the super high-pass, irregular, mixed, regular, and static mappings, respectively. Data underlying this figure can be found at https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.6338639.

    (TIF)

    S4 Fig. VOR data during physical sinusoidal rotation in the dark.

    (A) VOR gains across the frequency range (0.2–10 Hz). (B) The phase response of the VOR. (C) Example VOR traces. Dashed lines indicate inverted head velocity. There was a significant increase in the VOR gain mainly over the 5–10 Hz range for Monkey G (p < 0.05, Bonferroni corrected) but not for Monkey Y. There was also a significant increase in phase lead mainly over 5–10 Hz range for both monkeys (p < 0.05, Bonferroni corrected). There was no significant difference in the gain and phase responses for early and late cycles in both monkeys. Yellow, red, purple, blue, and gray refer to the super high-pass, irregular, mixed, regular, and static mappings, respectively. Data underlying this figure can be found at https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.6338639.

    (TIF)

    S5 Fig. VOR data during physical sinusoidal rotation in the light.

    (A) VOR gains across the frequency range (0.2–10 Hz). (B) The phase response of the VOR. (C) Example VOR traces. Dashed line indicated the inverted head velocity. There was a significant increase in the VOR gain mainly over the 5–10 Hz range for Monkey G (p < 0.05, Bonferroni corrected) and at 10 Hz for Monkey Y (p < 0.05, Bonferroni corrected). There was also a significant increase in phase lead mainly over 5–10 Hz range for both monkeys (p < 0.05, Bonferroni corrected). There was no significant difference in the gain and phase responses for early and late cycles in both monkeys. Yellow, red, purple, blue, and gray refer to the super high-pass, irregular, mixed, regular, and static mappings, respectively. Data underlying this figure can be found at https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.6338639.

    (TIF)

    S6 Fig. Evoked VOR during virtual transient head movements in the off-direction.

    (A) Traces of the evoked eye movements for Monkey G during transient head movements (off-direction), normalized as in Fig 4. Dashed lines indicate the inverted head velocity. Dotted vertical line indicates the start of the head movements. Solid vertical line denotes the peak of the head movements. Short colored vertical lines indicated the estimated onset of the evoked eye movement. (B, C, and D) Quantification of the traces in (A) for normalized gain [using the same normalization reference as in Fig 4], time difference between eye and head velocity peaks, and onset latency, respectively. Results from Monkey G and Monkey Y are plotted on the left and on the right, respectively. Error bars indicate the SEM. Yellow, red, purple, blue, and gray refer to the super high-pass, irregular, mixed, regular, and static mappings, respectively. Similar to the on-direction results, the biomimetic regular and mixed mappings evoked a peak VOR response that was well aligned with the peak head velocity (not significantly different from 0 ms except for Monkey G mixed mapping, which showed a significant but small lead of 12.3 ± 1.7 ms, p < 0.01, Bonferroni corrected). In contrast, the static mapping resulted in the VOR peak with a significant delay (p < 0.001 for Monkey G, not significant for Monkey Y, Bonferroni corrected), while the VOR peaks evoked by irregular and super high-pass mappings actually led the stimulus (p < 0.05 for Monkey G, not significant for Monkey Y). Data underlying this figure can be found at https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.6338639.

    (TIF)

    S7 Fig. Evoked VOR during physical transient head movements in the on-direction.

    (A) Traces of the evoked eye movements for Monkey G during physical transient head movements (on-direction), normalized as in Fig 4. Dashed lines indicate the inverted head velocity, scaled to facilitate timing comparison. Dotted vertical line indicates the start of the head movements. Solid vertical line denotes the peak of the head movements. Short vertical lines indicated the estimated onset of the evoked eye movement. (B, C, and D) Quantification of the traces in (A) for normalized gain, time difference between eye and head velocity peaks, and onset latency, respectively. Results from Monkey G and Monkey Y are plotted on the left and on the right, respectively. Error bars indicate the SEM. Yellow, red, purple, blue, and gray refer to the super high-pass, irregular, mixed, regular, and static mappings, respectively. Data underlying this figure can be found at https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.6338639.

    (TIF)

    S8 Fig. VAFs of the model prediction of virtual transient eye velocity using the virtual sinusoidal data.

    The VAFs are plotted for each trial of the head movements in the on (top row) and off (bottom rows) directions for Monkey Y (left column) and Monkey G (right column). Error bars indicate the SEM. Data underlying this figure can be found at https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.6338639.

    (TIF)

    S9 Fig. Conventional approach in programming the static nonlinearity artificially constrained the dynamic range of prostheses to an unnaturally narrow band.

    (A) Post-sigmoid stimulation rate plotted as a function of pre-sigmoid stimulation rate. The red arrow points to the data point from the super high-pass 2× gain mapping at 20 Hz, which showed large deviation from linearity. (B) Similar to the model in Fig 6C but with the addition of the CNS adaptation (17%; [18]). (C) Plots of the actual eye velocity and the predicted eye velocity from the model in (B) using 100% stimulation efficacy (top) and the lower stimulation efficacy that best fit the data (bottom). The dashed line is the unity line. Data underlying this figure can be found at https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.6338639.

    (TIF)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: 2022 06 27VOR_paper PlosBio Review Responses.docx

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: 2022 08 09 response.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    All data associated with this study are present in the paper or the Supplementary Materials. The dataset is available from Zenodo database: https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.6338639.


    Articles from PLoS Biology are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES