Abstract
Posture and gait are maintained by sensory inputs from the vestibular, visual, and somatosensory systems and motor outputs. Upon vestibular damage, the visual and/or somatosensory systems functionally substitute by cortical mechanisms called “sensory reweighting”. We investigated the cerebrocortical mechanisms underlying sensory reweighting after unilateral labyrinthectomy (UL) in mice. Arc-dVenus transgenic mice, in which the gene encoding the fluorescent protein dVenus is transcribed under the control of the promoter of the immediate early gene Arc, were used in combination with whole-brain three-dimensional (3D) imaging. Performance on the rotarod was measured as a behavioral correlate of sensory reweighting. Following left UL, all mice showed the head roll-tilt until UL10, indicating the vestibular periphery damage. The rotarod performance worsened in the UL mice from UL1 to UL3, which rapidly recovered. Whole-brain 3D imaging revealed that the number of activated neurons in S1, but not in V1, in UL7 was higher than that in sham-treated mice. At UL7, medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) and agranular insular cortex (AIC) activation was also observed. Therefore, sensory reweighting to the somatosensory system could compensate for vestibular dysfunction following UL; further, mPFC and AIC contribute to the integration of sensory and motor functions to restore balance.
Subject terms: Neuroscience, Molecular medicine, Neurology
Introduction
Following unilateral damage to the vestibular inner ear, oculomotor and postural symptoms such as nystagmus and postural instability occur, but gradually recover over time in a process known as vestibular compensation1–3. Brain plasticity is thought to be responsible for vestibular compensation, because functional recovery occurs without regeneration of the vestibular periphery. Vestibular compensation includes changes in the patterns of floccular inhibition of the vestibular nucleus complex (VNC) and the recovery of spontaneous firing and responsiveness in ipsilesional VNC neurons2,4,5. In addition to vestibular compensation, appropriate postural control after vestibular damage also depends on dynamic changes in multimodal sensorimotor control systems known as “sensory reweighting”6. Therefore, when studying the recovery after vestibular damage, it is important to consider not only vestibular compensation, in which the cerebellum-brainstem pathways are involved, but also the cerebrocortical mechanisms of sensory reweighting.
Immediately after acute vestibular insults, postural control shifts to visual and/or somatosensory dependence, which can substitute impaired vestibular function in humans7,8. Functional brain imaging studies have demonstrated that cerebral glucose metabolism is significantly decreased in the multisensory vestibular cortical and subcortical areas, such as the parieto-insular vestibular cortex (PIVC), but it is increased in the visual and somatosensory cortices in the chronic stage of vestibular neuritis9. A similar visual substitution in response to vestibular loss has also been observed in patients with bilateral vestibular failure10. Studies on spaceflight in humans have shown that sensory reweighting to the visual and somatosensory systems and adaptive cortical neuroplasticity occurred as a result of changes to vestibular signaling11,12. Sensory reweighting of visual/somatosensory dependence following these changes could be attributed to the functional changes observed in the cerebral cortices.
While cortical changes caused by vestibular stimulation and damage have been mostly assessed in humans, animal studies relevant to sensory reweighting in the vestibular system are limited. Based on the findings in humans, we investigated whether the visual and somatosensory cortices are related to sensory reweighting in mice. In particular, we focused on the barrel cortex (S1) in the somatosensory cortex because whisker sensation is superior in rodents. Since mice are genetically easy to manipulate, they are suitable as experimental animals for neuroscience research. In this study, we employed Arc-dVenus transgenic (Tg) mice, in which activated neurons were visualized using fluorescence. While Arc is not expressed in the cerebellum13, which is an essential site in the cerebellum-brainstem pathway for vestibular compensation, it does not interfere with the investigation of cerebral cortex mechanisms of sensory reweighting following vestibular injuries. Clear, Unobstructed Brain/Body Imaging Cocktails and Computational Analysis (CUBIC), and the light sheet fluorescence (LSF) microscope method enable tissue clearing with chemical reagents and 3D imaging of the whole brain14,15. Unlike brain histology with slicing, transparent CUBIC 3D whole-brain imaging provides comprehensive monitoring of the target cell population without technical loss of detection. In the present study, to investigate the cortical correlates of sensory reweighting following vestibular damage, cortical activation following chemical unilateral labyrinthectomy (UL) using arsanilate was investigated using CUBIC/LSF microscope methods in Arc-dVenus Tg mice. Chemical UL, rather than surgical UL, was chosen because surgical UL is technically difficult to perform at the small inner ear of mice. Arsanilate was used because previous studies have reported that UL using arsanilate is a suitable model to investigate the mechanisms of vestibular compensation in mice16.
Regions of interest (ROIs) were set at the visual cortex (V1) and somatosensory cortex (S1), which are known to substitute for vestibular loss in humans7–9. The posterior granular insular cortex (pGIC), a cortical area corresponding to the human/primate PIVC, was investigated following the loss of sensory inputs from the vestibular periphery. The primary auditory cortex (A1) was also investigated, since UL can affect not only the vestibular organ but also the cochlea. In addition to these preselected areas, newly identified areas that were activated following UL were also investigated. This includes the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), the agranular insular cortex (AIC), and the somatosensory cortex (S1). Behavioral correlates, including head roll-tilt and rotarod performance, were also investigated following UL.
Results
Behavioral study
Head roll-tilt
All mice that underwent left UL showed head roll-tilt towards the left side (Fig. 1A). The angle of head roll-tilt of UL mice gradually decreased, and was significantly larger than that of the sham-treated group from day 1 to day 10 following UL. The angle recovered to the same level as that exhibited by the sham-treated group by day 11 following UL (Fig. 1B).
Rotarod test
The time to fall (TTF) in the rotarod test group was significantly shorter in the UL than in the sham-treated group during the first 3 days after UL. The TTF in the UL group rapidly recovered to the same level as that in the sham-treated group by day 4 after UL, which was faster than the recovery observed on the head roll-tilt (Fig. 1C).
Whole-brain images
Figure 2 shows whole-brain images of sham-treated (Sham2 and Sham7) and UL individuals (UL2 and UL7) on day 2 and day 7 after surgery. Neural activation of the whole brain can be comprehensively visualized using a panoramic view, from which 3D images can also be obtained (Supplementary Movie 1). Hippocampal neurons were activated non-specifically in both groups.
Visual cortex (V1) and somatosensory cortex (S1)
On day 2 after surgery, there were no significant bilateral differences in the number of activated neurons in V1 and S1 between the UL and the sham-treated group. On day 7 after surgery, the number of activated neurons in the bilateral S1 was significantly higher in the UL than in the sham-treated mice, whereas no significant differences between these groups were found in V1 (Figs. 3, 4, 5, 6).
Posterior granular insular cortex (pGIC) and auditory cortex (A1)
There was no significant difference in the number of activated neurons in the pGIC between the UL and the sham-treated group at both day 2 and day 7 after surgery (Figs. 7, 8A). On day 2 after surgery, there were no significant differences in the number of activated neurons in A1 between the UL and the sham-treated group. However, on day 7 after surgery, the number of activated neurons in the bilateral A1 was significantly higher in the UL group than in the sham-treated group (Figs. 7, 8B).
Medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) and agranular insular cortex (AIC)
Whole-brain analysis under comprehensive and panoramic views demonstrated that activated neurons were found bilaterally in the medial-frontal cortex (Fig. 9A; arrowhead) and the parieto-temporal region (Fig. 9A; arrows) on UL2. A survey of arbitrary cross-sections (Fig. 9B–E) revealed that the activated neurons were located in the mPFC (Fig. 9C) and the caudate putamen (Fig. 9E). On UL7, activated neurons could also be found bilaterally in the anterior-temporal region (Fig. 10A; arrowheads) and the temporal region (Fig. 10A; arrows). A survey of arbitrary cross sections from UL7 (Fig. 10B–E) revealed that the activated neurons were located in the agranular insular cortex (AIC) (Fig. 10C) and in S1 (Fig. 10D), which is a target area for sensory reweighting. There were a few activated neurons in these areas in individuals Sham2 and Sham7 (data not shown). Regarding the cell count of the four subregions in the mPFC (Figs. 11, 12), the number of activated neurons was significantly higher in the UL group than in the sham-treated group on day 2 after surgery in the medial precentral cortex (mPC), dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dAC), and prelimbic cortex (PL). This number gradually decreased, but it was still significantly higher in the UL group on day 7 (Fig. 12). A few activated neurons were observed in the infralimbic cortex (IL). In the cell count of the three subregions in AIC (Figs. 11, 13), there were no significant differences between the groups on day 2, but the number of activated neurons in all the subregions was higher in mice from the UL group on day 7 compared to sham-treated mice.
Discussion
All the mice that underwent chemical UL showed ipsilesional head roll-tilt and impaired rotarod performance. Although the present study did not assess direct vestibular damages, these behavioral results were similar to those from a previous study using the same concentration of arsanilate in which peripheral vestibular organ damages were confirmed histologically16. The head roll-tilt peaked on day 2 and lasted up to 10 days after the UL (Fig. 1B). Ito et al. reported that the head roll-tilt peaked on day 2 and recovered by day 4 after UL in mice that had been administered arsanilate using a post-auricular approach. The effects of intratympanic injection of arsanilate were persistent in this study compared to the administration of the same arsanilate dose using a post-auricular approach16. This difference may therefore be explained by the different routes of administration. Intratympanic injection without skin incision may cause the arsanilate to be retained for longer in the tympanic cavity, resulting in stronger effects on the vestibular periphery. The rotarod test has been widely used to investigate vestibular imbalance after labyrinthectomy17 and motor deficits due to brain injury18,19. In our results, rotarod performance was disrupted from day 1 to day 3, and had recovered by day 4 after UL (Fig. 1C). The recovery observed on the rotarod performance was much faster than that for the head roll-tilt. Since the head roll-tilt is mainly controlled by otolith inputs, the recovery of the head roll-tilt may be directly proportional to vestibular compensation16. On the other hand, the rotarod performance seems to be related not only to vestibular function, but also to motor control and coordination by other sensory modalities, including sensory reweighting. Taken together, these results suggest that vestibular compensation had not yet been completed between days 2 to 10 after UL, and that sensory reweighting occurred on day 4 after UL. Our investigation of neural activation in the cerebral cortices revealed no significant differences in V1 and S1 between the UL and the sham-treated group on day 2, but significantly higher neural activity at S1 in the UL group on day 7. The results of the neural activity at S1 in UL animals are in agreement with the results of the rotarod behavioral tests.
Our study showed that on day 7 after UL, the number of activated neurons in both the right and left somatosensory cortices (S1) was significantly higher in UL than in sham-treated animals, while no significant differences were found on day 2 (Fig. 6B). Neural activation in S1 was not only noticed when the region was pre-selected as a potential area for sensory reweighting (Figs. 3B, 5), but also when a series of arbitrary cross-sections where scanned for signs of neural activation after UL (Fig. 10D). According to the rotarod results, sensory reweighting started not earlier, but later than day 4 after UL, and it was already present on day 7 after UL (Fig. 1C). Taking these results together, activation of the somatosensory cortex (S1) (Figs. 5, 6B) may play a role in sensory reweighting after UL in mice. In contrast, there were no significant differences in the number of activated neurons in either the right or left visual cortex (V1) between the UL and sham-treated groups on day 2 or on day 7 after UL (Figs. 4, 6A), demonstrating that the visual cortex (V1) was not activated after UL. This result suggests that sensory reweighting after vestibular loss in mice may not depend on visual input. According to motor function tests, including rotarod tests performed in normal and visually impaired mice with and without somatosensory inputs from the whiskers, the somatosensory inputs from whiskers are more essential for maintaining motor function in mice20. These results strongly support our findings that somatosensory (whisker) inputs play a key role in sensory reweighting after UL in mice, unlike the mechanism of sensory reweighting in humans. Although there is no evidence of direct neural connections from the peripheral vestibule or vestibular nuclei to S1 and V1, multisensory cortical areas in the mouse homologous to areas 2v, 3a, and PIVC in monkeys would convey vestibular, somatosensory, and visual information to S1 and V121.
It has been reported for rodents that the parieto-temporal (PT) region receives most of the projections from the vestibular nuclei22. However, there have been no reports on the vestibular cortex in mice. In this study, we assumed that the vestibular cortex in mice might be located in the pGIC, which is anatomically homologous to the vestibular cortex of other species (PIVC, ASSS, and PT). We set a 3D ROI for the pGIC as a potential vestibular cortex in mice. However, activated neurons were not found in the pGIC after the UL (Fig. 9A). It is suggested that the mouse vestibular cortex could not be identified in the pGIC using a vestibular deafferentation model, and that alternative research models, such as vestibular stimulation models, may be required to identify the vestibular cortex in mice.
Our study showed a significant increase in the number of activated neurons in the auditory cortex (A1) in mice on day 7 after UL, but not on day 2 (Fig. 9B). Because both UL and sham-treated animals were kept in the same conditions in terms of auditory input 2 days before decapitation, this difference was not due to reactivation by cochlear inputs from the healthy side. It may be considered that this is not the result of sensory reweighting but of disinhibition in A1, induced by the loss of peripheral sensory inputs due to cochlear damage by the UL.
In the survey on arbitrary cross sections from whole-brain imaging, the activated neurons were found in the mPFC on day 2 and day 7 after UL (Fig. 12). In rodents, the mPFC is divided into four subdivisions from dorsal to ventral: mPC, dAC, PL, and IL23–25 (Fig. 11A). The mPC and dAC are known to be linked to various motor behaviors, while PL and IL are associated with diverse emotional, cognitive, and mnemonic processes23,26. Stimulation of the mPC and dAC has been reported to induce movement of the eye, vibrissa, head, and hind limbs27–30. Therefore, the mPC and dAC in rodents are homologous to the frontal eye field, which acts as a supplementary motor and premotor cortex in primates31–33. The mPC overlaps with the vibrissa motor cortex (vM1) and receives numerous cortical inputs from the visual, somatosensory, auditory, parietal, retrosplenial, and orbital areas34–37. The mPC also projects to the superior colliculus38 and subcortical nuclei, involving oculomotor control39,40. Therefore, it is thought that the mPC plays a role in receiving multiple sensory inputs, in processing and in transforming them into motor functions41. In contrast to the motor-associated functions of the mPC and dAC, PL and IL are linked to the limbic system25,31,42 and profoundly influence visceral and autonomic activities, such as gastrointestinal motility and blood pressure control43,44. In our study, activated neurons were found in the mPC, dAC, and PL of the mPFC subdivision after UL (Fig. 12). Based on these neural connectivity and functions of the mPFC, the activation of the mPC and dAC at the early stage after UL may be related to changes in oculomotor control and integrative motor activity. PL activation may also be related to visceral and autonomic changes after UL. The basal ganglia (including the caudate putamen) control muscle tonus for posture, locomotion, and cognition through the basal ganglia-cortical and the basal ganglia-brainstem loops45,46. In our study, the activated neurons in the caudate putamen were observed in UL2 but decreased in UL7 (Figs. 9E, 10E). The results may be driven by the activation-deactivation of direct motor and postural control systems rather than sensory reweighting mechanisms.
The insular cortex (IC) comprises three different subareas, which differ in their cytoarchitecture of layer 4: the agranular (AIC), dysgranular (DIC), and granular (GIC) cortices (from ventral to dorsal). In our results, activated neurons were found in the AIC on day 7, but not on day 2 after UL. The IC has been suggested to function as a hub linking large-scale brain systems. It receives multisensory inputs from the outside environment, such as auditory, somatosensory, visual, olfactory, and gustatory information, as well as from inside the body, such as blood pressure, heartbeat, oxygenation, and input from the digestive system47. Gehrlach et al. divided the mouse IC into three equal subregions across its rostrocaudal extent and reported their connectivity to other cortical and subcortical regions. The middle and posterior subregions show similar connectivity with many sensory cortices and the amygdala. In contrast, the anterior subregion shows more connections to the motor cortex, prefrontal cortex, striatum, and sensory cortex48. In our results, activated neurons were found in all subregions (middle, posterior, and anterior) of the AIC on day 7 after UL. Neuronal activation of the somatosensory cortex (S1) (Figs. 5, 6B) may substitute for the loss of vestibular inputs following UL (that is, in sensory reweighting) leading to a recovery in the rotarod performance (Fig. 1C). Activation of the AIC (Fig. 13) may also play a role in sensory reweighting by integrating somatosensory and motor functions.
It has been reported that visual and somatosensory information are integrated at the early levels of the central nervous system such as the vestibular nuclei49–52, suggesting that sensory reweighting might occur at the brainstem level. Meanwhile, studies in humans have demonstrated changes of neural activities in the visual, somatosensory, and insular cortex after vestibular damage9, which is in partial agreement with our results: Only the somatosensory cortex, but not the visual cortex, was activated after UL, probably because rodents depend more on the sensory input from the whiskers than on visual inputs. Since how higher cortices such as V1, S1, mPFC, and IC influence the vestibular system is unknown, further research is needed.
Materials and methods
This study was carried out in compliance with the ARRIVE guidelines, and the protocols were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of Niigata University (IACUC; approval codes #SA00673 and #SD00787). All experiments were performed in accordance with the guidelines and regulations provided by IACUC.
Animals
Twenty C57BL/6J (CLEA Japan, Tokyo, Japan) and 16 Arc-dVenus male mice53, 8–10 weeks of age at the time of the experiments, were used for behavioral studies and whole-brain imaging studies, respectively. Because behavioral measurement could have affected the neuronal activities and CUBIC data, naive mice were used in behavioral studies. Arc-dVenus mice are transgenic heterozygotes mice that express fluorescent destabilized Venus protein under the control of the Arc immediate-early gene. The mice were housed in plastic cages with a 12-h light/dark cycle and under temperature-controlled conditions. Food and water were provided ad libitum.
Procedure of chemical unilateral labyrinthectomy
All surgical procedures were performed under deep anesthesia with pentobarbital (65 mg/kg, i.p.). Unilateral chemical labyrinthectomy was performed by transtympanic injection of 50 μL of sodium arsanilate (0.4 M; Tokyo Chemical Industry, Tokyo, Japan) dissolved in 0.9% saline16 into the left tympanic cavity. Animals belonging to the sham-treated group received injections of the same volume of 0.9% saline solution.
Behavioral studies
Twenty C57BL/6J naïve mice (10 in each group, UL and sham-treated) were used for behavioral observation. All of the mice that received intratympanic arsanilate showed behavioral abnormalities such as spontaneous nystagmus (SN) directed to the contralesional side and head roll-tilt directed to the ipsilesional side, indicating that intratympanic injection of arsanilate induced damage to the ipsilateral vestibular periphery. We did not use SN as a behavioral correlate of recovery after UL because of technical difficulties in the quantitative measurements of SN. To measure behavioral changes after left-side UL, the experimental animals were subjected to the head roll-tilt and rotarod tests. These two tests were used to estimate vestibular compensation and sensory reweighting, respectively, occurring in response to UL.
Head roll-tilt
Head roll-tilt, defined as the angle of the sagittal plane from the vertical plane, has been previously used to evaluate vestibular imbalance16. We measured the head roll-tilt on photographs once a day and used it as an indicator of vestibular imbalance following UL (Fig. 1A).
Rotarod test
The rotarod test has been widely applied to monitor motor performance in naïve mice54,55, in addition to changes in balance after labyrinthectomy17 and brain injury18,19. Mice were placed on the rod at a slow rotational speed (4 rpm). The speed was then gradually increased to 40 rpm over 120 s. Each session lasted 180 s. The session ceased if the mouse fell from the rod, at which point the time to fall (TTF) was recorded. The experiment consisted of three trials per day and the best performance was recorded. These measurements were performed during 14 consecutive days following UL.
Whole-brain imaging study
Sixteen Arc-dVenus mice were used for 3D visualization using CUBIC analysis. To minimize the effects on other sensory modalities, including external visual and auditory inputs, all mice used for imaging studies were placed in a light- and sound-deprived room for two days before decapitation. Animals were decapitated on days 2 or 7 after UL or sham surgery (4 groups of 4 mice each).
Tissue clearing
Arc-dVenus mice were anesthetized with a mixture of medetomidine hydrochloride (0.75 mg/kg body weight [BW]), midazolam (4 mg/kg BW), and butorphanol tartrate (5 mg/kg BW) on day 2 or day 7 after UL. The mice were then perfused with phosphate buffered saline (PBS pH 7.4) and 4% paraformaldehyde in PBS through the left ventricle. The brains were removed, post-fixed overnight in 4% paraformaldehyde in PBS at 4 °C and stored in PBS. The brains were rendered transparent for imaging according to the updated CUBIC method14,56. For 4–5 days, the brains were immersed in CUBIC-L [10/10 wt% chemical cocktail of N-butyldiethanolamine (B0725, Tokyo Chemical Industry) and Triton X-100 (12,967–45, Nacalai Tesque, Kyoto, Japan)] and shaken (80–100 rpm) at 37 °C. The brains were then washed three times with PBS at room temperature for 30 min and stained for 3 days at room temperature with RedDot™2 1:100 (#40061, Biotium, Fremont, CA, USA) in PBS containing 0.5 M NaCl. After staining, the brains were washed in PBS and shaken gently in 1:1 diluted CUBIC-R [45/30 wt% chemical cocktail of 45 wt% antipyrine (D1876, Tokyo Chemical Industry) and nicotinamide (N0078, Tokyo Chemical Industry), pH 10 adjusted by N-butyldiethanolamine] at room temperature for 6 h, and then shaken gently at room temperature overnight. Subsequently, the brains were immersed in fresh CUBIC-R and gently shaken at room temperature for at least 6 h.
Whole-brain imaging
Fluorescent images were acquired using custom-built LSF microscopes (MVX10-LS, Olympus, Tokyo, Japan) and a 0.63 × objective lens (numerical aperture = 0.15, working distance = 87 mm) with 1–1.6 × digital zoom. The LSF microscope was equipped with lasers emitting light at 488 and 637 nm. The objective lens accompanied the stage movement in the axial direction to avoid defocusing. During image acquisition, the refractive index (RI)-matched samples were immersed in a mixture (RI = 1.525) of silicon oil HIVAC-F4 (RI = 1.555, Shin-Etsu Chemical Co., Tokyo, Japan) and mineral oil (RI = 1.467, M8410, Sigma Aldrich, St. Louis, MO, USA). Images were collected by scanning the samples in the z-direction at a step size of 10 μm. Fluorescent signals were visualized using a laser of an appropriate wavelength with a sequential shift in the light sheet focal positions. The thinnest focal point of the LSF microscope was horizontally scanned six times per plane to reduce defocusing from the beam’s Gaussian shape. Scanning was performed on either side of the illuminated arm. Images of identical horizontal positions (dVenus, bandpass emission filter: 495–540 nm; autofluorescence at 488 nm, bandpass emission filter: 600–690 nm; and RedDotTM2 at 637 nm, bandpass emission filter: 660–750 nm) were merged using customized image software (MVX10-MG-SW version 1.1.3, Olympus). To remove autofluorescence signals in the finalized dVenus images, autofluorescence images were subtracted from the dVenus images using Fiji software. Furthermore, considering that the brains swell in response to the clearing process and that the size of the swollen brain differed across the experimental groups, the brain size was calculated and normalized using the ratio of the size. Acquired brain images were overlapped using an automatic transformation algorithm56 for quantitative comparison. Reconstitution and analysis of volume-rendered images were conducted using the 3D visualization and image processing software Imaris (version 8.1.2, Bitplane, Zürich, Switzerland). The dVenus signal was overlaid with the RedDot™2 signal, and the cortical layers were identified by the arrangement of neuronal nuclei. A whole-brain image for each experimental group was thus created, as shown in Fig. 2.
3D ROIs of targeted cortical areas and cell count
ROIs were set at the bilateral visual cortex (V1) and somatosensory cortex (S1), which are known to substitute for vestibular loss in humans7–9. The S1 area of the mice is mainly located in the barrel cortex57. The PIVC has been proposed as a possible “vestibular cortex” in humans58–62 and monkeys63–67. The anterior suprasylvian sulcus (ASSS) is analogous to the PIVC in cats68–70. Since both cortices are located in the posterior part of the insular cortex and are surrounded by the somatosensory and auditory cortices, the pGIC was set as a possible cortical area corresponding to the human/primate PIVC and cat ASSS. Since UL could influence not only the vestibular organ but also the cochlea, the 3D ROIs were set on the pGIC as a possible “vestibular cortex” in mice, and the primary auditory cortex (A1) to investigate the effects of loss of sensory inputs from the inner ear. In addition, potentially significant areas where the number of activated neurons could change following UL were surveyed by arbitrary cross sections in Imaris. Box-shaped 3D ROIs were set up on these target areas for sensory reweighting (V1 and S1) (Fig. 3), sensory deprivation (pGIC and A1) (Fig. 6), and newly identified cortical areas (mPFC and AIC) (Fig. 7) according to the mouse brain atlas57. The number of activated neurons in the 3D ROIs was automatically counted after the signal threshold was set, and compared between the UL and sham-treated groups. The mPFC and the agranular insular cortex (AIC) were divided into subregions. Because the mPFC in rodents is anatomically and functionally divided into four subregions, namely, the medial precentral cortex (mPC), dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dAC), prelimbic cortex (PL), and infralimbic cortex (IL), from dorsal to ventral23–25, the cell count in the mPFC was performed separately for these four subregions. Since the AIC is divided into three subregions from rostral to caudal (anterior [aAIC], middle [mAIC], and posterior [pAIC]) based on their connectivity with other cortical and subcortical regions48, the cell count in the AIC was performed separately for these three subregions.
Statistical analysis
The results for the head roll-tilt and rotarod tests were analyzed with repeated measures two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) followed by post-hoc Tukey`s tests to identify the postoperative days at which there were significant difference between the UL and sham-treated subjects. Differences in the total cell count were analyzed by the Mann–Whitney test using SPSS Statistics software (version 26.0 , IBM, Armonk, NY, USA). Statistical significance was set at P < 0.05.
Ethics approval
All experiments were approved by the ethics committee of animal experiments from Niigata University and were carried out in accordance with the approved guidelines.
Supplementary Information
Author contributions
R.K.: study design, acquisition, interpretation of data, and drafting of the manuscript. Ku.T.: study design, drafting of the manuscript, and supervision. Ka.T., His.N., N.S., T.S., and S.Y.: acquisition, analysis, and interpretation of data. Hir.N. and A.H.: supervision, interpretation, and final approval. All authors reviewed the final version of the manuscript.
Funding
This work was supported by JSPS KAKENHI (Grant Number 20K18245).
Data availability
The datasets generated during and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary Information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41598-022-19678-4.
References
- 1.Dutia MB. Mechanisms of vestibular compensation: recent advances. Curr. Opin. Otolaryngol. Head Neck Surg. 2010;18(5):420–424. doi: 10.1097/MOO.0b013e32833de71f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Smith PF, Curthoys IS. Mechanisms of recovery following unilateral labyrinthectomy: A review. Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 1989;14(2):155–180. doi: 10.1016/0165-0173(89)90013-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Vidal PP, de Waele C, Vibert N, Mühlethaler M. Vestibular compensation revisited. Otolaryngol.. Head Neck Surg. 1998;119(1):34–42. doi: 10.1016/s0194-5998(98)70171-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Darlington CL, Dutia MB, Smith PF. The contribution of the intrinsic excitability of vestibular nucleus neurons to recovery from vestibular damage. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2002;15(11):1719–1727. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2002.02024.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Kitahara T, Takeda N, Saika T, Kubo T, Kiyama H. Effects of MK801 on Fos expression in the rat brainstem after unilateral labyrinthectomy. Brain Res. 1995;700(1–2):182–190. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(95)00950-u. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Peterka RJ. Sensorimotor integration in human postural control. J. Neurophysiol. 2002;88(3):1097–1118. doi: 10.1152/jn.2002.88.3.1097. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Cousins S, Cutfield NJ, Kaski D, Palla A, Seemungal BM, Golding JF, et al. Visual dependency and dizziness after vestibular neuritis. PLoS ONE. 2014;9(9):e105426. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0105426. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Okumura T, Horii A, Kitahara T, Imai T, Uno A, Osaki Y, et al. Somatosensory shift of postural control in dizzy patients. Acta Otolaryngol. 2015;135(9):925–930. doi: 10.3109/00016489.2015.1040172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Dieterich M, Brandt T. Imaging cortical activity after vestibular lesions. Restor. Neurol Neurosci. 2010;28(1):47–56. doi: 10.3233/rnn-2010-0505. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Dieterich M, Bauermann T, Best C, Stoeter P, Schlindwein P. Evidence for cortical visual substitution of chronic bilateral vestibular failure (an fMRI study) Brain. 2007;130(Pt 8):2108–2116. doi: 10.1093/brain/awm130. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Hupfeld KE, McGregor HR, Koppelmans V, Beltran NE, Kofman IS, De Dios YE, et al. Brain and behavioral evidence for reweighting of vestibular inputs with long-duration spaceflight. Cereb. Cortex. 2022;32(4):755–769. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhab239. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Yuan P, Koppelmans V, Reuter-Lorenz P, De Dios Y, Gadd N, Wood S, et al. Vestibular brain changes within 70 days of head down bed rest. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2018;39(7):2753–2763. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Nakamura T, Sato A, Kitsukawa T, Sasaoka T, Yamamori T. Expression pattern of immediate early genes in the cerebellum of D1R KO, D2R KO, and wild type mice under vestibular-controlled activity. Front. Cell Dev. Biol. 2015;3:38. doi: 10.3389/fcell.2015.00038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Saito N, Tainaka K, Macpherson T, Hikida T, Yamaguchi S, Sasaoka T. Neurotransmission through dopamine D1 receptors is required for aversive memory formation and Arc activation in the cerebral cortex. Neurosci. Res. 2020;156:58–65. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2020.04.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Susaki EA, Tainaka K, Perrin D, Yukinaga H, Kuno A, Ueda HR. Advanced CUBIC protocols for whole-brain and whole-body clearing and imaging. Nat. Protoc. 2015;10(11):1709–1727. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2015.085. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Ito T, Tatsumi K, Takimoto Y, Nishimura T, Imai T, Yamanaka T, et al. Vestibular compensation after vestibular dysfunction induced by arsanilic acid in mice. Brain Sci. 2019;9(11):329. doi: 10.3390/brainsci9110329. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Kim G, Nguyen N, Kim KS. Postural control in paw distance after labyrinthectomy-induced vestibular imbalance. Med. Biol. Eng. Comput. 2020;58(12):3039–3047. doi: 10.1007/s11517-020-02276-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Hamm RJ, Pike BR, O'Dell DM, Lyeth BG, Jenkins LW. The rotarod test: An evaluation of its effectiveness in assessing motor deficits following traumatic brain injury. J. Neurotrauma. 1994;11(2):187–196. doi: 10.1089/neu.1994.11.187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Rogers DC, Campbell CA, Stretton JL, Mackay KB. Correlation between motor impairment and infarct volume after permanent and transient middle cerebral artery occlusion in the rat. Stroke. 1997;28(10):2060–2065. doi: 10.1161/01.str.28.10.2060. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Voller J, Potužáková B, Šimeček V, Vožeh F. The role of whiskers in compensation of visual deficit in a mouse model of retinal degeneration. Neurosci. Lett. 2014;558:149–153. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2013.11.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Dieterich M, Brandt T. The parietal lobe and the vestibular system. Handb. Clin. Neurol. 2018;151:119–140. doi: 10.1016/b978-0-444-63622-5.00006-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Nishiike S, Guldin WO, Bäurle J. Corticofugal connections between the cerebral cortex and the vestibular nuclei in the rat. J. Comp. Neurol. 2000;420(3):363–372. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(20000508)420:3<363::AID-CNE7>3.0.CO;2-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Heidbreder CA, Groenewegen HJ. The medial prefrontal cortex in the rat: Evidence for a dorso-ventral distinction based upon functional and anatomical characteristics. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2003;27(6):555–579. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2003.09.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Ongür D, Price JL. The organization of networks within the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex of rats, monkeys and humans. Cereb. Cortex. 2000;10(3):206–219. doi: 10.1093/cercor/10.3.206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Sesack SR, Deutch AY, Roth RH, Bunney BS. Topographical organization of the efferent projections of the medial prefrontal cortex in the rat: An anterograde tract-tracing study with Phaseolus vulgaris leucoagglutinin. J. Comp. Neurol. 1989;290(2):213–242. doi: 10.1002/cne.902900205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Vertes RP. Interactions among the medial prefrontal cortex, hippocampus and midline thalamus in emotional and cognitive processing in the rat. Neuroscience. 2006;142(1):1–20. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.06.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Donoghue JP, Wise SP. The motor cortex of the rat: Cytoarchitecture and microstimulation mapping. J. Comp. Neurol. 1982;212(1):76–88. doi: 10.1002/cne.902120106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Hall RD, Lindholm EP. Organization of motor and somatosensory neocortex in the albino rat. Brain Res. 1974;66(1):23–38. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(74)90076-6. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Neafsey E, Sievert C. A second forelimb motor area exists in rat frontal cortex. Brain Res. 1982;232(1):151–156. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(82)90617-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Sinnamon HM, Galer BS. Head movements elicited by electrical stimulation of the anteromedial cortex of the rat. Physiol. Behav. 1984;33(2):185–190. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(84)90098-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Condé F, Maire-Lepoivre E, Audinat E, Crépel F. Afferent connections of the medial frontal cortex of the rat. II. Cortical and subcortical afferents. J. Comp. Neurol. 1995;352(4):567–593. doi: 10.1002/cne.903520407. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Passingham RE, Myers C, Rawlins N, Lightfoot V, Fearn S. Premotor cortex in the rat. Behav. Neurosci. 1988;102(1):101–109. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.102.1.101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Reep RL, Corwin JV, Hashimoto A, Watson RT. Efferent connections of the rostral portion of medial agranular cortex in rats. Brain Res. Bull. 1987;19(2):203–221. doi: 10.1016/0361-9230(87)90086-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Hoover WB, Vertes RP. Anatomical analysis of afferent projections to the medial prefrontal cortex in the rat. Brain Struct. Funct. 2007;212(2):149–179. doi: 10.1007/s00429-007-0150-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Reep R, Goodwin G, Corwin J. Topographic organization in the corticocortical connections of medial agranular cortex in rats. J. Comp. Neurol. 1990;294(2):262–280. doi: 10.1002/cne.902940210. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Yamawaki N, Radulovic J, Shepherd GM. A corticocortical circuit directly links retrosplenial cortex to M2 in the mouse. J. Neurosci. 2016;36(36):9365–9374. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.1099-16.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Zingg B, Hintiryan H, Gou L, Song MY, Bay M, Bienkowski MS, et al. Neural networks of the mouse neocortex. Cell. 2014;156(5):1096–1111. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2014.02.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Leonard CM. The prefrontal cortex of the rat. I. Cortical projection of the mediodorsal nucleus. II. Efferent connections. Brain Res. 1969;12(2):321–343. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(69)90003-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Leichnetz GR, Hardy SG, Carruth MK. Frontal projections to the region of the oculomotor complex in the rat: A retrograde and anterograde HRP study. J. Comp. Neurol. 1987;263(3):387–399. doi: 10.1002/cne.902630306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Stuesse SL, Newman DB. Projections from the medial agranular cortex to brain stem visuomotor centers in rats. Exp. Brain Res. 1990;80(3):532–544. doi: 10.1007/bf00227994. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Barthas F, Kwan AC. Secondary motor cortex: where 'sensory' meets 'motor' in the rodent frontal cortex. Trends Neurosci. 2017;40(3):181–193. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2016.11.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Gabbott PL, Warner TA, Jays PR, Salway P, Busby SJ. Prefrontal cortex in the rat: Projections to subcortical autonomic, motor, and limbic centers. J. Comp. Neurol. 2005;492(2):145–177. doi: 10.1002/cne.20738. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Hardy SG, Holmes DE. Prefrontal stimulus-produced hypotension in rat. Exp. Brain Res. 1988;73(2):249–255. doi: 10.1007/bf00248217. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Hurley-Gius KM, Neafsey EJ. The medial frontal cortex and gastric motility: Microstimulation results and their possible significance for the overall pattern of organization of rat frontal and parietal cortex. Brain Res. 1986;365(2):241–248. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(86)91635-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Middleton FA, Strick PL. Basal ganglia and cerebellar loops: motor and cognitive circuits. Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 2000;31(2–3):236–250. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0173(99)00040-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Takakusaki K, Habaguchi T, Ohtinata-Sugimoto J, Saitoh K, Sakamoto T. Basal ganglia efferents to the brainstem centers controlling postural muscle tone and locomotion: A new concept for understanding motor disorders in basal ganglia dysfunction. Neuroscience. 2003;119(1):293–308. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(03)00095-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Gogolla N. The insular cortex. Curr. Biol. 2017;27(12):R580–r586. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.05.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Gehrlach DA, Weiand C, Gaitanos TN, Cho E, Klein AS, Hennrich AA, et al. A whole-brain connectivity map of mouse insular cortex. Elife. 2020 doi: 10.7554/eLife.55585. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Azzena GB, Mameli O, Tolu E. Distribution of visual input to the vestibular nuclei. Arch. Ital. Biol. 1980;118(2):196–204. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Beraneck M, Cullen KE. Activity of vestibular nuclei neurons during vestibular and optokinetic stimulation in the alert mouse. J. Neurophysiol. 2007;98(3):1549–1565. doi: 10.1152/jn.00590.2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.McCall AA, Miller DM, Balaban CD. Integration of vestibular and hindlimb inputs by vestibular nucleus neurons: Multisensory influences on postural control. J. Neurophysiol. 2021;125(4):1095–1110. doi: 10.1152/jn.00350.2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Rubin AM, Liedgren SR, Miline AC, Young JA, Fredrickson JM. Vestibular and somatosensory interaction in the cat vestibular nuclei. Pflugers Arch. 1977;371(1–2):155–160. doi: 10.1007/bf00580784. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Eguchi M, Yamaguchi S. In vivo and in vitro visualization of gene expression dynamics over extensive areas of the brain. Neuroimage. 2009;44(4):1274–1283. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.10.046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Deacon RM. Measuring motor coordination in mice. J. Vis. Exp. 2013;75:e2609. doi: 10.3791/2609. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Jones BJ, Roberts DJ. The quantiative measurement of motor inco-ordination in naive mice using an acelerating rotarod. J. Pharm. Pharmacol. 1968;20(4):302–304. doi: 10.1111/j.2042-7158.1968.tb09743.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Tainaka K, Murakami TC, Susaki EA, Shimizu C, Saito R, Takahashi K, et al. Chemical landscape for tissue clearing based on hydrophilic reagents. Cell Rep. 2018;24(8):2196–2210.e2199. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2018.07.056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Paxinos G, Franklin KBJ. The Mouse Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates. 5. Academic Press; 1997. [Google Scholar]
- 58.Bottini G, Sterzi R, Paulesu E, Vallar G, Cappa SF, Erminio F, et al. Identification of the central vestibular projections in man: A positron emission tomography activation study. Exp. Brain Res. 1994;99(1):164–169. doi: 10.1007/bf00241421. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Bucher SF, Dieterich M, Wiesmann M, Weiss A, Zink R, Yousry TA, et al. Cerebral functional magnetic resonance imaging of vestibular, auditory, and nociceptive areas during galvanic stimulation. Ann. Neurol. 1998;44(1):120–125. doi: 10.1002/ana.410440118. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Dieterich M, Brandt T. Functional brain imaging of peripheral and central vestibular disorders. Brain. 2008;131(Pt 10):2538–2552. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Frank SM, Wirth AM, Greenlee MW. Visual-vestibular processing in the human Sylvian fissure. J. Neurophysiol. 2016;116(2):263–271. doi: 10.1152/jn.00009.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Lobel E, Kleine JF, Leroy-Willig A, Van de Moortele PF, Le Bihan D, Grüsser OJ, et al. Cortical areas activated by bilateral galvanic vestibular stimulation. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1999;871:313–323. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1999.tb09194.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Akbarian S, Grüsser OJ, Guldin WO. Corticofugal projections to the vestibular nuclei in squirrel monkeys: further evidence of multiple cortical vestibular fields. J. Comp. Neurol. 1993;332(1):89–104. doi: 10.1002/cne.903320107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Akbarian S, Grüsser OJ, Guldin WO. Corticofugal connections between the cerebral cortex and brainstem vestibular nuclei in the macaque monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 1994;339(3):421–437. doi: 10.1002/cne.903390309. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Grüsser OJ, Pause M, Schreiter U. Localization and responses of neurones in the parieto-insular vestibular cortex of awake monkeys (Macaca fascicularis) J. Physiol. 1990;430:537–557. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1990.sp018306. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Guldin WO, Grüsser OJ. Is there a vestibular cortex? Trends Neurosci. 1998;21(6):254–259. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(97)01211-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Guldin WO, Mirring S, Grüsser OJ. Connections from the neocortex to the vestibular brain stem nuclei in the common marmoset. NeuroReport. 1993;5(2):113–116. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199311180-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Kempinsky WH. Cortical projection of vestibular and facial nerves in cat. J. Neurophysiol. 1951;14(2):203–210. doi: 10.1152/jn.1951.14.3.203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Landgren S, Silfvenius H, Wolsk D. Vestibular, cochlear and trigeminal projections to the cortex in the anterior suprasylvian sulcus of the cat. J. Physiol. 1967;191(3):561–573. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1967.sp008268. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Mickle WA, Ades HW. A composite sensory projection area in the cerebral cortex of the cat. Am. J. Physiol. 1952;170(3):682–689. doi: 10.1152/ajplegacy.1952.170.3.682. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The datasets generated during and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.