Abstract
Objective
COVID‐19 has dramatically affected Western Society's relationship with work and contributed to increased worker burnout. Existing studies on burnout have mostly emphasized workplace culture, leadership, and employee engagement as key contributors to burnout. In this cross‐sectional study, we examine the associations between Malach‐Pines Short Burnout Measure (MPSBM) scores and participant's self reported personal characteristics, financial strain, workplace conditions, work‐life balance, and social inclusion among Canadians living during the third wave of the COVID‐19 pandemic.
Methods
To identify the most salient correlates of burnout, Canadian residents, aged 16+, were recruited using paid social media advertisements in French and English to complete a cross‐sectional study. Multivariable linear regression and dominance analysis identified the most salient correlates of MPSBM scores. Exposure variables included demographic factors, financial strain, workplace conditions, work‐life balance, social support, and loneliness.
Results
Among 486 participants, family social support (adjusted β = −0.14, 95%CI = −0.23, −0.05), emotional loneliness (adjusted β = 0.26, 95% CI = 0.18, 0.35), insufficient sleep (adjusted β = 0.38, 95% CI = 0.16, 0.60) and “me time” (adjusted β = 0.22, 95% CI = 0.03, 0.42), and indicators of financial security (e.g., owning vs renting; adjusted β = −0.36, 95% CI = −0.54, −0.17; insufficient pay: adjusted β = −0.36, 95% CI = −0.54, −0.17) were key burnout indicators. People with a bachelor's degree (vs ≤high school diploma; adjusted β = 0.29, 95% CI = 0.01, 0.58) also had higher burnout scores.
Conclusion
Interventions addressing workplace culture, leadership, and other proximal workplace stressors, while important, are likely insufficient to meet the needs of workers. Our findings suggest that broader, holistic multicomponent approaches that address multiple upstream dimensions of health—including mental health—are likely necessary to prevent and reduce burnout.
Keywords: burnout, financial security, loneliness, sleep, social support
1. INTRODUCTION
Among any number of workplace maladies, “burnout” has emerged as an unfortunately common ailment facing workers today. 1 The concept of burnout was first introduced in the 1970s and, since then, a growing number of people, from minimum wage employees to high‐paid professionals, report experiencing burnout symptoms. 1 In this study, we use the definition of burnout proposed by Malach‐Pines, which describes it as a combination of (1) physical, mental, and emotional exhaustion, (2) feelings of dissatisfaction, being trapped, stressed, and hopeless, and (3) and the absence of drive, purpose, and enthusiasm for work. 1
In understanding and addressing the phenomenon of burnout, it is particularly important to understand its drivers among people living in the wake of the COVID‐19 pandemic. 1 Indeed, COVID‐19 has impacted the level of control workers have over their work, forced dramatic alterations to workers' routines and working environments, and put the health of many frontline workers at risk. 2 In the midst of this, people are leaving their jobs at alarming rates, particularly within service occupations—creating economic and social disruptions that have cascading effects on society more broadly. 3 Even prior to the COVID‐19 pandemic, burnout was heralded as an “emblematic illness” of the epoch. 4 Although it has been difficult for researchers to adequately differentiate burnout from other mental health challenges, 5 previous studies have centered discussions of burnout on management, working conditions, and culture. 6 , 7 These studies have provided valuable insight into the role that burnout has in contributing to physical disease, mental disorders, or workplace inefficiencies. 8 However, these studies highlight that a more holistic evaluation—accounting for individual and situational factors outside of the workplace—is needed if we are to truly extinguish burnout in contemporary work environments. 9
Several published literature reviews have previously shown that a diverse set of factors underlie burnout. 10 , 11 Many of these studies have centered discussions of burnout on management, working conditions, and culture 6 , 7 , 12 , 13 —factors which have been repeatedly identified as contributors to burnout. Other lifestyle factors have reviewed lesser attention, but also indicate that the factors contributing to burnout are wide ranging. For example, one review showed that physical activity has a moderately strong effect in preventing or reducing burnout 14 ; another linked burnout to eating behaviors and dietary patterns 15 ; a third to workplace violence. 16 Likewise, a growing body of literature has emphasized the importance of social determinants of health, such as gender and socioeconomic status, in understanding burnout. 17 The emergence of COVID‐19 has also highlighted social isolation as another key determinant of burnout 18 and it is increasingly evident that social networks and supports can provide some degree of protection against burnout. 19 , 20
Based on Hobfoll et al. 21 Social Support Resource Theory, it is hypothesized that social relationships provides a critical reserve of emotional, psychological, and existential, and physical sources that can fortify individuals against acute and chronic stressors—providing them with the energy and enthusiasm needed to thrive in the workplace. Unfortunately, most studies on burnout investigate a limited number of domains and, as such, it is difficult to identify what broad factors may be most important to address in seeking to mitigate the negative effects of burnout. 22 , 23 Social connection is increasingly researched in relation to burnout 24 , 25 , 26 —likely due to established benefits for other mental health conditions. 27 In the context of COVID‐19, it seems urgently necessary to understand what forms of social connection and support might be influencing people's work lives—especially considering that some may be isolated from not only their workplace social connections, but also those established outside of work. 18
This study aims to identify the most salient factors underlying burnout among Canadians during the third wave of the COVID‐19 pandemic using baseline data from a longitudinal cohort study that was established to monitor trends in social and mental health recovery throughout the late‐pandemic period.
2. METHODS
2.1. Data Collection
Between April 21 and June 1, 2021—during Canada's third wave of COVID‐19—we recruited participants using paid advertisements promoted on social media sites, including Facebook, Instagram, Twitter, and Google Ads. Geographic targeting was used to reach people living in Canada or individuals with Canadian accounts. Advertisements featured pictures of individuals represented across ages, genders, ethnicities, sexual orientations, family types, and body types. Advertising text, in English and French, provided information about the study's purpose, survey length, and honoraria amount. After clicking on advertisements, participants were directed to an informed consent page and were screened for eligibility. Eligibility criteria restricted participation to those who were 16 years of age or older (i.e., the age of consent for research participation without parental consent), lived in Canada, and provided informed consent. Additional analytic inclusion criteria restricted participation to individuals who were employed at time of survey completion. The survey, which was completed using the Qualtrics survey platform, took approximately 21 min (Q1–Q3: 10–35 min) to complete and could be completed in either English or French. The survey was structured around six modules. As a means of reducing participant burden, modules 4, 5, and 6 were randomized such that only one‐third of respondents completed each of the modules. As this is a secondary data analysis of existing data, these modules were selected as they explored issues not directly relevant to the core‐funded research questions. Upon conclusion of the study, participants were compensated for their time by entering their email in an optional lottery to win one of 25 $100 gift card prizes.
2.2. Ethics
Ethics review for the Canadian Social Connections Survey (CSCS) was conducted by the Research Ethics Board at the University of Victoria (Ethics protocol number 21‐0115). All participants provided informed consent prior to participation.
2.3. Variables
2.3.1. Burnout
Burnout was measured in module 1 using the 10‐item Malach‐Pines Short Burnout Measure (BM‐10), a scientifically validated and reliable scale used to assess the level of an individual's physical, emotional, and mental exhaustion in the workplace. 1 This measure was selected because it is a short form instrument for burnout suitable for large scale surveys. For each of the 10‐items (i.e., “disappointed with people,” “tired,” “difficulties sleeping,” “depressed,” “hopeless,” “physically weak or sickly,” “trapped,” “I've had it, helpless,” “worthless or like a failure.”) participants were asked to rate how frequently they felt a given burnout symptom on a 7‐point Likert scale from “Never” (1) to “Very Often” (7). Total burnout scores were calculated by taking the average of item scores and ranged from 1 to 7. Scores of 4 or more are traditionally treated as indicating participants are “Burnt out” while scores of less than 4 indicate lesser burnout.
2.3.2. Personal characteristics
Participant characteristics were assessed in module 3 using novel measures of age, education level, occupation, ethnicity, gender, sexual orientation, and disability identity. We also assessed self‐rated physical and mental health using a 5‐point Likert Scales ranging from Poor (1) to Excellent (5). Occupation was determined using the high‐level categories from the National Occupation Classifiers.
2.3.3. Financial strain
Several questions from the CFPB Financial Well‐Being Self‐assessment tool 28 were used to assess financial strain, including items regarding participant income and home ownership status (own, mortgaged, or rent) were included in module 6. Subjective financial well‐being was assessed across several questions measured on a 5‐point Likert scale (i.e., “I am just getting by financially,” “My finances control my life,” “I have money left over at the end of the month,” “Because of my money situation, I feel like I will never have the things I want in life”).
2.3.4. Working conditions
Novel questions related to workplace conditions were measured in module 6 and assessed how participants felt about work on a 5‐point Likert scale (i.e., “I feel that I am treated with dignity and respect in my workplace,” “I have a lot of control over how I do my work,” “I am getting paid enough for the work I do,” “I am appreciated for the work I do,” “I feel supported by my co‐workers,” “I feel my workplace is fair”, “I feel that my work‐load is unsustainable,” “I think about quitting my job,” “I feel stress about my job even when I am not at work.”). Likert scales were treated as continuous measures for ease of interpretation. Employment impacts related to COVID‐19 assessed whether participants were laid off, got a new job, or had their hours increased or decreased.
2.3.5. Work‐life balance
Questions about participant's perception of how they spend their time were assessed in module 1 by asking whether they spent too much, too little, or just the right amount of time sleeping, exercising, helping others and volunteering, practicing hobbies and skills, getting “me” time, winding down, and working.
2.3.6. Social Inclusion
We included multiple validated measures of social inclusion and support in modules 1 and 2 in order to identify specific potential pathways by which social conditions may impact burnout. The 6‐item DeJong Emotional and Social Loneliness Scale is a validated scale that consists of 6‐items, rated on a 3‐point Likert scale. The scale consists of two dimensions measuring emotional loneliness (e.g., “I miss having people around”, “I often feel rejected”) and social loneliness (i.e., “There are plenty of people I can rely on when I have problems”). Each subscale is scored from 0 to 3, with higher values indicating greater emotional and social loneliness. 29 Final scores on this measure are calculated as a sum of scale items. As is done with the originally designed scale, each item is scored dichotomously with answers indicating neutral (i.e., “more or less”) or loneliness receiving 1 point and those indicating no loneliness receiving 0 points.. The Zimet Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support is a validated scale that consists of 12 items rated on a 7‐point Likert scale from “Very Strongly Disagree” to “Very Strongly Agree.” The scale consists of three dimensions measuring support from friends (e.g., “I can count on my friends when things go wrong”), families (e.g., “I get the emotional help and support I need from my family”), and significant others (e.g., “There is a special person in my life who cares about my feelings”). Each scale dimension is scored on a scale from 1 to 7, representing the average response value across relevant items. 30 The Everyday Discrimination Scale consists of 9‐items scored on a 6‐point Likert scale measuring the frequency individual experience day‐to‐day discrimination (e.g., “You are treated with less respect that other people are,” “People act as if they are afraid of you.”). The final score consists of one dimension and ranges from 9 to 54, with higher values indicating greater discrimination.
2.4. Data analysis
All statistical analyses were conducted in R 4.1.3. 31 As an initial step, dishonest/ false participants/ poor quality data were screened for by examining IP addresses, completion time, and response patterns (e.g., unlikely response combinations, and straight lining)—erring on the side of inclusion for individual responses. For this analysis, additional analytic criteria restricted inclusion to individuals who were randomly assigned to complete module six, reported being currently employed, and provided responses to all variables used for the planned regression analyses. A complete case analysis was conducted due to our use of regression analyses, which do not tolerate missing values. We did not conduct imputation analyses, as we could not assume that data were missing at random, due to the potential that burnout could correlate with survey fatigue, and thus higher rates of nonresponse.
Descriptive statistics were calculated for the analytic sample, stratified by whether or not participants were classified as “Burnt out” (Score ≥4) or “Not Burnt Out” (Score <4; Malach‐Pines 1 ). In these calculations, frequencies (n) and proportions (%) are reported for categorical variables; means and standard deviations are reported for continuous variables. Differences between included and excluded participants were assessed using bivariable statistics. Chi‐square (χ2) test were used to test differences on categorical variables, one‐way ANOVA tests were used for continuous normal variables, and Kruskal–Wallis nonparametric tests were used for continuous non‐normal variables. The primary outcome for this analysis was continuous scores on the 10‐item Malach‐Pines Short Burnout Measure. Bivariable linear regression models were constructed to identify unadjusted effects of explanatory variables on burnout scores. Two multivariable models were constructed to assess the independent and adjusted effects of explanatory variables on burnout scores. The first of these two models included all factors of theoretical relevance. The second included variables selected using a standard backwards‐elimination, AIC‐optimization procedure. Given some controversy regarding the benefits of backwards‐elimination, we provide results of both models given to highlight differences in findings. However, we discuss and focus primarily on those of the backwards selected model as we feel the benefits of AIC in balancing model complexity and explanatory power is appropriate. As an additional model checking procedure, the VIF for the full and final models was calculated using the car package to check for multi‐collinearity. 32 A cutoff value of 5 was included to assess multicollinearity. No multicollinearity was identified in the final model at this threshold. After we arrived at our final model, we conducted dominance analysis to assess relative variable importance and to focus subsequent analyses on key mechanisms that might lead to burnout. Dominance analysis identifies the independent contribution of each factor to model by examining its contribution to model R‐squared. 33 For these analyses, a conditional dominance plot was constructed showing the average contribution to R 2 of each variable, across all possible models.
3. RESULTS
A total of 2286 participants initiated the survey, of these, 1218 completed module 6. However, 369 of these individuals were not currently employed and were thus excluded from the current analyses. After removing participants with missing data on variables of interest (n = 363), the final analytic sample size consisted of 486 participants. The average age of the analytic sample was 34.7 years (SD = 11.3). Nearly two‐thirds of participants (60.7%) reported being white; approximately half were identified as men (48.4%); 38.1% identified as Two‐Spirit, Lesbian, Gay, Bisexual, Transgender, Queer, or another sexual minority (2SLGBTQ+); and more than one‐in‐10 (11.1%) reported having a disability.
Table 1 shows bivariable associations with burnout scores for each explanatory factor. People who were burnt out had higher emotional (Mean = 2.55 vs 2.00) and social (2.11 vs 1.48) loneliness, reported more experiences of discrimination (28.17 vs 24.00), and reported lower support from family (4.37 vs 5.22), friends (4.61 vs 5.21), and significant others (4.68 vs 5.43) compared to those who were not burnt out. Burnout was also higher among individuals who were living with a disability (50.0% vs 28.9%). As indicated by bivariable associations in Table 1, general associations indicated that greater financial strain (e.g., Higher scores among burnt‐out individuals on items such as “Because of my money situation, I feel like I will never have the things I want in life.”), time imbalances (e.g., burnt‐out people being more likely to report too little or too much “me time”’), and workplace dissatisfaction (e.g., burnt‐out individuals that were less likely to feel that their workplace is “fair”) were also associated with greater burnout across multiple variables.
TABLE 1.
Descriptive and bivariable statistics showing associations with being burnt out (N = 486) “Burnt out” (Score ≥4) or “Not Burnt Out” (Score <4; Malach‐Pines, 2005)
Response | Not burnt out score <4 (n = 334) | Burnt out score ≥4 (n = 152) | P‐value b | Test c |
---|---|---|---|---|
Age a (M, SD) | 34.70 (11.28) | 34.58 (11.40) | .913 | ANOVA |
Gender (N, %) | ||||
Man | 168 (50.3) | 67 (44.1) | Reference | χ 2 |
Nonbinary | 5 (1.5) | 8 (5.3) | .056 | |
Woman | 161 (48.2) | 77 (50.7) | .044 | |
Sexual orientation (N, %) | ||||
Straight | 200 (59.9) | 101 (66.4) | Reference | χ2 |
2SLGBTQ+ | 134 (40.1) | 51 (33.6) | .879 | |
Ethnicity (N, %) | ||||
White | 203 (60.8) | 92 (60.5) | Reference | χ 2 |
African, Caribbean, or Black | 43 (12.9) | 14 (9.2) | .357 | |
Arab/West Asian | 9 (2.7) | 8 (5.3) | .061 | |
East Asian | 22 (6.6) | 14 (9.2) | .289 | |
Indigenous | 31 (9.3) | 7 (4.6) | .002 | |
Latin | 16 (4.8) | 6 (3.9) | .964 | |
South Asian | 3 (0.9) | 6 (3.9) | .176 | |
Other | 7 (2.1) | 5 (3.3) | .591 | |
Self‐rated physical health a (M, SD) | 3.70 (0.81) | 3.00 (1.05) | <.001 | |
Living with disability (N, %) | ||||
No | 307 (91.9) | 125 (82.2) | Reference | χ 2 |
Yes | 27 (8.1) | 27 (17.8) | <.001 | |
Education level (N, %) | ||||
High school diploma or lower | 34 (10.2) | 17 (11.2) | Reference | χ 2 |
College or advanced skills training | 130 (38.9) | 47 (30.9) | .634 | |
Bachelor's degree | 65 (19.5) | 44 (28.9) | .227 | |
Post‐graduate/professional degree | 105 (31.4) | 44 (28.9) | .547 | |
Employment status (N, %) | ||||
Part time | 218 (65.3) | 94 (61.8) | Reference | χ 2 |
Full time | 97 (29.0) | 39 (25.7) | .649 | |
Overtime | 19 (5.7) | 19 (12.5) | .336 | |
Occupation (N, %) | ||||
Sales and service | 66 (19.8) | 23 (15.1) | Reference | χ 2 |
Community and civil services | 90 (26.9) | 45 (29.6) | .031 | |
Health and science | 55 (16.5) | 28 (18.4) | .083 | |
Management, business, and finance | 86 (25.7) | 37 (24.3) | .317 | |
Trades | 37 (11.1) | 19 (12.5) | .069 | |
COVID‐19 related employment impacts (N, %) | ||||
I had my hours of work reduced | 174 (52.1) | 68 (44.7) | .598 | |
I had my hours of work increased | 46 (13.8) | 29 (19.1) | .198 | |
I was laid off temporarily or permanently | 29 (8.7) | 28 (18.4) | .056 | |
I got a new job | 38 (11.4) | 31 (20.4) | <.001 | |
Amount of work from home a (M, SD) | 3.38 (1.17) | 3.18 (1.29) | .077 | ANOVA |
Income (N, %) | ||||
Less than $30 000 | 77 (23.1) | 48 (31.6) | Reference | χ 2 |
$30 000–$59 999 | 96 (28.7) | 41 (27.0) | .873 | |
$60 000–$89 999 | 61 (18.3) | 32 (21.1) | .820 | |
$90 000 to $119 999 | 46 (13.8) | 17 (11.2) | .195 | |
$120 000 to $149 999 | 17 (5.1) | 6 (3.9) | .151 | |
$150 000 to $179 999 | 23 (6.9) | 2 (1.3) | .022 | |
$180 000 or more | 14 (4.2) | 6 (3.9) | .670 | |
I am just getting by financially a (M, SD) | 3.16 (1.04) | 3.32 (1.08) | .148 | ANOVA |
My finances control my life a (M, SD) | 3.05 (1.19) | 3.11 (1.12) | .514 | ANOVA |
I have money left over at the end of the month a (M, SD) | 3.36 (1.21) | 2.89 (1.25) | <.001 | KW |
Because of my money situation, I feel like I will never have the things I want in life a (M, SD) | 2.86 (1.14) | 3.33 (0.97) | <.001 | KW |
Home ownership (N, %) | ||||
Rent | 101 (30.2) | 81 (53.3) | Reference | χ2 |
Mortgaged | 81 (24.3) | 40 (26.3) | .033 | |
Own | 152 (45.5) | 31 (20.4) | .000 | |
Time exercising (N, %) | ||||
Just the right amount | 187 (56.0) | 57 (37.5) | Reference | χ 2 |
Too little | 98 (29.3) | 75 (49.3) | <.001 | |
Too much | 49 (14.7) | 20 (13.2) | .499 | |
Time helping or volunteering (N, %) | ||||
Just the right amount | 187 (56.0) | 70 (46.1) | Reference | χ2 |
Too little | 110 (32.9) | 69 (45.4) | .001 | |
Too much | 37 (11.1) | 13 (8.6) | .568 | |
Time practicing hobbies (N, %) | ||||
Just the right amount | 160 (47.9) | 53 (34.9) | Reference | χ2 |
Too little | 113 (33.8) | 75 (49.3) | <.001 | |
Too much | 61 (18.3) | 24 (15.8) | .248 | |
Me Time (N, %) | ||||
Just the right amount | 185 (55.4) | 62 (40.8) | Reference | χ 2 |
Too little | 85 (25.4) | 51 (33.6) | <.001 | |
Too much | 64 (19.2) | 39 (25.7) | .001 | |
Time sleeping (N, %) | ||||
Just the right amount | 242 (72.5) | 69 (45.4) | Reference | χ 2 |
Too little | 44 (13.2) | 51 (33.6) | <.001 | |
Too much | 48 (14.4) | 32 (21.1) | .001 | |
Time winding down (N, %) | ||||
Just the right amount | 229 (68.6) | 85 (55.9) | Reference | χ2 |
Too little | 55 (16.5) | 45 (29.6) | <.001 | |
Too much | 50 (15.0) | 22 (14.5) | .394 | |
Time working (N, %) | ||||
Just the right amount | 214 (64.1) | 68 (44.7) | Reference | χ2 |
Too little | 29 (8.7) | 27 (17.8) | <.001 | |
Too much | 91 (27.2) | 57 (37.5) | <.001 | |
I am appreciated for the work I do a (M, SD) | 3.74 (0.90) | 3.32 (0.97) | <.001 | ANOVA |
I have a lot of control over how I do my work a (M, SD) | 3.62 (0.93) | 3.30 (0.95) | <.001 | ANOVA |
I feel that I am treated with dignity and respect in my workplace a (M, SD) | 3.77 (0.92) | 3.32 (1.01) | <.001 | ANOVA |
I feel my workplace is fair a (M, SD) | 3.73 (0.88) | 3.41 (0.92) | <.001 | ANOVA |
I am getting paid enough for the work I do a (M, SD) | 3.61 (0.97) | 3.05 (1.03) | <.001 | ANOVA |
I think about quitting my job a (M, SD) | 2.54 (1.18) | 2.70 (1.20) | .032 | KW |
I feel stress about my job even when I am not at work a (M, SD) | 2.64 (1.13) | 2.82 (1.23) | .011 | KW |
I feel that my work‐load is unsustainable a (M, SD) | 3.05 (1.07) | 3.16 (1.02) | .661 | ANOVA |
I feel supported by my co‐workers a (M, SD) | 3.75 (0.90) | 3.35 (0.96) | <.001 | ANOVA |
Zimet family social support a (M, SD) | 5.22 (1.07) | 4.37 (1.11) | <.001 | ANOVA |
Zimet friend social support a (M, SD) | 5.21 (1.00) | 4.61 (1.05) | <.001 | ANOVA |
Zimet significant other social support a (M, SD) | 5.43 (1.01) | 4.68 (1.37) | <.001 | ANOVA |
DeJong emotional loneliness a (M, SD) | 2.00 (0.98) | 2.55 (0.66) | <.001 | KW |
DeJong social loneliness a (M, SD) | 1.48 (1.21) | 2.11 (1.06) | <.001 | KW |
Everyday discrimination a (M, SD) | 24.00 (11.93) | 28.17 (7.84) | <.001 | KW |
Numeric variable—Mean (SD) presented.
P‐value for differences between participants who were and were not burnt out (i.e., scores ≥4).
Chi‐square (χ2) test were used to test differences on categorical variables, one‐way ANOVA tests were used for continuous normal variables, and Kruskal–Wallis (KW) nonparametric tests were used for continuous non‐normal variables.
Table 2 shows results from our multivariable linear models. In final multivariable modeling, higher emotional loneliness (β = 0.26, 95% CI = 0.18, 0.35), lower social support from family (β = −0.14, 95% CI = −0.23, −0.05), having a bachelors degree (vs High school diploma or less, β = 0.29, 95%CI = 0.01, 0.58), getting too little sleep (β = 0.38, 95% CI = 0.16, 0.60) and too little “me time” (β = 0.22, 95% CI = 0.03, 0.42), lower self‐rated physical health (β = −0.13, 95% CI = −0.23, −0.04), owning your home (vs renting; β = −0.36, 95% CI = −0.54, −0.17), and not getting paid enough for the work you do (β = −0.36, 95%CI = −0.54, −0.17) were associated with higher burnout scores. We also note that in our full model that African, Caribbean, and Black people (vs white people, β = −0.28, 95% CI = −0.55, −0.01) and Indigenous people (vs white people, β = −0.34, 95% CI = −0.65, −0.02) had lower burnout scores—but this variable was not retained in the final model. Likewise, having an occupation in Health and Sciences (vs. salespeople, β = 0.31, 95% CI = 0.02, 0.60) or Management, Business, or finance (vs sales people, β = 0.30, 95% CI = 0.05, 0.56) was significantly associated with higher burnout in the full model, but this factor was not retained in the final model.
TABLE 2.
Multivariable Linear Regression Models Identifying Factors Associated with Higher Burnout Scores (N = 486)
Predictors | Full model | Final modela |
---|---|---|
β (95% CI) | β (95% CI) | |
Social inclusion factors | ||
Zimet Family Social Support, per 1 point | −0.12 (−0.23, −0.00) | −0.14 (−0.23, −0.05) |
Zimet Friend Social Support, per 1 point | −0.03 (−0.16, 0.11) | |
Zimet Significant Other Social Support, per 1 point | −0.07 (−0.18, 0.03) | −0.09 (−0.17, 0.00) |
DeJong Emotional Loneliness, per 1 point | 0.25 (0.15, 0.35) | 0.26 (0.18, 0.35) |
DeJong Social Loneliness, per 1 point | −0.01 (−0.08, 0.07) | |
Everyday Discrimination Scale, per 1 point | 0.00 (−0.01, 0.01) | |
Demographic factors and personal characteristics | ||
Age, per 1 year | −0.00 (−0.01,0.00) | |
Gender | ||
Man | Reference | |
Non‐binary | 0.02 (−0.54,0.58) | |
Woman | 0.10 (−0.07,0.28) | |
Sexual orientation | ||
Straight | Reference | |
2SLGBTQ+ | −0.01 (−0.19,0.18) | |
Ethnicity | ||
White | Reference | |
African, Caribbean, or Black | −0.28 (−0.55, −0.01) | |
Arab/West Asian | 0.18 (−0.27, 0.63) | |
East Asian | −0.16 (−0.48, 0.16) | |
Indigenous | −0.34 (−0.65, −0.02) | |
Latin | −0.04 (−0.44, 0.36) | |
South Asian | −0.06 (−0.66, 0.54) | |
Other | 0.11 (−0.40, 0.63) | |
Self‐rated physical health | −0.11 (−0.21, 0.00) | −0.13 (−0.23, −0.04) |
Living with disability | ||
No | Reference | |
Yes | 0.14 (−0.13,0.41) | |
Education level | ||
High school diploma or lower | Reference | Reference |
College or advanced skills training | −0.01 (−0.30, 0.28) | 0.02 (−0.24, 0.28) |
Bachelor's degree | 0.30 (−0.02, 0.62) | 0.29 (0.01, 0.58) |
Post‐graduate/professional degree | 0.05 (−0.25, 0.35) | 0.12 (−0.15, 0.39) |
Employment status | ||
Part time | Reference | |
Full time | −0.07 (−0.27,0.13) | |
Overtime | −0.18 (−0.50,0.14) | |
COVID‐19‐related employment impacts | ||
I was laid off temporarily or permanently | 0.03 (−0.23, 0.30) | |
I had my hours of work increased | 0.11 (−0.14, 0.36) | |
I had my hours of work reduced | 0.05 (−0.15, 0.24) | |
I got a new job | 0.15 (−0.10, 0.40) | 0.22 (−0.00, 0.44) |
Occupation | ||
Sales and service | Reference | |
Community and civil services | 0.25 (−0.02, 0.52) | |
Health and science | 0.31 (0.02, 0.60) | |
Management, business, and finance | 0.30 (0.05, 0.56) | |
Trades | 0.20 (−0.11, 0.51) | |
Financial strain | ||
Income | ||
Less than $30 000 | Reference | |
$30 000 to $59 999 | −0.03 (−0.26, 0.20) | |
$60 000 to $89 999 | −0.02 (−0.27, 0.23) | |
$90 000 to $119 999 | 0.01 (−0.28, 0.29) | |
$120 000 to $149 999 | −0.05 (−0.47, 0.37) | |
$150 000 to $179 999 | −0.12 (−0.54, 0.30) | |
$180 000 or more | 0.03 (−0.42, 0.48) | |
I am just getting by financially, per 1 point | 0.02 (−0.07, 0.11) | |
My finances control my life, per 1 point | −0.00 (−0.08, 0.07) | |
I have money left over at the end of the month, per 1 point | −0.04 (−0.11, 0.04) | |
Because of my money situation, I feel like I will never have the things I want in life, per 1 point | 0.02 (−0.08, 0.11) | |
Home ownership | ||
Rent | Reference | Reference |
Mortgaged | −0.12 (−0.34, 0.10) | −0.10 (−0.30, 0.09) |
Own | −0.31 (−0.53, −0.10) | −0.36 (−0.54, −0.17) |
Work‐life balance | ||
Time sleeping | ||
Just the right amount | Reference | Reference |
Too little | 0.41 (0.17, 0.65) | 0.38 (0.16, 0.60) |
Too much | 0.23 (−0.01, 0.48) | 0.19 (−0.03, 0.41) |
Time working | ||
Just the right amount | Reference | Reference |
Too little | 0.14 (−0.14,0.41) | 0.19 (−0.06,0.44) |
Too much | 0.18 (−0.01,0.37) | 0.16 (−0.01,0.33) |
Me time | ||
Just the right amount | Reference | Reference |
Too little | 0.14 (−0.08, 0.35) | 0.22 (0.03, 0.42) |
Too much | 0.14 (−0.08, 0.35) | 0.16 (−0.04, 0.36) |
Time winding down | ||
Just the right amount | Reference | Reference |
Too little | 0.05 (−0.18, 0.28) | |
Too much | −0.14 (−0.38, 0.11) | |
Time exercising | ||
Just the right amount | Reference | |
Too little | −0.02 (−0.25, 0.22) | |
Too much | −0.07 (−0.33, 0.19) | |
Time practicing hobbies | ||
Just the right amount | Reference | Reference |
Too little | 0.13 (−0.08, 0.35) | |
Too much | 0.07 (−0.18, 0.31) | |
Time helping or volunteering | ||
Just the right amount | Reference | Reference |
Too little | 0.09 (−0.11, 0.29) | 0.14 (−0.03, 0.31) |
Too much | −0.11 (−0.40, 0.18) | −0.15 (−0.41, 0.11) |
Workplace conditions | ||
I am appreciated for the work I do, per 1 point | −0.00 (−0.14, 0.13) | |
I have a lot of control over how I do my work, per 1 point | −0.02 (−0.13, 0.09) | |
I feel that I am treated with dignity and respect in my workplace, per 1 point | −0.08 (−0.19, 0.04) | −0.07 (−0.16, 0.02) |
I feel my workplace is fair, per 1 point | 0.01 (−0.13, 0.14) | |
Frequency of Working From Home, per 1 point | −0.02 (−0.09, 0.05) | |
I am getting paid enough for the work I do, per 1 point | −0.08 (−0.18, 0.03) | −0.09 (−0.18, ‐0.01) |
I think about quitting my job, per 1 point | −0.04 (−0.12, 0.04) | |
I feel stress about my job even when I am not at work, per 1 point | 0.05 (−0.03, 0.13) | |
I feel that my work‐load is unsustainable, per 1 point | 0.01 (−0.08, 0.10) | |
I feel supported by my co‐workers, per 1 point | 0.03 (−0.09, 0.15) | |
Observations | 486 | 486 |
R 2 | 0.476 | 0.431 |
AIC | 1279.287 | 1220.914 |
Note: Variable selection for our final model was conducted using a stepwise backwards selection with AIC minimization. All variables from the full theoretical model were initially included.
Bold values indicate statistically significant associations at 0.05.
Figure 1 shows results from our dominance analysis. The dominance analysis was based on the variables selected into the final multivariable model. Results of the dominance analysis indicate relatively strong effects of physical health, family social support, emotional loneliness, support from significant other, home ownership, and getting enough sleep. In adjusting for other variables, the effect of emotional loneliness becomes dominant, and the independent effects of other variables become comparable to the remaining factors in the model.
FIGURE 1.
Conditional dominance plot
4. DISCUSSION
The present study examined the role of personal characteristics, social health, time use, and workplace factors in levels of burnout experienced by people living in Canada, aged 16+. Multivariable regression models and dominance analyses highlighted the importance of social inclusion, sleep, and financial security as key factors in preventing burnout—factors which may not be easily addressed by simple alterations to workplace culture and leadership. As noted above, most studies on burnout tend to focus on management styles, workplace conditions, and employee engagement/inspiration as strategies for reducing or preventing burnout. 34 Our data suggest that these factors, while perhaps important and even necessary elements for workplace interventions, 35 are not the only, or even the most influential, contributors to burnout—once we account for a broader suite of socio‐ecological contributors, 36 as we do in the current study.
Although there is a growing body of literature focused on workplace wellness that aims to address employee mental health through interventions, such as promoting healthy habits (such as sleep habits 37 ) and mindfulness 38 , 39 —our analyses suggest that mental health interventions alone are likely insufficient to fully address burnout. A broad vision for wellness, not bounded by work hours or office settings, must be accounted for in order to more fulsomely address the determinants of burnout. 40 Indeed, as noted by Koutsimani and Montgomery, 41 burnout is a distinct phenomenon from typical mental health constructs like anxiety and depression; thus, requiring specialized solutions to address it. Such interventions might, for instance, provide structural and individual support an employee's social health and well‐being, using tailored, person‐centered approaches that address specific, individual needs. 23 The emphasis on social health and health more broadly is supported by research showing that workplace social encounters play an important role in protecting against burnout 42 —suggesting that workplaces might play a role in limiting the impact of loneliness on mental health, but are not likely the entire solution. Indeed, our finding that family social support was one of the most important preventative factors to burnout indicates that the solution to burnout must go beyond workplace boundaries and allow individuals to develop deeply meaningful bonds with family or chosen family. Based on Social Support Resource theory, which highlights the importance of social support resource reserves to personal well‐being, we interpret our findings to indicate that family social support may be especially critical in the contemporary landscape of worker wellness. This may be because workers generally have more immediate access to family resources (perhaps due to greater contact)—however, when the necessary social support is not provided via family sources (perhaps because individuals are cut off from family resources due to distance or other reasons) these individuals are made especially vulnerable—particularly if they are unable to tap into other sources of support (e.g., from friends and significant others). These data highlight the importance of considering the specific mechanisms by which social support might prevent burnout. For example, Koutsimani and Montgomery 41 reported that perceived family support mediated the relationships between depression and exhaustion (a subdimension of burnout), but not between burnout and depression or burnout and anxiety. The role of support in shaping burnout may therefore be more complex than simply acting as a buffer against stress. As noted by Koutsimani and Montgomery, 41 social relationships can act as a stressor for people experiencing considerable levels of distress. It is possible that people who are highly distressed or burnt‐out experience diminishing returns from social investments. Further research is needed to fully understand the conditions under which social support might reduce burnout and to integrate these findings with existing theoretical frameworks describing the mechanisms driving burnout.
Our findings also suggest that factors such as employee pay scales and worker appreciation may play a role in shaping experiences of burnout. 43 Lower income individuals may not have enough disposable income to engage in activities that mitigate direct risk factors for mental illness, such as healthy diet, leisure, and exercise. 44 , 45 Moreover, financial pressures may contribute to dissatisfaction with work and, in combination, these factors may create or facilitate conditions for burnout to emerge. 43 This may explain our finding that people with Bachelor's degrees are at elevated risk for burnout compared to those with high school diplomas or lower. It is possible that these individuals experience unique financial strains, perhaps due to student loans. Alternatively, they may feel they should be better valued by their employers given their university credentials. Of course, it is also possible that people with university educations self‐select into careers that are, in themselves, risk factors for burnout (e.g., caregiving and education).
Finally, we note that improving employee health and accommodating disability are key parts of workplace wellness. Our study showed that physical health is deeply related to burnout, both as measured by self‐rated physical health and by the impact of sleep. Employers, employees, and clients will benefit greatly from interventions that support proactive health and wellness for employees. Other studies have emphasized the significant return on investment for employers who invest in employee wellness. Ignoring worker's health therefore seems to have long term negative impacts for workplaces. Policymakers should therefore consider burnout as an occupational disorder with significant impacts for workers. Based on the findings of the present study, we would conclude that such an evidence‐based approach would include not only workplace interventions, but also those which help individuals build robust social support and physical health outside of the workplace.
4.1. Strengths and Limitations
This study has several strengths. We advance the literature—which mostly focuses on caring professions (e.g., medical professionals, social workers, and teachers)—by focusing on a general population of workers, with oversampling among key sociodemographic groups (including 2SLGBTQ+ populations). Furthermore, we assess the extent to which the effects of identified factors on burnout operate through a general impact on mental health. In doing so, we hope to understand whether the mechanisms that contribute to burnout are independent of general mental health stressors. These analyses are meant to address common concerns about the conceptual boundaries between burnout and general mental health concerns, while also illuminating specific opportunities for intervention that go beyond traditional mental health workplace wellness programs.
This study has limitations. First, our sample is a convenience sample conducted during the COVID‐19 pandemic. Participation was optional. Only people with active social media accounts would have likely seen our survey advertisements. This makes prevalence estimates difficult to certify and may introduce error in model estimation. For example, we recruited a high number of participants who identified as 2SLGBTQ+ and a low number who reported as living with a disability. Despite this challenge, we note that our use of multivariable statistics to adjust for potential biases is intended to partially address this challenge by accounting for potential confounding effects that might arise from sampling imbalances. Second, the exclusion of participants who had missing data on our outcome or explanatory factors may introduce an additional layer of bias beyond the inherent bias in online samples. To estimate the potential bias attributable to this effect, we conducted bivariable tests to compare included participants to those who were excluded due to missingness across any of our explanatory variables. Included participants were less burnt out and lonely and felt better about their work environments and financial situation than did the excluded participants. While effect sizes were generally small for these differences, these differences suggest that individuals with better mental health may have been more resilient to survey fatigue and error. As a result, the effect sizes in the primary analyses may be slightly biased. Regardless, the direction of these relationships seems to reinforce our theoretical framework and therefore we suspect that potential biases arising from missingness would not change our general conclusions or interpretations of these results. Third, our analyses are cross‐sectional, and the causal paths could be reversed. Many cyclical processes may also be at play—leading to multiple, reverse, and mutual causal effects. For example, it is likely that burnout is protected against by having strong families, but it is also likely that people who experience burnout are unable to maintain the relationships necessary to form socially supportive bonds. Fourth, we note that while we have attempted to capture multiple dimensions about people's workplace environments, it is possible that experiences during the pandemic may greatly differ across occupation groups. Subanalyses within occupations—which are done widely among healthcare and other helping professions—provide insight into how burnout might impact a range of occupations differently. The present study lends some preliminary support for this work—particular with a focus among people in business, finance, and management, as well as healthcare. Finally, we note that the present study is exploratory of nature—aiming to highlight the contribution of relatively unexplored issues that might contribute to burnout beyond traditional workplace indicators.
5. CONCLUSION
Based on our findings that burnout is correlated with broad social determinants beyond those which are easily modified as part of workplace or occupational interventions, we suggest that addressing workplace culture, leadership, and other proximal workplace stressors, while important, are likely insufficient to meet the needs of workers who are experiencing burnout. Our findings suggest that broader, holistic approaches that address multiple upstream dimensions of health are likely necessary to prevent and reduce burnout in the workplace. Efforts to tailor workplace interventions using person‐centered methodology will likely support efforts to support worker mental health and wellness.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
All authors contributed to design and conceptualization of study. KGC conducted statistical analyses and prepared the initial draft manuscript. All authors reviewed and provided feedback on analysis and writing. All authors approved final manuscript for publication.
DISCLOSURES
Approval or Research Protocol: N/A. Informed Consent: All participants provided informed consent. Registry and the Registration no. of the study/trial: N/A. Animal Studies: N/A. Conflict of interest: None.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
The authors would like to thank participants of the Canadian Social Connection Survey, the Workplace Management Consultants with the Idea Suite, and our funder—the GenWell Project. KGC was funded by a Michael Smith Health Research BC Scholar Award.
Card KG, Bodner A, Li R, et al. Loneliness and social support as key contributors to burnout among Canadians workers in the third wave of the COVID‐19 pandemic: A cross‐sectional study. J Occup Health. 2022;64:e12360. doi: 10.1002/1348-9585.12360
DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENT
The data available for this study is available at www.instituteforsocialconnection.ca/cscs or by request from the study authors.
REFERENCES
- 1. Malach‐Pines A. The burnout measure, short version. Int J Stress Manag. 2005;12(1):78‐88. doi: 10.1037/1072-5245.12.1.78 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Cabarkapa S, Nadjidai SE, Murgier J, Ng CH. The psychological impact of COVID‐19 and other viral epidemics on frontline healthcare workers and ways to address it: a rapid systematic review. Brain Behav Immun – Health. 2020;8:100144. doi: 10.1016/j.bbih.2020.100144 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Sheather J, Slattery D. The great resignation—how do we support and retain staff already stretched to their limit? BMJ. 2021;375:n2533. doi: 10.1136/bmj.n2533 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Han BC. The Burnout Society. Stanford University Press; 2015. doi: 10.1515/9780804797504 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Thiery C. What is burnout? Revue du praticien. 2018;68(8):892‐896. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Dahiya R. Does virtue in the organisation affect happiness of employees? Int J Indian Cult Bus Manag. 2020;21(4):459‐477. doi: 10.1504/IJICBM.2020.111362 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Hartog CS. Burnout‐a call for action. Med Klin Intensivmed Notfallmedizin. 2019;114(8):693‐698. doi: 10.1007/s00063-017-0362-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Azam K, Khan A, Alam MT. Causes and adverse impact of physician burnout: a systematic review. J Coll Physicians Surg—Pak. 2017;27(8):495‐501. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Bogue TL, Bogue RL. Extinguish burnout in critical care nursing. Crit Care Nurs Clin North Am. 2020;32(3):451‐463. doi: 10.1016/j.cnc.2020.05.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Chen H, Wu P, Wei W. New perspective on job burnout: exploring the root cause beyond general antecedents analysis. Psychol Rep. 2012;110(3):801‐819. doi: 10.2466/01.09.13.PR0.110.3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Listopad IW, Michaelsen MM, Werdecker L, Esch T. Bio‐psycho‐socio‐Spirito‐cultural factors of burnout: a systematic narrative review of the literature. Front Psychol. 2021;12:722862. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.722862 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Groenewal PH, Putrino D, Norman MR. Burnout and motivation in sport. Psychiatr Clin North Am. 2021;44(3):359‐372. doi: 10.1016/j.psc.2021.04.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Langer J. The Impact of the Physical Office Environment on Occupant Wellbeing. PhD, 2021. . Accessed December 21, 2021. https://orca.cardiff.ac.uk/144731/
- 14. Naczenski LM, de Vries JD, van Hooff MLM, Kompier MAJ. Systematic review of the association between physical activity and burnout. J Occup Health. 2017;59(6):477‐494. doi: 10.1539/joh.17-0050-RA [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Chui H, Bryant E, Sarabia C, Maskeen S, Stewart‐Knox B. Burnout, eating behaviour traits and dietary patterns. Br Food J. 2019;122(2):404‐413. doi: 10.1108/BFJ-04-2019-0300 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Chirico F, Capitanelli I, Bollo M, Ferrari G, Acquadro MD. Association between workplace violence and burnout syndrome among schoolteachers: a systematic review. J Health Soc Sci. 2021;6:187‐208. doi: 10.19204/2021/ssct6 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Templeton K, Bernstein CA, Sukhera J, et al. Gender‐based differences in burnout: issues faced by women physicians. NAM Perspect. Published online May 30, 2019. doi: 10.31478/201905a [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Sahebi A, Nejati‐Zarnaqi B, Moayedi S, Yousefi K, Torres M, Golitaleb M. The prevalence of anxiety and depression among healthcare workers during the COVID‐19 pandemic: an umbrella review of meta‐analyses. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2021;107:110247. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2021.110247 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Lin SH, Huang YC. Investigating the relationships between loneliness and learning burnout. Act Learn High Educ. 2012;13(3):231‐243. doi: 10.1177/1469787412452983 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Stoliker BE, Lafreniere KD. The influence of perceived stress, loneliness, and learning burnout on university Students' educational experience. Coll Stud J. 2015;49(1):146‐160. [Google Scholar]
- 21. Hobfoll S, Freedy J, Geller P. Conservation of social resources: social support resource theory. J Soc Pers Relat. 1990;7:465‐478. doi: 10.1177/0265407590074004 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Eckleberry‐Hunt J, Kirkpatrick H, Barbera T. The problems with burnout research. Acad Med J Assoc Am Med Coll. 2018;93(3):367‐370. doi: 10.1097/ACM.0000000000001890 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Mäkikangas A, Kinnunen U. The person‐oriented approach to burnout: a systematic review. Burn Res. 2016;3(1):11‐23. doi: 10.1016/j.burn.2015.12.002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Ofei‐Dodoo S, Mullen R, Pasternak A, et al. Loneliness, burnout, and other types of emotional distress among family medicine physicians: results from a National Survey. J Am Board Fam Med. 2021;34(3):531‐541. doi: 10.3122/jabfm.2021.03.200566 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Southwick SM, Southwick FS. The loss of social connectedness as a major contributor to physician burnout: applying organizational and teamwork principles for prevention and recovery. JAMA Psychiatry. 2020;77(5):449‐450. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2019.4800 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Velando‐Soriano A, Ortega‐Campos E, Gómez‐Urquiza JL, Ramírez‐Baena L, De La Fuente EI, Cañadas‐De La Fuente GA. Impact of social support in preventing burnout syndrome in nurses: a systematic review. Jpn J Nurs Sci. 2020;17(1):e12269. doi: 10.1111/jjns.12269 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Harandi TF, Taghinasab MM, Nayeri TD. The correlation of social support with mental health: a meta‐analysis. Electron Physician. 2017;9(9):5212‐5222. doi: 10.19082/5212 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Measuring financial well‐being: A guide to using the CFPB Financial Well‐Being Scale. Consumer Financial Protection Bureau. Accessed August 4, 2022. https://www.consumerfinance.gov/data‐research/research‐reports/financial‐well‐being‐scale/
- 29. Gierveld JDJ, Tilburg TV. A 6‐item scale for overall, emotional, and social loneliness: confirmatory tests on survey data. Res Aging. 2006;28(5):582‐598. doi: 10.1177/0164027506289723 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Zimet G. Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support (MSPSS)—scale items and scoring information. 2016.
- 31. R Core Team . R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Published online 2021. Accessed January 8, 2022. https://www.r‐project.org/
- 32. VIF function RDocumentation. Accessed August 4, 2022. https://www.rdocumentation.org/packages/regclass/versions/1.6/topics/VIF
- 33. Azen R, Traxel N. Using dominance analysis to determine predictor importance in logistic regression. J Educ Behav Stat. 2009;34(3):319‐347. [Google Scholar]
- 34. West CP, Dyrbye LN, Erwin PJ, Shanafelt TD. Interventions to prevent and reduce physician burnout: a systematic review and meta‐analysis. Lancet Lond Engl. 2016;388(10057):2272‐2281. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(16)31279-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Rasool SF, Wang M, Tang M, Saeed A, Iqbal J. How toxic workplace environment effects the employee engagement: the mediating role of organizational support and employee wellbeing. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2021;18(5):2294. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18052294 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Bianchi R, Truchot D, Laurent E, Brisson R, Schonfeld IS. Is burnout solely job‐related? A critical comment. Scand J Psychol. 2014;55(4):357‐361. doi: 10.1111/sjop.12119 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Brand S, Holsboer‐Trachsler E. The burnout syndrome—an overview. Ther Umsch Rev Ther. 2010;67(11):561‐565. doi: 10.1024/0040-5930/a000095 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Li C, Zhu Y, Zhang M, Gustafsson H, Chen T. Mindfulness and athlete burnout: a systematic review and meta‐analysis. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2019;16(3):E449. doi: 10.3390/ijerph16030449 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Suleiman‐Martos N, Gomez‐Urquiza JL, Aguayo‐Estremera R, Cañadas‐De La Fuente GA, De La Fuente‐Solana EI, Albendín‐García L. The effect of mindfulness training on burnout syndrome in nursing: a systematic review and meta‐analysis. J Adv Nurs. 2020;76(5):1124‐1140. doi: 10.1111/jan.14318 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Lianov L. A powerful antidote to physician burnout: intensive healthy lifestyle and positive psychology approaches. Am J Lifestyle Med. 2021;15(5):563‐566. doi: 10.1177/15598276211006626 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Koutsimani P, Montgomery A. A two‐wave study on the associations of burnout with depression and anxiety: the mediating and moderating role of perceived family support. Psychol Rep. Published online November 28, 2021. doi: 10.1177/00332941211051263 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Leiter MP. Assessment of workplace social encounters: social profiles, burnout, and engagement. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2021;18(7):3533. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18073533 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Kim J, Garman ET. Financial stress, pay satisfaction and workplace performance. Compens Benefits Rev. 2004;36(1):69‐76. doi: 10.1177/0886368703261215 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Algren MH, Ekholm O, Nielsen L, Ersbøll AK, Bak CK, Andersen PT. Associations between perceived stress, socioeconomic status, and health‐risk behaviour in deprived neighbourhoods in Denmark: a cross‐sectional study. BMC Public Health. 2018;18(1):250. doi: 10.1186/s12889-018-5170-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Sheikh MA, Vancampfort D, Stubbs B. Leisure time physical activity and future psychological distress: a thirteen year longitudinal population‐based study. J Psychiatr Res. 2018;101:50‐56. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2018.02.025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
The data available for this study is available at www.instituteforsocialconnection.ca/cscs or by request from the study authors.