Skip to main content
Learning & Memory logoLink to Learning & Memory
. 2022 Sep;29(9):274–282. doi: 10.1101/lm.053521.121

Revisiting sex differences in the acquisition and extinction of threat conditioning in humans

Zhenfu Wen 1, Jamie Fried 1, Edward F Pace-Schott 2,3, Sara W Lazar 2,3, Mohammed R Milad 1,4,5
PMCID: PMC9488021  PMID: 36206388

Abstract

Findings pertaining to sex differences in the acquisition and extinction of threat conditioning, a paradigm widely used to study emotional homeostasis, remain inconsistent, particularly in humans. This inconsistency is likely due to multiple factors, one of which is sample size. Here, we pooled functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and skin conductance response (SCR) data from multiple studies in healthy humans to examine sex differences during threat conditioning, extinction learning, and extinction memory recall. We observed increased functional activation in males, relative to females, in multiple parietal and frontal (medial and lateral) cortical regions during acquisition of threat conditioning and extinction learning. Females mainly exhibited higher amygdala activation during extinction memory recall to the extinguished conditioned stimulus and also while responding to the unconditioned stimulus (presentation of the shock) during threat conditioning. Whole-brain functional connectivity analyses revealed that females showed increased connectivity across multiple networks including visual, ventral attention, and somatomotor networks during late extinction learning. At the psychophysiological level, a sex difference was only observed during shock delivery, with males exhibiting higher unconditioned responses relative to females. Our findings point to minimal to no sex differences in the expression of conditioned responses during acquisition and extinction of such responses. Functional MRI findings, however, show some distinct functional activations and connectivities between the sexes. These data suggest that males and females might use different neural mechanisms, mainly related to cognitive processing, to achieve comparable levels of acquired conditioned responses to threating cues.


Sex differences in forms of learning and memory have been widely observed (Andreano and Cahill 2009). The influences of sex on emotion learning and memory are of special interest. From a basic neuroscience perspective, it is intriguing to understand whether and how males and females might differ in facing fundamental neurobiological phenomena such as threat. From a clinical perspective, examining sex differences in emotion learning and memory may facilitate our understanding of the observed prevalence disparity in fear- and anxiety-related disorders between males and females (Tolin and Foa 2008; McLean et al. 2011; Steel et al. 2014; Jalnapurkar et al. 2018). Despite the recent significantly increased interest in sex differences and the molecular, cellular, neural, and behavioral mechanisms underlying such differences, much remains to be understood, especially within the context of emotion regulation.

One widely used laboratory model to study emotional learning and memory is the Pavlovian threat conditioning and extinction model. In this model, a neutral conditioned stimulus (CS) is repeatedly paired with an aversive unconditioned stimulus (US), so that the presentation of the CS alone elicits a conditioned threat response. This threat association can then be extinguished via repeated presentations of CS in the absence of the US. Following extinction learning, subjects often demonstrate reduced conditioned threat responses to the presentation of the now extinguished CS, indicating the retention of extinction memory. In line with the call in the field to refine the use of the terms “fear” and “threat” to distinguish between conscious feelings of fear and nonconscious processes of threat detection and responding (LeDoux 2012; LeDoux and Pine 2016; but see Fanselow and Pennington 2017), we adopt this distinction in the present study. We note, however, that there is some broader debate about the utility of such distinction. Threat conditioning and extinction are known to engage several key nodes of the “threat network” (Shin and Liberzon 2010; Milad and Quirk 2012; Alexandra Kredlow et al. 2021), including the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC), dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC), insular cortex, hippocampus, and amygdala. Many of these regions are sexually dimorphic (Goldstein et al. 2001; Cosgrove et al. 2007; Sacher et al. 2013), which might associate with sex differences during emotional learning and memory.

Using threat conditioning paradigms, sex differences of neural activations within the “threat network,” as well as psychophysiological and behavioral responses, have been reported in both rodent (Milad et al. 2009a; Baran et al. 2010; Fenton et al. 2014; Gruene et al. 2015) and human (Lebron-Milad et al. 2012; Hwang et al. 2015; Lonsdorf et al. 2015) studies. Generally, these studies suggest comparable threat conditioning between males and females, but deficient extinction learning and extinction memory recall in females. Whereas some studies observed sex differences during threat conditioning and extinction, some did not observe any sex differences, and others observed sex differences in the opposite direction (for reviews, see Merz et al. 2018; Velasco et al. 2019). One possible reason for this inconsistency is that studies often have insufficient statistical power to robustly detect subtle sex differences. Most studies to date contain relatively small sample sizes (<50 for each group).

In this study, we examined sex differences in the acquisition and extinction of threat conditioning in a relatively large sample of healthy human participants. To increase detection power, we aggregated functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data (N = 395–435, depending on the experimental phase) and skin conductance response (SCR) data (N = 152–187, depending on the phase) from healthy individuals across multiple studies (Marin et al. 2016; Sevinc et al. 2019; Seo et al. 2021; Wen et al. 2021a) to examine sex differences in the acquisition of threat conditioning and extinction. All participants underwent a validated Pavlovian threat conditioning and extinction paradigm (Milad et al. 2007b, 2009b). During threat conditioning, two cues were paired with a shock (CS+) and one cue was not paired with a shock (CS−). During extinction learning, a CS+ and a CS− were presented without shock. The next day, extinction memory was tested by presenting all three cues: extinguished CS+ (CS+E), unextinguished CS+ (CS+U), and CS−. We examined sex differences in brain activations of several regions of interest (ROIs) of the “threat network” that are thought to be critical in threat processing. We then extended the analysis to whole-brain activation beyond the “threat network.” Furthermore, we conducted whole-brain functional connectivity analysis to examine potential sex differences in broadly distributed brain networks that are engaged during emotional learning and memory. SCR data from different experimental stages were also examined.

Results

Considering that prior human and rodent studies showed distinct neural activations across trials within each experimental phase (Quirk et al. 1995; Milad et al. 2007b, 2009b; Burgos-Robles et al. 2009; Milad and Quirk 2012), we divided each phase into multiple trial blocks. As in previous studies, we focused our analyses on the early (the first four trials CS+ vs. CS−) and late (the last four trials CS+ vs. CS−) stages during threat conditioning and extinction learning. For the extinction memory recall, we focused on the early CS+E versus CS+U tests (the first four trials) to minimize the confounds introduced by additional extinction learning during late stage. We examined the effect of sex on neural activations, functional connectivity, and SCR data within each trial block. Note that the available data for each phase are different. This was due to the following factors: (1) a subset of participants underwent threat conditioning outside the fMRI scanner, (2) there were sometimes difficulties in collecting SCR data in the fMRI scanner, and (3) some participants dropped out on day 2. In our analyses, all available data for each single phase were included.

Threat conditioning

SCR data from 73 males and 116 females were analyzed for this phase. We first examined the effect of CS type (CS+ vs. CS−) across all participants, which revealed that CS+ evoked stronger SCR than CS− during both early (t(376) = 12.72, P < 0.001) and late (t(376) = 6.80, P < 0.001) threat conditioning, indicating that participants exhibited significant differential acquisition. We then examined sex differences in differential SCR responses (CS+ minus CS−). We did not observe significant sex differences during either early (t(187) = 1.42, P = 0.16) or late (t(187) = 0.75, P = 0.45) threat conditioning, suggesting that both males and females exhibited comparable levels of differential threat conditioning. A two-way mixed (CS type [CS+ vs. CS−] × sex) analysis of variance (ANOVA) also suggested that males showed higher overall CS response relative to females (F(1, 187) = 5.93, P = 0.016) during late threat conditioning (Supplemental Fig. S1A; see the Supplemental Material for detailed ANOVA results on SCR data).

Neuroimaging data from 138 males and 257 females were analyzed for this phase. During early conditioning, we did not observe significant sex differences in neural activation within the predefined ROIs or across the brain that survived correction for multiple comparisons. During late conditioning, however, males showed greater activation than females in multiple brain regions (Fig. 1), including the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC; MNIxyz = [−6, −2, 36]), precuneus (MNIxyz = [−8, −62, 42]), inferior frontal gyrus (IFG; MNIxyz = [−48, 44, 8]), and postcentral gyrus (PCG; MNIxyz = [−50, −22, 34]).

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Sex differences during threat conditioning. (A) Males and females exhibited similar SCR measures (CS+ – CS−) during early and late threat conditioning. (B) Males exhibited higher neural activations in multiple brain regions compared with females during late but not early threat conditioning.

We subsequently examined whether males and females differed in their unconditioned response—the response to the electric shock delivery. Analyses of the SCR data revealed that males showed higher unconditioned responses relative to females (t(187) = 2.75, P = 0.006). For neural activations (Fig. 2), females exhibited higher activation (compared with males) in the amygdala (small volume correction, MNIxyz = [26, −6, −20], cluster size = 69, PFWE = 0.011) and somatomotor area (whole-brain analysis, MNIxyz = [−4, 8, 70]).

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Sex differences in unconditioned response. (A) Males showed higher unconditioned response to shocks. (B) Females showed higher activations in the amygdala and somatomotor area relative to males during shock delivery.

Extinction learning

SCR data from 70 males and 112 females were analyzed for this phase. Sex differences in differential SCR responses (CS+ minus CS−) were not significant during either early (t(180) = 0.84, P = 0.40) or late (t(180) = 1.43, P = 0.15) extinction learning. A two-way mixed (CS type [CS+ vs. CS−] × sex) ANOVA suggested that males showed marginally higher overall CS response relative to females (F(1, 180) = 3.67, P = 0.056) during early extinction learning (Supplemental Fig. S1B; see the Supplemental Material).

Neuroimaging data from 138 males and 297 females were analyzed. During early extinction learning, there was no evidence of sex differences in neural activation either within the predefined ROIs or within our whole-brain analyses. During late extinction learning, we did not observe significant effects of sex on predefined ROIs. Whole-brain analyses, however, revealed that males exhibited higher activations (compared with females) in multiple brain regions (Fig. 3), including the dorsal lateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC; MNIxyz = [42, 22, 44]), superior frontal gyrus (SFG; MNIxyz = [20, 26, 54]), rostral anterior cingulate cortex (rACC; MNIxyz = [4, 58, 12]), and precuneus (MNIxyz = [10, −60, 28]).

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

Sex differences during extinction learning. (A) Males and females exhibited similar SCR measures (CS+ – CS−) during early and late extinction learning. (B) Males exhibited higher neural activations in multiple brain regions relative to females during late but not early extinction learning.

Extinction memory recall

SCR data from 62 males and 90 females were analyzed. As shown in Figure 4A, sex differences in SCR data (CS+E minus CS+U) were not significant during early extinction memory recall (t(150) = −0.13, P = 0.90).

Figure 4.

Figure 4.

Sex differences during early extinction memory recall. (A) Males and females exhibited similar SCR measures (CS+E – CS+U) during early extinction memory recall. (B) Females exhibited higher neural activations in the amygdala and inferior parietal gyrus during early extinction memory recall.

Neuroimaging data from 133 males and 285 females were analyzed. During early extinction memory recall, females, relative to males, exhibited higher activation in the right amygdala (small volume correction; MNIxyz = [−48, 44, 8], k = 29, PFWE = 0.056) and inferior parietal gyrus (whole-brain analysis; MNIxyz = [26, −52, 54]).

Connectivity analyses

Since brain regions across distributed neural systems are likely to interact with each other during threat conditioning and extinction (Fullana et al. 2018; Berg et al. 2020; Wen et al. 2021a), we conducted whole-brain functional connectivity analyses to examine potential sex differences in task-based connectivity. Based on previous studies (Schaefer et al. 2018; Tian et al. 2020), we divided the whole brain into 432 functionally homogeneous parcels, including 400 cortical and 32 subcortical regions. We used jackknife-based beta series correlation (Richter et al. 2015; Thompson et al. 2018; Wen et al. 2021a, 2022) to estimate connections between every two of the 432 functionally homogeneous regions. The Network-Based Statistic (NBS) procedure (Zalesky et al. 2010)—a validated method for controlling family-wise error rate (FWER)—revealed a significant network component (permutation test, PFWE < 0.05, 215 connections/edges) showing higher connectivity in females, relative to males, during late extinction learning (Fig. 5A). To further examine the distribution of the identified connections, we divided the 432 regions into eight subnetworks according to previous resting-state fMRI studies (Thomas Yeo et al. 2011; Schaefer et al. 2018; Tian et al. 2020). As shown in Figure 5B, the component with greater connectivity in females involved mostly connections from the visual, ventral/dorsal attention, and somatomotor networks. There was no evidence of sex differences in functional connectivity during any of the other experimental phases.

Figure 5.

Figure 5.

Sex differences in functional connectivity during late extinction learning. (A) Females showed higher functional connectivity compared with males across multiple neural systems. Each sphere represents a brain region; the color of the sphere represents its network assignment, and the size of the sphere represents the weighted number of connections that showed group differences. (B) The proportion of significantly different connections within or between the eight subnetworks. A darkly shaded cell indicates that the connections of that network pair (indexed from the X-axis and Y-axis) were showing massive sex differences.

Discussion

We evaluated sex differences in neural and physiological signatures during threat conditioning, extinction learning, and extinction memory recall. We combined data from >400 participants from multiple studies, which allowed us the power to detect potential subtle differences in neural and physiological measures between males and females. We observed significant sex differences in neural activations during late threat conditioning, late extinction learning, early extinction memory recall, and responses to unconditioned (shock) stimuli. Sex differences were also observed in functional connectivity during late extinction learning. In contrast to the sex differences observed in neuroimaging data, we did not observe any significant difference between males and females in differential learning and memory SCR measures. However, compared with females, males exhibited higher reactivity to shock and higher overall response to CS presentations in late conditioning and early extinction learning, regardless of CS types. The observed sex differences in the neural correlates of the acquisition and extinction of threat conditioning suggest distinct neural mechanisms used by females and males to achieve comparable levels of learning across experimental phases.

There were minimal sex differences in the differential activation to the conditioned stimuli (CS+ vs. CS−) within the nodes of the threat detection network. We observed significant increased BOLD signal within the amygdala in females in response to the delivery of the shock and during extinction memory retrieval. These results suggest that females engage the amygdala to a much higher level compared with males in order to maintain expression of conditioned and unconditioned threat responding. Interestingly, the increased amygdala activation in females in response to the shock coincided with lower—not higher—unconditioned responding as measured by SCR. The increased amygdala activation in females during extinction recall could possibly be related to the maintenance of fear/threat homeostasis during extinction recall, as opposed to increased expression of conditioned threat responding. These interpretations, although speculative, are consistent with accumulating evidence suggesting that increased amygdala activation is not synonymous with heightened fear/threat reactivity, but rather can also be related to signaling safety (as in threat extinction learning) (LeDoux 2007; Herry et al. 2008; Tovote et al. 2015). These results further support the complex and dynamic functioning of the amygdala not just as a brain region important for threat detection, but also as a region important for maintaining emotional homeostasis (Herry et al. 2008; Pare and Duvarci 2012; Tovote et al. 2015; Gothard 2020).

The only other node within the threat detection network that exhibited sex differences was the dACC; higher activation in males was observed during the acquisition of threat conditioning. Within the context of threat conditioning, this brain region is often associated with the expression of heightened monitoring of error detection and processing as well as threat conditioning (Botvinick et al. 2004; Milad et al. 2007a; Alexander and Brown 2019). There were no differences between the sexes in the expression of conditioned responding, as indexed by SCR, during threat conditioning. Thus, this increased activation in the dACC might reflect heightened error monitoring/detection in males. Of note, in addition to heightened reactivity to the shock, males also expressed generally higher skin conductance response to all stimuli during late conditioning and early extinction learning (see the Supplemental Material), likely indicating general, nonspecific elevation of arousal/reactivity to emotionally salient stimuli in men, and not a reflection of sex differences associated with learning and memory. We also note that the direction of sex difference we report in this study is opposite to our previous study (Lebron-Milad et al. 2012). Similarly, although previous studies reported sex differences in other regions of the “threat network” (Lebron-Milad et al. 2012; Hwang et al. 2015; Velasco et al. 2019) such as the vmPFC and insular cortex, we did not observe such differences in threat conditioning in the current large pooled sample using the same paradigm. We note two plausible explanations. One is that sex hormone variance within the current sample and prior samples might have contributed to these apparent discrepancies. The other, more parsimonious explanation is that prior studies (including our own) used smaller sample sizes. With the substantially larger sample size in the current sample, the results obtained here might be more robust for testing group-level differences.

We observed sex differences in brain activations beyond the “threat network.” While the contribution of the threat network is critical for the acquisition and extinction of conditioned threat responses, recent conceptualizations and empirical results show that threat conditioning and extinction engage interactions between distributed neural systems involved in attention control, conscious awareness, and sensory motor functioning (Fullana et al. 2016; LeDoux and Pine 2016; Taschereau-Dumouchel et al. 2019; Wen et al. 2021a, 2022; Zhou et al. 2021). Moreover, differences between males and females in structural volumes, structural connectivity, and functional connectivity across the brain have been widely reported (Gong et al. 2011; Ruigrok et al. 2014; Satterthwaite et al. 2015). Consistent with these studies, our results show that males and females exhibited different activations in high-order association cortices such as the IFC and dlPFC. Furthermore, we observed significant sex differences in task-based functional connectivity during late extinction learning, mainly involving the visual, ventral/dorsal attention, and somatomotor networks. These results suggest that the effects of sex on emotional learning might be distributed across multiple neural systems implicated in attention, motor, and sensory function. Given the lack of any measurable sex differences at the psychophysiological levels, at least as observed with SCR, these data suggest that males and females might engage higher cognitive structures differently in order to achieve the same behavioral/psychophysiological outcomes.

The sex differences observed in our present study were relatively minimal and do not necessarily align with clinical observations that females are more prone to psychopathology. Possibly, larger sex differences could be observed if sex hormones, menstrual cycle phase, and use of contraceptives were to be included in the analytic strategy. Indeed, in our prior studies conducted in rodents and humans, sex differences were initially absent and became apparent only when considering hormonal status in females (Milad et al. 2009a, 2010). Previous studies have reported impact of sex hormones on brain activations and functional connectivity (Lebron-Milad and Milad 2012; Andreano et al. 2018; Pritschet et al. 2020). Notably, estradiol significantly modulates the recall of an extinguished threat response, with higher circulating estradiol levels predicting successful extinction recall and exogenous estradiol administration improving recall in rats and humans (Graham and Milad 2013; Hwang et al. 2015; Maeng et al. 2017; Wen et al. 2021b; for a recent review, see Hammoud et al. 2020). In one of our previous studies, we showed that sex differences in fear extinction are influenced by the phase of the estrous cycle in female rats and of the menstrual cycle in women (Zeidan et al. 2011), an effect also seen with other psychophysiological indices including fear-potentiated startle (Glover et al. 2012). Larger sex differences in threat conditioning and extinction were observed when females were divided into low- and high-estradiol groups (Milad et al. 2009a, 2010). The use of contraceptives can also impact estradiol level as well as learning and memory (Montoya and Bos 2017). Therefore, factors such as endocrine status and use of oral contraceptives are important considerations when examining sex differences. Future studies considering these factors could facilitate our understanding of sex differences in threat learning and memory.

Findings in this study may have clinical implications given that females have a significantly higher prevalence of fear- and anxiety-related disorders than males, and that sex differences in trauma exposure do not fully account for this differential prevalence (Gater et al. 1998; Tolin and Foa 2008). These vulnerability differences might be mediated by brain-based differences in the processing of emotional stimuli in females compared with males. Threat conditioning and extinction paradigms have been used to investigate the psychopathology of emotional disorders. Abnormal psychophysiological responses and neural activations in individuals with anxiety disorders and posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) during threat conditioning and extinction have been documented in previous studies (Norrholm et al. 2011; Milad and Quirk 2012; Duits et al. 2015; Shalev et al. 2017; Norrholm and Jovanovic 2018; Ressler 2020; Alexandra Kredlow et al. 2021). Sex differences among individuals with fear- and anxiety-related disorders have emerged in threat conditioning and extinction paradigms (Glover et al. 2012; Shvil et al. 2014; Sartin-Tarm et al. 2020). For example, psychophysiological and neural activation patterns associated with deficits of extinction memory recall were observed in males but not females with PTSD (Shvil et al. 2014). Further investigation of sex differences during threat learning and memory might facilitate our understanding the mechanisms for the higher risk of developing fear- and anxiety-related disorders in females than males.

We note a few limitations of the present study that should be considered. First, we did not collect subjective reports of fear, which prevented us from linking the observed sex differences in neural and physiological signatures with the conscious feelings of fear. Second, because of difficulties in collecting SCR data in the fMRI scanner, we only collected SCR measures from a subset of the participants, which might have decreased the statistical power available to detect sex differences in SCR measures. Third, it is possible that the sex differences in psychophysiological and neural patterns observed in our study might differ from other studies given different experimental settings such as the stimulus types and reinforcement rates across threat conditioning paradigms. Fourth, our peripheral measure of threat reactivity was SCR. It is possible that other psychophysiological indices, like startle, might be more sensitive in detecting sex differences during threat conditioning. Therefore, we cannot rule out the possibility that the increased amygdala activation in females during extinction recall might be related to increased fear/threat responding that was not detected by SCR. There is evidence supporting sex differences in threat conditioning and extinction when using other complementary psychophysiological measures, such as fear-potentiated startle and stimulus expectancy ratings across species (Merz et al. 2018; Velasco et al. 2019). For example, female rats exhibited less extinction retention than male rats when measured by fear-potentiated startle (Voulo and Parsons 2017). In human participants, sex differences in fear-potentiated startle, fear ratings, and unconditioned stimulus expectancy were observed (Gamwell et al. 2015; Lonsdorf et al. 2015). Thus, it is possible that other data modalities (other than those used in our present study) might reveal sex differences during threat conditioning and extinction.

In summary, the present study demonstrates that males and females likely exhibit different patterns of brain activation and functional connectivity during threat conditioning and extinction learning and its memory recall. These differences, however, appear to be minimal within the threat detection network itself but are distributed across multiple neural systems engaged in cognition, attention, and memory processes. Our results also support the idea that females and males appear to use different neural mechanisms to achieve comparable behavioral outputs. It is also worth noting that when analyzing larger sample sizes, sex differences appear to be relatively small and can easily be undetected. It is therefore important to consider sex as a factor when investigating neural circuits during threat conditioning and extinction, especially when studying psychopathology.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Data from a total of 435 healthy participants (297 female and 138 male) across multiple studies were included in the analyses. Among the 435 participants, 135 (82 females and 53 males) were trauma-exposed individuals without diagnosis of psychiatric disorders. Results from different subsets of the participants included in the analyses had been previously published with different scientific questions and different analytic strategies (Marin et al. 2016; Sevinc et al. 2019; Seo et al. 2021; Wen et al. 2021a). All participants were recruited at Massachusetts General Hospital. All procedures were approved by the Partners HealthCare Institutional Review Board of Massachusetts General Hospital. All participants provided written informed consent before they participated in the study.

Experimental design

Participants underwent a 2-d threat conditioning and extinction paradigm (Milad et al. 2007b, 2009b; Marin et al. 2017, 2020) during fMRI scanning. On day 1, participants first completed the threat conditioning phase. They were presented with three cues (conditioned stimuli [CSs]), two of which were partially reinforced (reinforcement rate: 62.5%) with a mild electric shock (CS+, eight trials for each) while the other was not reinforced (CS−, 16 trials). Following conditioning, participants underwent the extinction learning phase, during which the CS− (16 trials) and one of the CS+ (termed CS+ extinguished [CS+E], 16 trials) were repeatedly presented without shock. Twenty-four hours later (day 2), participants underwent the extinction recall test in the fMRI scanner. Participants were presented with all three cues: the CS+E (eight trials), the CS+ that was not extinguished on day 1 (termed CS+U, eight trials), and the CS− (16 trials) without shock.

Image acquisition and preprocessing

Neuroimaging data were acquired using three different MRI settings. Data from 64 participants were acquired in a Trio 3.0 Tesla whole-body MRI scanner (Siemens Medical Systems) using an eight-channel head coil. Functional data were acquired using a T2*-weighted echo-planar imaging (EPI) pulse sequence (TR: 3.0 sec, TE: 30 msec, slice number: 45, voxel size: 3 × 3 × 3 mm). Data from 180 participants were acquired in the same Trio 3.0 Tesla MRI scanner using a 32-channel head coil. Functional data were acquired using a T2*-weighted EPI pulse sequence (TR: 2.56 sec, TE: 30 msec, slice number: 48, voxel size: 3 × 3 × 3 mm). Imaging data from 97 participants were acquired on a Siemen's Prisma 3.0T scanner equipped with a 32-channel head coil. Functional data were acquired using a T2*-weighted EPI pulse sequence (TR: 3.0 sec, TE: 30 msec, slice number: 48, voxel size: 2.5 × 2.5 × 2.5 mm). High-resolution anatomical images were acquired for image registration.

Preprocessing was performed using the default pipeline of fMRIPrep 20.0.2—a standard toolbox for automatic fMRI data preprocessing (Esteban et al. 2019). Functional images were corrected for slice timing, realigned, coregistered with the structural image, normalized into the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space, and smoothed with a 6-mm full-width half-maximum Gaussian kernel.

Imaging data analyses

Task-based activation

For threat conditioning and extinction learning, we created contrast maps for the early (first four trials CS+ vs. CS−) and late (last four trials CS+ vs. CS−) phases. For the extinction memory test, we focused on the early CS+E versus CS+U (the first four trials) contrast map. The rationale for selecting these specific trials for each phase was based on prior fMRI and animal studies showing distinct neural activations for these trials within each examined phase (Quirk et al. 1995; Milad et al. 2007b, 2009b; Burgos-Robles et al. 2009; Milad and Quirk 2012). Individuals’ contrast maps were used as the dependent variable for the second-level group analyses and sex was used as an independent variable. The impact of age was controlled by including the age of participants as a covariate. Study site information was included as covariates in our analyses to control for site effects.

Based on prior studies on threat conditioning and extinction, we were specifically interested in the following regions of interest (ROIs): the amygdala, hippocampus, insular cortex, dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC), and ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC). The masks of the amygdala, hippocampus, and insular cortex were created based on the Harvard–Oxford structural atlas (probability threshold: 50%). For dACC and vmPFC ROIs, we searched the keyword “conditioning” in the Neurosynth database (https://neurosynth.org) and identified two activation peaks (dACC: [0, 14, 28], vmPFC: [−2, 46, −10]). We created 8-mm spheres around each coordinate to generate ROI masks. Activations detected in these ROIs with an uncorrected voxel-level P < 0.005 were tested with small volume corrections (family-wise error rate corrected PFWE < 0.05) using the corresponding masks. We also examined brain regions beyond the ROIs. In this analysis, group-level activation maps were thresholded with a voxel-level P < 0.005 and a cluster-level PFWE < 0.05.

Task-based connectivity

We examined task-based functional connectivity among brain regions. We divided the whole brain into 432 functionally homogeneous regions, including 400 cortical regions (Schaefer et al. 2018) and 32 subcortical regions (Tian et al. 2020). Each region was assigned to one of eight canonical subnetworks according to previous studies (Thomas Yeo et al. 2011), including the frontoparietal control network (CON), default mode network (DMN), dorsal attention network (DAN), limbic network (LIM), ventral attention network (VAN), somatomotor network (SMN), visual network (VIS), and subcortical network (SUB). For every two regions, we combined the beta series correlations method (Rissman et al. 2004) and the jackknife correlation method to estimate task-based connectivity (Richter et al. 2015; Thompson et al. 2018; Wen et al. 2021a). We focused on functional connectivity during early and late threat conditioning/extinction learning and early extinction memory recall. We constructed a symmetrized 432 × 432 FC matrix for each condition, with each value representing an edge (connection) between paired regions. We used the Combat harmonization approach to correct site effects (Fortin et al. 2017; Yu et al. 2018). We used the Network-Based Statistic (NBS) procedure (Zalesky et al. 2010)—a well-validated method for controlling family-wise error rate (FWER)—to identify edges that showed significant sex difference. In the NBS procedure, the edge-level threshold was set to P < 0.001 and the cluster-level threshold was set to PFWE < 0.05.

Physiological data analyses

Skin conductance response (SCR) data were analyzed as described in our prior studies (Milad et al. 2005, 2007b, 2009b). SCR to each stimulus was computed by subtracting the mean skin conductance level observed during the last 2 sec of context preceding CS onset from the maximal skin conductance level reached during CS presentation. Analyses were performed on square root transformed data (if negative values were obtained, square root transformation was applied to the absolute value and a negative sign was then assigned to the square root transformed value), with age and site effects regressed out. To align with the fMRI data analysis strategy, we used two-sample t-test to examine the effects of sex in differential SCRs (CS+ minus CS−) during early/late threat conditioning and extinction learning, and the difference between CS+E and CS+U during early extinction memory recall. For completeness, we also conducted two-way mixed analyses of variance (ANOVAs) on the SCR data, with sex (male vs. female) as a between-subjects factor and CS type (CS+ vs. CS− for conditioning and extinction learning, or CS+E vs. CS+U for extinction memory recall) as a within-subjects factor. The results are presented in the Supplemental Material.

Competing interest statement

Praxis Precision Medicines, Inc., provides partial salary support to E.F.P.-S. The other authors declare no competing interests.

Supplementary Material

Supplemental Material

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by National Institute of Mental Health grants R01MH097964 and R01MH097880 to M.R.M., R01MH109638 to E.F.P.-S., and 1R01AT006344-01 to S.W.L.

Footnotes

[Supplemental material is available for this article.]

References

  1. Alexander WH, Brown JW. 2019. The role of the anterior cingulate cortex in prediction error and signaling surprise. Top Cogn Sci 11: 119–135. 10.1111/tops.12307 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Alexandra Kredlow M, Fenster RJ, Laurent ES, Ressler KJ, Phelps EA. 2021. Prefrontal cortex, amygdala, and threat processing: implications for PTSD. Neuropsychopharmacol 47: 247–259. 10.1038/s41386-021-01155-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Andreano JM, Cahill L. 2009. Sex influences on the neurobiology of learning and memory. Learn Mem 16: 248–266. 10.1101/lm.918309 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Andreano JM, Touroutoglou A, Dickerson B, Barrett LF. 2018. Hormonal cycles, brain network connectivity, and windows of vulnerability to affective disorder. Trends Neurosci 41: 660–676. 10.1016/j.tins.2018.08.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Baran SE, Armstrong CE, Niren DC, Conrad CD. 2010. Prefrontal cortex lesions and sex differences in fear extinction and perseveration. Learn Mem 17: 267–278. 10.1101/lm.1778010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Berg H, Ma Y, Rueter A, Kaczkurkin A, Burton P, Deyoung C, MacDonald A, Sponheim S, Lissek S. 2020. Salience and central executive networks track overgeneralization of conditioned-fear in post-traumatic stress disorder. Psychol Med 51: 2610–2619. 10.1017/S0033291720001166 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Botvinick MM, Cohen JD, Carter CS. 2004. Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: an update. Trends Cogn Sci 8: 539–546. 10.1016/j.tics.2004.10.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Burgos-Robles A, Vidal-Gonzalez I, Quirk GJ. 2009. Sustained conditioned responses in prelimbic prefrontal neurons are correlated with fear expression and extinction failure. J Neurosci 29: 8474–8482. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0378-09.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Cosgrove KP, Mazure CM, Staley JK. 2007. Evolving knowledge of sex differences in brain structure, function, and chemistry. Biol Psychiatry 62: 847–855. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.03.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Duits P, Cath DC, Lissek S, Hox JJ, Hamm AO, Engelhard IM, van den Hout MA, Baas JMP. 2015. Updated meta-analysis of classical fear conditioning in the anxiety disorders. Depress Anxiety 32: 239–253. 10.1002/da.22353 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Esteban O, Markiewicz CJ, Blair RW, Moodie CA, Isik AI, Erramuzpe A, Kent JD, Goncalves M, DuPre E, Snyder M, et al. 2019. fMRIPrep: a robust preprocessing pipeline for functional MRI. Nat Methods 16: 111–116. 10.1038/s41592-018-0235-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Fanselow MS, Pennington ZT. 2017. The danger of LeDoux and Pine's two-system framework for fear. Am J Psychiatry 174: 1120–1121. 10.1176/appi.ajp.2017.17070818 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Fenton GE, Pollard AK, Halliday DM, Mason R, Bredy TW, Stevenson CW. 2014. Persistent prelimbic cortex activity contributes to enhanced learned fear expression in females. Learn Mem 21: 55–60. 10.1101/lm.033514.113 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Fortin J-P, Parker D, Tunç B, Watanabe T, Elliott MA, Ruparel K, Roalf DR, Satterthwaite TD, Gur RC, Gur RE, et al. 2017. Harmonization of multi-site diffusion tensor imaging data. Neuroimage 161: 149–170. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.08.047 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Fullana MA, Harrison BJ, Soriano-Mas C, Vervliet B, Cardoner N, Àvila-Parcet A, Radua J. 2016. Neural signatures of human fear conditioning: an updated and extended meta-analysis of fMRI studies. Mol Psychiatry 21: 500–508. 10.1038/mp.2015.88 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Fullana MA, Albajes-Eizagirre A, Soriano-Mas C, Vervliet B, Cardoner N, Benet O, Radua J, Harrison BJ. 2018. Fear extinction in the human brain: a meta-analysis of fMRI studies in healthy participants. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 88: 16–25. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.03.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Gamwell K, Nylocks M, Cross D, Bradley B, Norrholm SD, Jovanovic T. 2015. Fear conditioned responses and PTSD symptoms in children: sex differences in fear-related symptoms. Dev Psychobiol 57: 799–808. 10.1002/dev.21313 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Gater R, Tansella M, Korten A, Tiemens BG, Mavreas VG, Olatawura MO. 1998. Sex differences in the prevalence and detection of depressive and anxiety disorders in general health care settings: report from the world health organization collaborative study on psychological problems in general health care. Arch Gen Psychiatry 55: 405–413. 10.1001/archpsyc.55.5.405 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Glover EM, Jovanovic T, Mercer KB, Kerley K, Bradley B, Ressler KJ, Norrholm SD. 2012. Estrogen levels are associated with extinction deficits in women with posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychiatry 72: 19–24. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.02.031 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Goldstein JM, Seidman LJ, Horton NJ, Makris N, Kennedy DN, Caviness VS Jr, Faraone SV, Tsuang MT. 2001. Normal sexual dimorphism of the adult human brain assessed by in vivo magnetic resonance imaging. Cereb Cortex 11: 490–497. 10.1093/cercor/11.6.490 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Gong G, He Y, Evans AC. 2011. Brain connectivity: gender makes a difference. Neuroscientist 17: 575–591. 10.1177/1073858410386492 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Gothard KM. 2020. Multidimensional processing in the amygdala. Nat Rev Neurosci 21: 565–575. 10.1038/s41583-020-0350-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Graham BM, Milad MR. 2013. Blockade of estrogen by hormonal contraceptives impairs fear extinction in female rats and women. Biol Psychiatry 73: 371–378. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.09.018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Gruene TM, Roberts E, Thomas V, Ronzio A, Shansky RM. 2015. Sex-specific neuroanatomical correlates of fear expression in prefrontal-amygdala circuits. Biol Psychiatry 78: 186–193. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.11.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Hammoud MZ, Foa EB, Milad MR. 2020. Oestradiol, threat conditioning and extinction, post-traumatic stress disorder, and prolonged exposure therapy: a common link. J Neuroendocrinol 32: e12800. 10.1111/jne.12800 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Herry C, Ciocchi S, Senn V, Demmou L, Müller C, Lüthi A. 2008. Switching on and off fear by distinct neuronal circuits. Nature 454: 600–606. 10.1038/nature07166 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Hwang MJ, Zsido RG, Song H, Pace-Schott EF, Miller KK, Lebron-Milad K, Marin M-F, Milad MR. 2015. Contribution of estradiol levels and hormonal contraceptives to sex differences within the fear network during fear conditioning and extinction. BMC Psychiatry 15: 295. 10.1186/s12888-015-0673-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Jalnapurkar I, Allen M, Pigott T. 2018. Sex differences in anxiety disorders: a review. J Psychiatry Depress Anxiety 4: 011. 10.24966/PDA-0150/100011 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  29. Lebron-Milad K, Milad MR. 2012. Sex differences, gonadal hormones and the fear extinction network: implications for anxiety disorders. Biol Mood Anxiety Disord 2: 3. 10.1186/2045-5380-2-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Lebron-Milad K, Abbs B, Milad MR, Linnman C, Rougemount-Bücking A, Zeidan MA, Holt DJ, Goldstein JM. 2012. Sex differences in the neurobiology of fear conditioning and extinction: a preliminary fMRI study of shared sex differences with stress-arousal circuitry. Biol Mood Anxiety Disord 2: 7. 10.1186/2045-5380-2-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. LeDoux J. 2007. The amygdala. Curr Biol 17: R868–R874. 10.1016/j.cub.2007.08.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. LeDoux J. 2012. Rethinking the emotional brain. Neuron 73: 653–676. 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.02.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. LeDoux JE, Pine DS. 2016. Using neuroscience to help understand fear and anxiety: a two-system framework. Am J Psychiatry 173: 1083–1093. 10.1176/appi.ajp.2016.16030353 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Lonsdorf TB, Haaker J, Schümann D, Sommer T, Bayer J, Brassen S, Bunzeck N, Gamer M, Kalisc R. 2015. Sex differences in conditioned stimulus discrimination during context-dependent fear learning and its retrieval in humans: the role of biological sex, contraceptives and menstrual cycle phases. J Psychiatry Neurosci 40: 368–375. 10.1503/140336 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Maeng LY, Cover KK, Taha MB, Landau AJ, Milad MR, Lebrón-Milad K. 2017. Estradiol shifts interactions between the infralimbic cortex and central amygdala to enhance fear extinction memory in female rats. J Neurosci Res 95: 163–175. 10.1002/jnr.23826 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Marin M-F, Song H, VanElzakker MB, Staples-Bradley LK, Linnman C, Pace-Schott EF, Lasko NB, Shin LM, Milad MR. 2016. Association of resting metabolism in the fear neural network with extinction recall activations and clinical measures in trauma-exposed individuals. Am J Psychiatry 173: 930–938. 10.1176/appi.ajp.2015.14111460 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Marin M-F, Zsido RG, Song H, Lasko NB, Killgore WDS, Rauch SL, Simon NM, Milad MR. 2017. Skin conductance responses and neural activations during fear conditioning and extinction recall across anxiety disorders. JAMA Psychiatry 74: 622. 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2017.0329 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Marin M-F, Hammoud MZ, Klumpp H, Simon NM, Milad MR. 2020. Multimodal categorical and dimensional approaches to understanding threat conditioning and its extinction in individuals with anxiety disorders. JAMA Psychiatry 77: 618–627. 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2019.4833 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. McLean CP, Asnaani A, Litz BT, Hofmann SG. 2011. Gender differences in anxiety disorders: prevalence, course of illness, comorbidity and burden of illness. J Psychiatr Res 45: 1027–1035. 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2011.03.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Merz CJ, Kinner VL, Wolf OT. 2018. Let's talk about sex … differences in human fear conditioning. Curr Opin Behav Sci 23: 7–12. 10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.01.021 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  41. Milad MR, Quirk GJ. 2012. Fear extinction as a model for translational neuroscience: ten years of progress. Annu Rev Psychol 63: 129–151. 10.1146/annurev.psych.121208.131631 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Milad MR, Orr SP, Pitman RK, Rauch SL. 2005. Context modulation of memory for fear extinction in humans. Psychophysiology 42: 456–464. 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2005.00302.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Milad MR, Quirk GJ, Pitman RK, Orr SP, Fischl B, Rauch SL. 2007a. A role for the human dorsal anterior cingulate cortex in fear expression. Biol Psychiatry 62: 1191–1194. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.04.032 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Milad MR, Wright CI, Orr SP, Pitman RK, Quirk GJ, Rauch SL. 2007b. Recall of fear extinction in humans activates the ventromedial prefrontal cortex and hippocampus in concert. Biol Psychiatry 62: 446–454. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.10.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Milad MR, Igoe SA, Lebron-Milad K, Novales JE. 2009a. Estrous cycle phase and gonadal hormones influence conditioned fear extinction. Neuroscience 164: 887–895. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.09.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Milad MR, Pitman RK, Ellis CB, Gold AL, Shin LM, Lasko NB, Zeidan MA, Handwerger K, Orr SP, Rauch SL. 2009b. Neurobiological basis of failure to recall extinction memory in posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychiatry 66: 1075–1082. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.06.026 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Milad MR, Zeidan MA, Contero A, Pitman RK, Klibanski A, Rauch SL, Goldstein JM. 2010. The influence of gonadal hormones on conditioned fear extinction in healthy humans. Neuroscience 168: 652–658. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.04.030 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Montoya ER, Bos PA. 2017. How oral contraceptives impact social-emotional behavior and brain function. Trends Cogn Sci 21: 125–136. 10.1016/j.tics.2016.11.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Norrholm SD, Jovanovic T. 2018. Fear processing, psychophysiology, and PTSD. Harv Rev Psychiatry 26: 129–141. 10.1097/HRP.0000000000000189 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Norrholm SD, Jovanovic T, Olin IW, Sands LA, Karapanou I, Bradley B, Ressler KJ. 2011. Fear extinction in traumatized civilians with posttraumatic stress disorder: relation to symptom severity. Biol Psychiatry 69: 556–563. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.09.013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Pare D, Duvarci S. 2012. Amygdala microcircuits mediating fear expression and extinction. Curr Opin Neurobiol 22: 717–723. 10.1016/j.conb.2012.02.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Pritschet L, Santander T, Taylor CM, Layher E, Yu S, Miller MB, Grafton ST, Jacobs EG. 2020. Functional reorganization of brain networks across the human menstrual cycle. Neuroimage 220: 117091. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117091 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Quirk GJ, Repa JC, LeDoux JE. 1995. Fear conditioning enhances short-latency auditory responses of lateral amygdala neurons: parallel recordings in the freely behaving rat. Neuron 15: 1029–1039. 10.1016/0896-6273(95)90092-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Ressler KJ. 2020. Translating across circuits and genetics toward progress in fear- and anxiety-related disorders. Am J Psychiatry 177: 214–222. 10.1176/appi.ajp.2020.20010055 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Richter CG, Thompson WH, Bosman CA, Fries P. 2015. A jackknife approach to quantifying single-trial correlation between covariance-based metrics undefined on a single-trial basis. Neuroimage 114: 57–70. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.04.040 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Rissman J, Gazzaley A, D'Esposito M. 2004. Measuring functional connectivity during distinct stages of a cognitive task. Neuroimage 23: 752–763. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.06.035 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Ruigrok ANV, Salimi-Khorshidi G, Lai M-C, Baron-Cohen S, Lombardo MV, Tait RJ, Suckling J. 2014. A meta-analysis of sex differences in human brain structure. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 39: 34–50. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.12.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Sacher J, Neumann J, Okon-Singer H, Gotowiec S, Villringer A. 2013. Sexual dimorphism in the human brain: evidence from neuroimaging. Magn Reson Imaging 31: 366–375. 10.1016/j.mri.2012.06.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Sartin-Tarm A, Ross MC, Privatsky AA, Cisler JM. 2020. Estradiol modulates neural and behavioral arousal in women with posttraumatic stress disorder during a fear learning and extinction task. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging 5: 1114–1122. 10.1016/j.bpsc.2020.04.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Satterthwaite TD, Wolf DH, Roalf DR, Ruparel K, Erus G, Vandekar S, Gennatas ED, Elliott MA, Smith A, Hakonarson H, et al. 2015. Linked sex differences in cognition and functional connectivity in youth. Cereb Cortex 25: 2383–2394. 10.1093/cercor/bhu036 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Schaefer A, Kong R, Gordon EM, Laumann TO, Zuo X-N, Holmes AJ, Eickhoff SB, Yeo BTT. 2018. Local–global parcellation of the human cerebral cortex from intrinsic functional connectivity MRI. Cereb Cortex 28: 3095–3114. 10.1093/cercor/bhx179 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Seo J, Oliver KI, Daffre C, Moore KN, Gazecki S, Lasko NB, Milad MR, Pace-Schott EF. 2021. Associations of sleep measures with neural activations accompanying fear conditioning and extinction learning and memory in trauma-exposed individuals. Sleep 45: zsab261. 10.1093/sleep/zsab261 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Sevinc G, Hölzel BK, Greenberg J, Gard T, Brunsch V, Hashmi JA, Vangel M, Orr SP, Milad MR, Lazar SW. 2019. Strengthened hippocampal circuits underlie enhanced retrieval of extinguished fear memories following mindfulness training. Biol Psychiatry 86: 693–702. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2019.05.017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Shalev A, Liberzon I, Marmar C. 2017. Post-traumatic stress disorder. N Engl J Med 376: 2459–2469. 10.1056/NEJMra1612499 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Shin LM, Liberzon I. 2010. The neurocircuitry of fear, stress, and anxiety disorders. Neuropsychopharmacol 35: 169–191. 10.1038/npp.2009.83 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Shvil E, Sullivan GM, Schafer S, Markowitz JC, Campeas M, Wager TD, Milad MR, Neria Y. 2014. Sex differences in extinction recall in posttraumatic stress disorder: a pilot fMRI study. Neurobiol Learn Mem 113: 101–108. 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.02.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Steel Z, Marnane C, Iranpour C, Chey T, Jackson JW, Patel V, Silove D. 2014. The global prevalence of common mental disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis 1980–2013. Int J Epidemiol 43: 476–493. 10.1093/ije/dyu038 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Taschereau-Dumouchel V, Kawato M, Lau H. 2019. Multivoxel pattern analysis reveals dissociations between subjective fear and its physiological correlates. Mol Psychiatry 25: 2342–2354. 10.1038/s41380-019-0520-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Thomas Yeo BT, Krienen FM, Sepulcre J, Sabuncu MR, Lashkari D, Hollinshead M, Roffman JL, Smoller JW, Zöllei L, Polimeni JR, et al. 2011. The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J Neurophysiol 106: 1125–1165. 10.1152/jn.00338.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Thompson WH, Richter CG, Plavén-Sigray P, Fransson P. 2018. Simulations to benchmark time-varying connectivity methods for fMRI. PLoS Comput Biol 14: e1006196. 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1006196 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Tian Y, Margulies DS, Breakspear M, Zalesky A. 2020. Topographic organization of the human subcortex unveiled with functional connectivity gradients. Nat Neurosci 23: 1421–1432. 10.1038/s41593-020-00711-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Tolin DF, Foa EB. 2008. Sex differences in trauma and posttraumatic stress disorder: a quantitative review of 25 years of research. Psychol Trauma S 37–85. 10.1037/1942-9681.S.1.37 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Tovote P, Fadok JP, Lüthi A. 2015. Neuronal circuits for fear and anxiety. Nat Rev Neurosci 16: 317–331. 10.1038/nrn3945 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Velasco ER, Florido A, Milad MR, Andero R. 2019. Sex differences in fear extinction. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 103: 81–108. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.05.020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Voulo ME, Parsons RG. 2017. Response-specific sex difference in the retention of fear extinction. Learn Mem 24: 245–251. 10.1101/lm.045641.117 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Wen Z, Chen ZS, Milad MR. 2021a. Fear extinction learning modulates large-scale brain connectivity. Neuroimage 238: 118261. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118261 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Wen Z, Hammoud MZ, Scott JC, Jimmy J, Brown L, Marin M-F, Asnaani A, Gur RC, Foa EB, Milad MR. 2021b. Impact of exogenous estradiol on task-based and resting-state neural signature during and after fear extinction in healthy women. Neuropsychopharmacol 46: 2278–2287. 10.1038/s41386-021-01158-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Wen Z, Seo J, Pace-Schott EF, Milad MR. 2022. Abnormal dynamic functional connectivity during fear extinction learning in PTSD and anxiety disorders. Mol Psychiatry 27: 2216–2224. 10.1038/s41380-022-01462-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Yu M, Linn KA, Cook PA, Phillips ML, McInnis M, Fava M, Trivedi MH, Weissman MM, Shinohara RT, Sheline YI. 2018. Statistical harmonization corrects site effects in functional connectivity measurements from multi-site fMRI data. Hum Brain Mapp 39: 4213–4227. 10.1002/hbm.24241 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Zalesky A, Fornito A, Bullmore ET. 2010. Network-based statistic: identifying differences in brain networks. Neuroimage 53: 1197–1207. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.06.041 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Zeidan MA, Igoe SA, Linnman C, Vitalo A, Levine JB, Klibanski A, Goldstein JM, Milad MR. 2011. Estradiol modulates medial prefrontal cortex and amygdala activity during fear extinction in women and female rats. Biol Psychiatry 70: 920–927. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.05.016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Zhou F, Zhao W, Qi Z, Geng Y, Yao S, Kendrick KM, Wager TD, Becker B. 2021. A distributed fMRI-based signature for the subjective experience of fear. Nat Commun 12: 6643. 10.1038/s41467-021-26977-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplemental Material

Articles from Learning & Memory are provided here courtesy of Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press

RESOURCES