Abstract
Chronic heart failure (HF) is a serious disorder that afflicts more than 26 million patients worldwide. HF is comorbid with depression, anxiety and memory deficits that have serious implications for quality of life and self-care in patients who have HF. Still, there are few studies that have assessed the effects of severely reduced ejection fraction (≤40%) on cognition in non-human animal models. Moreover, limited information is available regarding the effects of HF on genetic markers of synaptic plasticity in brain areas critical for memory and mood regulation. We induced HF in male rats and tested mood and anxiety (sucrose preference and elevated plus maze) and memory (spontaneous alternation and inhibitory avoidance) and measured the simultaneous expression of 84 synaptic plasticity-associated genes in dorsal (DH) and ventral hippocampus (VH), basolateral (BLA) and central amygdala (CeA) and prefrontal cortex (PFC). We also included the hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus (PVN), which is implicated in neurohumoral activation in HF. Our results show that rats with severely reduced ejection fraction recapitulate behavioral symptoms seen in patients with chronic HF including, increased anxiety and impaired memory in both tasks. HF also downregulated several synaptic-plasticity genes in PFC and PVN, moderate decreases in DH and CeA and minimal effects in BLA and VH. Collectively, these findings identify candidate brain areas and molecular mechanisms underlying HF-induced disturbances in mood and memory.
Keywords: amygdala, hippocampus, paraventricular nucleus, synaptic plasticity, behavior
INTRODUCTION
Chronic heart failure (HF) is a serious cardiovascular disorder that afflicts more than 26 million patients worldwide [1]. In the United States, it is estimated that 6.2 million people suffer from HF and the prevalence is projected to increase by 46% by 2030, more than doubling the cost of health care from $30.7 to $68.8 billion [2]. Once established, HF morbidity, prognosis and mortality are largely influenced by the degree of neurohumoral activation, which is a pathophysiological mechanism that involves increased centrally-driven sympathetic activity and elevated circulating levels of neurohormones [3–5]. It is now becoming well-accepted that, in addition to neurohumoral activation and the associated systemic cardiac, renal and vascular complications, HF is comorbid with depression, anxiety and memory deficits, both in human patients [6–10] and experimental animal models [11, 12]. These mood and cognitive disturbances have serious implications for quality of life and self-care in patients who have HF because hospital readmission and mortality rates are significantly higher in HF patients that have depression and/or cognitive impairments compared to HF patients without these comorbidities [9, 13–15].
It is critical to identify the neurobiological mechanisms and substrates that underlie HF-induced mood and cognitive disturbances HF in order to identify treatments that may improve quality of life and decrease mortality in this prevalent disease. In HF patients, neurocognitive impairments, including learning and working memory deficits, delayed recall, depression, and anxiety are correlated with significant cerebral grey matter loss and damage in brain areas relevant to cognitive function, including hippocampus, amygdala and prefrontal cortex (PFC) [16–20]. However, the mechanisms through which HF produces mood and cognitive impairments are not known. This question is difficult to address in human patients due to the influence of genetic, environmental and psychosocial variables that cannot be controlled. As a result, it is critical to develop valid animal models of mood and cognitive disturbances in HF in order to begin to identify underlying mechanisms. Despite evidence that cognitive performance is worse in HF patients with reduced ejection fraction than in HF patients with preserved cardiac function [21, 22], there are limited studies that have assessed the effects of severely-reduced ejection fraction (≤ 40%) on cognition in non-human animal models. Moreover, very limited information is available regarding the effects of HF on genetic and molecular markers of synaptic plasticity in brain areas critical for memory and mood regulation. This information is necessary to begin to understand the mechanisms through which HF impairs mood and memory. As a result, in the present study we aimed to investigate the effects of HF that is characterized by severe reductions in ejection fraction on behavioral measures of mood and memory that are known to be dependent on the hippocampus, amygdala and PFC. To this end, we induced HF using a well-established ischemic HF rat model [23–26] and then tested the rats in a battery of behavioral tests to assess anhedonia and anxiety (sucrose preference test [SPT] and elevated plus maze [EPM], respectively) as well as spatial working memory (spontaneous alternation; SA) and emotional long-term memory (one-trial inhibitory avoidance; IA). Moreover, we used an unbiased quantitative real time polymerase reaction (qRT-PCR) approach to identify the effects of HF on the simultaneous expression of 84 genes critical for synaptic plasticity in brain areas critical for mood and memory, including dorsal and ventral hippocampus (DH and VH, respectively), basolateral and central amygdala (BLA and CeA, respectively), PFC and PVN.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Subjects
Male Wistar rats (5 wks-old, 180–200 g) were purchased from Envigo Laboratories (Indianapolis, IN) and housed in Optirat cages (Animal Care Systems, Centennial, CO, USA) on a 12 h light-dark cycle (lights on at 8:00 am) with ad libitum access to pelleted food and water in their home cages unless otherwise stated. Rats were housed individually following surgery and during behavioral testing. All procedures were performed in compliance with the NIH guidelines for the care of laboratory animals and approved by the Georgia State University Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee.
Induction of heart failure (HF)
HF was induced by coronary artery ligation as described previously [23–25]. Briefly, animals (4 days after arrival) were anesthetized with isoflurane (2–4%) and intubated for mechanical ventilation. A left thoracotomy was performed and the heart exteriorized. The ligation was placed on the main diagonal branch of the left anterior descending coronary artery. Buprenorphine SR-LAB (0.5 mg/kg sc; Zoo Pharm, Windsor, CO, USA) was given immediately after surgery to minimize postsurgical pain. In our hands, the mortality rate in this model is ~20% and 10–20% of rats than undergo this procedure develop mild or no functional HF (i.e., ejection fraction > 40%). Sham control animals underwent the same procedure with the exception that the coronary artery was not occluded. Transthoracic echocardiography (Vevo 3100 systems, Visual Sonics, Toronto, ON, Canada) was performed 4 weeks after surgery under light isoflurane (3–4 %) anesthesia. Previous studies have shown that the degree of HF following the myocardial infarction remains consistent over time for at least 12 weeks [27–29]. The measurements of the left ventricle internal diameter and its posterior and anterior walls in systole and diastole, and the ejection fraction and fractional shortening were obtained throughout the cardiac cycle from the short-axis motion imaging mode.
Behavioral measures of mood and memory
Behavioral testing was started 5–10 weeks after the induction of HF with the measures of mood in one cohort (Cohort 1) and memory in another (Cohort 2). All tests were selected because they assessed mood and cognition in a relatively minimally stressful manner and all behavioral testing and scoring were conducted by an experimenter blind to Sham/HF condition. Approximately 5 weeks after surgery, Cohort 1 was tested in SPT. SPT is a commonly used measure of anhedonia, which is a major symptom of depression [30]. SPT relies on the natural preference of rodents for sweet foods and solutions; a decreased preference for a sweet solution versus tap water is assumed to reflect depression-like behavior [31, 32]. SPT was assessed in home cages with continuous ad libitum access to standard chow. Rats were first habituated to sucrose (1% wt/vol) by removing their water and giving them access to two bottles that both contained 1% sucrose for 24 h. The next day they were adapted to the preference test by giving them access to one bottle containing 1% sucrose and one containing tap water for 18 h (2 pm to 8 am). For the preference test the following day, all fluids were removed for 12 h (8 am to 8 pm) and the rats were then given access to one bottle of tap water and one bottle of 1% sucrose for 12 h. To avoid biases in drinking behavior based on the location of the bottle (i.e., left versus right), the position of the sucrose and water bottles were counterbalanced across the adaptation and preference test days [33]. Sucrose preference was calculated by dividing the amount of sucrose ingested by the total amount of fluid consumed on the sucrose preference testing day. The experiment started with 12 Sham rats and 8 HF rats and then the data from four rats (Sham n = 3; HF n = 1) were excluded due to excessive leaking from bottles.
The 12 Sham rats and 8 HF rats in Cohort 1 were then tested in the elevated plus maze (EPM) approximately 10 days later. They were handled for 3 min/day for 2 days before the EPM test and all testing was conducted during the light phase between 8:00 am and 12:00 pm. The EPM is a well-established behavioral test of anxiety [34, 35] that relies on the natural tendency of rodents to prefer dark enclosed spaces and fear height and open spaces, and their innate motivation to explore novel environments [36, 37]. The plus-shaped stainless-steel apparatus was elevated 50 cm above the floor and composed of four arms (50 cm x 12 cm). Two arms were surrounded by a 40 cm high black acrylic wall (i.e., closed arms) and two arms did not have walls (i.e., open arms). The rats were placed in the central square (12 × 12 cm) facing the closed arm, their behavior was video-recorded for 5 min, and the video-recordings were analyzed by a trained observer. The apparatus was cleaned with 70% ethanol after each rat was tested. An arm entry was defined as entering an arm with all four paws. Measures of anxiety included the percentage of time spent in the open arms [(time spent in open arm/total time spent on the maze) x 100] and the percentage of entries into the open arms [(open arm entries/ total arm entries) x 100]. Risk assessment was also used as a measure of anxiety [38]. Specifically, anxious animals make more stretch attend attempts, which were operationally defined as when a rat stretched its body towards an open arm after having been motionless, and then retracted [38, 39].
IA and SA were assessed in Cohort 2 approximately 10 weeks after surgery. Due to a delay in securing IACUC approval for the IA task, we had more rats that participated in SA then IA and thus at the end we added a cohort in which we measured IA but not SA, resulting in 17 Sham rats and 13 HF rats for SA and 14 HF rats for IA. All rats were handled for 3 min/day for 2 days before SA and IA training, all testing was conducted during the light phase between 8:00 am and 12:00 pm, and the apparatuses were cleaned with 70% ethanol after each rat was tested. Rats were first assessed in SA, which is a test of spatial working memory [40, 41]. The underlying assumption is that rats must remember their visits to previous locations in order to successfully alternate between spatial locations. This is supported by the finding that removing extra-maze cues or increasing the interval between arm choices decreases alternation scores [40, 41]. To assess SA, eat rat was placed in a Y-maze composed of three equally spaced arms (120°; 60 cm x 17.5 cm). The floor of each arm was composed of stainless steel (3.5 cm wide) and the top (14 cm wide) was covered with black Plexiglas. All of the rats were placed in the same starting arm of the Y-maze and allowed to explore the maze freely for 8 min. An alternation was defined as entering three different arms consecutively, and a percent alternation score was computed by dividing the number of alternations made by the number of arms entered minus two and then multiplying the resulting quotient by 100. The number of arms entered was used as a measure of activity.
The rats were then given training in the one-trial IA task approximately 15 days after SA testing. IA is commonly used to measure emotional, long-term memory [42, 43]. Each rat was placed into a trough-shaped apparatus (91 × 15 × 20 × 6.4 cm) that was separated into two compartments by a retractable guillotine door. The dark compartment (60 cm long) had a metal floor through which shock could be delivered. A 15-W light was placed over the lighted compartment (31 cm long) and was the only source of illumination in the room. For training, each rat was placed into the lighted compartment facing away from the retractable door and the door was then opened after the rat turned toward the door or after 12 s had passed. When the rat entered the dark compartment with all four paws, it was given a footshock (0.5 mA, 1 s) and then removed from the apparatus and returned to its home cage. The current level was assessed regularly throughout the course of the experiment with a digital multi-meter (AstroAI AM33D). For the retention test 24 h later, each rat was placed in the lighted compartment, the retractable door was lowered, and the latency (s) to cross over to the dark compartment (maximum 600 s) was recorded and used as a measure of retention. Footshock was not delivered on the retention test. Compared to other emotional memory tasks that depend on the hippocampus, amygdala and PFC, such as classical fear conditioning and spatial water maze, IA is minimally stressful because rats are exposed to only one training trial and the footshock is very brief.
Unbiased qRT-PCR array approach to assess the impact of HF on 84 molecular markers of synaptic plasticity
The effect of HF on synaptic plasticity-associated gene expression was assessed in DH, VH, PFC, CeA and BLA because of their roles in mood and memory. Specifically, manipulations that impair DH or PFC function attenuate sucrose preference [44–46], which is consistent with evidence indicating that damage to these areas produces depression in humans [16, 17]. EPM is dependent on the integrity of CeA [47], BLA [48] and PFC [49, 50], which is congruent with a wealth of evidence indicating that these brain areas are involved in regulation of mood [51–55]. SA is a task that depends on the integrity of the DH [56, 57] and PFC [58, 59] and IA requires an intact DH [60–62], VH [63], BLA [64, 65], CeA [66], and PFC [63, 67, 68]. Approximately 10 weeks after surgery, a different group of rats (Sham n = 5; HF n = 5) were anesthetized deeply with pentobarbital (Euthasol, Virbac, ANADA #200–071, Fort Worth, TX, USA, 390 mg/kg, i.p.), decapitated quickly with a guillotine, and their brains dissected, frozen in 2-methylbutane (Fisher Scientific, USA) at −40°C and then stored at −80°C. To collect micropunches, brains were allowed to reach −20°C and then mounted in a cryostat (CM3050S; Leica Microsystems, Wetzlar, Germany) and brain slices (250 μm) were obtained using the following stereotaxic coordinates from the Paxinos and Watson atlas (infralimbic and prelimbic PFC, 3.7 to 2.2 mm; PVN, −1.4 to −2.12 mm; BLA and CeA, −1.8 to −3.14 mm; DH, −2.8 to −4.16 mm and VH, −4.8 to −6.04 mm from bregma) [69]. A 2.0 mm stainless-steel punch was used to collect punches from the PFC (n=4 midline punches/rat obtained from 4 different sections), whereas a 1.0 mm stainless-steel punch was used to collect punches from the PVN (n=3 midline punches/rat obtained from 3 different sections), DH (n=16 punches/rat obtained bilaterally from 4 different slices), VH (n=16 punches/rat obtained bilaterally from 4 different slices), BLA (n=10 punches/rat obtained bilaterally from 5 different slices) and CeA (n=10 punches/rat obtained bilaterally from 5 different slices) (see Figure 6). For each brain region, tissue from rats was pooled together for each treatment condition and kept on dry ice until RNA extraction. RNA extraction and isolation were performed using the miRNAeasy Mini kit (Qiagen, Hilden, Germany) and the QIAzol Lysis Reagent (Qiagen) following manufacturers’ protocols. The pooled RNA concentration was measured using NanoDrop One (Thermo Scientific, Waltham, Massachusetts, USA) and was in the range of 115 − 220 ng/μL prior to cDNA synthesis. cDNA synthesis was performed using the RT2 First Strand Kit (Cat. No. 330401, Qiagen) and the SimpliAmp Thermal Cycler (Applied Biosystems, Thermo Fisher Scientific) following the manufacturers’ protocols. The synthesized cDNA from the six brain areas and two treatment conditions was run in three independent gene array reactions using the RT2 Profiler™ Rat Synaptic Plasticity PCR Array (Cat. No. 330231, Qiagen; i.e., 36 arrays in total) on a Roche Lightcycler 96 (Roche Life Science, Indianapolis, IN, USA). This kit measures the expression of 84 genes critical for synaptic alterations implicated in memory and mood, including immediate early genes (IEG)s, genes implicated in long-term potentiation (LTP) and long-term depression (LTD), cell adhesion and extracellular matrix molecules, CREB cofactors, and neuronal receptors (plus five housekeeping genes and two negative controls).
Figure 6. Anatomical location of micro-punches used for gene expression studies.
Schematic representation displaying the location of the punches in the PFC, PVN, BLA, CeA, DH and VH performed for the tissue sample collection for the array and qRT-PCR. Brain coronal sections were obtained from Swanson, L. W. (2004) Brain maps: structure of the rat brain, 3rd edition [143]. The grids represent 1 mm in each vertical and horizontal orientation.
Validation of gene array results
To validate the array findings, qRT-PCR was conducted using specific 10x QuantiTect primers diluted in 1.1 mL TE pH 8.0 (Qiagen). The same cDNA from for the array was used for the validation. The genes that were analyzed were Adcy1 (QT00386421), Arc (QT00373086), Homer1 (QT00391111), Mapk1 (QT00190379), and Nr4a1 (QT00181048). B2m (QT00176295) was used as the reference gene. These genes were selected based on their common involvement in synaptic plasticity, and due to the fact than they showed variable responses in the array study (i.e., widespread or selective changes across brain regions, and up- or down-regulated). All individual qRT-PCR reactions (brain region, primer and condition) were performed in triplicate.
Statistical analyses
Statistical analyses were performed using IBM SPSS Statistics 25 (IBM, Armonk, New York) and GraphPad Prism 7 (GraphPad Software, San Diego, CA). Data points that were two standard deviations from the mean were considered outliers and excluded from further analyses. This resulted in the removal of two retention latency data points from HF rats and consequently the training latencies from these rats were also excluded. Outlier detection programs in Prism and SPSS corroborated that these two data points were outliers and no other data points were identified as outliers. Given that we predicted a priori that HF surgery would impair heart function, mood and memory, unpaired one-tailed t-tests were used to assess differences between Sham and HF rats in the echocardiographic parameters and behavioral tests. Behavioral data are expressed as means ± standard error of the mean (SEM), and an alpha level of 0.05 was used as the criterion for statistical significance.
We used the Multi Ontology Enrichment Tool (MOET) to explore functional categories of genes that were altered in HF within specific brain regions. MOET provides an unbiased analysis of groups of genes within each brain region that are functionally related, co-occur and are statistically enriched in a given gene population. To identify functional classes of genes that are statistically over-represented amongst the genes that are differentially expressed during HF, MOET uses the Gene Ontology (GO) consortium, which provides detailed annotations of genes with respect to their known or putative molecular function, cellular localization and biological processes [70]. MOET provides a quantitative measurement of enrichment based on well accepted statistical methods including hypergeometric distributions to calculate p-values and correct the p-values with Holm-Bonferroni corrections and presents the number of genes involved in different annotations and the corresponding significance level in disease, pathway, biological, cellular and molecular ontologies. Four numbers are used to calculate each p-value
where n is the number of genes in our list, N is the number of genes in the reference population, k is the number of genes annotated with this item in our list and M is the number of genes annotated with items in the reference population. The p-value was considered significant when p<0.01.
RESULTS
HF rats had severely reduced ejection fraction
Figure 1 shows representative echocardiographic images from a Sham and HF rat and the mean heart function values obtained are shown in Table 1. Compared to Sham, HF rats showed a significant increase in the left ventricle diameter and volume during systole and diastole (p<0.0001) and a significant reduction in the left ventricle posterior and anterior thickness during systole (p<0.0001). Also, the HF rats exhibited a significant reduction in the percentage of fractional shortening and ejection fraction (p<0.0001), but no significant difference in heart rate (HR, p>0.05).
Figure 1. HF rats displayed severely reduced left ventricular function.
Representative echocardiograph images of the movement-mode, short-axis view from a Sham (a) and a Heart Failure (HF) rat (B). AW, anterior wall; PW, posterior wall; LV, left ventricle; LVID, d/s: left ventricle internal diameter in diastole and systole. Images were taken 4 weeks after the myocardial infarction surgery in 9 week-old rats.
Table 1.
Summary data of echocardiography measurements of left ventricular parameters obtained from Sham and HF rats.
Sham |
HF |
|
---|---|---|
LVID,d (mm) | 7.98 ± 0.18 | 10.68 ± 0.20**** |
LVPW,d (mm) | 2.13 ± 0.11 | 2.02 ± 0.17 |
LVAW,d (mm) | 1.97 ± 0.05 | 2.02 ± 0.12 |
LV Vol,d (μL) | 347.34 ± 17.05 | 659.89 ± 27.97**** |
LVID,s (mm) | 3.84 ± 0.17 | 9.19 ± 0.21**** |
LVPW,s (mm) | 3.41 ± 0.13 | 2.18 ± 0.16**** |
LVAW,s (mm) | 3.49 ± 0.08 | 2.62 ± 0.15**** |
LV Vol,s (μL) | 68.35 ± 6.69 | 476.95 ± 24.40**** |
EF % | 81.02 ± 1.39 | 28.08 ± 1.55**** |
FS % | 52.19 ± 1.50 | 14.08 ± 0.84**** |
HR (bpm) | 288.88 ± 7.45 | 276.87 ± 6.07 |
Values (collected 4 weeks after the myocardial infarction surgery) are expressed as means ± SEM; Sham n = 34; HF n = 27. LVID, d and s: left ventricle internal dimension during diastole and systole; LVAW, d and s: left ventricle anterior wall thickness during diastole and systole; LVPW, d and s: left ventricle posterior wall thickness during diastole and systole; LV Vol, d and s: left ventricle volume during diastole and systole; EF, ejection fraction; FS, fractional shortening; HR (bpm), heart rate (beats per minute).
p<0.0001 vs. Sham, unpaired t-test.
HF rats displayed signs of increased anxiety
Our results show that HF rats did not have a statistically significant lower preference ratio for sucrose (Sham: 85.32±3.70% vs HF: 67.02±9.16%, p = 0.05 vs Sham; Fig. 2A) than Sham rats on the preference test and did not differ in the total amount of liquid consumed (Sham: 55.99±12.33g vs HF: 68.57±11.08g, p > 0.18 vs Sham; Figure 2B). HF rats displayed signs of anxiety in the EPM test. Specifically, HF rats spent less per cent time in the open arms of the maze (Sham: 11.13±2.47% vs HF: 5.33±1.56%, p = 0.048 vs Sham; Fig. 3A), made a comparable per cent number of entries into the open arms (Sham: 23.02±3.97% vs HF: 13.70±3.42%, p = 0.057 vs Sham; Fig. 3B) and made more stretch attend attempts than Sham rats (Sham: 3.00±0.45% vs HF: 4.00±0.51%, p = 0.041 vs Sham; Fig. 3C). HF rats entered fewer total arms (i.e. open and closed arms) than SHAM rats (Sham: 14±0.827 vs HF: 11±1.264, p = 0.013).
Figure 2. HF did not produce anhedonia.
(A) In the sucrose preference test, HF rats (n=7) did not have a significantly decreased preference for sucrose on the preference testing day relative to water, (B) and the two groups did not differ in the total amount of liquids consumed. P≥0.05 vs. Sham, unpaired t-tests.
Figure 3. HF increases anxiety and risk assessment.
(A) In the EPM, HF rats (n=8) spent significantly less per cent time in the open arms, (B) did not make fewer per cent entries into the open arms and (C) made more stretch attend attempts compared to Sham rats (n=12). *p<0.05 vs. Sham, unpaired t-tests.
HF rats had impaired spatial working memory and emotional, long-term memory
In the SA task, HF rats made fewer alternations between arms (Sham: 64.53±2.54% vs HF: 55.35±3.97%, p = 0.026 vs Sham; Fig. 4A), indicative of impaired spatial working memory. Although HF significantly disrupted the sequence of arm entries (i.e., percent alternation), it did not affect the number of arms the rats explored in the maze (Sham: 17.00±0.87 entries vs HF: 17.00±0.78 entries, p= 0.4 vs Sham; Fig. 4B), suggesting that impaired alternation was not due to changes in activity levels.
Figure 4. HF impairs spatial working memory without affecting activity levels.
(A) In the SA test, HF rats (n=13) made significantly fewer alternations between arms compared to Sham rats (n=17), but (B) entered a comparable number of arms. *p<0.05 vs. Sham, unpaired t-tests.
On the IA retention test, HF rats displayed significantly shorter latencies to enter the compartment in which they had previously received shock than Sham rats (Sham: 189.60±58.03% vs HF: 65.41±10.52%, p = 0.025 vs Sham; Fig. 5A), a result that is indicative of impaired emotional, long-term memory. Of note, rats with HF did not have shorter latencies than Sham rats on the training day (Sham: 6.58±0.89 s vs HF: 6.05±1.17 s, p>0.05 vs Sham; Fig. 5B), suggesting that the HF-induced decreases in the retention latencies were not due to hyperactivity.
Figure 5. HF impairs emotional, long-term memory.
(A) In the IA task, HF rats (n=12) entered the dark/shock compartment more quickly than Sham rats (n=17) on the retention test, (B) but their latencies did not differ during training. *p<0.05 vs. Sham, unpaired t-tests.
HF downregulates the expression of several genes involved in synaptic plasticity in the PFC, PVN, CeA and DH
To begin to identify potential molecular mechanisms underlying the HF-induced changes in mood and memory that we observed, we determined whether HF impacted expression of genes involved in synaptic plasticity in brain regions known to be critical neuronal substrates for mood and memory (i.e., PFC, DH, VH, BLA, and CeA). We also included the PVN, a region that is well-established to play a major role in neurohumoral activation in this disease state, in which altered synaptic function and plasticity were described previously [26, 71–74]. Figure 6 shows the size and specific location in the brain where the punches were obtained.
Given the extensive number of genes sampled in the array, and in order to prioritize for further validation and interpretation those genes that were more robustly affected, we set a qualitative and arbitrary threshold of a down- (≤ 40% change) or upregulation (≥ 40% change) in expression. Using this criterion, we generated a heatmap graph (Fig. 7A) that exhibits the profile of genes whose expression were robustly upregulated (≥ 40% change, in blue range color), downregulated (≤ 40% change, in red range color), or showed no changes (white rectangles) in expression when comparing HF to sham rats across the six different brain regions. Figure 7B provides a summary of the main changes observed by brain region. Overall, the results showed that HF downregulated several synaptic plasticity-related genes, with ~ 87% of genes showing a downregulation (≤ 40% in expression) in at least one brain region. Conversely, increased expression (≥ 40% in expression) was observed only in ~5% of genes tested. The PFC and PVN exhibited the largest number of down regulated genes during HF, with ~80% and ~54% genes decreased, respectively. A milder change was observed in the DH and CeA, where HF downregulated ~20% of genes tested compared to Sham rats. Lastly, HF had minimal effects on synaptic plasticity genes in the BLA and VH, with less than 10% of genes being altered (Fig. 7B). Interestingly, the few genes that showed increases were mostly (3/4 genes, 75%) restricted to the VH.
Figure 7. HF significantly alters synaptic plasticity-related gene expression.
(A) Heatmap graph showing the level of expression of different genes regulated by HF in relation to sham rats’ gene expression. Downregulation below 40% change is shown in the red range, upregulation above 40% change is shown in the blue range and the expression of genes that did not change below or above 40% is shown in white. Adcy1, Arc, Homer1, Mapk1 and Nr4a1, the genes used for the qPCR validation, are in bold. (B) Bar graph showing the percentage of genes that were upregulated (blue), downregulated (red), or did not change (white) during HF in PFC, PVN, DH, CeA, BLA and VH. (C1-C4) Heatmap graphs depicting clusters of genes that were upregulated (blue), downregulated (red) or not affected (white) due to heart failure. Clusters are organized according to genes that changed with HF vs. Sham in four to five brain areas (C1); three brain areas (C2); two brain areas (C3) or only one brain area (C4). Categories written in: red represents Egr family; green represents Gria family; purple represents Grm family; orange represents kinases and phosphatases related genes.
HF downregulated (≤ 40% in expression) several important groups of genes, including IEGs (activity-regulated cytoskeletal -associated protein [Arc; aka Arg3.1], the Egr family of zinc fingered transcription factors, and Jun), genes encoding for glutamate receptors, predominantly metabotropic receptors (Grm1, −2, −3, −4, −5, −7), but also ionotropic NMDA receptor subunits (Grin2a and Grin2b). HF also downregulated (≤ 40% in expression) various types of protein phosphatases (Ppp1cc and Ppp2ca) and protein kinases (Prkca, Prkcg and Prkg1) in the PFC and PVN. Given the large number of brain regions sampled coupled with the large number of genes present on the synaptic plasticity array, we clustered genes that exhibited HF-induced changes in gene expression by the number of brain regions in which those changes were observed. The rationale for this clustering was to highlight synaptic plasticity genes that were under coordinate regulation across brain regions implicated in mood, memory, and neurohumoral activation. Figure 7C depicts genes grouped according to the number of brain areas in which they were impacted, with genes changing in four to five brain areas grouped in Cluster 1 (C1), three in Cluster 2 (C2), two in Cluster 3 (C3) and only one brain area in Cluster 4 (C4). These clusters also potentially indicate synaptic plasticity machinery that operate in concert within, and across, different brain regions, or exhibit brain region-specific changes. The genes whose expression changed in most of the brain regions tested during HF included Arc, Adcy1 (type 1 adenylyl cyclase), Grm2 and Grm5, and the proto-oncogene serine/threonine-protein kinase Pim-1 (Pim1; see C1). Interestingly, another gene directly associated to metabotropic glutamate receptor (mGluR) function and synaptic plasticity, Homer1 (Fig. 7A, 7C2), was also downregulated (≤ 40% in expression) during HF in the PFC, PVN and CeA. Cyclic adenosine monophosphate (cAMP) responsive element binding protein 1 gene (Creb1), a gene critically linked to vascular dementia, learning, memory and cognitive abilities [75–77], was also reduced by HF in the PFC, PVN and DH (Fig. 7A, 7C2).
We then used the Multi Ontology Enrichment Tool (MOET), which provides an unbiased analysis of groups of genes within each brain region that are functionally related, co-occur and are statistically enriched in a given gene population. MOET uses the Gene Ontology (GO) consortium, in order to identify functional classes of genes that are statistically over-represented amongst the genes that are differentially expressed during HF [70]. For this analysis, we focused on the PFC, DH and CeA, which are the three brain regions that showed the most significant changes in gene expression and are implicated in the measures of mood and memory used in our experiments (see Fig. 8; results from the PVN and VH are shown in Supplementary Figure 1. Note that MOET was not applied to BLA given that only 1 gene changed in this region). We highlight two major groups of GOs related to (1) biological, cellular and/or molecular pathways/functions (left graphs, 8A, C and E) and (2) disease-related processes (right graphs, 8B, D and F). For simplicity and relevance, only the GO subcategories in which the gene counts represented 50% or more of the total genes associated with that GO that were down- or upregulated during HF, and that were considered statistically significant by MOET, are displayed.
Figure 8. Quantitative ontology analysis of gene expression in the PFC, DH and CeA of HF rats.
(A,C,E), Summary bar graphs showing the analysis of biological, cellular or molecular gene ontologies (GOs) terms that are significantly enriched based on the number of genes that were up- or downregulated in HF rats vs. Sham in PFC (A), DH (C) and CeA (E). (B,D,F) Summary bar graphs showing the analysis of disease ontology terms highlighting important disease categories that involve the up- or downregulated genes by HF in PFC (B), DH (D) and CeA (F). Categories written in red represent those enriched terms that show up in PFC, DH and CeA; green represents those that show up in PFC and DH; blue represents those that show up in PFC and CeA; and orange represents those that show up in DH and CeA. Hypergeometric distributions with Holm-Bonferroni corrections. All the categories included in the graphs reached statistical significance p<0.00001.
For the first major group of GOs (Fig. 8A, C and E), the MOET analysis revealed that multiple genes that were robustly downregulated during HF are involved in cell communication, cell-cell signaling, cellular response to stimulus and synaptic structure/function/modulation, particularly for glutamatergic synapses (Fig. 8A and C, written in green). In addition, there was a notable involvement of genes involved in cellular metabolic processes and intracellular organelles. Interestingly, an involvement for genes related to learning and memory and cognition was observed to occur only in the DH (Fig. 8C). For the GOs related to disease processes (Fig. 8B, D and F), there was an enriched involvement of multiple genes across the three brain regions, which are associated with CNs diseases, diseases of mental health, as well as cognitive disorders. Importantly, the three regions also displayed involvement of genes involved in cardiovascular/vascular diseases. Finally, and perhaps somewhat surprising, an involvement of genes related to cancer/neoplasms was also observed.
In order to independently validate our PCR array data, we performed qRT-PCR using individual primers for five genes, Adcy1, Arc, Homer1, Mapk1 and Nr4a1, with B2m as the housekeeping gene (see Fig. 9 and Table 2). In all cases, the qRT-PCR replicated the direction of the PCR array results (i.e., increase, decrease or no effects), although the magnitude did not match precisely. For example, the results with the individual primers showed that HF significantly downregulated Adcy1 in the PFC (−1.295±0.008), PVN (−1.541±0.022), CeA (−1.499±0.038), DH (−1.481±0.019), and BLA (−1.125±0.020), but upregulated it in the VH (1.523±0.028), (Fig. 9A), which is the same pattern that we observed in the PCR array (Fig. 9B). A similar profile of results was observed for the individual primers and array for Arc, Homer1, Mapk1 and Nr4a1 (Fig. 9A and B).
Figure 9. qRT-PCR validation of results obtained in the gene array study.
(A), Summary bar graphs showing the mean fold change in mRNA levels for Adcy1, Arc, Homer1, Mapk1 and Nr4a1 genes in HF rats in relation to Sham rats (theoretical mean = +/−1) in the PFC (black bars), PVN (red bars), CeA (blue bars), BLA (green bars), DH (orange bars) and VH (purple bars). Dashed lines indicate the theoretical means. (B), heatmap graph showing the magnitude of change in gene expression in the array and validation shown as fold change in gene expression from −2.39 to +2.39 induced by HF in relation to sham rats’ gene expression. *p<0.05, **p<0.01 and ***p<0.001 vs. theoretical mean, one sample paired t-test.
Table 2.
Summary statistical data for the qRT-PCR validation study.
Brain Region | Gene | Mean | SEM | P value (two tailed) |
---|---|---|---|---|
Adcy1 | −1.295 | 0.008 | 0.0007 | |
Arc | −1.448 | 0.022 | 0.0024 | |
PFC | Homer1 | −1.074 | 0.009 | 0.0141 |
Mapk1 | −1.067 | 0.004 | 0.0041 | |
Nr4a1 | −1.775 | 0.065 | 0.0070 | |
Adcy1 | −1.541 | 0.023 | 0.0018 | |
Arc | −1.237 | 0.010 | 0.0018 | |
PVN | Homer1 | −1.143 | 0.009 | 0.0043 |
Mapk1 | −1.057 | 0.007 | 0.0129 | |
Nr4a1 | −2.125 | 0.020 | 0.0003 | |
Adcy1 | −1.499 | 0.038 | 0.0057 | |
Arc | −1.956 | 0.040 | 0.0017 | |
CeA | Homer1 | −1.437 | 0.017 | 0.0016 |
Mapk1 | −1.107 | 0.005 | 0.0025 | |
Nr4a1 | −2.296 | 0.092 | 0.0051 | |
Adcy1 | −1.125 | 0.020 | 0.0253 | |
Arc | −1.357 | 0.029 | 0.0065 | |
BLA | Homer1 | 1.030 | 0.012 | 0.1217 |
Mapk1 | 1.147 | 0.012 | 0.0066 | |
Nr4a1 | −1.281 | 0.064 | 0.0484 | |
Adcy1 | −1.481 | 0.019 | 0.0016 | |
Arc | −1.370 | 0.031 | 0.0069 | |
DH | Homer1 | −1.192 | 0.019 | 0.0100 |
Mapk1 | −1.127 | 0.003 | 0.0004 | |
NAa1 | −1.332 | 0.014 | 0.0018 | |
Adcy1 | 1.523 | 0.028 | 0.0029 | |
Arc | −1.220 | 0.028 | 0.0162 | |
VH | Homer1 | −1.038 | 0.024 | 0.2484 |
Mapk1 | −1.081 | 0.072 | 0.3772 | |
Nr4a1 | −1.409 | 0.026 | 0.0040 |
Summary mean fold change (in relation to Sham rats; theoretical mean = +/−1) in mRNA levels for Adcy1, Arc, Homer1, Mapk1 and Nr4a1 genes in HF rats in the PFC, PVN, CeA, BLA, DH and VH.The mean, SEM and p values corresponding to each of the five genes validated in each of the six brain regions are provided. Brains from five rats were pooled per treatment condition and three independent replicates were performed.
DISCUSSION
Collectively, the present findings show that a rat model of HF characterized by severely reduced ejection fraction recapitulates behavioral symptoms seen in patients with chronic HF, including disruption of several behavioral measures of mood and memory. Moreover, we found that this rat model of HF altered the pattern of synaptic plasticity-associated gene expression in brain areas critical for mood and memory. Specifically, we found that HF increased measures of anxiety in the EPM and impaired SA and IA retention performance, the latter two findings indicating that HF disrupts spatial working memory and emotional, long-term memory, respectively. In order to gain insights into possible molecular underpinnings of cognitive and mood disorders in HF, we conducted a PCR array study that assessed the simultaneous expression of 84 genes critical for synaptic plasticity. Importantly, our findings showed that HF downregulated numerous genes implicated in synaptic plasticity in a brain region-dependent manner, with the most profound changes observed in PVN and PFC, moderate changes in DH and CeA, and minimal alterations in VH and BLA.
To the best of our knowledge, the present results are the first to show that HF impairs SA and IA. The finding that HF affected the sequence of arms that the rats entered in the maze in the SA task without affecting the number of arm entries suggests that the deficit reflects a spatial working memory deficit rather than an effect on another variable that might influence performance in the task, such as activity levels or motivation. Both the SA and IA memory tasks are dependent on the integrity of DH [13, 57, 60, 62] and PFC [58, 59, 63, 67], which are interconnected brain regions critical for memory. The gene array results point to altered synaptic plasticity in several brain areas critical for memory, namely DH, PFC and CeA. Collectively, the findings show that our animal model of HF impacts both working memory (i.e., SA) and long-term memory (i.e., IA), which is consistent with findings showing that rats with severely reduced ejection fraction have impaired long-term memory in the spatial water maze [78] and with results showing that HF patients have deficits in both immediate and delayed recall [21] and working memory [79]. Short-term memory deficits appear to be one of the most pervasive cognitive issues in HF, which is significant because these problems are associated with impaired ability for HF patients to manage their medical condition [21, 80].
The present findings are consistent with previous research showing that HF increases anxiety in the EPM [12, 81–84] but did not replicate the finding that HF decreases sucrose preference (p = .05). The present results extend the previous findings of increased anxiety by showing that these effects are observed in a rat model of HF characterized by severely reduced ejection fraction. It is interesting to note that HF rats entered less arms than Sham rats in the EPM but not in the y-maze used to assess SA. This decrease observed in the EPM may reflect increased anxiety in the presence of the open arms of the EPM whereas all of the arms in the y-maze are enclosed. The increased anxiety observed in HF rats was paralleled by changes in gene expression in several interconnected areas involved in anxiety, specifically the PFC, PVN and CeA, but not in other components of the anxiety circuit, namely VH [85–88]. Future studies may want to examine the impact of HF on other brain areas involved in anxiety, such as the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis and periaqueductal gray area.
Our findings show that HF downregulated multiple genes implicated in synaptic plasticity in a brain region-dependent manner. For instance, changes in genes involved in learning and memory were observed only in DH and not in CeA or PFC. This may reflect the different role of these brain areas in cognition. DH involvement in SA and IA is assumed to reflect an episodic (i.e., autobiographical) memory of where the animal has been (i.e., SA) and the context in which it received footshock (i.e., IA). In contrast, the amygdala’s role in IA may be more related to the emotional component of the task [89–91] and PFC may be necessary for SA and IA due to its critical role in executive function and decision-making [92]. Although spatial abilities and episodic memory appear to be the most compromised cognitive functions in HF [93], HF patients do also show impaired executive function [94–96].
The present results significantly advance our understanding of the consequences of HF by showing that HF characterized by a severe reduction in ejection fraction is associated with the downregulation of several genes in CeA and a relative sparing of BLA. To date, there is very limited evidence implicating the amygdala in any effects of HF in human patients or animal models. In patients, HF is associated with reduced blood flow to amygdala [18] and increased amygdala responses to sympathetic activation [97, 98]. In animal models, HF increases glial density (GFAP immunoreactivity) in the medial amygdala, which is critical for cardiovascular reflexes and central control of sympathetic responses [99].
Our findings show that HF impacts DH expression but has minimal effect on VH, which is consistent with extensive evidence indicating that the hippocampus is not a unitary structure and that there are clear distinctions between DH and VH. For example, DH and VH have different anatomical connections, cellular and circuit properties and patterns of gene expression that likely contribute to the different functions that they serve [100–105]. DH appears to be primarily responsible for the cognitive functions of the hippocampus [100, 103, 104, 106], which is consistent with our finding that that genes associated with learning and memory were only impacted in DH. Our findings also add to evidence suggesting that DH is more vulnerable to neuroinflammation and other pathology than VH [107–111].
Several critical families of genes were considerably affected in these brain regions in HF rats. These include various IEGs, such as Arc, Egr, Jun (but not Fos), which would suggest an overall reduction in neuronal activation in these areas, which could be a result of reduced inputs from upstream brain regions. Another major group of genes downregulated during HF were genes encoding glutamate receptors. Given the importance of glutamate receptor signaling in maintaining appropriate basal network activity levels, as well as a playing a critical role in both up- and downregulation of the strength of synaptic efficacy [112–114], the reported changes in GLURs may also contribute to lower levels of neuronal activation and compromised synaptic plasticity in HF rats. Other major gene families downregulated during HF included neurotrophic factors (e.g., BDNF, Ntrk2 and Ngf) and kinases and phosphatases (e.g., Ppp1cc, Ppp2ca, Prkca, Prkcg and Prkg1). We independently validated our PCR array data by performing qRT-PCR using individual primers for five genes, and future studies should determine whether these changes in gene expression are accompanied by concomitant changes in protein products.
The MOET analysis revealed that the majority of genes downregulated during HF were functionally related and are involved in cell communication, cellular response to stimulus, dendritic signaling, and synaptic structure/function/modulation, particularly for glutamatergic synapses. More importantly, MOET also revealed that multiple genes that changed across brain regions were associated with diseases of mental health, cognitive disorders as well as cardiovascular/vascular diseases. Several of the same genes that were impacted by HF have been linked to cognitive dysfunction and neurodegenerative diseases such as Alzheimer’s Disease [113–121].
Numerous putative mechanisms, not mutually exclusive, could contribute to the disruption of the molecular underpinnings of synaptic plasticity during HF. One obvious candidate is neuroinflammation, a hallmark pathophysiological phenomenon in HF [122]. Activation of neuroinflammatory pathways within the hypothalamic PVN has been directly implicated as an underlying mechanism contributing to sympathohumoral activation in this disease [123–125]. Importantly, findings in rodents have implicated neuroinflammation and the expression of inflammatory and signal transduction genes in HF-induced cognitive impairment, depression and anxiety-like behavior [11, 12, 27, 126]. This possibility is supported by evidence showing that HF impacts the expression of genes related to inflammation in the hippocampus and PFC of mice [126].
Another critical pathophysiological mechanism in HF is cerebral hypoperfusion associated with low cardiac output that chronically leads to brain ischemia and microinfarcts [79]. In fact, several clinical studies implicate compromised cerebral blood flow as a mechanism contributing to cognitive impairment and dementia in patients with HF [127–129]. Cerebral hypoperfusion has also been reported in rodent models of HF [130, 131]. It is noteworthy that hypoxia reduces the expression of genes that are required for the maintenance of synaptic structure and function [132], and several of the key genes found to be disrupted in HF in this study, including Arc, Egr and mGLURs were previously reported to be affected by hypoxia [133–140]. Of particular interest to this study, Arc expression in the hippocampus was shown to be significantly downregulated following ischemic conditions, both in vitro and in vivo [136]. Moreover, manipulations that increase Arc expression protected against hypoxia-related cognitive impairment and hippocampal damage [141]. Whether chronic brain hypoperfusion and/or neuroinflammation contribute to the disruption of molecular underpinnings of synaptic plasticity and cognitive and mood disorders in HF remains to be determined.
Collectively, the present findings provide support for the growing consensus that HF is not only a neurohumoral cardiovascular problem but is also a disorder of mood and memory. Our study shows that a rat model of HF characterized by severely reduced ejection fraction recapitulates behavioral symptoms seen in patients with HF, which is significant because impaired mood and cognition in HF patients is associated with higher hospital readmission and mortality rates [9, 13–15]. There are few studies of the impact of severely reduced ejection fraction on cognition and the present results appear to be the first to show that HF disrupts SA and IA, providing an animal model of the working memory and long-term memory deficits observed in HF patients. Our study provides the first comprehensive analysis of HF on synaptic plasticity-associated processes and suggests that pathological alterations in plasticity mechanisms in DH and PFC may contribute to mood and memory deficits, which is in agreement with the finding that learning and working memory deficits, depression, and anxiety are correlated with significant cerebral grey matter loss in hippocampus and PFC [16, 17]. Our findings advance studies conducted in humans by suggesting that the brain damage observed in patients with HF is likely accompanied by a decrease in the expression of many genes necessary for the molecular mechanisms involved in synaptic plasticity. Moreover, our findings suggest that structural damage and gene expression may not necessarily go hand in hand because HF impacts VH structure in rats [142], whereas our results show that HF has a negligible effect on synaptic plasticity-associated gene expression in this brain region. Our findings appear to be the first to suggest that the amygdala may also participate in the mood and memory deficits as HF downregulates several genes in CeA with a relative sparing of BLA. Finally, our findings also show that the majority of genes that were downregulated are involved in cell communication, cellular response to stimulus, dendritic signaling, and synaptic structure/function/modulation, particularly for glutamatergic synapse and are associated with diseases of mental health, cognitive disorders and cardiovascular/vascular diseases.
Supplementary Material
HIGHLIGHTS.
We induced heart failure (HF) in rats and tested mood, anxiety and memory function.
HF rats displayed signs of increased anxiety, and impaired memory.
Downregulation of synaptic plasticity genes was observed in related brain areas.
We identified candidate brain areas and molecular mechanisms related to mood and memory deficits in HF.
FUNDING
This work was supported by a National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute Grant NIH R01HL090948 (Stern JE), a National Institute of Diabetes and Digestive and Kidney Diseases Grant NIH R01DK114700 (Parent MB), a National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke Grant NIH R01NS115209 (Cox DN), a GSU Brains and Behavior seed grant (Stern JE and Parent MB), and a DFG Postdoc Fellowship AL 2466/1-1 (Althammer F).
ABBREVIATIONS
- Adcyl
Adenylate Cyclase 1
- Arc
Activity-regulated cytoskeleton-associated protein
- B2m
Beta-2-Microglobulin
- BLA
Basolateral Amygdala
- cDNA
complementary DNA
- CeA
Central Amygdala
- CNS
Central nervous system
- DH
Dorsal Hippocampus
- EPM
Elevated Plus Maze
- GO
Gene Ontology
- HF
Heart Failure
- Homerl
Homer Scaffold Protein 1
- IA
Inhibitory Avoidance
- LTD
Long-term depression
- LTP
Long-term potentiation
- Mapkl
Mitogen-activated protein kinase 1
- MOET
Multi Ontology Enrichment Tool
- Nr4a1
Nuclear Receptor Subfamily 4 Group A Member 1
- PFC
Prefrontal Cortex
- PVN
Paraventricular Nucleus
- qRT-PCR
quantitative Reverse Transcriptase - Polimerase Chain Reaction
- SA
Spontaneous Alternation
- SPT
Sucrose Preference Test
- VH
Ventral Hippocampus
Footnotes
ADDITIONAL INFORMATION
COMPETING INTERESTS
All authors acknowledge that they there are no conflict of interests to disclose in accordance with the Journal policy.
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an article that has undergone enhancements after acceptance, such as the addition of a cover page and metadata, and formatting for readability, but it is not yet the definitive version of record. This version will undergo additional copyediting, typesetting and review before it is published in its final form, but we are providing this version to give early visibility of the article. Please note that, during the production process, errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
REFERENCES
Uncategorized References
- [1].Ambrosy AP, Gheorghiade M, Chioncel O, Mentz RJ, Butler J, Global perspectives in hospitalized heart failure: regional and ethnic variation in patient characteristics, management, and outcomes, Curr Heart Fail Rep 11(4) (2014) 416–27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [2].Benjamin EJ, Muntner P, Alonso A, Bittencourt MS, Callaway CW, Carson AP, Chamberlain AM, Chang AR, Cheng S, Das SR, Delling FN, Djousse L, Elkind MSV, Ferguson JF, Fornage M, Jordan LC, Khan SS, Kissela BM, Knutson KL, Kwan TW, Lackland DT, Lewis TT, Lichtman JH, Longenecker CT, Loop MS, Lutsey PL, Martin SS, Matsushita K, Moran AE, Mussolino ME, O’Flaherty M, Pandey A, Perak AM, Rosamond WD, Roth GA, Sampson UKA, Satou GM, Schroeder EB, Shah SH, Spartano NL, Stokes A, Tirschwell DL, Tsao CW, Turakhia MP, VanWagner LB, Wilkins JT, Wong SS, Virani SS, E. American Heart Association Council on, C. Prevention Statistics, S. Stroke Statistics, Heart Disease and Stroke Statistics-2019 Update: A Report From the American Heart Association, Circulation 139(10) (2019) e56–e528. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [3].Mancia G, Sympathetic activation in congestive heart failure, Eur Heart J 11 Suppl A (1990) 3–11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [4].Packer M, Neurohormonal interactions and adaptations in congestive heart failure, Circulation 77(4) (1988) 721–30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [5].Zucker IH, Schultz HD, Li YF, Wang Y, Wang W, Patel KP, The origin of sympathetic outflow in heart failure: the roles of angiotensin II and nitric oxide, Prog Biophys Mol Biol 84(2–3) (2004) 217–32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [6].Cameron J, Pressler SJ, Ski CF. Thompson DR, Cognitive impairment in heart failure: towards a consensus on screening, Eur J Heart Fail 16(3) (2014) 235–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [7].Cannon JA, Moffitt P, Perez-Moreno AC, Walters MR, Broomfield NM, McMurray JJV, Quinn TJ, Cognitive Impairment and Heart Failure: Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis, J Card Fail 23(6) (2017) 464–475. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [8].Angermann CE, Frey A, Ertl G, Cognition matters in cardiovascular disease and heart failure, Eur Heart J 33(14) (2012) 1721–3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [9].Lossnitzer N, Herzog S W. Stork B. Wild, Muller-Tasch T, Lehmkuhl E, Zsugck C, Regitz-Zagrosek V, Pankuweit S, Maisch B, Ertl G, Gelbrich G, Angermann CE, F. Competence Network Heart, Incidence rates and predictors of major and minor depression in patients with heart failure, Int J Cardiol 167(2) (2013) 502–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [10].Rutledge T, Reis VA, Linke SE, Greenberg BH, Mills PJ, Depression in heart failure a meta-analytic review of prevalence, intervention effects, and associations with clinical outcomes, J Am Coll Cardiol 48(8) (2006) 1527–37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [11].Toledo C, Lucero C, Andrade DC, Diaz HS, Schwarz KG, Pereyra KV, Arce-Alvarez A, Lopez NA, Martinez M, Inestrosa NC, Del Rio R, Cognitive impairment in heart failure is associated with altered Wnt signaling in the hippocampus, Aging (Albany NY) 11(16) (2019) 5924–5942. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [12].Wang HW, Ahmad M, Jadayel R, Najjar F, Lagace D, Leenen FHH, Inhibition of inflammation by minocycline improves heart failure and depression-like behaviour in rats after myocardial infarction, PLoS One 14(6) (2019) e0217437. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [13].Dickson VV, Tkacs N, Riegel B, Cognitive influences on self-care decision making in persons with heart failure, Am Heart J 154(3) (2007) 424–31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [14].Lee CS, Gelow JM, Bidwell JT, Mudd JO, Green JK, Jurgens CY, Woodruff-Pak DS, Blunted responses to heart failure symptoms in adults with mild cognitive dysfunction, J Cardiovasc Nurs 28(6) (2013) 534–40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [15].Pressler SJ, Kim J, Riley P, Ronis DL, Gradus-Pizlo I, Memory dysfunction, psychomotor slowing, and decreased executive function predict mortality in patients with heart failure and low ejection fraction, J Card Fail 16(9) (2010) 750–60. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [16].O’Donnell M, Teo K, Gao P, Anderson C, Sleight P, Dans A, Marzona I, Bosch J, Probstfield J, Yusuf S, Cognitive impairment and risk of cardiovascular events and mortality, Eur Heart J 33(14) (2012) 1777–86. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [17].Almeida OP, Garrido GJ, Beer C, Lautenschlager NT, Arnolda L, Flicker L, Cognitive and brain changes associated with ischaemic heart disease and heart failure, Eur Heart J 33(14) (2012) 1769–76. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [18].Roy B, Woo MA, Wang DJJ, Fonarow GC, Harper RM, Kumar R, Reduced regional cerebral blood flow in patients with heart failure, Eur J Heart Fail 19(10) (2017) 1294–1302. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [19].Woo MA, Macey PM, Fonarow GC, Hamilton MA, Harper RM, Regional brain gray matter loss in heart failure, J Appl Physiol (1985) 95(2) (2003) 677–84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [20].Woo MA, Palomares JA, Macey PM, Fonarow GC, Harper RM, Kumar R, Global and regional brain mean diffusivity changes in patients with heart failure, J Neurosci Res 93(4) (2015) 678–85. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [21].Leto L, Feola M, Cognitive impairment in heart failure patients, J Geriatr Cardiol 11(4) (2014) 316–28. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [22].Jefferson AL, Himali JJ, Au R, Seshadri S, Decarli C, O’Donnell CJ, Wolf PA, Manning WJ, Beiser AS, Benjamin EJ, Relation of left ventricular ejection fraction to cognitive aging (from the Framingham Heart Study), Am J Cardiol 108(9) (2011) 1346–51. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [23].Biancardi VC, Son SJ, Sonner PM, Zheng H, Patel KP, Stern JE, Contribution of central nervous system endothelial nitric oxide synthase to neurohumoral activation in heart failure rats, Hypertension 58(3) (2011) 454–63. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [24].Ferreira-Neto HC, Biancardi VC, Stern JE, A reduction in SK channels contributes to increased activity of hypothalamic magnocellular neurons during heart failure, J Physiol 595(20) (2017) 6429–6442. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [25].Ferreira-Neto HC, Stern JE, Functional coupling between NMDA receptors and SK channels in rat hypothalamic magnocellular neurons: altered mechanisms during heart failure, J Physiol (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [26].Potapenko ES, Biancardi VC, Zhou Y, Stern JE, Astrocytes Modulate a Postsynaptic NMDA-GABAA-Receptor Crosstalk in Hypothalamic Neurosecretory Neurons, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 33(2) (2013) 631–40. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [27].Hay M, Vanderah TW, Samareh-Jahani F, Constantopoulos E, Uprety AR, Barnes CA, Konhilas J, Cognitive impairment in heart failure: A protective role for angiotensin-(1–7), Behav Neurosci 131(1) (2017) 99–114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [28].Francis J, Weiss RM, Wei SG, Johnson AK, Felder RB, Progression of heart failure after myocardial infarction in the rat, Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 281(5) (2001) R1734–45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [29].Pons S, Fornes P, Hagege AA, Heudes D, Giudicelli JF, Richer C, Survival, haemodynamics and cardiac remodelling follow up in mice after myocardial infarction, Clin Exp Pharmacol Physiol 30(1–2) (2003) 25–31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [30].Willner P, Towell A, Sampson D, Sophokleous S, Muscat R, Reduction of sucrose preference by chronic unpredictable mild stress, and its restoration by a tricyclic antidepressant, Psychopharmacology (Berl) 93(3) (1987) 358–64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [31].Krishnan V, Nestler EJ, Animal models of depression: molecular perspectives, Curr Top Behav Neurosci 7 (2011) 121–47. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [32].Liu MY, Yin CY, Zhu LJ, Zhu XH, Xu C, Luo CX, Chen H, Zhu DY, Zhou QG, Sucrose preference test for measurement of stress-induced anhedonia in mice, Nat Protoc 13(7) (2018) 1686–1698. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [33].Strekalova T, Spanagel R, Bartsch D, Henn FA, Gass P, Stress-induced anhedonia in mice is associated with deficits in forced swimming and exploration, Neuropsychopharmacology 29(11) (2004) 2007–17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [34].Pellow S, Chopin P, File SE, Briley M, Validation of open:closed arm entries in an elevated plus-maze as a measure of anxiety in the rat, J Neurosci Methods 14(3) (1985) 149–67. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [35].Kraeuter AK, Guest PC, Sarnyai Z, The Elevated Plus Maze Test for Measuring Anxiety-Like Behavior in Rodents, Methods Mol Biol 1916 (2019) 69–74. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [36].Montgomery KC, The relation between fear induced by novel stimulation and exploratory behavior, J Comp Physiol Psychol 48(4) (1955) 254–60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [37].Handley SL, Mithani S, Effects of alpha adrenoceptor agonists and antagonists in a maze-exploration model of ‘fear’-motivated behaviour, Naunyn Schmiedebergs Arch Pharmacol 327(1) (1984) 1–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [38].Holly KS, Orndorff CO, Murray TA, MATSAP: An automated analysis of stretch-attend posture in rodent behavioral experiments, Sci Rep 6 (2016) 31286. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [39].Mikics E, Barsy B, Barsvari B, Haller J, Behavioral specificity of non-genomic glucocorticoid effects in rats: effects on risk assessment in the elevated plus-maze and the open-field, Horm Behav 48(2) (2005) 152–62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [40].Lalonde R, The neurobiological basis of spontaneous alternation, Neurosci Biobehav Rev 26(1) (2002) 91–104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [41].Richman CL, Dember WN & Kim P, Spontaneous alternation behavior in animals: A review., Current Psychology 5 (1986) 358–391. [Google Scholar]
- [42].Jarvik ME, Kopp R, An improved one-trial passive avoidance learning situation, Psychol Rep 21(1) (167) 221–4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [43].Vazdarjanova A, Passive (Inhibitory) Avoidance, Fear Learning 2nbd Edition ed., Macmillian Reference USA, FArmington Hill, MI, 2002. [Google Scholar]
- [44].Kabir ZD, Lee AS, Burgdorf CE, Fischer DK, Rajadhyaksha AM, Mok E, Rizzo B, Rice RC, Singh K, Ota KT, Gerhard DM, Schierberl KC, Glass MJ, Duman RS, Rajadhyaksha AM, Cacna1c in the Prefrontal Cortex Regulates Depression-Related Behaviors via REDD1, Neuropsychopharmacology 42(10) (2017) 2032–2042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [45].Liu KC, Li JY, Tan HH, Du CX, Xie W, Zhang YM, Ma WL, Zhang L, Serotonin(6) receptors in the dorsal hippocampus regulate depressive-like behaviors in unilateral 6-hydroxydopamine-lesioned Parkinson’s rats, Neuropharmacology 95 (2015) 290–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [46].Banasr M, Duman RS, Glial loss in the prefrontal cortex is sufficient to induce depressive-like behaviors, Biol Psychiatry 64(10) (2008) 863–70. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [47].Ventura-Silva AP, Melo A, Ferreira AC, Carvalho MM, Campos FL, Sousa N, Pego JM, Excitotoxic lesions in the central nucleus of the amygdala attenuate stress-induced anxiety behavior, Front Behav Neurosci 7 (2013) 32. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [48].Yin F, Guo H, Cui J, Shi Y, Su R, Xie Q, Chang J, Wang Y, Lai J, The basolateral amygdala regulation of complex cognitive behaviours in the five-choice serial reaction time task, Psychopharmacology (Berl) 236(11) (2019) 3135–3146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [49].Lisboa SF, Stecchini MF, Correa FM, Guimaraes FS, Resstel LB, Different role of the ventral medial prefrontal cortex on modulation of innate and associative learned fear, Neuroscience 171(3) (2010) 760–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [50].de Visser L, Baars AM, van ‘t Klooster J, van den Bos R, Transient inactivation of the medial prefrontal cortex affects both anxiety and decision-making in male wistar rats, Front Neurosci 5 (2011) 102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [51].Gallagher M, Chiba AA, The amygdala and emotion, Curr Opin Neurobiol 6(2) (1996) 221–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [52].Prager EM, Bergstrom HC, Wynn GH, Braga MF, The basolateral amygdala gamma-aminobutyric acidergic system in health and disease, J Neurosci Res 94(6) (2016) 548–67. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [53].Price JL, Drevets WC, Neurocircuitry of mood disorders, Neuropsychopharmacology 35(1) (2010) 192–216. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [54].Dixon ML, Thiruchselvam R, Todd R, Christoff K, Emotion and the prefrontal cortex: An integrative review, Psychol Bull 143(10) (2017) 1033–1081. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [55].Teffer K, Semendeferi K, Human prefrontal cortex: evolution, development, and pathology, Prog Brain Res 195 (2012) 191–218. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [56].Dillon GM, Qu X, Marcus JN, Dodart JC, Excitotoxic lesions restricted to the dorsal CA1 field of the hippocampus impair spatial memory and extinction learning in C57BL/6 mice, Neurobiol Learn Mem 90(2) (2008) 426–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [57].Sakaguchi Y, Sakurai Y, Left-right functional difference of the rat dorsal hippocampus for short-term memory and long-term memory, Behav Brain Res 382 (2020) 112478. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [58].Woloszynowska-Fraser MU, Wulff P, Riedel G, Parvalbumin-containing GABA cells and schizophrenia: experimental model based on targeted gene delivery through adeno-associated viruses, Behav Pharmacol 28(8) (2017) 630–641. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [59].Delatour B, Gisquet-Verrier P, Prelimbic cortex specific lesions disrupt delayed-variable response tasks in the rat, Behav Neurosci 110(6) (1996) 1282–98. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [60].Quevedo J, Vianna M, Zanatta MS, Roesler R, Izquierdo I, Jerusalinsky D, Quillfeldt JA, Involvement of mechanisms dependent on NMDA receptors, nitric oxide and protein kinase A in the hippocampus but not in the caudate nucleus in memory, Behav Pharmacol 8(8) (1997) 713–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [61].Quiroz C, Martinez I, Quirarte GL, Morales T, Diaz-Cintra S, Prado-Alcala RA, Enhanced inhibitory avoidance learning prevents the memory-impairing effects of post-training hippocampal inactivation, Exp Brain Res 153(3) (2003) 400–2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [62].Izquierdo LA, Barros DM, Medina JH, Izquierdo I, Novelty enhances retrieval of one-trial avoidance learning in rats 1 or 31 days after training unless the hippocampus is inactivated by different receptor antagonists and enzyme inhibitors, Behav Brain Res 117(1–2) (2000) 215–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [63].Giovannini MG, Pazzagli M, Malmberg-Aiello P, Della Corte L, Rakovska AD, Cerbai F, Casamenti F, Pepeu G, Inhibition of acetylcholine-induced activation of extracellular regulated protein kinase prevents the encoding of an inhibitory avoidance response in the rat, Neuroscience 136(1) (2005) 15–32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [64].Nobre MJ, Changes on auditory physiology in response to the inactivation of amygdala nuclei in high anxiety rats expressing learned fear, Physiol Behav 118 (2013) 80–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [65].Parent MB, Tomaz C, McGaugh JL, Increased training in an aversively motivated task attenuates the memory-impairing effects of posttraining N-methyl-D-aspartate-induced amygdala lesions, Behav Neurosci 106(5) (1992) 789–97. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [66].Roozendaal B, McGaugh JL, Basolateral amygdala lesions block the memory-enhancing effect of glucocorticoid administration in the dorsal hippocampus of rats, Eur J Neurosci 9(1) (1997) 76–83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [67].Torkaman-Boutorabi A, Sheidadoust H, Hashemi-Hezaveh SM, Zarrindast MR, Influence of morphine on medial prefrontal cortex alpha2 adrenergic system in passive avoidance learning in rats, Pharmacol Biochem Behav 133 (2015) 92–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [68].Torres-Garcia ME, Medina AC, Quirarte GL, Prado-Alcala RA, Differential Effects of Inactivation of Discrete Regions of Medial Prefrontal Cortex on Memory Consolidation of Moderate and Intense Inhibitory Avoidance Training, Front Pharmacol 8 (2017) 842. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [69].Paxinos G, Watson C, The rat brain in stereotaxic coordinates: hard cover edition, Elsevier; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- [70].Smith JR, Hayman GT, Wang SJ, Laulederkind SJF, Hoffman MJ, Kaldunski ML, Tutaj M, Thota J, Nalabolu HS, Ellanki SLR, Tutaj MA, De Pons JL, Kwitek AE, Dwinell MR, Shimoyama ME, The Year of the Rat: The Rat Genome Database at 20: a multi-species knowledgebase and analysis platform, Nucleic Acids Res 48(D1) (2020) D731–D742. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [71].Han TH, Lee K, Park JB, Ahn D, Park JH, Kim DY, Stern JE, Lee SY, Ryu PD, Reduction in synaptic GABA release contributes to target-selective elevation of PVN neuronal activity in rats with myocardial infarction, Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 299(1) (2010) R129–39. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [72].Potapenko ES, Biancardi VC, Florschutz RM, Ryu PD, Stern JE, Inhibitory-excitatory synaptic balance is shifted toward increased excitation in magnocellular neurosecretory cells of heart failure rats, J Neurophysiol 106(3) (2011) 1545–57. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [73].Potapenko ES, Biancardi VC, Zhou Y, Stern JE, Altered astrocyte glutamate transporter regulation of hypothalamic neurosecretory neurons in heart failure rats, Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 303(3) (2012) R291–300. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [74].Stern JE, Potapenko ES, Enhanced NMDA receptor-mediated intracellular calcium signaling in magnocellular neurosecretory neurons in heart failure rats, Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 305(4) (2013) R414–22. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [75].Brightwell JJ, Gallagher M, Colombo PJ, Hippocampal CREB1 but not CREB2 is decreased in aged rats with spatial memory impairments, Neurobiol Learn Mem 81(1) (2004) 19–26. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [76].Han XR, Wen X, Wang YJ, Wang S, Shen M, Zhang ZF, Fan SH, Shan Q, Wang L, Li MQ, Hu B, Sun CH, Wu DM, Lu J, Zheng YL, Effects of CREB1 gene silencing on cognitive dysfunction by mediating PKA-CREB signaling pathway in mice with vascular dementia, Mol Med 24(1) (2018) 18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar] [Retracted]
- [77].Sadamoto H, Kitahashi T, Fujito Y, Ito E, Learning-Dependent Gene Expression of CREB1 Isoforms in the Molluscan Brain, Front Behav Neurosci 4 (2010) 25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [78].Lu Z, Yang T, Wang L, Qiu Q, Zhao Y, Wu A, Li T, Cheng W, Wang B, Li Y, Yang J, Zhao M, Comparison of different protocols of Morris water maze in cognitive impairment with heart failure, Brain Behav 10(2) (2020) e01519. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [79].Celutkiene J, Vaitkevicius A, Jakstiene S, Jatuzis D, Expert Opinion-Cognitive Decline in Heart Failure: More Attention is Needed, Card Fail Rev 2(2) (2016) 106–109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [80].Gaviria M, Pliskin N, Kney A, Cognitive impairment in patients with advanced heart failure and its implications on decision-making capacity, Congest Heart Fail 17(4) (201) 175–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [81].Liu Z, Wang Y, Pan S, Zhang Y, Zhu H, Liu Y, Zhu L, Zhang J, Platelets-released insulin-like growth factor 1 is correlated with anxiety in myocardial infarction, Biochem Biophys Res Commun 520(2) (2019) 441–448. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [82].Lu X, Wang Y, Liu C, Wang Y, Depressive disorder and gastrointestinal dysfunction after myocardial infarct are associated with abnormal tryptophan-5-hydroxytryptamine metabolism in rats, PLoS One 12(2) (2017) e0172339. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [83].Najjar F, Ahmad M, Lagace D, Leenen FHH, Sex differences in depression-like behavior and neuroinflammation in rats post-MI: role of estrogens, Am J Physiol Heart Circ Physiol 315(5) (2018) H1159–H1173. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [84].Prickaerts J, Raaijmakers W, Blokland A, Effects of myocardial infarction and captopril therapy on anxiety-related behaviors in the rat, Physiol Behav 60(1) (1996) 43–50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [85].Canteras NS, Resstel LB, Bertoglio LJ, Carobrez Ade P, Guimaraes FS, Neuroanatomy of anxiety, Curr Top Behav Neurosci 2 (2010) 77–96. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [86].Hasan MT, Althammer F, Silva da Gouveia M, Goyon S, Eliava M, Lefevre A, Kerspern D, Schimmer J, Raftogianni A, Wahis J, Knobloch-Bollmann HS, Tang Y, Liu X, Jain A, Chavant V, Goumon Y, Weislogel JM, Hurlemann R, Herpertz SC, Pitzer C, Darbon P, Dogbevia GK, Bertocchi I, Larkum ME, Sprengel R, Bading H, Charlet A, Grinevich V, A Fear Memory Engram and Its Plasticity in the Hypothalamic Oxytocin System, Neuron 103(1) (2019) 133–146 e8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [87].Knobloch HS Charlet A, Hoffmann LC, Eliava M, Khrulev S, Cetin AH, Osten P, Schwarz MK, Seeburg PH, Stoop R, Grinevich V, Evoked axonal oxytocin release in the central amygdala attenuates fear response, Neuron 73(3) (2012) 553–66. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [88].Perusini JN, Fanselow MS, Neurobehavioral perspectives on the distinction between fear and anxiety, Learn Mem 22(9) (2015) 417–25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [89].Bocchio M, Nabavi S, Capogna M, Synaptic Plasticity, Engrams, and Network Oscillations in Amygdala Circuits for Storage and Retrieval of Emotional Memories, Neuron 94(4) (2017) 731–743. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [90].Janak PH, Tye KM, From circuits to behaviour in the amygdala, Nature 517(7534) (2015) 284–92. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [91].Roozendaal B, McEwen BS, Chattarji S, Stress, memory and the amygdala, Nat Rev Neurosci 10(6) (2009) 423–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [92].Goldman-Rakic PS, The prefrontal landscape: implications of functional architecture for understanding human mentation and the central executive, Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 351(1346) (1996) 1445–53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [93].Hjelm C, Dahl A, Brostrom A, Martensson J, Johansson B, Stromberg A, The influence of heart failure on longitudinal changes in cognition among individuals 80 years of age and older, J Clin Nurs 21(7–8) (2012) 994–1003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [94].Dardiotis E, Giamouzis G, Mastrogiannis D, Vogiatzi C, Skoularigis J, Triposkiadis F, Hadjigeorgiou GM, Cognitive impairment in heart failure, Cardiol Res Pract 2012 (2012) 595821. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [95].Harkness K, Demers C, Heckman GA, McKelvie RS, Screening for cognitive deficits using the Montreal cognitive assessment tool in outpatients >/=65 years of age with heart failure, Am J Cardiol 107(8) (2011) 1203–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [96].Pressler SJ, Subramanian U, Kareken D, Perkins SM, Gradus-Pizlo I, Sauve MJ, Ding Y, Kim J, Sloan R, Jaynes H, Shaw RM, Cognitive deficits in chronic heart failure, Nurs Res 59(2) (2010) 127–39. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [97].Etkin A, Wager TD, Functional neuroimaging of anxiety: a meta-analysis of emotional processing in PTSD, social anxiety disorder, and specific phobia, Am J Psychiatry 164(10) (2007) 1476–88. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [98].Woo MA, Macey PM, Keens PT, Kumar R, Fonarow GC, Hamilton MA, Harper RM, Functional abnormalities in brain areas that mediate autonomic nervous system control in advanced heart failure, J Card Fail 11(6) (2005) 437–46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [99].Salazar AP, Quagliotto E, Alves J, Oliveira FA, Saur L, Xavier LL, Pagnussat AS, Rasia-Filho A, Effect of prior exercise training and myocardial infarction-induced heart failure on the neuronal and glial densities and the GFAP-immunoreactivity in the posterodorsal medial amygdala of rats, Histol Histopathol 29(11) (2014) 1423–35. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [100].Barkus C, McHugh SB, Sprengel R, Seeburg PH, Rawlins JN, Bannerman DM, Hippocampal NMDA receptors and anxiety: at the interface between cognition and emotion, Eur J Pharmacol 626(1) (2010) 49–56. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [101].Bienkowski MS, Bowman I, Song MY, Gou L, Ard T, Cotter K, Zhu M, Benavidez NL, Yamashita S, Abu-Jaber J, Azam S, Lo D, Foster NN, Hintiryan H, Dong HW, Integration of gene expression and brain-wide connectivity reveals the multiscale organization of mouse hippocampal networks, Nat Neurosci 21(11) (2018) 1628–1643. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [102].Dong HW, Swanson LW, Chen L, Fanselow MS, Toga AW, Genomic-anatomic evidence for distinct functional domains in hippocampal field CA1, Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106(28) (2009) 11794–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [103].Fanselow MS, Dong HW, Are the dorsal and ventral hippocampus functionally distinct structures?, Neuron 65(1) (2010) 7–19. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [104].Moser MB, Moser EI, Functional differentiation in the hippocampus, Hippocampus 8(6) (1998) 608–19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [105].Thompson CL, Pathak SD, Jeromin A, Ng LL, MacPherson CR, Mortrud MT, Cusick A, Riley ZL, Sunkin SM, Bernard A, Puchalski RB, Gage FH, Jones AR, Bajic VB, Hawrylycz MJ, Lein ES, Genomic anatomy of the hippocampus, Neuron 60(6) (2008) 1010–21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [106].Sahay A, Hen R, Adult hippocampal neurogenesis in depression, Nat Neurosci 10(9) (2007) 1110–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [107].Dobryakova YV, Kasianov A, Zaichenko MI, Stepanichev MY, Chesnokova EA, Kolosov PM, Markevich VA, Bolshakov AP, Intracerebroventricular Administration of (192)IgG- Saporin Alters Expression of Microglia-Associated Genes in the Dorsal But Not Ventral Hippocampus, Front Mol Neurosci 10 (2017) 429. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [108].Dobryakova YV, Volobueva MN, Manolova AO, Medvedeva TM, Kvichansky AA, Gulyaeva NV, Markevich VA, Stepanichev MY, Bolshakov AP, Cholinergic Deficit Induced by Central Administration of 192lgG-Saporin Is Associated With Activation of Microglia and Cell Loss in the Dorsal Hippocampus of Rats, Front Neurosci 13 (2019) 146. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [109].Fuster-Matanzo A, Llorens-Martin M, de Barreda EG, Avila J, Hernandez F, Different susceptibility to neurodegeneration of dorsal and ventral hippocampal dentate gyrus: a study with transgenic mice overexpressing GSK3beta, PLoS One 6(11) (2011) e27262. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [110].Stouffer EM, Warninger EE, Michener PN, A high-fat diet impairs learning that is dependent on the dorsal hippocampus but spares other forms of learning, Hippocampus 25(12) (2015) 1567–76. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [111].Tournier BB, Tsartsalis S, Rigaud D, Fossey C, Cailly T, Fabis F, Pham T, Gregoire MC, Kovari E, Moulin-Sallanon M, Savioz A, Millet P, TSPO and amyloid deposits in sub-regions of the hippocampus in the 3xTgAD mouse model of Alzheimer’s disease, Neurobiol Dis 121 (2019) 95–105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [112].Crupi R, Impellizzeri D, Cuzzocrea S, Role of Metabotropic Glutamate Receptors in Neurological Disorders, Front Mol Neurosci 12 (2019) 20. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [113].Park S, Park JM, Kim S, Kim JA, Shepherd JD, Smith-Hicks C1, Chowdhury S, Kaufmann W, Kuhl D, Ryazanov AG, Huganir RL, Linden DJ, Worley PF, Elongation factor 2 and fragile X mental retardation protein control the dynamic translation of Arc/Arg3.1 essential for mGluR-LTD, Neuron 59(1) (2008) 70–83. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [114].Waung MW, Pfeiffer BE, Nosyreva ED, Ronesi JA, Huber KM, Rapid translation of Arc/Arg3.1 selectively mediates mGluR-dependent LTD through persistent increases in AMPAR endocytosis rate, Neuron 59(1) (2008) 84–97. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [115].Davis M, Walker DL, Miles L, Grillon C, Phasic vs sustained fear in rats and humans: role of the extended amygdala in fear vs anxiety, Neuropsychopharmacology 35(1) (2010) 105–35. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [116].Guzowski JF, Lyford GL, Stevenson GD, Houston FP, McGaugh JL, Worley PF, Barnes CA, Inhibition of activity-dependent arc protein expression in the rat hippocampus impairs the maintenance of long-term potentiation and the consolidation of long-term memory, J Neurosci 20(11) p000) 3993–4001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [117].Plath N, Ohana O, Dammermann B, Errington ML, Schmitz D, Gross C, Mao X, Engelsberg A, Mahlke C, Welzl H, Kobalz U, Stawrakakis A, Fernandez E, Waltereit R, Bick-Sander A, Therstappen E, Cooke SF, Blanquet V, Wurst W, Salmen B, Bosl MR, Lipp HP, Grant SG, Bliss TV, Wolfer DP, Kuhl D, Arc/Arg3.1 is essential for the consolidation of synaptic plasticity and memories, Neuron 52(3) (2006) 437–44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [118].Velazquez R, Shaw DM, Caccamo A, Oddo S, Pim1 inhibition as a novel therapeutic strategy for Alzheimer’s disease, Mol Neurodegener 11(1) (2016) 52. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [119].Wang H, Ferguson GD, Pineda VV, Cundiff PE, Storm DR, Overexpression of type-1 adenylyl cyclase in mouse forebrain enhances recognition memory and LTP, Nat Neurosci 7(6) (2004) 635–42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [120].Wilkerson JR, Albanesi JP, Huber KM, Roles for Arc in metabotropic glutamate receptor-dependent LTD and synapse elimination: Implications in health and disease, Semin Cell Dev Biol 77 (2018) 51–62. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [121].Wong ST, Athos J, Figueroa XA, Pineda VV, Schaefer ML, Chavkin CC, Muglia LJ, Storm DR, Calcium-stimulated adenylyl cyclase activity is critical for hippocampus-dependent long-term memory and late phase LTP, Neuron 23(4) (1999) 787–98. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [122].Diaz HS, Toledo C, Andrade DC, Marcus NJ, Del Rio R, Neuroinflammation in heart failure: new insights for an old disease, J Physiol 598(1) (2020) 33–59. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [123].Kang YM, Zhang ZH, Xue B, Weiss RM, Felder RB, Inhibition of brain proinflammatory cytokine synthesis reduces hypothalamic excitation in rats with ischemia-induced heart failure, Am J Physiol Heart Circ Physiol 295(1) (2008) H227–36. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [124].Xu B, Li H, Brain mechanisms of sympathetic activation in heart failure: Roles of the reninangiotensin system, nitric oxide and proinflammatory cytokines (Review), Mol Med Rep 12(6) (2015) 7823–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [125].Yu Y, Wei SG, Weiss RM, Felder RB, Angiotensin II Type 1a Receptors in the Subfornical Organ Modulate Neuroinflammation in the Hypothalamic paraventricular Nucleus in Heart Failure Rats, Neuroscience 381 (2018) 46–58. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [126].Frey A, Popp S, Post A, Langer S, Lehmann M, Hofmann U, Siren AL, Hommers L, Schmitt A, Strekalova T, Ertl G, Lesch KP, Frantz S, Experimental heart failure causes depression-like behavior together with differential regulation of inflammatory and structural genes in the brain, Front Behav Neurosci 8 (2014) 376. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [127].Iadecola C, The pathobiology of vascular dementia, Neuron 80(4) (2013) 844–66. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [128].Pullicino PM, Hart J, Cognitive impairment in congestive heart failure?: Embolism vs hypoperfusion, Neurology 57(11) (2001) 1945–6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [129].Zuccala G, Onder G, Pedone C, Carosella L, Pahor M, Bernabei R, Cocchi A, Group G-OS, Hypotension and cognitive impairment: Selective association in patients with heart failure, Neurology 57(11) (2001) 1986–92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [130].Kaplan A, Yabluchanskiy A, Ghali R, Altara R, Booz GW, Zouein FA, Cerebral blood flow alteration following acute myocardial infarction in mice, Biosci Rep 38(5) (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [131].Mamalyga ML, Mamalyga LM, Effect of Progressive Heart Failure on Cerebral Hemodynamics and Monoamine Metabolism in CNS, Bull Exp Biol Med 163(3) (2017) 307–312. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [132].Bazan NG, Palacios-Pelaez R, Lukiw WJ, Hypoxia signaling to genes: significance in Alzheimer’s disease, Mol Neurobiol 26(2–3) (2002) 283–98. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [133].Alagappan D, Balan M, Jiang Y, Cohen RB, Kotenko SV, Levison SW, Egr-1 is a critical regulator of EGF-receptor-mediated expansion of subventricular zone neural stem cells and progenitor during recovery from hypoxia-hypoglycemia, ASN Neuro 5(3) (2013) 183–93. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [134].Chan YL, Saad S, Pollock C, Oliver B, Al-Odat I, Zaky AA, Jones N, Chen H, Impact of maternal cigarette smoke exposure on brain inflammation and oxidative stress in male mice offspring, Sci Rep 6 (2016) 25881. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [135].Chen X, Liu Y, Zhang Z, Miao X, Tian Y, Yao L, Liu J, Lu H, Liu Y, Hypoxia stimulates the proliferation of rat neural stem cells by regulating the expression of metabotropic glutamate receptors: an in vitro study, Cell Mol Biol (Noisy-le-grand) 62(3) (2016) 105–14. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [136].Hong YM, Jo DG, Lee JY, Chang JW, Nam JH, Noh JY, Koh JY, Jung YK, Downregulation of ARC contributes to vulnerability of hippocampal neurons to ischemia/hypoxia, FEBS Lett 543(1–3) (2003) 170–3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [137].Martin N, Bossenmeyer-Pourie C, Koziel V, Jazi R, Audonnet S, Vert P, Gueant JL, Daval JL, Pourie G, Non-injurious neonatal hypoxia confers resistance to brain senescence in aged male rats, PLoS One 7(11) (2012) e48828. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [138].Simonyi A, Zhang JP, Sun GY, Changes in mRNA levels for group I metabotropic glutamate receptors following in utero hypoxia-ischemia, Brain Res Dev Brain Res 112(1) (1999) 31–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [139].Su B, Wang XT, Sun YH, Long MY, Zheng J, Wu WH, Li L, Ischemia/hypoxia inhibits cardiomyocyte autophagy and promotes apoptosis via the Egr-1/Bim/Beclin-1 pathway, J Geriatr Cardiol 17(5) (2020) 284–293. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [140].Zhao L, Jiao Q, Chen X, Yang P, Zhao B, Zheng P, Liu Y, mGluR5 is involved in proliferation of rat neural progenitor cells exposed to hypoxia with activation of mitogen- activated protein kinase signaling pathway, J Neurosci Res 90(2) (2012) 447–60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [141].Zeng Q, Huang Z, Zhang J, Liu R, Li X, Zeng J, Xiao H, 3’-Daidzein Sulfonate Sodium Protects Against Chronic Cerebral Hypoperfusion-Mediated Cognitive Impairment and Hippocampal Damage via Activity-Regulated Cytoskeleton-Associated Protein Upregulation, Front Neurosci 13 (2019) 104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [142].Suzuki H, Sumiyoshi A, Matsumoto Y, Duffy BA, Yoshikawa T, Lythgoe MF, Yanai K, Taki Y, Kawashima R, Shimokawa H, Structural abnormality of the hippocampus associated with depressive symptoms in heart failure rats, Neuroimage 105 (2015) 84–92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [143].Swanson LW, Brain maps: structure of the rat brain, 3rd ed., Elsevier, Academic Press, 2004. [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.