Skip to main content
Springer logoLink to Springer
. 2022 May 28;272(7):1139–1155. doi: 10.1007/s00406-022-01416-6

Cognitive impairment in people with schizophrenia: an umbrella review

Yohannes Gebreegziabhere 1,2,, Kassahun Habatmu 3, Awoke Mihretu 2, Matteo Cella 4, Atalay Alem 2
PMCID: PMC9508017  PMID: 35633394

Abstract

Cognitive impairment is one of the core symptoms of schizophrenia. Quite a number of systematic reviews were published related to cognitive impairment in people with schizophrenia (PWS). This umbrella review, therefore, aimed at reviewing and synthesizing the findings of systematic reviews related to domains of cognition impaired and associated factors in PWS. We searched four electronic databases. Data related to domains, occurrence, and associated factors of cognitive impairment in PWS were extracted. The quality of all eligible systematic reviews was assessed using A MeaSurement Tool to Assess methodological quality of systematic Review (AMSTAR) tool. Results are summarized and presented in a narrative form. We identified 63 systematic reviews fulfilling the eligibility criteria. The included reviews showed that PWS had lower cognitive functioning compared to both healthy controls and people with affective disorders. Similar findings were reported among psychotropic free cases and people with first episode psychosis. Greater impairment of cognition was reported in processing speed, verbal memory, and working memory domains. Greater cognitive impairment was reported to be associated with worse functionality and poor insight. Cognitive impairment was also reported to be associated with childhood trauma and aggressive behaviour. According to our quality assessment, the majority of the reviews had moderate quality. We were able to find a good number of systematic reviews on cognitive impairment in PWS. The reviews showed that PWS had higher impairment in different cognitive domains compared to healthy controls and people with affective disorders. Impairment in domains of memory and processing speed were reported frequently.

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1007/s00406-022-01416-6.

Keywords: Cognitive impairment, Domains impaired, Determinants, Schizophrenia

Introduction

Cognitive function is a mental process which involves several intellectual abilities such as perception, reasoning, and remembering, while cognitive impairment is the malfunction of cognition or intellectual abilities [1]. Cognitive impairment in people with schizophrenia (PWS) can be considered as part of core symptoms of the disorder because: (1) almost all (98%) PWS showed cognitive decrement compared to their premorbid state [2], (2) PWS showed a broader domains of cognitive impairment with varying severity per domain [3], and (3) cognitive impairment is seen in PWS who are not taking anti-psychotic drugs [4].

Cognitive impairment in PWS was recognized from as early as the time of Emil Kraepelin, where Kraepelin named the disorder as dementia praecox to mean early-onset dementia (although due attention has not been given to it) [5]. However, different groups are now working to improve cognitive impairment in PWS as part of the intervention for the problem [6, 7]. One of these groups is the Measurement And Treatment Research to Improve Cognition in Schizophrenia (MATRICS) initiative [6]. The MATRICS initiative, through review of factor analytical studies and consensus, identified seven domains of cognition which are more affected in PWS [8]. Brief description of each of these domains is given below (Table 1).

Table 1.

Brief description of cognitive domains which are more affected and commonly reported in people with schizophrenia

Domain Definition Citation
Speed of processing The speed in which information is processed and encoded to give meaning about the outside world (Bowie and Harvey, 2005 [76]; Braff, 1993 [77])
Attention/concentration Attention is the ability to detect and focus on relevant stimuli, and ignore irrelevant stimuli. Concentration is the ability to sustain attention
Working memory The ability to store and manipulate information for short period of time
Verbal learning and memory The ability to learn and remember from verbal stimuli
Visual learning and memory The ability to learn and remember from visual stimuli
Reasoning and problem solving The ability to solve problem, think abstractly, and coordinate other cognitive domains
Social cognition The ability to perceive and make sense of the surrounding, which includes four interrelated sub-domains: (1) Theory of mind; the ability to make inference about other beliefs, dispositions, and intentions (2) Emotion (perception and) processing; is the ability of a person related to identification, facilitation, and managing emotion (3) Social perception and social knowledge; Social perception is the ability to use cues to identify social context, roles, and rules. Whereas, Social knowledge is the ability to identify context and rules to apply in an identified specific social context (4) Attributional bias; it is the kind of inference that a person makes about the causes of a positive and negative event in their life (Kayman and Goldstein, 2012 [78])

A large number of articles are published on cognitive impairment in PWS. A simple hit of “Cognition AND Schizophrenia” in PubMed yields around 30,000 articles. Consequently, quite a number of systematic reviews have been published on the subject. Although there are numerous systematic reviews on the magnitude of cognitive domains impaired and associated factors in PWS, we found only one published umbrella review on the subject [9]. However, this umbrella review has not addressed factors associated with cognitive impairment; and it only focused on the difference in the magnitude of cognitive impairment between PWS and people with bipolar disorder.

The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of mental disorders (DSM) [5] and the International Classification of Diseases (ICD) [10] have not, to date, considered cognitive impairment in their diagnostic criteria for schizophrenia. Whereas, cognitive impairment seems to be one of the core symptoms of this illness. In this umbrella review, we aimed to explore the evidence that cognitive impairment maybe a common problem in PWS by examining findings from systematic reviews and meta-analysis related to magnitude of cognitive domains impaired and associated factors in PWS. This umbrella review might help clinicians and experts in the area to have a comprehensive understanding of domains of cognition affected, and related factors in PWS. Additionally, this review would inform future researchers regarding which factors need to be considered while planning studies involving cognitive impairment in PWS. This umbrella review, therefore, aimed at synthesizing the evidence on magnitude of cognitive domains impaired and associated factors in PWS from systematic review studies.

Methods

We followed the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guideline to identify, search, extract articles, and report this umbrella review [11].

Databases searched

We searched major databases, including PubMed, Embase, PsycINFO, and Global Index Medicus from the date of inception of each database until July 05, 2018. An update search from the same databases was conducted on 13th August 2020. Google Scholar was used for forward and backward searching.

Search strategy

We used the following three key words: schizophrenia, Cognition, and Magnitude/Associated factors. Free terms and controlled vocabulary terms were used for those three big terms. We combined those three big terms with the Boolean term “AND”. The term systematic review was not included in the initial search to allow collection of systematic reviews that did not mention systematic review in their title or abstract, however, we later filtered each databases for review or systematic review or meta-analysis or other related terms according to the filter method of the database. For the complete search strategy, see online resource 1. To increase our chance of capturing all systematic reviews about the magnitude and associated factors of cognitive impairment in PWS, we conducted a forward and backward search.

Eligibility criteria

This umbrella review considered reviews that aimed at assessing domains of cognitive impairment and associated factors among adult PWS aged 18 years and older. Diagnoses needed to have been confirmed using either DSM [5], ICD [10], or other recognized diagnostic criteria. For studies with mixed populations at least 50% of the participants should be PWS.

We included any systematic reviews or meta-analysis which addressed concepts related to magnitude and/or associated factors of at least one full domain of cognition in PWS. If the review was about sub-domains of a single domain then that review was excluded.

We only included systematic review studies. In this review, systematic review was operationalized as any review which had a search term, searched at least one database, had eligibility criteria to include studies, and reported the findings systematically [12]. We employed such a stringent definition of systematic review, because there were numerous unstructured reviews, expert comments, editorials, overviews, guidelines, and other non-systematic reviews on the subject.

No restriction was employed in terms of settings of the systematic reviews. However, systematic reviews published only in English were included into this umbrella review.

Full-text identification process

We merged the articles we found from the databases and removed duplicates. The first author (YG) screened each article for eligibility using their title and abstract, followed by full-text screening. While, another author (AM) screened 10% of the articles and we found good agreement. Same procedure was employed in screening articles obtained through forward and backward search.

Data extraction

The first author (YG) extracted data from the included articles using a data extraction tool developed a priori. Another author (AM) checked a randomly selected articles for correct extraction using the same extraction tool. The extraction tool was developed referring to previous published systematic reviews, and the requirements for quality assessment in consultation with the other co-authors. This was followed by piloting it on two articles (the data extraction template is in online resource 2). The core components of the data extraction tool include authors’ detail, databases searched, number of included studies, total number of participants included, cognitive domains addressed, and findings about cognitive impairment and associated factors.

Risk of bias/quality assessment

The first author (YG) and another author (AM) assessed the quality of each review using a critical appraisal form designed for evaluating the methodological quality of systematic reviews: a measurement tool to assess methodological quality of systematic Review (AMSTAR) [13]. AMSTAR has eleven items each to be scored as one for “yes” and zero for “no”. The eleven items focused on the presence of a prior registration of the protocol, study identification process, quality assessment section, and other steps of conducting a review. A score of 8 or more is considered as a good-quality review, a score of 4 to 7 a moderate quality, and a score below 4 a poor-quality review.

Data synthesis

We used a narrative synthesis to report the findings in this review. A narrative synthesis approach was selected as this approach is more appropriate to summarize studies with heterogeneous outcomes. For each systematic review included, we have reported the databases searched, number of included studies, total number of participants in each group (if more than one group was involved), specific cognitive domains evaluated, and findings related to magnitude and associated factors of cognitive impairment. We have also reported methodological qualities of each review included. We have presented the findings considering the methodological quality of the reviews included.

Results

Study characteristics

The search yielded a total of 7734 articles (i.e., 6607 articles in the initial search and 1127 on the updated search). Title and abstract screening yielded 87 articles, while full-text screening, and forward and backward searching gave us 63 systematic reviews (Fig. 1). A list of excluded articles after full-text screening with the reason for their exclusion is provided in online resource 3.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

PRISMA (Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analysis) flow diagram showing how articles are identified, screened, and included.  Abbreviations: CIPWS cognitive impairment in people with schizophrenia

All the included systematic reviews evaluated cognitive impairment and associated factors in PWS with no geographical restriction, except one review from China. A diverse group of mental disorders were included in each review. In all the reviews, PWS were either compared within schizophrenia group or with other population groups, such as people with affective disorder or healthy controls. The majority of the reviews searched both PubMed and PsycINFO databases (n = 39). The quality of most of the included reviews was moderate (n = 38). We identified the following four themes from analysis of the data extracted from the included reviews.

  1. Domains of cognition impaired in PWS from specific single cognitive domain reviews (n = 15/63)

  2. Comparison of cognitive impairment in PWS with different groups from reviews with multiple domains (n = 17/63)

  3. Course/progress of cognitive impairment in PWS over time (n = 6/63)

  4. Cognitive impairment in PWS and associated factors (n = 25/63)

Domains of cognition impaired in PWS from specific single cognitive domain reviews

We identified a total of 15 systematic reviews which assessed the magnitude of cognitive impairment in PWS in a specific single cognitive domain. Five assessed memory, four executive function, two processing speed, two verbal fluency, and two social cognition. These reviews were published between 1999 and 2019. The majority of the reviews (n = 9/15) searched both PubMed and PsycINFO, and they included from 10 to 124 independent studies (1 review did not report the number of studies included). Ten of the 15 systematic reviews had moderate quality, while the remaining five studies had poor quality. Detailed description of the reviews included are given in (Table 2).

Table 2.

Characteristics of systematic reviews that assessed domains of cognitive impairment in people with schizophrenia from single domain reviews

Citation Databases searched Range of search year Included studies Total sample size of studies included Domain Concept addressed Quality assessment
Piskulic et al. (2007) –PubMed and MEDLINE –From 1992 to 2005 –33 studies –1112 PWS and 1241 HC –SWM Botha Poor quality
Aleman at al. (1999) –Psychist and MEDLINE –From 1975 through July 1998 –70 studies –60 studies, a total of 3315 in meta-analysis –Short and long-term memory Botha Poor quality
Lee and Park (2005) –PsycINFO and MEDLINE –From 1980 to September 2004 –124 studies –Sample size is not reported systematically –WM Magnitude, comparison Moderate quality
Doughty and Done (2009) –PubMed and PsycINFO –The start date is NR, search was conducted on October 2007 –91 studies –Sample size is not reported –Semantic memory Magnitude of impairment Moderate quality
Pelletier et al. (2005) –PubMed and PsycINFO –From 1965 to July 2003 –84 studies –Sample size is not reported systematically –EM Botha and comparison Moderate quality
Johnson-Selfridge and Zalewski (2001) –MEDLINE, PsycINFO, PsychLit –Until early 1997 –71 studies –Sample included is Not reported systematically –EF Botha Moderate quality
Dibben et al. (2009) –MEDLINE, PsycINFO, Embase –Until March 2008 –88 studies –Sample size are not reported systematically –EF Association with EF impairment Moderate quality
Li (2004) –MEDLINE –Until November 2003 –59 studies –Sample size is not reported –EF Error in the WCST (type of EF problem) Poor quality
Thai et al. (2019) –PsycINFO and PubMed –On 1st September 2017 –10 studies –375 PWS and 541 HC –EF Magnitude and comparison of sub-domains of EF Moderate quality
Bokat and Goldberg (2003) –MEDLINE –Until June 1, 2001 –13 studies –915(PWS = 526, HC = 389) –VF Magnitude and comparison Poor quality
Henry and Crawford (2005) –Web of science, Psych lit CD-ROM, and science direct –Between 1981 and 2002 –84 studies –2947 PWS and 2469 HC –Semantic and phonetic VF Magnitude, and comparison Moderate quality
Knowles et al. (2010) –MEDLINE and PsycINFO –From May 2006 to January 2009 –36 studies –4,135 PWS and 2,292 HC –PS Botha Moderate quality
Dickinson et al. (2007) –MEDLINE and PsycINFO –From 1990 to April 2006 –37 studies –1961 PWS and 1444 HC –PS Botha Moderate quality
Savla et al. (2013) –PsycINFO And PubMed –From 1980 to November, 2011 –112 studies –3980 PWS and 3570 HC –SC Botha Moderate quality
Javed and Charles (2018) –PubMed –Past 10 years –NR –NR –SC Botha Poor quality

EF executive function, HC healthy control, NR not reported, PS processing speed, PWS people with schizophrenia, SC social cognition, SWM spatial working memory, VF verbal fluency, WCST wisconsin card sorting test, WM working memory

aBoth is for magnitude and associated factors

The most frequently studied domain of cognition is memory; 5 of the 15 systematic reviews [1418] included were about different kinds of memory impairment in PWS. In all types of memory examined (spatial working memory, short-term and long-term memory, working memory, episodic memory, and semantic memory), PWS were found to have more impairment compared to healthy controls. The second most commonly studied domain was executive function. Four systematic reviews [1922] included were about the level of impairment in executive function in PWS. All the four reviews reported that PWS had greater impairment in executive function compared to healthy controls. Two reviews [23, 24] were on processing speed. These reviews found that PWS had greater impairment in processing speed compared to controls. The level of impairment of verbal fluency was examined in two systematic reviews [25, 26], which found that PWS were more impaired in verbal fluency compared to healthy controls. Semantic fluency was reported to be more impaired in PWS compared to letter/phonetic fluency. Finally, other two systematic reviews [27, 28] included under this sub-section examined social cognition. Both reviews disclosed that in all domains of social cognition PWS performed worse compared to controls (Fig. 2).

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Domain of cognition impaired in people with schizophrenia as extracted from systematic reviews of single domain reviews (n = 15)

Comparison of cognitive impairment in PWS with different groups from reviews with multiple domains

A total of 17 systematic reviews compared the magnitude of cognitive impairment in PWS with different groups in multiple domains of cognition. Of these, five reviews compared cognitive impairment in PWS and people with bipolar disorder/affective disorders and healthy controls; four compared cognitive impairment in PWS with healthy controls only. Five systematic reviews compared cognitive impairment in first episode/drug-free PWS with healthy controls. One review compared cognitive impairment in first episode schizophrenia patients with first episode bipolar patients and healthy controls. Two reviews compared cognitive domains to one another. Except for one which was rated as a good review, all the other reviews were of moderate quality (n = 12/17) or poor quality (n = 4/17) (Table 3).

Table 3.

Characteristics of systematic reviews that assessed domains of cognitive impairment in people with schizophrenia from multiple domains reviews

Citation Databases searched Range of search year Number of included studies Total sample size of studies included Domain Concept addressed Quality assessment
Trotta et al. (2015) –MEDLINE (PubMed), Embase and PsycINFO –From 1990 to 2013 –28 studies

–For premorbid 1993 PWS, 1270 PWBP, and 772,138 HC

–For post-onset 1087 PWS, 811 PWBP and 1400 HC

–General Comparison with PWBP and association Moderate quality
Stefanopoulou et al. (2009) –PsychLit, MEDLINE and Embase –From January 1980 to December 2005 –53 studies –2508 PWBD, 1067 PWS, 197 MDD pt and 992 HC –More than one domain Magnitude, comparison people with affective d/r and PWS Moderate quality
Bora et al. (2009) –PubMed, Scopus, PsycINFO, Embase and MEDLINE –January 1975 and December 2007 –31 studies –1979 PWS and 1314 people with affective psychosis or schizoaffective D/r –More than one domain Magnitude, comparison with schizoaffective and associated factors Moderate quality
Krabbendam et al. (2005) –MEDLINE and PsycLIT –Between 1985 and October 2004 –31 studies –Sample size is not reported –More than one domain Magnitude, comparison between PWBD and PWS Moderate quality
Kuswanto et al. (2016) –NCBI, PubMed/MEDLINE, Scieverse, Scidirect, and Web-of-Science –From January 2000 to July 2015 –36 studies –2605 PWS, 1972 PWBD and 2512 HC –More than one domain Botha, and comparison PWS and people with affective d/r Poor quality
Schaefer et al. (2013) –PubMed –January 2006 through June 2012, an update –100 studies –9048 PWS and 8814 HC –More than one domain Botha and comparison of PWS and HC Moderate quality review
Fioravanti et al. (2012) –PubMed, PsycINFO, PsycArticles –Up to March 2010 –240 studies –18,300 PWS –More than one domain –Botha and comparison of PWS and HC Moderate quality review
Fioravanti et al. (2005) –Psychlit, Psychiatry, and MEDLINE –Between 1990 and 2003 –113 studies –4365 PWS and 3429 HC –More than one domain Magnitude, comparison of PWS and HC Moderate quality
Heinrichs and Zakzanis (1998) –PsycINFO and Medline –Between 1980 and 1997 –204 studies –7,420 PWS, and 5,865 HC –More than one domain –Botha, and comparison, b/n PWS and HC Poor quality
Citation Databases searched Range of search year Number of included studies Total sample size of studies included Domain Concept addressed Quality assessment
Mesholam-Gately (2009) –MEDLINE/PubMed and PsycINFO –Until October 2007 –47 studies –2,204 FEPWS and 2,775 largely age- and gender matched HC –More than one domain Botha and comparison of FEPWS with HC Moderate quality
Rajji et al. (2009) –29 databases (including Embase, MEDLINE and PsycINFO) –1980 to 2008 –109 studies –5010 PWS Number of the HC are not systematically reported –More than one domain Magnitude, comparison Youth onset schizophrenia Moderate quality
Fatouros-Bergman et al. (2014) –PubMed and PsycINFO –Until 1st of November 2012 –23 studies –1106 PWS and 1385 HC –More than one domain Magnitude, comparison between PWS and HC Moderate quality
Aas et al. (2014) –MEDLINE (PubMed) and PsycINFO –Up to and including January 2013 –24 Studies –Not reported –More than one domain Magnitude, comparison FES and HC Poor quality
Bora and Pantelis (2015) –PubMed, PsycINFO, ProQuest, and Scopus –From January 1990 to July 2014 –22 studies –533 FEB pt with 1417 HC and 605 FEB pt and 822 FES pt –More than one domain Comparison b/n FEB pt and FES pt and association Moderate quality
Zhang et al. (2019) –PubMed, Embase, PsycINFO, Cochrane Library, CNKI, WANFANG DATA, WEIPU Journal Net (VIP) and SinoMed –Up to 13 March 2019 –56 studies only from Chinese –3167 FES and 3017 HC –More than one domain Botha, and comparison FES and HC Moderate quality
Thibaudeau et al. (2019) –PubMed, PsycINFO, Embase, Proquest, SciVerse, ScienceDirect and Cochrane Library –1980 and December 19th 2018 (including Epub) –91 studies –5462 PWS –More than one domain –Relationship b/n ToM and NC and factors affecting the association Magnitude b/n PWS and HC and Comparisons across domains Good quality
Pickup (2008) –PsycINFO and MEDLINE –In May 2007 –17 studies –Ranges between 16 and 128 –More than one domain

–Relationship b/n ToM and EF

–Magnitude b/n PWS and HC and Comparisons across domains

Poor quality

D/r disorder, HC healthy controls, NCBI national centre of bio-technology information, PWBD people with bipolar disorder, PWS people with schizophrenia, CNKI China national knowledge infrastructure, EF executive function, FEBP first episode bipolar, FEPWS first episode people with schizophrenia, FES first episode psychosis, HC healthy controls, NC neuro cognition, Pt patients, PWS people with schizophrenia, SinoMed sino biomedicine service system, ToM theory of mind

aFor both magnitude and associated factors

From the reviews that compared cognitive impairment in PWS and people with bipolar disorder or other affective disorders, we found that PWS had significantly higher cognitive impairment compared to both people with bipolar disorder, or other affective disorders, and healthy controls, whereas people with bipolar disorder had intermediate cognitive impairment compared with PWS and healthy controls [2933]. Regarding impairment in specific domains, one review [31], reported that greater impairment was observed in verbal fluency, whereas no difference was reported in the domains of delayed verbal memory, and fine motor skill between PWS and people with bipolar disorder. Similarly, another review [30] found that there was no significant group difference in the domains of verbal memory, attention (digit span), and spatial working memory between PWS and people with bipolar disorder.

Compared to healthy controls, PWS scored significantly lower across all cognitive domains [3, 3436]. Looking at specific domains, greater impairment was found in the domains of processing speed and memory, whereas lower impairment was reported in domains of language and Intelligence Quotient (IQ) [34, 36].

We found that people with first episode schizophrenia were more impaired compared to healthy controls in multiple domains [3740]. In the domain of verbal memory, greater impairment was reported in two reviews [37, 40]. Compared to people with first episode bipolar disorder cases and healthy controls, people with first episode schizophrenia were found to have worse performance while, people with bipolar disorder were reported to have cognitive impairment worse than healthy controls [41]. Similarly, in one systematic review of studies on cognitive impairment among treatment naive PWS [4], it was reported that PWS performed worse compared to healthy controls in all the domains considered, the three domains with greater impairment were verbal memory, processing speed, and working memory. These systematic reviews confirmed that cognitive impairment in PWS occurred in the absence of medication (i.e., antipsychotics).

Finally, two reviews focused on the relationship among the different cognitive domains. One of the two reviews [42] examined the relationship between theory of mind and neurocognitive domains. This review reported moderate association between theory of mind and each neurocognitive domain (Zrs 0.27–0.43), with no significant difference among domains in the neurocognitive domain. In the same review, stronger association was reported between theory of mind and executive function and abstraction domains. While the second review [43] that compared theory of mind impairment with executive function impairment reported that PWS had greater impairment in both domains and theory of mind continued to predict schizophrenia (rather than being a control participant) once executive function was controlled for. This review supported the hypothesis that theory of mind and executive function impairments in PWS are independent of one another.

Course/ progress of cognitive impairment in PWS over time

Six of the systematic reviews we identified were about the course of cognitive impairment in PWS [4449]. Methodologically, two reviews were rated as poor and the other four were rated as moderate quality (Table 4).

Table 4.

Characteristics of systematic reviews that assessed the course/progress of cognitive impairment in people with schizophrenia

Citation Databases searched Range of search year Number of included studies Sample size of studies included Domain addressed Concept addressed Quality assessment
Shah et al. (2012) –PubMed –Between 1960 and 2009 –23 studies –Sample size systematically NR –More than one domain Course of CIPWS Poor quality
Irani at al. (2010) –MEDLINE –From inception to November 30, 2007 and February 16, 2008, –43 studies

2110 PWS, 1738 HC form cross-sectional studies and

–954 PWS from longitudinal studies

–More than one domain Course and factors associated Moderate quality
Bozikas and Andreou (2011) –PubMed –Until 15 January 2010 –26 studies –Sample size is not reported systematically –More than one domain Progress and associated factors Moderate quality
Szöke et al. (2008) –MEDLINE –From 1978 to 2006 –53 studies –2476 PWS and 324 HC –General Course and associated factors Poor quality
Woodberry (2008) –PubMed –Until March 1, 2007 –18 studies –Sample size systematically NR –IQ Magnitude, and course Moderate quality
Khandaker et al. (2011) –MEDLINE–PubMed, PsycINFO and Embase –From January 1984 until February 2011 –14 studies –4396 cases and over 745,000 HC –IQ Comparison and course Moderate quality

CIPWS cognitive impairment in people with schizophrenia, HC healthy controls, NR not reported, IQ intelligence quotient, PWS people with schizophrenia

Only one review [44] reported cognitive function decline over time. Even this review reported mixed results with slightly more studies pointing towards decline with a 1 to 1.2 ratio of no decline vs decline. This review was rated as poor quality with our criteria. To the contrary, two moderate-quality reviews [45, 46] reported no decline in cognitive function of PWS from follow-up studies. Most of the included studies in this review showed no difference between those treated with typical and atypical antipsychotics; among various atypical antipsychotics, and between medicated and un-medicated participants in terms of cognitive impairment [46]. One systematic review [47], which is a poor-quality review, reported improvement in cognitive function over time.

Two reviews [48, 49] examined the course of IQ in PWS, and both reviews found that PWS had lower performance in IQ test compared to controls. In both reviews, mixed results (i.e., both decline over time and no decline) were reported, where no decline over time was reported in better quality studies.

Cognitive impairment in PWS and associated factors

Twenty-five reviews focused on cognitive impairment in PWS and associated factors such as functionality, symptom dimensions, substance use, age of patients, insight into their problem, childhood trauma, duration of untreated psychosis, treatment, and aggressive behaviour. Seven of the 25 reviews examined the relationship between cognitive impairment and functionality, while four of them evaluated the relationship between cognitive impairment and substance use. Other four reviews examined the relationship between cognitive impairment and insight and two others each examined the relationship between cognitive impairment and symptom dimensions, duration of untreated psychosis, treatment, and childhood trauma. One review each examined the relationship between cognitive impairment and age, and aggressive behaviour (Fig. 3). Twelve of the 25 systematic reviews were moderate quality, while two were high quality (Table 5).

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3

Pictorial presentation of factors associated with cognitive impairment in people with schizophrenia from reviews included (n = 25). Abbreviations: CIPWS cognitive impairment in people with schizophrenia, EF executive function, MMSE mini mental state examination, NC neuro cognition, RPS reasoning and problem solving, SC social cognition, SP speed of processing, ToM theory of mind, WM working memory

Table 5.

Characteristics of systematic reviews that assessed the association of cognitive impairment in people with schizophrenia with different factors

Citation Databases searched and Range of search year Number of Included studies Sample size of studies included Domain addressed Concept addressed Quality assessment
Allott et al. (2011) –PsycINFO and MEDLINE –Until 31st July 2010 –22 studies –Sample size systematically NR –More than one domain Association with functionality Good quality
Christensen (2007) –PubMed (1966), PsycINFO (1967), and Embase (1974) –Up to 2004 –21 studies –Range from 30 to 118 patients: and median = 75 –General Association with functionality Moderate quality
Fett et al. (2011) –MEDLINE and PsycINFO –January 1977 to August 2009 –52 studies –2692 people with non-affective psychosis –More than one domain Association with functionality Moderate quality
Couture et al. (2006) –PsycINFO and MEDLINE/ PubMed –Range of search year is NR –NR –Sample size included are systematically NR –SC Association of SC with functionality Poor quality
Rajji et al. (2014) –MEDLINE –1999 to June 20, 2012 –NR –NR –More than one domain Association with functionality from Long-studies Poor quality
Halverson et al. (2019) –PsycINFO, PubMed, and ProQuest PsyArXiv –Update of Fett et al. (2011) to July 2017 –166 studies –12,868 PWS; 518 for correlation study –More than one domain Association with functionality Good quality
Schmidt et al. (2011) –MEDLINE and PsycINFO –January 2004 and December 2010 –15 studies –Ranges from 26 to 178 –More than one domain Association with functionality Poor quality
Donoghue and Doody (2012) –PsycINFO, MEDLINE, Zetoc Embase, CINAHL, Web of Knowledge –1980 and up until October 2010 –21 studies –Comorbid diagnosis of a SUD was 763, and the totals with no comorbid diagnosis was 770 –More than one domain Association with substance Moderate quality
Yüce et al (2010) –PubMed, PsycINFO, Scopus –Between 1987 and March 2010 –10 studies –572 PWS (with and without cannabis use) –More than one domain Cannabis as an associated variable Moderate quality
Potvin et al. (2008) –PubMed and PsycINFO –Until 31st August 2006 –23 studies –1807 PWS with/without SUD, 789 PWS + SUD and 1106 PWS –More than one domain Substance as an associated variable Moderate quality
Coulston et al. (2007) –MEDLINE and PsycINFO –NR –65 studies –Sample size are not systematically NR –More than one domain Substance as an associated variable Poor quality
Dominguez et al. (2009) –MEDLINE and PsychINFO –January 1977 to April 2008 –58 studies –5,009 people with non-affective psychosis –More than one domain Association with sx dimensions Moderate quality
Nieuwenstein et al. (2001) –Psychlit and Medline –1980 to December 1999 –For WCST 16, and for CPT 6 studies –Sample size are not reported systematically –EF and At/vigilance Association with sx dimensions Poor quality
Citation Databases searched Range of search year Number of Included studies Sample size of studies included Domain addressed Concept addressed Quality assessment
Maltais et al. (2015) –PubMed and PsycINFO –Between 1975 and 2012 –56 studies –5,588 PWS –General Association with age Poor quality
Kurtz (2011) –PsycINFO (2010), MEDLINE (2008) –1980 up to 2008/2010 –20 studies –Sample size is not systematically reported –More than one domain Response for treatment Poor quality
Keefe et al. (1999) –MEDLINE and Psychology Abstract –Between 1990 and April 1998 –15 studies –Sample size are not reported systematically –More than one domain Comparison b/n typical and atypical antipsychotics Moderate quality
Subotnik et al. (2020) –PsychINFO, PubMed, and Google Scholar –January 1, 1993 to June 30, 2018 –123 studies –14,932 PWS –More than one domain Association with insight Poor quality
Nair at al. (2014) –PubMed and Web of Science –For insight study from 2004 until September 2012 –72 studies for insight study, 34 studies in PWS –5429 patients for insight study, 1821 PWS –More than one domain Association with insight in psychotic disorder Moderate quality
Agrawal at al. (2003) –PubMed and Web of Science –1980 and April 2004 –35 studies –2354 people with psychosis –More than one domain Association with insight Moderate quality
Cooke et al. (2005) –PsycINFO (from1967) and MEDLINE (from 1966) –Up to December 2004 –45 studies –Range is between 21 and 535 –More than one domain Association with insight Poor quality
Reinharth et al. (2014) –PsycINFO and MEDLINE –Up to April 2013 –29 studies –4764 PWS –More than one domain Association with Aggressive behaviour Moderate quality
Vargas et al. (2019) –PubMed, Ovid MEDLINE, and Web of Science –On July 28, 2018 –23 studies –3315 people with Psychotic –More than one domain Association with childhood trauma Moderate quality
Dauvermann and Donohoe (2019) –PubMed and PsycINFO –Between 1993 and December 2017 –21 studies –Range b/n 28 and 659 –More than one domain Association with childhood trauma Poor quality
Allott et al. (2018) –PubMed and Embase –Inception to June 2015 –43 studies –4647 FEP patients –More than one domain Association with DUP Poor quality
Bora et al. (2018) –PubMed and Scopus –January 1980 to February 2017 –27 studies –3127 FEP –More than one domain Association with DUP Moderate quality

CPT continuous performance test, Long-studies longitudinal studies, NR not reported, PWS people with schizophrenia, SC social cognition, SUD substance used disorder, Sx symptom; WCST wisconsin card sorting test, At attention, DUP duration of untreated psychosis, EF executive function, FEP first Episode Psychosis, PWS people with schizophrenia

As shown in (Fig. 3), the relationship between cognitive impairment in PWS and functionality was examined in seven systematic reviews [5056]. One good-quality review [50] concluded that there was no association between cognition and functional outcome. However, the other six reviews [5156] found some association between different domains of cognition and different aspects of functionality. Three of these reviews were poor quality, two moderate quality, and one good-quality review (Fig. 3 and Table 5).

Four reviews [5760] examined the relationship between substance use and cognitive impairment in PWS. These reviews reported inconsistent findings. The effect of substance use in cognitive impairment in PWS differed from substance to substance, and from domain to domain (Fig. 3 and Table 5).

Four reviews that examined the association between cognitive impairment in PWS and insight [6164] reported mixed findings. One poor-quality review [62] reported inconsistent results, with more studies reporting no association between insight and neurocognition. While the other three reported a significant relationship between poor insight and worse cognitive performance both in neurocognitive and social cognitive domains [61, 63, 64], with stronger relationship in some sub-domains of social cognition (such as theory of mind) [64] (Fig. 3 and Table 5).

With regards to the relationship between cognitive impairment and symptom dimensions, two reviews [65, 66] were included in the umbrella review. These reviews found that cognitive impairment was associated with negative symptoms and disorganized symptoms compared to positive symptoms and depressive symptoms (Fig. 3 and Table 5).

Two reviews examined the relationship between cognition and duration of untreated psychosis [67, 68]. One of these had poor quality and the other one moderate quality. Both concluded that there was no significant association between most neurocognition domains and duration of untreated psychosis, with the exception of general intellectual function, executive function, and trial making test A and B (Fig. 3 and Table 5).

Two reviews examined the relationship between childhood trauma and cognitive impairment in PWS [69, 70]. Both reviews reported significant association between higher rates of childhood trauma and reduced overall cognitive performance; one of these reviews was poor quality and the other one was moderate quality (Fig. 3 and Table 5).

Two reviews [71, 72] were about the relationship between cognitive impairment and treatment. One of these reviews [71] was about the effect of cognitive impairment on response to different forms of treatment. In this review, it was shown that improvement over several evidence-based treatments were linked with baseline measures of cognitive function. The other review was comparing the effect of typical and atypical antipsychotics in treating cognitive impairment in PWS [72]. This review reported that atypical antipsychotics significantly improved cognitive function compared to conventional antipsychotics. This review also concluded that the studies that were included had many methodological limitations that one needs to consider in interpreting the findings (Fig. 3 and Table 5).

One of the reviews we included [73] dealt with the association between cognitive impairment and age. The study concluded that age had direct effect on the score of mini-mental state examination (MMSE); for every four years increase in age of PWS, there was one MMSE point reduction (five times higher than the report in the general population). PWS living in institutions were more impaired compared to those who were living in the community. This was of a poor-quality review (Fig. 3 and Table 5).

The other review included under this section examined the association between cognitive impairment in PWS and aggressive behaviour [74]. This moderate-quality review concluded that cognitive impairment in PWS exerted a significant risk for aggression (Fig. 3 and Table 5).

Discussion

Multiple studies have been published on the magnitude of cognitive impairment in PWS and associated factors. We were able to identify 63 independent reviews, and the majority addressed several neurocognitive domains. Most of the reviews used PubMed and PsycINFO databases to search their included studies. Methodologically, the majority of the studies were of moderate quality.

Of the 63 reviews we identified, some (n = 24) were included in a previous umbrella review of the magnitude of cognitive dysfunction in PWS and people with bipolar disorder [9]. The difference between our review and this previous umbrella review is that, the former only included meta-analysis studies while our review included both meta-analysis and systematic reviews. This previous review was not specific to cognitive impairment in PWS rather it was an umbrella review of cognitive impairment in PWS and people with bipolar disorder. It also focused more on comparing the domains of cognitive impairment in PWS and people with bipolar disorder. The previous review only included meta-analysis of neurocognitive domains, ignoring social cognitive domains, which is considered to be an important predictor of functionality. Finally, the previous review was not comprehensive (searched PubMed only) and the last date of search for the previous umbrella review was on August 10th 2015 (5 years older than ours). These facts make our review more comprehensive compared to it.

Even though the area is extensively researched, none of the included reviews reported the problem from low and middle-income countries (LMICs), separately. Since there are reports which show that cognition can be affected by nutritional status [75], we expect different results in high-income countries compared to that of LMICs. This calls for a separate analysis of the magnitude of cognitive impairment and associated factors based on income level of countries. One of the reasons for the lack of separate analysis may be scarcity of studies from LMICs. Therefore, conducting studies focusing on the magnitude, associated factors, and other interventional studies might be worth considering.

In this umbrella review, we found that PWS have more severe cognitive impairment compared to both healthy controls, people with bipolar disorder, and people with other affective disorders, particularly in the domains of processing speed, verbal memory, and working memory. We also found that the magnitude of cognitive impairment is more or less the same in PWS who were drug free and first episode psychosis, and those who had taken pharmacological treatment. Even though the results were mixed, most of the reviews we included point toward no decline of cognitive impairment in PWS over time. These findings support the hypothesis that cognitive impairment is one of the core symptoms of schizophrenia even at the initial phase of the illness.

This review of reviews showed that cognitive impairment in PWS has significant association with a number of factors, including functionality, substance use, insight, symptom dimensions, history of childhood trauma, older age, and aggressive behaviour. Hence, we recommend future researchers to consider these factors in conducting a study related to factors associated with cognitive impairment in PWS.

This umbrella review is particularly useful for researchers, clinicians, and experts in the area. The implication of this review for research is that it would help researchers in identifying a summary of factors that are potentially associated with cognitive impairment in PWS when planning studies. This study will also have implication for interventional studies that future researchers may design focusing on domains reported to be impaired. Another potential implication of this review is for experts in the area. Experts can refer this review in designing policy and strategies related to cognition in PWS. Clinicians can refer to this review to have a comprehensive understanding of domains of cognition affected and factors associated with cognitive impairment in PWS.

This review has a number of strengths in that we followed PRISMA guideline in conducting and reporting the review. Our search can be considered more comprehensive compared to the one conducted before since we included four databases without restriction on the date of publication, and we also conducted forward and backward searching. Furthermore, we assessed the quality of each of the included reviews, and whenever possible results were presented separately considering the methodological quality of the reviews included.

However, our review is not free from limitations. First, the broad scope of the review made the data analysis difficult, particularly to conduct meta-analysis. The broad scope of the review allowed a wide variety of study outcomes to be included and hence made the review hard to follow. Second, non-English studies were excluded, which might limit the generalizability of the findings. Finally, the grey literature was not searched; however, we used Google Scholar for our forward and backward searching.

Conclusions

This umbrella review highlights cognition as an important factor contributing to functionality and insight in PWS. Considering the presence of 63 different reviews and no review looked for cognitive difference across the economic classification, we encourage reviews that compare cognitive impairment in PWS at different income settings. From this umbrella review, one can conclude that cognitive impairment is a core symptom in PWS and different factors such as functionality, insight, history of childhood trauma, age, and aggressive behavior have significant association with cognitive impairment in this population group.

Supplementary Information

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Acknowledgements

Our special thanks goes to the staff of the Department of Psychiatry, Addis Ababa University, who provided valuable comments on different occasions. We also would like to thank Debre Berhan University for sponsoring the primary investigator to conduct this review. Finally, we would like to thank the African Mental Health Research Initiative (AMARI), because this work was supported by the DELTAS Africa Initiative [DEL-15-01] through the first author (YG). The DELTAS Africa Initiative is an independent funding scheme of the African Academy of Sciences (AAS)’s Alliance for Accelerating Excellence in Science in Africa (AESA) and supported by the New Partnership for Africa’s Development Planning and Coordinating Agency (NEPAD Agency) with funding from the Wellcome Trust [DEL-15-01] and the UK government. The views expressed in this publication are those of the author(s) and not necessarily those of AAS, NEPAD Agency, WellcomeTrust or the UK government.

Abbreviations

AMSTAR

A measurement tool to assess methodological quality of systematic review

DSM

Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders

ICD

International classification of diseases

IQ

Intelligence quotient

LMIC

Low- and middle-income countries

MATRICS

Measurement and treatment research to improve cognition in schizophrenia initiative

MMSE

Mini-mental state examination

PANSS

Positive and negative syndrome scale

PRISMA

Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses

PWS

People with schizophrenia

Author contributions

YG, AA, KH, and MC: conceived and designed the study. YG and AM: involved in screening, extraction, quality assessment and drafting the manuscript. All the authors have read the manuscript and have given their final approval for publication.

Funding

This work was supported by the DELTAS Africa Initiative [DEL-15-01] through the first author (YG). The DELTAS Africa Initiative is an independent funding scheme of the African Academy of Sciences (AAS)’s Alliance for Accelerating Excellence in Science in Africa (AESA) and supported by the New Partnership for Africa’s Development Planning and Coordinating Agency (NEPAD Agency) with funding from the Wellcome Trust [DEL-15-01] and the UK government. The views expressed in this publication are those of the author(s) and not necessarily those of AAS, NEPAD Agency, Wellcome Trust or the UK government. The funder has no role in the interpretation of findings and decision for publication.

Data availability

All the data used are made available in the manuscript and supplementary materials.

Code availability

Not applicable.

Declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical approval

Not applicable.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

References

  • 1.Shahrokh NC, Hales RE, Phillips KA, Yudofsky SC (2011) The language of mental health: a glossary of psychiatric terms. American Psychiatric Publishing
  • 2.Keefe RS, Eesley CE, Poe MP. Defining a cognitive function decrement in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiat. 2005;57(6):688–691. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.01.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Heinrichs RW, Zakzanis KK. Neurocognitive deficit in schizophrenia: a quantitative review of the evidence. Neuropsychology. 1998;12(3):426. doi: 10.1037//0894-4105.12.3.426. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Fatouros-Bergman H, et al. Meta-analysis of cognitive performance in drug-naïve patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2014;158(1–3):156–162. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2014.06.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.American Psychiatric Association, D. S., and American Psychiatric Association (2013) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5, vol 5. American psychiatric association, Washington, DC
  • 6.Marder SR, Fenton W. Measurement and treatment research to improve cognition in schizophrenia: NIMH MATRICS initiative to support the development of agents for improving cognition in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2004;72(1):5–9. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2004.09.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Carter CS, Barch DM. Cognitive neuroscience-based approaches to measuring and improving treatment effects on cognition in schizophrenia: the CNTRICS initiative. Schizophr Bull. 2007;33(5):1131–1137. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbm081. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Nuechterlein KH, et al. Identification of separable cognitive factors in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2004;72(1):29–39. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2004.09.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Bortolato B, et al. Cognitive dysfunction in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia: a systematic review of meta-analyses. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2015;11:3111. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S76700. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.World Health Organization (2004) ICD-10: international statistical classification of diseases and related health problems: tenth revision. World Health Organization
  • 11.Moher D, et al. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. PLoS Med. 2009;6(7):e1000097. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Aromataris E, Pearson A. The systematic review: an overview. AJN The Am J Nurs. 2014;114(3):53–58. doi: 10.1097/01.NAJ.0000444496.24228.2c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Shea BJ, et al. Development of AMSTAR: a measurement tool to assess the methodological quality of systematic reviews. BMC Med Res Methodol. 2007;7:10. doi: 10.1186/1471-2288-7-10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Piskulic D, et al. Behavioural studies of spatial working memory dysfunction in schizophrenia: a quantitative literature review. Psychiatry Res. 2007;150(2):111–121. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2006.03.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Aleman A, et al. Memory impairment in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Am J Psychiatry. 1999;156(9):1358–1366. doi: 10.1176/ajp.156.9.1358. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Lee J, Park S. Working memory impairments in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. J Abnorm Psychol. 2005;114(4):599. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.114.4.599. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Pelletier M, et al. Cognitive and clinical moderators of recognition memory in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Schizophr Res. 2005;74(2–3):233–252. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2004.08.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Doughty O, Done D. Is semantic memory impaired in schizophrenia? A systematic review and meta-analysis of 91 studies. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. 2009;14(6):473–509. doi: 10.1080/13546800903073291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Johnson-Selfridge M, Zalewski C. Moderator variables of executive functioning in schizophrenia: meta-analytic findings. Schizophr Bull. 2001;27(2):305–316. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a006876. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Dibben C, et al. Is executive impairment associated with schizophrenic syndromes? A Meta-Anal Psychological Med. 2009;39(3):381–392. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708003887. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Li C-SR. Do schizophrenia patients make more perseverative than non-perseverative errors on the wisconsin card sorting test? A Meta-Anal Study Psychiatry Res. 2004;129(2):179–190. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2004.06.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Thai ML, Andreassen AK, Bliksted V. A meta-analysis of executive dysfunction in patients with schizophrenia: different degree of impairment in the ecological subdomains of the behavioural assessment of the dysexecutive syndrome. Psychiatry Res. 2019;272:230–236. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.12.088. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Knowles EE, David AS, Reichenberg A. Processing speed deficits in schizophrenia: reexamining the evidence. Am J Psychiatry. 2010;167(7):828–835. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09070937. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Dickinson D, Ramsey ME, Gold JM. Overlooking the obvious: a meta-analytic comparison of digit symbol coding tasks and other cognitive measures in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64(5):532–542. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.5.532. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Bokat CE, Goldberg TE. Letter and category fluency in schizophrenic patients: a meta-analysis. Schizophr Res. 2003;64(1):73–78. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(02)00282-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Henry J, Crawford J. A meta-analytic review of verbal fluency deficits in schizophrenia relative to other neurocognitive deficits. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. 2005;10(1):1–33. doi: 10.1080/13546800344000309. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Savla GN, et al. Deficits in domains of social cognition in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis of the empirical evidence. Schizophr Bull. 2013;39(5):979–992. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbs080. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Javed A, Charles A. The importance of social cognition in improving functional outcomes in schizophrenia. Front Psych. 2018;9:157. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00157. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Trotta A, Murray R, MacCabe J. Do premorbid and post-onset cognitive functioning differ between schizophrenia and bipolar disorder? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol Med. 2015;45(2):381–394. doi: 10.1017/S0033291714001512. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Bora E, Yucel M, Pantelis C. Cognitive functioning in schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder and affective psychoses: meta-analytic study. Br J Psychiatry. 2009;195(6):475–482. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.108.055731. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Krabbendam L, et al. Cognitive functioning in patients with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder: a quantitative review. Schizophr Res. 2005;80(2–3):137–149. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2005.08.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Stefanopoulou E, et al. Cognitive functioning in patients with affective disorders and schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Int Rev Psychiatry. 2009;21(4):336–356. doi: 10.1080/09540260902962149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Kuswanto C, et al. Shared and divergent neurocognitive impairments in adult patients with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder: whither the evidence? Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2016;61:66–89. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.12.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Schaefer J, et al. The global cognitive impairment in schizophrenia: consistent over decades and around the world. Schizophr Res. 2013;150(1):42–50. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2013.07.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Fioravanti M, Bianchi V, Cinti ME. Cognitive deficits in schizophrenia: an updated metanalysis of the scientific evidence. BMC Psychiatry. 2012;12(1):64. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-12-64. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Fioravanti M, et al. A meta-analysis of cognitive deficits in adults with a diagnosis of schizophrenia. Neuropsychol Rev. 2005;15(2):73–95. doi: 10.1007/s11065-005-6254-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Mesholam-Gately RI, et al. Neurocognition in first-episode schizophrenia: a meta-analytic review. Neuropsychology. 2009;23(3):315. doi: 10.1037/a0014708. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Rajji T, Ismail Z, Mulsant B. Age at onset and cognition in schizophrenia: meta-analysis. Br J Psychiatry. 2009;195(4):286–293. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.108.060723. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Zhang H, et al. Meta-analysis of cognitive function in Chinese first-episode schizophrenia: MATRICS consensus cognitive battery (MCCB) profile of impairment. Gen Psychiatry. 2019;32(3):e100043. doi: 10.1136/gpsych-2018-100043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Aas M, et al. A systematic review of cognitive function in first-episode psychosis, including a discussion on childhood trauma, stress, and inflammation. Front Psych. 2014;4:182. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00182. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Bora E, Pantelis C. Meta-analysis of cognitive impairment in first-episode bipolar disorder: comparison with first-episode schizophrenia and healthy controls. Schizophr Bull. 2015;41(5):1095–1104. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbu198. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Thibaudeau E, Achim AM, Parent C, Turcotte M, Cellard C. A meta-analysis of the associations between theory of mind and neurocognition in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2019;216:118–128. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2019.12.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Pickup GJ. Relationship between theory of mind and executive function in schizophrenia: a systematic review. Psychopathology. 2008;41(4):206–213. doi: 10.1159/000125554. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Shah JN, et al. Is there evidence for late cognitive decline in chronic schizophrenia? Psychiatr Q. 2012;83(2):127–144. doi: 10.1007/s11126-011-9189-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Irani F, et al. Neuropsychological performance in older patients with schizophrenia: a meta-analysis of cross-sectional and longitudinal studies. Schizophr Bull. 2010;37(6):1318–1326. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbq057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Bozikas VP, Andreou C. Longitudinal studies of cognition in first episode psychosis: a systematic review of the literature. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2011;45(2):93–108. doi: 10.3109/00048674.2010.541418. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Szöke A, et al. Longitudinal studies of cognition in schizophrenia: meta-analysis. Br J Psychiatry. 2008;192(4):248–257. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.106.029009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Woodberry KA, Giuliano AJ, Seidman LJ. Premorbid IQ in schizophrenia: a meta-analytic review. Am J Psychiatry. 2008;165(5):579–587. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.07081242. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Khandaker GM, et al. A quantitative meta-analysis of population-based studies of premorbid intelligence and schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2011;132(2–3):220–227. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2011.06.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Allott K, et al. Cognition at illness onset as a predictor of later functional outcome in early psychosis: systematic review and methodological critique. Schizophr Res. 2011;125(2–3):221–235. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2010.11.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Christensen TØ. The influence of neurocognitive dysfunctions on work capacity in schizophrenia patients: a systematic review of the literature. Int J Psychiatry Clin Pract. 2007;11(2):89–101. doi: 10.1080/13651500600969061. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Couture SM, Penn DL, Roberts DL. The functional significance of social cognition in schizophrenia: a review. Schizophr Bull. 2006;32(suppl_1):S44–S63. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbl029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Fett A-KJ, et al. The relationship between neurocognition and social cognition with functional outcomes in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2011;35(3):573–588. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.07.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Halverson TF, et al. Pathways to functional outcomes in schizophrenia spectrum disorders: meta-analysis of social cognitive and neurocognitive predictors. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2019;105:212–219. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.07.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Schmidt SJ, Mueller DR, Roder V. Social cognition as a mediator variable between neurocognition and functional outcome in schizophrenia: empirical review and new results by structural equation modeling. Schizophr Bull. 2011;37(suppl_2):S41–S54. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbr079. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Rajji TK, Miranda D, Mulsant BH. Cognition, function, and disability in patients with schizophrenia: a review of longitudinal studies. The Canadian J Psychiatry. 2014;59(1):13–17. doi: 10.1177/070674371405900104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Donoghue K, Doody GA. Effect of illegal substance use on cognitive function in individuals with a psychotic disorder: a review and meta-analysis. Neuropsychology. 2012;26(6):785–801. doi: 10.1037/a0029685. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Yücel M, et al. The impact of cannabis use on cognitive functioning in patients with schizophrenia: a meta-analysis of existing findings and new data in a first-episode sample. Schizophr Bull. 2010;38(2):316–330. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbq079. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Coulston CM, Perdices M, Tennant CC. The neuropsychology of cannabis and other substance use in schizophrenia: review of the literature and critical evaluation of methodological issues. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2007;41(11):869–884. doi: 10.1080/00048670701634952. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Potvin S, et al. Contradictory cognitive capacities among substance-abusing patients with schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Schizophr Res. 2008;100(1–3):242–251. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.04.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Agrawal N, et al. Insight in psychosis and neuropsychological function: a meta-analysis. Schizophr Res. 2003;1(60):121. [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Cooke M, et al. Disease, deficit or denial? Models of poor insight in psychosis. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2005;112(1):4–17. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2005.00537.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Nair A, et al. Relationship between cognition, clinical and cognitive insight in psychotic disorders: a review and meta-analysis. Schizophr Res. 2014;152(1):191–200. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2013.11.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Subotnik KL, Ventura J, Hellemann GS, Zito MF, Agee ER, Nuechterlein KH. Relationship of poor insight to neurocognition, social cognition, and psychiatric symptoms in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Schizophr Res. 2020;220:164–171. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2020.03.038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Dominguez Mde G, et al. Are psychotic psychopathology and neurocognition orthogonal? A systematic review of their associations. Psychol Bull. 2009;135(1):157–171. doi: 10.1037/a0014415. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Nieuwenstein MR, Aleman A, de Haan EH. Relationship between symptom dimensions and neurocognitive functioning in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis of WCST and CPT studies. J Psychiatr Res. 2001;35(2):119–125. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3956(01)00014-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Allott K, et al. Duration of untreated psychosis and neurocognitive functioning in first-episode psychosis: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol Med. 2018;48(10):1592–1607. doi: 10.1017/S0033291717003002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Bora E, et al. Duration of untreated psychosis and neurocognition in first-episode psychosis: a meta-analysis. Schizophr Res. 2018;193:3–10. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2017.06.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Dauvermann MR, Donohoe G. The role of childhood trauma in cognitive performance in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder–A systematic review. Schizophr Res: Cognit. 2019;16:1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.scog.2018.11.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Vargas T, et al. Childhood trauma and neurocognition in adults with psychotic disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Schizophr Bull. 2019;45(6):1195–1208. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sby150. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Kurtz MM. Neurocognition as a predictor of response to evidence-based psychosocial interventions in schizophrenia: what is the state of the evidence? Clin Psychol Rev. 2011;31(4):663–672. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2011.02.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Keefe RS, et al. The effects of atypical antipsychotic drugs on neurocognitive impairment in schizophrenia: a review and meta-analysis. Schizophr Bull. 1999;25(2):201–222. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a033374. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Maltais J-R, et al. Correlation between age and MMSE in schizophrenia. Int Psychogeriatr. 2015;27(11):1769–1775. doi: 10.1017/S1041610215000459. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Reinharth J, et al. Cognitive predictors of violence in schizophrenia: a meta-analytic review. Schizophr Res: Cognit. 2014;1(2):101–111. [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Kerac M, Postels DG, Mallewa M, Jalloh AA, Voskuijl WP, Groce N, Gladstone M, Molyneux E (2014) The interaction of malnutrition and neurologic disability in Africa. In: Seminars in pediatric neurology, vol 21, no 1. WB Saunders, pp 42–49 [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 76.Bowie CR, Harvey PD. Cognition in schizophrenia: impairments, determinants, and functional importance. Psychiatr Clin North Am. 2005;28(3):613–633. doi: 10.1016/j.psc.2005.05.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Braff DL. Information processing and attention dysfunctions in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 1993;19(2):233–259. doi: 10.1093/schbul/19.2.233. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Kayman DJ, Goldstein MF. Cognitive deficits in schizophrenia. Curr Transl Geriatr Gerontol Rep. 2012;1(1):45–52. [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Data Availability Statement

All the data used are made available in the manuscript and supplementary materials.

Not applicable.


Articles from European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Springer

RESOURCES