Skip to main content
PLOS One logoLink to PLOS One
. 2022 Sep 29;17(9):e0273819. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0273819

Iron deficiency and common neurodevelopmental disorders—A scoping review

Scout McWilliams 1,#, Ishmeet Singh 1,#, Wayne Leung 1, Sylvia Stockler 1,2,3,*, Osman S Ipsiroglu 1,2,4
Editor: Efthimios M C Skoulakis5
PMCID: PMC9522276  PMID: 36173945

Abstract

Background

A wealth of human and experimental studies document a causal and aggravating role of iron deficiency in neurodevelopmental disorders. While pre-, peri-, and early postnatal iron deficiency sets the stage for the risk of developing neurodevelopmental disorders, iron deficiency acquired at later ages aggravates pre-existing neurodevelopmental disorders. Yet, the association of iron deficiency and neurodevelopmental disorders in childhood and adolescence has not yet been explored comprehensively.

In this scoping review, we investigate 1) the association of iron deficiency in children and adolescents with the most frequent neurodevelopmental disorders, ADHD, ASD, and FASD, and 2) whether iron supplementation improves outcomes in these disorders.

Method

Scoping review of studies published between 1994 and 2021 using “iron deficiency / iron deficiency anemia” AND “ADHD” OR “autism” OR “FASD” in four biomedical databases. The main inclusion criterion was that articles needed to have quantitative determination of iron status at any postnatal age with primary iron markers such as serum ferritin being reported in association with ADHD, ASD, or FASD.

Results

For ADHD, 22/30 studies and 4/4 systematic reviews showed an association of ADHD occurrence or severity with iron deficiency; 6/6 treatment studies including 2 randomized controlled trials demonstrated positive effects of iron supplementation. For ASD, 3/6 studies showed an association with iron deficiency, while 3/6 and 1/1 systematic literature review did not; 4 studies showed a variety of prevalence rates of iron deficiency in ASD populations; 1 randomized controlled trial found no positive effect of iron supplementation on behavioural symptoms of ASD. For FASD, 2/2 studies showed an association of iron deficiency with growth retardation in infants and children with prenatal alcohol exposure.

Conclusion

Evidence in favor of screening for iron deficiency and using iron supplementation for pediatric neurodevelopmental disorders comes primarily from ADHD studies and needs to be further investigated for ASD and FASD. Further analysis of study methodologies employed and populations investigated is needed to compare studies against each other and further substantiate the evidence created.

Introduction

Neurodevelopmental disorders (NDDs) are a leading cause of morbidity in children, characterized by impairments in cognitive, adaptive and social functioning, and resulting in large burdens to patients, caregivers, and society [1]. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), autism spectrum disorder (ASD), and fetal alcohol spectrum disorder (FASD) are currently the most frequently diagnosed NDDs. Notably, ADHD and ASD are increasingly seen in high-income countries such as Canada and the United States [2].

ADHD is defined as “impairing levels of inattention, disorganization, and/or hyperactivity- impulsivity” and is prevalent in up to 7% of children and adolescents [3]. ASD is characterized by persistent deficits in social and emotional interaction across multiple contexts, hampering normal development in young children and frequently progressing to mental health conditions in adulthood. The prevalence of ASD is 2.5% among US children [4]. FASD is a spectrum of conditions related to prenatal alcohol exposure, which results in physical, behavioural, and cognitive impairments. The global prevalence of FASD among children and youth in the general population is estimated to be 0.8%, but ranges significantly between countries and certain subpopulations with peak prevalence rates between 5 to 11% [5].

Iron deficiency (ID) has been positively associated with these NDDs with the common underlying pathophysiology being derived from iron’s central role in the brain as a cofactor in neurotransmitter (dopamine and serotonin) synthesis, as well as in ATP production and myelination [6]. The impact of ID on the development and severity of NDDs depends on the developmental stage during which the brain is exposed to iron depletion. Intrauterine / maternal and early postnatal ID is likely to impact the development of brain structures, resulting in irreversible neurocognitive and behavioural deficits as the end point of the pathophysiological cascade [7]. The hippocampus appears particularly vulnerable, resulting in persistent hippocampus-based cognitive deficits in adulthood despite iron supplementation [8, 9]. Moreover, a recent study found that iron deficiency anemia (IDA) diagnosed earlier in pregnancy was associated with an increased risk of the development of ASD, ADHD, and particularly ID in offspring [10].

There is also evidence that postnatal ID may aggravate pre-existing NDDs. For ADHD, studies have shown that ID plays a role in the occurrence and the severity of hyperactive day and nighttime symptomology [1114], and that iron supplementation is beneficial in the treatment of affected children [15]. Children with ASD are at particular risk for ID due to co-occurring selective food intake, food sensitivities, and gastrointestinal problems. Indeed, studies in children with ASD suggest a high prevalence of ID [16, 17] and a positive effect of iron supplementation on sleep, one of the most debilitating morbidities in patients on the spectrum [18]. For FASD, experimental studies suggest a role of brain specific ID in FASD modulated by iron status of the dam [19], and observational studies support the similarities of behavioural phenotypes exhibiting challenging day and nighttime behaviours, including sleep [20].

According to the World Health Organization, ID is the most common and widespread micronutrient deficiency worldwide, affecting more than 30% of the world’s population [21, 22]. However, despite all of the evidence outlined above, ID is not routinely investigated in patients with NDDs, including ADHD, ASD, and FASD, and iron supplementation has not been considered as a potentially effective treatment strategy.

We performed a scoping review with a twofold aim: first to assess existing evidence of an etiologic relationship between postnatal ID and prevalence and severity of ADHD, ASD, and FASD; second to determine effects of iron supplementation on outcomes of these NDDs. Furthermore, we were interested in the types of iron markers used for the identification of ID.

Methods

Literature search

We performed a literature search in the electronic databases MEDLINE (OVID), CINAHL (EBSCOhost), PsycINFO (EBSCOhost), and EMBASE (OVID) from 1994 to 2017, using the search terms “iron deficiency an(a)emia” AND “ADHD” OR “autism” OR “FASD” (Table 1), on February 3rd, 2017.

Table 1. Detailed search strategies used in each database.

Medline (OVID) Embase (OVID) CINAHL (EBSCOhost) PsycINFO (EBSCOhost)
Iron Anemia, Iron deficiency/ OR iron metabolism disorders/ OR iron/bl [blood] OR iron/df [deficiency] OR exp ferritins/bl, df [blood, deficiency] exp iron deficiency anemia/ OR (iron adj2 deficiency) OR (iron adj2 deficient) MH “Anemia, Iron Deficiency” OR MH “ferritin/BL/DF” OR MH ‘Iron/DF” OR anemia OR anaemia OR iron deficiency DE “iron” OR DE “anemia” OR iron status OR ferritin OR ((anemia OR anaemia) AND iron deficiency)
ADHD exp “Attention deficit and disruptive behaviour disorders”/ attention deficit OR ADHD OR attention deficit disorder/ OR (attention deficit or ADHD) MH “attention deficit hyperactivity disorder” OR adhd OR attention deficit DE “Attention deficit disorder with hyperactivity” OR attention deficit OR ADHD
ASD exp Child Development Disorders, Pervasive/ OR autism OR autistic OR ASD exp autism/ OR (autism or autistic or ASD) MH “autistic disorder” OR autism OR autistic OR asd OR autism spectrum disorder OR DE “Autism spectrum disorders” OR DE autism OR autistic OR ASD
FASD Prenatal exposure, delayed effects/ OR fetal alcohol spectrum disorders/ OR prenatal alcohol exposure OR PAE OR fetal alcohol syndrome OR foetal alcohol syndrome OR fetal alcohol spectrum OR foetal alcohol spectrum OR FASD fetal alcohol syndrome/ OR prenatal alcohol exposure OR PAE OR fetal alcohol syndrome OR foetal alcohol syndrome OR fetal alcohol spectrum OR foetal alcohol spectrum OR FASD MH “Fetal alcohol syndrome” OR ((fetal OR foetal) AND alcohol) OR prenatal alcohol exposure OR PAE OR FASD DE “fetal alcohol syndrome” OR prenatal alcohol exposure OR PAE OR fetal alcohol syndrome OR foetal alcohol syndrome OR fetal alcohol spectrum OR foetal alcohol spectrum OR FASD

Bolded words are subject headings.

We updated our review in July 2019 by performing a literature search in the electronic databases MEDLINE (OVID), CINAHL (EBSCOhost), PsycINFO (EBSCOhost), and EMBASE (OVID) from January 1st 2016 to July 20th, 2019 using the same search terms “iron deficiency anemia” AND “ADHD” OR “autism” OR “FASD”. In order to optimize the yield for treatment studies, we amended these original search terms by “iron supplementation” OR “iron substitution” OR iron replacement” OR “iron fortification” OR “micronutrient”, but no additional references were identified. We repeated the entire search in August, 2020, and again in May, 2021 to provide the most updated information by the time of acceptance for publication. Here we have combined the results of all these searches.

Inclusion and exclusion criteria

The inclusion criteria for this review were 1) the study discusses the iron status of patients in association with their pre-existing diagnosis of ADHD, ASD, FASD; 2) the iron status is determined quantitatively using one or more iron markers that are primarily reflecting iron metabolism (e.g. serum ferritin (SF), transferrin saturation (TSAT), total iron binding capacity (TIBC) (primary iron markers)); 3) study methodologies including case reports, case series, case controls, cross-sectional studies, cohort studies, clinical trials, controlled clinical trials, randomized controlled trials (RCT), and systematic reviews.

References were excluded if: 1) subjects did not have iron deficient status reported; 2) iron status was not reported quantitatively using primary iron markers; 3) iron status of subjects was determined using only red blood cell (RBC)-related markers such as hemoglobin (Hb), mean corpuscular volume (MCV), or measurements of serum iron (SI) only; 4) the NDD was reported in association with prenatal / maternal / intrauterine ID; 5) study was not published in English; 6) full-length text was not available; 7) specific study types, including non-systematic reviews, dissertations, animal studies, correspondences, editorials, conference proceedings, health letters.

Study selection

Three investigators independently selected the studies (SM, IS, WL) and 3 investigators (SM, IS, WL) classified the studies. SS and OI were consulted to settle any disagreement on study selection.

Classification of study types

Studies identified after application of inclusion and exclusion criteria were subdivided:

  1. According to the NDD investigated: ADHD, ASD, FASD.

  2. Studies within each NDD were subdivided into:
    1. Association studies describing an association between ID and the respective NDD. Studies which described the frequency of ID within an NDD population, not supported by further statistical analysis, were termed as “prevalence studies”.
    2. Treatment studies describing the effects of iron supplementation on symptoms of the respective NDD.
    3. Systematic reviews.

Association studies were classified as case control, case series, cohort- and cross sectional studies [23]. A positive association was considered if a study showed a relationship between one of the 3 NDDs and the presence of ID indicated by at least one primary iron deficiency marker. No association was considered if the studies did not show a relationship between ID and one of the 3 NDDs. Association studies were rated as positive associations when an association between ID and an NDD was supported through statistical analysis such as 1) between subjects with and without the neurodevelopmental disorder, 2) between subjects with and without ID, 3) correlation between iron levels and symptom severity, 4) odds ratio showing increased odds of having a NDD with ID. Studies reporting rates of ID in individuals with an NDD without further correlation studies were separated as prevalence studies.

Treatment studies were categorized as experimental studies, which included clinical trials, controlled clinical trials, and RCTs; and non- experimental studies, which included observational case series, and case reports. Experimental studies were analyzed for methodological quality using the Jadad scale [24], in which one point was awarded for each of 1) through 3). One additional point was awarded if the study fulfilled each of 4) or 5), while and one point was deduced for 6) and 7), yielding a maximum score of five.

  1. The study was described as randomized (this includes the use of words such as randomly, random, and randomization).

  2. The method used to generate the sequence of randomization was described and appropriate (e.g. use of computer program to generate random allocations).

  3. The study was described as double blinded.

  4. The method of double blinding was described and appropriate (e.g. use of identical placebo).

  5. The study included a description of withdrawals and dropouts.

  6. The method used to generate the sequence of randomization was described and inappropriate (patients were allocated based on where they lived or when they were born).

  7. The method of double blinding was described and inappropriate (e.g., identical placebo not used; study personnel and/or study participant could identify the intervention).

Systematic reviews were divided into those investigating the association of the various NDDs with ID, and reviews investigating the effects of iron supplementation.

Results

Overview of studies (Fig 1)

Fig 1. Scoping review flow chart.

Fig 1

Among the 54 studies selected for this review, 40 investigated ADHD, including 4 systematic reviews, 30 association studies and 6 treatment studies (2 RCTs, 3 case reports, 1 case series). 12 studies investigated ASD including 1 systematic review, 10 association studies (including 4 prevalence studies), and 1 treatment study (RCT). Two association but no treatment studies were found for FASD (Fig 1).

Overview of association studies

Among the 30 association studies identified for ADHD, 22 showed an association with ID and 8 did not. Among the 10 association studies identified for ASD, 3 showed an association with ID and 3 did not. 4 studies showed prevalence rates of ID within groups of patients with ASD without further statistical evaluation of associations / correlations. For FASD, both association studies identified an association of ID with growth, an FASD related manifestation. Numbers and types of studies, cases investigated as well as sex distribution are shown in Table 2.

Table 2. Overview of studies investigating the association of ID and an NDD and / or prevalence studies.

Association No Association Prevalence
Case control Case Series Cohort Cross Section Case control Case Series Cohort Cross Section
ADHD
Studies 12 8 1 1 4 3 1
Cases 1323 1568 1018* 205 251 458 2805*
Controls 1124 ---- ---- ---- 203 ---- ----
Cases m/f 746/576 1279/219 519/499 137/68 212/39 355/103 1429/1376
ASD
Studies 2 1 1 2 4
Cases 408 96 154 64 556
Controls 408 ---- 73 ---- ----
Cases m/f 237/171 78/18 141/13 54/10 462/93
FASD
Studies 2
Cases 127
Controls ----
Cases m/f 61/66

m/f = number of male/female patients.

Association = studies showing an association between NDD and ID; No Association = studies not showing an association between NDD and ID.

*total number of participants in cohort study.

Prevalence studies only reported about rates of ID in children with an NDD without further correlation studies.

Association ADHD and ID (Fig 2, S3 Table in S1 File and S4 Tables in S2 File)

Fig 2. Overview of ADHD association studies.

Fig 2

Pediatric/adolescent studies: Age 19 years or less; adult studies: Age 19 years or greater.

Overall, 30 association studies were identified. All but 1 study [25] were carried out in children/adolescents. The age of children/adolescents enrolled in the various studies was 4–19 years. The main instruments employed for the diagnosis and severity grading of ADHD were the Conners Parent Rating Scale (CPRS), used in 18/30 studies, the Conners Teacher Rating Scale (CTRS) used in 10/30 studies, and the Kiddie Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia (KSADS) used in 10/30 studies. The instruments used in the individual studies are indicated in S3 and S4 Tables in S2 File.

An association of ADHD and/or ADHD severity with ID was shown in 22/30 studies including 1 cohort study, 12 case control studies and 8 case series, and 1 cross-sectional. Overall, 1018 cases were enrolled in the only cohort study, 1323 cases and 1124 controls were enrolled in case control studies, 1568 cases were enrolled in the case series, and 205 records analyzed in the cross-sectional study. 16/22 studies investigated the degree of ID and the severity of ADHD symptoms, with greater severity being associated with lower iron levels [11, 14, 2538], and 10/22 studies concluded that SF levels were lower in the ADHD population [12, 13, 25, 3032, 36, 3941]. 10/22 studies showed an association with the hyperactivity symptoms of ADHD [25, 2729, 32, 33, 35, 37, 38, 42]. Oppositional defiant/conduct disorder (10/22 studies) was the most common comorbidity in the ADHD populations investigated [14, 2629, 31, 33, 34, 39, 42].

No association of ADHD and/or ADHD severity with ID was found in 8/30 studies (4 case control, 3 case series, and 1 cohort). 2805 cases were enrolled in the only cohort study showing no association between ADHD and ID [43], 251 cases and 203 controls were enrolled in the case control studies, and 458 cases were enrolled in case series studies. 7 studies investigated the degree of ID and severity of ADHD symptoms, with greater severity not being associated with lower iron levels [4349], and 4 studies concluded that SF levels were not lower in the ADHD population [44, 46, 49, 50]. 6 studies showed lack of association with the hyperactivity symptoms of ADHD [4346, 48, 49]. Oppositional defiant/conduct disorder (2/8 studies) was the most common comorbidity in the ADHD population investigated [44, 49].

Association ASD and ID (Fig 3, S4–S6 Tables in S2 File)

Fig 3. Overview of ASD association studies.

Fig 3

Pediatric/adolescent studies: Age 19 years or less; adult studies: Age 19 years or greater.

Overall, 6 studies investigated associations between ID and ASD and 4 additional case series investigated the prevalence rates of ID in children with ASD. All studies were done in children and adolescents aged 1–18 years. Instruments employed for ASD assessment included the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS) (4/10 studies), the Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS) (4/10 studies), Autism Behaviour Checklist (AuBC) (2/10 studies), and the Aberrant Behaviour Checklist (AbBC) (2/10 studies). The instruments used in the individual studies are indicated in S4–S6 Tables in S2 File.

3 studies (2 case control with 408 cases and 408 controls, and 1 case series with 96 cases enrolled) showed an association of ID and ASD and/or ASD-related symptoms. 3 studies (1 case control with 154 cases, 2 case series with 64 cases enrolled) did not. The 2 studies showing correlations between severity of ASD (as determined by the various scores used) and ID [17, 51] were contrasted by 3 other studies not showing such correlations [18, 52, 53]. The study by Dosman et al. [17] deserves a special explanation. The primary aim of this study was to investigate the effect of iron supplementation in children with ASD on sleep in an n-of-1 trial setting (each individual as their own control). As part of patient characterization at baseline, this study also analyzed the association between the degree of ID and ASD diagnostic scores. Because no association was found between SF levels and total ADOS or ADI-R scores we included these results in the no association category. Nonetheless, it must be mentioned that in a subgroup of school-aged children, there was an inverse relationship between SF and the ADOS sub domain communication (p = 0.009). We did not include this study as a treatment study, as none of the ASD-specific scores was used as an outcome measure.

Lastly, among the 4 studies reporting ID prevalence in children diagnosed with ASD (enrolling 556 participants), low SF levels were found in 6.6–24% [16, 5456].

Association FASD and ID

We found 2 case control studies, including 85 and 42 cases, and 63 and 54 controls respectively, investigating the effect of heavy prenatal alcohol exposure on postnatal growth and postnatal ID compared to moderate/light exposure [57, 58]. Both studies found that in infants with heavy prenatal alcohol exposure, the extent of growth restriction was associated with ID, whereas there was no further modification of growth if ID or food insecurity persisted to the age of 5 years [58]. One of these studies [57] also showed an association of maternal binge drinking with an increased incidence of ID anemia at the age of 12 months. This study formally did not meet our inclusion criteria as only RBC-related markers were used to determine ID. We still included this study because of the paucity of studies identified for FASD in general and its close relation to the other study identified [58].

Sex differences

Overall, there was a strong preponderance of male participants both in the ADHD and ASD studies (Table 2). The 30 ADHD association studies included 4677 males versus 2880 females (cases only). 12/30 studies examined potential sex differences in risk and severity of ADHD, while 6/30 studies examined potential sex differences in iron levels. 4 of these studies found that there were no significant differences between sex and ADHD symptoms [33, 39, 42, 47]. 5 studies found that males had more ADHD symptoms than females [11, 28, 34, 41, 50]. One study [26] compared iron status (iron sufficient, ID without anemia, ID anemia) and sex, and found a statistically significant difference (p<0.001) between the aforementioned groups. Similarly, Cortese et al. [14] reported lower serum ferritin levels in males compared to females, though this finding was not supported by any statistical analysis. The remaining 4 studies that examined sex differences and ID did not find any significant correlations [28, 39, 47, 50].

The 10 ASD association and prevalence studies included 972 males and 305 females (cases only). 2/10 studies examined potential sex differences in individuals with ASD. 1 study reported no significant differences between sex and ASD symptoms [16], while another study found a significant difference but did not elaborate on what this entailed [52]. 1 study analyzed differences between sex and iron status (including ferritin, MCV, hematocrit (Hct), hemoglobin), but did not find any statistically significant difference between males and females [16].

Iron markers used in ADHD and ASD association studies (Figs 4 and 5, S1 Table, S2, S3 Tables in S1 File, S4-S6 Tables in S2 File)

Fig 4. Overview of biomarkers used in ADHD association studies.

Fig 4

Fig 5. Overview of biomarkers used in ASD association studies.

Fig 5

Apart from 4 ADHD studies which measured only SF as a primary iron marker [4143, 46] and 1 ADHD study which measured only SF and SI [50], all studies measured both iron-related and RBC-related markers for ID. A significant association of SF, but not of RBC-related or other iron markers was found in 9 ADHD [14, 2832, 35, 37, 39] and 2 ASD studies [17, 54].

Overall, the most common indicators of statistically significant positive associations in studies were SF (n = 21) [1214, 17, 25, 2734, 36, 37, 3942, 48, 59], followed by Hb (n = 8) [12, 13, 17, 30, 33, 40, 51, 59], SI (n = 6) [12, 25, 37, 40, 51, 59], MCV (n = 5) [27, 33, 40, 51, 59], Hct (n = 2) [51, 59], and total iron binding capacity (TIBC) (n = 2) [25, 40]. Brain iron (BI) [39], mean corpuscular hemoglobina concentration (MCHC) [40], red cell distribution width (RDW) [34], sTfR/log ferritin index (SFI) [38], soluble transferrin receptor (sTfR) [38], and transferrin saturation (TSAT) [38], were indicators of a statistically significant positive association in 1 study each. The remaining studies which used the aformentioned biomarkers, but which did not represent a statistically significant positive association with ADHD or ASD are presented in Figs 4 and 5. Additional biomarkers used in the association studies include free erythrocyte protoporphyrin (FEP), transferrin (TF), and mean corpuscular hemoglobin (MCH).

In the 4 prevalence studies, the most commonly employed ID biomarkers were SF (n = 4), SI (n = 2), TSAT (n = 1), TIBC (n = 1), iron saturation (n = 1), Hb (n = 4), MCV (n = 2), Hct (n = 1), and RDW (n = 1) (Fig 5).

4 association studies [38, 43, 52, 59] measured c reactive protein (CRP) in conjunction with SF levels, while only 1 association study measured erythrocyte sedimentation rate (ESR) in addition to CRP [38]. The remainder of studies did not state the use of such inflammatory markers.

Serum ferritin (SF) cut off-values (S3 Table in S1 File, S4-S6 Tables in S2 File, S7, S8 Tables in S3 File)

Among the 30 ADHD studies, SF cut-off values were reported in 20 studies. 7 studies used <12 μg/L and 13 studies used a range between <7 and 45 μg/L as cut-off value depending on the age of participants and / or presence or absence of high CRP levels. Among the 10 ASD studies, SF cut off levels were reported in 9, with cut off at < 10 μg/L for preschoolers and <12 μg/L for older children. Studies which did not report SF cut-off values, investigated correlations between SF and severity of ADHD / ASD scores without defining a ferritin-based threshold for ID.

Fasting state for iron markers

Only 11 association studies identified that fasting blood samples were taken for measuring iron parameters [16, 28, 29, 31, 33, 35, 36, 40, 42, 45, 51]. In the remainder, no information on timing of blood sampling was given.

Treatment studies (S7, S8 Tables in S3 File)

Studies investigating the effect of iron supplementation were available for ADHD and ASD. No such intervention studies were found for FASD. Overall, for ADHD 6 studies (2 RCTs and 3 case reports, and 1 case series) investigated the effects of iron supplementation. All of them demonstrated a benefit in treating symptoms with supplemental iron [15, 6064], which includes 2 RCTs [15, 60].

The first RCT [15] included 22 (17 male, 5 female) non-anemic children (17 treated; 5 placebo) who had SF levels <30 ng/mL. While there was no significant improvement in the primary outcome measure (CPRS) after 12 weeks of oral ferrous sulfate supplementation (80 mg/d) in the treatment group, there was a significant improvement of the ADHD Rating Scale (ADHD-RS), which was chosen as a secondary outcome measure. Both the hyperactivity and the inattention subscales improved, but the improvement was more pronounced in the inattention subscale. A similar result was observed in the second RCT, which included 42 children (35 male, 7 female) treated with methylphenidate and serum ferritin levels <30 ng/mL [60]. 21 children (16 male, 5 female) were treated with additional ferrous sulphate, 21 served as controls (19 male, 2 female). The study showed that the addition of ferrous sulfate resulted in an increase of SF levels and improvements in both hyperactivity and inattention symptomatology, measured using Child Symptoms Inventory-4 (CSI-4).

For ASD, 1 RCT was found, which included 20 children (17 male, 3 female) with insomnia. 9 children (6 male, 3 female) were treated with ferrous sulfate, 11 served as controls [65]. No significant differences were found in behaviour, as was measured using the SNAP-IV scale.

Systematic reviews

We identified 5 systematic reviews, of which 4 investigated the association of iron indices (SF) with ADHD [6669] and 1 with ASD [70]. The numbers and types of studies reviewed, and the conclusions are shown in Table 3. Overall, the 3 systematic reviews on ADHD and ID concluded that there is an association with SF levels (lower levels being associated with ADHD), while one systematic review concluded that the association between ADHD pathophysiology and ID has more to do with brain, rather than systemic iron [69]. The ASD study found no difference compared to controls. We did not identify any systematic literature review investigating the efficacy of iron supplementation.

Table 3. Systematic literature reviews analyzing the role of iron deficiency in ADHD and ASD.

Systematic reviews
N = 5
Reference Review Conclusion Comments
Scassellati et al ADHD Descriptive Not significant after Bonferroni correction
2012 [66] Systematic Review SF is a potential biomarker for ADHD
N = 7
case control studies Meta-analysis
SF is lower in patients with ADHD
Wang et al ADHD Descriptive
2017 [67] Systematic Review SF is lower in patients with ADHD
N = 11
case control studies
Cortese et al ADHD Descriptive No correlation with SI
2012 [68] Systematic Review 22 studies reviewed used SF as biomarker for ID;
N = 22
Cohort, case control, cross-sectional, clinical trials, RCTs Significant correlation between low SF and ADHD in 9/22 studies using SF as biomarker
Tseng et al ASD Descriptive
2018 [70] Scoping Review No association of iron indices (including SF) with ASD
N = 25
Cohort and cross sectional studies
Multiple iron indices (SF< SI< TSAT, TIBC, hair iron, iron in food) Meta-analysis
Levels of SF in children with ASD did not differ significantly from children without ASD
Degremont et al ADHD Descriptive
2021 [69] Systematic Review 18 studies measured SF; 10/18 found higher SF in those with ADHD compared to controls
N = 20
Case control
10 studies measured SI; 2/10 reported lower SI in the ADHD group
3 studies measured brain iron; all reported lower brain iron in the ADHD group
Our review ADHD, ASD, FASD Descriptive Heterogeneity of study designs, outcome measures and study participants.
Association with ID
Scoping Review ADHD 22/30
Association and treatment studies ASD 3/6
FASD 2/2
Positive effect of iron supplementation
ADHD 5/5 (2RCTs, 5 case reports)
No effect of iron supplementation
ASD 1/1 (RCT)

Year and location of studies

Association studies: The majority of the association studies (n = 19) were conducted in Europe [14, 16, 25, 28, 29, 3237, 39, 4244, 48, 51, 53, 54], followed by 7 in Asia [12, 31, 38, 40, 47, 52, 59], 6 in North America [11, 17, 18, 27, 49, 56], 5 in Africa [13, 30, 41, 46, 50], 2 in South America [26, 45], and 1 in Australia [55]. Moreover, these studies were predominantly (29/40) conducted in the 2010 decade [1113, 16, 2528, 30, 31, 34, 35, 3948, 5053, 55, 56, 59] with the remaining conducted in the 2020 [37, 38] and 2000 decades [14, 17, 18, 29, 32, 33, 36, 49, 54].

Treatment studies: The majority of the treatment studies were conducted in North America [61, 64, 65], followed by 2 in Europe [15, 62], and 2 in Asia [60, 63]. Moreover, 1 study was conducted in the 2020 decade [65], 2 conducted in the 2010 decade [60, 64], 2 conducted in the 2000 decade [15, 62], and 1 conducted in the 1990 decade [61].

Discussion

We conducted this scoping review to obtain an overview of the role of ID in ADHD, ASD, and FASD. We chose these conditions not only because they represent the most frequently diagnosed NDDs in children, but also because they have the tendency to occur comorbidly (e.g. children with ASD often have phenotypical characteristics of ADHD and children with FASD often have phenotypical characteristics of ADHD and ASD). Furthermore, these conditions occur in combination with intellectual disability, which is prevalent in up to 3% of the population worldwide. While causal treatment is available only for a minority of intellectual disabilities [71], symptomatic treatment aiming to improve behaviours is a prerequisite to optimize residual developmental outcomes. The common clinical characteristics of the three NDDs investigated here are hypermotor restlessness and hyperarousability not only during the wake, but also during the sleep state, and hypoarousability during performance of cognitive tasks characterizing the circumscribed behavioural phenotypes (H-behaviours) [20, 72]. Together, these clinical characteristics suggest a common pathophysiology of the dopaminergic system for which iron is an important metabolic cofactor [73]. Our goal was to map the existing evidence as a basis for the development of guidelines for screening and treatment of ID in these conditions, and to develop a pragmatic best practice approach on how to further investigate these behavioural phenotypes. Our scoping review also helped to identify gaps in evidence generation and ideas to further analyse existing and to create prospective data.

In our review, ADHD has been found to be the most extensively investigated condition, whereas fewer studies have been conducted for ASD, and only 2 studies have been identified for FASD.

For ADHD, 22 of the 30 identified association studies suggested a relation of types and severity of ADHD-related symptoms with ID. These findings are supported by 4 systematic reviews concluding that ID plays a role in the pathogenesis of ADHD. We also found 8 studies which did not show evidence for an association of ID with ADHD.

There are several reasons for discrepant results in these ADHD association studies. One of them is the inconsistency in the use of instruments employed for the diagnosis and severity grading of ADHD. Overall, we identified 15 scales including a variety of self-report scales (which potentially capture internalizing behaviours that may go unnoticed by caregivers) and scales completed by adult informants (which mainly capture externalizing behaviours that are publicly observable). There are also differences in the bandwidth of the scales. Narrow band scales such as the CPRS and CTRS are robust measures to diagnose ADHD. In contrast, broadband scales cover the breadth of patients’ problems by eliciting information across an array of symptoms which can be associated with ADHD (e.g., Child Behaviour Checklist). For an overview see Collett et al [74].

When using narrow band scales, more than one baseline assessment should be performed to account for a change in scores related to treatment. However, in most of the ADHD association studies identified in our review, detailed information about the establishment of baseline values is not provided. Another limitation of the currently available scales is that they originally have been developed for male elementary school-aged children. Suitability to other populations investigated in the ADHD association studies reported here, e.g., preschoolers [75], girls [76], and children with comorbid intellectual disability and/or speech problems [77] has not been evaluated.

Inconsistencies in the choice of ID markers and interpreting ID results are additional concerns related to the discrepant results obtained in the ADHD association studies. In approximately three quarters of those studies which measured both iron- and RBC-related biomarkers, iron-related (predominantly SF), but not RBC-related markers were statistically associated with the diagnosis of ADHD and / or ADHD severity. For example, the case-control study by Konofal et al. [32], including 53 cases, and by Juneja et al. [31] including 25 cases, concluded a positive association between ID and ADHD. However, the positive association was only statistically significant for low SF levels in children with ADHD compared to controls, while SI, Hct, and Hb were in the normal range for both groups. Similar results were found in the series of 52 cases reported by Oner et al. [29], demonstrating a significant relationship between lower SF levels and ADHD symptom severity. Moreover, a case series by Cortese et al. [14] only conducted a statistical analysis on low and high SF level comparisons in students with ADHD, even though they had also used SI and Hb for the assessment of ID.

Similar to the ADHD association studies, contradictory results were also found in the ASD studies. Although the 6 association studies were comparable in study design and participant numbers, 3 found statistical evidence for an association of ID and ASD / ASD severity, and 3 did not. Further contradictory results were reported in the 4 studies investigating the prevalence of ID in children with ASD. While Hergüner et al. [16] found a prevalence of ID and IDA in 24.1% and 15.5% of the 116 ASD children investigated, a similar sized study by Sidrak et al. [55] found ID and IDA in only 6.6% and 4.1% respectively. Selective eating habits and gastrointestinal comorbidities, which are frequently observed in ASD may be one reason for these discrepancies [78, 79] along with discrepancies in the choice of tools and indicators to assess the severity of ASD and ID.

For FASD, we found only 2 studies with both of them investigating the association of ID and postnatal growth restriction in infants with heavy prenatal alcohol exposure [57, 58]. Although there is evidence from experimental studies showing that maternal iron status has a unique influence upon FASD outcomes [19, 80, 81], the role of maternal ID as a synergistic modifier of FASD risk has not been further investigated in humans. Studies investigating a potential correlation of maternal and neonatal ID with FASD-related neurodevelopmental outcomes as well as studies aiming to treat maternal ID are needed to demonstrate a potential FASD risk-reducing effect of intrauterine alcohol exposure in humans.

SF is currently recommended as the most practical, universally available biomarker for the detection of low iron stores [82]. One major advantage of SF over other primary iron markers is its widely accepted independence of the fasting / postprandial state, compared to SI levels and TSAT which are subject to postprandial elevations [83]. Indeed, in all studies analysed here, SF was used as a primary iron marker. Low SF levels were found to be most frequently associated with the NDDs investigated, whereas among the RBC-related iron markers, besides low Hb, low MCV was most frequently observed in association with NDDs. However, like other RBC-related markers, MCV often remained unremarkable despite low SF levels. Likewise, most studies in our recently published scoping review about the role of ID in sleep disorders used SF, but not RBC-related measures as the main biomarker of ID [84].

Ferritin is a multimeric protein composed of light (L-ferritin) and heavy (H-ferritin) subunits which have distinct functions in oxidizing and binding (H-ferritin) and storing (L-ferritin) iron. While SF mainly contains L-ferritin for iron storage, the various ferritin species localized in cellular compartments, such as cytosol, golgi, lysosomes, and mitochondria, play roles in ferroptosis, a form of programmed cell death, ferrophagy, a form of autophagy, and handling of oxygen radicals [85].

Despite all advantages, the following limitations of SF’s role as an indicator of ID should be considered in the interpretation of the results of this review. SF also acts as an acute phase protein and any increase in SF due to systemic inflammation confounds its relationship with ID. The parallel measurement of CRP as an independent indicator for acute inflammation enhances SF’s value as an ID marker. In this review, CRP measurements in conjunction with serum ferritin were provided in only 2 ADHD [38, 43] and 2 ASD association studies [52, 59], while one study also measured erythrocyte sedimentation rate (ESR). In all 4 studies, none of the participants had an elevated CRP. However, the single study that measured both CRP and ESR found that ESR, but not CRP, was significantly higher in the ADHD group compared to controls (p = 0.001) [38]. The parallel measurement of CRP or ESR and / or the use of other biomarkers such as sTfR or sTfR/log ferritin ratio may be superior to SF alone, particularly in the presence of systemic inflammation [86]. Serum hepcidin is another promising biomarker indicating systemic iron regulation and inflammatory response, which could particularly useful in cases with ID resistant to enteral iron supplementation [87].

Another limitation is that SF represents iron stores mainly from macrophages, but it does not reflect ID contained to single organs [44]. For example, low brain iron (BI) levels, which are not always reflected by SF, play a role in the pathophysiology of restless legs syndrome (RLS), periodic limb movements in sleep [88], which can cause restless sleep disorders [89]. Given the presumptive overlap between RLS and ADHD, the use of BI imaging [39] has been recommended for a presumed subgroup of ADHD patients who may have deficient BI transport systems in the presence of normal SF levels. The finding of isolated BI deficiency in ADHD helps consolidating the concept of functional ID, as described in RLS [88, 90].

A final aspect in the interpretation of the results obtained in this review is the variance in normal SF values depending on age, sex and genotype. SF < 15 μg/L is generally accepted as the threshold for absence of iron stores [91], however, studies have shown that thresholds for ID in younger children are as low as <10–12 μg/L [92, 93]. Whereas the majority of the ASD studies analysed here used SF cut-off values were between 10 and 15 μg/L, SF cut-off values were higher than 15 μg/L in the majority of the ADHD studies.

The heterogeneous thresholds of SF as an indicator for ID might be another reason for discrepant results in the numerous studies performed among various age groups in countries across the globe. Also, if studies were performed in countries where infectious diseases are more common than in high resource countries (e.g., Africa, Asia South America), or during periods of seasonal infectious diseases, results based on SF only could have been confounded by its additional function as an acute phase protein.

Compared to the relative abundance of association studies, the yield for treatment studies in our search was low, with only 6 treatment studies for ADHD and 1 for ASD being identified. Although the 2 ADHD RCTs [15, 60] demonstrated a benefit of iron supplementation on both inattention and hyperactivity, they included low numbers of participants (18 treated cases vs 5 controls [15] and 21 treated cases vs 21 control [60]) and their Jaded scores were low at 2/ 5 and 3/5 respectively.

The overall paucity of ID treatment studies for ADHD and ASD stands in stark contrast to the abundance of clinical trials published for pharmacological treatments for ADHD. A scoping review investigating interventional ADHD RCTs that used sleep as an outcome measure identified 53 pharmacological RCTs published between 1995–2020 [94]. Importantly, this represents only a small proportion of pharmacological RCTs for ADHD, given that most studies do not use sleep as an outcome measure.

In the light of frequent adverse reactions [95] and of unknown long-term outcomes in stimulant users, well-designed and sufficiently powered clinical trials are needed to identify whether, and for which sub-populations iron supplementation can be a beneficial alternative or add on to pharmacotherapy for ADHD and other NDDs.

In conclusion, our scoping review identified a body of studies investigating a potential role of ID in frequent NDDs. However, the methodological designs, choice of outcome measures, and study participant numbers were quite heterogeneous. The paucity of treatment studies and the low numbers of enrolled participants is of particular concern given the high prevalence of the investigated NDDs and the easy access to iron supplementation.

While this review format allowed for a descriptive assessment of the identified studies, comparison of studies against each other was not attempted mainly due to the inconsistent methodological quality of the single studies. We accepted associations and related statistical results as described by the authors of the respective studies and did not exclude any study based on methodological design, participant numbers, choice of outcome measures for NDDs, or laboratory methods employed for measurement of iron markers.

The majority of studies identified in our scoping review supported the role of ID in NDDs, however there were also studies using similar study protocols, which did not. Furthermore, the paucity of treatment studies and the low numbers of enrolled participants deserves particular consideration, given the high prevalence of the investigated NDDs and the easy access to iron supplementation.

Given the global distribution of ID [22] paralleled by a similar distribution of NDDs [2], affecting both low and high resource countries, studies to corroborate the importance of prevention and treatment of ID are of utmost public health importance. In future studies, standardized protocols using harmonized outcome measures for symptom severity scores and iron indices will contribute to higher quality and consistency amongst studies. For treatment studies, besides adherence to high evidence study designs, the use N-of-1 trial designs with the treated individuals being their own control should be considered in single case studies, small sample sizes, and populations with heterogeneous clinical characteristics [96].

SF as the most accepted outcome measure for ID should be combined with iron markers independent of inflammation and / or with markers indicating inflammation and cut off levels should be harmonized. Additional markers addressing ID as a proxy for micronutrient deficiency or familial predispositions, and iron’s spatial distribution (e.g., brain iron deficiency) should be developed and utilized particularly for studies investigating NDDs.

Supporting information

S1 Table. Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic reviews and Meta-Analyses extension for Scoping Reviews (PRISMA-ScR) checklist.

Adapted from: Tricco AC, Lillie E, Zarin W, O’Brien KK, Colquhoun H, Levac D, et al. PRISMA Extension for Scoping Reviews (PRISMAScR): Checklist and Explanation. Ann Intern Med. 2018;169:467–473. doi: 10.7326/M18-0850.

(DOCX)

S1 File. S2 and S3 Tables.

Studies investigating the association of ADHD and ID.

(DOCX)

S2 File. S4, S5, S6 Tables.

Studies investigating the association of ASD and ID.

(DOCX)

S3 File. S7 and S8 Tables.

Treatment studies investigating the effect of iron supplementation in ADHD and ASD.

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

We thank Andrea Ryce, Librarian, BCCH for helping us with the online database search; Dr. Evelyn Stewart for funding the Iron Conundrum Workshop with the Michael Smith Foundation for Health Research Award 2017 (BCCH Research & Department of Psychiatry).

Dedication

The authors would like to dedicate this article to Professor Richard P. Allen († Dec. 9, 2020), clinician and sleep scientist at Johns Hopkins University in Baltimore, Maryland, one of the pioneer investigators of Restless Legs Syndrome. Dr. Allen’s iron deficiency research motivated us to review literature from different perspectives and approach day- and nighttime behaviours in unity.

Abbreviations

AbBC

Aberrant behaviour checklist

ADHD

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder

ADI-R

Autism Diagnostic Interview–Revised

ADOS

Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule

ASD

Autism spectrum disorder

AuBC

Autism behaviour checklist

BI

Brain iron

BMI

Body mass index

CARS

Childhood autism rating scale

CBL

Child behaviour checklist

CGI-S

Clinical global impression severity

CHCM

Children’s interview for psychiatric symptoms: parent version

CPRS

Conners parent rating scale

CRP

C-reactive protein

CRS

Conners rating scale

CSI-4

child symptoms inventory-4

CTRS

Conners teacher rating scale

ESR

Erythrocyte sedimentation rate

FASD

Fetal alcohol spectrum disorder

FEP

Free erythrocyte protoporphyrin

GDD

Global developmental delay

Hb

Hemoglobin

Hct

Hematocrit

ID

Iron deficiency

IDA

Iron deficiency anemia

IQ

Intelligence quotient

K-SADS

Kiddie schedule for affective disorders and schizophrenia

MCH

Mean corpuscular hemoglobin

MCHC

Mean corpuscular hemoglobin concentration

MCV

Mean corpuscular volume

MRI

Magnetic resonance imaging

NDD

Neurodevelopmental disorder

P-ChIPS

Cell hemoglobin concentration mean

RCT

Randomized controlled trial

RDW

Red cell distribution width

RLS

Restless legs syndrome

SCID-1

Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV-I

SDQ

Strengths and difficulties questionnaire

SF

Serum ferritin

SFI

sTfR/log ferritin index

SNAP

Swanson, Nolan, Pelham scale

sTfR

Soluble transferrin receptor

SWTD

Sleep wake transition disorders

T-DSM-IV-S

Turgay DSM-IV-Based Child and Adolescent Behaviour Disorders Screening and Rating Scale

TIBC

Total iron binding capacity

TSAT

Transferrin saturation

WURS

Wender Utah Rating Scale

Data Availability

All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting information files.

Funding Statement

SM was supported by a summer studentship from BC Children’s Hospital Research Institute (https://www.bcchr.ca/), IS was supported by funds from the BC Children’s Hospital Research Institute for sleep wake behaviour research (to OI) and for treatable intellectual disability research (to SS). WL was supported by the Mach-Gaensslen Foundation (https://mach-gaensslen.ca/). The Iron Conundrum Workshop was funded by an award from the Michael Smith Foundation for Health Research (https://www.msfhr.org/). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Hansen BH, Oerbeck B, Skirbekk B, Petrovski BÉ, Kristensen H. Neurodevelopmental disorders: prevalence and comorbidity in children referred to mental health services. Nord J Psychiatry. 2018. May;72(4):285–91. doi: 10.1080/08039488.2018.1444087 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.GBD 2019 Mental Disorders Collaborators. Global, regional, and national burden of 12 mental disorders in 204 countries and territories, 1990–2019: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2019. Lancet Psychiatry. 2022. Feb;9(2):137–50. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(21)00395-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Sayal K, Prasad V, Daley D, Ford T, Coghill D. ADHD in children and young people: prevalence, care pathways, and service provision. Lancet Psychiatry. 2018. Feb;5(2):175–86. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(17)30167-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Kogan MD, Vladutiu CJ, Schieve LA, Ghandour RM, Blumberg SJ, Zablotsky B, et al. The Prevalence of Parent-Reported Autism Spectrum Disorder Among US Children. Pediatrics [Internet]. 2018. Dec;142(6). doi: 10.1542/peds.2017-4161 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Lange S, Probst C, Gmel G, Rehm J, Burd L, Popova S. Global Prevalence of Fetal Alcohol Spectrum Disorder Among Children and Youth: A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis. JAMA Pediatr. 2017. Oct 1;171(10):948–56. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2017.1919 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Hare D, Ayton S, Bush A, Lei P. A delicate balance: Iron metabolism and diseases of the brain. Front Aging Neurosci. 2013. Jul 18;5:34. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2013.00034 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Lozoff B, Beard J, Connor J, Felt B, Georgieff M, Schallert T. Long-lasting neural and behavioral effects of iron deficiency in infancy. Nutr Rev. 2006. May;64(5 Pt 2):S34–43 discussion S72-91. doi: 10.1301/nr.2006.may.s34-s43 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Rao R, Tkac I, Schmidt AT, Georgieff MK. Fetal and neonatal iron deficiency causes volume loss and alters the neurochemical profile of the adult rat hippocampus. Nutr Neurosci. 2011. Mar;14(2):59–65. doi: 10.1179/1476830511Y.0000000001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Siddappa AM, Georgieff MK, Wewerka S, Worwa C, Nelson CA, Deregnier RA. Iron deficiency alters auditory recognition memory in newborn infants of diabetic mothers. Pediatr Res. 2004. Jun;55(6):1034–41. doi: 10.1203/01.pdr.0000127021.38207.62 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Wiegersma AM, Dalman C, Lee BK, Karlsson H, Gardner RM. Association of Prenatal Maternal Anemia With Neurodevelopmental Disorders. JAMA Psychiatry. 2019. Dec;76(12):1294–304. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2019.2309 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Doom JR, Georgieff MK, Gunnar MR. Institutional care and iron deficiency increase ADHD symptomology and lower IQ 2.5–5 years post-adoption. Dev Sci. 2015. May;18(3):484–94. doi: 10.1111/desc.12223 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Bener A, Kamal M, Bener H, Bhugra D. Higher prevalence of iron deficiency as strong predictor of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children. Ann Med Health Sci Res. 2014. Sep;4(Suppl 3):S291–7. doi: 10.4103/2141-9248.141974 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Mahmoud MM, El-Mazary AAM, Maher RM, Saber MM. Zinc, ferritin, magnesium and copper in a group of Egyptian children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Ital J Pediatr. 2011. Dec 29;37:60. doi: 10.1186/1824-7288-37-60 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Cortese S, Konofal E, Bernardina BD, Mouren MC, Lecendreux M. Sleep disturbances and serum ferritin levels in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2009. Jul;18(7):393–9. doi: 10.1007/s00787-009-0746-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Konofal E, Lecendreux M, Deron J, Marchand M, Cortese S, Zaïm M, et al. Effects of iron supplementation on attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children. Pediatr Neurol. 2008. Jan;38(1):20–6. doi: 10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2007.08.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Hergüner S, Keleşoğlu FM, Tanıdır C, Cöpür M. Ferritin and iron levels in children with autistic disorder. Eur J Pediatr. 2012. Jan;171(1):143–6. doi: 10.1007/s00431-011-1506-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Dosman CF, Drmic IE, Brian JA, Senthilselvan A, Harford M, Smith R, et al. Ferritin as an indicator of suspected iron deficiency in children with autism spectrum disorder: prevalence of low serum ferritin concentration. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2006. Dec;48(12):1008–9. doi: 10.1017/S0012162206232225 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Dosman CF, Brian JA, Drmic IE, Senthilselvan A, Harford MM, Smith RW, et al. Children with autism: effect of iron supplementation on sleep and ferritin. Pediatr Neurol. 2007. Mar;36(3):152–8. doi: 10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2006.11.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Rufer ES, Tran TD, Attridge MM, Andrzejewski ME, Flentke GR, Smith SM. Adequacy of maternal iron status protects against behavioral, neuroanatomical, and growth deficits in fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. PLoS One. 2012. Oct 19;7(10):e47499. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0047499 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Ipsiroglu OS, Wind K, Hung YHA, Berger M, Chan F, Yu W, et al. Prenatal alcohol exposure and sleep-wake behaviors: exploratory and naturalistic observations in the clinical setting and in an animal model. Sleep Med. 2019. Feb;54:101–12. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2018.10.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.De Betonist B, McLean E, Egli I, Cogswell M. Worldwide prevalence of anemia 1993 2005: WHO global database on anemia. Geneva: World Health Organization. 2008. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Stevens GA, Finucane MM, De-Regil LM, Paciorek CJ, Flaxman SR, Branca F, et al. Global, regional, and national trends in haemoglobin concentration and prevalence of total and severe anaemia in children and pregnant and non-pregnant women for 1995–2011: a systematic analysis of population-representative data. The Lancet Global Health. 2013;1(1):e16–25. doi: 10.1016/S2214-109X(13)70001-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Celentano DD, Moyses S. Gordis Epidemiology. Philadelphia: Elsevier; 2014. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Jadad AR, Moore RA, Carroll D, Jenkinson C, Reynolds DJ, Gavaghan DJ, et al. Assessing the quality of reports of randomized clinical trials: is blinding necessary? Control Clin Trials. 1996. Feb;17(1):1–12. doi: 10.1016/0197-2456(95)00134-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Demirci K, Yildirim Baş F, Arslan B, Salman Z, Akpinar A, Demirdaş A. The Investigation of Symptoms and Diagnoses of Adult-Attention Deficit/ Hyperactivity Disorder in Women with Iron Deficiency Anemia. Noro Psikiyatr Ars. 2017. Mar;54(1):72–7. doi: 10.5152/npa.2016.12464 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Doom JR, Richards B, Caballero G, Delva J, Gahagan S, Lozoff B. Infant Iron Deficiency and Iron Supplementation Predict Adolescent Internalizing, Externalizing, and Social Problems. J Pediatr. 2018. Apr;195:199–205.e2. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2017.12.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Calarge C, Farmer C, DiSilvestro R, Arnold LE. Serum ferritin and amphetamine response in youth with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol. 2010. Dec;20(6):495–502. doi: 10.1089/cap.2010.0053 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Oner P, Oner O, Azik FM, Cop E, Munir KM. Ferritin and hyperactivity ratings in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Pediatr Int. 2012. Oct;54(5):688–92. doi: 10.1111/j.1442-200X.2012.03664.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Oner O, Alkar OY, Oner P. Relation of ferritin levels with symptom ratings and cognitive performance in children with attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder. Pediatr Int. 2008. Feb;50(1):40–4. doi: 10.1111/j.1442-200X.2007.02496.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Seleem MA, El-Gohary TM, Eid MA, Sroor EA. Serum ferritin is negatively correlated with inattention in a sample of Egyptian children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. The Arab Journal of Psychiatry. 2014;44(1446):1–22. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Juneja M, Jain R, Singh V, Mallika V. Iron deficiency in Indian children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Indian Pediatr. 2010. Nov;47(11):955–8. doi: 10.1007/s13312-010-0160-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Konofal E, Lecendreux M, Arnulf I, Mouren MC. Iron deficiency in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2004. Dec;158(12):1113–5. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.158.12.1113 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Oner P, Oner O. Relationship of ferritin to symptom ratings children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: effect of comorbidity. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2008. Sep;39(3):323–30. doi: 10.1007/s10578-007-0095-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Oner O, Oner P, Bozkurt OH, Odabas E, Keser N, Karadag H, et al. Effects of zinc and ferritin levels on parent and teacher reported symptom scores in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2010. Aug;41(4):441–7. doi: 10.1007/s10578-010-0178-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Percinel I, Yazici KU, Ustundag B. Iron Deficiency Parameters in Children and Adolescents with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2016. Apr;47(2):259–69. doi: 10.1007/s10578-015-0562-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Konofal E, Cortese S, Marchand M, Mouren MC, Arnulf I, Lecendreux M. Impact of restless legs syndrome and iron deficiency on attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children. Sleep Med. 2007. Nov;8(7–8):711–5. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2007.04.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Öztürk Y, Topal Z, Demir N, Tufan AE. Iron and ferritin levels of children and adolescents with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder-not otherwise specified. j pediatr res. 2020. Jun 26;7(3):216–22. [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Sahu S, Raman V, Selvam S, Mysore A, Others. Soluble Transferrin Receptor and SFI Index--A new biomarker to identify Iron Deficiency in Drug Naïve Children with ADHD--A Case-Control Study. Journal of Indian Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health-ISSN 0973-1342. 2020;16(4):64–81. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Cortese S, Azoulay R, Castellanos FX, Chalard F, Lecendreux M, Chechin D, et al. Brain iron levels in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a pilot MRI study. World J Biol Psychiatry. 2012. Mar;13(3):223–31. doi: 10.3109/15622975.2011.570376 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Islam K, Seth S, Saha S, Roy A, Das R, Datta A. A study on association of iron deficiency with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in a tertiary care center. Indian J Psychiatry. 2018;60(1):131. doi: 10.4103/psychiatry.IndianJPsychiatry_197_17 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Abd El Naby SA, Naguib YM. Sociodemographic, Electrophysiological, and Biochemical Profiles in Children with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder and/or Epilepsy. Behav Neurol. 2018. Nov 29;2018:8932817. doi: 10.1155/2018/8932817 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Oner P, Oner O, Cop E, Munir KM. Effect of Ferritin on Short-Term Treatment Response in Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. Klinik Psikofarmakol Bulteni. 2012;22(4):325–31. doi: 10.5455/bcp.20120403015900 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Romanos M, Tiesler CMT, Koletzko S, Berdel D, von Berg A, Hoffmann B, et al. No cross-sectional and longitudinal association of ferritin and symptoms of attention-deficit-/hyperactivity disorder in a large population-based sample of children: results from the GINIplus and LISAplus studies [Internet]. Vol. 5, ADHD Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorders. 2013. p. 313–20. doi: 10.1007/s12402-013-0108-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Donfrancesco R, Parisi P, Vanacore N, Martines F, Sargentini V, Cortese S. Iron and ADHD: time to move beyond serum ferritin levels. J Atten Disord. 2013. May;17(4):347–57. doi: 10.1177/1087054711430712 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Menegassi M, de Mello ED, Guimarães LR, Matte BC, Driemeier F, Pedroso GL, et al. Food intake and serum levels of iron in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Braz J Psychiatry. 2010. Jun;32(2):132–8. doi: 10.1590/s1516-44462009005000008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Abou-Khadra MK, Amin OR, Shaker OG, Rabah TM. Parent-reported sleep problems, symptom ratings, and serum ferritin levels in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a case control study. BMC Pediatr. 2013. Dec 30;13:217. doi: 10.1186/1471-2431-13-217 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Eli Lahat MD, Eli Heyman MD, Amir Livne MD, Michael Goldman MD, Matitiahu Berkovitch MD, Ditza Zachor MD. Iron deficiency in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Isr Med Assoc J. 2011;13:530–3. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Unal D, Çelebi F, Bildik HN, Koyuncu A, Karahan S. Vitamin B12 and haemoglobin levels may be related with ADHD symptoms: a study in Turkish children with ADHD. null. 2019. Oct 2;29(4):515–9. [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Millichap JG, Yee MM, Davidson SI. Serum ferritin in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Pediatr Neurol. 2006. Mar;34(3):200–3. doi: 10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2005.09.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Magula L, Moxley K, Lachman A. Iron deficiency in South African children and adolescents with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Child Adolesc Ment Health. 2019. Sep;31(2):85–92. doi: 10.2989/17280583.2019.1637345 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Gunes S, Ekinci O, Celik T. Iron deficiency parameters in autism spectrum disorder: clinical correlates and associated factors. Ital J Pediatr. 2017. Sep 21;43(1):86. doi: 10.1186/s13052-017-0407-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Liu X, Liu J, Xiong X, Yang T, Hou N, Liang X, et al. Correlation between Nutrition and Symptoms: Nutritional Survey of Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder in Chongqing, China. Nutrients [Internet]. 2016. May 14;8(5). doi: 10.3390/nu8050294 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Bilgiç A, Gürkan K, Türkoğlu S, Akça ÖF, Kılıç BG, Uslu R. Iron deficiency in preschool children with autistic spectrum disorders. Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2010. Oct 1;4(4):639–44. [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Latif A, Heinz P, Cook R. Iron deficiency in autism and Asperger syndrome. Autism. 2002. Mar;6(1):103–14. doi: 10.1177/1362361302006001008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Sidrak S, Yoong T, Woolfenden S. Iron deficiency in children with global developmental delay and autism spectrum disorder. J Paediatr Child Health. 2014. May;50(5):356–61. doi: 10.1111/jpc.12483 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Reynolds A, Krebs NF, Stewart PA, Austin H, Johnson SL, Withrow N, et al. Iron status in children with autism spectrum disorder. Pediatrics. 2012. Nov;130 Suppl 2:S154–9. doi: 10.1542/peds.2012-0900M [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Carter RC, Jacobson SW, Molteno CD, Jacobson JL. Fetal alcohol exposure, iron-deficiency anemia, and infant growth. Pediatrics. 2007. Sep;120(3):559–67. doi: 10.1542/peds.2007-0151 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Carter RC, Jacobson JL, Molteno CD, Jiang H, Meintjes EM, Jacobson SW, et al. Effects of heavy prenatal alcohol exposure and iron deficiency anemia on child growth and body composition through age 9 years. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2012. Nov;36(11):1973–82. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2012.01810.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Bener A, Khattab AO, Bhugra D, Hoffmann GF. Iron and vitamin D levels among autism spectrum disorders children. Ann Afr Med. 2017. Oct;16(4):186–91. doi: 10.4103/aam.aam_17_17 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Panahandeh G, Vatani B, Safavi P, Khoshdel A. The effect of adding ferrous sulfate to methylphenidate on attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children. J Adv Pharm Technol Res. 2017. Oct;8(4):138–42. doi: 10.4103/japtr.JAPTR_45_17 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Tu JB, Shafey H, VanDewetering C. Iron deficiency in two adolescents with conduct, dysthymic and movement disorders. Can J Psychiatry. 1994. Aug;39(6):371–5. doi: 10.1177/070674379403900610 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Konofal E, Cortese S, Lecendreux M, Arnulf I, Mouren MC. Effectiveness of iron supplementation in a young child with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Pediatrics. 2005. Nov;116(5):e732–4. doi: 10.1542/peds.2005-0715 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Ghogare AS, John S, Patil PS, Vankar GK. Study of iron profile and effect of oral iron supplementation in patients of attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: A case series. J Clin Diagn Res [Internet]. 2020. 14(10): VR01–VR05. [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Qubty W, Renaud DL. Cognitive impairment associated with low ferritin responsive to iron supplementation. Pediatr Neurol. 2014. Dec;51(6):831–3. doi: 10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2014.08.035 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Reynolds AM, Connolly HV, Katz T, Goldman SE, Weiss SK, Halbower AC, et al. Randomized, placebo-controlled trial of ferrous sulfate to treat insomnia in children with autism spectrum disorders. Pediatr Neurol. 2020. Mar;104:30–9. doi: 10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2019.07.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Scassellati C, Bonvicini C, Faraone SV, Gennarelli M. Biomarkers and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a systematic review and meta-analyses. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2012. Oct;51(10):1003–1019.e20. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2012.08.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Wang Y, Huang L, Zhang L, Qu Y, Mu D. Iron Status in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. PLoS One. 2017. Jan 3;12(1):e0169145. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0169145 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Cortese S, Angriman M, Lecendreux M, Konofal E. Iron and attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: What is the empirical evidence so far? A systematic review of the literature. Expert Rev Neurother. 2012. Oct;12(10):1227–40. doi: 10.1586/ern.12.116 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Degremont A, Jain R, Philippou E, Latunde-Dada GO. Brain iron concentrations in the pathophysiology of children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a systematic review. Nutr Rev. 2021. Apr 7;79(5):615–26. doi: 10.1093/nutrit/nuaa065 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Tseng PT, Cheng YS, Yen CF, Chen YW, Stubbs B, Whiteley P, et al. Peripheral iron levels in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Sci Rep. 2018. Jan 15;8(1):788. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-19096-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.van Karnebeek CDM, Stockler S. Treatable inborn errors of metabolism causing intellectual disability: a systematic literature review. Mol Genet Metab. 2012. Mar;105(3):368–81. doi: 10.1016/j.ymgme.2011.11.191 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Silvestri R. The importance of diagnosing and treating iron deficiency in sleep disorders [Internet]. Vol. 51, Sleep Medicine Reviews. England: Elsevier Ltd; 2020. p. 101314. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Walters AS, Silvestri R, Zucconi M, Chandrashekariah R, Konofal E. Review of the possible relationship and hypothetical links between attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and the simple sleep related movement disorders, parasomnias, hypersomnias, and circadian rhythm disorders. J Clin Sleep Med. 2008;4(6):591–600. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Collett BR, Ohan JL, Myers KM. Ten-year review of rating scales. V: scales assessing attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2003. Sep;42(9):1015–37. doi: 10.1097/01.CHI.0000070245.24125.B6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Mahone EM, Schneider HE. Assessment of attention in preschoolers. Neuropsychol Rev. 2012. Dec;22(4):361–83. doi: 10.1007/s11065-012-9217-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Mowlem F, Agnew-Blais J, Taylor E, Asherson P. Do different factors influence whether girls versus boys meet ADHD diagnostic criteria? Sex differences among children with high ADHD symptoms. Psychiatry Res. 2019. Feb;272:765–73. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.12.128 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Deb S, Dhaliwal AJ, Roy M. The usefulness of Conners’ Rating Scales-Revised in screening for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children with intellectual disabilities and borderline intelligence. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2008. Nov;52(11):950–65. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2007.01035.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Curtin C, Hubbard K, Anderson SE, Mick E, Must A, Bandini LG. Food selectivity, mealtime behavior problems, spousal stress, and family food choices in children with and without autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2015. Oct;45(10):3308–15. doi: 10.1007/s10803-015-2490-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Chaidez V, Hansen RL, Hertz-Picciotto I. Gastrointestinal problems in children with autism, developmental delays or typical development. J Autism Dev Disord. 2014. May;44(5):1117–27. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1973-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Huebner SM, Tran TD, Rufer ES, Crump PM, Smith SM. Maternal iron deficiency worsens the associative learning deficits and hippocampal and cerebellar losses in a rat model of fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2015. Nov;39(11):2097–107. doi: 10.1111/acer.12876 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Helfrich KK, Saini N, Kling PJ, Smith SM. Maternal iron nutriture as a critical modulator of fetal alcohol spectrum disorder risk in alcohol-exposed pregnancies. Biochem Cell Biol. 2018. Apr;96(2):204–12. doi: 10.1139/bcb-2017-0206 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Peyrin-Biroulet L, Williet N, Cacoub P. Guidelines on the diagnosis and treatment of iron deficiency across indications: a systematic review. Am J Clin Nutr. 2015. Dec;102(6):1585–94. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.114.103366 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Nguyen LT, Buse JD, Baskin L, Sadrzadeh SMH, Naugler C. Influence of diurnal variation and fasting on serum iron concentrations in a community-based population. Clin Biochem. 2017. Dec;50(18):1237–42. doi: 10.1016/j.clinbiochem.2017.09.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Leung W, Singh I, McWilliams S, Stockler S, Ipsiroglu OS. Iron deficiency and sleep–A scoping review [Internet]. Vol. 51, Sleep Medicine Reviews. 2020. p. 101274. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2020.101274 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Plays M, Müller S, Rodriguez R. Chemistry and biology of ferritin. Metallomics [Internet]. 2021. May 12;13(5). doi: 10.1093/mtomcs/mfab021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.World Health Organization. Serum transferrin receptor levels for the assessment of iron status and iron deficiency in populations. Vitamin and Mineral Nutrition Information System. 2014. [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Hare DJ. Hepcidin: a real-time biomarker of iron need. Metallomics. 2017;9(6):606–18. doi: 10.1039/c7mt00047b [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Allen RP, Picchietti DL, Auerbach M, Cho YW, Connor JR, Earley CJ, et al. Evidence-based and consensus clinical practice guidelines for the iron treatment of restless legs syndrome/Willis-Ekbom disease in adults and children: an IRLSSG task force report [Internet]. Vol. 41, Sleep Medicine. 2018. p. 27–44. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2017.11.1126 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.DelRosso LM, Bruni O, Ferri R. Restless sleep disorder in children: a pilot study on a tentative new diagnostic category. Sleep [Internet]. 2018. Aug 1;41(8). doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsy102 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Zhu XY, Wu TT, Wang HM, Li X, Ni LY, Chen TJ, et al. Correlates of nonanemic iron deficiency in restless legs syndrome. Front Neurol. 2020. Apr 30;11:298. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2020.00298 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.World Health Organization. Nutritional Anaemias: Tools for Effective Prevention and Control. 2017. [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Cook JD, Finch CA, Smith NJ. Evaluation of the iron status of a population. Blood. 1976. Sep 1;48(3):449–55. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Looker AC, Dallman PR, Carroll MD, Gunter EW, Johnson CL. Prevalence of iron deficiency in the United States. JAMA. 1997. Mar 26;277(12):973–6. doi: 10.1001/jama.1997.03540360041028 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.McWilliams S, Zhou T, Stockler S, Elbe D, Ipsiroglu OS. Sleep as an outcome measure in interventional ADHD randomized controlled trials: A scoping review. Sleep Medicine Reviews. 2022. 63: 01613. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Barr AM, Dorffner G, Elbe D, Frieling A, Ipsiroglu OS, Kloesch G, et al. Managing the Disruptive Behaviour Prevalence with a NRCT = 1 Trial Registry. Aktuelle Kinderschlafmedizin; [Internet]. 2019. [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Kravitz RL, Duan N, Vohra S, Li J, the DEcIDE Methods Center N-of-1 Guidance Panel. Introduction to N-of-1 Trials. Indications and Barriers. In: Kravitz RL, Duan N, eds, and the DEcIDE Methods Center N-of-1 Guidance Panel (Duan N, Eslick I, Gabler NB, Kaplan HC, Kravitz RL, Larson EB, Pace WD, Schmid CH, Sim I, Vohra S). Design and Implementation of N-of-1 Trials. A User’s Guide. AHRQ Publication No. 13(14)-EHC 122-EF. Rockville, MD: Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality January 2014: Chapter1, pp.1-11.

Decision Letter 0

Efthimios M C Skoulakis

8 Feb 2022

PONE-D-21-23571Iron deficiency and common neurodevelopmental disorders - A scoping reviewPLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Stockler,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

==============================

Both reviewers agree that the manuscript has significant merit and are positive towards publication, but both present a series of comments and concerns aiming at improving the manuscript that should be thoroughly addressed before final acceptance.

==============================

Please submit your revised manuscript by Mar 25 2022 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Efthimios M. C. Skoulakis, PhD

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf".

2. Thank you for stating the following in the Funding Section of your manuscript:

“SM was supported by a summer studentship from BCCHR, IS was supported by funds from the BCCHRI for sleep wake behaviour research (to OI) and for treatable intellectual disability research (to SS). WL was supported by the Mach-Gaensslen Foundation. The Iron Conundrum Workshop was funded by an award from the Michael Smith Foundation for Health Research.”

We note that you have provided funding information that is not currently declared in your Funding Statement. However, funding information should not appear in the Acknowledgments section or other areas of your manuscript. We will only publish funding information present in the Funding Statement section of the online submission form.

Please remove any funding-related text from the manuscript and let us know how you would like to update your Funding Statement. Currently, your Funding Statement reads as follows:

“SM was supported by a summer studentship from BC Children's Hospital Research Institute (https://www.bcchr.ca/), IS was supported by funds from the BC Children's Hospital Research Institute for sleep wake behaviour research (to OI) and for treatable intellectual disability research (to SS). WL was supported by the Mach-Gaensslen Foundation (https://mach-gaensslen.ca/). The Iron Conundrum Workshop was funded by an award from the Michael Smith Foundation for Health Research (https://www.msfhr.org/).

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.”

Please include your amended statements within your cover letter; we will change the online submission form on your behalf.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: N/A

Reviewer #2: N/A

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: The manuscript presents a scoping review of existing evidence that low serum ferritin, likely due to iron deficiency, either during pregnancy or during child growth and development, correlates with ADHD severity of young patients.

Discussing the point of whether "iron deficiency" is a helpful term to describe under one umbrella the complex possibilities of altered iron metabolism in humans, where a diagnostic parameter suggests iron unavailability is certainly outside the scope of the present review. I offer this comment, that the medical community might benefit by considering many more markers of iron status and how they are interconnected in disease conditions instead of a simplistic trichotomy of iron deficiency/adequacy/overload, for future reflections by the authors, in memory of Professor Richard P. Allen to whom the manuscript is dedicated. The authors themselves suggest the importance of measuring hepcidin and I have a question below why they seem skeptical about serum iron. In terms of the relationship between low serum ferritin and iron deficiency, there are a few interesting papers, and also there is literature around the physiologic function of serum ferritin. Should these points be discussed, given the much stronger evidence of correlation for this marker? The decision should be left with the authors in my opinion.

Other minor comments:

1. Inclusion and Exclusion criteria

Why do the authors exclude measurements of serum iron as primarily reflecting iron metabolism?

2. Association studies:

Were any criteria considered/applied to accept/exclude reported correlations?

3. The sentence in lines 177-188 could benefit from rephrasing so as not to repeat the word “studies” thrice.

4. Line 197: should the authors add an explanatory note of what they consider inappropriate methodology for double blinding?

5. Lines 234-244: would this information be best included in Supplementary Tables 1 & 2 with reference to the table in the text? Alternatively, this could also be done in their figures. I would have personally benefited by a concise descriptive comparison of these methods in the introduction, but this is a suggestion open to the authors to consider. If there is an available review, it could be a single introductory sentence of the paragraph where the methods are first mentioned.

6. Lines 414-415: order trials in ascending or descending temporal order?

7. Abstract:

“Conclusion: Screening for iron deficiency and use of iron supplementation is useful for neurodevelopmental disorders. Evidence is higher for ADHD, lower for ASD, and needs to be further investigated for FASD.”

Suggested change in wording:

“Conclusion: Evidence in favor of screening for iron deficiency and using iron supplementation for pediatric neurodevelopmental disorders comes primarily from ADHD studies and needs to be further investigated for ASD and FASD.”

Reviewer #2: In this scoping, the authors present an investigation of the association of iron deficiency (ID) in children and adolescents with the most frequent neurodevelopmental disorders (NDD), ADHD, ASD, and FASD. They also assess whether iron supplementation improves outcomes in these disorders. This is set on the backdrop of a role of ID in NDDs, but a paucity of evidence for the impact of ID and NDDSl disorders in childhood and adolescence. Particularly, that postnatal ID may aggravate pre-existing NDDs. This is an interesting and, seemingly novel review.

Introduction:

Well written, providing good background and context to the study. One missing area relates to the global distribution of ID and NDDs. There is plentiful data mapping the global distribution of ID, showing greatest prevalence in low-resource settings of the world, but also among at risk sub-groups. Is there any similar parallel with the global distribution of NDDs?

Methods

Was the review pre-registered on PROSPERO?

I can’t see the age limitation of children for included vs. excluded studies. Where is this information?

Results

The results are clearly presented.

The sex differences reported (lines 314 onwards), how did these differences relate to iron status. As this information is presented separately, it is difficult to work this out. Boys are at risk of iron deficiency in contexts of food insecurity, especially during periods of rapid growth. Are boys at greater risk because of differential iron status, rather than other sex-specific factors? It would be good to see the results a little more linked up.

On a similar point, it was hard to find details on actual iron status of the populations under investigation. This could be key to interpreting the findings (e.g. no associations may be observed in populations where there is not sufficient variance in iron status or in populations where status does not cross a ‘threshold’ for deficiency to impact on the outcomes under investigation. I have looked at all the Tables in the supporting information and this detail is also missing from there. I think it could be useful and potentially informative to position the papers against each other with respect to iron status of the children included. This could be done as a Figure, potentially showing levels of iron deficiency anaemia in each context.

Discussion

The discussion is well written and provides a good commentary on the results presented.

However, I was left wondering how much of the evidence for any associations presented was (i) because of iron deficiency per se (e.g. not because of nutritional insufficiency, with iron status being a proxy marker for global micronutrient deficiency) and (ii) how much of the evidence was confounded by other familial risk factors. I can see the benefit of including all types of study design in a scoping review and where trial evidence is limited. However, I felt that insufficient weighting was given to the weight of the evidence being presented and, therefore, the associated limitations. This is linked in with the point above about teasing out causal pathways; I think there is scope within this review to do this more thoroughly.

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: Yes: Fanis Missirlis

Reviewer #2: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2022 Sep 29;17(9):e0273819. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0273819.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


8 Aug 2022

Reviewer #1: The manuscript presents a scoping review of existing evidence that low serum ferritin, likely due to iron deficiency, either during pregnancy or during child growth and development, correlates with ADHD severity of young patients.

Discussing the point of whether "iron deficiency" is a helpful term to describe under one umbrella the complex possibilities of altered iron metabolism in humans, where a diagnostic parameter suggests iron unavailability is certainly outside the scope of the present review. I offer this comment, that the medical community might benefit by considering many more markers of iron status and how they are interconnected in disease conditions instead of a simplistic trichotomy of iron deficiency/adequacy/overload, for future reflections by the authors, in memory of Professor Richard P. Allen to whom the manuscript is dedicated. The authors themselves suggest the importance of measuring hepcidin and I have a question below why they seem skeptical about serum iron. In terms of the relationship between low serum ferritin and iron deficiency, there are a few interesting papers, and also there is literature around the physiologic function of serum ferritin. Should these points be discussed, given the much stronger evidence of correlation for this marker? The decision should be left with the authors in my opinion.

Answer: We addressed this important aspect in the last paragraph of the discussion (page 31):

SF as the most accepted outcome measure for ID should be combined with iron markers independent of inflammation and / or with markers indicating inflammation and cut off levels should be harmonized. Additional markers addressing iron’s spatial distribution (e.g., brain iron determined via in vivo magnetic resonance technology) and ferritin’s various functions, should be developed and utilized particularly for studies investigating NDDs.

Other minor comments:

1. Inclusion and Exclusion criteria

Why do the authors exclude measurements of serum iron as primarily reflecting iron metabolism?

Answer:

Our main rationale is that serum iron is susceptible to post-prandial elevation, making it an unreliable marker of iron status unless fasting bloods are taken. However, we would like to highlight that none of the studies identified in our search were excluded for this reason. We have mentioned serum iron’s dependence on the postprandial state in the discussion section on page 26:

SF is currently recommended as the most practical, universally available biomarker for the detection of low iron stores [81]. One major advantage of SF over other primary iron markers is its widely accepted independence of the fasting / postprandial state, compared to SI levels and TSAT which are subject to postprandial elevations [80].

2. Association studies:

Were any criteria considered/applied to accept/exclude reported correlations?

Answer: we have addressed this question in the limitations section of the discussion (page 30).

While this review format allowed for a descriptive assessment of the identified studies, comparison of studies against each other was not attempted mainly due to the inconsistent methodological quality of the single studies. We accepted associations and related statistical results as described by the authors of the respective studies and did not exclude any study based on methodological design, participant numbers, choice of outcome measures for NDDs, or laboratory methods employed for measurement of iron markers.

3. The sentence in lines 177-188 could benefit from rephrasing so as not to repeat the word “studies” thrice.

Answer: This sentence has been rephrased.

4. Line 197: should the authors add an explanatory note of what they consider inappropriate methodology for double blinding?

Answer: We have added an explanatory note of what inappropriate double blinding could look like (page 10-11):

7) The method of double blinding was described and inappropriate (e.g., identical placebo not used; study personnel and/or study participant could identify the intervention).

5. Lines 234-244: would this information be best included in Supplementary Tables 1 & 2 with reference to the table in the text? Alternatively, this could also be done in their figures. I would have personally benefited by a concise descriptive comparison of these methods in the introduction, but this is a suggestion open to the authors to consider. If there is an available review, it could be a single introductory sentence of the paragraph where the methods are first mentioned.

Answer:

We have added the tools used to determine the severity of ADHD and ASD to Tables S2, S3 for ADHD and S4, S5, S6 for ASD) to make this section more concise and easier to follow. We also have created a paragraph in the discussion describing the characteristics and differences of the ADHD tools (page 24-25):

Overall, we identified 15 scales including a variety of self-report scales (which potentially capture internalizing behaviours that may go unnoticed by caregivers) and scales completed by adult informants (which mainly capture externalizing behaviours that are publicly observable). There are also differences in the bandwidth of the scales. Narrow band scales such as the CPRS and CTRS are robust measures to diagnose ADHD. In contrast, broadband scales cover the breadth of patients’ problems by eliciting information across an array of symptoms which can be associated with ADHD (e.g., Child Behaviour Checklist). For an overview see Collett et al [75].

When using narrow band scales, more than one baseline assessment should be performed to account for a change in scores related to treatment. However, in most of the ADHD association studies identified in our review, detailed information about the establishment of baseline values is not provided. Another limitation of the currently available scales is that they originally have been developed for male elementary school-aged children. Suitability to other populations investigated in the ADHD association studies reported here, e.g., preschoolers [76], girls [77], and children with comorbid intellectual disability and/or speech problems [78] has not been evaluated .

6. Lines 414-415: order trials in ascending or descending temporal order?

We have re-ordered these trials.

7. Abstract:

“Conclusion: Screening for iron deficiency and use of iron supplementation is useful for neurodevelopmental disorders. Evidence is higher for ADHD, lower for ASD, and needs to be further investigated for FASD.”

Suggested change in wording:

“Conclusion: Evidence in favor of screening for iron deficiency and using iron supplementation for pediatric neurodevelopmental disorders comes primarily from ADHD studies and needs to be further investigated for ASD and FASD.”

Answer: Thank you for this suggestion - we have used this in the amended conclusion sentence.

Reviewer #2: In this scoping, the authors present an investigation of the association of iron deficiency (ID) in children and adolescents with the most frequent neurodevelopmental disorders (NDD), ADHD, ASD, and FASD. They also assess whether iron supplementation improves outcomes in these disorders. This is set on the backdrop of a role of ID in NDDs, but a paucity of evidence for the impact of ID and NDDSl disorders in childhood and adolescence. Particularly, that postnatal ID may aggravate pre-existing NDDs. This is an interesting and, seemingly novel review.

Introduction:

Well written, providing good background and context to the study. One missing area relates to the global distribution of ID and NDDs. There is plentiful data mapping the global distribution of ID, showing greatest prevalence in low-resource settings of the world, but also among at risk sub-groups. Is there any similar parallel with the global distribution of NDDs?

Answer: Thank you for pointing this out. We have found a paper entitled Global, regional, and national burden of 12 mental disorders in 204 countries and territories, 1990–2019: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2019, which we have now referenced in the introduction (page 4) and the discussion (page 31):

Given the global distribution of ID [22] paralleled by a similar distribution of NDDs [2], affecting both low and high resource countries, future studies to corroborate the importance of prevention and treatment of ID are of utmost public health importance.

Methods

Was the review pre-registered on PROSPERO?

Answer: PROSPERO does not accept scoping reviews, therefore we were not able to register it. We have, however, included the PRISMA-Scr Checklist (Table S1) to further structure our paper.

I can’t see the age limitation of children for included vs. excluded studies. Where is this information?

Answer: We did not include any age limitations in our inclusion/exclusion criteria. However, we realized that several sentences in the manuscript were referring only to children (when in fact they should have been referring to both children and adults). We have revised this wording where necessary.

Results

The results are clearly presented. The sex differences reported (lines 314 onwards), how did these differences relate to iron status. As this information is presented separately, it is difficult to work this out. Boys are at risk of iron deficiency in contexts of food insecurity, especially during periods of rapid growth. Are boys at greater risk because of differential iron status, rather than other sex-specific factors? It would be good to see the results a little more linked up.

Answer: Thank you for this comment. We have re-analyzed the studies and have updated our results to reflect those studies which looked at any correlation between ID and sex/gender (page 16-17):

Overall, there was a strong preponderance of male participants both in the ADHD and ASD studies (Table 2). The 30 ADHD association studies included 4677 males versus 2880 females (cases only). 12/30 studies examined potential sex differences in risk and severity of ADHD, while 6/30 studies examined potential sex differences in iron levels. 4 of these studies found that there were no significant differences between sex and ADHD symptoms [33,39,42,47]. 5 studies found that males had more ADHD symptoms than females [11,28,34,41,50]. One study [26] compared iron status (iron sufficient, ID without anemia, ID anemia) and sex, and found a statistically significant difference (p<0.001) between the aforementioned groups. Similarly, Cortese et al. [14] reported lower serum ferritin levels in males compared to females, though this finding was not supported by any statistical analysis. The remaining 4 studies that examined sex differences and ID did not find any significant correlations [28,39,47,50].

The 10 ASD association and prevalence studies included 972 males and 305 females (cases only). 2/10 studies examined potential sex differences in individuals with ASD. 1 study reported no significant differences between sex and ASD symptoms [16], while another study found a significant difference but did not elaborate on what this entailed [52]. 1 study analyzed differences between sex and iron status (including ferritin, MCV, hematocrit (Hct), hemoglobin), but did not find any statistically significant difference between males and females [16].

On a similar point, it was hard to find details on actual iron status of the populations under investigation. This could be key to interpreting the findings (e.g. no associations may be observed in populations where there is not sufficient variance in iron status or in populations where status does not cross a ‘threshold’ for deficiency to impact on the outcomes under investigation. I have looked at all the Tables in the supporting information and this detail is also missing from there. I think it could be useful and potentially informative to position the papers against each other with respect to iron status of the children included. This could be done as a Figure, potentially showing levels of iron deficiency anaemia in each context.

Answer: We were not able to find information about the iron status of the populations investigated. However, we now included both the country in which the respective studies were performed and the serum ferritin cut off values chosen in the various studies in tables S2, S3 for ADHD association studies and in S4-S6 for ASD studies. We also appreciate the importance of the recommended cut off levels for SF concentrating as indicators for iron deficiency as well as population-specific confounders in the discussion (page 28-29):

A final aspect in the interpretation of the results obtained in this review is the variance in normal SF values depending on age, sex and genotype. SF < 15 �g/L is generally accepted as the threshold for absence of iron stores [92], however, studies have shown that thresholds for ID in younger children are as low as <10-12 �g/L [93,94]. Whereas the majority of the ASD studies analysed here used SF cut-off values were between 10 and 15 �g/L, SF cut-off values were higher than 15 �g/L in the majority of the ADHD studies.

The heterogeneous thresholds of SF as an indicator for ID might be another reason for discrepant results in the numerous studies performed among various age groups in countries across the globe. Also, if studies were performed in countries where infectious diseases are more common than in high resource countries (e.g., Africa, Asia South America), or during periods of seasonal infectious diseases, results based on SF only could have been confounded by its additional function as an acute phase protein.

Discussion

The discussion is well written and provides a good commentary on the results presented.

However, I was left wondering how much of the evidence for any associations presented was (i) because of iron deficiency per se (e.g. not because of nutritional insufficiency, with iron status being a proxy marker for global micronutrient deficiency) and (ii) how much of the evidence was confounded by other familial risk factors. I can see the benefit of including all types of study design in a scoping review and where trial evidence is limited.

Answer: We addressed the possibility that ID is only a proxy marker for nutritional deficiencies and familial risk factors in the discussion (page 31):

SF as the most accepted outcome measure for ID should be combined with iron markers independent of inflammation and / or with markers indicating inflammation and cut off levels should be harmonized. Additional markers addressing ID as a proxy for micronutrient deficiency or familial predispositions, and iron’s spatial distribution (e.g., brain iron deficiency) should be developed and utilized particularly for studies investigating NDDs.

However, I felt that insufficient weighting was given to the weight of the evidence being presented and, therefore, the associated limitations. This is linked in with the point above about teasing out causal pathways; I think there is scope within this review to do this more thoroughly.

Answer: We addressed the limitations of our review in weighing the evidence presented in the discussion (page 30):

While this review format allowed for a descriptive assessment of the identified studies, comparison of studies against each other was not attempted mainly due to the inconsistent methodological quality of the single studies. We accepted associations and related statistical results as described by the authors of the respective studies and did not exclude any study based on methodological design, participant numbers, choice of outcome measures for NDDs, or laboratory methods employed for measurement of iron markers.

Decision Letter 1

Efthimios M C Skoulakis

17 Aug 2022

Iron deficiency and common neurodevelopmental disorders - A scoping review

PONE-D-21-23571R1

Dear Dr. Stockler,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Efthimios M. C. Skoulakis, PhD

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: N/A

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: The authors have responded to all comments in a considerate and careful manner. Their scoping review should be of interest and help to the community of researchers and medical professionals that work with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, autism, and fetal alcohol syndrome. The role of iron metabolism in ADHD is well highlighted by the present review.

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: Yes: Fanis Missirlis

**********

Acceptance letter

Efthimios M C Skoulakis

19 Sep 2022

PONE-D-21-23571R1

Iron deficiency and common neurodevelopmental disorders – A scoping review

Dear Dr. Stockler:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Efthimios M. C. Skoulakis

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Table. Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic reviews and Meta-Analyses extension for Scoping Reviews (PRISMA-ScR) checklist.

    Adapted from: Tricco AC, Lillie E, Zarin W, O’Brien KK, Colquhoun H, Levac D, et al. PRISMA Extension for Scoping Reviews (PRISMAScR): Checklist and Explanation. Ann Intern Med. 2018;169:467–473. doi: 10.7326/M18-0850.

    (DOCX)

    S1 File. S2 and S3 Tables.

    Studies investigating the association of ADHD and ID.

    (DOCX)

    S2 File. S4, S5, S6 Tables.

    Studies investigating the association of ASD and ID.

    (DOCX)

    S3 File. S7 and S8 Tables.

    Treatment studies investigating the effect of iron supplementation in ADHD and ASD.

    (DOCX)

    Data Availability Statement

    All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting information files.


    Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES