Abstract
The brain’s “default mode network” (DMN) enables flexible switching between internally and externally focused cognition. Precisely how this modulation occurs is not well understood, although it may involve key subcortical mechanisms, including hypothesized influences from the basal forebrain (BF) and mediodorsal thalamus (MD). Here, we used ultra-high field (7 T) functional magnetic resonance imaging to examine the involvement of the BF and MD across states of task-induced DMN activity modulation. Specifically, we mapped DMN activity suppression (“deactivation”) when participants transitioned between rest and externally focused task performance, as well as DMN activity engagement (“activation”) when task performance was internally (i.e., self) focused. Consistent with recent rodent studies, the BF showed overall activity suppression with DMN cortical regions when comparing the rest to external task conditions. Further analyses, including dynamic causal modeling, confirmed that the BF drove changes in DMN cortical activity during these rest-to-task transitions. The MD, by comparison, was specifically engaged during internally focused cognition and demonstrated a broad excitatory influence on DMN cortical activation. These results provide the first direct evidence in humans of distinct BF and thalamic circuit influences on the control of DMN function and suggest novel mechanistic avenues for ongoing translational research.
Keywords: default mode network, basal forebrain, thalamus, self, deactivation
Introduction
Since its first characterization over two decades ago (Andreasen et al. 1995; Shulman et al. 1997; Raichle et al. 2001), the brain’s “default mode network” (DMN) has achieved a special status in the neuroscientific study of cognition and behavior (Davey and Harrison 2018; Buckner and DiNicola 2019; Yeshurun et al. 2021). Current conceptualizations of the DMN, for instance, place it atop of a rich hierarchy of brain systems supporting advanced cognitive processes—a view that emphasizes its unique anatomical organization and connectivity (Margulies et al. 2016; Smallwood et al. 2021). DMN function, in particular, has been predominantly linked to internally-driven cognitive modes, such as self-directed thought, which rely on internal mental representations rather than immediate processing of the external environment (Gusnard et al. 2001; Buckner and Carroll 2007; Andrews-Hanna et al. 2014; Davey and Harrison 2018). Precisely how the DMN coordinates these processes is not yet well understood, though must involve mechanisms that enable the control and integration of activity across its widespread cortical territories, spanning the frontal and posterior midline, and inferior parietal cortices. Based on emerging evidence from human imaging studies, as well as other novel insights (Nair et al. 2018), there is reason to believe that such mechanisms might exist in deeper subcortical structures of the brain, despite such structures being traditionally overlooked in the study of the DMN.
In important recent work, Alves et al. (2019) provided an optimized mapping of DMN structural and functional connectivity that identified the basal forebrain (BF) and anterior- and mediodorsal thalamus (MD) as its major subcortical nodes, with evidence to suggest specific roles in network integration and resilience. Their findings with respect to the BF complement other recent imaging studies that have mapped its functional connectivity with DMN cortical regions (Markello et al. 2018; Yuan et al. 2019), as well as evidence from rodents indicating a role for the BF in modulating DMN dynamics (Nair et al. 2018; Lozano-Montes et al. 2020). In this latter work, BF neurons were shown to exhibit robust activity suppression when animals were transitioned between resting-state and novel task environments—a result that mirrors the well-known pattern of DMN suppression (“deactivation”) in humans when performing externally focused imaging tasks (Shulman et al. 1997; Raichle et al. 2001; Harrison et al. 2008). Critically, in the rodent studies, BF signaling was further shown to drive changes in medial frontal cortex activity, especially during rest, suggesting a mechanism for regulating the DMN between internal and external cognitive states (Nair et al. 2018). Thus, while deactivation has been long appreciated as a unique characteristic of the DMN that reflects its role in facilitating cognitive state-transitions, it is only recently that specific mechanisms underlying this phenomenon have been examined.
By comparison, renewed interest in the contribution of the thalamus to higher cognition focuses on its hypothesized role in coordinating and sustaining information flow and plasticity across large-scale cortical networks (Parnaudeau et al. 2018; Pergola et al. 2018; Halassa and Sherman 2019; Wolff and Vann 2019). Of the higher-order thalamic nuclei, the MD shows preferential connectivity with midline frontal regions of the DMN, especially the medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC), whereas the anterior thalamus is more densely connected to its posterior midline (Goldman-Rakic and Porrino 1985; Vogt et al. 1987; Barbas 2000; Aggleton et al. 2014; Phillips et al. 2019). Both divisions were implicated as DMN hubs in the Alves et al. (2019) study, whereas in a related study by Greene et al. (2020), the MD was mapped more distinctly as a “cognitive integration zone,” linking the DMN to other large-scale cortical networks. Recent work by our group has complemented these findings by mapping a broad excitatory influence of the MD on multiple prefrontal regions, including the MPFC, during a cognitive self-regulation task (Steward et al. 2021). Its results argue for a distinct coordinating role of the MD during higher cognition, whereby its task-evoked activity helps to sustain the engagement of cortical function.
In the current study, we set out to examine the roles of the BF and MD on the function of DMN cortical regions, hypothesizing distinct modulatory influences during states of task-induced deactivation and activation, respectively. Specifically, we hypothesized that the BF would be involved in modulating DMN cortical activity between rest and external cognitive states, consistent with its role in driving DMN state transitions, whereas the MD would broadly upregulate DMN cortical activity during self-oriented higher cognition. The task chosen to address these aims was used in previous work by our group that established a dynamic network model of DMN function, targeting its primary cortical regions, the MPFC, posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) and inferior parietal lobule (IPL; Davey et al. 2016; Davey et al. 2017). These past results also support the task’s capacity to induce significant deactivation of the BF when external cognition is compared to rest, and activation of the MD during self-related versus external cognition (Davey et al. 2016). Here, we used ultra-high field functional magnetic resonance imaging (UHF fMRI) to target their involvement more precisely and to test neural models of their directional influence on DMN cortical regions, with results confirming their unique contributions to network functioning.
Materials and Methods
Participants
Forty participants were recruited to the study. All participants met the following eligibility criteria: (i) they were aged between 18 and 25 years, (ii) had no current or past diagnosis of mental illness, (iii) were competent English speakers, (iv) were not taking any psychoactive medication, and (v) had no contraindications to MRI, including pregnancy. All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and provided written informed consent, following a complete description of the study protocol, which was approved by The University of Melbourne Human Research Ethics Committee. Of the initial sample, 2 participants did not complete the task; 1 did not have physiological data available; and a further 3 participants were excluded due to excessive head motion (see section “Image Pre-processing”). The final sample consisted of 34 participants (21 female) with a mean age of 22.0 years (± 2.0 years).
Experimental Design
Participants completed the “self-appraisal” task, as detailed in Davey et al. (2016), which comprised three task elements: self-related cognition, external cognition, and rest. In the self and external conditions, participants were presented with personality trait adjectives and responded to them as instructed below. Words were drawn from a frequently used list of trait adjectives (Anderson 1968): we selected 96 words distributed around the median rating for “likeableness” reported in the original dataset. The words were selected so as not to be extremely favorable or unfavorable, with the intention of engaging participants in self-referential appraisal (adjectives included words such as “skeptical,” “perfectionistic,” and “lucky”). For the self-condition, participants viewed 8 blocks of 6 words, presented for 5 s each, and responded to the question, “Does this word describe you?,” by pressing the left or right button on the button-box (Lumina, Cedrus Corporation). In the external condition, participants viewed 8 blocks of 6 words, also presented for 5 s each, and responded to the question, “Does this word have 4 or more vowels?” The 2 lists of 48 words that formed these conditions were matched on valence and number of vowels and were counterbalanced across participants. Each 32 s block (2 s instruction followed by 6 words presented for 5 s each) was interspersed with a 10 s rest-fixation block (17 in total) in which participants were asked to fixate on a centrally presented cross-hair. The task was programmed in Paradigm software (http://www.paradigmexperiments.com) and was presented on a 32” LCD BOLD screen (Cambridge Research Systems) visible via a reverse mirror mounted to the head coil.
UHF Image Acquisition
Imaging was performed on a 7 T research scanner (Siemens Healthcare, Erlangen, Germany) equipped with a 32-channel head-coil (Nova Medical Inc., Wilmington MA, USA). The functional sequence consisted of a multi-band (6 times) and grappa (2 times) accelerated GE-EPI sequence in the steady state (TR, 800 ms; TE, 22.2 ms; pulse/flip angle, 45°; field-of-view, 20.8 cm; slice thickness (no gap), 1.6 mm; 130 × 130-pixel matrix; 84 interleaved axial slices aligned to AC-PC line; Setsompop et al. 2012). The total sequence time was 11 minutes and 22 seconds, corresponding to 852 whole-brain EPI volumes. A T1-weighted high-resolution anatomical image (MP2RAGE; Marques et al. 2010) was acquired for each participant to assist with functional time series co-registration (TR = 5000 ms; TE, 3.0 ms; inversion times, 700/2700 ms; pulse/flip angles, 4/5°; field-of-view, 24 cm; slice thickness (no gap), 0.73 mm; 330 × 330–pixel matrix; 84 sagittal slices aligned parallel to the midline). The total sequence time was 7 minutes and 12 seconds. To assist with head immobility, foam-padding inserts were placed on either side of the participants’ head. Cardiac and respiratory recordings were sampled at 50 Hertz (Hz) using a Siemens (Bluetooth) pulse-oximeter and respiratory belt. Information derived from these recordings were used for physiological noise correction (see further).
Image Pre-Processing
Imaging data were pre-processed using Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM) 12 (v7771, Welcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, London, UK) within a MATLAB 2019b environment (The MathWorks Inc., Natick, MA). Motion artifacts were corrected by realigning each participant’s time-series to the mean image, and all images were resampled using 4th Degree B-Spline interpolation. Individualized motion regressors were created using the Motion Fingerprint approach to account for movement (Wilke 2012). As noted above, 3 participants were excluded due to a mean total scan-to-scan displacement over 1.6 mm (i.e., the size of one voxel). Each participant’s anatomical images were co-registered to their respective mean functional image, segmented, and normalized to the International Consortium of Brain Mapping template using the unified segmentation plus DARTEL approach. Smoothing was applied with a 3.2-mm3 full-width-at-half-maximum (FWHM) Gaussian kernel to preserve spatial specificity.
Physiological noise was modeled at the first level using the PhysIO Toolbox (Kasper et al. 2017). This toolbox applies noise correction to fMRI sequences using physiological recordings and has been found to improve blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) signal sensitivity and temporal signal-to-noise ratio (tSNR) at 7 T (Reynaud et al. 2017; see also Steward et al. 2021). The Retrospective Image-based Correction function (RETROICOR; Glover et al. 2000) was applied to model the periodic effects of heartbeat and breathing on BOLD signals, using acquired cardiac/respiratory phase information. The Respiratory Response Function (RRF; Birn et al. 2008), convolved with respiration volume per time (RVT) was used to model low frequency signal fluctuations, which arose from changes in breathing depth and rate. Heart rate variability (HRV) was convolved with a predefined Cardiac Response Function (CRF; Chang et al. 2009) to account for BOLD variances due to heartrate-dependent changes in blood oxygenation.
General Linear Modeling
Each participant’s pre-processed time-series and nuisance regressors (i.e., physiological noise and Motion Fingerprint regressors) were included in a first-level GLM analysis. Primary regressors for the self and external conditions, as well as task instruction periods, were created by specifying the onset and duration of each block, followed by convolution with a canonical hemodynamic response function. The rest condition periods formed an implicit baseline. A 128-Hz high-pass filter was applied to account for low-frequency noise and temporal autocorrelation was estimated using SPM’s FAST method, which has been shown to outperform AR(1) at short TRs and yield superior reliability (Olszowy et al. 2019). Primary contrast images (external vs. baseline/rest; self vs. baseline/rest) for each participant were entered in a second-level random-effects GLM analysis (2 level, within-subjects ANOVA). Three main group effects were estimated from this model: rest > external (to map regions of DMN deactivation); self > external (to map regions of DMN activation); and their conjunction (to map common “core” DMN cortical regions; see Davey et al. 2016). These analyses were used as both initial tests of our hypotheses regarding the involvement of the BF and MD during states of DMN deactivation and activation, respectively, and to support the mapping of specific regions to be included in dynamic causal modeling (DCM), as will be described below. For all GLM analyses, whole-brain, false discovery rate (FDR) corrected statistical thresholds were applied (PFDR < 0.05; 10 voxel cluster-extent threshold).
Dynamic Causal Modeling
The aim of DCM is to infer the causal architecture of a network of brain regions, which in the case of fMRI time-series data, involves using generative models to estimate functional interactions from underlying (hidden) neurophysiological activity (Friston et al. 2003; Stephan et al. 2010; Friston et al. 2019). Using a Bayesian approach, a model is selected from a set of predefined models that is the most likely to generate the observed imaging data while also penalizing for model complexity. DCMs delineate how dynamics in one brain region influence dynamics in others, incorporating both the core set of interregional (“baseline”) connections and the modulation of those influences by experimental (i.e., task) manipulation. As outlined in Stephan et al. (2010), DCMs are both sensitive to temporal precedence effects between neuronal populations (i.e., how the present activity state of one region causes change in other), as well as the spatiotemporal structure of a network’s dynamics: that is, specifically “where and when” these interactions occur under the influence of experimental (i.e., task) manipulations. The relative evidence of these causal influences can then be compared through model comparison to determine which model architecture optimally explains the data. Compared to temporal correlation measures of functional connectivity, DCM provides a more detailed and physiologically valid mapping of effective connectivity—the directed causal influences of brain regions on one another. In DCM, modulation is measured in Hz, which signifies the rates of change in activity (connection strength) caused by the influence of one region on another. Positive effective connectivity reflects putative excitatory upregulation of activity, whereas negative connectivity represents inhibitory downregulation. Notably, it has been shown that 7 T fMRI furnishes more efficient estimates of effective connectivity than those provided at lower field strengths (Tak et al. 2018).
Region Selection and Time-Series Extraction
As is standard with DCM, initial group-level GLM analyses were performed to map our specific regions of interest (BF, MD, MPFC, PCC, IPL; see section “Results” for a full description). The regional time-series (VOIs) for each of them were extracted at an individual subject-level following recently published guidelines (Zeidman et al. 2019a). This method summarizes regional time-series by calculating the first eigenvariate across all activated voxels within a 4-mm radial sphere around the subject specific maxima (P < 0.05, uncorrected), which can be no more than 8 mm from the group maxima. To ensure that voxels included in BF and MD VOIs did not capture adjacent regions, their time-series extraction was run after applying inclusive BF and thalamic masks, respectively, as provided by Alves et al. (2019; https://neurovault.org/collections/CTTXXAYJ/). Because the Alves et al. “limbic thalamus” mask extends to the anterior thalamic nucleus, we further applied the Automated Anatomical Labelling 3 (AAL3) atlas (1 × 1 × 1 mm3 resolution) to limit data extraction to the MD (Rolls et al. 2020). Participant time-series were pre-whitened to reduce serial correlations, high-pass filtered, and nuisance effects not covered by the “effects of interest” F-contrast were regressed out (i.e., “adjusted” to the F-contrast).
Model Specification and Selection
Our model space was specified using DCM 12.5. To address our hypotheses of distinct modulatory influences of the BF and MD, we specified separate full models assuming bidirectional endogenous (“baseline”) connections between all regions, with the condition “task”—the onset of all blocks comprising the self and rest conditions—set as their driving input. We specified the models separately to prevent unnecessary complexity and to provide the most parsimonious account of the observed model evidence (Stephan et al. 2010). For the BF model, the rest condition was specified as the modulatory input on each connection to and from the BF (i.e., BF to MPFC, PCC, IPL, MD and MPFC, PCC, IPL, MD to the BF), whereas the self-condition was set as the modulatory input for the MD model (MD to MPFC, PCC, IPL, MD and MPFC, PCC, IPL, BF to MD). In both models, we examined the modulation of BF–MD connections given their well-characterized anatomical relationship, particularly regarding BF modulation of MD function (Young et al. 1984; Groenewegen 1988). Figure 1 illustrates the model testing structure for the “BF-rest” and “MD-self” DCMs.
Full models of effective connectivity were fitted to each participant’s time-series data to calculate the posterior parameter estimates and their probabilities for each connection. At the group level, the connectivity parameter estimates from all participants’ DCMs were assessed using Parametric Empirical Bayes (PEB). The PEB framework affords robust group-level analyses of effective connectivity by means of a hierarchical model, comprising DCMs at the single-subject level and a GLM of connectivity parameters between subjects (Zeidman et al. 2019b). After estimating the PEB model, parameters that did not contribute to the model evidence were pruned using Bayesian model reduction. Bayesian model averaging was then performed over these reduced models to determine parameter estimates: a process which prioritizes simpler and more generalizable models at the group level. A conservative threshold of posterior probability (Pp) > 0.99 was applied to identify only those parameters that demonstrated very strong evidence (that is, those parameters with a 99% probability of parameters being present vs. absent).
Results
General Linear Modeling
As expected, DMN deactivation (rest > external) was associated with broad involvement of characteristic cortical regions of the DMN: the anterior medial wall cortex, spanning ventral to dorsal MPFC, including rostral anterior cingulate cortex; the posteromedial wall cortex, spanning the PCC, retrosplenial, mid cingulate cortex, and extending to the precuneus and medial parieto-occipital zone (Fig. 2A; Table S1). Additional cortical deactivations included the ventral and dorsal posterior insular cortex; dorsal prefrontal cortex (frontal eye fields); anterior lingual gyrus; anterior and posterior hippocampus (CA1–3; para/subiculum; parahippocampal areas). Subcortically, significant deactivation was observed in the BF; the MD and superior colliculus. Deactivation of the BF was primarily localized to the medial septum and diagonal band areas but extended to surrounding territories including the ventral pallidum (Fig. S1A).
DMN activation (self > external) was also associated with the broad engagement of characteristic cortical DMN regions: the anterior medial wall cortex, spanning ventral to dorsal MPFC, including rostral anterior cingulate cortex; the posteromedial wall cortex, spanning the PCC, retrosplenial and mid cingulate cortex; the bilateral IPL (angular gyri) and middle temporal gyrus extending to anterior temporal pole (Fig. 2B; Table S2). Additional areas of cortical activation included the left frontal opercular, lateral prefrontal, and posterior temporal cortices; anterior hippocampus (CA1–3; subiculum); as well as the posterior cerebellum (Crus 1/II). Subcortically, significant activation was observed in the thalamus, encompassing the MD and anterior thalamic nuclei; the medial caudate nucleus (body); the internal globus pallidus; the ventral tegmentum extending to substantia nigra; and the insular claustrum. Activation of the MD was primarily localized to its internal (magnocellular) division, but extended anterior-laterally, reaching the thalamic reticular border (Fig. S1B).
The “core” DMN cortical regions identified by conjunction analysis are shown in Fig. 2C (Table S3) and confirm a common involvement of the MPFC, with a ventromedial focus; PCC and IPL, particularly the left hemisphere (as in Davey et al. 2016). Additional areas of common engagement included the retrosplenial and mid-cingulate cortex; the medial parieto-occipital zone; the middle temporal gyrus extending to anterior temporal pole; the anterior hippocampus (CA1–3; subiculum); and the posterior cerebellum (Crus 1/II).
Figure S2 provides a further direct comparison between the rest and self-conditions. This comparison is useful in illustrating that the BF and MD were significantly differentially engaged across these conditions, with greater BF deactivation occurring during rest versus self, and greater MD activation during self vs. rest. All GLM results are presented on the “Synthesized_FLASH25” (500 μm, MNI space) ex vivo template (Edlow et al. 2019).
Dynamic Causal Modeling
We applied DCM to test specific hypotheses regarding the modulatory influence of the BF (under the rest condition) and MD (under the self-condition) on the function of the DMN cortical regions. For the BF and MD, we used inclusive masks from the Alves et al. study to localize group maxima for these regions and to further constrain their time-series extraction across participants. Figure S1 illustrates the specific areas of BF deactivation and MD activation that were localized within the respective boundaries of the Alves et al. masks.
BF Modulation of the DMN
Under the rest condition, all BF connections, except for the MD-to-BF path, showed evidence of modulation that exceeded our stringent threshold (Pp > 0.99; Fig. 3A). BF activity was found to have a strong excitatory (positive) influence on the activity of all cortical DMN regions (range 2.23–2.85 Hz), with a low/moderate excitatory influence on MD activity (0.44 Hz). The MPFC had a strong reciprocal excitatory influence on BF activity (2.06 Hz), whereas the PCC (−0.85 Hz) and IPL (−1.16 Hz) had more moderate inhibitory (negative) influences. Table S4 provides a complete list of parameter estimates for the baseline and modulatory connections for the BF model. Compared to the observed modulatory effects, the BF demonstrated low/moderate strength baseline connections with the other regions, and vice versa.
MD Modulation of the DMN
Under the self-condition, all MD connections, except for the MPFC-to-MD path, showed evidence of modulation that exceeded our threshold (Pp > 0.99; Fig. 3B). MD activity was found to have a strong excitatory influence on the activity of all cortical regions (range 0.87–1.94 Hz), with a more moderate excitatory influence on BF activity (0.48 Hz). The PCC and IPL had moderate to strong excitatory influence on MD activity (0.87 and − 0.38 Hz, respectively), while the BF had a notably strong inhibitory influence (−4.34 Hz). Table S5 provides a complete list of parameter estimates for the baseline and modulatory connections for the MD model. Compared to the observed modulatory effects, the MD demonstrated low/moderate strength baseline connections with other regions (and vice versa), with no significant connections for the MD to BF; PCC or IPL to MD paths. The full set of DCM results (BF and MD models) are available at https://github.com/benharrison-uom/DCM_subcortex_modulation.
Regional Dynamic Activity
Figure 4 presents group-level (model predicted) responses for the BF (cyan), MD (orange) and MPFC (purple) across the entire task duration and when averaged across a full task epoch (self, rest, external, rest). Figure S3 presents the three cortical regional responses together. These results highlight the consistency of the task evoked neural dynamics, as broadly endorsed by the GLM and DCM findings. Regarding the BF, we note a pattern of evoked responses that preceded changes in MPFC activity during the external and subsequent rest condition blocks, as compared to during the self and subsequent rest condition block. For both the BF and MD, we note a pattern of activity coupling versus decoupling with the MPFC across the self-versus external condition blocks, respectively, as well as their more sustained activity across the self-condition block.
Discussion
The basal forebrain and mediodorsal thalamus have been identified as key subcortical structures of the DMN, with evidence pointing towards their unique contributions to dynamic network functioning (Nair et al. 2018; Alves et al. 2019; Buckner and DiNicola 2019; Steward et al. 2021). In this study, we used task-based UHF fMRI to confirm their involvement across states of DMN activity suppression and engagement, and tested neural systems models of their modulatory influence on DMN cortical regions. Our results endorse a specific role for the BF in driving DMN states transitions between internal and external cognitive modes, extending recent work in rodents (Nair et al. 2018). They also endorse a role for the MD in supporting computations involving major prefrontal circuits (Pergola et al. 2018), which we link here directly to the DMN and its involvement in self-related cognition.
The BF showed an overall pattern of reduced activity during the external task condition compared to rest, suggesting that it was “deactivated” together with the broader DMN, as seen extensively in human functional imaging studies (Shulman et al. 1997; Raichle et al. 2001; Harrison et al. 2008). However, closer inspection of our data revealed that while BF activity was decreased on average when comparing these two conditions, it in fact demonstrated a positive evoked response to the external condition, while DMN cortical regions showed consistent activity suppression (Figs. 4 and S3). These responses appeared to be driven by the condition’s onset phase (i.e., were not sustained), and consistently preceded changes in DMN cortical regions—an observation that extended to the subsequent rest condition blocks. Given that the self-condition, like rest, promotes an internal mental focus, this suggests that BF responses generally tracked the switch between internal and external cognitive states. While DMN cortical regions also exhibited switching responses during the rest to external task transitions, consistent with other recent studies (Crittenden et al. 2015; Smith et al. 2018, 2021), they were less prominent than the BF’s response and were more broadly defined by activity suppression. In the rodent study by Nair et al. (2018), the transition between rest and external task states, especially the reengagement of DMN cortical activity during rest, was causally attributed to BF function. Network modeling of our task’s rest condition also supports these findings: we found the BF to have a robust excitatory influence over DMN cortical regions, whilst receiving distinct reciprocal modulation by the MPFC. We also note that the strength of its influence on DMN cortical regions was several magnitudes stronger that its influence on the MD, which suggests that its influence was likely direct, as opposed to being indirectly mediated via the MD.
In human imaging studies, the BF’s prominent structural and functional connectivity with DMN cortical regions has been suggested to parallel its known modulatory projections involving cholinergic, GABA- and glutamatergic systems (Markello et al. 2018; Alves et al. 2019; Yuan et al. 2019). Using a connectome-based approach, Alves et al. (2019) identified the BF medial septum and diagonal band nuclei as the most densely coupled to DMN cortex, which aligns well with our primary mapping of BF responses. In the Nair et al. study, BF modulation sites more specifically targeted the ventral pallidum and nucleus basalis. Study differences aside, however, the regulation of DMN activity is likely to reflect mechanisms implicating the extended BF circuitry. In recent follow-up studies to Nair et al., for instance, the ascending BF GABAergic (parvalbumin positive; PV+) projection neuron system has been specifically linked to the control of DMN state transitions via its modulation of DMN cortical gamma activity (Lozano-Montes et al. 2020; Klaassen et al. 2021). These neurons, which are distributed throughout the BF in rodents, have long-range connections to DMN cortical regions, particularly with the MPFC and retrosplenial PCC (McKenna et al. 2013; Do et al. 2016). While equivalent data in humans are lacking, there is compelling evidence that links human DMN cortical function to high-gamma modulation (itself a robust correlate of fMRI BOLD signal change; (Fox et al. 2018), as well as local GABA concentrations (Northoff et al. 2007; Hu et al. 2013). Thus, overall, our findings may suggest underlying mechanisms, including GABAergic PV+ influences, that generally index the cortical modulatory function of BF circuitry, particularly in driving cognitive state-transitions.
As expected, the MD was activated with DMN cortical regions during the self-compared to the external (and rest) conditions. This result is consistent with past studies, including our own, that have reported significant MD involvement in tasks probing aspects of self-referential cognition (Araujo et al. 2015; Davey et al. 2016). Recently, in Steward et al. (2021), we examined the MD’s specific modulatory influence on distributed prefrontal regions, addressing the hypothesis that it has a key orchestrating influence on the activity of task-relevant higher cortical circuits (Parnaudeau et al. 2018; Pergola et al. 2018; Halassa and Sherman 2019; Wolff and Vann 2019). We used a task that evoked aspects of self-referential cognition and regulatory control and found the MD to have a robust excitatory influence on multiple PFC subregions, whilst receiving distinct reciprocal modulation from the MPFC. Our current results broadly replicate these findings, with the MD again having a robust excitatory influence on DMN cortical regions, while receiving more moderate reciprocal input from the cortex. In Steward et al. (2021), we argued that these broad excitatory influences likely serve to increase the synchrony of cortical regions to help sustain and update complex mental representations (i.e., self-representations) that emerge over time. This idea is well supported by animal studies of MD-cortical circuit connectivity (Schmitt et al. 2017), and gains some further support here, where we observed MD recruitment to be more sustained than DMN cortical regions over the course of the self-condition epoch (Fig. 4). A focus for ongoing work will be to understand how these dynamics specifically shape patterns of cortico-cortical connectivity across the DMN under distinct cognitive contexts.
We localized MD activity primarily to its internal (magnocellular) segment, which has characteristically dense connections with the MPFC, but only sparse connections with posterior DMN regions (Asanuma et al. 1985; Goldman-Rakic and Porrino 1985; Vogt et al. 1987; Groenewegen 1988; Barbas 2000). Thus, although we observed excitatory links between the MD and DMN cortical regions, the links between the MD and PCC/IPL likely reflect indirect (i.e., intermediary or global network) influences (Adachi et al. 2012). In the MD model, we also identified a prominent inhibitory influence of the BF. The strength of this connection was almost twice that observed for the BF’s excitatory influence on cortical connections (during rest), suggesting that it had a particularly potent influence on MD activity. It is well known that the thalamus receives significant modulatory input from the BF, with the largest proportion of projections (predominately GABAergic) terminating in the reticular nucleus and more moderately in the MD internal segment (Hallanger et al. 1987; Groenewegen 1988; Churchill et al. 1996; Gritti et al. 1998). As noted in Brown and McKenna (2015), these BF projections maintain a high rate of tonic thalamic inhibition, however, in situations requiring cognitive/attentional engagement, their influence is transiently suppressed, potentiating thalamocortical transmission. Importantly, this relationship is recognized to be distinct from the influence of cortically projecting BF GABAergic neurons, which preferentially target inhibitory interneurons, allowing disinhibitory effects and entrainment of fast cortical (i.e., gamma) oscillations. Thus, although fMRI connectivity estimates reflect complex modulatory effects, the observation of distinct cortical excitatory versus thalamic inhibitory effects has a strong anatomical precedent with regards to BF GABAergic function.
Conclusion
Since its discovery, the study of the DMN has been principally one concerned with higher cortical function, as exemplified by recent perspectives on the DMN as the brain’s “apex transmodal association network” (Margulies et al. 2016; Buckner and DiNicola 2019; Smallwood et al. 2021). However, unique features of the DMN, particularly its characteristic activity suppression during externally focused cognitive tasks, have raised questions about the existence of a subcortical modulatory axis that may support its capacity for dynamic integrative processing. Here, we provide the first direct evidence in humans that confirms distinct basal forebrain and thalamic circuit influences on the control of DMN function. These observations, together with ongoing work, may ultimately help to deliver a more complete, whole-brain perspective on the contribution of the DMN to cognition and behavior, including its role in enigmatic mental processes, such as those related to “the self.”
Funding
National Health and Medical Research Council of Australia (NHMRC) (Project Grant 1161897 to B.J.H.); NHMRC Career Development Fellowship (1124472 and 1141738, respectively to B.J.H. and C.G.D.). Australian Government Research Training Program (RTP) Scholarships to A.J.J. and H.S.S; NHMRC-MRFF Investigator (grant MRF1193736 to T.S.); a BBRF Young Investigator Grant; the University of Melbourne McKenzie Fellowship.
Notes
We thank Lisa Incerti and Cristian Stella for their contributions to data collection and acknowledge the scientific and technical assistance of the Australian National Imaging Facility—a National Collaborative Research Infrastructure Strategy (NCRIS) capability at the Melbourne Brain Centre Imaging Unit (MBCIU), The University of Melbourne. The multiband fMRI sequence was generously supported by a research collaboration agreement with CMRR, The University of Minnesota and the MP2RAGE works in progress sequence was provided by Siemens Healthineers (Germany). Conflict of Interest: None declared.
Supplementary Material
Contributor Information
Ben J Harrison, Melbourne Neuropsychiatry Centre, Department of Psychiatry, The University of Melbourne, Parkville, Victoria 3010, Australia.
Christopher G Davey, Department of Psychiatry, The University of Melbourne, Parkville, Victoria 3010, Australia.
Hannah S Savage, Melbourne Neuropsychiatry Centre, Department of Psychiatry, The University of Melbourne, Parkville, Victoria 3010, Australia.
Alec J Jamieson, Melbourne Neuropsychiatry Centre, Department of Psychiatry, The University of Melbourne, Parkville, Victoria 3010, Australia.
Christine A Leonards, Melbourne Neuropsychiatry Centre, Department of Psychiatry, The University of Melbourne, Parkville, Victoria 3010, Australia.
Bradford A Moffat, Melbourne Brain Centre Imaging Unit, Department of Radiology, The University of Melbourne, Parkville, Victoria 3010, Australia.
Rebecca K Glarin, Melbourne Brain Centre Imaging Unit, Department of Radiology, The University of Melbourne, Parkville, Victoria 3010, Australia.
Trevor Steward, Melbourne Neuropsychiatry Centre, Department of Psychiatry, The University of Melbourne, Parkville, Victoria 3010, Australia; Melbourne School of Psychological Sciences, The University of Melbourne, Parkville, Victoria 3010, Australia.
References
- Adachi Y, Osada T, Sporns O, Watanabe T, Matsui T, Miyamoto K, Miyashita Y. 2012. Functional connectivity between anatomically unconnected areas is shaped by collective network-level effects in the macaque cortex. Cereb Cortex. 22:1586–1592. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aggleton JP, Saunders RC, Wright NF, Vann SD. 2014. The origin of projections from the posterior cingulate and retrosplenial cortices to the anterior, medial dorsal and laterodorsal thalamic nuclei of macaque monkeys. Eur J Neurosci. 39:107–123. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alves PN, Foulon C, Karolis V, Bzdok D, Margulies DS, Volle E, Thiebaut de Schotten M. 2019. An improved neuroanatomical model of the default-mode network reconciles previous neuroimaging and neuropathological findings. Commun Biol. 2:370. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anderson NH. 1968. Likableness ratings of 555 personality-trait words. J Pers Soc Psychol. 9:272–279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andreasen NC, O’Leary DS, Cizadlo T, Arndt S, Rezai K, Watkins GL, Ponto LL, Hichwa RD. 1995. Remembering the past: two facets of episodic memory explored with positron emission tomography. Am J Psychiatry. 152:1576–1585. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andrews-Hanna JR, Smallwood J, Spreng RN. 2014. The default network and self-generated thought: component processes, dynamic control, and clinical relevance. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1316:29–52. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Araujo HF, Kaplan J, Damasio H, Damasio A. 2015. Neural correlates of different self domains. Brain Behav. 5:e00409. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Asanuma C, Andersen RA, Cowan WM. 1985. The thalamic relations of the caudal inferior parietal lobule and the lateral prefrontal cortex in monkeys: divergent cortical projections from cell clusters in the medial pulvinar nucleus. J Comp Neurol. 241:357–381. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barbas H. 2000. Connections underlying the synthesis of cognition, memory, and emotion in primate prefrontal cortices. Brain Res Bull. 52:319–330. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Birn RM, Smith MA, Jones TB, Bandettini PA. 2008. The respiration response function: the temporal dynamics of fMRI signal fluctuations related to changes in respiration. NeuroImage. 40:644–654. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown RE, McKenna JT. 2015. Turning a negative into a positive: ascending GABAergic control of cortical activation and arousal. Front Neurol. 6:135. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buckner RL, Carroll DC. 2007. Self-projection and the brain. Trends Cogn Sci. 11:49–57. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buckner RL, DiNicola LM. 2019. The brain’s default network: updated anatomy, physiology and evolving insights. Nat Rev Neurosci. 20:593–608. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chang C, Cunningham JP, Glover GH. 2009. Influence of heart rate on the BOLD signal: the cardiac response function. NeuroImage. 44:857–869. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Churchill L, Zahm DS, Kalivas PW. 1996. The mediodorsal nucleus of the thalamus in rats--I. forebrain gabaergic innervation. Neuroscience. 70:93–102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crittenden BM, Mitchell DJ, Duncan J. 2015. Recruitment of the default mode network during a demanding act of executive control. elife. 4:e06481. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davey CG, Harrison BJ. 2018. The brain’s center of gravity: how the default mode network helps us to understand the self. World Psychiatry. 17:278–279. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davey CG, Pujol J, Harrison BJ. 2016. Mapping the self in the brain’s default mode network. NeuroImage. 132:390–397. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davey CG, Breakspear M, Pujol J, Harrison BJ. 2017. A brain model of disturbed self-appraisal in depression. Am J Psychiatry. 174:895–903. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Do JP, Xu M, Lee SH, Chang WC, Zhang S, Chung S, Yung TJ, Fan JL, Miyamichi K, Luo L, et al. 2016. Cell type-specific long-range connections of basal forebrain circuit. elife. 5:e13214. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Edlow BL, Mareyam A, Horn A, Polimeni JR, Witzel T, Tisdall MD, Augustinack JC, Stockmann JP, Diamond BR, Stevens A, et al. 2019. 7 tesla MRI of the ex vivo human brain at 100 micron resolution. Sci Data. 6:244. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fox KCR, Foster BL, Kucyi A, Daitch AL, Parvizi J. 2018. Intracranial electrophysiology of the human default network. Trends Cogn Sci. 22:307–324. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friston KJ, Harrison L, Penny W. 2003. Dynamic causal modelling. NeuroImage. 19:1273–1302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friston KJ, Preller KH, Mathys C, Cagnan H, Heinzle J, Razi A, Zeidman P. 2019. Dynamic causal modelling revisited. NeuroImage. 199:730–744. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glover GH, Li TQ, Ress D. 2000. Image-based method for retrospective correction of physiological motion effects in fMRI: RETROICOR. Magn Reson Med. 44:162–167. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldman-Rakic PS, Porrino LJ. 1985. The primate mediodorsal (MD) nucleus and its projection to the frontal lobe. J Comp Neurol. 242:535–560. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greene DJ, et al. Dosenbach NUF. 2020. Integrative and Network-Specific Connectivity of the Basal Ganglia and Thalamus Defined in Individuals. Neuron. 105(4):742–758.e6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gritti I, Mariotti M, Mancia M. 1998. GABAergic and cholinergic basal forebrain and preoptic-anterior hypothalamic projections to the mediodorsal nucleus of the thalamus in the cat. Neuroscience. 85:149–178. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Groenewegen HJ. 1988. Organization of the afferent connections of the mediodorsal thalamic nucleus in the rat, related to the mediodorsal-prefrontal topography. Neuroscience. 24:379–431. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gusnard DA, Akbudak E, Shulman GL, Raichle ME. 2001. Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: relation to a default mode of brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 98:4259–4264. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Halassa MM, Sherman SM. 2019. Thalamocortical circuit motifs: a general framework. Neuron. 103:762–770. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hallanger AE, Levey AI, Lee HJ, Rye DB, Wainer BH. 1987. The origins of cholinergic and other subcortical afferents to the thalamus in the rat. J Comp Neurol. 262:105–124. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harrison BJ, Pujol J, López-Solà M, Hernández-Ribas R, Deus J, Ortiz H, Soriano-Mas C, Yücel M, Pantelis C, Cardoner N. 2008. Consistency and functional specialization in the default mode brain network. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 105:9781–9786. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hu Y, Chen X, Gu H, Yang Y. 2013. Resting-state glutamate and GABA concentrations predict task-induced deactivation in the default mode network. J Neurosci. 33:18566–18573. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kasper L, Bollmann S, Diaconescu AO, Hutton C, Heinzle J, Iglesias S, Hauser TU, Sebold M, Manjaly ZM, Pruessmann KP, et al. 2017. The PhysIO toolbox for Modeling physiological noise in fMRI data. J Neurosci Methods. 276:56–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klaassen AL, Heiniger A, Vaca Sánchez P, Harvey MA, Rainer G. 2021. Ventral pallidum regulates the default mode network, controlling transitions between internally and externally guided behavior. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 118(36):e2103642118. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lozano-Montes L, Dimanico M, Mazloum R, Li W, Nair J, Kintscher M, Schneggenburger R, Harvey M, Rainer G. 2020. Optogenetic stimulation of basal forebrain Parvalbumin neurons activates the default mode network and associated Behaviors. Cell Rep. 33:108359. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Margulies DS, Ghosh SS, Goulas A, Falkiewicz M, Huntenburg JM, Langs G, Bezgin G, Eickhoff SB, Castellanos FX, Petrides M, et al. 2016. Situating the default-mode network along a principal gradient of macroscale cortical organization. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 113:12574–12579. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Markello RD, Spreng RN, Luh WM, Anderson AK, De Rosa E. 2018. Segregation of the human basal forebrain using resting state functional MRI. NeuroImage. 173:287–297. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marques JP, Kober T, Krueger G, van der Zwaag W, Van de Moortele PF, Gruetter R. 2010. MP2RAGE, a self bias-field corrected sequence for improved segmentation and T1-mapping at high field. NeuroImage. 49:1271–1281. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McKenna JT, Yang C, Franciosi S, Winston S, Abarr KK, Rigby MS, Yanagawa Y, McCarley RW, Brown RE. 2013. Distribution and intrinsic membrane properties of basal forebrain GABAergic and parvalbumin neurons in the mouse. J Comp Neurol. 521:1225–1250. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nair J, Klaassen AL, Arato J, Vyssotski AL, Harvey M, Rainer G. 2018. Basal forebrain contributes to default mode network regulation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 115:1352–1357. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Northoff G, Walter M, Schulte RF, Beck J, Dydak U, Henning A, Boeker H, Grimm S, Boesiger P. 2007. GABA concentrations in the human anterior cingulate cortex predict negative BOLD responses in fMRI. Nat Neurosci. 10:1515–1517. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olszowy W, Aston J, Rua C, Williams GB. 2019. Accurate autocorrelation modeling substantially improves fMRI reliability. Nat Commun. 10:1220. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parnaudeau S, Bolkan SS, Kellendonk C. 2018. The Mediodorsal thalamus: an essential partner of the prefrontal cortex for cognition. Biol Psychiatry. 83:648–656. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pergola G, Danet L, Pitel AL, Carlesimo GA, Segobin S, Pariente J, Suchan B, Mitchell AS, Barbeau EJ. 2018. The regulatory role of the human Mediodorsal thalamus. Trends Cogn Sci. 22:1011–1025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Phillips JM, Fish LR, Kambi NA, Redinbaugh MJ, Mohanta S, Kecskemeti SR, Saalmann YB. 2019. Topographic organization of connections between prefrontal cortex and mediodorsal thalamus: evidence for a general principle of indirect thalamic pathways between directly connected cortical areas. NeuroImage. 189:832–846. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raichle ME, MacLeod AM, Snyder AZ, Powers WJ, Gusnard DA, Shulman GL. 2001. A default mode of brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 98:676–682. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reynaud O, Jorge J, Gruetter R, Marques JP, van der Zwaag W. 2017. Influence of physiological noise on accelerated 2D and 3D resting state functional MRI data at 7 T. Magn Reson Med. 78:888–896. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolls ET, Huang CC, Lin CP, Feng J, Joliot M. 2020. Automated anatomical labelling atlas 3. NeuroImage. 206:116189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmitt LI, Wimmer RD, Nakajima M, Happ M, Mofakham S, Halassa MM. 2017. Thalamic amplification of cortical connectivity sustains attentional control. Nature. 545(7653):219–223. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Setsompop K, Gagoski BA, Polimeni JR, Witzel T, Wedeen VJ, Wald LL. 2012. Blipped-controlled aliasing in parallel imaging for simultaneous multislice echo planar imaging with reduced g-factor penalty. Magn Reson Med. 67:1210–1224. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shulman GL, Fiez JA, Corbetta M, Buckner RL, Miezin FM, Raichle ME, Petersen SE. 1997. Common blood flow changes across visual tasks: II. Decreases in cerebral cortex. J Cogn Neurosci. 9:648–663. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smallwood J, Bernhardt BC, Leech R, Bzdok D, Jefferies E, Margulies DS. 2021. The default mode network in cognition: a topographical perspective. Nat Rev Neurosci. 22:503–513. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith V, Mitchell DJ, Duncan J. 2018. Role of the default mode network in cognitive transitions. Cereb Cortex. 28:3685–3696. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith V, Duncan J, Mitchell DJ. 2021. Roles of the default mode and multiple-demand networks in naturalistic versus symbolic decisions. J Neurosci. 41:2214–2228. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stephan KE, Penny WD, Moran RJ, den Ouden HE, Daunizeau J, Friston KJ. 2010. Ten simple rules for dynamic causal modeling. NeuroImage. 49:3099–3109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steward T, Kung PH, Davey CG, Moffat BA, Glarin RK, Jamieson A, Felmingham K, Harrison BJ. 2021. A thalamo-centric neural signature for restructuring negative self-beliefs. Mol Psychiatry. In Press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tak S, Noh J, Cheong C, Zeidman P, Razi A, Penny WD, Friston KJ. 2018. A validation of dynamic causal modelling for 7T fMRI. J Neurosci Methods. 305:36–45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vogt BA, Pandya DN, Rosene DL. 1987. Cingulate cortex of the rhesus monkey: I. Cytoarchitecture and thalamic afferents. J Comp Neurol. 262:256–270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilke M. 2012. An alternative approach towards assessing and accounting for individual motion in fMRI timeseries. NeuroImage. 59:2062–2072. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolff M, Vann SD. 2019. The cognitive thalamus as a gateway to mental representations. J Neurosci. 39:3–14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yeshurun Y, Nguyen M, Hasson U. 2021. The default mode network: where the idiosyncratic self meets the shared social world. Nat Rev Neurosci. 22(3):181–192. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Young WS, Alheid GF, Heimer L. 1984. The ventral pallidal projection to the mediodorsal thalamus: a study with fluorescent retrograde tracers and immunohistofluorescence. J Neurosci. 4:1626–1638. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yuan R, Biswal BB, Zaborszky L. 2019. Functional subdivisions of magnocellular cell groups in human basal forebrain: test-retest resting-state study at ultra-high field, and meta-analysis. Cereb Cortex. 29:2844–2858. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zeidman P, Jafarian A, Corbin N, Seghier ML, Razi A, Price CJ, Friston KJ. 2019a. A guide to group effective connectivity analysis, part 1: first level analysis with DCM for fMRI. NeuroImage. 200:174–190. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zeidman P, Jafarian A, Seghier ML, Litvak V, Cagnan H, Price CJ, Friston KJ. 2019b. A guide to group effective connectivity analysis, part 2: second level analysis with PEB. NeuroImage. 200:12–25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.