Skip to main content
Alzheimer's Research & Therapy logoLink to Alzheimer's Research & Therapy
. 2022 Oct 4;14:144. doi: 10.1186/s13195-022-01081-w

Axonal degeneration and amyloid pathology predict cognitive decline beyond cortical atrophy

Anna Linnéa Svenningsson 1,2,, Erik Stomrud 1,2, Sebastian Palmqvist 1,2, Oskar Hansson 1,2,#, Rik Ossenkoppele 1,3,#
PMCID: PMC9531524  PMID: 36192766

Abstract

Background

Cortical atrophy is associated with cognitive decline, but the association is not perfect. We aimed to identify factors explaining the discrepancy between the degree of cortical atrophy and cognitive decline in cognitively unimpaired elderly.

Methods

The discrepancy between atrophy and cognitive decline was measured using the residuals from a linear regression analysis between change in whole brain cortical thickness over time and change in a cognitive composite measure over time in 395 cognitively unimpaired participants from the Swedish BioFINDER study. We tested for bivariate associations of this residual measure with demographic, imaging, and fluid biomarker variables using Pearson correlations and independent-samples t-tests, and for multivariate associations using linear regression models. Mediation analyses were performed to explore possible paths between the included variables.

Results

In bivariate analyses, older age (r = −0.11, p = 0.029), male sex (t = −3.00, p = 0.003), larger intracranial volume (r = −0.17, p < 0.001), carrying an APOEe4 allele (t = −2.71, p = 0.007), larger white matter lesion volume (r = −0.16, p = 0.002), lower cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) β-amyloid (Aβ) 42/40 ratio (t = −4.05, p < 0.001), and higher CSF levels of phosphorylated tau (p-tau) 181 (r = −0.22, p < 0.001), glial fibrillary acidic protein (GFAP; r = −0.15, p = 0.003), and neurofilament light (NfL; r = −0.34, p < 0.001) were negatively associated with the residual measure, i.e., associated with worse than expected cognitive trajectory given the level of atrophy. In a multivariate analysis, only lower CSF Aβ42/40 ratio and higher CSF NfL levels explained cognition beyond brain atrophy. Mediation analyses showed that associations between the residual measure and APOEe4 allele, CSF Aβ42/40 ratio, and CSF GFAP and p-tau181 levels were mediated by levels of CSF NfL, as were the associations with the residual measure for age, sex, and WML volume.

Conclusions

Our results suggest that axonal degeneration and amyloid pathology independently affect the rate of cognitive decline beyond the degree of cortical atrophy. Furthermore, axonal degeneration mediated the negative effects of old age, male sex, and white matter lesions, and in part also amyloid and tau pathology, on cognition over time when accounting for cortical atrophy.

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1186/s13195-022-01081-w.

Keywords: Resilience, Cognition, Amyloid, Neurodegeneration, Aging

Background

Grey matter atrophy is associated with cognitive decline in chronological ageing [1], mild cognitive impairment (MCI) [2], and Alzheimer’s disease (AD) dementia [3, 4]. However, the association between brain structure and cognitive performance is not perfect, and there are great interindividual differences [5]. This discrepancy between pathological markers and clinical outcome has been described within the cognitive resilience and reserve framework but can also be discussed from the other perspective, by looking at risk factors for cognitive decline beyond what is explained by a specific pathological marker.

Cognitive resilience is an umbrella term that refers to multiple processes involved in avoiding (brain maintenance) and withstanding or coping with (brain and cognitive reserve) the effects of brain pathology and ageing [6]. Intracranial volume (ICV) is a proxy measure that has been used to account for brain reserve [6, 7], while education, intelligence quotient (IQ), and occupational complexity are often used as proxies for cognitive reserve [811]. Another method for studying reserve is the residual approach [6, 12, 13], where reserve is defined as the variance in cognition that cannot be explained by a defined pathological marker. This results in a direct, quantitative, and participant-specific measure of reserve.

As mentioned above, the opposite of resilience factors are factors that worsen cognitive decline beyond a specified pathological process. Both β-amyloid (Aβ) [14, 15] and tau [3, 16] pathology have been shown to be associated with cognitive decline, independent of grey matter atrophy. Cerebrovascular small vessel pathology is also associated with cognitive decline, although not consistently when accounting for brain volume loss [17, 18]. Cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) levels of neurofilament light (NfL), a marker of axonal integrity [19], correlate with cognitive performance [20, 21], but the interplay between NfL levels, grey matter atrophy, and cognitive performance is unclear. Studies have also shown associations between cognitive performance and CSF markers of astrocytic activation [22], microglial activation [23], and synaptic dysfunction [24], but few studies have investigated whether this is independent of cortical atrophy. Also, cardio- and cerebrovascular co-morbidities, such as diabetes mellitus [25], congestive heart failure [26], and stroke [27, 28], increase the risk of cognitive decline and dementia.

Prevention and development of non-pharmaceutical interventions to promote healthy brain ageing are of great importance, as delaying the onset of dementia or slowing cognitive decline improves quality of life and reduces public health costs [29, 30]. Through studies of resilience and risk factors, novel ways to prevent or postpone cognitive decline in the presence of pathology and atrophy can be discovered. Factors that impact future cognitive decline beyond brain atrophy are to a great extent unclear, as there have been few longitudinal studies investigating this. In this study, we calculated the discrepancy between whole brain cortical atrophy and change in global cognition at an individual level and then investigated whether resilience and risk factors, such as educational attainment, cardiovascular co-morbidities, and imaging and fluid biomarkers of amyloid, tau, and cerebrovascular pathology, astrocytic and microglial activation, and axonal and synaptic integrity, may explain this discrepancy. The residual approach renders a quantitative measure of reserve, allowing us to perform subsequent analyses to investigate possible paths between the included variables using mediation analysis.

Methods

Participants

Longitudinal data from cognitively unimpaired (CU) elderly participants from the Swedish BioFINDER study (https://biofinder.se; NCT01208675) were obtained. Participants performing within the normal range on cognitive testing and who did not fulfil criteria for MCI or dementia were included. This included both cognitively unimpaired participants, recruited as volunteers without subjective cognitive complaints, and people referred to the memory clinic at Skåne University Hospital or Ängelholm hospital in Sweden due to cognitive symptoms experienced by the patient and/or informant but performing within the normal range on cognitive testing (hereafter referred to as subjective cognitive decline (SCD)). The participants underwent cognitive testing, magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), and lumbar puncture every 2 years. To be included in the current study, participants had to have completed at least two MRI scans and two visits with cognitive testing, from which change in cortical thickness and change in cognitive performance were calculated. Information on demographics (age, sex, years of education), APOEe4 status, cardiovascular co-morbidities, CSF biomarker data, and white matter lesion (WML) volume was collected at baseline. Information on cardiovascular co-morbidities was obtained from patient history and included presence of hypertension, hyperlipidaemia, diabetes mellitus, ischemic heart disease, atrial fibrillation, congestive heart failure, and stroke/transient ischemic attack (TIA). The study was approved by the ethics committee at Lund University and the participants gave their written informed consent.

Cognition

Since the population included in the study was cognitively unimpaired, we used a modification of the Preclinical Alzheimer’s Cognitive Composite 5 (mPACC5), which has been shown to be sensitive for detecting early cognitive changes [31], to measure cognitive performance over time. The mPACC5 was calculated using the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) [32], Alzheimer’s Disease Assessment Scale – Cognitive Subscale (ADAS-Cog) delayed word recall [33], Trailmaking Test B (TMTB) [34], and Animal fluency test [35]. Z scores were calculated for each cognitive test, using the means and standard deviations from the same tests in a separate cohort of cognitively unimpaired participants from the BioFINDER 2 study (NCT03174938; n = 128). When appropriate, the results of the tests were multiplied by −1 so that higher z score always represented better cognition. The means from the z scores formed the mPACC5. As in previous publications [36], ADAS-Cog delayed word recall was weighted double since the original PACC5 includes two memory tests.

Imaging biomarkers

The participants underwent 3-T MRI scans. Cortical reconstruction and volumetric segmentation were performed with the FreeSurfer image analysis suite, which is documented and freely available for download online (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/). To extract reliable volume and thickness estimates, images were automatically processed with a longitudinal pipeline [37] in FreeSurfer. We performed manual quality assessment on the images. We computed a mean whole brain cortical thickness measure by averaging all cortical regions of interest (n = 34) from the Desikan-Killiany atlas [38]. Regions from both hemispheres were included and adjusted for their respective surface area. WML volume, seen as hyperintensities in T2-weighted scans, was measured using the Lesion Segmentation Tool [39, 40].

Fluid biomarkers

Lumbar CSF samples were collected and stored in −80°C pending analyses. Levels of Aβ42, Aβ40, and phosphorylated tau 181 (p-tau181) were measured using Elecsys immunoassays [41]. The Aβ42/40 ratio was used as a measure of brain amyloid deposition [42]. Levels of NfL, glial fibrillary acidic protein (GFAP; a marker of astrocytic activation [43]), soluble triggering receptor expressed on myeloid cells 2 (sTREM2; a marker of microglial activation [44]), and neurogranin (a marker of synaptic dysfunction [45]) were measured using the NeuroToolKit panel of automated immunoassays [46].

Statistics

We used a two-step approach, where we first calculated individual slopes for change in cortical thickness and change in cognitive performance by performing linear regressions between time (independent variable) and mean whole brain cortical thickness or mPACC5 (dependent variables), including all available data points for cortical thickness and mPACC5. Second, we performed a linear regression with change in cortical thickness as independent variable and change in mPACC5 as dependent variable, from which the standardized residuals were used as a measure of the discrepancy between brain atrophy and cognitive change over time at an individual level. A positive residual reflects a more favourable cognitive trajectory than expected given the level of atrophy, and a negative residual reflects a worse cognitive trajectory than expected. The residuals were used as the dependent variable in further analyses with baseline demographics as well as CSF and MRI biomarkers as predictors.

In bivariate analyses, Pearson correlations (rp) and independent-samples t-tests were performed for continuous and dichotomous variables, respectively. To look for differing results for participants with only two follow-up visits compared to those with three or more, the bivariate analyses were also performed separately for these groups. To assess independent associations between the residual measure and these variables, the variables significantly associated with the residual measure in the bivariate analyses were entered in a multivariate linear regression model adjusted for presence/absence of SCD, baseline mean whole brain cortical thickness, and baseline mPACC5. The multivariate analysis was also performed with the individual specific intercepts from the initial linear regression models for mean whole brain cortical thickness and mPACC5 as baseline measures of these variables, to account for the fact that the slopes are estimated together with an estimated baseline (intercept). We additionally tested for interaction effects between demographic variables and biomarkers on the residual measure.

To elucidate why a variable was significantly associated with the residual measure in the bivariate but not in the multivariate analysis, mediation analyses were performed using the PROCESS macro [47] in the Statistical Package for Social Sciences (SPSS), with models including either a single or multiple mediator variables. In short, this macro performs sequential linear regression analyses to establish mediation effects and then uses bootstrapping (5000 bootstrap samples) to calculate a 95% confidence interval (CI) for the direct and indirect effects of the mediation models. Effects where the bootstrapped 95% CI did not include 0 were considered significant. The designs of the mediation models were outlined in two steps. First, we checked for bivariate associations between all variables in the multivariate analysis. Second, for each variable that was significantly associated with the residual measure in the bivariate but not the multivariate analysis, a directed acyclic graph (DAG, a graphical way to display the assumptions about the relationship between variables) was drawn, incorporating the variables that were associated in the bivariate analysis. The directions of the associations were inferred from previous literature. The hypothesized mediation models (visualized as DAGs) were then tested statistically using the PROCESS macro in SPSS.

Since the Aβ42/40 ratio shows a bimodal distribution, this was used as a dichotomous variable based on a previously established cut-off level [48]. This cut-off was defined using mixture modelling, a method that has been successfully used to identify cut-offs for biomarkers of amyloid pathology [49, 50]. The distributions for WML volume as well as CSF levels of p-tau181, NfL, neurogranin, sTREM2, and GFAP were positively skewed, and the values for these variables were therefore log10-transformed to meet model assumptions of normal distribution. We corrected the bivariate models for multiple comparisons using false discovery rate (FDR). Statistical significance was set at α < 0.05. Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS Statistics for Mac (version 27).

Results

Sample characteristics

The characteristics of the 395 study participants are described in Table 1. The mean age was 72.4 (standard deviation (SD) 5.3) years at baseline, 59% were women, and the mean education level was 12.4 (SD 3.5) years. The median number of MRI scans was three scans and median number of cognitive test visits was three visits. Spaghetti plots showing individual slopes for whole brain cortical thickness and mPACC5 are shown in Supplementary Fig. 1.

Table 1.

Sample characteristics for the whole sample and divided by negative or positive residual measure (better or worse cognitive trajectory than expected given the level of atrophy). Mean (standard deviation (SD)) are presented if not otherwise specified

Whole sample (n = 395) Negative residual measure (n = 160) Positive residual measure (n = 235)
Age at baseline (years) 72.4 (5.3) 73.2 (5.6) 71.8 (5.1)
Sex (% women) 59% 49% 66%
Years of education 12.4 (3.5) 12.3 (3.6) 12.5 (3.5)
Number of MRI scans (median (range)) 3 (2–5) 3 (2–4) 3 (2–5)
Mean whole brain cortical thickness at baseline (mm) 2.31 (0.11) 2.29 (0.11) 2.33 (0.10)
Change in mean whole brain cortical thickness (mm/year) −0.012 (0.013) −0.011 (0.015) −0.012 (0.011)
Number of cognitive test visits (median (range)) 3 (2–4) 3 (2–4) 3 (2–4)
MMSE score at baseline 28.9 (1.2) 28.8 (1.3) 28.9 (1.1)
ADAS-Cog delayed recall at baseline (incorrect answers) 2.4 (2.0) 2.8 (2.3) 2.1 (1.8)
TMTB at baseline (seconds) 99.1 (41.1) 107 (45.0) 93.9 (37.4)
Animal fluency test at baseline (correct answers) 21.2 (5.9) 20.4 (6.7) 21.8 (5.2)
mPACC5 at baseline (z score) 0.11 (0.71) −0.06 (0.84) 0.22 (0.59)
Change in mPACC5 (z score/year) −0.087 (0.20) −0.25 (0.21) 0.02 (0.09)
ICV (dm3) 1.10 (0.13) 1.11 (0.13) 1.08 (0.12)
WML volume (cm3; median (range); 11 missing) 5.1 (0.01–89.5) 7.0 (0.06–89.5) 4.1 (0.01–71.5)
APOEe4 allele (% carriers; 5 missing) 33% 37% 30%
CSF Aβ42/40 (% abnormal; 1 missing) 28% 38% 22%
CSF P-tau181 (median (range); pg/ml) 18.1 (8.0–72.5) 19.5 (8.0–56.4) 17.5 (8.0–72.5)
CSF NfL (median (range); pg/ml; 9 missing) 125 (41.5–861) 142 (60.9–861) 119 (41.5–463)
CSF sTREM2 (median (range); ng/ml; 9 missing) 9.7 (3.9–21.3) 9.9 (4.6–21.0) 9.5 (3.9–21.3)
CSF neurogranin (median (range); pg/ml; 9 missing) 711 (217–2179) 728 (225–1932) 708 (217–2179)
CSF GFAP (median (range); ng/ml; 9 missing) 11.9 (3.8–44.9) 12.6 (5.1–44.9) 11.5 (3.8–35.9)
Smoking (% current or former; 56 missing) 48% 52% 45%
Hypertension (%; 1 missing) 37% 33% 39%
Hyperlipidaemia (%; 1 missing) 33% 33% 33%
Diabetes mellitus (%; 1 missing) 8.4% 8.8% 8.1%
Ischemic heart disease (&; 1 missing) 7.9% 9.4% 6.8%
Atrial fibrillation (%; 1 missing) 1.8% 1.9% 1.7%
Congestive heart failure (%; 1 missing) 1.3% 1.3% 1.3%
Stroke/TIA (%; 1 missing) 4.8% 6.3% 3.8%

Abbreviations: MMSE Mini Mental State Examination, ADAS-Cog Alzheimer’s Disease Assessment Scale – Cognitive subscale, TMTB Trailmaking Test B, mPACC5 modified Preclinical Alzheimer’s Cognitive Composite 5, ICV intracranial volume, WML white matter lesion, CSF cerebrospinal fluid, β-amyloid, P-tau phosphorylated tau, NfL neurofilament light, sTREM2 soluble triggering receptor expressed on myeloid cells 2, GFAP glial fibrillary acidic protein, TIA transient ischemic attack

Residual measure

Across the whole group, the mean change in mean whole brain cortical thickness was −0.012 (SD 0.013) mm/year and mean change in mPACC5 was −0.087 (SD 0.20) z scores/year. Changes in these measures were modestly correlated (rp = 0.3, p < 0.001; Supplementary Fig. 2).

Bivariate analyses

In bivariate analyses, older age (r = −0.11, p = 0.029), male sex (t = −3.00, p = 0.003), larger ICV (r = −0.17, p < 0.001), larger WML volume (r = −0.16, p = 0.002), carrying an APOEe4 allele (t = −2.71, p = 0.007), having a lower Aβ42/40 ratio (t = −4.05, p < 0.001), and higher levels of p-tau181 (r = −0.22, p < 0.001), GFAP (r = −0.15, p = 0.003), and NfL (r = −0.34, p < 0.001) were significantly associated with the residual measure (i.e. worse than expected cognitive trajectory given the level of atrophy). Only age did not survive correction for multiple comparisons. Years of education (r = 0.04, p = 0.431), smoking (t = −0.05, p = 0.958), cardiovascular co-morbidities (t = −1.4 to 1.08, p = 0.178–0.903), and CSF levels of sTREM2 (r = −0.07, p = 0.183) and neurogranin (r = −0.10, p = 0.059) were not associated with the residual measure (Table 2). For most variables, the general directions of the associations were the same for participants with two MRI scans compared to those with three or more, as well as for participants with two cognitive test visits compared to those with three or more. The direction of association differed between the groups for smoking, diabetes mellitus, ischaemic heart disease, and stroke/TIA, but none of these results were statistically significant (Supplementary Tables 1A and B).

Table 2.

Bivariate analyses. Associations between the residual measure and demographic, co-morbidity, and biomarker variables. Pearson correlation coefficients with bootstrapped 95% CIs for continuous variables and independent samples t values for binary variables with p values before and after FDR correction are presented

Pearson correlation coefficient (95% CI) T value P value P value after FDR correction
Age at baseline −0.11 (−0.21 to −0.02) - 0.029 0.064
Sex - −3.00 0.003 0.009
Years of education 0.04 (0.06 to 0.14) - 0.431 0.571
ICV −0.17 (−0.27 to −0.07) - < 0.001 0.005
WML volume −0.16 (−0.25 to −0.07) - 0.002 0.008
APOEe4 allele - −2.71 0.007 0.018
Low CSF Aβ42/40 ratio - −4.05 < 0.001 < 0.001
CSF P-tau181 −0.22 (−0.32 to −0.12) - < 0.001 < 0.001
CSF NfL −0.34 (−0.44 to −0.23) - < 0.001 < 0.001
CSF sTREM2 0.07 (0.16 to 0.02) - 0.183 0.305
CSF neurogranin 0.10 (0.19 to 0.00) - 0.059 0.118
CSF GFAP −0.15 (-0.26 to −0.05) - 0.003 0.009
Smoking (current or former) - 0.05 0.958 0.958
Hypertension - 1.08 0.281 0.432
Hyperlipidaemia - 0.88 0.382 0.546
Diabetes mellitus - 0.35 0.726 0.807
Ischemic heart disease - 0.12 0.903 0.951
Atrial fibrillation - 0.74 0.457 0.571
Congestive heart failure - 0.56 0.575 0.676
Stroke/TIA - 1.40 0.178 0.305

Abbreviations: CI confidence interval, FDR false discovery rate, ICV intracranial volume, WML white matter lesion, β-amyloid, P-tau phosphorylated tau, NfL neurofilament light, sTREM2 soluble triggering receptor expressed on myeloid cells 2, GFAP glial fibrillary acidic protein, TIA transient ischemic attack

Multivariate analysis

A multivariate linear regression analysis was performed, including the residual measure as dependent variable and the variables significantly associated with the residual measure in bivariate analyses (see above) as independent variables, controlling for presence/absence of SCD, baseline mean whole brain cortical thickness, and baseline mPACC5. In this analysis, only higher NfL levels (β = −0.20 (95% CI −0.34 to (−0.05)), p = 0.009) and having a lower Aβ42/40 ratio (β = −0.11 (95% CI −0.23 to (−0.004)), p = 0.049) were associated with the residual measure (Table 3, Fig. 1A, B). The results were similar when using the individual specific intercepts for whole brain cortical thickness and mPACC5 from the initial linear regression models as baseline measures of these variables (Supplementary Table 2).

Table 3.

Multivariate analysis. Multivariate linear regression model with the residual measure as dependent variable and the variables statistically significant in bivariate analyses (Table 2) as independent variables, controlling for presence/absence of subjective cognitive decline, baseline mean whole brain cortical thickness, and baseline mPACC5. Standardized beta coefficients with bootstrapped 95% CIs are presented

Standardized beta coefficient (95% CI) P value
Age at baseline 0.02 (−0.10 to 0.16) 0.768
Male sex 0.01 (−0.13 to 0.11) 0.813
ICV −0.10 (−0.21 to 0.03) 0.108
WML volume 0.003 (−0.10 to 0.11) 0.960
APOEe4 allele −0.04 (−0.15 to 0.06) 0.405
Abnormal CSF Aβ42/40 0.11 (0.23 to 0.004) 0.049
CSF P-tau181 −0.03 (−0.19 to 0.12) 0.687
CSF NfL 0.20 (0.34 to 0.05) 0.009
CSF GFAP −0.01 (−0.11 to 0.12) 0.920

Abbreviations: CI confidence interval, ICV intracranial volume, WML white matter lesion, β-amyloid, P-tau phosphorylated tau, NfL neurofilament light, GFAP glial fibrillary acidic protein, mPACC5 modified Preclinical Alzheimer’s Cognitive Composite 5

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Scatterplot showing the negative association between baseline CSF levels of NfL (log10-transformed) and the residual measure (A), and boxplot showing that participants with normal Aβ42/40 ratio have higher residual measure than participants with abnormal Aβ42/40 ratio at baseline (B). Abbreviations: CSF, cerebrospinal fluid; NfL, neurofilament light; Aβ, β-amyloid

There were no significant two-way interactions for ICV, WML volume, APOEe4, Aβ42/40 ratio, or levels of p-tau181, GFAP, or NfL with age, sex, or education on the residual measure (Supplementary Table 3).

Mediation analyses

As a first step, we constructed the directed acyclic graphs (DAGs) for variables that were significantly associated with the residual measure in bivariate but not multivariate analysis to visualize hypothesized paths of mediation (Supplementary Fig. 3). These were based on associations in this sample between the variables significant in bivariate analyses (Supplementary Table 4) and the hypothesized directions of the associations were based on either the inherent nature of the variables (i.e. if there is a temporal association between age or sex with any of the biomarkers, age or sex would come first in the ordering) or hypotheses based on existing literature (i.e. the amyloid cascade hypothesis [51], GFAP being closely linked to amyloid pathology [52], and WMLs preceding brain atrophy [53]). ICV could not, based on previous literature, be fitted in a hypothesized causal model predicting NfL and was therefore included as a covariate instead.

 As a second step, we statistically tested the hypothesized mediation models using the PROCESS macro in SPSS. This showed that the associations between the residual measure and APOEe4 allele, lower Aβ42/40 ratio (partially), GFAP levels, and p-tau181 levels were mediated by levels of NfL, as were the associations with the residual measure for age, sex, and WML volume. More detailed results from these analyses are shown in Table 4.

Table 4.

Mediation analyses testing hypothesized paths (Supplementary Fig. 3). Variables significantly associated with the residual measure in bivariate analyses (Table 2) that were not included as mediators were included as covariates as were the presence/absence of subjective cognitive decline, baseline mean whole brain cortical thickness, and baseline mPACC5. Significant mediation paths (defined as bootstrapped 95% CI not including 0) are highlighted in bold

Included potential mediators Path Beta (95% CI) Standardized beta (95% CI)a
Age p-tau181, WML volume, NfL
 Direct effect 0.003 (-0.018 to 0.026)
 Total indirect effects -0.010 (-0.022 to -0.0002) -0.058 (-0.127 to -0.001)
 Indirect effects Age – P-tau181 – NfL – residual -0.0006 (-0.002 to 0.0004) -0.003 (-0.014 to 0.002)
Age – NfL – residual -0.007 (-0.016 to -0.001) -0.043 (-0.094 to -0.009)
Age – WML volume – NfL – residual -0.002 (-0.004 to -0.0003) -0.012 (-0.025 to -0.002)
Male sex WML volume, NfL
 Direct effect 0.029 (-0.209 to 0.267)
 Total indirect effect -0.045 (-0.112 to 0.006) -0.048 (-0.120 to 0.006)
 Indirect effects Sex – NfL – residual -0.050 (-0.116 to -0.006) -0.054 (-0.125 to -0.007)
Sex – WML volume – NfL – residual 0.006 (-0.001 to 0.017) 0.006 (-0.002 to 0.018)
WML volume NfL
 Direct effect 0.004 (-0.172 to 0.181)
 Total indirect effect -0.049 (-0.101 to -0.009) -0.034 (-0.070 to -0.006)
 Indirect effects WML volume – NfL – residual -0.049 (-0.101 to -0.009) -0.034 (-0.070 to -0.006)
APOEe4 allele Aβ42/40b, GFAP, P-tau181, NfL
 Direct effect -0.067 (-0.285 to 0.152)
 Total indirect effect -0.173 (-0.315 to -0.051) -0.184 (-0.324 to -0.057)
 Indirect effects APOE4 – Aβ42/40 – GFAP – P-tau181 – NfL – residual -0.0038 (-0.0087 to -0.0005) -0.0041 (-0.010 to -0.0006)
APOE4 – Aβ42/40 – GFAP – NfL – residual -0.0069 (-0.0164 to -0.001) -0.0074-(-0.0174 to -0.0011)
APOE4 – Aβ42/40 – P-tau181 – NfL – residual -0.0237 (-0.050 to -0.006) -0.025 (-0.054 to -0.006)
APOE4 – Aβ42/40 – NfL – residual 0.004 (-0.016 to 0.034) 0.004 (-0.018 to 0.037)
APOE4 – Aβ42/40 – residual -0.129 (-0.315 to -0.051) -0.137 (-0.268 to -0.021)

Abbreviations: WML white matter lesion, β-amyloid, P-tau phosphorylated tau, GFAP glial fibrillary acidic protein, NfL neurofilament light, mPACC5 modified Preclinical Alzheimer’s Cognitive Composite 5, CI confidence interval

aFor dichotomous independent variables (sex, APOEe4 allele, and pathological Aβ42/40), these are partially standardized beta coefficients (original metric for x but standardized y)

bSince the macro does not allow dichotomous mediators, continuous Aβ42/40 ratio was used for this mediation analysis

Discussion

Using a residual approach to quantify the discrepancy between whole brain cortical atrophy (change in cortical thickness) and change in cognitive performance in a sample of cognitively unimpaired elderly, we show that this discrepancy is partly explained by levels of NfL and lower Aβ42/40 ratio in CSF. Also, levels of NfL mediated associations between the residual measure and age, sex, WML volume, APOEe4 allele, Aβ42/40 ratio, and levels of GFAP and p-tau181. Altogether, our results suggest that axonal degeneration and amyloid pathology affect the rate of cognitive decline beyond the degree of cortical atrophy.

NfL levels in CSF correlate with cognitive performance [20, 21] and NfL is often considered a possible marker of neurodegenerative processes (akin to brain atrophy on MRI) [5456]. Here we show that NfL and cortical atrophy provide complementary information when predicting cognitive change, suggesting that they reflect overlapping yet distinct processes in the brain. Levels of NfL also mediated the associations between the residual measure and several other predictors (age, sex, WML volume, and AD and astrocytic biomarkers), which could be expected given that NfL is a sensitive, but non-specific marker of neurological disorders [57], is associated with older age and male sex in CU [58, 59], and is closely associated with global cognitive performance and future cognitive decline [20, 60]. In Fig. 2, we present an aggregated, hypothetical model including the significant mediation effects of NfL from Table 4. It is, however, important to emphasize that the possible associations in this model have not been tested statistically as a combined model, and the model is to be seen as a hypothetical model to be tested in further studies.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Aggregated, hypothetical model of the associations between different predictors and the residual measure based on the results from the mediation analyses (Table 4). Plus indicates a positive association and minus a negative association between the variables. Abbreviations: WML, white matter lesion; Aβ, β-amyloid; GFAP, glial fibrillary acidic protein; P-tau, phosphorylated tau; NfL, neurofilament light

The presence of AD pathology was another predictor of worse than expected cognitive decline given the degree of brain atrophy, as presence of an APOEe4 allele, as well as amyloid and tau pathology were associated with the residual measure in bivariate analyses, and amyloid pathology also in multivariate analysis. This atrophy independent association between amyloid pathology and the residual measure indicate amyloid-related effects on cognition that are independent of gross cortical atrophy or axonal degeneration. Instead, amyloid pathology could induce network disruption (i.e. functional rather than structural) alterations [61]. Mediation analyses showed that the association between the residual measure and CSF p-tau181 was mediated by CSF levels of NfL, and both p-tau181 and NfL levels partially mediated the association between amyloid pathology and the residual measure. This is in line with existing evidence of especially tau pathology being associated with neurodegeneration [15, 62, 63], which also could also explain why CSF p-tau181 did not remain significant in the multivariate model, i.e. when taking atrophy into account, as tau pathology did not explain additional cognitive decline beyond atrophy.

Older age was associated with a worse than expected cognitive trajectory given the level of cortical atrophy in univariate, but not multivariate, analyses. Considering chronological age as a proxy for many different processes that occur in the ageing brain, this observation is in line with our a priori hypotheses. With higher age, changes within and between brain networks have been observed [64, 65], which could explain the association between age and cognitive performance beyond atrophy. The association was mediated by levels of NfL, in part via cerebrovascular pathology measured by WML volume. NfL has been shown to correlate with white matter changes in elderly individuals with and without dementia [66, 67]. In contrast, a recent study showed no association between WML volume and levels of NfL, but this study included individuals at all ages [68].

In bivariate analyses, females showed more favourable cognitive trajectories than expected on a group level. Previous studies using different definitions and measures of reserve and resilience have indicated clear sex differences in cognitive resilience. For example, women showed relative preservation of cortical thickness when exposed to tau pathology compared to men [69] indicating greater brain resilience, and in a cohort of cognitively unimpaired elderly, women had higher entorhinal cortical tau than men [70], indicating greater cognitive resilience. These results indicate that women can tolerate more aggregated tau in their brains before exhibiting neurodegeneration and cognitive decline. The negative association between male sex and cognition was mediated by levels of NfL. ICV, often used as a marker of brain reserve [6, 7], was negatively associated with the residual measure in bivariate analysis. To some degree, this was due to confounding by sex with men having lower residual measure and larger ICV than women.

CSF levels of GFAP were associated with the residual measure in bivariate but not multivariate analysis. It also partially mediated the associations between amyloid pathology and subsequent p-tau181 levels, NfL levels, and the residual measure. GFAP in CSF is associated with cognitive performance [22] and is increased in different neurodegenerative diseases compared to controls [71] and in CU individuals with AD pathology compared to those without [59]. No association was seen between the residual measure and CSF levels of sTREM2 or neurogranin. For sTREM2, the association with neurodegeneration and cognition is not consistent, with studies showing both increased levels in AD patients compared to controls [72] as well as an association between higher levels and attenuated cognitive decline in individuals with AD [23]. Neurogranin is associated with cognitive decline [24], but the lack of association with cognitive trajectory in our study could be due to its high expression in associative cortical regions [73], since the association between cognition and cortical atrophy/function has already been accounted for in our analysis.

There was no significant association between the residual measure and education, which is often used as a proxy for cognitive reserve [9]. However, a recent review concluded that education level does not affect the rate of cognitive decline; instead, it showed that people with higher education start at a higher cognitive level, and therefore, they have more to lose before reaching the level of dementia [74]. The results from the present study are in line with that conclusion, with education not being associated with better or worse cognitive trajectory over time given the rate of atrophy.

No association was found for any cardiovascular comorbidity or smoking with the residual measure. Previous studies have shown associations for diabetes mellitus [25], congestive heart failure [26], and stroke [27, 28], with risk of cognitive decline and dementia, but there are few studies investigating the association when accounting for brain atrophy. The lack of association for diabetes (8.4%) and stroke/TIA (4.8%) could be due to lack of power in this sample of 395 participants, but hypertension (37%), hyperlipidaemia (33%), and smoking (48%) were frequently observed in our sample.

Limitations

Strengths of this study include the longitudinal design and the well-characterized and relatively large sample of participants. However, there are limitations. First, measuring changes in cortical thickness and cognitive performance in parallel, when atrophy is hypothesized to precede cognitive decline, may affect the estimated association between the two measures. Second, even if the longitudinal design is of great value to assess changes over time, there is a possibility of attrition bias, with the inherent risk of selective drop-out of individuals showing the most rapid cognitive deterioration during follow-up. Third, for the mediation analysis, the main part of the variables included are measured at the same time point, even though the dependent variable is based on longitudinal data. When using cross-sectional data in mediation, the results must be interpreted with caution. This issue is addressed by building DAGs based on previous knowledge and hypotheses about causal associations between the variables, but they are built on assumptions on the directions of associations and do not guarantee that these directions are true. Fourth, the residual approach for studying cognitive resilience can be discussed. It is a useful method since it directly reflects the concept of interest, i.e. the variance in cognition not explained by a specified pathology, and renders a quantitative measure of resilience. However, one needs to bear in mind that apart from resilience, it also reflects random error, and the results are highly dependent on the variables chosen for the regression analysis and the sample participants. Therefore, results must be interpreted with caution.

Conclusions

In conclusion we show, that in cognitively unimpaired elderly individuals, axonal degeneration and amyloid pathology predict cognitive decline beyond what can be explained by cortical atrophy. This is in line with previous findings indicating that amyloid pathology has a small but significant association with cognition independent of atrophy. It confirms that neurodegeneration is closely linked to cognition, but also suggests that different measures used to assess neurodegeneration, e.g. grey matter atrophy and axonal degeneration, provide complementary information when predicting cognitive performance. Additionally, these results suggest that axonal degeneration mediates the negative effects of old age, male sex, WML volume, astrocytic activation, tau, and in part also amyloid pathology, on cognition over time when accounting for cortical atrophy.

Supplementary Information

13195_2022_1081_MOESM1_ESM.docx (13.5MB, docx)

Additional file 1: Supplementary Tables 1A and B. Bivariate analyses divided into participants with two or more than two MRI scans or cognitive visits. Supplementary Table 2. Multivariate analysis. Supplementary Table 3. Interaction effects. Supplementary Table 4. Associations between all variables significantly associated with the residual measure in bivariate analyses. Supplementary Figure 1. Spaghetti plots showing individual trajectories over time for mean whole brain cortical thickness and mPACC5. Supplementary Figure 2. Scatterplot showing the positive association between change in mean whole brain cortical thickness and change in global cognition. Supplementary Figure 3. Directed acyclic graphs (DAGs) constructed for inclusion of variables in mediation analyses.

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Abbreviations

AD

Alzheimer’s disease

ADAS-Cog

Alzheimer’s Disease Assessment Scale – Cognitive Subscale

APOEe4

Apolipoprotein E4

β-Amyloid

CI

Confidence interval

CSF

Cerebrospinal fluid

CU

Cognitively unimpaired

DAG

Directed acyclic graph

FDR

False discovery rate

GFAP

Glial fibrillary acidic protein

ICV

Intracranial volume

IQ

Intelligence quotation

MCI

Mild cognitive impairment

MMSE

Mini-Mental State Examination

MRI

Magnetic resonance imaging

NfL

Neurofilament light

PACC

Preclinical Alzheimer’s Cognitive Composite

P-tau181

Tau phosphorylated at threonine 181

SCD

Subjective cognitive decline

SD

Standard deviation

SPSS

Statistical Package for Social Sciences

sTREM2

Soluble triggering receptor expressed on myeloid cells 2

TIA

Transient ischemic attack

TMTB

Trailmaking Test B

WML

White matter lesion

Authors’ contributions

RO, OH, and ALS designed the study. ALS, ES, SP, and OH contributed to the acquisition of data. ALS analysed the data and drafted the manuscript. ALS, ES, SP, OH, and RO interpreted the data and revised the manuscript for intellectual content. The authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

Open access funding provided by Lund University. Work at the authors’ research centre was supported by the Swedish Research Council (2016-00906), the Knut and Alice Wallenberg foundation (2017-0383), the Marianne and Marcus Wallenberg foundation (2015.0125), the Strategic Research Area MultiPark (Multidisciplinary Research in Parkinson’s disease) at Lund University, the Swedish Alzheimer Foundation (AF-939932), the Swedish Brain Foundation (FO2021-0293), The Parkinson foundation of Sweden (1280/20), the Konung Gustaf V:s och Drottning Victorias Frimurarestiftelse, the Skåne University Hospital Foundation (2020-O000028), Regionalt Forskningsstöd (2020-0314), and the Swedish federal government under the ALF agreement (2018-Projekt0279).

The funding sources had no role in the design and conduct of the study; in the collection, analysis, and interpretation of the data; or in the preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript.

Availability of data and materials

Anonymized data will be shared by request from a qualified academic investigator for the sole purpose of replicating procedures and results presented in the article and as long as data transfer is in agreement with EU legislation on the general data protection regulation and decisions by the Swedish Ethical Review Authority and Region Skåne, which should be regulated in a material transfer agreement.

Declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study was approved by the ethics committee at Lund University and the participants gave their written informed consent.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

OH has acquired research support (for the institution) from ADx, AVID Radiopharmaceuticals, Biogen, Eli Lilly, Eisai, Fujirebio, GE Healthcare, Pfizer, and Roche. In the past 2 years, he has received consultancy/speaker fees from Amylyx, Alzpath, Biogen, Cerveau, Fujirebio, Genentech, Novartis, Roche, and Siemens. All other authors declare no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Oskar Hansson and Rik Ossenkoppele contributed equally as senior authors.

References

  • 1.Fjell AM, et al. Accelerating cortical thinning: unique to dementia or universal in aging? Cereb Cortex. 2014;24:919–934. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs379. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.McDonald CR, et al. Relationship between regional atrophy rates and cognitive decline in mild cognitive impairment. Neurobiol Aging. 2012;33:242–253. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2010.03.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Bejanin A, et al. Tau pathology and neurodegeneration contribute to cognitive impairment in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain. 2017;140:3286–3300. doi: 10.1093/brain/awx243. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Jack CR, Jr, et al. Tracking pathophysiological processes in Alzheimer’s disease: an updated hypothetical model of dynamic biomarkers. Lancet Neurol. 2013;12:207–216. doi: 10.1016/s1474-4422(12)70291-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Stern Y. Cognitive reserve in ageing and Alzheimer’s disease. Lancet Neurol. 2012;11:1006–1012. doi: 10.1016/s1474-4422(12)70191-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Stern Y, et al. Whitepaper: defining and investigating cognitive reserve, brain reserve, and brain maintenance. Alzheimers Dement. 2020;16:1305–1311. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2018.07.219. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.van Loenhoud AC, Groot C, Vogel JW, van der Flier WM, Ossenkoppele R. Is intracranial volume a suitable proxy for brain reserve? Alzheimers Res Ther. 2018;10:91. doi: 10.1186/s13195-018-0408-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Pettigrew C, et al. Cognitive reserve and rate of change in Alzheimer’s and cerebrovascular disease biomarkers among cognitively normal individuals. Neurobiol Aging. 2020;88:33–41. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2019.12.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Soldan A, et al. Cognitive reserve and long-term change in cognition in aging and preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Aging. 2017;60:164–172. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2017.09.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Boots EA, et al. Occupational complexity and cognitive reserve in a middle-aged cohort at risk for Alzheimer’s disease. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2015;30:634–642. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acv041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Steffener J, et al. The role of education and verbal abilities in altering the effect of age-related gray matter differences on cognition. PLoS One. 2014;9:e91196. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0091196. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Bartrés-Faz D, et al. Theoretical frameworks and approaches used within the Reserve, Resilience and Protective Factors professional interest area of the Alzheimer’s Association International Society to Advance Alzheimer’s Research and Treatment. Alzheimers Dement (Amst) 2020;12:e12115. doi: 10.1002/dad2.12115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Bocancea DI, et al. Measuring resilience and resistance in aging and Alzheimer disease using residual methods: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurology. 2021;97:474–488. doi: 10.1212/wnl.0000000000012499. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Chételat G, et al. Independent contribution of temporal beta-amyloid deposition to memory decline in the pre-dementia phase of Alzheimer’s disease. Brain. 2011;134:798–807. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq383. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Wang L, et al. Spatially distinct atrophy is linked to β-amyloid and tau in preclinical Alzheimer disease. Neurology. 2015;84:1254–1260. doi: 10.1212/wnl.0000000000001401. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Digma LA, et al. Tau and atrophy: domain-specific relationships with cognition. Alzheimers Res Ther. 2019;11:65. doi: 10.1186/s13195-019-0518-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Dong C, et al. Cognitive correlates of white matter lesion load and brain atrophy: the Northern Manhattan Study. Neurology. 2015;85:441–449. doi: 10.1212/wnl.0000000000001716. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Tuladhar AM, et al. Relationship between white matter hyperintensities, cortical thickness, and cognition. Stroke. 2015;46:425–432. doi: 10.1161/strokeaha.114.007146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Khalil M, et al. Neurofilaments as biomarkers in neurological disorders. Nat Rev Neurol. 2018;14:577–589. doi: 10.1038/s41582-018-0058-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Olsson B, et al. Association of cerebrospinal fluid neurofilament light protein levels with cognition in patients with dementia, motor neuron disease, and movement disorders. JAMA Neurol. 2019;76:318–325. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2018.3746. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Dhiman K, et al. Cerebrospinal fluid neurofilament light concentration predicts brain atrophy and cognition in Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement (Amst) 2020;12:e12005. doi: 10.1002/dad2.12005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Teitsdottir UD, et al. Association of glial and neuronal degeneration markers with Alzheimer’s disease cerebrospinal fluid profile and cognitive functions. Alzheimers Res Ther. 2020;12:92. doi: 10.1186/s13195-020-00657-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Ewers M, et al. Increased soluble TREM2 in cerebrospinal fluid is associated with reduced cognitive and clinical decline in Alzheimer’s disease. Sci Transl Med. 2019;11. 10.1126/scitranslmed.aav6221. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 24.Kvartsberg H, et al. Cerebrospinal fluid levels of the synaptic protein neurogranin correlates with cognitive decline in prodromal Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2015;11:1180–1190. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2014.10.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Cukierman T, Gerstein HC, Williamson JD. Cognitive decline and dementia in diabetes--systematic overview of prospective observational studies. Diabetologia. 2005;48:2460–2469. doi: 10.1007/s00125-005-0023-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Cannon JA, et al. Cognitive impairment and heart failure: systematic review and meta-analysis. J Card Fail. 2017;23:464–475. doi: 10.1016/j.cardfail.2017.04.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Pendlebury ST, Rothwell PM. Prevalence, incidence, and factors associated with pre-stroke and post-stroke dementia: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet Neurol. 2009;8:1006–1018. doi: 10.1016/s1474-4422(09)70236-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Mellon L, et al. Cognitive impairment six months after ischaemic stroke: a profile from the ASPIRE-S study. BMC Neurol. 2015;15:31. doi: 10.1186/s12883-015-0288-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Lenox-Smith A, Reed C, Lebrec J, Belger M, Jones RW. Potential cost savings to be made by slowing cognitive decline in mild Alzheimer’s disease dementia using a model derived from the UK GERAS observational study. BMC Geriatr. 2018;18:57. doi: 10.1186/s12877-018-0748-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Mukadam N, Anderson R, Knapp M, Wittenberg R, Karagiannidou M, Costafreda SG, Tutton M, Alessi C, Livingston G. Effective interventions for potentially modifiable risk factors for late-onset dementia: a costs and cost-effectiveness modelling study. Lancet Healthy Longev. 2020;1(1):e13–e20. doi: 10.1016/S2666-7568(20)30004-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Papp KV, Rentz DM, Orlovsky I, Sperling RA, Mormino EC. Optimizing the preclinical Alzheimer’s cognitive composite with semantic processing: the PACC5. Alzheimers Dement (N Y) 2017;3:668–677. doi: 10.1016/j.trci.2017.10.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12:189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Rosen WG, Mohs RC, Davis KL. A new rating scale for Alzheimer’s disease. Am J Psychiatry. 1984;141:1356–1364. doi: 10.1176/ajp.141.11.1356. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Reitan RM. The relation of the trail making test to organic brain damage. J Consult Psychol. 1955;19:393–394. doi: 10.1037/h0044509. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Tombaugh TN, Kozak J, Rees L. Normative data stratified by age and education for two measures of verbal fluency: FAS and animal naming. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 1999;14:167–177. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Insel PS, et al. Determining clinically meaningful decline in preclinical Alzheimer disease. Neurology. 2019;93:e322–e333. doi: 10.1212/wnl.0000000000007831. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Reuter M, Schmansky NJ, Rosas HD, Fischl B. Within-subject template estimation for unbiased longitudinal image analysis. Neuroimage. 2012;61:1402–1418. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.084. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Desikan RS, et al. An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. Neuroimage. 2006;31:968–980. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Schmidt P, et al. An automated tool for detection of FLAIR-hyperintense white-matter lesions in multiple sclerosis. Neuroimage. 2012;59:3774–3783. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.van Westen D, et al. Cerebral white matter lesions - associations with Aβ isoforms and amyloid PET. Sci Rep. 2016;6:20709. doi: 10.1038/srep20709. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Hansson O, et al. CSF biomarkers of Alzheimer’s disease concord with amyloid-β PET and predict clinical progression: a study of fully automated immunoassays in BioFINDER and ADNI cohorts. Alzheimers Dement. 2018;14:1470–1481. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2018.01.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Janelidze S, et al. CSF Aβ42/Aβ40 and Aβ42/Aβ38 ratios: better diagnostic markers of Alzheimer disease. Ann Clin Transl Neurol. 2016;3:154–165. doi: 10.1002/acn3.274. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Eng LF, Ghirnikar RS. GFAP and astrogliosis. Brain Pathol. 1994;4:229–237. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-3639.1994.tb00838.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Yeh FL, Hansen DV, Sheng M. TREM2, microglia, and neurodegenerative diseases. Trends Mol Med. 2017;23:512–533. doi: 10.1016/j.molmed.2017.03.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Hellwig K, et al. Neurogranin and YKL-40: independent markers of synaptic degeneration and neuroinflammation in Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Res Ther. 2015;7:74. doi: 10.1186/s13195-015-0161-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Van Hulle C, et al. An examination of a novel multipanel of CSF biomarkers in the Alzheimer’s disease clinical and pathological continuum. Alzheimers Dement. 2021;17:431–445. doi: 10.1002/alz.12204. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Hayes AF. Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis : a regression-based approach. New York: The Guilford Press; 2018. [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Palmqvist S, et al. Performance of fully automated plasma assays as screening tests for Alzheimer disease-related β-amyloid status. JAMA Neurol. 2019;76:1060–1069. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2019.1632. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Bertens D, Tijms BM, Scheltens P, Teunissen CE, Visser PJ. Unbiased estimates of cerebrospinal fluid β-amyloid 1-42 cutoffs in a large memory clinic population. Alzheimers Res Ther. 2017;9:8. doi: 10.1186/s13195-016-0233-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Palmqvist S, et al. Earliest accumulation of β-amyloid occurs within the default-mode network and concurrently affects brain connectivity. Nat Commun. 2017;8:1214. doi: 10.1038/s41467-017-01150-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Hardy JA, Higgins GA. Alzheimer’s disease: the amyloid cascade hypothesis. Science. 1992;256:184–185. doi: 10.1126/science.1566067. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Pereira JB, et al. Plasma GFAP is an early marker of amyloid-β but not tau pathology in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain. 2021;144:3505–3516. doi: 10.1093/brain/awab223. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Barnes J, et al. Vascular and Alzheimer’s disease markers independently predict brain atrophy rate in Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative controls. Neurobiol Aging. 2013;34:1996–2002. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2013.02.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Sánchez-Benavides G, et al. Amyloid-β positive individuals with subjective cognitive decline present increased CSF neurofilament light levels that relate to lower hippocampal volume. Neurobiol Aging. 2021;104:24–31. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2021.02.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Moore EE, et al. Cerebrospinal fluid biomarkers of neurodegeneration, synaptic dysfunction, and axonal injury relate to atrophy in structural brain regions specific to Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2020;16:883–895. doi: 10.1002/alz.12087. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Jack CR, Jr, et al. NIA-AA Research Framework: toward a biological definition of Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2018;14:535–562. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2018.02.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Obrocki P, et al. Perspectives in fluid biomarkers in neurodegeneration from the 2019 biomarkers in neurodegenerative diseases course-a joint PhD student course at University College London and University of Gothenburg. Alzheimers Res Ther. 2020;12:20. doi: 10.1186/s13195-020-00586-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Bridel C, et al. Diagnostic value of cerebrospinal fluid neurofilament light protein in neurology: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Neurol. 2019;76:1035–1048. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2019.1534. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Milà-Alomà M, et al. Amyloid beta, tau, synaptic, neurodegeneration, and glial biomarkers in the preclinical stage of the Alzheimer’s continuum. Alzheimers Dement. 2020;16:1358–1371. doi: 10.1002/alz.12131. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Mielke MM, et al. Plasma and CSF neurofilament light: relation to longitudinal neuroimaging and cognitive measures. Neurology. 2019;93:e252–e260. doi: 10.1212/wnl.0000000000007767. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Sperling RA, et al. Amyloid deposition is associated with impaired default network function in older persons without dementia. Neuron. 2009;63:178–188. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.07.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Mattsson N, et al. Cerebrospinal fluid tau, neurogranin, and neurofilament light in Alzheimer’s disease. EMBO Mol Med. 2016;8:1184–1196. doi: 10.15252/emmm.201606540. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Ossenkoppele R, et al. Tau PET patterns mirror clinical and neuroanatomical variability in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain. 2016;139:1551–1567. doi: 10.1093/brain/aww027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Damoiseaux JS. Effects of aging on functional and structural brain connectivity. Neuroimage. 2017;160:32–40. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.01.077. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Sala-Llonch R, Bartrés-Faz D, Junqué C. Reorganization of brain networks in aging: a review of functional connectivity studies. Front Psychol. 2015;6:663. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00663. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Jonsson M, et al. Cerebrospinal fluid biomarkers of white matter lesions - cross-sectional results from the LADIS study. Eur J Neurol. 2010;17:377–382. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-1331.2009.02808.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Sjögren M, et al. Neurofilament protein in cerebrospinal fluid: a marker of white matter changes. J Neurosci Res. 2001;66:510–516. doi: 10.1002/jnr.1242. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Skillbäck T, et al. Sex differences in CSF biomarkers for neurodegeneration and blood-brain barrier integrity. Alzheimers Dement (Amst) 2021;13:e12141. doi: 10.1002/dad2.12141. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Ossenkoppele R, et al. Assessment of demographic, genetic, and imaging variables associated with brain resilience and cognitive resilience to pathological tau in patients with Alzheimer disease. JAMA Neurol. 2020;77:632–642. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2019.5154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Buckley RF, et al. Sex differences in the association of global amyloid and regional tau deposition measured by positron emission tomography in clinically normal older adults. JAMA Neurol. 2019;76:542–551. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2018.4693. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Abu-Rumeileh S, et al. CSF biomarkers of neuroinflammation in distinct forms and subtypes of neurodegenerative dementia. Alzheimers Res Ther. 2019;12:2. doi: 10.1186/s13195-019-0562-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Suárez-Calvet M, et al. sTREM2 cerebrospinal fluid levels are a potential biomarker for microglia activity in early-stage Alzheimer’s disease and associate with neuronal injury markers. EMBO Mol Med. 2016;8:466–476. doi: 10.15252/emmm.201506123. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Bogdanovic N, Davidsson P, Gottfries J, Volkman I, Winblad B, Blennow K. Regional and cellular distribution of synaptic proteins in the normal human brain. Brain Aging. 2002;2(2):18–30. [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Lövdén M, Fratiglioni L, Glymour MM, Lindenberger U, Tucker-Drob EM. Education and cognitive functioning across the life span. Psychol Sci Public Interest. 2020;21:6–41. doi: 10.1177/1529100620920576. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

13195_2022_1081_MOESM1_ESM.docx (13.5MB, docx)

Additional file 1: Supplementary Tables 1A and B. Bivariate analyses divided into participants with two or more than two MRI scans or cognitive visits. Supplementary Table 2. Multivariate analysis. Supplementary Table 3. Interaction effects. Supplementary Table 4. Associations between all variables significantly associated with the residual measure in bivariate analyses. Supplementary Figure 1. Spaghetti plots showing individual trajectories over time for mean whole brain cortical thickness and mPACC5. Supplementary Figure 2. Scatterplot showing the positive association between change in mean whole brain cortical thickness and change in global cognition. Supplementary Figure 3. Directed acyclic graphs (DAGs) constructed for inclusion of variables in mediation analyses.

Data Availability Statement

Anonymized data will be shared by request from a qualified academic investigator for the sole purpose of replicating procedures and results presented in the article and as long as data transfer is in agreement with EU legislation on the general data protection regulation and decisions by the Swedish Ethical Review Authority and Region Skåne, which should be regulated in a material transfer agreement.


Articles from Alzheimer's Research & Therapy are provided here courtesy of BMC

RESOURCES