Skip to main content
BMC Pediatrics logoLink to BMC Pediatrics
. 2022 Oct 5;22:572. doi: 10.1186/s12887-022-03636-0

Impact of pet dog or cat exposure during childhood on mental illness during adolescence: a cohort study

Anne Gadomski 1,, Melissa B Scribani 2, Nancy Tallman 6, Nicole Krupa 3, Paul Jenkins 4, Lawrence S Wissow 5
PMCID: PMC9532803  PMID: 36199055

Abstract

Background

In our prior study of 643 children, ages 4–11 years, children with pet dogs had lower anxiety scores than children without pet dogs. This follow-up study examines whether exposure to pet dogs or cats during childhood reduces the risk of adolescent mental health (MH) disorders.

Methods

Using a retrospective cohort study design, we merged our prior study database with electronic medical record (EMR) data to create an analytic database. Common MH diagnoses (anxiety, depression, ADHD) occurring from the time of prior study enrollment to 10/27/21 were identified using ICD-9 and ICD-10 codes. We used proportional hazards regression to compare time to MH diagnoses, between youths with and without pets. From 4/1/20 to 10/27/21, parents and youth in the prior study were interviewed about the amount of time the youth was exposed to a pet and how attached s/he was to the pet. Exposure included having a pet dog at baseline, cumulative exposure to a pet dog or cat during follow-up, and level of pet attachment. The main outcomes were anxiety diagnosis, any MH diagnosis, and MH diagnosis associated with a psychotropic prescription.

Results

EMR review identified 571 youths with mean age of 14 years (range 11–19), 53% were male, 58% had a pet dog at baseline. During follow-up (mean of 7.8 years), 191 children received a MH diagnosis: 99 were diagnosed with anxiety (52%), 61 with ADHD (32%), 21 with depression (11%), 10 with combined MH diagnoses (5%). After adjusting for significant confounders, having a pet dog at baseline was associated with lower risk of any MH diagnosis (HR = 0.74, p = .04) but not for anxiety or MH diagnosis with a psychotropic prescription. Among the 241 (42%) youths contacted for follow-up, parent-reported cumulative exposure to pet dogs was borderline negatively associated with occurrence of any MH diagnosis (HR = 0.74, p = .06). Cumulative exposure to the most attached pet (dog or cat) was negatively associated with anxiety diagnosis (HR = 0.57, p = .006) and any MH diagnosis (HR = 0.64, p = .013).

Conclusion

Cumulative exposure to a highly attached pet dog or cat is associated with reduced risk of adolescent MH disorders.

Keywords: Pet exposure in childhood, Mental illness in adolescence, Pet attachment, Pet dog, Pet cat

Background

Nearly 1 in 3 US teens are diagnosed with an anxiety disorder [14]. Anxiety disorders in adolescence and adulthood often begin in childhood with a median age of onset at 11 years [13]. Anxiety is also often comorbid with, or even misclassified as, Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) in children and teens, so current estimates may under-estimate its true prevalence [5]. Anxious children often develop impaired social relationships, academic underachievement, substance abuse, and depression [68]. While early treatment of childhood anxiety is effective, only about half the children with any mental health (MH) disorders are treated due to limited access to MH therapy [911]. The COVID-19 pandemic has exacerbated these pre-existing MH challenges, thus increasing the prevalence of elevated anxiety and depression symptoms among children and adolescents [12, 13].

Preventing or mitigating evolution of MH problems in childhood to MH disorders in adolescence and later life would be very impactful, yet little is known on what prevention strategy may work [14]. Pet dogs may ameliorate childhood anxiety through emotional support and attachment [1519]. Dogs have long been recognized as salient agents for child emotional development because they follow human communicative cues [15, 16]. The psychological support that pets provide may be greatest during childhood and early adolescence [18]. Youth challenged by psychosocial, developmental and self-esteem transitions may be supported by pets before and during adolescence [20, 21].

Our prior cross-sectional study of 643 children recruited in a primary care clinic showed that children, ages 4 to 11 years, with pet dogs had lower anxiety scores (for separation and social anxiety) than children without pet dogs [22]. That study used tablet screening tools at pediatric annual visits to determine how pet dogs may affect child mental and physical health. The mean scores for the Screen for Child Anxiety Related Emotional Disorders (SCARED-5) that is based on Diagnostic and Statistical Manual, Fourth Edition (DSM-IV) symptoms were 1.13 (1.00, 1.26) for children with pet dogs and 1.40 (1.23, 1.58) for children without (p = .01). The proportion of children with a score  3, for which more in-depth diagnostic work is indicated, was 12% for children with pet dogs compared to 21% of children without pet dogs (p = .002). We also found that, in a secondary analysis focused on pet cat ownership controlling for child age, poverty, and parental depression, pet cat ownership was associated with more attention problems (b = 1.38, SE = 0.53, p < .01) [23].

Divergent results such as these in studies of human animal interaction (HAI) are often attributed to small sample sizes, lack of adjustment for confounding factors, misinterpretation of correlational factors as causal factors, and inability to randomize subjects to experimental or control conditions [2427]. Prospective studies that control for a wide range of confounders are needed [28], but have also led to inconsistent results as illustrated by the following examples. A longitudinal birth cohort study of 627 children in the US showed that prenatal exposure to pet dogs, but not cats, was associated with twice the odds of ADHD in 10 to 12 year old boys [29]. A longitudinal study of 3017 children in Australia found that pet dog or cat ownership was associated with a small positive effect on the socioemotional development of young children [30]. As with our prior study [23], children with cats had higher hyperactivity scores compared to children not owning any pets (OR, 1.40; 95% CI, 1.08–1.80). A cohort study in Japan showed that dog exposure (but not cat exposure) at six months of life was associated with decreased risk of developmental delay among infants 12 months of age [31].

The strength of a child’s attachment to the pet, rather than pet ownership per se, may confer benefit. While the underlying mechanisms are unclear, attachment theory stipulates a bidirectional connectivity between humans and their pets that may exert a significant influence on a child’s socioemotional development [3234]. The degree of attachment between a child and a pet dog or cat is associated with improved child social-cognitive and socioemotional development, and psychosocial health [18, 35, 36]. However, longitudinal studies of the impact of children’s attachment to pet dogs or cats on youth MH are limited.

Building upon our prior studies as well as addressing limitations of HAI studies and the potential role of child pet attachment, we investigated what impact child/youth HAI may have on adolescent MH. We sought evidence of whether exposure to pet dogs or cats during childhood is inversely related to the occurrence of adolescent MH disorders. Specifically, using the electronic medical record (EMR) to determine the time to first MH disorder diagnosis, the aim of this study was to compare the incidence density of MH diagnosis between youths with pet dog and/or cat exposure to those without pet exposure. Additional aims were to measure the association between pet exposure and the occurrence of sub-threshold or threshold but undiagnosed MH disorders over the follow-up period, and to explore how attachment and potential confounders affect the relationship between pet dog and/or cat exposure and youth MH.

Methods

In a retrospective cohort study design, clinical data for this study was extracted from Bassett Healthcare Network’s system-wide integrated Epic EMR (Epic Systems Inc., Verona, WI) that has been used in this health network since 2011. Our prior study database was merged with EMR data to create a patient-level analytic database that included common adolescent MH diagnoses (anxiety, depression, ADHD) occurring from the time of enrollment in the prior study to 10/27/21. In addition, parents and youth included in the original study were contacted to collect data on subsequent pet exposure and pet attachment data, covariates (demographics etc.), and youth MH scales to measure anxiety, depression, and social support. In order to account for the differential follow-up time among the subjects, time to event and incidence density were the main outcomes of the study rather than cumulative incidence.

EMR data abstraction for MH Diagnoses

MH diagnoses of children and adolescents are increasingly considered categorical siloes that do not easily lend themselves to developmental trajectory, maturation, progression, extension or resolution [37]. Nevertheless, MH diagnoses do signify a functional problem, a clinical need and/or troublesome symptoms in a child or adolescent’s life that are brought to medical attention. MH diagnoses of interest in this study population included anxiety, any common MH diagnoses and MH diagnosis associated with psychotropic medication, assuming that acquiring an MH diagnosis in the EMR was a significant event reflecting functional limitation or troublesome symptoms. International Classification of Disease, Ninth Edition, Clinical Modification (ICD-9) codes before 10/1/15, and ICD-10 codes thereafter, were used to identify the first occurring MH diagnoses among all EMR encounters. Diagnosis and prescription orders are structured data fields in the Epic EMR. These data were drawn from all outpatient and inpatient encounters (including pediatric, general medical, emergency department, surgical, dental and specialty mental health). Previous studies have shown that EMR prescription data for children has very high quality whereas ICD-9/ICD-10 codes may be more variable but still sensitive [38, 39]. In addition, validation studies of EMR ICD-9 code of 314.X for ADHD have shown that these codes are highly sensitive in identifying ADHD cases [40].

Psychotropic medication prescribing was defined as evidence of any oral psychotropic medication prescription included in Epic orders. These included antidepressant, anxiolytic/sedative-hypnotic, antipsychotic, and medications for ADHD (including stimulants, atomoxetine, selective alpha-2 receptor agonists), but excluded anticonvulsants used for seizure treatment, amitriptyline used for migraine prophylaxis and pre-procedure benzodiazepine. Anticonvulsants (carbamazepine, lamotrigine, valproate/divalproex) were included as psychotropic medications if the patient did not have a seizure disorder diagnosis. Prescriptions for psychotropic medication were retrieved not only to validate the MH diagnosis but also to create the category of MH diagnosis prescribed a psychotropic medication, as a proxy for more severe MH disorders for youth seen in primary care.

After MH diagnosis and medication extraction, 10% of EMR records with an MH diagnosis and all records with psychotropic prescription were validated by manual full text EMR encounter review to resolve any discrepancies in date of diagnosis or prescription, or reason for prescription use. These cases were reviewed with a consulting child psychiatrist (LSW) to determine whether they met inclusion criteria. Children who had an MH diagnosis at baseline, i.e. before or on the date of enrollment in the original study 8 years ago, were excluded from the proportional hazards regression analysis. Post hoc exclusion criteria of MH diagnoses made during the follow-up period included: (i) chronic, serious MH diagnoses unlikely to be significantly impacted by presence of a pet dog or cat, (ii) operative dental encounters with the diagnosis of dental anxiety, and no subsequent visits with anxiety diagnosis, and (iii) anxiety visit diagnosis associated with physical exams, procedures, ED visits and no subsequent visits with an anxiety diagnosis.

Follow-up interview and survey data collection

This part of the study gathered self-reported data from parents and youth ~ eight years after the prior study. Re-recruitment was planned to occur before, during or after primary care clinic visits as had occurred in the prior study. Unfortunately, the onset of the COVID-19 pandemic coincided with the start of this study so in-person recruitment was not possible. Instead, research staff contacted prior study participants by phone and/or email in the same order as they were recruited previously to arrange a time for telephone interview. After informed consent was obtained from the parent and youth, the parent was interviewed about the youth’s pet exposure over the past 8 years. Then the parent and youth completed a secure, HIPAA compliant, digital questionnaire. Data from these questionnaires were downloaded on the secure Research Electronic Data Capture (REDCap) [41] server at the Bassett Research Institute. Covariates included demographics (education, employment status, income level), number of children at home, the index child’s birth order, asthma/allergy status and adverse experiences with dogs or cats, and family history of mental illness or autism. The following rating scales were included:

Screen for Child Anxiety Related Emotional Disorders (SCARED41) includes dimensional measures of anxiety symptoms and is used to assess general anxiety, separation anxiety, social phobia, school phobia and physical symptoms of anxiety. It has 41 items that measure these dimensions with cut-off values for each [42, 43].

Adverse Child Experiences (ACE-Q) questionnaire yields a cumulative score of stressors (as opposed to revealing specific stressors). The 10 item ACE–Q score was completed by the parent who indicated the number of ACE related statements that pertained to the index youth [44]. Aggregate-level versus item-level reporting was utilized because parents may be more likely to disclose ACEs when this level of privacy is provided [45].

Patient Health Questionnaire 8-item (PHQ-8) is a commonly used screener for depression, a condition that is frequently co-morbid with anxiety [46, 47].

Companion Animal Bonding Scale (CABS) has 8 items that measure attachment to a companion animal using behavioral interactions between people and their pets [48, 49]. This questionnaire was completed for all pet dogs and cats by the parent, whereas the youth only completed the CABS for the pet with the highest parent rated CABS.

Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support (MSPSS) is designed to measure perceptions of support from 3 sources: Family, Friends, and a Significant Other [50]. It has 12 items, with 4 items for each of the three subscales. Mean MSPSS scores 1-2.9 are considered low support; 3–5 moderate support and 5.1-7 high support.

Study participants were mailed gift cards after completion of the interview and questionnaires.

Statistical analysis

Demographics and other characteristics were compared across study groups using chi-square for sex, Fisher’s Exact test for race, and the t-test for age. Categorical data collected via questionnaire, such as residence, rating scales, etc. were compared across study groups using chi-square test or Fisher’s Exact test as necessary. These tests were used to assess whether known or potential sociodemographic confounders were associated with both pet exposure and time to MH diagnosis. All covariates were tested to see if they were statistically associated with both MH diagnosis and pet exposure; if associated with both, they were included in all final proportional hazards regression models. Missing values for the household income variable were imputed using a multiple imputation Markov chain Monte Carlo method. This method was employed because the missing value pattern was not monotonic. Spearman’s rank correlation coefficient was used to measure the correlation between categorical or ordinal variables.

EMR derived MH diagnosis

We used proportional hazards regression to analyze (1) time to anxiety diagnosis, (2) time to any common MH diagnosis and (3) time to any MH diagnosis associated with a psychotropic prescription. Models two and three excluded subjects with any MH diagnosis preceding baseline and included 571 youth. Model one excluded only subjects diagnosed with anxiety prior to baseline and included 605 youth. Pet exposure, defined as the presence of the pet dog at baseline, was derived from the baseline study and was coded as yes or no. Proportional hazard models were adjusted for age, sex, zip code poverty level and baseline SCARED5 (score  3 on the 5 item SCARED5 completed by the parent for the child at the time of the original study). Results were reported as adjusted hazards ratios (HR) with 95% confidence intervals (CI).

EMR plus 8 year follow-up data

Models for youth who completed 8 year follow-up were additionally adjusted for potentially confounding covariates. Confounders were selected based on having a significant association with dog or cat ownership (exposure) and time to MH diagnosis (outcome). The mean and median follow-up time was calculated as the time between enrollment in our prior study and the follow-up phone call ~ eight years later.

Pet exposure

Based on parental interview at the ~ 8 year follow-up, cumulative pet exposure variables were constructed for dogs and/or cats. The cumulative value includes the number of days of pet dog and/or cat exposure and the number of these pets during follow-up as a fixed value established for each subject. Cumulative exposure was designed as a measure of the child’s exposure to dogs over the entire course of follow-up expressed in units of 100%. For example, if one dog was present with the child over the entire course of follow-up, the cumulative exposure would be 100% (for 2 dogs the entire time = 200% etc.) If a child was splitting his/her time evenly between two households and a dog was present at one household for the entire follow-up, this child would be assigned a value of 50%. Changes in exposure that occurred over the course of follow-up were also accounted for. For example, if a dog in the child’s home was lost for some reason 75% of the way through follow-up, the child would be assigned a value of 75%. Similarly, if two dogs were present and one was lost half way through follow-up with the other being present the entire time, the child was assigned a value of 150%. Similar logic was applied if dogs were added to the child’s household partway through follow-up.

Pet attachment

At follow-up, the parent CABS and youth CABS for the most attached pet were highly correlated (n = 226, Spearman’s rho = 0.80, p < .0001). Regardless of the amount of pet exposure time, the pet with the highest CABS score per the parent was classified as the child’s most attached pet. Children without pets were excluded from this model. The hazard ratio was expressed as a function of a 50% increase in the presence of the most attached pet (the range for this pet exposure was 0 to 100% with a mean of 54%). An interaction term of the type of pet (cat versus dog) by cumulative exposure was tested and was not statistically significant in the prediction of time to MH diagnosis, and was therefore dropped. The final proportional hazards model included a main effect for type of pet in addition to the cumulative exposure.

All statistical analyses were performed using SAS version 9.4 statistical software (Cary, NC).

Results

EMR analysis revealed 643 subjects who had been in the prior study. Fourteen children did not have a subsequent visit, therefore these subjects had their follow-up time set to zero, leaving 629 subjects available for follow-up analysis.

Exclusions

Given that a MH diagnosis was found prior to the follow-up period, 56 children with any pre-existing MH diagnoses were excluded from further analysis. These diagnoses were ADHD (42/56, 75%), anxiety (10/56, 18%), anxiety and depression (2/56, 3.6%), other serious emotional disorders (2/56, 3.6%). Four additional subjects with chronic, serious MH diagnosis unlikely to be significantly mitigated by the presence of a pet dog or cat were excluded from analysis. There were 12 subjects missing SCARED5 score at baseline who were excluded, leaving 571 in the proportional hazards regression analysis (Table 1).

Table 1.

Baseline Covariates for 571 youth included in modeling of pet dog at baseline

Covariate Total With pet dog(s) at baseline Without pet dog(s) at baseline P value (with pet dog vs. without)
Youth characteristics
# (%) a 571 330 (57.79) 241 (42.21)
Child age, years Mean (SD) 6.57 (2.13) 6.60 (2.13) 6.53 (2.14) 0.6913
# (% ) male 304 (53.24) 175 (53.03) 129 (53.53) 0.9065
SCARED ≥ 3 (%) at baseline b 73 (12.78) 33 (10.00) 40 (16.60) 0.0197
PARENT or Guardian Characteristics
# (%) relationship: motherc 451 (80.25) 250 (76.92) 201 (84.81) 0.0204
Mean % (SD) subjects in baseline zip code living in poverty 14.76 (5.40) 14.94 (5.36) 14.50 (5.47) 0.3370
# (%) parent/guardians with positive PHQ at baselined 10 (1.81) 4 (1.25) 6 (2.56) 0.3360

a Total sample size is 571, that excludes 60 subjects with pre-existing/complicated MH diagnoses and excludes 12 subjects with missing baseline SCARED5.

b This statistical difference was published in a prior study (see Reference 22)

c Nine parent/guardians did not report on relationship to child

d 19 parent/guardians did not have baseline PHQ score

Inclusions

Six subjects with anxiety diagnosis associated with physical exams, procedures, ED visits, dental extraction, and no subsequent visits with anxiety diagnosis were included, but these encounters with anxiety diagnoses were excluded. There were 34 instances of psychotropic medications that were not associated with any visit MH diagnosis; these encounters were included as any MH diagnoses associated with a psychotropic medication.

Among the 571 youth included in the EMR analysis, 97.8% reside in a county that is a Mental Health Professional Shortage Area (MHPSA). Overall, 55.8% of non-dog owning households at baseline became dog owners during the follow-up period.

Covariate analysis

Table 2 compares covariates between those who completed follow-up by phone (241) and those who did not. Importantly, the incidence density (per 100 child-years) of MH diagnosis among those who completed follow-up was 5.89 and not significantly different from 4.98 to 100 child-years among those without follow-up (p = .27). Youth who had a pet dog at baseline were more likely to complete follow-up 8 years later (p = .03). Otherwise, the groups were similar.

Table 2.

Comparison of 571 youth who did or did not complete eight year follow-up (fup) interview based on baseline covariates

Covariate Completed 8 year fup interview Did not complete fup interview P value (fup vs. no fup)
# 216a 355
Mean (SD) youth age in years 14.1 (2.3) 14.0 (2.1) 0.7995
% male 53.2 53.2 0.9999
% pet dog at baseline 64.4 54.4 0.0335
SCARED ≥ 3 (%) at baseline 10.2 14.4 0.1468
% mothers responded at baseline 84.0 77.9 0.0780
Mean % population in current zip code living in poverty 14.4 15.0 0.2433
% parent PHQ positivity 1.4 2.0 0.7495
Incidence density of MH diagnosis (per 100 child-years) 5.89 4.96 0.2627
# (%) Anxiety diagnosis 44 (51.2) 55 (52.4) 0.9058
# (%) ADHD diagnosis 28 (32.5) 33 (31.4)
# (%) Depression diagnosis 10 (11.6) 11 (10.5)
# (%) Combined MH diagnosis 4 (4.7) 6 (5.7)

a216 completed 8 year follow-up does not match the 241 who completed phone follow-up due to exclusion of prior MH diagnoses and 2 subjects with missing baseline SCARED5.

Table 3 shows the covariate associations with cumulative dog exposure for parents and youth who completed follow-up interview 8 years after the baseline study. The only true confounders related to both exposure and outcome were reported income (with imputed values) and type of health insurance. Birth order and history of child having asthma/allergies were associated with cumulative dog exposure (p < .05), but were NOT associated with time to any MH diagnosis. The total MSPSS and subscale scores were not associated with pet exposure. MSPSS was not associated with time to MH diagnosis (HR = 0.987, p = .0945), however a higher level of social support from friends subscale was significantly associated with decreased risk of MH diagnosis (HR = 0.821, p = .0082). Parent-reported household income, imputed for missing values, was associated with cumulative dog exposure and time to MH diagnosis and thus was included in all final models. Public insurance was associated with lower cumulative dog exposure (p = .0159) and was also associated with MH diagnosis (HR = 2.02, p = .0021). Public insurance was highly correlated with income (having public insurance was associated with a higher proportion of subjects in lower income categories; p < .0001 by chi-square), therefore only income was included as a confounder in the final models.

Table 3.

Covariate associations with cumulative dog exposure for parents and youth who completed follow-up interview 8 years after the baseline study

Covariate Total Spearman Correlation Coefficient for Cumulative Dog Exposure t-statistic (*F-value) P value
YOUTH
# 241b
Mean (SD) youth age in years 14.82 (2.29) 0.077 -- 0.2550
% male 55.51 0.69 0.4903
Mean (SD) of children in the home 2.23 (1.63) -0.041 -- 0.5302
Mean (SD) Birth order of index child 1.69 (1.08) 0.147 -- 0.0313
% full time live with parent 94.17 2.02 0.0616
% with public insurance 38.01 -2.43 0.0159
% teens who have asthma or allergies 27.66 2.29 0.0238
% teens who ever had a bad experience with dog or cat 11.49 0.53 0.6025
% Pet dog at T1 a 61.22 8.92 < 0.0001
Mean (SD) SCARED41 score at follow-up 19.50 (16.19) 0.042 -- 0.5206
Mean (SD) PHQ8 score at follow-up 4.00 (5.16) -0.021 -- 0.7491
Mean (SD) MSPSS score at follow-up 70.00 (13.62) -0.056 -- 0.3875
Mean (SD) ACE-Q score at follow-up 1.11 (1.41) -0.023 -- 0.7288
PARENT or GUARDIAN
Mean (SD) parent age in years 43.40 (6.74) 0.003 -- 0.9680
% mothers 91.29 1.40 0.1761
% married and living together 70.95 1.53 0.1278
% who had a dog or cat growing up (Always/Usually) 80.08 1.26* 0.2857
% working full timec 66.39 0.48* 0.6204
% some college education or higher 80.91 -1.56 0.1235
% rating their own health as excellent or very good 67.22 0.22 0.8285
% Hispanic or Latino 1.70 -1.69 0.1847
% white 96.27 0.09 0.9329
% Annual household income > FPL for 4-person household d 94.19 2.27* 0.0489
% live in a house 87.97 -0.73 0.4721
% live in a city or near urban area 1.24 -1.57 0.2503
% with family history of mental illness 29.77 1.08 0.2810
% with autism in family member 13.16 1.19 0.2432
Mean (SD) PHQ8 score at follow-up 3.00 (4.46) 0.079 -- 0.2211
Summary Statistics Mean (SD) Median (IQR) Range
Cumulative Dog Exposure

91.65

(78.96)

87.49 (15.38-149.85) 0-316.28
Cumulative Cat Exposure 74.57 (102.23)

27.31

(0-119.16)

0-684.37

aexcludes all outdoor housed dog and cats. b 241 is the total number of subjects who completed phone follow up. 19 were excluded from MH models due to MH diagnosis at baseline and 2 excluded due to missing baseline SCARED5. c Asked during COVID-19 pandemic so may not be representative of pre-pandemic conditions. d The lowest income category used was the cutoff for federal poverty level (FPL) for a 4 person household (0–26,200).The F-statistic and p-value relate to the association of dog exposure with the full range of income levels (not just dichotomous +/- FPL).

*F-values presented for variables with more than two levels

Pet dog at baseline

Proportional hazards regression modeling of time to any MH diagnosis based on the presence of a pet dog at baseline included 571 adolescents (mean age 14 years, range 11 to 19 at follow-up), 53% were male, 58% had a pet dog at baseline. During follow-up (mean of 7.8 years), 191 children received a MH diagnosis. Of these, 99 were diagnosed with anxiety (52%), 61 with ADHD (32%), 21 with depression (11%) and 10 with combined MH diagnoses (5%). These models were adjusted for relevant covariates i.e. age, sex, baseline SCARED 5  3 and baseline percent of population living in poverty within the zip code [51] (this variable served as a proxy for household income that was not assessed at baseline) (Table 4). The sample sizes included in these models vary based on the diagnostic outcome being modeled and whether the baseline SCARED5 was missing. Having a pet dog at baseline was associated with reduced risk for the occurrence of any MH diagnosis (HR = 0.74, p = .04, 95% CI 0.56–0.99), but not for anxiety diagnosis (HR = 0.88, p = .44, 95% CI 0.63–1.22) or any MH diagnosis associated with a psychotropic prescription (HR = 0.79, p = .23, 95% CI 0.55–1.15). Presence of a pet cat was not specifically ascertained at baseline.

Table 4.

Adjusted proportional hazards regression modeling of risk of mental health diagnosis over the eight-year follow-up period associated with pet exposure

Pet Dog at baselinea Cumulative exposure to pet dogb Cumulative Exposure to pet catb Cumulative exposure to most attached petb,c
N HR (95% CI) p N HR (95% CI) p N HR (95% CI) p N HR (95% CI) p
Anxiety diagnosis 605 0.88 (0.63–1.22) 0.44 233 0.84 (0.60–1.17) 0.30 233 0.93 (0.71–1.21) 0.58 220 0.57 (0.38–0.85) 0.006
Any MH diagnosis 571 0.74 (0.56–0.99) 0.04 220 0.74 (0.54–1.02) 0.06 220 0.87 (0.68–1.11) 0.28 204 0.64 (0.44–0.91) 0.013
Any MH diagnosis with psychotropic prescription 571 0.79 (0.55–1.15) 0.23 220 0.83 (0.57–1.21) 0.33 220 0.78 (0.56–1.10) 0.16 204 0.69 (0.44–1.08) 0.106

aadjusted for age, sex, zip code level poverty and baseline SCARED 5  3

badjusted for baseline SCARED5  3 and household income

climited to subjects with at least one pet during follow-up

Cumulative exposure to pet dog or cat during follow-up

Of those 241 youth who completed phone follow-up, 19 were excluded from MH models due to the presence of an MH diagnosis at baseline and two more were excluded due to a missing baseline SCARED5. In this group, there were 75 MH diagnosis associated with visits made during the follow-up period. The distribution of diagnoses was similar to the larger group of 571 subjects, in that, 52% were diagnosed with anxiety (n = 39), 35% with ADHD (n = 26), 9% with depression (n = 7) and 4% with combined MH diagnoses (n = 3). Proportional hazards regression of time to any MH diagnosis based on the cumulative exposure to a pet dog during the eight year follow-up included 220 teens (mean age 14.8 years, range 11 to 19), 53.2% were male, 62.7% had a pet dog at baseline. The mean length of follow-up time was 7.8 and the median was 7.7. Only 21 subjects had no pet exposure during the 8-year follow-up period. Using cumulative exposure to pet dogs reported by the parent during follow-up and adjusting for baseline SCARED5 and self-reported income, pet dog(s) were marginally associated with a reduced risk of any MH diagnosis (HR = 0.74, p = .06, 95% CI 0.54–1.02), but pet cats were not (HR = 0.87, p = .28, 95% CI 0.68–1.11) (Table 4). Neither pet dogs (HR = 0.84, p = .30, 95% CI 0.60–1.17) nor cats (HR = 0.93, p = .58, 95% CI 0.71–1.21) were associated with a reduced risk of an anxiety diagnosis or any MH diagnosis associated with a psychotropic medication.

Most attached pet

Cumulative exposure to the most highly attached pet dog or cat exposure was associated with a reduced risk for the occurrence of anxiety diagnosis (HR = 0.53, p = .002, 95% CI 0.36–0.79) and any MH diagnosis (HR = 0.58, p = .0032, 95% CI 0.41–0.83), but not for any MH diagnosis associated with a psychotropic medication (Table 4). After adjustment for income, a significant protective effect of the most attached pet for anxiety (HR = 0.57, p = .006, 95% CI 0.38–0.85) and any MH diagnosis (HR = 0.64, p = .013, 95% CI 0.44–0.91) remained. These HR’s indicate that exposure to most attached pet during follow-up is associated with a 43% reduced risk of anxiety and 36% reduced risk in any MH diagnosis. While pet type (dog vs. cat) was not a statistically significant covariate in this model for most attached pet, the variable was retained as a means to adjust for type of pet.

Screening results for youth without a MH diagnosis

Because youth in this study resided in MHPSA’s and thus could be at a higher risk of not being diagnosed with MH disorder, we examined the association between pet exposure and the occurrence of sub-threshold or threshold but undiagnosed MH disorders over the follow-up period. For 241 youth with self-reported SCARED41 and PHQ8 at the time of follow-up, there was no correlation between cumulative dog exposure and these total scores (for SCARED41 Spearman’s rho = 0.058, p = .40, and for PHQ8 Spearman’s rho=-0.058, p = .39).

Adverse childhood experience score (ACE-Q)

Of the 228 youth who completed this instrument, 45% of youth had a score of zero, 26.3% had a score of 1, 13.6% had a score of 3, and 15% had a score  3, for a mean of 1.11. There was no correlation between cumulative dog exposure and ACE-Q score (Spearman’s Rho = -0.023, p = .7288). Both PHQ8 and SCARED41 scores were highly correlated with the ACE-Q score for the 228 youth who had ACE scores (between ACE-Q score and PHQ8, Spearman’s Rho = 0.284, p < .0001; and ACE-Q and SCARED41, Spearman’s Rho = 0.155, p = .019). There was no correlation between CABS and ACE-Q scores.

Discussion

Exposure to pet dogs in childhood was negatively associated with the incidence of adolescent MH diagnoses, an association that was accentuated by high levels of attachment to the pet. Even at the crude measure of exposure (Y/N pet dog(s) at baseline), pet dogs were negatively associated with the risk of any MH diagnosis (p = .04). The most attached pet was significantly associated with a decreased risk for anxiety diagnosis (HR = 0.57, p = .006, 95% CI 0.38–0.85) and any MH diagnosis (HR = 0.64, p = .013, 95% CI 0.44–0.91). However, we found no significant relationship between any measure of pet exposure and more severe MH diagnoses for which psychotropic medications were prescribed.

During the eight year follow-up, 130 subjects lost 1 or more pets to death which can have a significant effect on mental health. However, this study was not structured to quantify this effect because the variability in time lapsed between pet death and when PHQ9 was administered precluded analysis of the impact of pet loss.

Despite the suggestion that neglected and abused children may be more likely to trust and attach to dogs [18], we found no correlation between cumulative dog or cat exposure and ACE-Q score, or with CABS and ACE-Q. This finding is consistent with a recent study showing that emotional closeness with a childhood pet dog did not moderate a significant association between ACE score and mental health [52].

Given the observational nature of the study, the possibility that pet dog exposure is simply a marker for a child and/or environment that is less vulnerable to mental illness must be considered. Understandably, a randomized trial of pet dog or cat ownership is neither feasible nor ethical. Given these two facts, the possibility of a causal link between pet dog exposure and reduced probability of MH diagnosis remains speculative, as the underlying mechanism for this relationship is unknown, although several have been proposed. Theoretical frameworks include HAI effects on dampening of the HPA axis in response to stress [53]. For example, dog owners interacting with their dogs have significant increases in plasma oxytocin levels that, in turn, decrease cortisol levels [54]. Another proposed mechanism is that pets transfer beneficial microbes to infants and young children that increase the diversity of their microbiome thereby improving the gut–microbiota–brain axis [5557]. While this study was not designed to test underlying mechanisms, it provides further evidence of a positive relationship between pet dogs and youth mental health.

The COVID19 pandemic occurred 3/7/20 shortly after study start date 2/1/20, so it is important to consider how this context affected study results. Several studies have documented that the prevalence of anxiety, depression, and other MH problems among children and adolescents doubled during the pandemic [12, 58, 59]. This pandemic effect would presumably affect both subjects with and without pets equally in our study and therefore not bias the results. Next, there is some evidence that the pandemic initially reduced psychotropic medication prescriptions, but then later was associated with an increase in antidepressant prescriptions. These findings from a population-based study of 0 to 18 year olds in Manitoba, Canada, were attributed to lock downs in March 2020 that led to a decline in psychotropic medication use. This decline was followed by an increase in new-onset anxiety or depression during the pandemic that led to increased antidepressant prescription [60]. Again, these potential effects are likely equally distributed across our study population, and therefore would not bias our study outcome of MH diagnosis associated with a psychotropic prescription.

Strengths

HAI research has been criticized for the lack of longitudinal studies, small sample sizes, poorly defined HAI instrument validity and reliability, and heterogeneous outcome assessment [2428]. Our study addresses these weaknesses in the rigor of prior studies as follows. We leveraged the study results of our cross-sectional sample of children recruited 8 years prior to conduct a naturalistic, longitudinal analysis of the relationship between pet dog and cat exposure and the development of youth mental illness reflected by MH diagnosis. The study was strengthened by minimizing the biases associated with using only one outcome, e.g. parent or self-report of MH symptoms, by testing many potential covariates for confounding, and adjusting the final multivariate analysis for known confounders, i.e. income and baseline SCARED5  3, that were associated with both pet ownership and MH diagnosis.

Limitations

Our study was observational and nonrandomized. While we adjusted our analysis for demonstrated confounders, unmeasured confounders (e.g., pandemic-related variable school attendance, social isolation, etc.) may have influenced results. The stress of the pandemic may have limited parents’ willingness to participate in the follow-up interview and thereby contributed to non-response bias for data obtained by interview. Responders were more likely to have had a dog at baseline and to have an MH diagnosis during follow-up than were non-responders, however it is difficult to assess how these imbalances affect our study results. Measuring cumulative pet exposure may be subject to recall bias especially over long time intervals, thus calling into question the precision of duration of pet exposure reported by parents in this study. However, the exposure variable of pet dog exposure (Y/N) at baseline was not subject to recall bias, yet this model showed that pet dogs were negatively associated with the risk of any MH diagnosis (p = .04).

This study was conducted in a rural MHPSA, where limited access to MH services (exacerbated by the pandemic) implies a higher likelihood of lack of MH diagnosis or the potential for delayed or mis-diagnosis. Although MH diagnoses may be subject to mis-classification, under reporting, and are sometimes not reflective of full MH outcome assessment [37], they do signify significant impairment and in this study serve as a marker for MH disorder. These MH diagnosis limitations led us to use trans-diagnostic categories of any MH diagnosis that combines common MH disorders, but is less specific and does not inform what the underlying mechanism of the youth pet dog effect might be. Lastly, EMR data are collected in the context of providing healthcare and may be subject to within visit factors that influence documentation and accuracy of diagnosis and history of prior medications [61].

Conclusion

Cumulative exposure to a highly attached pet dog or cat during childhood is associated with reduced risk of adolescent MH disorders. Although the underlying mechanism for this association is unknown, this study adds to the growing evidence supporting the potential benefits of companion animal interaction for youth socioemotional development. This study also underscores the need to measure and account for the level of child or youth pet attachment and duration of exposure, rather than simply studying the pet ownership [62].

Acknowledgements

We gratefully acknowledge the participation of the families included in this study. Funding from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development made this work possible.

List of abbreviations

ACE-Q

Adverse Child Experiences Questionnaire.

ADHD

Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder.

CABS

Companion Animal Bonding Scale.

EMR

electronic medical record.

HAI

human animal interaction.

HR

hazard ratio.

ICD-9

International Classification of Diseases, Ninth revision.

ICD-10

International Classification of Diseases, Tenth revision.

MH

mental health.

MHPSA

Mental Health Professional Shortage Area.

MSPSS

Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support.

PHQ-8

Patient Health Questionnaire 8-item.

SCARED

Screen for Child Anxiety Related Emotional Disorders.

Author contribution

AG, LW, PJ, MS designed the study and wrote the grant application.

NT collected primary data.

MS, NK, PJ performed data management and analysis.

AG and LW reviewed the mental health diagnoses and medications extracted from the EMR.

AG wrote the main manuscript text and MS, NK, PJ wrote the data analysis sections.

MS and NK prepared Tables 1, 2, 3 and 4.

All authors reviewed the manuscript.

Funding

Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, NICHD 5 R21 HD101078.

Data availability

Materials may be requested by contacting the corresponding author (anne.gadomski@bassett.org). The database contains patient data and will not be shared.

Declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

This study, project number 1482814-1, was approved by the Mary Imogene Bassett Hospital IRB on September 16, 2019 (Federal Wide assurance number 00003628, IRB registration # IRB00000200). The study methods were carried out in accordance with Federal HHS regulations for the protection of human subjects (45 C.F.R. § 46) including subparts A and D. Initial written informed consent was obtained from parents who participated in the baseline study. At follow-up, full informed consent from the parent, legal guardian or youth 18 years of age or older was obtained at the time of the follow-up interview. Youth assent for those under 18 years of age, was obtained at the time of the interview. These documents were signed and are securely stored at the Bassett Research Institute.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors have no competing interests as defined by BMC, or other interests that might be perceived to influence the results and/or discussion reported in this paper.

Footnotes

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

References

  • 1.Merikangas KR, He JP, Burstein M, Swanson SA, Avenevoli S, Cui L, Benjet C, Georgiades K, Swendsen J. Lifetime prevalence of mental disorders in U.S. adolescents: results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication–Adolescent Supplement (NCS-A) J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2010;49:980–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2010.05.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Kessler RC, Petukhova M, Sampson NA, Zaslavsky AM, Wittchen HU. Twelve month and lifetime prevalence and lifetime morbid risk of anxiety and mood disorders in the United States. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res. 2012;21:169–84. doi: 10.1002/mpr.1359. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Eley TC, McAdams TA, Rijsdijk FC. The Intergenerational Transmission of Anxiety: A Children-of-Twins Study. Am J Psychiatry. 2015;172(7):630–7. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2015.14070818. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.McLaughlin KA, Kubzansky LD, Dunn EC, Waldinger R, Vaillant G, Koenen KC. Childhood social environment, emotional reactivity to stress, and mood and anxiety disorders across the life course. Depress Anxiety. 2010;27:1087–94. doi: 10.1002/da.20762. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Leichsenring F, Leweke F. Social Anxiety Disorder. N Engl J Med. 2017;376:2255–64. doi: 10.1056/NEJMcp1614701. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Grover RL, Ginsburg GS, Ialongo N. Childhood predictors of anxiety symptoms: a longitudinal study. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2005;36:133–53. doi: 10.1007/s10578-005-3491-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Sciberras E, Lycett K, Efron D, Mensah F, Gerner B, Hiscock H. Anxiety in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Pediatrics. 2014;133:801–8. doi: 10.1542/peds.2013-3686. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Kendall PC, Compton SN, Walkup JT, Birmaher B, Albano AM, Sherrill J, Ginsburg G, Rynn M, McCracken J, Gosch E, Keeton C, Bergman L, Sakolsky D, Suveg C, Iyengar S, March J, Piacentini J. Clinical characteristics of anxiety disordered youth. J Anxiety Disord. 2010;24:360–5. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2010.01.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Anderson LE, Chen ML, Perrin JM, Van Cleave J. Outpatient Visits and Medication Prescribing for US Children With Mental Health Conditions. Pediatrics. 2015;136(5):e1178-85. doi: 10.1542/peds.2015-0807. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Whitney DG, Peterson MD. US National and State-Level Prevalence of Mental Health Disorders and Disparities of Mental Health Care Use in Children. JAMA Pediatr. 2019;173(4):389–91. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2018.5399. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Cree RA, Bitsko RH, Robinson LR, Holbrook JR, Danielson ML, Smith C, Kaminski JW, Kenney MK, Peacock G. Health care, family, and community factors associated with mental, behavioral, and developmental disorders and poverty among children aged 2–8 years—United States. 2016. MMWR Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report. 2018;67:1137–83. doi: 10.15585/mmwr.mm6750a1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Racine N, McArthur BA, Cooke JE, Eirich R, Zhu J, Madigan S. Global Prevalence of Depressive and Anxiety Symptoms in Children and Adolescents During COVID-19: A Meta-analysis. JAMA Pediatr. 2021 Nov 1;175(11):1142–1150.10.1001/jamapediatrics.2021.2482. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 13.HHS. Protecting Youth Mental Health: The U.S. Surgeon General’s Advisory. Office of the Surgeon General. Published online December 6, 2021. Accessed February 1. 2022. https://www.hhs.gov/sites/default/files/surgeon-general-youth-mental-health-advisory.pdf.
  • 14.Malhi GS, Outhred T, Morris G, Hamilton A, Das P, Mannie Z. Primary prevention of mood disorders: A primary concern that requires urgent action. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2018;57(9):629–31. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2018.05.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Silva K, de Sousa L. ’Canis empathicus’? A proposal on dogs’ capacity to empathize with humans. Biol Lett. 2011;7:489–92. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2011.0083. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Udell MA, Dorey NR, Wynne CD. Can your dog read your mind?: Understanding the causes of canine perspective taking. Learn Behav. 2011;39:289–302. doi: 10.3758/s13420-011-0034-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Melson GF. Principles for human–animal interaction research. In: McCardle P, McCune S, Griffin JA, Maholmes V, editors. How animals affect us: Examining the influences of human–animal interaction on child development and human health. Washington, DC, US: American Psychological Association; 2011. pp. 13–33. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Payne E, Bennett PC, McGreevy PD. Current perspectives on attachment and bonding in the dog–human dyad. Psychol Res Behav Manage. 2015;8:71–9. doi: 10.2147/PRBM.S74972. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Marsa-Sambola F, Williams J, Muldoon J, Lawrence A, Connor M, Currie C. Quality of life and adolescents’ communication with their significant others (mother, father, and best friend): the mediating effect of attachment to pets. Attach Hum Dev. 2017;19(3):278–97. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2017.1293702. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Purewal R, Christley R, Kordas K, Joinson C, Meints K, Gee N, Westgarth C. Companion Animals and Child/Adolescent Development: A Systematic Review of the Evidence. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2017;14(3):234. doi: 10.3390/ijerph14030234. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Van Houtte BA, Jarvis PA. The role of pets in preadolescent psychosocial development. J Appl Dev Psychol. 1995;16:463–79. doi: 10.1016/0193-3973(95)90030-6. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Gadomski AM, Scribani MB, Krupa N, Jenkins P, Nagykaldi Z. Olson AL Pet Dogs and Children’s Health: Opportunities for Chronic Disease Prevention? Prev Chronic Dis. 2015;12:E205. doi: 10.5888/pcd12.150204. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Riley MR, Gibb B, Gadomski AM. Household Cats and Children’s Mental Health. Human-Animal Interact Bull. 2018;6(2):17–24. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Herzog H. The Impact of Pets on Human Health and Psychological Well-Being: Fact, Fiction, or Hypothesis? Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2011;20:236–9. doi: 10.1177/0963721411415220. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Herzog H. The research challenge: threats to the validity of animal-assisted therapy studies and suggestions for improvement. In: Fine AH, editor. Handbook on Animal-Assisted Therapy. Foundations and Guidelines for Animal-Assisted Interventions. London: Academic Press; 2015. pp. 402–6. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Serpell J, McCune S, Gee N, Griffin JA. Current challenges to research on animal-assisted interventions. Appl Dev Sci. 2017;21(3):223–33. doi: 10.1080/10888691.2016.1262775. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Purewal R, Christley R, Kordas K, et al. Socio-demographic factors associated with pet ownership amongst adolescents from a UK birth cohort. BMC Vet Res. 2019;15:334. doi: 10.1186/s12917-019-2063-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Mueller MK, King EK, Callina K, Dowling-Guyer S, McCobb E. Demographic and contextual factors as moderators of the relationship between pet ownership and health. Health Psychol Behav Med. 2021;9(1):701–23. doi: 10.1080/21642850.2021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Cassidy-Bushrow AE, Sitarik AR, Johnson-Hooper TM, Phillips JM, Jones K, Johnson CC, Straughen JK. Prenatal pet keeping and caregiver-reported attention deficit hyperactivity disorder through preadolescence in a United States birth cohort. BMC Pediatr. 2019;19(1):390. doi: 10.1186/s12887-019-1719-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Christian H, Mitrou F, Cunneen R, Zubrick SR. Pets are associated with fewer peer problems and emotional symptoms, and better prosocial behavior: Findings from the longitudinal study of Australian children. J Pediatr. 2020;220:200–6. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2020.01.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Minatoya M, Araki A, Miyashita C, et al. Cat and Dog Ownership in Early Life and Infant Development: A Prospective Birth Cohort Study of Japan Environment and Children’s Study. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2020;17(1):205. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17010205. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Poresky RH. Companion Animals and other Factors Affecting Young Children’s Development. Anthrozoös. 1996;9:159–68. doi: 10.2752/089279396787001437. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Endenburg N, van Lith HA. The influence of animals on the development of children. Vet J (London, England: 1997) 2011;190(2):208–214. 10.1016/j.tvjl.2010.11.020. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 34.Julius H, Beetz A, Kotrschal K, Turner D, Uvnas-Moberg K. Attachment to Pets Cambridge,MA.; Hogrefe Publishing, 2013.
  • 35.Kerns KA, Koehn AJ, van Dulmen MHM, Stuart-Parrigon KL, Coifman KG. Preadolescents’ Relationships with Pet Dogs: Relationship Continuity and Associations with Adjustment. Appl Dev Sci. 2017;21(1):67–80. doi: 10.1080/10888691.2016.1160781. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Wanser SH, Vitale KR, Thielke LE, Brubaker L, Udell MA. Spotlight on the psychological basis of childhood pet attachment and its implications. Psychol Res Behav Manag. 2019;12:469–79. doi: 10.2147/PRBM.S158998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Shah JL, Scott J, McGorry PD, Cross SPM, Keshavan MS, Nelson B, Wood SJ, Marwaha S, Yung AR, Scott EM, Öngür D, Conus P, Henry C, Hickie IB. International Working Group on Transdiagnostic Clinical Staging in Youth Mental Health. Transdiagnostic clinical staging in youth mental health: a first international consensus statement. World Psychiatry. 2020;19(2):233–42. doi: 10.1002/wps.20745. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Sutherland SM, Kaelber DC, Downing NL, Goel VV, Longhurst CA. Electronic Health Record–Enabled Research in Children Using the Electronic Health Record for Clinical Discovery. Pediatr Clin North Am. 2016;63(2):251–68. doi: 10.1016/j.pcl.2015.12.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Mayne SL, Ross ME, Song L, et al. Variations in Mental Health Diagnosis and Prescribing Across Pediatric Primary Care Practices. Pediatrics. 2016;137(5):e20152974. doi: 10.1542/peds.2015-2974. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Shi Y, Schulte PJ, Hanson AC, Zaccariello MJ, Hu D, Crow S, Flick RP, Warner DO. Utility of medical record diagnostic codes to ascertain attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and learning disabilities in populations of children. BMC Pediatr. 2020; 7;20(1):510. 10.1186/s12887-020-02411-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 41.Harris PA, Taylor R, Thielke R, Payne J, Gonzalez N, Conde JG. Research electronic data capture (REDCap)—a metadata-driven methodology and workflow process for providing translational research informatics support. J Biomed Inform. 2009;42(2):377–81. doi: 10.1016/j.jbi.2008.08.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Birmaher B, Brent DA, Chiappetta L, Bridge J, Monga S, Baugher M. Psychometric properties of the screen for child anxiety related emotional disorders (SCARED): A replication study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1999;38(10):1230–6. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199910000-00011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Wren FJ, Berg EA, Heiden LA, Kinnamon CJ, Ohlson LA, Bridge JA, Birmaher B, Bernal MP. Childhood anxiety in a diverse primary care population: parent-child reports, ethnicity and SCARED factor structure. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2007;46:332–40. doi: 10.1097/chi.0b013e31802f1267. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Burke Harris N, Renschler T. Center for Youth Wellness ACE-Questionnaire (CYW ACE-Q Child, Teen, Teen SR version 7/2015). Center for Youth Wellness. San Francisco, CA. centerforyouthwellness.org/wp-content/uploads/2018/06/CYW-ACE-Q-USer-Guide-copy.pdf.
  • 45.Gillespie RJ, Folger AT. Feasibility of Assessing Parental ACEs in Pediatric Primary Care: Implications for Practice-Based Implementation. Journ Child Adol Trauma. 2017;10:249–56. doi: 10.1007/s40653-017-0138-z. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Kroenke K, Strine TW, Spitzer RL, Williams JB, Berry JT, Mokdad AH. The PHQ-8 as a measure of current depression in the general population. J Affect Disord. 2009;14(1–3):163–73. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2008.06.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Wu Y, Levis B, Riehm KE, et al. Equivalency of the diagnostic accuracy of the PHQ-8 and PHQ-9: a systematic review and individual participant data meta-analysis. Psychol Med. 2020;50(8):1368–80. doi: 10.1017/S0033291719001314. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Poresky RH, Hendriz C, Mosier JE, Samuelson ML. The Companion Animal Bonding Scale: Internal reliability and construct validity. Psychol Rep. 1987;60:743–6. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1987.60.3.743. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Poresky RH. The young children’s empathy measure: Reliability, validity and effects of companion animal bonding. Psychol Rep. 1990;66:931–6. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1990.66.3.931. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Zimet GD, Dahlem NW, Zimet SG, Farley GK. The Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support. J Pers Assess. 1988;52:30–41. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa5201_2. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Berkowitz SA, Traore CY, Singer DE, Atlas SJ. Evaluating Area-Based Socioeconomic Status Indicators for Monitoring Disparities within Health Care Systems: Results from a Primary Care Network. Health Serv Res. 2015;50(2):398–417. doi: 10.1111/1475-6773.12229. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Rodriguez KE, McDonald SE, Brown SM. Relationships among Early Adversity, Positive Human and Animal Interactions, and Mental Health in Young Adults. Behav Sci. (Basel) 2021;11(12):178. doi: 10.3390/bs11120178. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Kertes DA, Liu J, Hall NJ, Hadad NA, Wynne CDL, Bhatt SS. Effect of Pet Dogs on Children’s Perceived Stress and Cortisol Stress Response. Soc Dev. 2017;26(2):382–401. doi: 10.1111/sode.12203. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Petersson M, Uvnäs-Moberg K, Nilsson A, Gustafson L-L, Hydbring-Sandberg E, Handlin L. Oxytocin and Cortisol Levels in Dog Owners and Their Dogs Are Associated with Behavioral Patterns: An Exploratory Study. Front Psychol. 2017;8:1796. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01796. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Tun HM, Konya T, Takaro TK, Brook JR, Chari R, Field CJ, et al. Exposure to household furry pets influences the gut microbiota of infants at 3–4 months following various birth scenarios. Microbiome. 2017;5:40. doi: 10.1186/s40168-017-0254-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Valles-Colomer M, Falony G, Darzi Y, et al. The neuroactive potential of the human gut microbiota in quality of life and depression. Nat Microbiol. 2019;4:623–32. doi: 10.1038/s41564-018-0337-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Kates Ashley E, Omar J, Skarlupka Joseph H, Ajay S, Megan D, Lauren W, Garret S, Poulsen Keith, Safdar Nasia. Household Pet Ownership and the Microbial Diversity of the Human Gut Microbiota. Frontiers in Cellular and Infection Microbiology. 2020;10:73 DOI = 10.3389/fcimb.2020.00073. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 58.Loades ME, Chatburn E, Higson-Sweeney N, et al. Rapid Systematic Review: The Impact of Social Isolation and Loneliness on the Mental Health of Children and Adolescents in the Context of COVID-19. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2020;59(11):1218–39. doi: 10.1016/J.JAAC.2020.05.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Gassman-Pines A, Ananat EO, Fitz-Henley J. COVID-19 and Parent-Child Psychological Well-being. Pediatrics. 2020;146(4):e2020007294. doi: 10.1542/peds.2020-007294. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Leong C, Katz LY, Bolton JM, et al. Psychotropic Drug Use in Children and Adolescents Before and During the COVID-19 Pandemic. JAMA Pediatr Published online January 04, 2022. 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2021.5634. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 61.Stewart R, Davis K. ’Big data’ in mental health research: current status and emerging possibilities. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2016;51(8):1055–72. doi: 10.1007/s00127-016-1266-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Barcelos AM, Kargas N, Maltby J, et al. A framework for understanding how activities associated with dog ownership relate to human well-being. Sci Rep. 2020;10:11363. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-68446-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

Materials may be requested by contacting the corresponding author (anne.gadomski@bassett.org). The database contains patient data and will not be shared.


Articles from BMC Pediatrics are provided here courtesy of BMC

RESOURCES