Abstract
Laboratory studies show that increased physiological burden during development results in cognitive impairment. In the wild, animals experience a wide range of developmental conditions, and it is critical to understand how variation in such conditions affects cognitive abilities later in life, especially in species that strongly depend on such abilities for survival. We tested whether variation in developmental condition is associated with differences in spatial cognitive abilities in wild food-caching mountain chickadees. Using tail feathers grown during development in juvenile birds, we measured feather corticosterone (Cortf) levels and growth rates and tested these birds during their first winter on two spatial learning tasks. In only 1 of the 3 years, higher feather Cortf was negatively associated with memory acquisition. No significant associations between feather Cortf and any other measurement of spatial cognition were detected in the other 2 years of the study or between feather growth rate and any measurement of cognition during the entire study. Our results suggest that in the wild, naturally existing variation in developmental condition has only a limited effect on spatial cognitive abilities, at least in a food-caching species. This suggests that there may be compensatory mechanisms to buffer specialized cognitive abilities against developmental perturbations.
Keywords: developmental burden, cognition, memory, food-caching, chickadee, paridae
1. Introduction
Animals exhibit large variation, both across and within species, in cognitive abilities, yet the mechanisms underlying such variation remain poorly understood [1–3]. While selection can act on heritable traits associated with genetic differences [4,5], it remains uncertain how much individual variation in cognitive abilities observed in non-human animal populations may be produced by other factors, most notably differences in development [1,2,4,6–8]. Most research on animal cognition takes place in laboratory environments, which allows for the precise control of environmental conditions during both cognitive testing and development [3]. Past laboratory-based work has shown that increased exposure to stress-inducing events during early development can have profoundly negative impacts on the future cognition of young animals [2,7,9–11]. Cognitive traits depend on energetically expensive, specialized tissues (i.e. brain regions) to properly function and non-optimal conditions during development diverts energy expenditure towards short-term survival rather than neural investment [3,7,8,10]. The hippocampus, a brain region involved in spatial navigation and cognitive function, is known to be a major target of stress-mediated effects, which appears to be driven by elevation in glucocorticoid hormones [6,9,12].
Glucocorticoids are a well-studied class of steroid hormone that are known to mediate broad metabolic processes [13–16]. Prolonged exposure to elevated levels of these hormones (namely cortisol and corticosterone) has been shown to increase the overall wear and tear on organisms over time [9,13,14]. Although these processes are regarded as normal signs of ageing, when they occur in young animals, they can severely alter developmental trajectories and appear to have a direct, negative impact on cognitive abilities in general [6,8,14,17], and particularly on hippocampus-dependent spatial abilities [7,10].
The ability to navigate through and remember past environments is a ubiquitous trait in animals. However, some species rely on the hippocampus for more specialized spatial cognitive abilities, for example, food-caching species [3,18]. Scatter caching animals store thousands of individual food items throughout their environments and rely on spatial memory for future cache retrieval [3,18]. In some North American chickadee species (Poecile atricapillus & Poecile gambeli), the dependence on food caches for overwinter survival has been shown to vary across environments that differ in climatic harshness [18–21]. This environmental variation has also been linked with differences in spatial learning and memory ability and associated hippocampal morphology [18,20,22,23]. Moreover, individual variation in spatial cognitive abilities in food-caching species appears to be associated with differences in overwinter survival [21] and has a genetic basis [5]. This suggests that differences in spatial cognitive abilities in food-caching species probably arose via natural selection [21].
Considering that spatial cognitive abilities are critical for survival in food-caching species, it has been suggested that selection should favour the evolution of mechanisms that may protect them and their associated neural tissue from environmental perturbations during development [2]. Yet, laboratory-based studies still show that increased developmental stress associated with impacts from malnutrition and prolonged exposure to elevated glucocorticoid levels, sometimes directly related to food deficits, result in impaired spatial cognitive abilities in some food-caching species [10,11]. However, laboratory conditions provide highly impoverished environments compared to natural settings and may potentially restrict animals' abilities to resist or cope with developmental stress [2]. Such restrictions include limited use of cognition (i.e. decreased opportunities to learn and practice) as well as rather restricted and researcher determined nutrition (i.e. limited food diversity). Currently, little is known about whether developmental stress is strongly associated with impaired spatial cognitive abilities in wild animals living in their natural environment [1].
In this study, we investigated whether natural variation in developmental conditions in juvenile birds was associated with differences in their spatial cognitive abilities. This study took place across three years using three separate cohorts of first-year wild mountain chickadees (P. gambeli). Mountain chickadees are small, highly sedentary songbirds that rely on specialized spatial cognition associated with food-caching to survive the montane winter environments in temperate western North America [5,18,21,24]. Birds residing at higher elevations that experience longer harsher winters compared to lower elevations appear to generally have better spatial learning and memory abilities [22,25] associated with a larger hippocampus and more hippocampal neurons [22]. Additionally, strong directional selection favouring first-year birds with better spatial learning and memory performance was detected in an ongoing long-term study [21].
The condition of individual birds during post-hatching development was evaluated via differences in glucocorticoid hormone levels (corticosterone or Cortf) [26,27] and growth rates [28] measured in tail feathers. Malnutrition is known to result in slower feather growth rates and elevated corticosterone levels (e.g. [10,11,28]). Chickadees grow their first plumage while in the nest environment and before becoming nutritionally independent [24]. The quality of each feather, which is indicative of overall developmental conditions, is reflective of both the local environment and parental investment [24,26–31]. Unlike measurements using blood samples, feather Cortf represents conditions during the entire early developmental period including time in the nest and the early post-fledging period, when tail feathers continue to grow [24,26,27,32]. However, this method does come with some limitations including limited temporal understanding of when the hormone deposition took place and it is used here as a relative measure [26,27].
Tail feather daily growth rates were assessed using feather growth bars (horizontal bands along the length of the feather), which accurately reflect caloric intake during a 24 h period [28]. We predicted that patterns of yearly variation in individual developmental conditions would align with patterns of reproductive success in the larger population (i.e. years with smaller broods or lower nestling mass would also show more Cortf). We collected tail feathers from juvenile chickadees throughout the autumn and winter and then tested their cognitive abilities in the same winter using two spatial cognitive tasks: a spatial learning and memory task and a single spatial reversal learning task (flexibility) following our well-established methods [21,25,33–35]. We predicted that chickadees that experienced worse developmental conditions (higher amounts of Cortf deposited in feathers across development and smaller, slower growing feathers) would perform worse in both cognitive tasks compared to individuals with less Cortf exposure and larger, faster growing feathers.
2. Methods
(a) . Study site
All data for this study were collected during 2018–2021 at the Sagehen Experimental Forest (Sagehen Creek Field Station, University of California, Berkeley) in the Sierra Nevada, USA. We monitor mountain chickadees year-round and collect annual measurements on reproductive success [36] and spatial cognitive performance [19,21,25,33–35]. We focus on two subsets of the population at two montane elevations, a high elevation site (approx. 2400 m) and a low elevation site (approx. 1900 m), which differ greatly in winter environmental conditions [36]. High elevations are associated with harsher winters characterized by lower temperatures, higher and longer lasting snow levels and unpredictable winter storms compared to lower elevations [35]. We track individually marked birds across years via unique combinations of plastic colour bands, a passive integrated transponder (PIT) tag and in some cases an aluminium US Geological Survey band. We capture chickadees via mist net during the autumn and winter (non-breeding), and at nestboxes during spring and summer (breeding), for banding. Nestlings are banded with only an aluminium leg band at day 16 post-hatch and, if recaptured, are then equipped with a PIT tag and an additional colour band. All birds are classified as juveniles (less than a year of age) or adults (more than a year of age) at time of initial capture based on multiple plumage characteristics [37], but only juveniles were included in this study.
(b) . Spatial cognitive testing
Across three years (2018–2021), we tested juvenile chickadees at both elevations using two spatial cognitive tasks—a spatial learning and memory task and a single spatial reversal learning task [25,33,34]. For cognitive testing, we used four feeder arrays, each composed of eight radio frequency identification (RFID)-based feeders positioned equidistant on a square frame (122 × 122 cm) and suspended ca. 4 m above the ground [21,25,33,34]. Two spatial arrays were used at each elevation, positioned approximately 1.5 km apart [25,33]. Each RFID-equipped feeder contained an antenna embedded in a perch that was linked to an RFID-enabled Arduino circuit board with built-in memory and time keeping [38]. This setup allowed us to record all visits by PIT-tagged birds (identification and visitation timing) and operate a motor-controlled door that managed food access [38]. Before cognitive testing, all array feeders were maintained in ‘open’ mode, in which feeder doors were open and food was clearly visible and available to all visiting birds. One week prior to cognitive testing, all feeders were switched to ‘all’ mode in which all doors were closed but would open when any PIT-tagged bird landed on any feeder perch. This stage allows birds to habituate to the moving doors prior to testing. Following ‘all’ mode, we started the spatial learning and memory task by programming all feeders to ‘target’ mode, in which feeders continue to track visits from all birds, but only one feeder on the array opens for each bird with all birds pseudo-randomly assigned to different feeders within an array [25,33,34]. Following our previous work, we used two metrics to measure cognitive performance in each task: the mean number of ‘location errors’ (non-rewarding feeders visited prior to visiting the correct rewarding feeder) per trial over the first 20 trials and over the entire task (5 days) [21,25,33–35]. A trial started when a bird visited any feeder in the array and ended with a visit to the rewarding feeder [25]. Performance during the first 20 trials of the spatial task represents initial memory acquisition and performance over the entire task reflects longer term memory ability [21]. Previously, we have shown that variation in both metrics is associated with fitness consequences (i.e. individuals that make more errors show a fitness reduction) [21,39] and has genetic basis [5].
Immediately following the spatial learning and memory task, we carried out a single spatial reversal learning task by re-assigning each bird to a different rewarding feeder in each array. Birds assigned to the same feeder during the spatial learning and memory task were re-assigned to different feeders during the reversal task to prevent social learning [33,34]. As in the previous task, we measured performance using the mean number of location errors per trial over the first 20 trials and over the entire spatial reversal learning task (5 days).
We conducted spatial learning and memory tasks from 8 to 13 April in the 2018–2019 winter (both elevations), from 20 to 24 January at low elevation and from 3 to 7 February at high elevation in 2019–2020, and from 13 to 17 January in 2020–2021 (both elevations). We ran spatial reversal learning tasks at both elevations from 13 to 18 April in 2019, from 24 to 29 January at low elevation and from 7 to 13 February at high elevation in 2020, and at both elevations from 20 to 26 January 2021. Variation in testing dates between years and elevations is due to technical difficulties or environmental perturbations (e.g. winter storms, deep snow) that impeded our ability to access the feeders; however, for each test type the data from five consecutive days was used for analyses.
(c) . Feather size and growth rate
We collected a single tail feather from juvenile (first year) birds during the autumn and early winter months in 2018–2020 when birds were captured in mist nets prior to the onset of cognitive testing. We removed the single outermost rectrix (r12) from each bird and placed feathers in labelled coin envelopes that were stored in a cool dry place until processing as previous work has shown that feathers do not degrade overtime [27]. We did not use any damaged or severely worn feathers in the study. Feathers were assessed for overall mass via an electronic scale to the nearest 0.1 mg and length via ruler to the nearest 0.5 mm. This process was repeated before and after the removal of the calamus or the barbless tip of the feather. Calamus removal was required for the glucocorticoid extraction protocol (see below).
We measured feather growth rate using daily growth bar width [28]. Feathers were placed on a white 10.16 × 15.24 cm notecard and exposed to low-angle light to visualize growth bars. The beginning and end of each growth bar were marked on a notecard in light pencil, allowing for clearer visualization and measurement [28]. After marking, each feather card was photographed and ImageJ software (National Institute of Health) was used to measure each growth bar to the nearest 0.1 mm [40]. Mean feather growth bar width was used in all analyses.
(d) . Feather corticosterone (Cortf)
We subjected chickadee tail feathers to a standard corticosterone extraction procedure followed by an enzyme-linked immunosorbent (ELISA) assay using methods established by Bortolotti et al. in 2008–2009 and modified following Grant et al. in 2020 [26,27,41]. Feathers from all years were extracted in the spring of 2021 and immediately assayed. Individual feathers were cut into multiple pieces (approx. 5 mm in length) and placed (excluding the calamus) into 20 ml scintillation vials with seven mL of HPLC grade 100% methanol. Scintillation vials were sonicated in a room temperature water bath for 30 min and then placed in a shaking 50°C water bath overnight (16 h). The methanol and feather material were separated using vacuum filtration and each vial was washed twice with two ml of methanol each wash. The separated methanol was then dried in a FlexiVap station, which is composed of a heat block that also supplies constant airflow over the surface of the methanol to aid with evaporation rate. We reconstituted dried samples in 500 ml of assay buffer and samples were sealed and frozen until assayed. Reconstitution volume and parallelism were determined by previous assays of serial dilutions of chickadee feathers not used in this experiment. We ran samples diluted by 1 : 300, 1 : 600 and 1 : 900. Parallel curves were generated, and a dilution of 1 : 500 was selected due to this dilution having the highest level of parallelism.
All samples were assayed using a corticosterone ELISA kit supplied by Enzo life sciences (catalog no. ADI-901-097) following the manufacturer's instructions. Feathers from all years were assayed using the same kit to reduce batch bias. We used a serial dilution of known Cort concentration to create a standard curve and used the coefficient of variation (CV) of known standards (20 000 pg ml−1, 4000 pg ml−1, 800 pg ml−1, 160 pg ml−1 and 32 pg ml−1) to calculate inter- (18%) and intra-assay (plate 1: 15%, plate 2: 6%) variation. Inter-plate CV was calculated from duplicated controls made from pooled Cortf from previous samples and was taken through the entire assay process. Cort values were then standardized by feather length to account for variation in Cortf deposition during growth [26,27,41]. Samples were randomized across plates.
(e) . Reproductive monitoring
We predicted yearly variation in Cortf and feather growth would match the larger population's reproductive pattern (i.e. years with lower resource availability should lead to both higher Cortf and smaller and lower quality broods). To evaluate annual reproductive output of the study population during the 3 years of study, we analysed reproductive parameters used in previous studies as an index of environmental and developmental conditions (e.g. [36,42]). Mountain chickadees rarely produce more than a single brood each year and all juvenile birds produced in a given season are considered a cohort. Only a handful of nestlings reared in provided nestboxes were recaptured for Cortf analysis and returned to participate in cognitive testing. However, the individuals that were sampled for both Cortf and cognitive performance were sampled from the same yearly cohorts, allowing for general comparisons between annual trends in measures of developmental condition and reproductive output. Adult chickadees were monitored throughout the spring and summer months for the onset of breeding behaviour, including nest building in provided nestboxes and first egg date [36]. Nests were subsequently monitored on a biweekly basis for onset of incubation, hatching date, brood size and overall success or failure. Nestlings were processed on day 16 post-hatch where we counted brood size, measured individual mass and banded all nestlings with an aluminium USGS band. Larger clutch and brood sizes and higher fledgling mass is typically associated with better environmental conditions during a given year [42]. Within-brood CV in fledgling mass was also estimated. Higher CV of fledgling mass indicates a larger variance in condition of individual fledglings within a nest and may reflect relatively worse developmental conditions (i.e. food availability, parental quality, conditions of the nest).
(f) . Statistical analyses
All analyses and associated figures for this study were generated using R v. 4.1.2 [43,44]. We used linear and generalized linear models to investigate the relationship between Cortf and feather morphometrics and individual variation in spatial cognitive performance (mean number of location errors per trial during the first 20 trials and during the entire task) of first-year chickadees. We tested each model for its residual fit using the R package ‘DHARMa’ [45].
We quantified the presence of corticosterone in each feather in picograms per mm and used these values for the majority of analyses. The only exceptions were analyses that included feather length or mass as a response variable; in these cases, we used the total picogram values per sample. We fit models to test for variation across time and location using year and elevation as fixed effects and Cortf (controlling for feather mass), feather length (mm), feather mass (g) and mean feather growth rate (mm per 24 h) as response variables. Each response variable was investigated in an independent model. We ran an additional model with capture time (Julian date) as a fixed effect and Cortf as the response to test whether earlier sampled individuals had higher Cortf compared to individuals sampled later in the season. The importance of each fixed effect was tested using a type III Wald chi-square test. When year or elevation was significant, we conducted post hoc Tukey analyses to determine significant differences across levels using the ‘emmeans’ R package [46].
We fit models testing for Cortf’s relationship with spatial cognitive metrics using Cortf, elevation and year as fixed effects and mean location errors per trial made during the first 20 trials as well as during the entire task (both spatial learning and memory or single spatial reversal learning) as the response variables. For models using mean location errors over the entire trial, we included the total number of trials completed during the task as a control predictor to account for differences in the total number of trials across individuals [25,34]. The mean number of errors per trial over the entire task was log-transformed for both the spatial and reversal learning tasks to fit the assumptions of the model. We only used birds that completed a minimum of 20 trials in both tasks for the analyses [25,34]. We fit similar models using mean daily feather growth, feather length and feather mass as fixed effects. All initial models were first tested for an interaction between the focal predictor (Cortf, growth, length and mass) and year as well as an interaction between year and elevation. If there was a significant interaction found in the interaction model, each year was subsequently analysed separately to investigate the nature of the interaction; otherwise, the interactions were removed. Sample sizes broken down by elevation and year are included in a summary table within the supplementary information.
Lastly, to investigate overall differences among the 3 years of breeding conditions, we fit general linear mixed-effects models using the package ‘glmmTMB’ [47] to compare reproductive parameters of the nestbox population. We used all available data from each study year despite the vast majority of individuals from the nestbox population not being recaptured for feather analysis or returned to participate in the cognitive tasks. We used elevation and year as fixed effects and breeding pair identification as a random effect to test for differences in clutch size, brood size, mean nestling mass and the CV of nestling mass. Clutch size and brood size models used a generalized Poisson distribution to adjust for count data containing a lack of zero values [48]. Each of these four response variables was run separately but clutch size was also used as a fixed predictor in the brood size model. This allows for the examination of how many nestlings fledged relative to eggs laid.
3. Results
(a) . Variation in feather Cortf, mass, length and growth rate across years and elevations
There were no significant differences in Cortf between elevations, but Cortf varied significantly among years (electronic supplementary material, table S1, figure S1A). In 2018, Cortf was significantly higher compared to 2019 (p = 0.04) and 2020 (p < 0.001) and in 2020, Cortf was significantly lower than in 2019 (p = 0.015). Neither variation in feather mass, length, nor 24 h growth rate were significantly associated with differences in Cortf (electronic supplementary material, table S2); larger, longer and faster growing feathers did not have significantly higher or lower Cortf. Additionally, capture time (Julian date) did not significantly predict Cortf after controlling for elevation and year showing that feathers collected earlier in the season did not contain more or less Cortf compared to individuals sampled later (electronic supplementary material, table S2). Capture dates varied from early autumn (20 August) to late winter (29 March), and these results suggest that we were not capturing individuals with more or less Cortf earlier or later in a given season.
Feather mass did not vary significantly across elevations but there were significant differences across years (electronic supplementary material, table S1, figure S1B). Feather mass was significantly higher in 2019 compared to both 2018 (p < 0.001) and 2020 (p < 0.001). At the same time, there were no significant differences in feather mass between 2018 and 2020 (p = 0.44, electronic supplementary material, figure S1B). Similarly, feathers were significantly longer in 2019 compared to both 2018 (p = 0.01) and 2020 (p = 0.012), with no significant differences in feather length between 2018 and 2020 (p = 0.92) and no differences associated with elevation (electronic supplementary material, table S1). This year-to-year variation was also evident in feather growth rates (i.e. mean feather bar width), which were significantly higher in 2019 compared to 2020 (p < 0.001) but not significantly different from growth rate in 2018 (p = 0.05). There were no significant differences in feather growth rate between 2018 and 2020 (p = 0.23, electronic supplementary material, table S1, figure S1c), and again there were no significant differences between elevations.
(b) . Spatial learning and memory
There was no significant association between performance in the first 20 trials of the learning and memory task (mean number of errors per trial) and Cortf, elevation or year (table 1). However, there was a significant interaction between Cortf and year (table 1). When analysed separately within each year, higher levels of Cortf were associated with worse performance across the first 20 trials (larger mean number of errors per trial) but only in 2018 and only at high elevation (as no data from low elevation were included in this model due to low sample size; table 2; figure 1a). There were no significant associations between performance on the first 20 trials of the task and Cortf in either 2019 or 2020 (table 2; figure 1b,c).
Table 1.
Mean location errors per trial over two spatial tasks and Cortf.
spatial learning task |
single reversal learning task |
|||
---|---|---|---|---|
location errors per trial in the first 20 trials | location errors per trial in the entire task | location errors per trial in the first 20 trials | location errors per trial in the entire task | |
predictor | n = 101a | n = 87b | ||
elevation | , p = 0.277 | , p = 0.714 | , p = 0.874 | , p = 0.756 |
year | , p = 0.248 | , p = 0.033 | , p = 0.05 | , p = 0.094 |
Cortf (pg mm–1) | , p = 0.499 | , p = 0.052 | , p = 0.702 | , p = 0.238 |
total trial number | NA | , p < 0.001 | NA | , p < 0.001 |
year × Cortf | , p = 0.017 | , p = 0.019 | NA | NA |
adjusted R2 | 0.06 | 0.47 | 0.07 | 0.38 |
aSample sizes by elevation and year for the spatial learning task: 2018 (low: 2; high: 26); 2019 (low: 6; high: 22); 2020 (low: 26; high: 19).
bSample sizes by elevation and year for the single reversal learning task: 2018 (low: 1; high: 20); 2019 (low: 6; high: 21); 2020 (low: 22; high: 17).
Table 2.
Mean location errors per trial over the entire learning and memory task and for the first 20 trials and Cortf by year.
location errors/trial in the first 20 trials | location errors/trial in the entire task | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
year | n | predictor | ||
2018 | total: 26 | elevation | removed due to lack of samples | removed due to lack samples |
low: 0 | ||||
high: 26 | Cortf (pg mm–1) | estimate = 0.05 ± 0.02, t = 2.56, p = 0.017 | estimate = 0.03 ± 0.01, t = 1.88, p = 0.072 | |
total trial number | NA | estimate = −2.7 × 10−3 ± 5.0 × 10−4, t = −5.45, p < 0.001 | ||
Adj. R2 | 0.21 | 0.59 | ||
2019 | total: 28 | elevation | estimate = 0.36 ± 0.19, t = 1.93, p = 0.065 | estimate = 0.26 ± 0.17, t = 1.54, p = 0.137 |
low: 6 | ||||
high: 22 | Cortf (pg mm–1) | estimate = −4.3 × 10−3 ± 0.01, t = −0.37, p = 0.712 | estimate = −4.6 × 10−3 ± 0.01, t = −0.46 p = 0.653 | |
total trial number | NA | estimate = −1.2 × 10−3 ± 3.4 × −4, t = −3.64, p = 0.001 | ||
Adj. R2 | 0.13 | 0.46 | ||
2020 | total: 45 | elevation | estimate = 0.05 ± 0.12, t = 0.37, p = 0.708 | estimate = −3.0 × 19−3 ± 0.11, t = −0.03, p = 0.980 |
low: 26 | ||||
high: 19 | Cortf (pg mm–1) | estimate = −6.1×10−3 ± 0.01, t = −0.48, p = 0.635 | ||
estimate = −0.01 ± 0.01, t = −0.17, p = 0.169 | ||||
total trial number | NA | estimate = −1.4 × 10−−3 ± 2.4 × 10−4, t = −5.89, p < 0.001 | ||
Adj. R2 | 0.01 | 0.47 |
Figure 1.
Performance on the spatial learning and memory task, showing mean location errors per trial (a–c) during the first 20 trials and (d–f) over the entire task in relation to Cortf across the 3 years of the study, with high elevation marked in green and low elevation marked in orange.
There were no significant associations between performance in the entire spatial learning and memory task and Cortf, but there was a significant year effect and significant Cortf by year interaction (table 1). Post hoc analyses showed no statistically significant differences in performance across the entire task between any years (2018|2019: p = 0.89; 2018|2020: p = 0.92; 2019|2020: p = 0.99). There were also no statistically significant associations between the number of location errors per trial over the entire task and Cortf within each of the 3 years when run in separate independent models (table 1; figure 1d–f).
There were no significant associations between performance either in the first 20 trials of the spatial learning and memory task or during the entire task and mean feather bar width (electronic supplementary material, table S3). Variation in feather length and mass similarly was not associated with differences in cognitive performance (electronic supplementary material, table S3).
(c) . Spatial reversal learning
There were no significant associations between Cortf and performance (mean number of errors per trial) on a single reversal task performance in the first 20 trials (table 1; figure 2a–c) or during the entire task (table 1, figure 2d–f).
Figure 2.
Performance on the single reversal spatial learning and memory task, showing mean location errors per trial (a–c) during the first 20 trials and (d–f) over the entire task in relation to Cortf across the 3 years of the study, with high elevation marked in green and low elevation marked in orange.
There was no significant association between performance in the first 20 trials of the single reversal learning task and mean feather growth bar width, but there was a significant elevation by year interaction (electronic supplementary material, table S3). Post hoc analyses revealed no significant differences between elevations (p = 0.81) or between years (2018|2019: p = 0.18; 2018|2020: p = 0.58; 2019|2020: p = 0.20) despite the overall results of the model.
There was no significant association between mean feather bar width and performance over the entire spatial reversal learning task (electronic supplementary material, table S3). Variation in feather mass and length was similarly not associated with differences in performance in a spatial reversal learning task (electronic supplementary material, tables S3 and S4).
(d) . Population-level year differences in fledgling condition
Mean fledgling mass was not significantly different between elevations but varied significantly across years and the interaction between elevation and year was statistically significant (electronic supplementary material, table S5). Post hoc analyses confirmed that there were no significant differences between elevations (p = 0.82) but there was a significant difference across years with 2020 associated with significantly lower mass compared to 2018 (p < 0.001) and 2019 (p < 0.001). There were no significant differences in mass between 2018 and 2019 (p = 0.14).
The variation of nestling mass within nests (CV) also varied significantly across years but not elevations (electronic supplementary material, table S5). Post hoc analyses showed that the CV of fledgling mass was higher in 2020 compared to both 2018 (p = 0.005) and 2019 (p = 0.012) but there were no significant differences between 2018 and 2019 (p = 0.96).
4. Discussion
Overall, we found limited support for the hypothesis that the condition during development of wild mountain chickadees results in impaired spatial cognitive abilities. We only detected a significant negative association between spatial cognitive ability and feather Cortf in the initial memory acquisition phase (first 20 trials) during 1 year of the 3-year study. At the same time, we did not detect a significant association between developmental Cortf and spatial cognitive ability in the memory retention phase (performance across entire testing period) during any of the 3 years. As the first 20 trials reflect the memory acquisition phase, while performance over the entire task represents longer term memory ability [21], our results suggest that elevated developmental Cortf may have limited effects on memory acquisition, but no detectable effects on longer term memory ability. However, as this effect only occurred in a single year this result may be due to chance. We did not detect differences between Cortf and performance on the single spatial reversal learning task during any year of the study. It is possible that individuals with worse developmental conditions died before our sampling—if this were the case, our results would only be limited to individuals that survived at least a month after fledging and to the age where spatial abilities are relevant to their survival. Although, we did not detect a relationship between Cortf and sampling date, which argues against the idea that birds with higher Cortf may die earlier in the post-fledging period. Overall, our results are likely reflective of the relevant naturally existing variation in developmental condition.
Across years, there were significant differences in the total Cortf: mean Cortf was highest in the 2018 cohort, followed by the 2019 cohort and finally lowest in the 2020 cohort. Despite the detected mean yearly differences, each year was associated with large individual variation. Feather growth, mass and length also varied across years but only 2019 had significantly larger and faster growing feathers compared to both 2018 and 2020. We detected no significant associations between feather growth rates or feather mass and any measure of spatial cognitive performance.
There were no significant differences in cognitive performance across years on either the spatial learning and memory or the spatial reversal tasks. The only exception was a small difference among years in spatial learning and memory performance, but only in models with feather length and mass (see electronic supplemental materials). This disparity is likely to bedue to a slight difference in sample size as not all feathers survived the hormone extraction process. These models showed cognitive performance in 2018 (year with the highest Cortf) was worse than 2019 but statistically similar to that in 2020 (the year with the lowest Cortf). Thus, these results corroborate our conclusions that developmental conditions had only a limited effect on spatial cognitive abilities.
There were significant differences in the reproductive parameters among years. Chickadees fledged the lightest massed nestlings in 2020 compared to both 2018 and 2019. The within brood variation in nestling mass was significantly larger in 2020 compared to both 2018 and 2019. All these results suggest that 2020 was associated with lower quality developmental conditions. However, feather Cortf in 2020 was the lowest among the 3 years of study. These data suggest that there were no broad associations between developmental conditions (as indicated by fledgling body mass and CV of fledgling mass within a nest) and feather Cortf across years. One explanation for this mismatch is that the pattern seen between years in reproductive output may only be evident at the population level and not the level of the individual. Additionally, there were no striking differences in cognitive performance between years despite large differences in overall developmental conditions.
Despite strong laboratory-based evidence that perturbations during early development can lead to negative impacts on cognitive abilities (e.g. [2,6,7,17]), we did not find strong support for this pattern in wild food-caching mountain chickadees tested in their natural environment. This may partly be driven by the inability of laboratory animals to respond to such effects in the absence of resources from natural environments [2]. The basic needs of captive animals are met in the laboratory but if there are compensatory mechanisms mediated via resources only available in natural settings, this may inhibit animals from mounting a compensatory response [2].
For example, it has been shown that the amino sulfonic acid, taurine can rescue neural tissue and related cognitive functions from the negative effects of environmental stressors [49–52]. Taurine is found ubiquitously in nature and while most organisms can synthesize their own source, some avian species may lack this ability during early development and can only gain taurine by consuming certain food sources. For example, spiders contain 40–100 times more taurine than other common arthropod food resources and are a common component of chickadee diets [53]. Past work has shown that chickadees and close relatives seek out spiders to consume and feed to developing nestlings [54,55] and that experimental supplementation of taurine results in rescued and in some cases better spatial cognitive abilities [49,50,54]. Thus, adult chickadees may seek out spider prey to buffer against the negative impacts of developmental stress on structures like the hippocampus, preserving future spatial cognitive function. As a result, additional taurine in the diet may counteract the negative effects of lower daily nutrition due to lower food availability [51,52]. Laboratory environments restrict parental behaviors such as selective feeding, as they lack diverse resources, which may explain the consistent negative effects of increased developmental stress on cognitive function [2,3].
Animals in the laboratory are also severely limited in their opportunities to use cognition. This is especially true for spatial cognition—captive environments are well known to result in reduced hippocampal volume [56,57]. It is possible that wild animals in their natural environment may be able to counter negative effects of developmental stress on spatial cognitive abilities through extensive use of such abilities later in life [3]. Food-caching birds cache tens of thousands of individual food caches every autumn and use spatial memory to recover these caches [18]. It is possible that this considerable daily use of spatial cognition will compensate or rescue cognitive function from some developmental perturbations [2].
The use of Cortf to evaluate nestling condition was established by Bortolotti and colleagues [26,27]. Although, the use of this method for examining short-term stressors has been discouraged [58], our use of Cortf as an index of nesting conditions aligns with several other studies that focus on tradeoffs during early development [30–32]. However, this method does not allow for a temporal understanding of Cort deposition into the feather tissue. A bird with higher Cortf compared to others in its cohort may have initially experienced significant perturbations that then declined throughout the rest of development. Thus, some individuals with higher Cortf may have had the ability to recover from this exposure. It is also possible that the detected levels of Cortf were indicative of system-wide Cort concentrations that were below a harmful threshold. Additionally, there have been some studies showing that poor developmental conditions result in lower levels of glucorticoid hormones [59,60], and that higher levels may be more indicative of a normal function [61]. We did see our predicted relationship between higher levels of hormone and worse memory performance but only in a single year and elevation. This result does not remove the possibility that in natural populations higher Cort may be related to more ideal conditions but in either case, variation in developmental conditions in our study is likely representative of naturally present and rather large variation across multiple years in our population.
As the survival of young songbirds during the post-fledging period is very low [42], and our study only includes birds that survived at least a few months after fledgling, our sample may be biased towards higher-quality individuals that received lower exposure to developmental harm. On the other hand, laboratory-based studies showed negative effects of rather small differences in developmental stress associated with limited variation in nutrition [10,11]. If young birds with worse developmental condition indeed die earlier, we would expect samples collected earlier in the autumn to contain higher Cortf. Yet we detected no such pattern. This suggests that birds with higher levels were not being selected out of the population more quickly during the post-fledging period compared to those with lower levels. Also, if the individuals with worse developmental conditions die soon after fledging, their mortality could not be associated with worse spatial cognitive abilities as these are impossible to sample that early in development in a wild population. This study was completely dependent on individuals that not only survived to fledging but also survived long enough to participate in the cognitive tasks later in the winter.
Overall, our results show that mountain chickadees in their natural environment do not exhibit strong negative effects associated with large natural variation in developmental condition. This may be due to evolved mechanisms allowing them to protect their cognitive abilities during development [2]. However, the nature of such potential mechanisms remains unclear. While future work should focus on untangling such potential mechanisms, this study shows that wild animals in their natural environment may be largely unaffected by naturally existing variation in developmental condition, which is in direct contrast to a large body of laboratory-based research. Our results suggest that individual cognitive variation in our population is probably not strongly influenced by differences in naturally existing developmental conditions. This emphasizes the importance of investigating basic biological processes in wild animals in their natural environments, as previous results gathered in captive environments may not accurately reflect inherent abilities of these populations.
Acknowledgements
Constructive comments from three anonymous reviewers and an associate editor significantly improved the manuscript.
Ethics
All work was in accordance with the University of Nevada Reno Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (Protocol 00818, 00046 and 00603), California Department of Fish, and Wildlife Permit D-0011776516-4.
Data accessibility
The data are provided in electronic supplementary material [62].
Authors' contributions
B.R.S.: conceptualization, data curation, formal analysis, funding acquisition, resources, writing—original draft, writing—review and editing; V.K.H.: data curation, investigation, writing—review and editing; A.M.P.: data curation, investigation, writing—review and editing; L.M.B.: data curation, investigation, writing—review and editing; C.L.B.: data curation, investigation, writing—review and editing; E.S.B.: funding acquisition, methodology, resources, writing—review and editing; J.Q.O.: conceptualization, methodology, resources, writing—review and editing; V.V.P.: conceptualization, data curation, formal analysis, funding acquisition, investigation, methodology, resources, writing—review and editing.
All authors gave final approval for publication and agreed to be held accountable for the work performed therein.
Conflict of interest declaration
We declare we have no competing interests.
Funding
This research was supported by the National Science Foundation (NSF) grants OS-1856181 and IOS2119824 to V.V.P. and by the National Institute of Health grant no. R15E2030548 to J.Q.O. The NSF Graduate Research Fellowship Program (Fellow IDs 2019287870 and 2020305313, respectively) supported B.R.S. and L.M.B. Additional materials grants from the University of Nevada's Graduate Student Association and the Western Field Ornithologists allowed for adequate funding.
References
- 1.Boogert NJ, Madden JR, Morand-Ferron J, Thornton A. 2018. Measuring and understanding individual differences in cognition. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 373, 20170280. ( 10.1098/rstb.2017.0280) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Buchanan KL, Grindstaff JL, Pravosudov VV. 2013. Condition dependence, developmental plasticity, and cognition: implications for ecology and evolution. Trends Ecol. Evol. 28, 290-296. ( 10.1016/j.tree.2013.02.004) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Shettleworth SJ. 2009. Cognition, evolution, and behavior. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Croston R, Branch CL, Kozlovsky DY, Dukas R, Pravosudov VV. 2015. Heritability and the evolution of cognitive traits. Behav. Ecol. 26, 1447-1459. ( 10.1093/beheco/arv088) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Branch CL, Semenov GA, Wagner DN, Sonnenberg BR, Pitera AM, Bridge ES, Taylor SA, Pravosudov VV. 2022. The genetic basis of spatial cognitive variation in a food-caching bird. Curr. Biol. 32, 210-219.e4. ( 10.1016/j.cub.2021.10.036) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Hedges DW, Woon FL. 2011. Early-life stress and cognitive outcome. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 214, 121-130. ( 10.1007/s00213-010-2090-6) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Naninck EFG, Hoeijmakers L, Kakava-Georgiadou N, Meesters A, Lazic SE, Lucassen PJ, Korosi A. 2015. Chronic early life stress alters developmental and adult neurogenesis and impairs cognitive function in mice. Hippocampus 25, 309-328. ( 10.1002/hipo.22374) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Schoech SJ, Rensel MA, Heiss RS. 2011. Short- and long-term effects of developmental corticosterone exposure on avian physiology, behavioral phenotype, cognition, and fitness: a review. Cur. Zool. 57, 514-530. ( 10.1093/czoolo/57.4.514) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 9.McEwen BS. 2002. Sex, stress and the hippocampus: allostasis, allostatic load and the aging process. Neurobiol. Aging 23, 921-939. ( 10.1016/S0197-4580(02)00027-1) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Pravosudov VV, Lavenex P, Omanska A. 2005. Nutritional deficits during early development affect hippocampal structure and spatial memory later in life. Behav. Neurosci. 119, 1368. ( 10.1037/0735-7044.119.5.1368) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Pravosudov VV, Kitaysky AS. 2006. Effects of nutritional restrictions during post-hatching development on adrenocortical function in western scrub-jays (Aphelocoma californica). Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 145, 25-31. ( 10.1016/j.ygcen.2005.06.011) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Sapolsky RM, Uno H, Rebert CS, Finch CE. 1990. Hippocampal damage associated with prolonged glucocorticoid exposure in primates. J. Neurosci. 10, 2897-2902. ( 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.10-09-02897.1990) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Guidi J, Lucente M, Sonino N, Fava GA. 2021. Allostatic load and its impact on health: a systematic review. Psychother. Psychosom. 90, 11-27. ( 10.1159/000510696) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.McEwen BS, Wingfield JC. 2010. What is in a name? Integrating homeostasis, allostasis and stress. Horm. Behav. 57, 105-111. ( 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2009.09.011) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Romero LM. 2004. Physiological stress in ecology: lessons from biomedical research. Trends Ecol. Evol. 19, 249-255. ( 10.1016/j.tree.2004.03.008) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Vitousek MN, Taff CC, Ryan TA, Zimmer C. 2019. Stress resilience and the dynamic regulation of glucocorticoids. Integr. Comp. Biol. 59, 251-263. ( 10.1093/icb/icz087) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Eyck HJF, Buchanan KL, Crino OL, Jessop TS. 2019. Effects of developmental stress on animal phenotype and performance: a quantitative review. Biol. Rev. 94, 1143-1160. ( 10.1111/brv.12496) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Pravosudov VV, Roth TC II. 2013. Cognitive ecology of food hoarding: the evolution of spatial memory and the hippocampus. Ann. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 44, 173-193. ( 10.1146/annurev-ecolsys-110512-135904) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Branch CL, Pitera AM, Kozlovsky DY, Sonnenberg BR, Benedict LM, Pravosudov VV. 2019a. Elevation-related differences in the age structure of breeding birds suggest stronger selection at harsher elevations. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 73, 143. ( 10.1007/s00265-019-2750-4) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Roth TC, LaDage LD, Freas CA, Pravosudov VV. 2012. Variation in memory and the hippocampus across populations from different climates: a common garden approach. Proc. R. Soc. B 279, 402-410. ( 10.1098/rspb.2011.1020) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Sonnenberg BR, Branch CL, Pitera AM, Bridge E, Pravosudov VV. 2019. Natural selection and spatial cognition in wild food-caching Mountain Chickadees. Curr. Biol. 29, 670-676.e3. ( 10.1016/j.cub.2019.01.006) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Freas CA, LaDage LD, Roth TC, Pravosudov VV. 2012. Elevation-related differences in memory and the hippocampus in mountain chickadees, Poecile gambeli. Anim. Behav. 84, 121-127. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.04.018) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Freas CA, Roth TC II, LaDage LD, Pravosudov VV. 2013. Hippocampal neuron soma size is associated with population differences in winter climate severity in food-caching chickadees. Funct. Ecol. 27, 1341-1349. ( 10.1111/1365-2435.12125) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 24.McCallum DA, Grundel R, Dahlsten DL. 2020. Mountain chickadee (Poecile gambeli), version 1.0. Birds of the World. See https://birdsoftheworld.org/bow/species/mouchi.
- 25.Croston R, Kozlovsky DY, Branch CL, Parchman TL, Bridge ES, Pravosudov VV. 2016. Individual variation in spatial memory performance in wild mountain chickadees from different elevations. Anim. Behav. 111, 225-234. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.10.015) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Bortolotti GR, Marchant TA, Blas J, German T. 2008. Corticosterone in feathers is a long-term, integrated measure of avian stress physiology. Funct. Ecol. 22, 494-500. ( 10.1111/j.1365-2435.2008.01387.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Bortolotti GR, Marchant T, Blas J, Cabezas S. 2009. Tracking stress: localisation, deposition and stability of corticosterone in feathers. J. Exp. Biol. 212, 1477-1482. ( 10.1242/jeb.022152) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Grubb TCG Jr. 2006. Ptilochronology: feather time and the biology of birds. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 29.Jenni-Eiermann S, Helfenstein F, Vallat A, Glauser G, Jenni L. 2015. Corticosterone: effects on feather quality and deposition into feathers. Methods Ecol. Evol. 6, 237-246. ( 10.1111/2041-210X.12314) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Jenni-Eiermann S, Olano Marin J, Bize P. 2022. Variation in feather corticosterone levels in Alpine swift nestlings provides support for the hypo-responsive hypothesis. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 316, 113946. ( 10.1016/j.ygcen.2021.113946) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Will AP, Suzuki Y, Elliott KH, Hatch SA, Watanuki Y, Kitaysky AS. 2014. Feather corticosterone reveals developmental stress in seabirds. J. Exp. Biol. 217, 2371-2376. ( 10.1242/jeb.098533) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Johns DW, Marchant TA, Fairhurst GD, Speakman JR, Clark RG. 2018. Biomarker of burden: feather corticosterone reflects energetic expenditure and allostatic overload in captive waterfowl. Funct. Ecol. 32, 345-357. ( 10.1111/1365-2435.12988) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Croston R, Branch CL, Pitera AM, Kozlovsky DY, Bridge ES, Parchman TL, Pravosudov VV. 2017. Predictably harsh environment is associated with reduced cognitive flexibility in wild food-caching mountain chickadees. Anim. Behav. 123, 139-149. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2016.10.004) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Tello-Ramos MC, Branch CL, Pitera AM, Kozlovsky DY, Bridge ES, Pravosudov VV. 2018. Memory in wild mountain chickadees from different elevations: comparing first-year birds with older survivors. Anim. Behav. 137, 149-160. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2017.12.019) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Heinen VK, Pitera AM, Sonnenberg BR, Benedict LM, Branch CL, Bridge ES, Pravosudov VV. 2021. Specialized spatial cognition is associated with reduced cognitive senescence in a food-caching bird. Proc. R. Soc. B 288, 20203180. ( 10.1098/rspb.2020.3180) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Kozlovsky DY, Branch CL, Pitera AM, Pravosudov VV. 2018. Fluctuations in annual climatic extremes are associated with reproductive variation in resident mountain chickadees. R. Soc. Open Sci. 5, 171604. ( 10.1098/rsos.171604) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Pyle P. 1997. Identification guide to North American birds: Columbidae to Ploceidae. Point Reyes Station, CA: Slate Creek Press. [Google Scholar]
- 38.Bridge ES, et al. 2019. An Arduino-based RFID platform for animal research. Front. Ecol. Evol. 7, 257. ( 10.3389/fevo.2019.00257) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Branch CL, Pitera AM, Kozlovsky DY, Bridge ES, Pravosudov VV. 2019. Smart is the new sexy: female mountain chickadees increase reproductive investment when mated to males with better spatial cognition. Ecol. Lett. 22, 897-903. ( 10.1111/ele.13249) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Schneider CA, Rasband WS, Eliceiri KW. 2012. NIH Image to ImageJ: 25 years of image analysis. Nat. Methods 9, 671-675. ( 10.1038/nmeth.2089) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Grant AR, Baldan D, Kimball MG, Malisch JL, Ouyang JQ. 2020. Across time and space: hormonal variation across temporal and spatial scales in relation to nesting success. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 292, 113462. ( 10.1016/j.ygcen.2020.113462) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Rodríguez S, van Noordwijk AJ, Álvarez E, Barba E. 2016. A recipe for postfledging survival in great tits Parus major: be large and be early (but not too much). Ecol. Evol. 6, 4458-4467. ( 10.1002/ece3.2192) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.R Core Team. 2021. R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. [Google Scholar]
- 44.Wickham H, et al. 2019. Welcome to the tidyverse. J. Open Source Softw. 4, 1686. ( 10.21105/joss.01686) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Hartig F. 2021. DHARMa: residual diagnostics for hierarchical (multi-level / mixed) regression models. See https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=DHARMa.
- 46.Lenth RV. 2021. Emmeans: estimated marginal means, aka least-squares means. See https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=emmeans.
- 47.Brooks ME, Kristensen K, Benthem KJv, Magnusson A, Berg CW, Nielsen A, Skaug HJ, Maechler M, Bolker BM. 2017. GlmmTMB balances speed and flexibility among packages for zero-inflated generalized linear mixed modeling. R J. 9, 378-400. ( 10.32614/RJ-2017-066) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Joe H, Zhu R. 2005. Generalized Poisson distribution: the property of mixture of Poisson and comparison with negative binomial distribution. Biom. J. 47, 219-229. ( 10.1002/bimj.200410102) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Jangra A, Rajput P, Dwivedi DK, Lahkar M. 2020. Amelioration of repeated restraint stress-induced behavioral deficits and hippocampal anomalies with taurine treatment in mice. Neurochem. Res. 45, 731-740. ( 10.1007/s11064-019-02945-8) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Jia N, Sun Q, Su Q, Dang S, Chen G. 2016. Taurine promotes cognitive function in prenatally stressed juvenile rats via activating the Akt-CREB-PGC1α pathway. Redox Biol. 10, 179-190. ( 10.1016/j.redox.2016.10.004) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Kumari N, Prentice H, Wu J-Y. 2013. Taurine and its neuroprotective role. In Taurine 8 (eds El Idrissi A, L'Amoreaux WJ), pp. 19-27. Berlin, Germany: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- 52.Pasantes-Morales H, Hernández-Benítez R. 2010. Taurine and brain development: trophic or cytoprotective actions? Neurochem. Res. 35, 1939-1943. ( 10.1007/s11064-010-0262-8) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Ramsay SL, Houston DC. 2003. Amino acid composition of some woodland arthropods and its implications for breeding tits and other passerines. Ibis 145, 227-232. ( 10.1046/j.1474-919X.2003.00133.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Arnold KE, Ramsay SL, Donaldson C, Adam A. 2007. Parental prey selection affects risk-taking behaviour and spatial learning in avian offspring. Proc. R. Soc. B 274, 2563-2569. ( 10.1098/rspb.2007.0687) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.van Oers K, Kohn GM, Hinde CA, Naguib M. 2015. Parental food provisioning is related to nestling stress response in wild great tit nestlings: Implications for the development of personality. Front. Zool. 12, S10. ( 10.1186/s12983-015-0102-4) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.LaDage LD, Roth TC II, Fox RA, Pravosudov VV. 2009. Effects of captivity and memory-based experiences on the hippocampus in mountain chickadees. Behav. Neurosci. 123, 284-291. ( 10.1037/a0014817) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Smulders TV, Sasson AD, DeVoogd TJ. 1995. Seasonal variation in hippocampal volume in a food-storing bird, the black-capped chickadee. J. Neurobiol. 27, 15-25. ( 10.1002/neu.480270103) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Romero LM, Fairhurst GD. 2016. Measuring corticosterone in feathers: strengths, limitations, and suggestions for the future. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. A: Mol. Integr. Physiol. 202, 112-122. ( 10.1016/j.cbpa.2016.05.002) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Heim C, Ehlert U, Hellhammer DH. 2000. The potential role of hypocortisolism in the pathophysiology of stress-related bodily disorders. Psychoneuroendocrinology 25, 1-35. ( 10.1016/S0306-4530(99)00035-9) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Badanes LS, Watamura SE, Hankin BL. 2011. Hypocortisolism as a potential marker of allostatic load in children: associations with family risk and internalizing disorders. Dev. Psychopathol 23, 881-896. ( 10.1017/S095457941100037X) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Patterson AGL, Kitaysky AS, Lyons DE, Roby DD. 2015. Nutritional stress affects corticosterone deposition in feathers of Caspian tern chicks. J. Avian Biol. 46, 18-24. ( 10.1111/jav.00397) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Sonnenberg BR, Heinen VK, Pitera AM, Benedict LM, Branch CL, Bridge ES, Ouyang JQ, Pravosudov VV. 2022. Data from: Natural variation in developmental condition has limited effect on spatial cognition in a wild food-caching bird. Figshare. ( 10.6084/m9.figshare.c.6204497) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Citations
- Sonnenberg BR, Heinen VK, Pitera AM, Benedict LM, Branch CL, Bridge ES, Ouyang JQ, Pravosudov VV. 2022. Data from: Natural variation in developmental condition has limited effect on spatial cognition in a wild food-caching bird. Figshare. ( 10.6084/m9.figshare.c.6204497) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
Data Availability Statement
The data are provided in electronic supplementary material [62].