Skip to main content
PLOS Neglected Tropical Diseases logoLink to PLOS Neglected Tropical Diseases
. 2022 Sep 23;16(9):e0010722. doi: 10.1371/journal.pntd.0010722

Impact of Shigella infections and inflammation early in life on child growth and school-aged cognitive outcomes: Findings from three birth cohorts over eight years

Elizabeth T Rogawski McQuade 1,*, Rebecca J Scharf 2, Erling Svensen 3, Amber Huggins 2, Angelina Maphula 4, Eliwaza Bayo 5, Ladislaus Blacy 5, Paula Pamplona E de Souza 6, Hilda Costa 6, Eric R Houpt 7, Pascal O Bessong 4, Estomih R Mduma 5, Aldo A M Lima 6, Richard L Guerrant 7
Editor: Abiola Senok8
PMCID: PMC9534434  PMID: 36149931

Abstract

Background

Shigella infections cause inflammation, which has been hypothesized to mediate the associations between Shigella and child development outcomes among children in low-resource settings. We aimed to assess whether early life inflammation and Shigella infections affect school-aged growth and cognitive outcomes from 6–8 years of age.

Methodology/principal findings

We conducted follow-up assessments of anthropometry, reasoning skills, and verbal fluency in 451 children at 6–8 years of age in the Brazil, Tanzania, and South Africa sites of MAL-ED, a longitudinal birth cohort study. We estimated the associations between Shigella burden and inflammation with linear growth at 2, 5, and 6–8 years of age, and with the cognitive test scores using linear regression and adjusting for potential confounding variables. We also assessed whether inflammation mediated the associations between Shigella and school-aged outcomes using a regression-based approach to mediation analysis. A high prevalence of Shigella was associated with a 0.32 (95% CI: 0.08, 0.56) z-score lower height-for-age z-score (HAZ) at 6–8 years compared to a low prevalence of Shigella. Intestinal inflammation had a smaller association with HAZ at 6–8 years. Shigella burden had small and consistently negative associations with cognitive outcomes in Brazil and Tanzania, but not South Africa, and the estimates were not statistically significant. Systemic inflammation was strongly associated with lower verbal fluency scores in Brazil (semantic fluency z-score difference: -0.57, 95% CI: -1.05, -0.10; phonemic fluency z-score difference: -0.48, 95% CI: -0.93, -0.03). There was no evidence that intestinal inflammation mediated the association between Shigella and HAZ or cognitive outcomes.

Conclusions/significance

While Shigella infections were consistently associated with long-term deficits in linear growth, the estimates of the negative associations between Shigella and cognitive outcomes were imprecise and only observed in the Brazil and Tanzania sites. Systemic inflammation was strongly associated with lower semantic and phonemic fluency scores in Brazil only, highlighting the site-specificity of effects.

Author summary

Shigella infections are common among children in low-resource settings and cause inflammation, which may contribute to poor child development outcomes. We studied children from birth to 6–8 years of age in Brazil, Tanzania, and South Africa to assess whether Shigella infections and markers of inflammation in the first 2 years of life were associated with child development outcomes at school age, including height and cognitive assessment scores. Shigella infections were consistently associated with long-term deficits in linear growth. However, the associations between Shigella and cognitive outcomes were smaller, not statistically significant, and inconsistent across the three sites. There was no evidence that inflammation caused by Shigella explained the associations between Shigella and school-aged outcomes. Systemic inflammation was strongly associated with lower cognitive assessment scores in Brazil but not in the other sites, highlighting the differences in determinants of child development across settings. Quantifying the impact and understanding mechanisms for the effect of early life exposures to Shigella on long-term cognitive outcomes remains challenging.

Introduction

Intestinal inflammation is one of the defining components of environmental enteric dysfunction (EED), a subclinical condition of the gut that is also characterized by dysfunction in the intestinal epithelial barrier, reduced gut immune function, and malabsorption [1]. EED is common among children in low-resource settings and results from chronic environmental exposure to enteric pathogens through inadequate access to safe water and sanitation [2,3]. Several enteric pathogens directly cause inflammation, most notably Shigella, which secretes virulence factors that have enterotoxic activity and allow Shigella to invade the colonic epithelium [4]. Shigella-associated inflammation has been observed even in the absence of other clinical symptoms. Shigella detections in both diarrheal and non-diarrheal stools in the first two years of life were associated with increases in a biomarker of intestinal inflammation, myeloperoxidase (MPO), in the 8-site MAL-ED birth cohort study. The association depended on Shigella quantity, such that MPO levels were more highly elevated as the quantity of Shigella increased [5].

Both enteric pathogens and inflammation have been associated with linear growth faltering among children in low-resource settings. Bacterial and protozoal pathogens were associated with larger decrements in length than viral pathogens in MAL-ED. Compared to other pathogens, Shigella was associated with the largest decrements in length-for-age z-score at 2 years of age per log increase in the mean pathogen quantity detected in non-diarrheal stools from 0–2 years [6]. Furthermore, Shigella was the only pathogen for which the association with length persisted to 5 years of age, suggesting uniquely sustained impact. Inflammation, as measured by fecal MPO and serum alpha-1-acid glycoprotein (AGP), a marker of systemic inflammation, was associated with reduced linear growth in this study [7], and models suggested that the associations between invasive bacteria and growth were mediated by inflammation [8].

Linear growth is commonly used as a proxy for cognitive function [9], arguably a more public health-relevant outcome, since anthropometry is easier to measure and requires shorter-term follow-up. However, while some mechanistic pathways for effects on growth and cognition may be shared, there could be independent effects of inflammation on cognitive function [10], though the evidence is mixed [11]. Inflammation particularly affects brain development in children [12], and is thought to cause brain damage through the direct effects of inflammatory cytokines [13] and inflammation-related proteins [14,15]. In support of these pathways, fever and biomarkers of inflammation during infancy were associated with reductions in cognitive assessment scores in Dhaka, Bangladesh [16]. Total enteric pathogen burden was weakly associated with cognitive outcomes at 2 years of age [17], and was not significantly associated with cognition at 5 years of age in MAL-ED [18]. However, cognitive effects are likely to be pathogen-specific, as were effects on linear growth [6], given major differences in pathogenesis.

To directly assess whether early life inflammation and exposures to the inflammatory pathogen, Shigella, affect long-term child development, we conducted a follow-up study of children at three sites of the MAL-ED birth cohort study to assess impact on linear growth and school-aged cognitive outcomes from 6–8 years of age.

Methods

Ethics statement

The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board for Health Sciences Research, University of Virginia, USA (HSR IRB #14595) as well as the respective governmental, local institutional, and collaborating institutional ethical review boards at each site: Committee for Ethics in Research, Universidade Federal do Ceara; National Ethical Research Committee, Health Ministry, Council of National Health (Brazil); Health, Safety and Research Ethics Committee, University of Venda; Department of Health and Social Development, Limpopo Provincial Government (South Africa); Medical Research Coordinating Committee, National Institute for Medical Research; Chief Medical Officer, Ministry of Health and Social Welfare (Tanzania). Informed written consent was obtained from the parent or guardian of each participating child on their behalf.

The study design and methods for the first two years of follow-up in the MAL-ED study have been previously described in detail [19]. Briefly, children were enrolled within the first two weeks of life and followed twice weekly through active surveillance at home visits for illnesses and feeding practices until two years of age. Anthropometry was measured monthly, and stools were collected monthly in the absence of diarrhea and whenever a diarrhea episode was identified. Enteric pathogens were detected in stool samples using quantitative PCR with the Taqman Array Card platform, as previously described [20]. Shigella was defined using the ipaH target as previously [5]. Myeloperoxidase (MPO; log [ng/ml]), a marker of neutrophil activation and infiltration, was measured in stools collected monthly in the first year of life and quarterly in the second year of life [7,21] α-1-acid glycoprotein (AGP), a marker of systemic inflammation, was measured in serum at 7, 15, and 24 months of age.

Active surveillance completed at 2 years of age, but additional follow-up visits for anthropometry and cognitive assessments were conducted at 5 years of age at all sites and at 6–8 years of age in 3 sites. At 6–8 years of age, cognitive assessments were performed to assess reasoning skills using the Raven Coloured Progressive Matrices (RCM) [22,23], and verbal fluency using a component of the NEPSY Neuropsychological Assessment [24]. Broadly, these tests were chosen to capture executive function, which is best measured once children are school-aged and is most relevant to adult functioning and human capital.

The RCM is a visual, non-language-based assessment that is applicable across languages and cultures since it tests the ability to solve a series of puzzles using analogies. RCM was chosen for children at 6–8 years because this measure was previously used among mothers in MAL-ED [25], is appropriate when access to formal schooling is limited, and is language-independent. The assessment was piloted among children who were not in the MAL-ED cohort at all three sites. RCM was acceptable and provided a range of scores, suggesting it was able to disaggregate cognitive ability in these children. The assessment was administered to study children by trained assessors in a quiet and distraction-free location.

Based on the NEPSY, semantic fluency was assessed by asking the children to name as many animals as they could in one minute, and then to name as many foods as they could in one minute. Phonemic fluency was assessed by asking them to name as many words that began with S as they could in one minute, and then to name as many words that begin with F as they could in one minute. This brief assessment has been previously used at our research site in Brazil, where childhood diarrhea burden was associated with verbal fluency [26]. This assessment was piloted at all three sites using foods, animals, and words that start with S, N, F, and P. In all languages and across sites, children generated more words that started with S and F; therefore, these letters were included in the assessments of MAL-ED study children.

Statistical analysis

All analyses were conducted among children at the 3 sites in Haydom, Tanzania; Venda, South Africa; and Fortaleza, Brazil in which follow-up cognitive assessments were completed from 6–8 years of age.

We summarized Shigella burden in the first two years of life with a measure of prevalence and quantity, as done previously. [6] Shigella prevalence was defined as the proportion of non-diarrheal stools in which Shigella was detected by quantitative PCR from 0–23 months of age, and Shigella quantity was defined as the mean log-copy numbers per g of stool of Shigella detected in all non-diarrheal stools from 0–23 months. In a sensitivity analysis, we defined Shigella prevalence based on both non-diarrheal and diarrheal stools. To characterize intestinal inflammation, we first accounted for the observed decrease in MPO concentrations with age [21] by regressing log-MPO quantities from all measurements against a continuous variable of age in days. The mean of residuals from this model for each child was then calculated and converted to site-specific z-scores as a summarized metric of intestinal inflammation between birth to 2 years. Systemic inflammation was characterized by the mean log-AGP concentration from all available measurements at 7, 15, and 24 months of age.

Length measurements for children 2 years and younger and height measurements at 5 years of age were converted to length/height-for-age z-scores (LAZ/HAZ) using the 2006 WHO child growth standards [27]. Height measurements at 6–8 years were converted to height-for-age z-scores using the 2007 WHO Growth reference data for 5–19 years [28]. Measurements from the NEPSY were summarized into a measure of semantic fluency by summing the number of words correctly reported for animals and foods, and into a measure of phonemic fluency, by summing the number of words correctly reported that started with F and S. Both scores were converted to site-specific z-scores. Total Raven scores as a measure of reasoning skills were calculated by summing the number of correct answers in each subtest and were converted to site-specific z-scores.

We separately estimated the associations between Shigella burden (prevalence and quantity, defined above) and height-for-age z-score and cognitive outcomes (reasoning skills, semantic fluency, and phonemic fluency) using linear regression adjusting for site, age at the 6–8 year assessment, enrollment weight-for-age z-score (or enrollment LAZ for height outcomes), sex, socioeconomic status, exclusive breastfeeding in the first 6 months, maternal height, and the burden of each of the 12 most prevalent pathogens identified in the first 2 years of life (excluding Shigella). Similar models were used to estimate the associations between intestinal and systemic inflammation, defined above, and each of the outcomes. We also estimated the effects of each exposure (Shigella burden, Shigella quantity, intestinal inflammation, and systemic inflammation) on HAZ measured at 2 and 5 years of age, adjusting for the same variables except age at the 6–8 year assessment. This analysis was restricted to children who were also assessed at 6–8 years so that results could be compared across timepoints among the same children. Effects of Shigella prevalence, intestinal inflammation, and systemic inflammation were scaled to compare the 90th percentile with the 10th percentile of the observed site-specific distributions (high versus low burden), as previously [6]. Effects of Shigella quantity were scaled per 1 log increase in mean quantity.

Finally, we conducted a causal mediation analysis to assess whether associations between Shigella burden and growth and cognitive outcomes were mediated by intestinal inflammation. We used the regression-based approach by Baron and Kenny [29] using the mediation package in R [30]. We first specified a model for intestinal inflammation as the mediator with Shigella prevalence and all covariates included above as predictors. The outcome models included Shigella prevalence, intestinal inflammation, and the covariates above. We then computed the average causal mediation effect (i.e. the magnitude of the effect of Shigella on the outcome that is explained by Shigella’s effect on intestinal inflammation), the average direct effect (i.e. the magnitude of the effect of Shigella that is not explained by Shigella’s effect on intestinal inflammation), and the proportion of the total effect mediated by intestinal inflammation under these models and the sequential ignorability assumption. We conducted similar analyses by site and including Shigella quantity as the exposure.

Results

We analyzed data from 451 children (73.8%) who had school-aged follow-up visits among 611 children who completed the original MAL-ED study with follow-up to 2 years of age. Specifically, we included 117 children (70.1%) in Fortaleza, Brazil, 163 (78.0%) in Haydom, Tanzania, and 171 (72.2%) in Venda, South Africa. There were no differences in sociodemographic characteristics, Shigella burden, inflammation, or anthropometry between the children assessed at 6–8 years of age and all children followed to 2 years (S1 Table). Children in Brazil were younger at the time of the school-aged visit (median age: 6.8 years, IQR: 6.4, 7.2) compared to those in South Africa (8.0, IQR: 7.0, 8.2) and Tanzania (7.7, IQR: 7.3, 8.0; Table 1). Of all 8 sites in the original MAL-ED study, the 3 included sites were at the extremes of the distribution of socioeconomic status. Households in Brazil and South Africa were of the highest socioeconomic status and households in Tanzania were of the lowest.

Table 1. Sociodemographic characteristics, burden of Shigella and inflammation in the first 2 years of life, and growth through 6–8 years of age among 451 children in the Brazil, South Africa, and Tanzania MAL-ED cohorts.

Fortaleza, Brazil
(n = 117)
Venda, South Africa
(n = 171)
Haydom, Tanzania
(n = 163)
All
(n = 451)
Median (IQR)
Sociodemographic characteristics
Age at 6–8 year assessment 6.8 (6.4, 7.2) 8.0 (7.0, 8.2) 7.7 (7.3, 8.0) 7.5 (7.0, 8.0)
Female sex (n; %) 53 (45.3) 84 (49.1) 82 (50.3) 219 (48.6)
Socioeconomic status 0.83 (0.77, 0.90) 0.80 (0.72, 0.85) 0.21 (0.14, 0.28) 0.72 (0.27, 0.84)
Percent days exclusively breastfed < 6 mo. 49 (28, 71) 17 (10, 29) 32 (19, 44) 28 (15, 46)
Maternal education (years) 9 (7, 12) 11 (9, 12) 7 (3, 7) 8 (7, 11)
Maternal height (cm) 155 (150, 160) 158 (155, 162) 156 (152, 160) 157 (152, 161)
Shigella burden
Number of non-diarrheal stools collected 20 (18, 22) 23 (22, 24) 23 (22, 24) 22 (21, 23)
Proportion positive for Shigella 0.05 (0, 0.07) 0.05 (0, 0.11) 0.17 (0.10, 0.23) 0.08 (0.04, 0.16)
Mean quantity of Shigella detected 0.21 (0, 0.4) 0.27 (0, 0.54) 1.00 (0.62, 1.42) 0.42 (0.18, 0.88)
Intestinal inflammation
Number of measurements 10 (8, 11) 13 (11, 14) 12 (10, 13) 12 (10, 13)
Mean MPO concentration (log[ng/mL]) 7.8 (7.4, 8.3) 8.4 (8.2, 8.7) 8.5 (8.3, 8.8) 8.4 (8.0, 8.7)
Systemic inflammation *
Number of measurements 3 (2, 3) 2 (2, 3) 2 (1, 3) 2 (2, 3)
Mean AGP concentration (mg/dL) 98.5 (81.3, 117.0) 128.0 (108.7, 157.0) 115.3 (98.3, 133.6) 115.8 (96.8, 141.3)
Anthropometry
Enrollment weight-for-age z-score -0.17 (-0.77, 0.47) -0.40 (-0.96, 0.14) -0.02 (-0.61, 0.64) -0.20 (-0.83, 0.43)
Enrollment length-for-age z-score -0.72 (-1.31, -0.18) -0.74 (-1.41, -0.18) -0.89 (-1.59, -0.23) -0.79 (-1.44, -0.19)
2 year length-for-age z-score -0.03 (-0.87, 0.73) -1.62 (-2.46, -1.05) -2.59 (-3.22, -1.98) -1.73 (-2.60, -0.74)
5 year height-for-age z-score -0.08 (-0.76, 0.38) -1.05 (-1.56, -0.40) -1.93 (-2.53, -1.24) -1.15 (-1.92, -0.36)
6–8 year height-for-age z-score 0.01 (-0.73, 0.69) -0.07 (-0.69, 0.59) -1.56 (-2.15, -1.03) -0.58 (-1.54, 0.23)
Cognitive outcomes
Number of completed Raven tests 114 165 162 441
Number of completed NEPSY tests 113 161 161 435

* Missing for 7 children in Brazil, 5 children in South Africa, and 11 children in Tanzania

Missing for 5 and 2 children at 5 and 6–8 years, respectively, in Brazil; missing for 2 and 8 children in South Africa; missing for 1 and 1 child in Tanzania.

Absolute scores not reported due to historical abuse.

The median proportion of non-diarrheal stools that were positive for Shigella was 17% in Tanzania compared to 5% in Brazil and South Africa. The biomarkers of intestinal and systemic inflammation were lower in Brazil compared to the other two sites. While anthropometry was similar across sites at enrollment, linear growth faltering was highly common in South Africa and Tanzania by 2 years of age, with median LAZ at 2 years of -1.62 (IQR: -2.46, -1.05) and -2.59 (IQR: -3.22, -1.98) at the two sites, respectively. While there was catch-up growth in height in South Africa (median HAZ at 6–8 years: -0.07, IQR: -0.69, 0.59), there was no catch-up growth in Tanzania (median HAZ at 6–8 years: -1.56, IQR: -2.15, -1.03; Table 1).

Shigella prevalence was consistently associated with decrements in height at 6–8 years (Fig 1A and S2 Table). A high prevalence of Shigella (at the site-specific 90th percentile) was associated with a 0.32 (95% CI: 0.08, 0.56) z-score lower HAZ at 6–8 years compared to a low prevalence of Shigella (at the site-specific 10th percentile). The association was observed in all sites, but was largest in South Africa (HAZ difference: -0.42, 95% CI: -0.79, -0.04). Shigella quantity was similarly associated with lower HAZ at 6–8 years, with a 0.26 (95% CI: 0.06, 0.47) z-score decrement per log increase in average quantity in non-diarrheal stools (Fig 1B and S3 Table). Intestinal inflammation had a smaller negative association with HAZ at 6–8 years (HAZ difference: -0.23, 95% CI: -0.46, -0.01), and in contrast to the association with Shigella, the association between inflammation and HAZ was close to the null in South Africa (Fig 2A and S4 Table). Systemic inflammation was not associated with HAZ at 6–8 years at any site (Fig 2B and S5 Table).

Fig 1.

Fig 1

Associations between Shigella prevalence (A) and quantity (B) in the first 2 years of life with linear growth and cognitive outcomes at 6–8 years of age among 451 children in the Brazil, South Africa, and Tanzania MAL-ED cohorts. Mean Z-score difference estimates and 95% confidence intervals were derived from linear regression models adjusting for site, age at the 6–8 year assessment, enrollment weight-for-age z-score (or enrollment length-for-age z-score for height outcomes), sex, socioeconomic status, exclusive breastfeeding in the first 6 months, maternal height, and the burden of each of the 12 most prevalent pathogens identified in the first 2 years of life (excluding Shigella).

Fig 2.

Fig 2

Associations between intestinal (A) and systemic (B) inflammation in the first 2 years of life with linear growth and cognitive outcomes at 6–8 years of age among 451 children in the Brazil, South Africa, and Tanzania MAL-ED cohorts. Mean Z-score difference estimates and 95% confidence intervals were derived from linear regression models adjusting for site, age at the 6–8 year assessment, enrollment weight-for-age z-score (or enrollment length-for-age z-score for height outcomes), sex, socioeconomic status, exclusive breastfeeding in the first 6 months, maternal height, and the burden of each of the 12 most prevalent pathogens identified in the first 2 years of life (excluding Shigella).

The associations between Shigella prevalence and quantity with height at school-age were consistent with and even larger than associations with earlier height measurements at 2 and 5 years of age (Fig 3 and S2 and S3 Tables). Particularly, the associations between Shigella and HAZ in South Africa were larger at the oldest time point compared to 2 and 5 years. Inconsistencies across sites in the association between intestinal inflammation and height observed at 6–8 years were also observed at the earlier time points and were most extreme at 2 years of age, where high mean MPO was associated with more than half a z-score lower HAZ in Brazil and Tanzania, but higher HAZ in South Africa (LAZ difference: 0.27, 95% CI: -0.08, 0.62; S4 and S5 Tables).

Fig 3.

Fig 3

Associations between Shigella and inflammation in the first 2 years of life with linear growth at 2, 5, and 6–8 years of age among 451 children in the Brazil, South Africa, and Tanzania MAL-ED cohorts. Mean Z-score difference estimates and 95% confidence intervals were derived from linear regression models adjusting for site, enrollment length-for-age z-score, sex, socioeconomic status, exclusive breastfeeding in the first 6 months, maternal height, and the burden of each of the 12 most prevalent pathogens identified in the first 2 years of life (excluding Shigella).

The associations between Shigella and cognitive outcomes at 6–8 years were smaller, consistently negative in Brazil and Tanzania, and close to the null in South Africa (Fig 1 and S2 and S3 Tables). A high prevalence of Shigella and a 1-log increase in Shigella quantity were associated with between -0.1 and -0.2 z-score difference in reasoning skills, semantic fluency, and phonemic fluency scores overall. The largest site-specific effect sizes observed were modest, at approximately -0.3 z-scores, and none of the estimates were statistically significant. The associations were very similar when defining Shigella prevalence with both diarrheal and non-diarrheal stools.

The associations between intestinal inflammation and cognitive outcomes were mixed (Fig 2 and S4 Table). High mean MPO was associated with 0.17 (95% CI: -0.07, 0.41) z-score lower reasoning skills z-scores overall, and the association was largest in Tanzania (z-score difference: -0.36, 95% CI: -0.76, 0.03), though the estimates were not statistically significant. The associations with semantic and phonemic fluency were closer to the null and inconsistent across sites.

In contrast to its null associations with height, systemic inflammation was associated with an almost 0.4 z-score decrement in reasoning skills z-scores in Brazil and Tanzania, though again neither estimate was statistically significant. There was no corresponding association in South Africa (Fig 2 and S5 Table). Systemic inflammation was strongly associated with lower semantic and phonemic fluency scores in Brazil (semantic fluency z-score difference: -0.57, 95% CI: -1.05, -0.10; phonemic fluency z-score difference: -0.48, 95% CI: -0.93, -0.03), but was not associated with these outcomes at the other sites.

There was no evidence that intestinal inflammation mediated the association between Shigella and HAZ or cognitive outcomes (Table 2). The magnitude of the average causal mediation effect of Shigella prevalence through intestinal inflammation was close to zero for all growth and cognitive outcomes, and the proportion of the total effects of Shigella prevalence mediated by intestinal inflammation was less than 10% for all outcomes. The mediation effects did not differ substantially by site or when considering Shigella quantity as the exposure.

Table 2. Mediation effects to assess whether intestinal inflammation mediated the associations between Shigella prevalence and linear growth and cognitive outcomes at 6–8 years of age among 451 children in the Brazil, South Africa, and Tanzania MAL-ED cohorts.

Total effect of Shigella prevalence Average direct effect of Shigella prevalence Average causal mediation effect of Shigella prevalence through intestinal inflammation Proportion of the effect of Shigella prevalence mediated by intestinal inflammation
Z-score difference (95% CI) Proportion (95% CI)
6–8 year height-for-age z-score -0.32 (-0.57, -0.08) -0.31 (-0.55, -0.05) -0.02 (-0.05, 0.00) 0.05 (-0.02, 0.26)
Reasoning skills -0.12 (-0.36, 0.12) -0.11 (-0.35, 0.12) -0.01 (-0.04, 0.01) 0.09 (-0.59, 1.48)
Semantic fluency -0.13 (-0.39, 0.13) -0.13 (-0.39, 0.13) -0.00 (-0.03, 0.02) 0.02 (-0.28, 0.69)
Phonemic fluency -0.11 (-0.34, 0.13) -0.11 (-0.33, 0.13) -0.01 (-0.04, 0.01) 0.07 (-0.54, 0.74)

Discussion

This study provides the first evidence of the associations between early life exposures to Shigella, an inflammatory pathogen, and biomarkers of inflammation on growth and cognitive outcomes at school age. A high burden of Shigella was associated with decrements in height at 6–8 years at all sites. We previously reported a negative association between Shigella and height that was sustained to 5 years of age, in contrast to diminished associations between other pathogens and height at 5 years [6]. The association between Shigella and height was maintained and even increased at 6–8 years. These associations corresponded to small negative associations with Raven and NEPSY cognitive test scores in Brazil and Tanzania only.

Because the presence of an association with linear growth was not necessarily predictive of an association with cognitive outcomes, anthropometric measures, and particularly height, may be limited proxies of cognitive outcomes. In Brazil, systemic inflammation was associated with lower cognitive scores, but not with linear growth faltering, which may suggest that there are pathways between early life exposures and cognitive outcomes that are independent of growth. Alternatively, since the associations with cognitive scores were all close to the null, the cognitive tests conducted in this study may not have adequately disaggregated high and low functioning children in these settings.

Given the diverse multifactorial causes of brain development, it is not surprising that direct links between early life pathogen exposures and cognition are difficult to quantify, especially in settings where resource availability is universally low and exposure to contaminated environments is high. Importantly, Shigella burden was driven by prevalence in non-diarrheal stools, rather than as a cause of diarrhea since 10 or fewer children in each site had Shigella-attributable diarrhea. Shigella-attributable diarrhea alone was not associated with long-term growth or cognitive outcomes in MAL-ED [31], or with linear growth and cognitive scores at 2 years of age in a cohort in Bangladesh [32].

The evidence that inflammation in the absence of illness early in life affected school-aged outcomes was also limited. There was substantial variability among associations with the biomarkers of inflammation across outcomes and sites. Intestinal inflammation was more strongly associated with height than cognitive outcomes, while systemic inflammation showed the reverse. However, the magnitudes of association were small and not statistically significant, and there was substantial heterogeneity by site, with systemic inflammation most clearly associated with poor cognitive outcomes in Brazil. These site-specific results are consistent with studies from the same location in Brazil 20 years ago, which found high diarrhea burdens were associated with deficits in verbal fluency [26,33,34].

Furthermore, despite a hypothesized causal pathway from Shigella to cognitive outcomes through inflammation, we found no evidence that associations between Shigella and outcomes were mediated by inflammation. This is apparent from the discordance in site-specific estimates between Shigella and inflammation on height, for example; Shigella had the strongest associations with decrements in HAZ at 6–8 years of age in South Africa, but inflammation was not associated with HAZ at that site. These results suggest that there may be pathways between Shigella and long-term outcomes that are not mediated by the pro-inflammatory properties of Shigella. Conversely, intestinal myeloperoxidase and serum alpha-1-acid glycoprotein measured at a few time points may inadequately describe inflammation in early life. Importantly, inflammation was not measured during symptomatic diarrhea episodes, and the effects of inflammation with diarrhea need further study.

The 3 sites included in this study represent a wide range of low-resource settings. Children in the Brazil cohort were of relatively high socioeconomic status, had low prevalence of Shigella and intestinal inflammation, and had little linear growth faltering. On the other extreme, children in the Tanzania cohort were of lower socioeconomic status, had a high prevalence of Shigella and intestinal inflammation, and a high and persistent burden of linear growth faltering. Children in South Africa were also of relatively high socioeconomic status but experienced high inflammation and had a high prevalence of stunting at 2 years of age. We appropriately accounted for heterogeneities across sites by considering site-specific z-scores, focusing on comparisons that were relevant to the observed high and low burden in each site, and estimating site-specific associations. However, the variability of results across sites highlights the limited generalizability of the determinants of growth and child development in low-resource settings. Furthermore, because the burden of Shigella was relatively low at two of the three sites studied, the comparisons of high-to-low burden at these sites represent small differences in these populations. Importantly, our study directly measured both cognitive outcomes and components of the hypothesized mechanism for the impact of enteric infections on child development, which improves on using height at 2 years of age as a proxy and leaving causal pathways uninterrogated. Executive function, which is most relevant for long-term human capital, is best measured when children are school aged; therefore, we were able to evaluate the long-term impact of exposures in the critical first two years of life.

A better understanding of the potential role of inflammation, local or systemic, on potential pathways between Shigella and growth and cognition is needed. Many markers, including CRP and pro-inflammatory cytokines, such as IL-1β, IL-6, TNF-α, IL-4, and IL-10, have been considered in addition to MPO and AGP as biomarkers of inflammation. It is unclear which marker is most predictive of outcomes and indicative of pathways related to the effects of enteropathogens. Furthermore, the most relevant timepoints for measurement are unknown. The heterogeneity in associations with inflammation across sites mirrors the variability in the characteristics of EED documented from intestinal biopsies across sites. [35] Yet another potential determinant of developmental outcomes affected by intestinal or systemic inflammation could be population genetic differences. The somewhat surprising association of ApoE4, for example, with protection against heavy diarrhea burdens and semantic fluency deficits is an example of this possibility [36].

In summary, early life exposure to Shigella was associated with sustained decrements in linear growth, that in some cases associated with slightly lower cognitive scores in school-aged children. While Shigella as a cause of inflammation may explain the impact of Shigella on growth, measures of MPO and AGP did not account for the associations observed in this study. Quantifying the impact and understanding mechanisms for the effect of early life exposures to enteric pathogens and EED on long-term cognitive outcomes remains challenging.

Supporting information

S1 Table. Comparison of characteristics between the 451 children assessed at 6–8 years of age and the 611 children in Brazil, South Africa, and Tanzania who completed the original MAL-ED study with follow-up to 2 years of age.

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Unadjusted and adjusted associations between Shigella prevalence in the first 2 years of life with linear growth and cognitive outcomes at 6–8 years of age among 451 children in the Brazil, South Africa, and Tanzania MAL-ED cohorts.

(DOCX)

S3 Table. Unadjusted and adjusted associations between Shigella quantity in the first 2 years of life with linear growth and cognitive outcomes at 6–8 years of age among 451 children in the Brazil, South Africa, and Tanzania MAL-ED cohorts.

(DOCX)

S4 Table. Unadjusted and adjusted associations between intestinal inflammation in the first 2 years of life with linear growth and cognitive outcomes at 6–8 years of age among 451 children in the Brazil, South Africa, and Tanzania MAL-ED cohorts.

(DOCX)

S5 Table. Unadjusted and adjusted associations between systemic inflammation in the first 2 years of life with linear growth and cognitive outcomes at 6–8 years of age among 451 children in the Brazil, South Africa, and Tanzania MAL-ED cohorts.

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

We thank the MAL-ED Network Investigators for their contributions to the MAL-ED study and MAL-ED study participants for their participation.

Data Availability

The data underlying the results presented in the study are available from the ClinEpiDB database (https://clinepidb.org/ce/app/workspace/analyses/DS_5c41b87221/new/details). One must register and request access to download data; data can be downloaded after a committee reviews the request and grants access. Cognitive testing data contain sensitive patient information that have been subject to historical abuse even after deidentification and are available on reasonable request to the Institutional Review Board for Health Sciences Research at the University of Virginia (IRBHSRadmin@virginia.edu).

Funding Statement

The Etiology, Risk Factors, and Interactions of Enteric Infections and Malnutrition and the Consequences for Child Health and Development Project (MAL-ED) was carried out as a collaborative project supported by the Bill and Melinda Gates Foundation (https://www.gatesfoundation.org/; OPP1131125 to RLG), the Foundation for the National Institutes of Health, and the National Institutes of Health, Fogarty International Center. This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health, National Institute of Allergy and Infectious Diseases (https://www.niaid.nih.gov/; grant K01AI130326 to ETRM). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Keusch GT, Denno DM, Black RE, Duggan C, Guerrant RL, Lavery JV, et al. Environmental enteric dysfunction: pathogenesis, diagnosis, and clinical consequences. Clin Infect Dis. 2014;59 Suppl 4: S207–212. doi: 10.1093/cid/ciu485 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Korpe PS, Petri WA Jr. Environmental enteropathy: critical implications of a poorly understood condition. Trends in Molecular Medicine. 2012;18: 328–336. doi: 10.1016/j.molmed.2012.04.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Rogawski ET, Guerrant RL. The Burden of Enteropathy and “Subclinical” Infections. Pediatric Clinics of North America. 2017;64: 815–836. doi: 10.1016/j.pcl.2017.03.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Mattock E, Blocker AJ. How Do the Virulence Factors of Shigella Work Together to Cause Disease? Frontiers in Cellular and Infection Microbiology. 2017;7: 64. doi: 10.3389/fcimb.2017.00064 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Rogawski McQuade ET, Shaheen F, Kabir F, Rizvi A, Platts-Mills JA, Aziz F, et al. Epidemiology of Shigella infections and diarrhea in the first two years of life using culture-independent diagnostics in 8 low-resource settings. PLoS Negl Trop Dis. 2020;14: e0008536. doi: 10.1371/journal.pntd.0008536 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Rogawski ET, Liu J, Platts-Mills JA, Kabir F, Lertsethtakarn P, Siguas M, et al. Use of quantitative molecular diagnostic methods to investigate the effect of enteropathogen infections on linear growth in children in low-resource settings: longitudinal analysis of results from the MAL-ED cohort study. Lancet Glob Health. 2018;6: e1319–e1328. doi: 10.1016/S2214-109X(18)30351-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Kosek M, Haque R, Lima A, Babji S, Shrestha S, Qureshi S, et al. Fecal markers of intestinal inflammation and permeability associated with the subsequent acquisition of linear growth deficits in infants. Am J Trop Med Hyg. 2013;88: 390–396. doi: 10.4269/ajtmh.2012.12-0549 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Kosek M, Ahmed T, Bhutta Z, Caulfield L, Guerrant R, Houpt E, et al. Causal Pathways from Enteropathogens to Environmental Enteropathy: Findings from the MAL-ED Birth Cohort Study. EBioMedicine. 2017;18: 109–117. doi: 10.1016/j.ebiom.2017.02.024 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Victora CG, Adair L, Fall C, Hallal PC, Martorell R, Richter L, et al. Maternal and child undernutrition: consequences for adult health and human capital. Lancet. 2008;371: 340–357. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(07)61692-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Pinkerton R, Oriá RB, Lima AAM, Rogawski ET, Oriá MOB, Patrick PD, et al. Early Childhood Diarrhea Predicts Cognitive Delays in Later Childhood Independently of Malnutrition. Am J Trop Med Hyg. 2016. doi: 10.4269/ajtmh.16-0150 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Fischer Walker CL, Lamberti L, Adair L, Guerrant RL, Lescano AG, Martorell R, et al. Does Childhood Diarrhea Influence Cognition Beyond the Diarrhea-Stunting Pathway? PLoS ONE. 2012;7: e47908. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0047908 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Anthony D, Dempster R, Fearn S, Clements J, Wells G, Perry VH, et al. CXC chemokines generate age-related increases in neutrophil-mediated brain inflammation and blood-brain barrier breakdown. Curr Biol. 1998;8: 923–926. doi: 10.1016/s0960-9822(07)00373-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Dammann O, O’Shea TM. Cytokines and perinatal brain damage. Clin Perinatol. 2008;35: 643–663, v. doi: 10.1016/j.clp.2008.07.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.O’Shea TM, Allred EN, Kuban KCK, Dammann O, Paneth N, Fichorova R, et al. Elevated concentrations of inflammation-related proteins in postnatal blood predict severe developmental delay at 2 years of age in extremely preterm infants. J Pediatr. 2012;160: 395–401.e4. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2011.08.069 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.O’Shea TM, Shah B, Allred EN, Fichorova RN, Kuban KCK, Dammann O, et al. Inflammation-initiating illnesses, inflammation-related proteins, and cognitive impairment in extremely preterm infants. Brain Behav Immun. 2013;29: 104–112. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2012.12.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Jiang NM, Tofail F, Moonah SN, Scharf RJ, Taniuchi M, Ma JZ, et al. Febrile illness and pro-inflammatory cytokines are associated with lower neurodevelopmental scores in Bangladeshi infants living in poverty. BMC Pediatr. 2014;14: 50. doi: 10.1186/1471-2431-14-50 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.MAL-ED Network Investigators. Early childhood cognitive development is affected by interactions among illness, diet, enteropathogens and the home environment: findings from the MAL-ED birth cohort study. BMJ Global Health. 2018;3: e000752. doi: 10.1136/bmjgh-2018-000752 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.McCormick BJJ, Richard SA, Caulfield LE, Pendergast LL, Seidman JC, Koshy B, et al. Early Life Child Micronutrient Status, Maternal Reasoning, and a Nurturing Household Environment have Persistent Influences on Child Cognitive Development at Age 5 years: Results from MAL-ED. J Nutr. 2019;149: 1460–1469. doi: 10.1093/jn/nxz055 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.MAL-ED Network Investigators. The MAL-ED study: a multinational and multidisciplinary approach to understand the relationship between enteric pathogens, malnutrition, gut physiology, physical growth, cognitive development, and immune responses in infants and children up to 2 years of age in resource-poor environments. Clin Infect Dis. 2014;59 Suppl 4: S193–206. doi: 10.1093/cid/ciu653 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Platts-Mills JA, Liu J, Rogawski ET, Kabir F, Lertsethtakarn P, Siguas M, et al. Use of quantitative molecular diagnostic methods to assess the aetiology, burden, and clinical characteristics of diarrhoea in children in low-resource settings: a reanalysis of the MAL-ED cohort study. Lancet Glob Health. 2018;6: e1309–e1318. doi: 10.1016/S2214-109X(18)30349-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.McCormick BJJ, Lee GO, Seidman JC, Haque R, Mondal D, Quetz J, et al. Dynamics and Trends in Fecal Biomarkers of Gut Function in Children from 1–24 Months in the MAL-ED Study. Am J Trop Med Hyg. 2017;96: 465–472. doi: 10.4269/ajtmh.16-0496 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Bingham WC, Burke HR, Murray S. Raven’s progressive matrices: construct validity. J Psychol. 1966;62: 205–209. doi: 10.1080/00223980.1966.10543785 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Raven J. The Raven’s progressive matrices: change and stability over culture and time. Cogn Psychol. 2000;41: 1–48. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0735 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Korkman ME. Nepsy a neuropsychological test battery for children based on luria’s investigation. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 1987;9: 36. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Murray-Kolb LE, Rasmussen ZA, Scharf RJ, Rasheed MA, Svensen E, Seidman JC, et al. The MAL-ED Cohort Study: Methods and Lessons Learned When Assessing Early Child Development and Caregiving Mediators in Infants and Young Children in 8 Low- and Middle-Income Countries. Clin Infect Dis. 2014;59: S261–S272. doi: 10.1093/cid/ciu437 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Patrick PD, Oriá RB, Madhavan V, Pinkerton RC, Lorntz B, Lima AAM, et al. Limitations in verbal fluency following heavy burdens of early childhood diarrhea in Brazilian shantytown children. Child Neuropsychol. 2005;11: 233–244. doi: 10.1080/092970490911252 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.World Health Organization. WHO Child Growth Standards: Length/height-for-age, weight-for-age, weight-for-length, weight-for-height and body mass index-for-age, Methods and development. 2006. Available from: http://www.who.int/childgrowth/standards/Technical_report.pdf?ua=1.
  • 28.de Onis M, Onyango AW, Borghi E, Siyam A, Nishida C, Siekmann J. Development of a WHO growth reference for school-aged children and adolescents. Bull World Health Organ. 2007;85: 660–667. doi: 10.2471/blt.07.043497 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Baron RM, Kenny DA. The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1986;51: 1173–1182. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.51.6.1173 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Tingley D, Yamamoto T, Hirose K, Keele L, Imai K. mediation: R Package for Causal Mediation Analysis. Journal of Statistical Software. 2014;59: 1–38. doi: 10.18637/jss.v059.i0526917999 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Scharf RJ, Rogawski McQuade ET, Svensen E, Huggins A, Maphula A, Bayo E, et al. Early Life Enteric Pathogen Exposure and School-Age Cognitive Outcomes: Findings from Three Sites of the Malnutrition and Enteric Disease: Consequences for Child Development (MAL-ED) Study. Manuscript under review. 2022. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Donowitz JR, Drew J, Taniuchi M, Platts-Mills JA, Alam M, Ferdous T, et al. Diarrheal Pathogens Associated With Growth and Neurodevelopment. Clin Infect Dis. 2021;73: e683–e691. doi: 10.1093/cid/ciaa1938 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Guerrant DI, Moore SR, Lima AA, Patrick PD, Schorling JB, Guerrant RL. Association of early childhood diarrhea and cryptosporidiosis with impaired physical fitness and cognitive function four-seven years later in a poor urban community in northeast Brazil. Am J Trop Med Hyg. 1999;61: 707–713. doi: 10.4269/ajtmh.1999.61.707 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Niehaus MD, Moore SR, Patrick PD, Derr LL, Lorntz B, Lima AA, et al. Early childhood diarrhea is associated with diminished cognitive function 4 to 7 years later in children in a northeast Brazilian shantytown. Am J Trop Med Hyg. 2002;66: 590–593. doi: 10.4269/ajtmh.2002.66.590 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Kelly P, Mouksassi S, Smith G, VanBuskirk K, Newhall K, Yan J, et al. A novel scoring system for environmental enteropathy underlying EED reveals geographic differences. American Society of Tropical Medicine and Hygiene 70th Annual Meeting; 2021 Nov 21; Symposium 123: Undernutrition in Children: the Role of Enteric Dysfunction.
  • 36.Oriá RB, Patrick PD, Zhang H, Lorntz B, de Castro Costa CM, Brito GAC, et al. APOE4 Protects the Cognitive Development in Children with Heavy Diarrhea Burdens in Northeast Brazil. Pediatr Res. 2005;57: 310–316. doi: 10.1203/01.PDR.0000148719.82468.CA [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

S1 Table. Comparison of characteristics between the 451 children assessed at 6–8 years of age and the 611 children in Brazil, South Africa, and Tanzania who completed the original MAL-ED study with follow-up to 2 years of age.

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Unadjusted and adjusted associations between Shigella prevalence in the first 2 years of life with linear growth and cognitive outcomes at 6–8 years of age among 451 children in the Brazil, South Africa, and Tanzania MAL-ED cohorts.

(DOCX)

S3 Table. Unadjusted and adjusted associations between Shigella quantity in the first 2 years of life with linear growth and cognitive outcomes at 6–8 years of age among 451 children in the Brazil, South Africa, and Tanzania MAL-ED cohorts.

(DOCX)

S4 Table. Unadjusted and adjusted associations between intestinal inflammation in the first 2 years of life with linear growth and cognitive outcomes at 6–8 years of age among 451 children in the Brazil, South Africa, and Tanzania MAL-ED cohorts.

(DOCX)

S5 Table. Unadjusted and adjusted associations between systemic inflammation in the first 2 years of life with linear growth and cognitive outcomes at 6–8 years of age among 451 children in the Brazil, South Africa, and Tanzania MAL-ED cohorts.

(DOCX)

Data Availability Statement

The data underlying the results presented in the study are available from the ClinEpiDB database (https://clinepidb.org/ce/app/workspace/analyses/DS_5c41b87221/new/details). One must register and request access to download data; data can be downloaded after a committee reviews the request and grants access. Cognitive testing data contain sensitive patient information that have been subject to historical abuse even after deidentification and are available on reasonable request to the Institutional Review Board for Health Sciences Research at the University of Virginia (IRBHSRadmin@virginia.edu).


Articles from PLoS Neglected Tropical Diseases are provided here courtesy of PLOS

RESOURCES