Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2022 Oct 6.
Published in final edited form as: Handb Exp Pharmacol. 2017;244:219–236. doi: 10.1007/164_2016_97

Sigma Receptors and Alcohol Use Disorders

Valentina Sabino 1, Pietro Cottone 1
PMCID: PMC9536191  NIHMSID: NIHMS1839809  PMID: 28039543

Abstract

Although extensive research has focused on understanding the neurobiological mechanisms underlying alcohol addiction, pharmacological treatments for alcohol use disorders are very limited and not always effective. This constraint has encouraged the search for novel pharmacological targets for alcoholism therapy. Sigma receptors were shown to mediate some of the properties of cocaine and amphetamine, which was attributed to the direct binding of psychostimulants to these receptors. More recently, the role of sigma receptors in the rewarding and reinforcing effects of alcohol was also proposed, and it was suggested that their hyperactivity may result in excessive alcohol drinking. This chapter reviews current knowledge on the topic, and suggests that the sigma receptor system may represent a new therapeutic target for the treatment of alcohol use disorders.

Keywords: Addiction, Alcohol, Alcoholism, Alcohol dependence, Consumption, Drinking, Ethanol, Preferring, Withdrawal

1. Epidemiology and Associated Medical Conditions

The global status report on alcohol and health by the World Health Organization (WHO) indicates that worldwide alcohol consumption in 2010 was equal to 6.2 l of pure alcohol consumed per person aged 15 years or older per day (World Health Organization 2014). The WHO also indicates that in 2012, over 3 million deaths (~6% of all global deaths) were attributable to alcohol consumption (World Health Organization 2014). Globally, alcohol misuse is the first risk factor for premature death and disability for people between the ages of 15 and 49, and it is ranked fifth when all ages are accounted for. One-fourth of total deaths in people between 20 and 39 years are dependent on alcohol (World Health Organization 2014; Lim et al. 2012). In addition, estimates of the global economic burden of alcohol consumption suggest that alcohol is responsible for 1.3–3.3% of total health costs, 6.4–14.4% of total public order and safety costs, 0.3–1.4% of gross domestic product GDP for criminal damage costs, 1.0–1.7% of GDP for drunk driving costs, and 2.7–10.9% of GDP for workplace costs (e.g., absenteeism, unemployment, and premature mortality) (Baumberg 2006).

The adverse consequences on health associated with alcohol consumption are numerous. The WHO indicates that alcohol is a causal factor in 60 types of diseases and injury-related health conditions, including addiction, gastrointestinal diseases, cardiovascular diseases, cancers, fetal alcohol spectrum disease, and alcohol-related injuries (World Health Organization 2014).

Acute alcohol consumption is responsible for a variety of physiological and behavioral effects which are resultant of blood alcohol concentrations (BACs), (Koob and Le Moal 2005). At BACs of 10–50 mg/dl, alcohol increases locomotor activity, disinhibits behavior, and relieves anxiety. When BACs reach 80 mg/dl, alcohol impairs judgment, cognition, and motor function. Individuals with BACs of 150 mg/dl experience marked motor impairment and ataxia, memory lapse, as well as decreased reaction time. BACs of 300 mg/dl produce hypnosis and can cause general anesthesia and coma. At BACs of 400 mg/dl, death is observed in 50% of the people (Koob and Le Moal 2005).

Alcohol is responsible for a plethora of psychiatric disorders, the most relevant being alcohol use disorder (AUD). The diagnosis of AUD in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-5, American Psychiatric Association 2013) integrates in a single disorder the diagnoses of alcohol abuse and alcohol dependence previously described in the fourth edition of the manual. The manual lists 11 criteria for AUD, and the disorder is diagnosed as mild, moderate, or severe as a function of the number of criteria met (2–3 mild, 4–5 moderate, >5 severe). According to the DSM-5, the diagnosis of AUD is therefore based on the presence of impaired control, social impairment, risky use, as well as pharmacological indicators.

2. Definitions of Alcohol Use Disorders

Alcohol represents the most commonly used and abused substance in the world and it has been consumed for centuries in several cultures. Alcohol exerts beneficial effects when consumed in moderation, but it has abuse potential when consumed in excess. According to the Dietary Guidelines for Americans, moderate alcohol consumption is defined as up to one drink per day for women and up to two drinks per day for men. A standard drink is defined as 14 g of pure alcohol, which are equivalent to a 12-ounce can of beer, a 5-ounce glass of wine, or a 1.5-ounce glass of 80-proof liquor. The National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (NIAAA) defines binge drinking as a pattern of drinking which results in BAC levels of 80 mg/dL (NIAAA 2004). Binge drinking typically occurs with four drinks for women and five drinks for men in a time window of approximately 2 h. The Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration (SAMHSA) defines binge drinking as drinking five or more alcoholic drinks on the same occasion on at least 1 day in the past 30 days, while heavy drinking is defined as drinking five or more drinks on the same occasion on each of 5 or more days in the past 30 days (Koob and Le Moal 2005).

3. Molecular Targets of Alcohol

The molecular mechanisms of action of alcohol are several and complex, and still not entirely understood. The complexity of alcohol mechanisms is mainly due to its molecular structure: alcohol is a very small molecule with both polar and nonpolar properties and as such it can easily travel through both hydrophilic and lipophilic molecular and cellular structures. As a consequence, alcohol interacts with both plasma membrane and intracellular proteins. Given the plethora of molecular effects that alcohol can produce, here we will limit our discussion to a brief description of the main mechanisms underlying ethanol’s putative direct interaction with specific target proteins.

A well-known mechanism of action of alcohol is related to its direct interaction with ligand-gated ion channel membrane proteins, especially the pentameric (five subunits) Cys-loop superfamily of neurotransmitter receptors including GABAA receptor (GABAAR), nicotinic acetylcholine receptor (nAChRs), and glycine receptor (GlyR) (Olsen et al. 2014; Trudell et al. 2014). Alcohol directly binds and agonizes GABAAR, and the specific receptor subunit composition makes it more or less responsive to ethanol (Lobo and Harris 2008; Santhakumar et al. 2007; Mehta and Ticku 1988; Suzdak et al. 1988). α4β2δ, α4β3δ, and α6β3δ GABAARs are very sensitive to alcohol, with concentrations of 0.1–1 mM of ethanol significantly enhancing GABA currents (Sundstrom-Poromaa et al. 2002; Wallner et al. 2003). In addition, alcohol is hypothesized to directly act on nAChRs and the net effect of this interaction depends on the receptor subunit composition; alcohol enhances the function of α4β2, α4β4, α2β2, and α2β4 nAChRs, while it exerts no effect on α3β2 and α3β4 nAChRs, and inhibits α7 nAChRs (Narahashi et al. 1999; Cardoso et al. 1999; Davis and de Fiebre 2006). Furthermore, alcohol can bind and positively modulate GlyRs (Perkins et al. 2010).

Another well-described mechanism of action of alcohol is its antagonistic action on the N-methyl-D-aspartate glutamate receptor (NMDAR); alcohol is thought to interact allosterically with NMDARs, reducing the affinity of the agonist for the receptors (Lima-Landman and Albuquerque 1989; Wright et al. 1996).

Alcohol has also been demonstrated to directly interact with G-protein-gated inwardly rectifying potassium (GIRK) channels activating them through a direct binding to a hydrophobic pocket. Interestingly, GIRK channels can be occupied and activated by chemical groups different than those of alcohol (Bodhinathan and Slesinger 2013).

Sigma receptor (SigR) ligands have been shown to influence the effects of psychostimulants, in particular cocaine and methamphetamine, which were demonstrated to bind directly to SigR, although at low (micromolar) affinity (Brammer et al. 2006; Nguyen et al. 2005; Sharkey et al. 1988). For this reason, until a few years ago, only a few studies had examined the possibility of a SigR modulation of ethanol’s actions. However, growing evidence indicates that indirect SigR-mediated effects may exist for other substances of abuse besides psychostimulants, including ethanol. Therefore it is conceivable that, for example in the context of cocaine, some of the molecular mechanisms described for SigR may also be common to those of alcohol. Important mechanisms include the described interactions of SigR with dopamine D1 and D2 receptors, potassium channels and opioid receptors, as well as proteins of the nuclear envelope and histone deacetylases (Navarro et al. 2010, 2013; Kourrich et al. 2013; Tsai et al. 2015; Kim et al. 2010; Mei and Pasternak 2007).

4. Sigma Receptors and the Locomotor-Activating and Sedative Effects of Alcohol

Alcohol effects on locomotor activity are a direct function of the BACs attained. At low BACs, alcohol exerts locomotor-stimulating effects and increases locomotor activity, while at higher BACs, the depressant and sedative effects of alcohol become evident. The locomotor-stimulating properties of alcohol are interpreted as an index of its rewarding properties and abuse liability, and they are thought to be dependent on the activation of the mesolimbic dopaminergic system (Phillips and Shen 1996). In rodents, the locomotor-stimulating effects of alcohol and drugs are typically evaluated by placing subjects in an arena equipped with infrared sensor photobeams; the interruption of these photobeams, caused by the subjects’ movement, is recorded by a computer and the number of interruptions is a direct index of the locomotor activity of the subjects.

The selective sigma-1 receptor (Sig-1R) antagonist N-[2-(3,4-dichlorophenyl)ethyl]-N-methyl-2-(dimethylamino)ethylamine (BD1047), injected at doses of 3–30 mg/kg, dose-dependently blocked the locomotor-stimulating effects induced by 1 g/kg of ethanol in Swiss mice (Maurice et al. 2003). In the same study, it was shown that the selective Sig-1R agonist PRE-084, administered at doses of 1–10 mg/kg, failed to affect alcohol-induced locomotion stimulation. Interestingly, neither drug affected locomotor activity when administered alone (Maurice et al. 2003).

Accordingly, in a recent study, Valenza et al. (2015) found that C57BL/6J mice lacking the SIGMAR1 (previously known as Oprs1) gene, which encodes the Sig-1R, were less sensitive to the locomotor-stimulant effects of 1.5 g/kg of ethanol as compared to the wild-type counterpart. Since the C57BL/6J strain is particularly insensitive to the locomotor-stimulant effects of ethanol, mice in this study were pretreated with the benzodiazepine (BDZ) partial inverse agonist Ro 15–4513 (Miczek and Weerts 1987), which is able to unmask the stimulant effects of ethanol by blocking the depressant properties of ethanol (Becker and Hale 1989). These observations, therefore, confirm the notion that Sig-1R is involved in mediating the locomotor-stimulating effects of alcohol. Together these studies suggest that Sig-1R activation may mediate or at least contribute to the locomotor-activating effects of ethanol, and therefore perhaps also to its abuse potential.

In the same study, the effects of SIGMAR1 knockout (KO) on the sedative effects of high doses of alcohol were tested using the loss of righting reflex procedure. In this procedure, following the administration of a high dose of alcohol (4 g/kg), mice are placed on a V-shaped surface, and the latency to lose the righting reflex (inability to right itself from a supine position) and the sleep duration are recorded. SIGMAR1 KO mice were shown not to differ from wild-type mice neither in the latency to lose the righting reflex nor in time spent sleeping, suggesting a similar sensitivity between the two genotypes and therefore opposing the involvement of Sig-1R in the sedative effects of alcohol (Valenza et al. 2015).

5. Sigma Receptors and the Rewarding Properties of Alcohol

Alcohol can increase the salience of the contextual stimuli, such as places in which positive alcohol effects are experienced. Once, through associative learning, contextual neutral stimuli have acquired rewarding properties, they can then exert powerful control over behavior. This mechanism plays a critical role in maintaining alcohol taking behavior, as approaching an alcohol-associated context can set the occasion for drinking to begin (Bardo and Bevins 2000). An experimental procedure to evaluate whether a substance has rewarding properties is place conditioning (also known as conditioned place preference), a task where a compartment equipped with specific contextual cues is repeatedly paired with a rewarding substance (in this case ethanol) and therefore becomes preferred versus a second, neutral compartment (Bardo and Bevins 2000). Even though technically challenging depending on the specific species and strain used, alcohol is able to induce conditioned place preference in rodents (Cunningham and Noble 1992). Pharmacological agents can be administered either before each of the conditioning sessions to assess their influence on the acquisition of place preference or before the post-conditioning test to instead assess their influence on the expression of place preference.

Sig-1R antagonism has been shown to successfully block the expression of the conditioned place preference induced by alcohol. Indeed, pretreatment with the selective Sig-1R antagonist BD1047 (3–30 mg/kg), administered during conditioning, has been shown to dose dependently block the acquisition of place preference induced by repeated injections of 2 g/kg of alcohol in male mice (Maurice et al. 2003). In the same study, the authors demonstrated a bidirectionality of the process, as the selective Sig-1R agonist 2-(4-morpholino) ethyl 1-phenylcyclohexane-1-carboxylate (PRE-084, 1–10 mg/kg), given before a dose of ethanol (0.5 g/kg) (which was per se inert), resulted in a dramatic dose-dependent facilitation of ethanol-induced place preference (Maurice et al. 2003). These results were confirmed and extended in a study in which Sig-1R ligands were administered intracerebroventricularly (Bhutada et al. 2012). In this study, BD1047 (0.1–10 μg/mouse) dose dependently blocked not only the acquisition, but also the expression of ethanol-induced conditioned place preference. It is important to note that both BD1047 and PRE-084 have been repeatedly shown not to exert any effect on place preference when administered alone (Romieu et al. 2000, 2002; Maurice et al. 2003).

6. Sigma Receptors and Alcohol Drinking

Strong evidence from both human and animal studies supports the overarching hypothesis that SigR activation modulates alcohol intake and proposes a role for Sig-1R antagonists as potential pharmacological agents for the treatment of alcohol-use disorder.

A functional relationship between alcoholism and polymorphisms in the human SIGMAR1 gene has been shown in a study by Miyatake et al. (2004), who measured the differential representation of SIGMAR1 functional polymorphisms in a Japanese population of alcoholic subjects. This study showed that the frequency of the A-485 allele and the TT-241–240/Pro2 haplotype, whose transcriptional activity was significantly reduced compared with that of the T-485 allele and the GC-241–240 allele, was higher in controls relative to alcoholic subjects, suggesting that this polymorphism in SIGMAR1 may act as protective factors for alcohol dependence.

At a preclinical level, a relatively large body of evidence has shown a bidirectional role for SigRs in regulating alcohol drinking, and these studies are reviewed below based on the experimental procedure used to assess alcohol drinking behavior: home cage vs. operant self-administration.

6.1. Home Cage Drinking

A procedure used to evaluate drinking in rats is the two-bottle choice. In this procedure, alcohol drinking is measured in rats that are provided continuous access (24-h day) in their home cage with two bottles: one containing a solution of ethanol (usually 10% v/v), and the other one water. Intake and preference are both measured.

In the context of SigR pharmacology, many studies using the two-bottle choice procedure have been performed in selectively bred Sardinian alcohol-preferring (sP) rat. Lines of rodents genetically selected for high alcohol intake and preference represent a successful tool to study the genetic factors underlying excessive alcohol consumption (Ciccocioppo and Hyytia 2006). In particular, rats of the sP rat line have been shown to voluntarily drink large quantities of ethanol, to have a strong innate preference for ethanol over water, and to possess a heritable component analog to human alcohol dependence (Cloninger et al. 1981; Prescott and Kendler 1999; Sigvardsson et al. 1996). Therefore, sP rats represent a model of genetic predisposition to high ethanol drinking and a tool for identifying potential pharmacotherapies for alcoholism (Colombo et al. 2006).

Sig-1Rs have been demonstrated to exert a key role in both the acquisition and the maintenance of excessive alcohol drinking in sP rats. Sabino and colleagues have shown that chronic systemic administration of the selective Sig-1R antagonist BD1063 (30 mg/kg) dramatically reduced the acquisition of alcohol-drinking behavior in sP rats, reducing both intake and preference for alcohol (Blasio et al. 2015). In this study, vehicle-treated sP rats rapidly escalated their alcohol intake to 6 g/kg of ethanol per day within the 2 weeks of observation. Ethanol drinking acquisition was also accompanied by a rapid increase in the preference for alcohol as the consumption of water gradually decreased to maintain a stable overall fluid intake. On the other hand, BD1063-treated sP rats showed a marked reduction in alcohol drinking accompanied by an increase in water intake. Notably, the drug treatment did not affect overall fluid intake and significantly decreased the preference for alcohol, indicating that Sig-1R antagonism is able to shift the innate inclination to drink alcohol over water of sP rats (Blasio et al. 2015). sP rats were also shown to have innately higher levels of Sig-1R protein in the nucleus accumbens (NAcc) as compared to outbred Wistar rats, which provides critical information about the genetic basis of high alcohol drinking (Blasio et al. 2015). Interestingly, increased Sig-1R levels in the NAcc were normalized by chronic alcohol consumption, which may be consistent with the reduced motivation to drink alcohol which follows recent alcohol consumption (Blasio et al. 2015).

Sig-1R antagonism has also been demonstrated to decrease the maintenance of alcohol drinking in sP rats (Sabino et al. 2009b). The selective Sig-1R antagonist NE100 (10–30 mg/kg) reduced the intake of alcohol consumed by sP rats when injected either acutely or chronically. Following acute administration, NE100 dramatically reduced excessive ethanol intake, and decreased the preference for alcohol by increasing the volume of water consumed without affecting total fluid intake. In addition, when sP rats were offered a two-bottle choice between sucrose and water, acute NE100 treatment did not decrease the consumption of sucrose. Overall, these results suggest that the effect of the drug was selective for alcohol and that it was not due to malaise or secondary to an overall behavioral deficit (Sabino et al. 2009b). In addition, the alcohol-suppressive effect of NE100 was not due to changes in ethanol pharmacokinetics, as drug treatment did not affect BACs when ethanol was administered by gavage (Sabino et al. 2009b). Chronic NE100 treatment to sP rats (30 mg/kg) also significantly reduced alcohol intake, with a peak reduction by the treatment day 3. Starting from day 6, some tolerance to NE100’s effect was evident, similar to what was also observed with opioid receptor antagonists (e.g., naloxone and naltrexone), for which tolerance has been shown to develop after 5–14 days of treatment (Cowen et al. 1999; Overstreet et al. 1999; Parkes and Sinclair 2000). Chronic treatment with NE100 did not affect daily food intake (Sabino et al. 2009b).

NE100 treatment was also shown to fully block the increase in alcohol consumption observed when alcohol access is reinstated following a period of deprivation (Sabino et al. 2009b). This transient increase in alcohol consumption is referred to as “alcohol deprivation effect” and it has been posited to be an animal model for alcohol craving and relapse (Rodd-Henricks et al. 2000; Agabio et al. 2000). In this procedure, sP rats, trained under a two-bottle choice continuous access condition, were forced to abstain from alcohol for 1 week, and on the test day, either NE100 or vehicle was administered to the rats before access renewal. Under vehicle conditions, abstinent sP rats dramatically increased the intake of alcohol upon renewing access to the bottle of alcohol as compared to non-abstinent rats; this alcohol deprivation effect was fully prevented by pretreatment with the selective Sig-1R antagonist (Sabino et al. 2009b).

It has been recently shown that SIGMAR1 KO mice show greater alcohol intake and greater alcohol preference in a two-bottle choice procedure as compared to WT mice (Valenza et al. 2015). Interestingly, the higher the concentration of alcohol provided (3%, 6%, and 20% v/v), the more pronounced the observed increase in alcohol intake was. Conversely, when mice were tested in two-bottle choice for either saccharin or quinine, neither the intake of the sweet nor of the bitter solution was changed in SIGMAR1 KO mice, ruling out that the deletion of SIGMAR1 results in altered taste perception or in a general increase in intake of fluids (Valenza et al. 2015). Results from this study seem to contradict the overarching hypothesis that Sig-1R activation mediates the effects of alcohol and that Sig-1R antagonism decreases excessive alcohol drinking (Sabino et al. 2009a, b, 2011). However, the species difference (mice vs. rats) may be responsible for the differential effects observed. In addition, it cannot be excluded that in whole-body KO mice developmental mechanisms play a counteradaptive role and may confound the results obtained.

6.2. Operant Self-Administration

The reinforcing effects of alcohol are studied using instrumental conditioning, a form of associative learning in which subjects (typically rats or mice) learn to self-administer alcohol (or water) by pressing a lever inside an operant chamber. Following a single press on one of the two levers (fixed ratio 1), a syringe pump containing the solution is activated and the respective fluid is dispensed into a drinking cup. In this procedure alcohol drinking is evaluated as the number of responses emitted on the alcohol lever.

Two major studies have been pivotal in demonstrating the bidirectional modulatory role of SigR in the reinforcing properties of alcohol.

In a first study, the effects of the selective Sig-1R antagonist BD1063 on alcohol reinforcement were evaluated in both a genetic and an environmental animal model of excessive alcohol drinking (Sabino et al. 2009a). The genetic animal model used in this study was the sP rats described above. The environmental animal model was outbred Wistar rats made dependent through the exposure to chronic intermittent ethanol (CIE). Briefly, rats were housed for a period of 4–6 weeks in sealed chambers into which ethanol vapor was intermittently introduced (for review, see Vendruscolo and Roberts 2014); BACs were kept at approximately 150–200 mg% across the exposure period. During acute withdrawal from alcohol, CIE rats show heightened levels of ethanol self-administration, anxiety-like behavior, and increased threshold in the intracranial self-stimulation, compared to control, air-exposed rats (Sabino et al. 2006; Funk et al. 2006; O’Dell et al. 2004). Results from this study showed that the selective Sig-1R antagonist BD1063 (3.3–11 mg/kg) dose dependently reduced excessive ethanol self-administration in both sP rats and CIE rats during acute withdrawal (Sabino et al. 2009a). BD1063 did not, however, reduce ethanol self-administration in control rats. In addition, BD1063 treatment did not affect responding for water or for an equally reinforcing solution of saccharin, suggesting that the Sig-1R antagonist effects were selective for alcohol (Sabino et al. 2009a).

In the same study, the effects of BD1063 were also tested in a progressive ratio schedule of reinforcement for alcohol, which represents a highly validated operant model to assess subjects’ motivation for alcohol (Hodos 1961). In this procedure, the number of lever presses (ratio) required to obtain a single reinforcer increases progressively, with the last ratio defined as the “breakpoint.” The breakpoint, therefore, represents the maximum effort a subject expends to obtain the desired reinforcing stimulus, and is an objective measure of the subject’s motivation. Results from this study showed that BD1063 (3.3–11 mg/kg) dose dependently reduced the breakpoint for ethanol in sP rats (Sabino et al. 2009a).

Collectively, these data suggest that Sig-1Rs are recruited in conditions of excessive ethanol intake and/or heightened motivation, thus likely contributing to innate and ethanol-induced increases in susceptibility to drink excessively. In addition, the increase in the NAcc Sig-1R expression levels in sP rats compared to outbred Wistar rats observed by Blasio et al. (2015) can be speculated to explain the increased sensitivity of sP rats to pharmacological blockade with Sig-1R antagonists found in this study.

The results of a second study demonstrated the bidirectionality of the modulation of ethanol drinking exerted by the SigR system. Daily systemic treatment (2/day for 7 consecutive days) with the SigR agonist 1,3-di-(2-tolyl)guanidine (DTG) (15 mg/kg) was shown to increase ethanol self-administration in sP rats under a fixed ratio 1 schedule of responding (Sabino et al. 2011). Importantly, the increased self-administration in DTG-treated rats resulted in BACs exceeding 80 mg%, which can therefore be regarded as “binge-like” according to the definition provided by the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (NIAAA 2004). Importantly, SigR agonist treatment might represent a novel way to induce binge drinking in laboratory animals, which historically has been difficult to achieve (Sabino et al. 2011). Treatment with DTG also increased breakpoint for ethanol in a progressive ratio schedule of reinforcement, suggesting a greater motivation to work for alcohol. Notably, the DTG-induced increase in ethanol intake was reversed by a subthreshold dose of the Sig-1R antagonist BD1063, confirming that the Sig-1R subtype mediated the DTG effects (Sabino et al. 2011). In addition, considering that both sP rats and acutely withdrawn CIE rats show alterations of Sig-1R levels in the NAcc (Blasio et al. 2015; Sabino et al. 2009a), it is conceivable that Sig-1R of the NAcc may mediate the susceptibility to excessive drinking, both innate and induced by chronic alcohol exposure.

Repeated treatment with DTG induced an increase in μ- and δ-opioid receptor gene expression in the ventral tegmental area (VTA) of sP rats, suggesting that SigR agonists may facilitate ethanol’s ability to activate the mesolimbic dopaminergic system through this mechanism which involved the endogenous opioid system of the VTA. These results suggest a key facilitatory role for SigR in the reinforcing effects of ethanol and identify a potential mechanism that contributes to excessive drinking.

7. Sigma Receptors and Alcohol Seeking

One of the major issues encountered in the treatment of alcohol addiction is relapse following abstinence. In alcoholic individuals, abstinence is accompanied by craving, a strong desire to engage in alcohol drinking often referred to as alcohol seeking behavior, which is in turn responsible for relapse (Martin-Fardon and Weiss 2013; Everitt and Robbins 2000; Le and Shaham 2002). Craving is typically triggered by a number of different factors, of which the most common are exposure to stress, exposure to alcohol (i.e., priming), and exposure to conditioned environmental stimuli previously associated with alcohol (i.e., conditioned cues). In this chapter, we focus on seeking behavior triggered by exposure to either priming or alcohol conditioned cues, as they are factors triggering relapse studied in relation to SigR system.

7.1. Priming-Induced Alcohol Seeking Behavior

In alcoholics, relapse and craving during abstinence are often triggered by acute reexposure to alcohol (Chutuape et al. 1994; Hodgson et al. 1979). Small amounts of alcohol can act much like hors d’oeuvres, thereby contributing to the “first-drink” relapse phenomenon (Ludwig et al. 1974). Literature suggests that SigRs are involved in the mechanisms underlying priming-induced alcohol seeking behavior. Indeed, Bhutada and colleagues examined the effects of SigR ligands on priming-induced reinstatement of ethanol conditioned place preference (Bhutada et al. 2012). This procedure is based on the conditioned place paradigm described previously. Briefly, specific tactile and visual stimuli of one of the two compartments of a place preference apparatus are associated with the effects of alcohol, while the stimuli of the other compartment remain neutral. Once ethanol place preference has been established, subjects are repeatedly exposed to the alcohol-paired compartment until preference is gradually extinguished. Once the alcohol preference is extinguished, it can be reinstated by exposure to alcohol or to another pharmacological agent (i.e., cross-reinstatement). In this study, the authors demonstrated that alcohol seeking behavior could be reinstated by systemic administration of 1 g/kg of ethanol or cross-reinstated by intracerebroventricular microinfusion of the selective Sig-1R PRE-084 (1–10 μg/mouse). In addition, the selective Sig-1R antagonist BD1047 (1–10 μg/mouse), microinfused intracerebroventricularly, was able to dose dependently block both ethanol-induced reinstatement and the PRE-084-induced cross-reinstatement of ethanol-induced conditioned place preference, suggesting that reinstatement of ethanol conditioned place preference involves the activation of central Sig-1Rs (Bhutada et al. 2012).

7.2. Cue-Induced Alcohol Seeking Behavior

As previously mentioned, once contextual stimuli are associated with the positive effects of alcohol through Pavlovian conditioning, they can exert a strong control over behavior. These conditioned cues become particularly relevant in occasions in which the effects of alcohol are not being experienced (i.e., during abstinence), and can lead to resumption of alcohol drinking. In preclinical psychopharmacological research, different animal models of alcohol seeking behavior have been developed to study the influence of stimuli associated with alcohol. Here we will be describing two operant responding alcohol seeking procedures, which have been used to assess the role of SigRs in the modulation of the influence of alcohol-associated cues over behavior.

A classical experimental procedure used to assess seeking behavior is the cue-induced reinstatement of seeking behavior. In this task, subjects are trained to self-administer alcohol by pressing a lever, and each lever response is contiguously paired with a brief presentation of a conditioned stimulus (e.g., an olfactory stimulus, a light, a tone). Following the initial training, ethanol-reinforced responding is extinguished by withholding both alcohol delivery and presentation of the conditioned stimulus. Once extinction of lever responding is obtained, reinstatement of alcohol seeking behavior is induced by presenting the alcohol-associated conditioned stimulus. Using this procedure, Martin-Fardon and colleagues showed that the selective Sig-1R BD1047 (3–20 mg/kg) was able to block cue-induced reinstatement of alcohol seeking induced by presentation of an olfactory stimulus.

Another classical experimental procedure used to assess seeking behavior is the seeking-taking chain in a second-order schedule of reinforcement, where responding on a seeking lever is maintained not only by the self-administered reinforcer, but also by contingent presentation of reinforcer-paired stimuli that serve as conditioned reinforcers of instrumental behavior (Velazquez-Sanchez et al. 2015; Everitt and Robbins 2000; Giuliano et al. 2015). Typically, a second inactive lever is present and responses on this lever result in no consequences, but are recorded as an index of motor activity. This procedure has been recently established employing alcohol as the reinforcer, and it has been used to determine the role of SigR in alcohol seeking behavior (Blasio et al. 2015). The selective Sig-1R antagonist BD1063 (3–30 mg/kg) systemically administered was shown to be able to dramatically and dose dependently reduce alcohol seeking behavior. All doses of BD1063 tested significantly decreased the number of lever presses and importantly BD1063 did not affect responding on the inactive lever, ruling out an overall behavioral suppression.

Altogether, these data suggest that the ability of alcohol-associated cues to induce seeking behavior involves the activation of Sig-1R.

8. Sigma Receptors and Cognitive Impairment During Alcohol Withdrawal

Withdrawal from chronic consumption of alcohol is characterized by a plethora of physical, motivational, cognitive, and emotional symptoms (Pitel et al. 2007; Beatty et al. 1995; McKeon et al. 2008; Koob 2003). Withdrawal symptoms can be unpleasant and intense, and can develop from several hours to a few days after the cessation (or reduction) of heavy and prolonged alcohol use; while certain symptoms may be short lasting, others can persist for months and contribute to relapse (Koob 2000, 2003; American Psychiatric Association 2013).

The impairment in cognitive function is a symptom associated with chronic alcohol exposure withdrawal (Beatty et al. 1995; Pitel et al. 2007), and has been demonstrated to involve the Sig-1R system (Meunier et al. 2006; Sabeti 2011; Sabeti and Gruol 2008). In a study conducted by Meunier and colleagues, mice were shown to develop cognitive dysfunction in a novel object recognition task, during a 16-day withdrawal period which followed 4 months of chronic alcohol consumption. In this task, mice were tested for their ability to habituate to familiar objects, to correctly locate familiar object in different spatial locations, and to recognize familiar vs. novel objects. Alcohol-withdrawn mice showed increased locomotion, anxiety, and object exploration, which impeded correct reaction to object habituation, spatial change, and novelty. Importantly the authors showed that treatment with either a nonselective Sig-1R agonist (igmesine) or a Sig-1R antagonist (BD1047) restored correct reactions to spatial change and novelty in mice (Meunier et al. 2006). In addition, these mice had upregulated Sig-1R expression in the hippocampus, which was attenuated following repeated administration of either Sig-1R ligand, suggesting that the increase in hippocampal Sig-1R levels may mediate the ethanol withdrawal-induced cognitive impairments (Meunier et al. 2006).

In addition, it was shown using slice electrophysiology that withdrawal from chronic intermittent ethanol vapors during adolescence significantly alters long-term potentiation in the hippocampus via a Sig-1R-related mechanism (Sabeti and Gruol 2008; Sabeti 2011). In a first study, authors examined how chronic ethanol exposure during adolescence affects long-term potentiation (LTP) mechanisms in the hippocampus (Sabeti and Gruol 2008). The study shows that the selective Sig-1R antagonist BD1047 blocked a slow-developing NMDAR-independent LTP in excitatory CA1 synapses in hippocampal slices at 24 h after CIE vapor exposure. In addition, in alcohol-withdrawn early-adolescent animals, authors observed a Sig-1R-dependent increased presynaptic function during NMDAR-independent LTP induction.

In a second study, the same authors found that, in slices obtained from adolescent rats exposed to chronic intermittent alcohol, CA1 neurons responded to the induction of large-amplitude LTP stimulations with a reduced excitability during ethanol withdrawal compared to slices obtained from ethanol-naϊve rats. Importantly these impairments, which manifested as decreased spike efficacy and impaired activity-induced field excitatory postsynaptic potential-to-spike (E-S) potentiation, were normalized by the Sig-1R antagonist BD1047. These data suggest that acute ethanol withdrawal recruits Sig-1Rs, which in turn act to depress the efficacy of excitatory inputs in triggering action potentials during LTP.

9. Concluding Remarks

As reviewed above, there is growing evidence that the Sig-1R system may represent a novel target for the pharmacological treatment of alcohol-use disorders. Sig-1R antagonists have proven effective in reducing excessive alcohol drinking and alcohol seeking behavior in multiple animal models, suggesting that Sig-1R activation mediates the susceptibility to drink high quantities of alcohol. However, the exact mechanisms through which the Sig-1R system influences the actions of alcohol are still not entirely clear. Therefore, mechanistic studies aimed at understanding the interaction between the Sig-1R system and alcohol are warranted to improve our understanding of the neurobiological bases of alcoholism and help develop novel therapeutic options for this disorder.

References

  1. Agabio R, Carai MA, Lobina C, Pani M, Reali R, Vacca G, Gessa GL, Colombo G (2000) Development of short-lasting alcohol deprivation effect in Sardinian alcohol-preferring rats. Alcohol 21(1):59–62 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. American Psychiatric Association (2013) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, 5th edn. American Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC [Google Scholar]
  3. Bardo MT, Bevins RA (2000) Conditioned place preference: what does it add to our preclinical understanding of drug reward? Psychopharmacology (Berl) 153(1):31–43 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Baumberg B (2006) The global economic burden of alcohol: a review and some suggestions. Drug Alcohol Rev 25(6):537–551. doi: 10.1080/09595230600944479 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Beatty WW, Katzung VM, Moreland VJ, Nixon SJ (1995) Neuropsychological performance of recently abstinent alcoholics and cocaine abusers. Drug Alcohol Depend 37(3):247–253 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Becker HC, Hale RL (1989) Ethanol-induced locomotor stimulation in C57BL/6 mice following RO15–4513 administration. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 99(3):333–336 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bhutada PS, Mundhada YR, Ghodki YR, Chaware P, Dixit PV, Jain KS, Umathe SN (2012) Influence of sigma-1 receptor modulators on ethanol-induced conditioned place preference in the extinction-reinstatement model. Behav Pharmacol 23(1):25–33. doi: 10.1097/FBP.0b013e32834eafe6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Blasio A, Valenza M, Iyer MR, Rice KC, Steardo L, Hayashi T, Cottone P, Sabino V (2015) Sigma-1 receptor mediates acquisition of alcohol drinking and seeking behavior in alcohol-preferring rats. Behav Brain Res 287:315–322. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2015.03.065 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bodhinathan K, Slesinger PA (2013) Molecular mechanism underlying ethanol activation of G-protein-gated inwardly rectifying potassium channels. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 110(45):18309–18314. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1311406110 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Brammer MK, Gilmore DL, Matsumoto RR (2006) Interactions between 3,4-methylenedioxy-methamphetamine and sigma1 receptors. Eur J Pharmacol 553(1–3):141–145. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2006.09.038 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Cardoso RA, Brozowski SJ, Chavez-Noriega LE, Harpold M, Valenzuela CF, Harris RA (1999) Effects of ethanol on recombinant human neuronal nicotinic acetylcholine receptors expressed in Xenopus oocytes. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 289(2):774–780 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Chutuape MA, Mitchell S, De Wit H (1994) Ethanol preloads increase ethanol preference under concurrent random-ratio schedules in social drinkers. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 2:310–318 [Google Scholar]
  13. Ciccocioppo R, Hyytia P (2006) The genetic of alcoholism: learning from 50 years of research. Addict Biol 11(3–4):193–194. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2006.00028.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Cloninger CR, Bohman M, Sigvardsson S (1981) Inheritance of alcohol abuse: cross-fostering analysis of adopted men. Arch Gen Psychiatry 38(8):861–868 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Colombo G, Lobina C, Carai MA, Gessa GL (2006) Phenotypic characterization of genetically selected Sardinian alcohol-preferring (sP) and -non-preferring (sNP) rats. Addict Biol 11 (3–4):324–338. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2006.00031.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Cowen MS, Rezvani AH, Jarrott B, Lawrence AJ (1999) Ethanol consumption by Fawn-Hooded rats following abstinence: effect of naltrexone and changes in mu-opioid receptor density. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 23(6):1008–1014 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Cunningham CL, Noble D (1992) Conditioned activation induced by ethanol: role in sensitization and conditioned place preference. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 43(1):307–313 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Davis TJ, de Fiebre CM (2006) Alcohol’s actions on neuronal nicotinic acetylcholine receptors. Alcohol Res Health 29(3):179–185 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Everitt BJ, Robbins TW (2000) Second-order schedules of drug reinforcement in rats and monkeys: measurement of reinforcing efficacy and drug-seeking behaviour. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 153(1):17–30 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Funk CK, O’Dell LE, Crawford EF, Koob GF (2006) Corticotropin-releasing factor within the central nucleus of the amygdala mediates enhanced ethanol self-administration in withdrawn, ethanol-dependent rats. J Neurosci 26(44):11324–11332 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Giuliano C, Goodlett CR, Economidou D, Garcia-Pardo MP, Belin D, Robbins TW, Bullmore ET, Everitt BJ (2015) The novel mu-opioid receptor antagonist gsk1521498 decreases both alcohol seeking and drinking: evidence from a new preclinical model of alcohol seeking. Neuropsychopharmacology 40(13):2981–2992. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.152 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Hodgson R, Rankin H, Stockwell T (1979) Alcohol dependence and the priming effect. Behav Res Ther 17(4):379–387 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Hodos W (1961) Progressive ratio as a measure of reward strength. Science 134(3483):943–944 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Kim FJ, Kovalyshyn I, Burgman M, Neilan C, Chien CC, Pasternak GW (2010) Sigma 1 receptor modulation of G-protein-coupled receptor signaling: potentiation of opioid transduction independent from receptor binding. Mol Pharmacol 77(4):695–703. doi: 10.1124/mol.109.057083 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Koob GF (2000) Animal models of craving for ethanol. Addiction 95(Suppl 2):S73–S81 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Koob GF (2003) Alcoholism: allostasis and beyond. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 27(2):232–243. doi: 10.1097/01.ALC.0000057122.36127.C2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Koob GF, Le Moal M (2005) Neurobiology of addiction. Academic Press, London [Google Scholar]
  28. Kourrich S, Hayashi T, Chuang JY, Tsai SY, Su TP, Bonci A (2013) Dynamic interaction between sigma-1 receptor and Kv1.2 shapes neuronal and behavioral responses to cocaine. Cell 152 (1–2):236–247. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2012.12.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Le A, Shaham Y (2002) Neurobiology of relapse to alcohol in rats. Pharmacol Ther 94(1–2):137–156 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Lim SS, Vos T, Flaxman AD, Danaei G, Shibuya K, Adair-Rohani H, Amann M, Anderson HR, Andrews KG, Aryee M, Atkinson C, Bacchus LJ, Bahalim AN, Balakrishnan K, Balmes J, Barker-Collo S, Baxter A, Bell ML, Blore JD, Blyth F, Bonner C, Borges G, Bourne R, Boussinesq M, Brauer M, Brooks P, Bruce NG, Brunekreef B, Bryan-Hancock C, Bucello C, Buchbinder R, Bull F, Burnett RT, Byers TE, Calabria B, Carapetis J, Carnahan E, Chafe Z, Charlson F, Chen H, Chen JS, Cheng AT, Child JC, Cohen A, Colson KE, Cowie BC, Darby S, Darling S, Davis A, Degenhardt L, Dentener F, Des Jarlais DC, Devries K, Dherani M, Ding EL, Dorsey ER, Driscoll T, Edmond K, Ali SE, Engell RE, Erwin PJ, Fahimi S, Falder G, Farzadfar F, Ferrari A, Finucane MM, Flaxman S, Fowkes FG, Freedman G, Freeman MK, Gakidou E, Ghosh S, Giovannucci E, Gmel G, Graham K, Grainger R, Grant B, Gunnell D, Gutierrez HR, Hall W, Hoek HW, Hogan A, Hosgood HD 3rd, Hoy D, Hu H, Hubbell BJ, Hutchings SJ, Ibeanusi SE, Jacklyn GL, Jasrasaria R, Jonas JB, Kan H, Kanis JA, Kassebaum N, Kawakami N, Khang YH, Khatibzadeh S, Khoo JP, Kok C, Laden F, Lalloo R, Lan Q, Lathlean T, Leasher JL, Leigh J, Li Y, Lin JK, Lipshultz SE, London S, Lozano R, Lu Y, Mak J, Malekzadeh R, Mallinger L, Marcenes W, March L, Marks R, Martin R, McGale P, McGrath J, Mehta S, Mensah GA, Merriman TR, Micha R, Michaud C, Mishra V, Mohd Hanafiah K, Mokdad AA, Morawska L, Mozaffarian D, Murphy T, Naghavi M, Neal B, Nelson PK, Nolla JM, Norman R, Olives C, Omer SB, Orchard J, Osborne R, Ostro B, Page A, Pandey KD, Parry CD, Passmore E, Patra J, Pearce N, Pelizzari PM, Petzold M, Phillips MR, Pope D, Pope CA 3rd, Powles J, Rao M, Razavi H, Rehfuess EA, Rehm JT, Ritz B, Rivara FP, Roberts T, Robinson C, Rodriguez-Portales JA, Romieu I, Room R, Rosenfeld LC, Roy A, Rushton L, Salomon JA, Sampson U, Sanchez-Riera L, Sanman E, Sapkota A, Seedat S, Shi P, Shield K, Shivakoti R, Singh GM, Sleet DA, Smith E, Smith KR, Stapelberg NJ, Steenland K, Stöckl H, Stovner LJ, Straif K, Straney L, Thurston GD, Tran JH, Van Dingenen R, van Donkelaar A, Veerman JL, Vijayakumar L, Weintraub R, Weissman MM, White RA, Whiteford H, Wiersma ST, Wilkinson JD, Williams HC, Williams W, Wilson N, Woolf AD, Yip P, Zielinski JM, Lopez AD, Murray CJ, Ezzati M, AlMazroa MA, Memish ZA (2012) A comparative risk assessment of burden of disease and injury attributable to 67 risk factors and risk factor clusters in 21 regions, 1990–2010: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2010. Lancet 380(9859):2224–2260. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(12)61766-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Lima-Landman MT, Albuquerque EX (1989) Ethanol potentiates and blocks NMDA-activated single-channel currents in rat hippocampal pyramidal cells. FEBS Lett 247(1):61–67 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Lobo IA, Harris RA (2008) GABA(A) receptors and alcohol. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 90(1):90–94. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2008.03.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Ludwig AM, Wikler A, Stark LH (1974) The first drink: psychobiological aspects of craving. Arch Gen Psychiatry 30(4):539–547 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Martin-Fardon R, Weiss F (2013) Modeling relapse in animals. Curr Top Behav Neurosci 13:403–432. doi: 10.1007/7854_2012_202 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Maurice T, Casalino M, Lacroix M, Romieu P (2003) Involvement of the sigma 1 receptor in the motivational effects of ethanol in mice. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 74(4):869–876 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. McKeon A, Frye MA, Delanty N (2008) The alcohol withdrawal syndrome. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 79(8):854–862. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2007.128322 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Mehta AK, Ticku MK (1988) Ethanol potentiation of GABAergic transmission in cultured spinal cord neurons involves gamma-aminobutyric acidA-gated chloride channels. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 246(2):558–564 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Mei J, Pasternak GW (2007) Modulation of brainstem opiate analgesia in the rat by sigma 1 receptors: a microinjection study. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 322(3):1278–1285 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Meunier J, Demeilliers B, Célérier A, Maurice T (2006) Compensatory effect by sigma1 (σ 1) receptor stimulation during alcohol withdrawal in mice performing an object recognition task. Behav Brain Res 166(1):166–176 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Miczek KA, Weerts EM (1987) Seizures in drug-treated animals. Science 235(4793):1127–1128 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Miyatake R, Furukawa A, Matsushita S, Higuchi S, Suwaki H (2004) Functional polymorphisms in the sigma1 receptor gene associated with alcoholism. Biol Psychiatry 55(1):85–90 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Narahashi T, Aistrup GL, Marszalec W, Nagata K (1999) Neuronal nicotinic acetylcholine receptors: a new target site of ethanol. Neurochem Int 35(2):131–141 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Navarro G, Moreno E, Aymerich M, Marcellino D, McCormick PJ, Mallol J, Cortes A, Casado V, Canela EI, Ortiz J, Fuxe K, Lluis C, Ferre S, Franco R (2010) Direct involvement of sigma-1 receptors in the dopamine D1 receptor-mediated effects of cocaine. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 107(43):18676–18681. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1008911107 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Navarro G, Moreno E, Bonaventura J, Brugarolas M, Farre D, Aguinaga D, Mallol J, Cortes A, Casado V, Lluis C, Ferre S, Franco R, Canela E, McCormick PJ (2013) Cocaine inhibits dopamine D2 receptor signaling via sigma-1-D2 receptor heteromers. PLoS One 8(4):e61245. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0061245 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Nguyen EC, McCracken KA, Liu Y, Pouw B, Matsumoto RR (2005) Involvement of sigma (sigma) receptors in the acute actions of methamphetamine: receptor binding and behavioral studies. Neuropharmacology 49(5):638–645. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2005.04.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. NIAAA (2004) NIAAA council approves definition of binge drinking. NIAAA Newsletter. http://pubs.niaaa.nih.gov/publications/Newsletter/winter2004/Newsletter_Number3.pdf [Google Scholar]
  47. O’Dell L, Roberts AJ, Smith RT, Koob GF (2004) Enhanced alcohol self-administration after intermittent versus continuous alcohol vapor exposure. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 28:1676–1682 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Olsen RW, Li GD, Wallner M, Trudell JR, Bertaccini EJ, Lindahl E, Miller KW, Alkana RL, Davies DL (2014) Structural models of ligand-gated ion channels: sites of action for anesthetics and ethanol. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 38(3):595–603. doi: 10.1111/acer.12283 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Overstreet DH, Kampov-Polevoy AB, Rezvani AH, Braun C, Bartus RT, Crews FT (1999) Suppression of alcohol intake by chronic naloxone treatment in P rats: tolerance development and elevation of opiate receptor binding. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 23(11):1761–1771 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Parkes H, Sinclair JD (2000) Reduction of alcohol drinking and upregulation of opioid receptors by oral naltrexone in AA rats. Alcohol 21(3):215–221 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Perkins DI, Trudell JR, Crawford DK, Alkana RL, Davies DL (2010) Molecular targets and mechanisms for ethanol action in glycine receptors. Pharmacol Ther 127(1):53–65. doi: 10.1016/j.pharmthera.2010.03.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Phillips TJ, Shen EH (1996) Neurochemical bases of locomotion and ethanol stimulant effects. Int Rev Neurobiol 39:243–282 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Pitel AL, Beaunieux H, Witkowski T, Vabret F, Guillery-Girard B, Quinette P, Desgranges B, Eustache F (2007) Genuine episodic memory deficits and executive dysfunctions in alcoholic subjects early in abstinence. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 31(7):1169–1178. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2007.00418.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Prescott CA, Kendler KS (1999) Genetic and environmental contributions to alcohol abuse and dependence in a population-based sample of male twins. Am J Psychiatry 156(1):34–40 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Rodd-Henricks ZA, McKinzie DL, Shaikh SR, Murphy JM, McBride WJ, Lumeng L, Li TK (2000) Alcohol deprivation effect is prolonged in the alcohol preferring (P) rat after repeated deprivations. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 24(1):8–16 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Romieu P, Martin-Fardon R, Maurice T (2000) Involvement of the sigma1 receptor in the cocaine-induced conditioned place preference. Neuroreport 11(13):2885–2888 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Romieu P, Phan VL, Martin-Fardon R, Maurice T (2002) Involvement of the sigma(1) receptor in cocaine-induced conditioned place preference: possible dependence on dopamine uptake blockade. Neuropsychopharmacology 26(4):444–455. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(01)00391-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Sabeti J (2011) Ethanol exposure in early adolescence inhibits intrinsic neuronal plasticity via sigma-1 receptor activation in hippocampal CA1 neurons. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 35(5):885–904 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Sabeti J, Gruol DL (2008) Emergence of NMDAR-independent long-term potentiation at hippocampal CA1 synapses following early adolescent exposure to chronic intermittent ethanol: role for sigma-receptors. Hippocampus 18(2):148–168 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Sabino V, Cottone P, Koob GF, Steardo L, Lee MJ, Rice KC, Zorrilla EP (2006) Dissociation between opioid and CRF1 antagonist sensitive drinking in Sardinian alcohol-preferring rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 189(2):175–186 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Sabino V, Cottone P, Zhao Y, Iyer MR, Steardo L Jr, Steardo L, Rice KC, Conti B, Koob GF, Zorrilla EP (2009a) The sigma-receptor antagonist BD-1063 decreases ethanol intake and reinforcement in animal models of excessive drinking. Neuropsychopharmacology 34(6):1482–1493. doi: 10.1038/npp.2008.192 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Sabino V, Cottone P, Zhao Y, Steardo L, Koob GF, Zorrilla EP (2009b) Selective reduction of alcohol drinking in Sardinian alcohol-preferring rats by a sigma-1 receptor antagonist. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 205(2):327–335. doi: 10.1007/s00213-009-1548-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Sabino V, Cottone P, Blasio A, Iyer MR, Steardo L, Rice KC, Conti B, Koob GF, Zorrilla EP (2011) Activation of sigma-receptors induces binge-like drinking in Sardinian alcohol-preferring rats. Neuropsychopharmacology 36(6):1207–1218. doi: 10.1038/npp.2011.5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Santhakumar V, Wallner M, Otis TS (2007) Ethanol acts directly on extrasynaptic subtypes of GABAA receptors to increase tonic inhibition. Alcohol 41(3):211–221. doi: 10.1016/j.alcohol.2007.04.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Sharkey J, Glen KA, Wolfe S, Kuhar MJ (1988) Cocaine binding at sigma receptors. Eur J Pharmacol 149(1–2):171–174 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Sigvardsson S, Bohman M, Cloninger CR (1996) Replication of the Stockholm Adoption Study of alcoholism: confirmatory cross-fostering analysis. Arch Gen Psychiatry 53(8):681–687 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Sundstrom-Poromaa I, Smith DH, Gong QH, Sabado TN, Li X, Light A, Wiedmann M, Williams K, Smith SS (2002) Hormonally regulated alpha(4)beta(2)delta GABA(A) receptors are a target for alcohol. Nat Neurosci 5(8):721–722. doi: 10.1038/nn888 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Suzdak PD, Schwartz RD, Skolnick P, Paul SM (1988) Alcohols stimulate gamma-aminobutyric acid receptor-mediated chloride uptake in brain vesicles: correlation with intoxication potency. Brain Res 444(2):340–345 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Trudell JR, Messing RO, Mayfield J, Harris RA (2014) Alcohol dependence: molecular and behavioral evidence. Trends Pharmacol Sci 35(7):317–323. doi: 10.1016/j.tips.2014.04.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Tsai SY, Chuang JY, Tsai MS, Wang XF, Xi ZX, Hung JJ, Chang WC, Bonci A, Su TP (2015) Sigma-1 receptor mediates cocaine-induced transcriptional regulation by recruiting chromatin-remodeling factors at the nuclear envelope. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 112(47):E6562–E6570. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1518894112 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Valenza M, DiLeo A, Steardo L, Cottone P, Sabino V (2015) Ethanol-related behaviors in mice lacking the sigma-1 receptor. Behav Brain Res. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2015.10.013 (Epub ahead of print) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Velazquez-Sanchez C, Santos JW, Smith KL, Ferragud A, Sabino V, Cottone P (2015) Seeking behavior, place conditioning, and resistance to conditioned suppression of feeding in rats intermittently exposed to palatable food. Behav Neurosci 129(2):219–224. doi: 10.1037/bne0000042 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Vendruscolo LF, Roberts AJ (2014) Operant alcohol self-administration in dependent rats: focus on the vapor model. Alcohol 48(3):277–286 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Wallner M, Hanchar HJ, Olsen RW (2003) Ethanol enhances alpha 4 beta 3 delta and alpha 6 beta 3 delta gamma-aminobutyric acid type A receptors at low concentrations known to affect humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 100(25):15218–15223. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2435171100 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. World Health Organization (2014) Global status report on alcohol and health. World Health Organization, Geneva [Google Scholar]
  76. Wright JM, Peoples RW, Weight FF (1996) Single-channel and whole-cell analysis of ethanol inhibition of NMDA-activated currents in cultured mouse cortical and hippocampal neurons. Brain Res 738(2):249–256 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES