Skip to main content
Wiley Open Access Collection logoLink to Wiley Open Access Collection
. 2021 May 4;55(11-12):3352–3364. doi: 10.1111/ejn.15208

No behavioural evidence for rhythmic facilitation of perceptual discrimination

Wy Ming Lin 1,2,*, Djamari A Oetringer 2,*, Iske Bakker‐Marshall 2, Jill Emmerzaal 2, Anna Wilsch 3, Hesham A ElShafei 2, Elie Rassi 2, Saskia Haegens 2,4,5,
PMCID: PMC9540985  PMID: 33772897

Abstract

It has been hypothesized that internal oscillations can synchronize (i.e., entrain) to external environmental rhythms, thereby facilitating perception and behaviour. To date, evidence for the link between the phase of neural oscillations and behaviour has been scarce and contradictory; moreover, it remains an open question whether the brain can use this tentative mechanism for active temporal prediction. In our present study, we conducted a series of auditory pitch discrimination tasks with 181 healthy participants in an effort to shed light on the proposed behavioural benefits of rhythmic cueing and entrainment. In the three versions of our task, we observed no perceptual benefit of purported entrainment: targets occurring in‐phase with a rhythmic cue provided no perceptual benefits in terms of discrimination accuracy or reaction time when compared with targets occurring out‐of‐phase or targets occurring randomly, nor did we find performance differences for targets preceded by rhythmic versus random cues. However, we found a surprising effect of cueing frequency on reaction time, in which participants showed faster responses to cue rhythms presented at higher frequencies. We therefore provide no evidence of entrainment, but instead a tentative effect of covert active sensing in which a faster external rhythm leads to a faster communication rate between motor and sensory cortices, allowing for sensory inputs to be sampled earlier in time.

Keywords: active sensing, auditory perception, entrainment, pitch discrimination, rhythm


In a series of auditory pitch discrimination tasks with 181 healthy human participants, we provide no evidence for the popular hypothesis that entrainment to external rhythms (cues) facilitates perception and behavior. Instead, we found that behavior (reaction time) correlates positively with the frequency of these external rhythms. Thus, in a rhythmic environment covert active sensing, rather than entrainment, might underlie behavioral or perceptual improvements.

graphic file with name EJN-55-3352-g002.jpg

1. INTRODUCTION

When presented with rhythmic input we tend to produce rhythmic behaviour. Think of clapping to a drumbeat and being able to continue clapping to the beat after the drum stops. Such rhythmic behaviour, driven by temporal expectations, could be subserved by rhythmic brain activity (i.e., neural oscillations), a prominent feature of brain dynamics. In this view, internal neural oscillations that synchronize (entrain) to external environmental rhythms reflect temporal predictions, thereby facilitating perception and behaviour (Lakatos et al., 2008).

This entrainment proposal rests on the key idea that neural oscillations reflect alternating excitability states (excitation/inhibition) of neuronal ensembles (Başar et al., 2013; Bishop, 1932). While there is some evidence that the phase of ongoing oscillations at the time of sensory stimulation impacts the neural response to that stimulus, as well as subsequent behavioural performance (e.g., Busch et al., 2009; Mathewson et al., 2009; Ten Oever & Sack, 2019; for a review see VanRullen, 2016), this evidence is far from conclusive as several studies have reported null results (e.g., Benwell et al., 2017; O'Hare, 1954; Ruzzoli et al., 2019; Vigué‐Guix et al., 2020; Walsh, 1952).

Recently, the proposal of entrainment as a key mechanism for synchronizing with external input in order to optimize perceptual processing has gained traction, particularly in the fields of speech and language comprehension (for reviews see: Haegens & Golumbic, 2018; Meyer et al., 2019; Obleser & Kayser, 2019).

However, there seems to be no consensus as to the definition of neural entrainment as a biophysical process (Haegens, 2020; Haegens & Golumbic, 2018; Lakatos et al., 2019; Obleser & Kayser, 2019). One such proposal (Haegens & Golumbic, 2018)—on which the current study is theoretically framed—argues for a strict definition of entrainment where (a) an endogenous oscillator exists in the absence of rhythmic stimulation; that is, there is internally generated oscillatory brain activity at a certain frequency, (b) the endogenous oscillator adjusts its phase to align with external rhythmic stimulation, but only as long as the external rhythm falls within a range near that of the intrinsic frequency, and (c) the oscillatory activity continues for a number of cycles after the external rhythm stops.

Entrainment is often investigated with rhythmic cueing paradigms where participants are presented with a stimulus stream at a certain frequency. This rhythmic stream is then followed by a target stimulus that might occur in‐phase or out‐of‐phase with the rhythmic cue, one or more cycles later (Jones et al., 2002). While several studies have shown that rhythmic cues indeed facilitate target processing, particularly for targets occurring in‐phase (Jones et al., 2002, 2006; Rohenkohl et al., 2011; Rohenkohl & Nobre, 2011), others have reported opposite (Barnes & Johnston, 2010; Bauer et al., 2015; Hickok et al., 2015; Spaak et al., 2014, see Haegens & Golumbic for review) or null effects (Bosker & Kösem, 2017).

If entrainment indeed optimizes perception, we expect rhythmic cueing paradigms to produce certain behavioural benefits that follow from the criteria outlined above. Namely, we expect participants to perform better in conditions where temporal expectations are more readily built up: (a) when the cue is rhythmic (versus random, arrhythmic, or continuous), that is, providing explicit rhythmic temporal information, (b) when the target timing is rhythmically aligned with the cue (versus occurring at a random time), that is, providing implicit rhythmic structure, and (c) occurs in‐phase (versus out‐of‐phase) with respect to the cue. Further, we expect this behavioural benefit to wane over time as the entrained oscillation persists for a number of cycles after the external rhythm stops. Thus, we expect (d) participants to perform better for targets occurring shortly after the rhythm (versus later). Finally, we expect (e) this behavioural benefit to be tightly linked to the frequency of the external rhythm, that is, frequencies closest to endogenous oscillations are more behaviourally beneficial than others.

In a series of three behavioural experiments, we aimed to test these key predictions. A total of 181 healthy human participants performed an auditory pitch discrimination task where a target tone was presented after a rhythmic or random (i.e., continuous) auditory cue, with the timing of the target either rhythmically aligned to the cue or randomly timed. We manipulated the timings such as to be able to test each of our predictions listed above, and report no support for any of them.

2. METHODS

2.1. Participants

Thirty‐two healthy participants (21 female, 11 male; age range: 18–31 years, median = 23 years) took part in Experiment I. We excluded two participants from the analysis due to low performance levels and one participant due to low number of trials left after preprocessing. A total of 119 healthy participants (77 female, 42 male; age range: 18–35 years; median = 22 years) took part in Experiment II. Five participants were excluded due to low performance levels. Of the remaining participants, 30 performed the rhythmic cue‐rhythmic target condition, 29 the rhythmic cue‐random target condition, 29 the random cue‐rhythmic target condition, and 26 the random cue‐random target condition. Thirty healthy participants (22 female, 8 male; age range: 18–33 years, median = 22 years) took part in Experiment III. One participant was excluded due to low trial number. All participants provided written informed consent before testing and were fully debriefed about the goals of the study. The study was approved by the local ethics committee (CMO Arnhem‐Nijmegen).

2.2. Experimental task and stimuli

Participants performed an auditory target discrimination task in which they had to indicate whether a brief target tone either increased or decreased in pitch (Wilsch et al., 2020; Figure 1). The target tone consisted of 30 base frequencies that were randomly drawn from 500 to 1,500 Hz. We modulated the pitch to either decrease or increase over time. This modulation was adjusted for each participant individually during a practice session, with a bigger slope (i.e., a larger difference between the pitch frequency at the start and end of the stimulus) being easier, such that participants performed at approx. 75% accuracy. The tone started and ended with a 10‐ms cosine ramp fading in or out. The resulting target tone had a sample rate of 44,100 Hz and a duration of 40 ms.

FIGURE 1.

FIGURE 1

Experimental paradigm. All experiments used a variation of the auditory pitch discrimination task where a four‐tone rhythmic sequence or a continuous tone (black) cued a target tone that was either rhythmically aligned with the cue or randomly timed (red). Participants indicated by button press whether the target tone had an increasing or decreasing pitch. All combinations of (rhythmic/random cue × rhythmic/random target are shown. Solid red lines represent one presentation of the target, dashed lines show other possible timings. Note that the random target could be presented at any point during the target period

The target was preceded by a temporal auditory cue, which could be either rhythmic or random (i.e., continuous). In the rhythmic‐cue condition, we presented four identical tones at a particular presentation rate. These tones had a pitch frequency of 400 Hz, a duration of 40 ms, and a sample rate of 44,100 Hz. We used a Hanning taper to remove sharp edges. We normalized all tones, including target tones, to the same sound pressure level. In the random‐cue condition, we presented the same tone continuously for a time duration that mirrored the rhythmic‐cue window. Note that while ‘continuous cue’ might be the label that better reflects the nature of the cue, we have chosen ‘random’ such that we would have the same labels for the factors cue and target rhythmicity in this 2 × 2 design (see below).

The timing of the target presentation could similarly be either rhythmic or random. In the rhythmic‐target condition, we presented the target tones either in‐phase (80% of trials) or out‐of‐phase (20%) with respect to the preceding cue rhythm, within a window of at most four cycles, that is, in‐phase targets could occur 1, 2, 3, or 4 cycles after the cue, out‐of‐phase targets could occur 1.5, 2.5, or 3.5 cycles after the cue. In the random‐target condition, we drew the timing of the tone from a flat probability distribution, with the full window matching that of the rhythmic‐target condition. We instructed participants to respond as fast as possible via a button press.

In all three experiments, we used multiple cue frequencies, represented as their inverse or period, that is, the duration of one cycle. For trials with rhythmic cues, this meant manipulating the period of the tone stream; for trials with continuous cues, this meant simply manipulating the total duration of the cue. Similarly, we determined the timing of the target presentation according to that trial's period.

Note that on rhythmic cue‐rhythmic target trials, the rhythmic cue provides explicit temporal information with regard to target timing, whereas on rhythmic cue‐random target trials the rhythm does not provide information beyond the length of the full window in which the target can occur. In both cases, the participant could form an (automatic) rhythmic prediction, but only in the former is it helpful for the task. On random cue‐rhythmic target trials, the cue provides implicit temporal information, and cue offset can be used to predict the timing of the implicit rhythm (that is, if the participant has learned the rhythmic target‐structure over the course of a block), whereas on random cue‐random target trials there is no temporal information available beyond the full target window length. Whether these two conditions differ in terms of temporal predictions depends on whether the participant picks up on the implicit statistics of the task.

2.3. Experimental protocol

Experiment I consisted of a within‐subject 2 × 2 design with factors cue (rhythmic versus random) and target (rhythmic versus random), that is, all participants performed all combinations of rhythmic/random cue/target conditions. Additionally, we used three different periods (500, 600, and 700 ms, corresponding to 2.0, ~1.6, and ~1.4 Hz, respectively) for the rhythmic conditions, and corresponding window lengths for the random conditions. Participants performed 12 blocks of 60 trials each, with fixed condition (i.e., rhythmic‐rhythmic, rhythmic‐random, random‐rhythmic, random‐random) and period (i.e., 500, 600, and 700 ms) per block.

Experiment II consisted of a between‐subject design in which each participant performed only one of the four task conditions. We used three different periods for each participant (400, 600, and 900 ms, corresponding to 2.5, ~1.6, and ~1.1 Hz, respectively). Participants performed nine blocks of 60 trials each, with fixed period per block.

Experiment III consisted of only the rhythmic cue‐rhythmic target condition. We used 10 different periods in order to determine frequency specificity of potential temporal facilitation effects (100, 120, 150, 200, 250, 400, 600, 800, 1,000, and 1,250 ms, corresponding to 10, ~8.3, ~6.6, 5, 4, 2.5, ~1.6, 1.25, 1, and 0.8 Hz, respectively). Participants performed 10 blocks of 60 trials each, with randomized period across trials per block.

2.4. Data analysis and statistics

We analysed behavioural performance in terms of accuracy and reaction time (RT) and excluded participants with accuracy scores lower than 55% (see Section 2.1). We included trials with rhythmic targets occurring out‐of‐phase (20%) when addressing whether target phase influenced performance but removed these out‐of‐phase trials from the data for all other analyses.

We then normalized RT per participant (for raw values, see Figure S1) by dividing single‐trial RTs by the participant's mean RT and removed outlier trials outside the boundaries of Tukey fences (average excluded trials per participant; exp I: 36/480; exp II: 25/540; exp III: 28/480). Next, we equalized trial numbers across conditions by randomly omitting trials and excluded participants with fewer than five trials in any condition (resulting in one participant removed from experiment I).

Finally, on the remaining data, we calculated accuracy (% correct trials) per condition, then removed incorrect trials (average incorrect trials per participant; exp I: 156/480; exp II: 76/540; exp III: 73/480) and calculated mean RT per condition. The minimum, maximum, and mean number of trials that went into each of these contrasts are reported in Table S1.

For experiment I, we computed classical and Bayesian repeated measures ANOVAs to estimate differences in accuracy and RT using the factors cue rhythmicity (rhythmic versus random), target rhythmicity (rhythmic versus random), and period (the different cue frequencies, represented as their inverse). For experiment II we did the same but with cue rhythmicity and target rhythmicity conditions as between‐subject factors, and period as within‐subject factors. For experiment III, there was only the within‐subjects factor period. We applied Greenhouse‐Geisser correction whenever the assumption of sphericity was violated.

To estimate accuracy and RT differences in target phase (in‐phase versus out‐of‐phase) and whether they interacted with cue rhythmicity, we computed separate classical and Bayesian repeated measures ANOVAs with these two factors in experiments I and II, and a t test to contrast in‐phase versus out‐of‐phase trials in experiment III.

Across the three experiments, we modelled the hazard rate as a linear increase in accuracy, and a linear decrease in RT, across the seven possible target latencies (four in‐phase latencies and 3 out‐of‐phase latencies). For the conditions with random target latencies, we binned the latencies into seven bins corresponding to the seven rhythmic target latencies. We pooled the data from the different conditions together and extracted, for each participant, deviations from the hazard rate predictions at the different latencies (Fiebelkorn et al., 2013; Spaak et al., 2014). We then analysed those data in the same way as reported above.

3. RESULTS

3.1. No benefit of rhythmicity or in‐phase target presentation

First, we investigated whether there was any benefit of rhythmic cues and/or targets (versus random ones) on behavioural performance (RT and accuracy). We manipulated the rhythmicity of the cues and targets in experiments I (within‐subjects) and II (between‐subjects) and found the same pattern of results in both experiments (Figure 2).

FIGURE 2.

FIGURE 2

Effect of cue and target rhythmicity. In both experiment I (within‐subjects; top half) and experiment II (between‐subjects; bottom half), participants were neither faster (left) nor more accurate (right) in responding to the target when the cue was rhythmic (versus random) or when the target was rhythmic (versus random)

In experiment I, whether the cue was rhythmic or random had no effect on RT (F(1,29) = 0.86, p = 0.362, BF 10 = 0.09) nor on accuracy (F(1,29) = 2.57, p = 0.12, BF 10 = 0.172), and whether the target was rhythmic or random also had no effect on RT nor on accuracy (RT: F(1,29) = 0.22, p = 0.64, BF 10 = 0.054; accuracy: F(1,29) = 0.71, p = 0.4, BF 10 = 0.058). Similarly, in experiment II, the cue rhythmicity had no effect on RT (F(1,110) = 8.1e‐5, p = 0.92, BF 10 = 0.58) nor on accuracy (F(1,110) = 0.36, p = 0.55, BF 10 = 0.22), and target rhythmicity also had no effect on RT (F(1,110) = 2.33, p = 0.13, BF 10 = 0.38) nor on accuracy (F(1,113) = 0.15, p = 0.69, BF 10 = 0.19).

Moreover, contrary to our expectations, none of the interactions showed a significant effect. This included the interaction of interest in the context of entrainment: that is, between rhythmicity of cue and target in experiment I (RT: F(1,29) = 0.19, p = 0.66, BF 10 = 0.01; accuracy: F(1,29) = 0.04, p = 0.83, BF 10 = 0.01) and experiment II (RT: F(1,110) = 3.12, p = 0.08, B10 = 0.54; accuracy: F(1,110) = 0.12, p = 0.72, B10 = 0.12). In other words, people were not better at discriminating a target tone occurring at a predictable time point after a rhythmic cue, compared to when the target tone occurred at a random time point after a random cue.

It could be argued that potential rhythmicity effects might only be observed at early target latencies, for example, at the first possible target position, as rhythmicity effects might fade or vanish at later positions. To account for this, we repeated the above analyses only including targets occurring at the first post‐cue position and still found no effect of cue rhythmicity (RT: F(1,27) = 2.22, p = 0.147, BF10 = 0.31; accuracy: F(1,27) = 0.004, p = 0.948, B10 = 0.16) nor target rhythmicity (RT: F(1,27) = 0.905, p = 0.35, BF10 = 0.23; accuracy: F(1,27) = 1.94, p = 0.17, B10 = 0.35) in experiment I nor in experiment II (cue RT: F(1,110) = 2.18, p = 0.14, B10 = 0.37; target RT: F(1,110) = 2.54, p = 0.113, B10 = 0.442; cue accuracy: F(1,110) = 0.004, p = 0.948, B10 = 0.138; target accuracy: F(1,110) = 0.01, p = 0.92, B10 = 0.139). Note that there was a cue × target rhythmicity interaction for accuracy in experiment I (F(1,27) = 5.3, p = 0.029, B10 = 0.17). However, given that none of the post hoc contrasts were significant and that the Bayes factor indicated evidence for the null hypothesis (no interaction), we will not consider this interaction any further.

Next, we asked whether targets occurring in‐phase with a rhythmic cue were better discriminated compared to targets occurring out‐of‐phase, and found no evidence for such an effect on RT (experiment I: F(1,28) = 0.7, p = 0.41, BF10 = 0.85; experiment II F(1,57) = 2, p = 0.16, BF10 = 0.34; experiment III: t(28) = 1.24, p = 0.22; BF10 = 0.39) nor accuracy (experiment I: F(1,28) = 0.11, p = 0.74, BF10 = 1.52; experiment II F(1,57) = 0.03, p = 0.85, BF10 = 0.14; experiment III: t(28) = 0.35, p = 0.72; BF10 = 0.2) (Figure 3). This did not depend on whether the cue was rhythmic or random, that is, no significant cue‐by‐phase interaction for RT (experiment I: F(1,28) = 0.06, p = 0.8, BF10 = 0.07; experiment II: F(1,57) = 0.23, p = 0.63, BF10 = 0.13) nor accuracy (experiment I: F(1,28) = 1.82, p = 0.19, BF10 = 0.17; experiment II: F(1,57) = 0.12, p = 0.72, BF10 = 0.064).

FIGURE 3.

FIGURE 3

Effect of phase. Across all three experiments (top, middle, and bottom panels), participants were neither faster (left) nor more accurate (right) in responding to a target occurring in‐phase (versus out‐of‐phase) with the cue stream

Finally, it could be argued that phase effects are only observable for particular, individually preferred frequencies, and that including multiple rhythms dilutes this effect at the group level. To account for this concern, we repeated this analysis for experiment III, only including individually preferred rhythms (defined as the frequency with the highest accuracy) and still found no effect of in‐phase versus out‐of‐phase target presentation (t(28) = −0.06, p = 0.954, BF10 = 0.21).

3.2. Better performance for later‐occurring targets

We then asked whether the duration of the time window between cue offset and target onset (the cue‐target delay) had an influence on behavioural performance. The reasoning is that as time after cue offset increases, so does the probability of the target occurring. We would expect this hazard rate effect to lead to better performance on later occurring targets.

To disentangle this non‐rhythmic temporal expectation effect from any possible rhythmic entrainment effects, we modelled the hazard rate for each participant and extracted deviations from the predicted outcomes. We modelled accuracy at each target position as linearly increasing from the first to the last position, and reaction time as linearly decreasing. On average, the accuracy trend had a positive slope (experiment I = 0.014; experiment II = 0.012; experiment III = 0.007) and the RT trend had a negative slope (experiment I = −0.017; experiment II = −0.014; experiment III = −0.008). These slopes were significantly different from zero based on one‐sampled t tests (all p < 0.001, results in Table 1), suggesting that participants indeed performed better for later‐occurring targets (Figure 4).

TABLE 1.

Hazard rate effects

Experiment Measure t value df p value
Experiment I Accuracy 6.05 31 <0.0001
RT −3.37 31 0.002
Experiment II Accuracy 9.34 118 <0.0001
RT −7.79 118 <0.0001
Experiment III Accuracy 4.00 29 <0.0001
RT −3.95 29 <0.0001

Results from the one‐sample t‐tests (against zero) of the estimated slopes of accuracy and RT trends in all experiments. Generally, accuracy trend had a positive slope and the RT trend had a negative slope.

FIGURE 4.

FIGURE 4

Hazard rate effect. In all three experiments, we modelled the hazard rate effect across the seven possible target onsets. Left panels: participants responded faster, the later the target onset was. Right panels: participants responded more accurately, the later the target onset. Asterisk indicates slope is significantly higher or lower than zero

We then repeated the analyses reported in the previous section, but with the detrended values extracted after modelling the hazard rate. These results largely conformed with the ones reported above. That is, after accounting for the hazard rate, cue and target rhythmicity and whether the target occurred in or out of phase still had no influence on performance (Tables S2–S7). In sum, this analysis suggests that despite nonrhythmic temporal expectation effects (described by hazard rate) being present in our data, rhythmic entrainment effects were not.

3.3. Faster responses following faster rhythmic cues

Our last question whether some cueing rhythms were more behaviourally beneficial than others (Figure 5). Across all three experiments, we observed a remarkably robust speeding up of RT with faster cues (exp I: F(2,58) = 15.01, p < 0.0001, BF 10 > 100; exp II: F(2,220) = 80.42, p < 0.001, BF 10 > 100; exp III: F(9,243) = 34.87, p < 0.001, BF 10 > 100). This effect was most evident in experiment III, which included 10 different periods rather than three, and where the cues were exclusively rhythmic and varied trial‐wise (in frequency) rather than block‐wise. Accuracy also increased with faster cues in experiment II (F(2,220) = 6.6, p = 0.002, BF 10 = 4.46), but not in experiments I and III (exp I: F(2,58) = 0.1, p = 0.9, BF 10 = 0.014; exp III: F(9,243) = 0.93, p = 0.49, BF 10 = 0.018), suggesting the RT effect is not necessarily reflective of a speed‐accuracy trade‐off.

FIGURE 5.

FIGURE 5

Effect of period. Left panels: In all three experiments, participants were faster to respond on trials with faster cueing frequencies (shorter period). Right panels: In experiment II (middle) but not experiments I and III (top and bottom), participants were more accurate in responding on trials with slower cueing frequencies (longer period). *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001

In experiments I and II, where cues and targets could be rhythmic or random, we then asked whether the rhythmicity of cues and targets interacted with the RT effect of period reported here (Figure 6). In experiment I, the effect of faster cues on RT depended neither on the rhythmicity of the cue (F(2,58) = 3.05, p = 0.06, BF 10 = 0.1), nor that of the target (F(2,58) = 0.17, p = 0.83, BF 10 = 0.01). In experiment II, the RT effect of period did not depend on target rhythmicity (F(2,220) = 1.014, p = 0.32, BF 10 = 0.54), but it did depend on cue rhythmicity (F(2,220) = 5.16, p = 0.006, BF 10 = 2.35), such that rhythmic cues led to more RT speed‐up. We could not test this in experiment III because we only used rhythmic cues. Finally, the RT effect of period held robustly after accounting for the hazard rate effect (Tables S2–S4). Overall, the speeding up of RT following faster cues seems to benefit weakly from rhythmic cueing.

FIGURE 6.

FIGURE 6

Interaction between period and cue/target properties. Left panels: In experiments I and II, the speeding up of responses with faster cueing frequencies (shorter period) was more prominent when the cue was rhythmic (compared to random). Middle panels: In both experiments there was no interaction between the cueing frequencies and the target rhythmicity. Left panels: In both experiments there was no interaction between the cueing frequencies and the target timing. Error bars represent standard error of mean

4. DISCUSSION

In a series of three experiments, we found no behavioural benefit of rhythmic cueing, compared to random cueing (i.e., a nonrhythmic continuous tone), neither on a within‐subjects level (experiment I) nor on a between‐subjects level (experiment II). We also found no behavioural advantage for targets appearing at a rhythmically consistent timing, compared to those appearing at a random timing. In addition, we found no behavioural benefit for targets occurring in‐phase with rhythmic cues, compared to those occurring out‐of‐phase. However, we found that shortening the duration of the cue—that is, speeding it up—consistently resulted in faster reaction times.

The idea of neural entrainment as a mechanism to facilitate sensory processing rests on the assumption that neural oscillations reflect rhythmic phases of high and low neural excitability that coincide with phases of good and bad perceptual performance, respectively. This is supported by evidence that such phase effects occur spontaneously, that is, without exposure to an external rhythm (e.g., Busch et al., 2009; Mathewson et al., 2009). Within the entrainment framework, it is then thought that these internal phases can be adjusted to external rhythms, potentially providing a mechanism for temporal prediction. The influence of external rhythms on perception and subsequent behaviour has been tentatively shown (e.g., Jones et al., 2002, 2006), but these results are now being scrutinized by the field, for example in the current special issue (also see Haegens & Golumbic, 2018 for review). From an electrophysiological point of view, it has been difficult to show that neural oscillatory phase entrains to external rhythms (e.g., Wilsch et al., 2020), as an observed ‘entrained’ brain rhythm is difficult to disentangle from a series of evoked responses, a series of top‐down predictions, or simple resonance (Haegens, 2020; Helfrich et al., 2019; Obleser & Kayser, 2019). Since we did not collect electrophysiological data in our studies, we will restrict our discussion to the behavioural aspect of entrainment.

If the assumptions of entrainment are met, one would expect the entrained neural oscillations (and hence the concomitant behavioural benefit) to persist after the external rhythm stops (Lakatos et al., 2008). That rhythmicity in input streams offers perceptual and behavioural advantages has been shown repeatedly (Henry & Obleser, 2012; Jones et al., 2002, 2006; Rohenkohl et al., 2011; Rohenkohl & Nobre, 2011); however, most of these studies report these advantages when targets occur within rhythmic streams, with very few reporting advantages persisting after the stream stops. To the best of our knowledge, the few studies that have reported a persistent advantage have relied on relatively low numbers of participants (e.g., Farahbod et al., 2020; Hickok et al., 2015; five participants each; Mathewson et al., 2010; Mathewson et al., 2012; 13–16 participants each) and did not explicitly test for temporal predictions (i.e., rhythmic cues were uninformative). In our current study we tested whether rhythmicity in an auditory cue stream influences the discrimination of a target occurring after offset of the stream and found no such evidence.

There are several possible explanations for these discrepancies: first, the nature of the task (detection versus discrimination) could play a role in limiting the behavioural facilitation of neural entrainment (Bauer et al., 2015). Rhythmic facilitation has been observed in demanding detection tasks, for example where near‐threshold targets are embedded in noise (e.g., Ten Oever et al., 2017), and arguably more precise temporal predictions are needed. Though our stimuli were short‐lived and the discrimination task fairly demanding, it is possible that rhythmic facilitation effects are only (or mostly) relevant in paradigms where time is a more critical factor, such as in speeded tasks, time estimation, and near‐threshold detection. However, if that is the case, it would argue against entrainment as an automatic, bottom‐up effect (Haegens & Golumbic, 2018). It is also possible that rhythmicity only impacts early‐occurring targets as the effect fades over time, but we found no evidence for this in our data. Furthermore, it has been suggested that temporal expectation effects mostly boost other forms of attention and prediction (Morillon et al., 2016; Rohenkohl et al., 2014), that is, perhaps by themselves the effects are too weak to detect in most scenarios.

Other possibilities are that four cue tones were not enough to build temporal expectations (though see Breska & Deouell, 2014), or that the variability in the rhythms used made it harder to build expectations and predict cue onset (though note we used a blocked design in experiments I and II). The predictability of rhythmic cues was previously shown to correlate with the degree of phase alignment of brain oscillations (Stefanics et al., 2010; but see Breska & Deouell, 2017), something we cannot assess here as we did not collect electrophysiological data. Furthermore, one could posit that if participants are exposed to both rhythmic and random cues in a single experiment, a less cognitively‐demanding strategy is to entirely ignore the cues (both rhythmic and random) as they provide no perceived behavioural benefit. However, our experiment III was designed with cue rhythmicity as a between‐subject factor to avoid such a carry‐over effect, and nevertheless we found no effect of cue rhythmicity.

Further, there might be interindividual variability in preferred frequency (and phase) on which such behavioural benefits depend (Zoefel et al., 2018), so the use of one frequency (and phase) for all participants might not lead to an observable effect at the group level. In all our experiments we used multiple frequencies (three in experiment I‐II and 10 in experiment III) and still did not find an impact of cue rhythmicity for any of the frequencies, not even when taking into account interindividual variability in preferred frequencies. Another possible source of interindividual variability is that different people might have different sensitivity to entrainment (Assaneo et al., 2019). If this were the case in our experiments, we would expect the individual‐level effects to be bi‐modally distributed, but the distributions of effects do not suggest this (Figures S2–S4). Finally, musical ability and training have been suggested as factors contributing to interindividual differences in entrainment, with musicians being better entrainers (Doelling & Poeppel, 2015). However, if true, would render entrainment less likely as a candidate mechanism for general temporal prediction.

Another question we aimed to address was whether different cueing frequencies had different effects on behaviour either due to the mechanistic roles ascribed to these frequencies: ramping up activity at slower frequencies (delta to theta) could lead to behavioural facilitation, as these low‐frequency rhythms are thought to play a role in sensory sampling (Fiebelkorn & Kastner, 2019; VanRullen, 2016), while a similar increase in higher frequencies (especially alpha) could lead to a behavioural cost, as these oscillations are thought to play a role in functional inhibition (Klimesch et al., 2007). We found no evidence for certain frequencies inducing differential behavioural effects. Instead, we found that faster cues led to faster responses, a robust effect observed in all three experiments but most strikingly in experiment III, which was designed to address this question on a trial‐by‐trial level.

In paradigms with varying cue‐target delays, RTs are usually faster and accuracy scores higher on trials with long delays, as uncertainty of target timing decreases the later the target occurs (hazard rate effect; Näätänen, 1971). This hazard rate effect was present in our data but did not confound any of our results. In fact, all our results held even after accounting for the hazard rate. In our experiment, faster cues were followed by earlier‐occurring targets on average, so based on the hazard rate prediction, one would expect responses to faster cues to be slower, but we found them to be faster. Taken together, these findings suggest that hazard rate and the observed RT speed‐up with faster cues were dissociated in our data.

Our results can tentatively be explained with the notion of covert active sensing: that is, the motor system actively coordinates the sensory system to adjust to the current environment (Schroeder et al., 2010). As a result, a faster external rhythm might increase the communication rate between the sensory and motor cortices. A faster communication rate in turn gives a faster response on average, since input can be sampled earlier in time. This interpretation is particularly supported by experiment III, where different frequencies were randomized across trials (i.e., not blocked as in the first two experiments), suggesting this is a rapidly adaptive mechanism suitable for real‐life situations with varying temporal (ir‐)regularities. Future work should further address this potential mechanism on the neural level, and, more generally, the role of the motor system in (rhythmic) temporal prediction (Balasubramaniam et al., 2021; Cannon & Patel, 2020).

Finally, we found that faster responses followed faster cues, particularly when the cues were rhythmic. The RT speed‐up depended on cue rhythmicity in experiment II and showed a similar trend in experiment I (note that we used smaller differences between periods in experiment I). We do not offer conclusive evidence as to whether this RT effect is exclusive to rhythmic contexts, but rhythmicity has been shown to improve response readiness (Morillon et al., 2016). Future work should address this question by manipulating cue rhythmicity on a trial‐by‐trial basis, in addition to independently varying the speed and duration of the cue. That is, in our current design, length of cue and target window scaled with the frequency of the cued rhythm (since we used a fixed number of cycles). Separately manipulating these factors would allow disentangling effects driven by the cue rhythm per se, versus effects driven by the task rhythm. This would provide more insight into whether this speed‐up is specific to micro (i.e., within trial) rhythmic contexts or reflects a more general adaptation to faster macro (i.e., across trials) rhythms.

CONFLICTS OF INTEREST

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS

HE, SH, ER, and AW conceived and designed the study. IB‐M, JE, WML, and DO collected the data. HE, WML, DO, and ER analysed the data. HE, SH, WML, DO, & ER wrote the manuscript.

Peer Review

The peer review history for this article is available at https://publons.com/publon/10.1111/ejn.15208.

Supporting information

Fig S1

Fig S2

Fig S3

Fig S4

Supplementary Material

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

We would like to thank Aarti Ramchandran and Camille Gret for assistance with data collection, and Sanne ten Oever for thoughtful comments on an earlier version of the manuscript. This work was supported by NWO grants Veni 451‐14‐027 and 016.Vidi.185.137 to SH.

Lin WM, Oetringer DA, Bakker‐Marshall I, et al. No behavioural evidence for rhythmic facilitation of perceptual discrimination. Eur J Neurosci. 2022;55(11–12): 3352–3364. 10.1111/ejn.15208

Edited by: Manuela Ruzzoli

DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENT

The data that support the findings of this study and the code that was used to analyse it will be publicly available after publication, on the OSF page https://osf.io/spt24/.

REFERENCES

  1. Assaneo, M. F. , Ripollés, P. , Orpella, J. , Lin, W. M. , de Diego‐Balaguer, R. , & Poeppel, D. (2019). Spontaneous synchronization to speech reveals neural mechanisms facilitating language learning. Nature Neuroscience, 22(4), 627–632. 10.1038/s41593-019-0353-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Balasubramaniam, R. , Haegens, S. , Jazayeri, M. , Merchant, H. , Sternad, D. , & Song, J.‐H. (2021). Neural encoding and representation of time for sensorimotor control and learning. Journal of Neuroscience, 41(5), 866–872. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1652-20.2020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Barnes, R. , & Johnston, H. (2010). The role of timing deviations and target position uncertainty on temporal attending in a serial auditory pitch discrimination task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63(2), 341–355. 10.1080/17470210902925312 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Başar, E. , Aygölü, Ü. , Panayırcı, E. , & Poor, H. V. (2013). Orthogonal frequency division multiplexing with index modulation. IEEE Transactions on Signal Processing, 61(22), 5536–5549. 10.1109/TSP.2013.2279771 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bauer, A.‐K.‐R. , Jaeger, M. , Thorne, J. D. , Bendixen, A. , & Debener, S. (2015). The auditory dynamic attending theory revisited: A closer look at the pitch comparison task. Brain Research, 1626, 198–210. 10.1016/j.brainres.2015.04.032 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Benwell, C. S. , Tagliabue, C. F. , Veniero, D. , Cecere, R. , Savazzi, S. , & Thut, G. (2017). Prestimulus EEG power predicts conscious awareness but not objective visual performance. Eneuro, 4(6). 10.1523/ENEURO.0182-17.2017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bishop, G. H. (1932). Cyclic changes in excitability of the optic pathway of the rabbit. American Journal of Physiology‐Legacy Content, 103(1), 213–224. 10.1152/ajplegacy.1932.103.1.213 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bosker, H. R. , & Kösem, A. (2017). An entrained rhythm's frequency, not phase, influences temporal sampling of speech. In Proceedings of Interspeech 2017 (pp. 2416–2420). 10.21437/Interspeech.2017-73 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  9. Breska, A. , & Deouell, L. Y. (2014). Automatic bias of temporal expectations following temporally regular input independently of high‐level temporal expectation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 26(7), 1555–1571. 10.1162/jocn_a_00564 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Breska, A. , & Deouell, L. Y. (2017). Neural mechanisms of rhythm‐based temporal prediction: Delta phase‐locking reflects temporal predictability but not rhythmic entrainment. PLoS Biology, 15(2), e2001665. 10.1371/journal.pbio.2001665 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Busch, N. A. , Dubois, J. , & VanRullen, R. (2009). The phase of ongoing EEG oscillations predicts visual perception. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(24), 7869–7876. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0113-09.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Cannon, J. J. , & Patel, A. D. (2020). How beat perception co‐opts motor neurophysiology. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 25(2):137–150. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Doelling, K. B. , & Poeppel, D. (2015). Cortical entrainment to music and its modulation by expertise. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 112(45), E6233–E6242. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Farahbod, H. , Saberi, K. , & Hickok, G. (2020). The rhythm of attention: Perceptual modulation via rhythmic entrainment is lowpass and attention mediated. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 82(7), 3558–3570. 10.3758/s13414-020-02095-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Fiebelkorn, I. C. , & Kastner, S. (2019). A rhythmic theory of attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 23(2), 87–101. 10.1016/j.tics.2018.11.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Fiebelkorn, I. C. , Saalmann, Y. B. , & Kastner, S. (2013). Rhythmic sampling within and between objects despite sustained attention at a cued location. Current Biology, 23(24), 2553–2558. 10.1016/j.cub.2013.10.063 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Haegens, S. (2020). Entrainment revisited: A commentary on Meyer, Sun, and Martin. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 35, 1119–1123. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Haegens, S. , & Golumbic, E. Z. (2018). Rhythmic facilitation of sensory processing: A critical review. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 86, 150–165. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.12.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Helfrich, R. F. , Breska, A. , & Knight, R. T. (2019). Neural entrainment and network resonance in support of top‐down guided attention. Current Opinion in Psychology, 29, 82–89. 10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.12.016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Henry, M. J. , & Obleser, J. (2012). Frequency modulation entrains slow neural oscillations and optimizes human listening behavior. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109(49), 20095–20100. 10.1073/pnas.1213390109 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Hickok, G. , Farahbod, H. , & Saberi, K. (2015). The rhythm of perception: Entrainment to acoustic rhythms induces subsequent perceptual oscillation. Psychological Science, 26(7), 1006–1013. 10.1177/0956797615576533 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Jones, M. R. , Johnston, H. M. , & Puente, J. (2006). Effects of auditory pattern structure on anticipatory and reactive attending. Cognitive Psychology, 53(1), 59–96. 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2006.01.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Jones, M. R. , Moynihan, H. , MacKenzie, N. , & Puente, J. (2002). Temporal aspects of stimulus‐driven attending in dynamic arrays. Psychological Science, 13(4), 313–319. 10.1111/1467-9280.00458 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Klimesch, W. , Sauseng, P. , & Hanslmayr, S. (2007). Jensen and Mazaheri. Brain Research Reviews, 53(1), 63–88. 10.1016/J.BRAINRESREV.2006.06.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Lakatos, P. , Gross, J. , & Thut, G. (2019). A new unifying account of the roles of neuronal entrainment. Current Biology, 29(18), R890–R905. 10.1016/j.cub.2019.07.075 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Lakatos, P. , Karmos, G. , Mehta, A. D. , Ulbert, I. , & Schroeder, C. E. (2008). Entrainment of neuronal oscillations as a mechanism of attentional selection. Science, 320(5872), 110–113. 10.1126/science.1154735 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Mathewson, K. E. , Fabiani, M. , Gratton, G. , Beck, D. M. , & Lleras, A. (2010). Rescuing stimuli from invisibility: Inducing a momentary release from visual masking with pre‐target entrainment. Cognition, 115(1), 186–191. 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.11.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Mathewson, K. E. , Gratton, G. , Fabiani, M. , Beck, D. M. , & Ro, T. (2009). To see or not to see: Prestimulus α phase predicts visual awareness. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(9), 2725–2732. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Mathewson, K. E. , Prudhomme, C. , Fabiani, M. , Beck, D. M. , Lleras, A. , & Gratton, G. (2012). Making waves in the stream of consciousness: Entraining oscillations in EEG alpha and fluctuations in visual awareness with rhythmic visual stimulation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24(12), 2321–2333. 10.1162/jocn_a_00288 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Meyer, L. , Sun, Y. , & Martin, A. E. (2019). Synchronous, but not entrained: Exogenous and endogenous cortical rhythms of speech and language processing. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 35(9), 1089–1099. 10.1080/23273798.2019.1693050 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  31. Morillon, B. , Schroeder, C. E. , Wyart, V. , & Arnal, L. H. (2016). Temporal prediction in lieu of periodic stimulation. Journal of Neuroscience, 36(8), 2342–2347. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0836-15.2016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Näätänen, R. (1971). Non‐aging fore‐periods and simple reaction time. Acta Psychologica, 35(4), 316–327. 10.1016/0001-6918(71)90040-0 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  33. Obleser, J. , & Kayser, C. (2019). Neural entrainment and attentional selection in the listening brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 23(11), 913–926. 10.1016/j.tics.2019.08.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. O'Hare, J. (1954). The variability of audiory and visual reaction time with change in amplitude and phase of alpha rhythm. American Psychologist, 9, 444. [Google Scholar]
  35. Rohenkohl, G. , Coull, J. T. , & Nobre, A. C. (2011). Behavioural dissociation between exogenous and endogenous temporal orienting of attention. PLoS One, 6(1), e14620. 10.1371/journal.pone.0014620 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Rohenkohl, G. , Gould, I. C. , Pessoa, J. , & Nobre, A. C. (2014). Combining spatial and temporal expectations to improve visual perception. Journal of Vision, 14(4), 8. 10.1167/14.4.8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Rohenkohl, G. , & Nobre, A. C. (2011). Alpha oscillations related to anticipatory attention follow temporal expectations. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(40), 14076–14084. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3387-11.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Ruzzoli, M. , Torralba, M. , Fernández, L. M. , & Soto‐Faraco, S. (2019). The relevance of alpha phase in human perception. Cortex, 120, 249–268. 10.1016/j.cortex.2019.05.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Schroeder, C. E. , Wilson, D. A. , Radman, T. Scharfman, H. , & Lakatos, P. (2010). Dynamics of Active Sensing and perceptual selection. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 20(2), 172–176. 10.1016/j.conb.2010.02.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Spaak, E. , de Lange, F. P. , & Jensen, O. (2014). Local entrainment of alpha oscillations by visual stimuli causes cyclic modulation of perception. Journal of Neuroscience, 34(10), 3536–3544. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4385-13.2014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Stefanics, G. , Hangya, B. , Hernádi, I. , Winkler, I. , Lakatos, P. , & Ulbert, I. (2010). Phase entrainment of human delta oscillations can mediate the effects of expectation on reaction speed. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(41), 13578–13585. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0703-10.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Ten Oever, S. , & Sack, A. T. (2019). Interactions between rhythmic temporal and feature predictions for parallel time‐content associations. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 13, 791. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Ten Oever, S. , Schroeder, C. E. , Poeppel, D. , Van Atteveldt, N. , Mehta, A. D. , Mégevand, P. , Groppe, D. M. , & Zion‐Golumbic, E. (2017). Low‐frequency cortical oscillations entrain to subthreshold rhythmic auditory stimuli. Journal of Neuroscience, 37(19), 4903–4912. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3658-16.2017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. VanRullen, R. (2016). Perceptual cycles. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(10), 723–735. 10.1016/j.tics.2016.07.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Vigué‐Guix, I. , Morís Fernández, L. , Torralba Cuello, M. , Ruzzoli, M. , & Soto‐Faraco, S. (2020). Can the occipital alpha‐phase speed up visual detection through a real‐time EEG‐based brain–computer interface (BCI)? European Journal of Neuroscience, 1–17. 10.1111/ejn.14931. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Walsh, E. (1952). Visual reaction time and the α‐rhythm, an investigation of a scanning hypothesis. The Journal of Physiology, 118(4), 500. 10.1113/jphysiol.1952.sp004811 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Wilsch, A. , Mercier, M. , Obleser, J. , Schroeder, C. E. , & Haegens, S. (2020). Spatial attention and temporal expectation exert differential effects on visual and auditory discrimination. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 32(8), 1562–1576. 10.1162/jocn_a_01567 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Zoefel, B. , Archer‐Boyd, A. , & Davis, M. H. (2018). Phase entrainment of brain oscillations causally modulates neural responses to intelligible speech. Current Biology, 28(3), 401–408. 10.1016/j.cub.2017.11.071 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Fig S1

Fig S2

Fig S3

Fig S4

Supplementary Material

Data Availability Statement

The data that support the findings of this study and the code that was used to analyse it will be publicly available after publication, on the OSF page https://osf.io/spt24/.


Articles from The European Journal of Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES