Skip to main content
Wiley Open Access Collection logoLink to Wiley Open Access Collection
. 2021 Dec 8;62(2):355–364. doi: 10.1111/trf.16755

Sexual risk behavior questions: Understanding and mitigating donor discomfort

Jennie Haw 1,2,, Hyunjin Woo 3, Taylor Kohut 4, William Fisher 5
PMCID: PMC9542493  PMID: 34877677

Abstract

Background

Blood operators are working to improve donor screening and eligibility for gay, bisexual and other men who have sex with men (gbMSM), and trans and nonbinary donors. Many consider screening all donors for specific sexual risk behaviors to be a more equitable approach that maintains the safety of the blood supply. Feasibility considerations with this change include ensuring donor understanding of additional sexual behavior questions and minimizing donor loss due to discomfort.

Study design and methods

Qualitative one‐on‐one interviews were conducted with Canadian whole blood and plasma donors (N = 40). A thematic analysis was conducted to assess participants' understandings of the questions, examine their comfort/discomfort, and identify strategies to mitigate donor discomfort.

Results

All participants understood what the sexual behavior questions were asking and thought the questions were appropriate. Themes related to comfort/discomfort include: their expectations of donor screening, social norms that they bring to donation, whether their answer felt like personal disclosure, knowing the reasons for the question, trusting confidentiality, confidence in knowing their sexual partner's behavior, and potential for the question to be discriminatory. Strategies to mitigate discomfort include: providing an explanation for the questions, forewarning donors of these questions, reducing ambiguity, and using a self‐administered questionnaire.

Conclusion

While many blood operators and regulators view the move to sexual behavior–based screening for all donors as a significant paradigmatic shift, donors may not perceive additional sexual behavior questions as a significant change to their donation experience. Further research is needed to evaluate the effectiveness of strategies to mitigate donor discomfort.

Keywords: behavior‐based screening, blood donors, discomfort, donor questionnaire, gbMSM, plasma donors, sexual behavior questions

1. INTRODUCTION

Blood operators around the world are working to improve donor screening and eligibility policies for gay, bisexual and other men who have sex with men (gbMSM) and trans and nonbinary donors. 1 , 2 , 3 , 4 , 5 Currently in Canada, gbMSM must abstain from any sexual activity for a minimum of 3 months to be eligible to donate whole blood. Critics of this population‐based approach argue that it is discriminatory and inequitable because it excludes all sexually active gbMSM and some trans women and men, screens populations based on sexual identity and not individual risk behaviors, and does not recognize within‐group differences in risk among these populations. 6 , 7 , 8 , 9 , 10 Moreover, advocates for change argue that the current screening approach, first implemented in the 1980s, is no longer acceptable today, given advancements in testing technologies and scientific evidence. 11 , 12 Current policies have also been criticized for limiting social inclusion and full participation in meaningful acts of social citizenship for gbMSM and gender diverse communities. 13 , 14 Screening all donors for specific sexual risk behaviors is an alternative to population‐based screening that many gbMSM and proponents view as more equitable and acceptable. 15 , 16 This approach is being taken up by an increasing number of blood operators globally, including Canadian Blood Services. 3 , 17 , 18 , 19

In Canada, donors must complete the donor questionnaire (DQ) either on a web‐based app prior to arriving at the donor center or on a digital tablet at the center prior to donation. Donors complete one of two gendered versions of the DQ based on their male/female (binary options only) designation in the computerized donor system. The two questions currently under consideration for removal are, “In the last 3 months, have you had sex with a man?” (asked to donors registered as male) and “In the last 3 months, have you had sex with a man who, in the last 12 months, had sex with another man?” (asked to donors registered as female). If a donor answers “yes” to these questions, they are deferred for 3 months since the date of their last sexual contact. Prior to donation, donors are required to read an information pamphlet that includes a section on the possibility of HIV and hepatitis transmission through sexual contact with a footnote defining “sexual contact” as vaginal, oral, and anal intercourse with or without a condom.

Implementing a sexual behavior–based screening approach would remove the two questions above and add specific sexual behavior questions (e.g., concerning sexual activity with a new sexual partner or multiple sexual partners) in the DQ. Blood operators must consider: (1) the validity and reliability of sexual behavior questions to maintain safety of the blood supply and (2) understandability and acceptability of the questions to new and current donors to ensure sufficiency of the blood supply is maintained. While most donors would not be deterred from donating if asked new questions, 20 some concern remains that discomfort with these questions may lead to donor loss and compromise sufficiency. 20 , 21

We report analytic results of qualitative data from Canadian blood and plasma donors that assess their views on alternative sexual behavior questions that may potentially be added to the DQ. Our analysis extends extant research in two ways. First, by examining both comfort and discomfort with alternative sexual behavior questions, rather than applying an a priori assumption assessing discomfort only, a more thorough understanding of the impact of these questions on donors is possible. Second, results provide donor insights on how to mitigate discomfort with new sexual behavior questions.

2. MATERIALS AND METHODS

Qualitative data were collected to explore donors' understanding and acceptability of and comfort level with alternative sexual behavior questions in the DQ. Research Ethics Board approval was received from Canadian Blood Services and Western University.

2.1. Participant recruitment

Purposive and maximum variation sampling were used to identify and select individuals who would be knowledgeable about the topic of interest and provide as diverse a range of perspectives as possible. 22 , 23 , 24 Study participants were recruited through Canadian Blood Services' donor database. Inclusion criteria were: (1) 18 years or older, (2) able to speak and understand English, (3) donated whole blood (WB) or plasma at least once in the previous 12 months, and 4) not been recruited in the previous 6 months to participate in a study. Recruitment emails were sent to randomly selected donors meeting inclusion criteria, stratified by region and donation type (WB and plasma). In total, 400 recruitment emails were sent with an aim to obtain a sample of N = 40 donors (20 WB and 20 plasma) since 20–30 interviews are generally sufficient to achieve saturation of themes. 24 Donors who received a recruitment email were directed to a secure online platform to complete a prescreening questionnaire asking their age, gender, sexual orientation, city of residence, ethnicity, highest education level, and approximate number of donations. Ninety‐two donors responded (23% response rate) and 40 were selected based on their answers in the prescreening questionnaire. Participants were offered a $25 gift card for participation in an interview.

2.2. Data collection and analysis

Semi‐structured one‐on‐one interviews were conducted by JH from January 2021–March 2021. Participants were given the option to complete the interview either by videoconference (n = 23) or by telephone (n = 17). Prior to beginning the interview, verbal consent was obtained from all participants and interviews were audio‐recorded with their consent. Interviews ranged from 30 to 75 min.

Interviews included both open‐ended, exploratory questions and a “cognitive‐interview” component 25 , 26 whereby participants were asked to provide detailed responses regarding their understanding of, and comfort with, sexual risk behavior screening questions. Following the presentation of each question, participants were asked to reflect on what they understood the question to be asking, any points of ambiguity, their comfort/discomfort with answering the question in the DQ, and their views on how comfortable/uncomfortable others might feel answering the question in the DQ. Cognitive interview questions included sex assigned at birth, current gender, and sexual behaviors; however, this article reports analysis of responses to the sexual behavior questions only (See Table 1 for cognitive interview questions and topics explored).

TABLE 1.

Interview guide

Cognitive interview questions Topics explored with open‐ended questions

Q1: What was your sex assigned at birth?

Q2: What is your current gender?

Q3: In the last 3 months, have you had a new sexual partner? a

Q4: In the last 3 months, have you and your sexual partner only had sex with each other? a

Q5: In the last 3 months, have you had anal sex? a

  • Experiences with blood/plasma donation

  • Reasons for donating

  • Experiences with completing the DQ

  • Awareness of Canadian Blood Services' efforts to advance screening and eligibility for gbMSM and trans, nonbinary, and gender diverse donors

  • Views on Canadian Blood Services' efforts

a

Analysis of this question is included in the article.

Audio‐recordings were transcribed by a professional transcriptionist and de‐identified. Analysis was conducted using NVivo 12 qualitative analysis software. Data analysis was informed by thematic 27 and grounded theory 28 approaches. JH and HW independently open‐coded the same two interview transcripts to identify codes. These included categorical codes informed by questions asked, such as “understanding” of each sexual behavior question, and thematic codes to capture emergent themes in the data, such as “confidentiality” and “equity.” Next, they compared and discussed interview transcripts and codes to develop a coding framework. JH and HW then independently coded two additional transcripts using the coding framework and compared application of codes to ensure codes were applied consistently. JH and HW divided the remaining transcripts and coded according to the framework.

3. RESULTS

3.1. Summary of participants

Study participants ranged in age from 21–80+ with most either ≤29 (27.5%, n = 11) or ≥ 60 (27.5%, n = 11). Compared to all current active donors during the same period, our sample population has disproportionately greater representation from donors aged 20–29 and 60 years and older (see Table 2 for details). Just over half identified as male (52.5%, n = 21), 45.0% (n = 18) identified as female, and one participant identified as nonbinary. This deviates slightly from the gender breakdown of the donor population, which has slightly more donors registered as female (53.6%) than male (46.4%). Choosing a gender outside the binary is currently not an option for donors due to limitations in the computerized donor system. Study participants were more equitably distributed across the four regions than the donor population. Most participants identified as heterosexual (90.0%, n = 36), with two identifying as bisexual, and one each queer and questioning. Most identified as Caucasian (77.5%, n = 31), and nine participants (22.5%) indicated a visible ethnocultural identity. Participants were mostly return donors with 85.0% (n = 34) having donated at least 4 times.

TABLE 2.

Summary of participants

Participant characteristics (N = 40) n % Donor population characteristics b (N = 389,342) n %
Age Age
19 a 0 0.0 17–19 17, 595 4.5
20–29 11 27.5 20–29 85,022 21.8
30–39 7 17.5 30–39 78,645 20.2
40–49 5 12.5 40–49 65,298 16.8
50–59 6 15.0 50–59 72,838 18.7
60–69 7 17.5 60–69 55,232 14.2
70+ 4 10.0 70+ 14,712 3.8
Gender Gender
Female 18 45.0 Female 208,637 53.6
Male 21 52.5 Male 180,705 46.4
Nonbinary 1 2.5 Nonbinary u/k u/k
Region Region
BC/Yukon 10 25.0 BC/Yukon 66,630 17.1
Prairies 12 30.0 Prairies 118,290 30.3
Ontario 12 30.0 Ontario 166,452 42.8
Atlantic 6 15.0 Atlantic 37,970 9.8
Sexuality
Heterosexual 36 90.0
Bisexual 2 5.0
Queer 1 2.5
Questioning 1 2.5
Ethnicity
Caucasian 31 77.5
Middle Eastern 2 5.0
Chinese 2 5.0
East Asian 2 5.0
Filipino 1 2.5
South Asian 1 2.5
Bi‐racial 1 2.5
No. of donations
1–3 5 12.5
4–20 11 27.5
21–100 11 27.5
101–999 11 27.5
1000+ 1 2.5
Unknown 1 2.5
a

Minimum age of recruitment was 19.

b

All unique whole blood and plasma donors over the same period of recruitment (February 2020–January 2021).

3.2. Understandings and ambiguities

All participants indicated that they understood what the three sexual behavior questions were asking. Interpretations of Q3, “In the last 3 months, have you had a new sexual partner?” and Q4, “In the last 3 months, have you and your partner only had sex with each other?” were understood as either asking about: (1) sexual behavior or (2) the relationship. Notably, more participants understood Q4 as asking about “infidelity” in their relationship, than Q3. Several participants also thought Q4 might be seeking to identify people in “open relationships” (WB09: 37 years/F/questioning/Caucasian). For Q5, “In the last 3 months, have you had anal sex?”, most participants understood this question as asking about a specific sexual behavior and described the question as “direct” and “clear.”

Regarding ambiguities with question wording, all participants said they would not have any difficulties answering the questions, but many asked for clarification regarding some terms. Participants thought “sex,” “new,” and “partner” could be interpreted in multiple ways. For example, several thought “sex” could refer to a range of activities: “a lot of people only think of sex as penetrative sex or even more, specifically, penetrative vaginal sex. And I know many other people would think a lot of other things, like what is the clinical word for, manual sex?” (WB01: 27 years/nonbinary/queer/Caucasian). “New” might be asking about a person with whom they have engaged in sexual activity for the first time ever or someone with whom they had sex with in the past, stopped, and had sex again within the last 3 months. Several participants wondered if “partner” was referring to “a sexual relationship, like an actual relationship, rather than a one‐night stand” (P07: 23 years/F/bisexual/Filipino). Several participants also questioned how “anal sex” was defined in Q5. For example, “you'd have to consider giving or receiving.” (WB10: 68 years/M/heterosexual/Caucasian). Several participants thought ambiguities in these questions might make it challenging for some people to answer and thought greater clarity could improve accuracy of answers.

3.3. Reasons for comfort and discomfort

Overall, most participants expressed feeling comfortable answering all the sexual behavior questions in the context of donation. All participants thought the questions were acceptable and would not be deterred from donating if they were asked. Participants' feelings of comfort and discomfort with sexual behavior questions are related to the following themes: expectations of donor screening, social norms that donors bring to donation, whether their answer felt like personal disclosure, knowing the reasons for the questions, trusting confidentiality, confidence in knowing their sexual partner's behavior, and potential for the question to be discriminatory.

3.3.1. Comfort

Many participants explained that they were comfortable with all three questions since they were consistent with their expectations of donor screening, which already includes personal questions. They considered the proposed questions as no more uncomfortable than those in the current DQ. As one donor explained, “It's not that dissimilar from… asking if you've paid money for sex” (P18: 66 years/M/heterosexual/Caucasian). Several participants thought a question asking about anal sex was “basically stating the [current] question in a different way” (WB10: 68 years/M/heterosexual/Caucasian) and therefore no more uncomfortable than the current question. One participant said he was comfortable with the question because “this [is] a common thing to ask at an STI clinic” (P11: 21 years/M/heterosexual/South Asian) suggesting that donors who attend sexual health clinics may bring these expectations to the donor setting.

Participants felt comfortable with the sexual behavior questions asking about a “new” partner or “exclusivity” of partner because these questions did not deviate from their social norms regarding appropriate topics for public discussion. For example, “sexual partners, people talk about that [in] day‐to‐day conversation” (WB16: 32 years/F/heterosexual/Caucasian). Several participants, however, thought donors from different cultural or social backgrounds might experience discomfort. “Some people from specific cultures might have issues answering this [Q3] question, like especially in cultures where premarital sex is not exactly accepted” (P05: 36 years/M/heterosexual/Middle Eastern).

Participants expressed comfort with all three questions because they would answer “no,” and therefore answering the question did not feel like personal disclosure. They explained that answering was “easy” and/or would always be “no” since they are in a long‐term relationship or not sexually active. Interestingly, results show that regardless of how the sexual behavior questions were phrased, participants generally understood a “no” answer as indicating not engaging in an activity that could lead to deferral. Most participants also said they felt comfortable answering the questions because they know the reasons why they are asked in donor screening. At least two participants, however, said they did not know why the questions were necessary but were comfortable with answering because their answers would always be “no.” This suggests that if a donor answers “no” to the questions, knowing the reason for the question may be less relevant to comfort level. Lastly, participants explained that knowing their answers would be kept confidential contributed to their comfort with sexual behavior questions. Underlying this was their trust in the blood operator to keep their answers private.

3.3.2. Discomfort

Participants thought that newer, less experienced donors might feel discomfort if they are unfamiliar with the types of questions asked in the DQ, suggesting discomfort may arise if the questions deviate from expectations of donor screening. Notably, no participants who were newer donors reported discomfort with the questions for this reason. Several participants expressed some discomfort with the question asking about anal sex because it deviated from social norms of public conversation. Some considered it “pushing the boundaries” of acceptable topics and thought people generally viewed it as “more of a taboo subject” (WB06: 24 years/F/bisexual/Asian)

Several participants expressed potential discomfort if their answer to the question would feel like they were disclosing personal information that may elicit social judgment. For example, “I think it [Q5] would be [uncomfortable] if it applied to me as in, yes I had.” (P13: 56 years/F/heterosexual/Caucasian). Participants described discomfort arising from personal disclosure as compounded for donors who may not trust that their information would be kept confidential. One participant thought this could be a particular concern for donors in smaller communities, “there could be some small communities where everyone knows each other, I'm from a small town originally, so I know that. And I can see people not wanting to start the gossip train type thing.” (WB14: 35 years/M/heterosexual/Caucasian).

Participants identified unique reasons for discomfort with Q4, exclusivity with sexual partner, and Q5, anal sex. They thought potential discomfort might arise from uncertainty about their partner's sexual behavior, including sexual activities outside of a monogamous relationship, and having to reflect on their relationship. One participant in her 60s thought this question was potentially more uncomfortable to answer than the question asking about anal sex. For Q5, some participants expressed discomfort if they perceived it as a “politically correct” way to discriminate against gbMSM. Concerns regarding how gbMSM might perceive this question were also raised.

3.4. Mitigating discomfort

Participants offered several ways to mitigate discomfort explicitly by making suggestions and implicitly in their discussions of their comfort level with the questions (see Table 3 for details). First, ensuring adequate explanation for why the questions are being asked and how they relate to blood safety and equity were suggested as ways to mitigate discomfort. Results suggest offering explanations that provide different levels of detail (e.g., from short explanations to the scientific research supporting the questions) would satisfy a range of donor interest levels. In addition, a brief explanation for the sexual behavior questions embedded within the DQ may decrease discomfort. Second, ensuring donors are alerted to and prepared for new sexual behavior questions in the DQ may mitigate discomfort. While providing an explanation for the questions increases donor understanding of the reasons for the questions, forewarning donors functions to shape donor expectations of the screening questions. Several participants also suggested that knowing about the new questions in advance would enable people to self‐defer and avoid the discomfort of being deferred at the donor center. Third, while some blood operators' may be concerned that explicit sexual behavior questions will cause discomfort, participants suggested that ambiguity in questions may cause uncertainty. Several explained that greater specificity would reduce the guesswork of what activity the blood operator is screening for and reduce the need to clarify with staff. Lastly, answering questions in a self‐administered DQ, such as on a web‐based app, was preferable to answering the questions with staff in‐person. Several participants mentioned that having to talk about personal matters with staff would cause greater discomfort, especially if staff are not comfortable with sexual behavior questions and topics.

TABLE 3.

Mitigating discomfort in donors

What? Why? How?

Providing an explanation for the questions

  • To ensure that donors understand the relevance of the SBB questions to blood safety
  • To explain how the sexual behaviour based questions may increase equity in blood donation
  • To explain what happens based on the answer to sexual behavior questions (e.g., deferral? additional testing?)
  • To explain the rationale for the progression of sexual behavior questions
  • Group all sexual behavior questions together in the DQ and provide a short explanation in the DQ prior to these questions
  • Ensure that explanations are nonstigmatizing and nonjudgmental of sexual activities and sexual identities
  • Offer a range of explanations from very brief to detailed scientific evidence
  • Provide comprehensive staff training to ensure staff can provide clear explanations for the questions
  • Provide comprehensive staff training to ensure staff are comfortable discussing sexual activities in a nonstigmatizing and nonjudgmental manner

Providing forewarning of questions

  • To inform donors and prospective donors of questions to be asked to shape expectations
  • To enable donors to self‐defer if they would answer “yes” to the questions (i.e., the questions would screen them out)
  • Ensure a comprehensive communications plan to donors and prospective donors prior to implementation

Reducing ambiguity in questions

  • To decrease the need for donors to clarify with staff the meaning of terms in the questions
  • To increase clarity and understanding of the specific behaviours that the blood operator is trying to identify
  • To encourage greater accuracy in answers
  • Implement questions that are clear and specific
  • Provide online and hardcopy materials that clarify potentially ambiguous terms in sexual behavior questions
  • Provide comprehensive staff training to ensure staff can clarify and explain the terms used in the questions
Answering questions in a self‐administered format
  • To enable donors to answer sexual behavior questions in a way that mitigates discomfort and supports a feeling of personal privacy
  • Implement sexual behavior questions in a self‐administered format (e.g., web‐based app)
  • Minimize staff‐mediated administration of sexual behavior questions

4. DISCUSSION

Results suggest that donors will find the proposed sexual behavior questions clear, direct, and understandable with clarity enhanced by blood operators' providing definitions of key terms used. Results expand understandings of the potential impact of sexual behavior questions on donors by exploring both comfort and discomfort with these questions within the context of donation. In particular, a novel finding is the preference some donors expressed for the question asking about anal sex over the current “MSM question.” Many viewed it as clearer and less discriminatory by screening all donors, regardless of sexuality, for specific behaviors. Results suggest that some donors would prefer answering new sexual behavior questions to the current questions provided it is clear to them why the questions are being asked and that they provide a more equitable approach to screening and eligibility. This is consistent with research that shows German and Austrian donors found the “MSM question” unfavorable and that it had a negative impact on their mood. 29 That donors value equity in donor screening is consistent with findings that blood donors are more concerned with equity and fairness than nondonors. 30

Our results demonstrate that comfort level with sexual behavior questions is influenced by the expectations and social norms donors bring to the donation experience, and their specific sexual behaviors close to the time of donation. Experienced donors with a single, long‐term sexual partner would likely experience no or minimal discomfort with these questions. How newer and first‐time donors might differ from experienced donors requires further research. While some participants who were experienced donors thought newer donors would feel discomfort because they may not know what to expect in donor screening, we did not find this among our participants who were newer donors. On this point, first‐time donors may be the group most likely to experience discomfort if they do not know what questions are asked in donor screening. However, whether first‐time donors would experience any greater discomfort with new sexual behavior questions than current questions in screening, including questions about paying money for sex, is yet unclear. Donors who have engaged in the sexual behaviors being screened for may experience some discomfort with having to disclose this information. However, our results suggest that social factors (e.g., sociocultural group and norms) will influence the level of discomfort and ensuring confidentiality will mitigate discomfort. Results also suggest that over time, as donors come to expect sexual behavior questions in donor screening, any donor deterrence due to discomfort will likely decrease.

Results also suggest that some small proportion of donors will experience discomfort with new sexual behavior questions and blood operators would be well‐advised to make efforts to mitigate such discomfort. Mitigation strategies include ensuring adequate explanation to donors for why the new questions are being asked and reducing ambiguity in the terms used in the questions. Studies have shown that reducing ambiguity may facilitate answering questions more honestly. 29 While many donors may skim read materials provided, pay little attention, and may have low literacy, 31 , 32 , 33 our results suggest that some donors do want details including the scientific evidence supporting these questions. As such, blood operators may consider a range of explanations from very short to more detailed explanations. Sandner et al.'s 29 suggestion that blood operators consider providing information in short videos rather than written text is also worth considering here.

Blood operators may also mitigate donor discomfort by forewarning new and return donors of sexual behavior questions that will be asked in donor screening. Knowing in advance what questions will be asked will enable self‐deferral and shape donors' expectations of the screening process. Blood operators may also consider grouping all sexual behavior questions together in the DQ. While it is yet unclear whether it is better to organize DQ questions by topic or chronology, 29 , 34 it makes some intuitive sense to group sexual behavior questions together and add explanatory information as a preface to these questions. For blood operators that have implemented a computerized self‐administered DQ, incorporating sexual behavior questions into this format will likely mitigate discomfort. Our results are consistent with research showing that computer‐assisted self‐administered interviews result in higher reporting of sensitive activities, including sexual behaviors, than phone interviews 35 , 36 and are perceived as more private. 37 Results suggest that blood operators should develop strong communication plans to prepare donors and prospective donors for the new questions. Comprehensive staff preparedness training would also facilitate staff comfort with discussing and explaining sexual behavior questions to donors in center.

This study has several limitations. Participants were all current donors and over half were experienced donors (i.e., >20 donations). Given their familiarity with screening questions, it is possible that the study population may report less discomfort with sexual behavior questions than first‐time or prospective donors. This study is also limited in its ability to offer a comparative analysis of comfort level with the new sexual behavior questions compared to the current questions in the DQ. To build on qualitative findings, we will conduct a quantitative survey of a representative sample of donors for comparative analysis of (1) relative comfort/discomfort with the questions under study and (2) comfort/discomfort with a DQ including new questions compared to the current DQ. We will also evaluate the effectiveness on donor understanding and discomfort of grouping sexual behavior questions together in the DQ with a heading and brief explanation.

Implementing sexual behavior‐based screening for all donors is a move toward greater equity in blood donation while maintaining the safety of the blood supply. Blood operators may be encouraged by evidence that suggests new sexual behavior questions will not deter most donors from donating. These results are consistent with prior research that shows that only a very small minority of Canadian donors would be deterred from donation because of discomfort with new sexual behavior questions. 20 While many blood operators and regulators view the move to sexual behavior‐based screening as a significant paradigmatic shift, donors may not perceive additional sexual behavior questions as a significant change to their donation experience.

CONFLICT OF INTEREST

The authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest relevant to the manuscript submitted to TRANSFUSION.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

The authors would like to thank the donors who participated in this research study. They would also like to thank Mindy Goldman for her insightful feedback on earlier versions of this manuscript and input on the design of the study. Thank you also to Sheila O'Brien and Terrie Butler‐Foster for their contribution to the study design and Samra Uzicanin for her assistance with retrieving donor data.

This research received funding support from Canadian Blood Services MSM Plasma Program, funded by the federal government (Health Canada) and the provincial and territorial ministries of health. The views herein do not necessarily reflect the views of the federal, provincial, or territorial governments of Canada.

Haw J, Woo H, Kohut T, Fisher W. Sexual risk behavior questions: Understanding and mitigating donor discomfort. Transfusion. 2022;62:355–364. 10.1111/trf.16755

REFERENCES

  • 1. Brailsford S, Kelly D, Kohli H, Slowther A, Watkins N. Who should donate blood? Policy decisions on donor deferral criteria should protect recipients and be fair to donors. Transfus Med (Oxford, England). 2015;25:234–238. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Custer B, Murcia K, Robinson WT, McFarland W, Raymond HF. Blood donation history and eligibility assessment in a community‐based sample of men who have sex with men. Transfusion. 2018;58:969–73. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. FAIR . Can donor selection policy move from a population‐based donor selection policy to one based on a more individualised risk assessment? Conclusions from the For the Assessment of Individualised Risk (FAIR) group. 2020.
  • 4. Goldman M, Shih W‐YA, O'Brien SF, Devine D. Donor deferral policies for men who have sex with men: past, present and future. Vox Sang. 2018;113:95–103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Levy I, Olmer L, Livnat Y, Shalhavi R, Hizki O, Shinar E. Attitudes and perceptions among men having sex with men towards a new non‐deferral blood donation policy in Israel. Vox Sang. 2019;114:310–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Berman M. Regulating the risk of blood‐borne related infections: men who have sex with men deferral policy. Public Health Rep. 2018;6(1):1–10. [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Cahill S & Wang T An end to lifetime blood donation ban in Israel for MSM would be a major step toward a science‐based policy that reduces stigma. Israel Journal of Health Policy Research 2017;6: 15. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 8. Jubran B, Billick M, Devlin G, Cygler J, Lebouché B. Reevaluating Canada's policy for blood donations from men who have sex with men (MSM). J Public Health Policy. 2016;37:428–39. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Suligoi B, Simonetta P, Regine V, Raimondo M, Velati C, Grazzini G. Changing blood donor screening criteria from permanent deferral for men who have sex with men to individual sexual risk assessment: No evidence of a significant impact on the human immunodeficiency virus epidemic in Italy. Blood Transfus Trasfus Sang. 2013;11:1–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Kesby M, Sothern M. Blood, sex and trust: the limits of the population‐based risk management paradigm. Health Place. 2014;26:21–30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Park C, Gellman C, O'Brien M, Eidelberg A, Subudhi I, Gorodetsky EF, et al. Blood donation and COVID‐19: reconsidering the 3‐month deferral policy for gay, bisexual, transgender, and other men who have sex with men. Am J Public Health. 2021;111:247–52. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Skelly A, Kolla L, Tamburro M, Bar K. Science over stigma: the need for evidence‐based blood donation policies for men who have sex with men in the USA. Lancet Haematol. 2020;7:e779–e82. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Grace D, Gaspar M, Klassen B, Lessard D, Brennan DJ, Lachowsky NJ, et al. It's in me to give: Canadian gay, bisexual, and queer Men's willingness to donate blood if eligible despite feelings of policy discrimination. Qual Health Res. 2020;30:2234–47. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Valentine K. Citizenship, identity. Blood Donation Body Soc. 2005;11:113–28. [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Caruso J, Germain M, Godin G, Myhal G, Pronovost F, Morin M, et al. ‘One step closer’: acceptability of a programme of plasma donation for fractionation from men who have sex with men. Vox Sang. 2019;114:675–86. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Grace D, Gaspar M, Lessard D, Klassen B, Brennan DJ, Adam BD, et al. Gay and bisexual men's views on reforming blood donation policy in Canada: a qualitative study. BMC Public Health. 2019;19:772. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Blanco S, Carrizo LH, Moyano RW, Mangeaud A, Gallego SV. Gender‐neutral donor deferral policies: experience in Argentina implementing individual risk‐assessment policies. Vox Sang. 2020;115:548–54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Epstein J, Ganz PR, Seitz R, Jutzi M, Schaerer C, Michaud G, et al. A shared regulatory perspective on deferral from blood donation of men who have sex with men (MSM). Vox Sang. 2014;107:416–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Raimondo M, Facco G, Regine V, Pupella S, Grazzini G, Suligoi B. HIV‐positive blood donors unaware of their sexual at‐risk behaviours before donation in Italy. Vox Sang. 2016;110:134–42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. O'Brien SF, Goldman M, Robillard P, Osmond L, Myhal G, Roy É. Donor screening question alternatives to men who have sex with men time deferral: potential impact on donor deferral and discomfort. Transfusion. 2021;61:94–101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Pierik RHM, Verweij MF. Facing difficult but unavoidable choices: blood safety, donor selection, and MSM deferral. Netherlands; 2020. https://www.rijksoverheid.nl/documenten/rapporten/2021/03/11/facing-difficult-but-unavoidable-choices-blood-safety-donor-selection-and-msm-deferral [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Palinkas LA, Horwitz SM, Green CA, Wisdom JP, Duan N, Hoagwood K. Purposeful sampling for qualitative data collection and analysis in mixed method implementation research. Adm Policy Ment Health. 2015;42:533–44. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Palys T. Purposive sampling. In: Given LM, editor. The sage encyclopedia of qualitative research methods, vol. 2. Los Angeles: Sage; 2008. p. 697–8. [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Whittaker S. Qualitative research: what is it and how can it be applied to transfusion medicine research? Vox Sang. 2002;83:251–60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Willson S, Miller K, Seem D, Kuehnert M. Cognitive evaluation of the AABB uniform donor history questionnaire. Transfusion. 2016;56:1662–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Wilson S, Miller K, Seem D, Kuehnert MJ. Cognitive interview study findings of the uniform blood donor history questionnaire. Transfusion. 2013;56:1662–67. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Braun V, Clarke V. Using thematic analysis in psychology. Qual Res Psychol. 2006;3:77–101. [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Thornberg R, Charmaz K. Grounded theory and theoretical coding. The SAGE handbook of qualitative data analysis. London: SAGE Publications Ltd; 2014. p. 153–69. [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Sandner S, Merz E‐M, van den Hurk K, van Kraaij M, Mikkelsen C, Ullum H, et al. Validation of a standardized donor health questionnaire across substances of human origin. Vox Sang. 2021;116:645–55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Ferguson E. Strategies and theories to attract and retain blood donors: fairness, reciprocity, equity and warm‐glow. ISBT Sci Ser. 2021;16:219–25. [Google Scholar]
  • 31. O'Brien SF, Osmond L, Choquet K, Yi Q‐L, Goldman M. Donor attention to reading materials. Vox Sang. 2015;109:336–42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Townsend M, Buccino T, Katz L. Evaluation of donor informed consents and associated predonation educational materials in the United States and Canada: variability in elements of consent and measures of readability and reading burden. Transfusion. 2020;60:1747–55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Wehrli G, Rossmann SN, Waxman DA, Katz LM. Evaluation and improvement of blood donor educational materials: results from a multicenter randomized controlled trial. Transfusion. 2020;60:1756–64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. O'Brien SF, Ram SS, Vamvakas EC, Goldman M. The Canadian blood donor health assessment questionnaire: lessons from history, application of cognitive science principles, and recommendations for change. Transfus Med Rev. 2007;21:205–22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Tourangeau R, Smith TW. Asking sensitive questions: the impact of data collection mode, question format, and question context. Public Opin Q. 1996;60:275–304. [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Tourangeau R, Yan T. Sensitive questions in surveys. Psychol Bull. 2007;133:859–83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Beckenbach A. Computer‐assisted questioning: the new survey methods in the perception of the respondents. Bull Sociol Methodol. 1995;48:82–100. [Google Scholar]

Articles from Transfusion are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES