Skip to main content
Wiley Open Access Collection logoLink to Wiley Open Access Collection
. 2022 May 11;101(1):92–99. doi: 10.1111/jfb.15074

Large and expensive brain comes with a short lifespan: The relationship between brain size and longevity among fish taxa

Gavin Stark 1,
PMCID: PMC9544989  PMID: 35482011

Abstract

Vertebrates show substantial interspecific variation in brain size in relation to body mass. It has long been recognized that the evolution of large brains is associated with both costs and benefits, and it is their net benefit which should be favoured by natural selection. On one hand, the substantial energetic cost imposed by the maintenance of neural tissue is expected to compromise the energetic budget of organisms with large brains and their investment in other critical organs (expensive brain framework, EBF) or important physiological process, such as somatic maintenance and repair, thus accelerating ageing that shortens lifespan, as predicted by the disposable soma theory (DST). However, selection towards larger brain size can provide cognitive benefits (e.g., high behavioural flexibility) that may mitigate extrinsic mortality pressures, and thus may indirectly select for slower ageing that prolongs lifespan, as predicted by the cognitive buffer hypothesis (CBH). The relationship between longevity and brain size has been investigated to date only among terrestrial vertebrates, although the same selective forces acting on those species may also affect vertebrates living in aquatic habitats, such as fish. Thus, whether this evolutionary trade‐off for brain size and longevity exists on a large scale among fish clades remains to be addressed. In this study, using a global dataset of 407 fish species, I undertook the first phylogenetic test of the brain size/longevity relationship in aquatic vertebrate species. The study revealed a negative relationship between brain size and longevity among cartilaginous fish confirming EBF and DST. However, no pattern emerged among bony fish species. Among sharks and rays, the high metabolic cost of producing neural tissue transcends the cognitive benefits of evolving a larger brain. Consequently, my findings suggest that the cost of maintaining brain tissue is relatively higher in ectothermic species than in endothermic ones.

Keywords: aquatic, brain mass, cognition, life history, ray‐finned fishes, sharks

1. INTRODUCTION

Variation in relative brain size is extensive at all taxonomic levels and across all vertebrates (Striedter, 2005). This relatively large and complex organ is one of the most characteristic features of this lineage, and is related to various fundamental aspects of vertebrate behaviour and ecology (Amiel et al., 2011). Highly flexible cognitive abilities are central to the success of many vertebrate taxa and may have imposed powerful selection for increase in relative brain size (Benson‐Amram et al., 2016; Martin, 1981; Sol et al., 2008). In contrast, maintaining a large brain is energetically expensive: the metabolic cost of brain function is among the highest of any organ (the proportion of metabolic energy allocated to the brain in the resting state for both endotherms and ectotherms is on average >5% of the total energetic budget; Mink et al., 1981). We thus might expect the benefits of increased cognition to be balanced against metabolic costs, with relative brain size in any given species reflecting this trade‐off (Gillooly & McCoy, 2014; Martin, 1981).

A number of hypotheses aiming to explain the evolution of brain size variation among and within different vertebrate taxa have been formulated (Isler & van Schaik, 2009; Kotrschal et al., 2019; Sol, 2009). On one side the cognitive buffer hypothesis (CBH; Sol, 2009) predicts that large relative brain masses (to body size) are associated with increased cognitive abilities with the potential to enhance the efficiency of behavioural responses to novel socio‐ecological challenges (González‐Lagos et al., 2010; Sol, 2009; Vonk, 2016), such as rapid reactions to unexpected threats (e.g., avoiding predation), finding new food sources or obtaining a mate (Allman et al., 1993; Benson‐Amram et al., 2016; Holekamp & Benson‐Amram, 2017; Striedter, 2005). On the other side, however, the metabolic costs associated with the development of larger brains have to be met by either increasing the total energy budget (e.g., by increasing foraging time) or compensating for changes through energy allocation to other maintenance or developmental functions, as predicted by the expensive brain framework (EBF; Isler & van Schaik, 2009). Moreover, evolving a larger brain could also divert energy away from somatic maintenance and repair, risking the rapid decline of physiological process and the rapid accumulation of harmful mutations, which can accelerate ageing rates (Kirkwood, 1992). In this context, the disposable soma theory (DST) suggests that organisms adjust their investment of energy and resources into somatic maintenance or other costly functions, such as reproduction (Kirkwood, 2017). Thus, investment in the development of a larger brain could compromise energy allocation into somatic maintenance, consequently accelerating ageing and reducing lifespan overall (Kotrschal et al., 2019).

These hypotheses consistently recognize that beyond its key role in cognitive advantages that can influence survival (Striedter, 2005), the brain influences a range of crucial fitness‐relevant processes, such as the rate of ageing, by means of its regulatory function in the maintenance of physiological and hormonal processes (Hofman, 1983). Provided that these regulatory, as well as cognitive, benefits of large brain size outbalance the metabolic cost of its development, a positive correlation between brain size and lifespan would be expected and vice versa.

Evidence for the relationships between brain size and longevity has exclusively been focused on large‐scale studies examining tetrapod species (e.g., González‐Lagos et al., 2010; Jiménez‐Ortega et al., 2020; Minias & Podlaszczuk, 2017; Sol et al., 2007, 2016; Stark & Pincheira‐Donoso, 2022; Yu et al., 2018) and has revealed mixed results with support for both the CBH and EBF/DST. Surprisingly, the evidence for any link between brain size and longevity remains lacking for fish, i.e., broadly across species, clades and regions. Interestingly, a major experimental study using guppies (Poecilia reticulata) artificially selected for divergence in brain size found that increased brain size led to accompanying reduction in fecundity, gut size and lifespan (Kotrschal et al., 2013, 2019). This suggests that the evolutionary implications of energetic constraints could be similar for both tetrapod and fish species. If, as hypothesized, brain tissue is relatively more costly to generate and maintain in fish, this could facilitate the proposed macroevolutionary relationship between brain size and life span. However, without formally testing this pattern among fish clades, we cannot be confident that the important relationship between brain size and lifespan is a common feature of all vertebrates.

This paper presents the first large‐scale study investigating the association between brain size and lifespan among the Osteichthyes (bony fish) and Chondrichthyes (cartilaginous fish), while controlling for a set of possible confounding effects, such as body size, sampling size and life‐history traits. The two alternative predictions proposed above are addressed: (1) larger relative brain size prolongs lifespan among fish clades (in line with the CBH) and (2) the metabolic cost imposed by large brains reduces somatic maintenance, and consequently also lifespan, by accelerating ageing, leading to a trade‐off whereby smaller brains are associated with longer lifespans (in line with the EBF and DST).

2. MATERIALS AND METHODS

2.1. Data collection

A dataset on whole brain sizes for 407 fish species encompassing 36 orders (~60% of all fish orders) and 120 families (~25% of all fish families) of Osteichthyes and Chondrichthyes was compiled. In these comparative analyses, the whole brain mass was used because it has been consistently shown that whole‐brain size positively correlates with several measures of behavioural flexibility and the ability to survive in novel ecological conditions (Lefebvre & Sol, 2008; Striedter, 2005). Data on brain mass (and the other variables, see below) were primarily collected from Mull et al. (2020) for sharks and rays, and from Tsuboi et al. (2018) for teleost species. The primary literature is available together with the full database and metadata in Supporting Information Appendix S1.

Fish body size was represented by mass in grams and was collected from the same source of brain‐size data for each species. Following recommendations by Sol et al. (2007), the residuals of brain size against body mass were extracted using log–log least‐square linear regression (R 2 = 0.88, P < 0.0001), which is one of the most popular methods in comparative studies on brain size evolution and has the advantage of eliminating problems of collinearity (while using brain and body size as covariates), while effectively removing body size effects (Sol et al., 2007; González‐Lagos et al., 2010; Galván & Møller, 2011; Abelson, 2016; Minias & Podlaszczuk, 2017; Yu et al., 2018). Brain size residuals were calculated with phylogenetic corrected least‐square regression using the Analysis of Phylogenetics and Evolution (APE) package (Paradis et al., 2004).

Longevity data of bony and cartilaginous species were taken primarily from the AnAge database (De Magalhaes & Costa, 2009), with the rest of the longevity data retrieved from multiple literature sources (for the full reference list see Supporting Information Appendix S1.2). The data values for longevity reflect the maximum lifespan for each species, which is the most commonly reported measure in the literature (Magalhães et al., 2007; Scharf et al., 2015; Stark et al., 2020a; Wilkinson & Adams, 2019).

2.2. Confounding variables

Records for maximum longevity may provide information on lifespan only if adjusted for sample size, as the probability of recording extremely old individuals increases with sample size (Stark & Meiri, 2018). Furthermore, there are some drawbacks in using maximum values as estimates of longevity given that the maximum age of an individual may not be representative of the species as a whole (Carey, 2003) or that estimates could be based on unequal numbers of individuals for different species (Stark et al., 2018). Therefore, the sample sizes for which longevity is estimated for each species were used to correct for these potential biases inherent in the use of maxima. Following Stark et al. (2020b), data on sample sizes from AnAge and classified small sample as species with one individual, medium samples as having 11 specimens per species, and large samples as having 101 individuals sampledwere used. Moreover, it was noted whether maximum longevity was measured in captivity or in the wild due to the possible bias of using only captive specimens that are not exposed to extrinsic mortality factors (in captivity there are no predators and animals receive ample food and veterinary care) and thus may have higher values (Stark & Meiri, 2018). Accordingly, data obtained from the wild or from captivity may not be equivalent. Thus, the source of longevity data was also included (as well as sample size) as a fixed factor in all the models.

Most life‐history traits may covary with life span and brain size (Healy et al., 2019; Tsuboi et al., 2018), therefore it is important to examine their correlative relationship with brain size and longevity. Covariation of life‐history traits is common across species (Bakewell et al., 2020; Bielby et al., 2007; Healy et al., 2019), therefore data on life‐history traits that are typically correlated with life span from previous studies on fish taxa was extracted (Mull et al., 2020; Rochet et al., 2000), including indicators of parental investment such as age at sexual maturity, clutch and egg size, and reproduction mode.

2.3. Phylogenetic and statistical analyses

Prior to analyses, continuous variables were checked for normality using the Shapiro–Wilk test and log transformed to reduce heteroscedasticity. In comparative studies, closely related species are more likely to demonstrate similar life‐history strategies and phenotypes due to shared ancestry (Healy et al., 2019). Thus, to control for the shared evolutionary history, phylogenetic generalized least squares (PGLS) regression (Freckleton et al., 2002) was conducted using the most recent phylogeny for the Osteichthyes and Chondrichthyes groups. Rabosky et al. (2013) phylogeny was used for >30,000 living species of ray‐finned fishes, while for sharks, rays and chimaeras, the tree from Stein et al. (2018) was used, which includes 610 species. The ‘caper’ package in R (Orme et al., 2013) was used to estimate the maximum likelihood value of Pagel's λ, a statistically powerful index for measuring whether data exhibit phylogenetic dependence and its strength (Freckleton et al., 2002). The scaling parameter λ represents the magnitude of the phylogenetic signal in the data and model residuals (Freckleton et al., 2002), with λ values covering a scale ranging from 0 (phylogenetic nonindependence) to 1 (complete phylogenetic dependence; Freckleton et al., 2002).

To determine whether multicollinearity existed in the data among the predictor variables, the variance inflation factors (VIF; O'Brien, 2007) were calculated. In all the analyses none of the predictors had a VIF value higher than 5 and the analyses are thus not biased by multicollinearity (O'Brien, 2007). A phylogenetic ANCOVA test was conducted starting from a full model and using a backwards stepwise elimination procedure, sequentially deleting factors with P values >0.05 until a minimum adequate model was obtained in which all predictors were significant (at α < 0.05). There were two main models: the first was for body size (predictor) and brain size (response), and the second, the PGLS model, included maximum longevity (years) as the dependent variable. Relative brain size (extracted residuals from a brain mass in grams against body mass in grams log–log least‐square linear regression), log age at sexual maturity (years), log clutch (number of eggs of per batch) and egg size (mm) and reproductive mode (oviparous, ovoviviparous and viviparous) were included as covariates, while sampling size and the source of longevity data were entered as fixed factors. Each major fish group was analysed separately (Osteichthyes and Chondrichthyes) to determine whether the basic division of the Tree of Life among these groups influences the life‐history traits (following Stark et al., 2020b), which may in turn affect the relationship between brain size and longevity. All statistical analyses in this study were performed in R 4.1.2 (R Core Team, Vienna, Austria) using the RStudio 1.4.1106 (RStudio Inc., Boston, MA, USA) interface.

3. RESULTS

3.1. Brain size and longevity patterns

Brain size varied significantly among both Osteichthyes (n = 329) and Chondrichthyes (n = 79) species, and spanned several orders of magnitude among both groups (Figure 1a,b). Brain sizes in the database varied from 3 mg (banded pipefish Doryrhamphus dactyliophorus; Tsuboi et al., 2017) to almost 4 g (yellowfin tuna Thunnus albacares; Tsuboi et al., 2018) among bony fish, and from 355 mg (shortnose sawshark Pristiophorus nudipinnis; Mull et al., 2020) to around 100 g (great hammerhead Sphyrna mokarran; Mull et al., 2020) among cartilaginous fish. Maximum lifespan also varied extensively across bony fish species, from that of the blue sprat (Spratelloides delicatulus; Milton et al., 1993), which lives only 5 months, to 104 years for the white sturgeon (Acipenser transmontanus; Cailliet et al., 2001; Figure 1c). For cartilaginous fish, one of the shortest lifespan species in the dataset was the spadenose shark (Scoliodon laticaudus; Fowler & Cavanagh, 2005), which lives about 6 years, while the longest‐living species (and also the longest‐lived vertebrate known) was the Greenland shark (Somniosus microcephalus; Nielsen et al., 2016), which can live up to a maximum of 392 years (Figure 1d).

FIGURE 1.

FIGURE 1

Distribution of the relative brain size (extracted residuals from a brain mass in grams against body mass in grams log–log least‐square linear regression) and maximum longevity (years in log transformed) of (a) and (c) bony fish (Osteichthyes, black columns) and (b) and (d) cartilaginous fish (Chondrichthyes, yellow columns)

3.2. Body and brain mass among fish

The phylogenetic analyses (PGLS) performed between brain size and body size showed that brain mass increased with larger body mass (Table 1) for bony fish (Figure 2a) and cartilaginous fish (Figure 2b). In general, body size explained a major part of the variance in brain size among fish species (80%–88% for Chondrichthyes and Osteichthyes, respectively).

TABLE 1.

Brain size as a function of body mass for bony and cartilaginous fishes using phylogenetic analysis

Factor Estimate Standard error t P
Intercept (Osteichthyes) −1.889 0.186 −10.1 <0.0001
Body mass 0.506 0.010 47.4 <0.0001
λ = 0.97, R 2 = 0.88, n = 316, P < 0.0001
Intercept (Chondrichthyes) −0.895 0.152 −5.8 <0.0001
Body mass 0.411 0.029 13.8 <0.0001

Note: λ = 0.93, R 2 = 0.80, n = 51, P < 0.0001.

FIGURE 2.

FIGURE 2

Top panels: the relationship between brain mass and body mass (both grams in log transformed) among (a) bony fish (Osteichthyes, black points, yellow dashed line) and (b) cartilaginous fish (Chondrichthyes, yellow points, black dashed line). Bottom panels: the relationship between relative brain size (extracted residuals from a brain mass in grams against body mass in grams log–log least‐square linear regression) and maximum longevity (years in log transformed) among (c) bony fish and (d) cartilaginous fish

3.3. Brain size and longevity

When the full PGLS models for the relationship between relative brain size and lifespan were analysed, while correcting for the confounding variables (i.e., life‐history traits and biasing factors, e.g., origin of data and sample size), no pattern emerged among bony fish (Table. 2 and Figure 2). In contrast, the PGLS analysis on cartilaginous fish only found a negative relationship between relative brain size and maximum lifespan (slope = −0.473 ± 0.225, P = 0.04). When bony fish species only were analysed, the PGLS model found an effect of some of the life‐history traits on longevity, while others did not affect longevity (reproduction mode and egg size). Among cartilaginous fish species, a similar (positive) effect of age at maturity on longevity was found, as well as a positive effect of pup size on cartilaginous lifespans. Surprisingly, almost no effect of the fixed factors (sample size or origin of data) on maximum longevity among bony or cartilaginous fish was found (Table. 2).

TABLE 2.

Minimal adequate model for the analysis among fish classes

Factor Estimate a SE t P
Osteichthyes
Intercept 0.693 0.225 3.0 0.002
Residual brain size −0.064 0.118 −0.5 0.59
Age at sexual maturity 0.553 0.070 7.9 <0.0001
Clutch size 0.044 0.021 2.0 0.04
Sample size 0.046 0.028 1.6 0.11
Origin of data (wild) −0.006 0.135 −0.1 0.96
Chondrichthyes
Intercept 0.322 0.461 0.7 0.49
Residual brain size −0.473 0.225 −2.1 0.04
Age at sexual maturity 0.717 0.093 7.6 <0.0001
Pup/egg size 0.480 0.178 2.6 0.01
Sample size −0.057 0.066 −0.8 0.39
Origin of data (wild) −0.391 0.294 −1.3 0.19

Note: Osteichthyes (bony fish) full model results: λ = 0.615, R 2 = 0.35, n = 156, P < 0.0001. Chondrichthyes (cartilaginous fish) full model results: λ = 0.99, R 2 = 0.26, n = 35, P < 0.0001. Model predictors shown below the intercept. SE, standard error.

a

Estimates for predictors in each model are slopes.

4. DISCUSSION

This study presents the first large‐scale analysis aimed at expanding our understanding of the relationship between lifespan and brain size across all vertebrate classes by contributing comparative evidence for fish clades that complements the existing evidence for tetrapods. Although the current study demonstrates considerable variations in relative brain size and maximum lifespans across bony and cartilaginous fish species, no evidence was found for any association between these two variables after controlling for several potential confounding factors, such as age at maturity, for bony fish clades (Osteichthyes). However, for sharks and rays (Chondrichthyes) a pattern did emerge. This finding (for Chondrichthyes species only) aligns with recent experimental evidence in guppies (Poecilia reticulata), in which lifespan decreases with larger brains (Kotrschal et al., 2019). Thus, this research suggests that evolving a larger brain can increase the metabolic cost and divert energy from more life‐extending processes (e.g., somatic maintenance), which may promote the evolution of a shorter lifespan.

Evolutionary selective forces shaping brain size variation among vertebrates are constrained by the trade‐offs between the cognitive advantages that are attained with a larger brain and the high energetic costs allocated towards its maintenance (Dunbar & Shultz, 2007; González‐Lagos et al., 2010; Harraan, 1955; Jiménez‐Ortega et al., 2020; Lefebvre et al., 1997; Minias & Podlaszczuk, 2017; Mink et al., 1981; Sol, 2009; Yu et al., 2018). As a result, natural selection can favour either one strategy or the other depending on the extent to which a given increment in brain mass is accompanied by certain cognitive advantages that increase lifetime fitness despite its energetic costs. The pattern that seems to emerge among endotherms was a positive relationship between brain size and longevity, while a negative relationship emerged across terrestrial ectotherms (Barrickman et al., 2008; Barton & Capellini, 2011; González‐Lagos et al., 2010; Isler & van Schaik, 2009; Jiménez‐Ortega et al., 2020; Kotrschal et al., 2019; Minias & Podlaszczuk, 2017; Sol et al., 2007, 2016; Stark & Pincheira‐Donoso, 2022). Although the relative brain mass of birds and mammals can be up to 10‐fold heavier than in amphibians and reptiles (Font et al., 2019), this positive relationship between brain size and longevity is not observed in both fish classes. The correlational results presented here may suggest a trade‐off between brain‐size and longevity across the Chondrichthyes class, but no such relationship was observed among Osteichthyes species. Consequently, it is suggested that the increment in cognitive faculties contributed by a larger brain mass remains below the threshold at which the fitness benefits of behavioural flexibility outweigh the energetic cost. Indeed, this study's findings are consistent with previous evidence that the cost of maintaining brain tissue is relatively higher in ectothermic species than in endothermic ones (Stark & Pincheira‐Donoso, 2022).

There are several well‐known differences between endotherms and ectotherms regarding brain metabolism: (1) ectotherms have a 10‐fold lower whole‐body resting metabolic rate (Mink et al., 1981), (2) metabolic oxygen consumption of the central nervous system and the metabolic cost of ectothermic brain tissue are as high as those of birds and mammals (Stark et al., 2020b), and (3) there is strong support for negative trade‐offs between brain size and other traits (as proposed by the EBF; Isler & van Schaik, 2009), observed from studies on ectothermic groups such as fishes (Kotrschal et al., 2013; Tsuboi et al., 2014) and amphibians (Liao et al., 2016). Overall, these differences reinforce the assumption that brain tissue may be relatively more costly to generate and maintain in ectotherms, which could lead to the negative relationship between brain size and lifespan observed in cartilaginous species.

The inconsistency observed in the brain size/lifespan relationship among fish classes is difficult to explain. It may be that these pattens emerged from the fundamental differences in the major life‐history and phylogenetic discrepancies between the two classes, which are likely to lead to internal differences in how key traits trade off the allocation of energetic budgets as a function of their contribution to lifetime fitness. Reproduction investment is critical for management of energy allocation, with both groups differing considerably in their reproductive activities and outputs. For example, bony fish generally engage in energetically demanding reproductive output, with females tending to have much larger numbers of offspring than do sharks or rays (e.g., the Atlantic sturgeon Acipenser oxyrinchus can produce up to 2.5 million eggs, while the ocean sunfish Mola mola can release 300 million eggs over the course of one spawning season; Van Eenennaam et al., 1996; Forsgren et al., 2020). To support this reasoning, it may be essential to look into further biometric parameters such as the gonadosomatic index (GSI), which can act as a good indicator of reproductive activity in fish while also helping to evaluate the way the species get resources from the habitat. This kind of parameter should be incorporated in future studies that investigate reproductive differences among fish clades.

Furthermore, bony fishes may make use of the extra computing power a larger brain confers, with better cognitive flexibility or similar. This may outweigh the energetic costs of such a brain and so conceal the relationship between brain size and longevity. In cartilaginous fishes a relatively larger brain may not be useful as it may not transfer into any cognitive advantage that may increase survival. However, the specific explanation for these observed discrepancies remains difficult to establish at present without risking much speculation.

In conclusion, the results of this study indicate that the link between brain size and longevity may only be contingent on the mechanism of mortality, as was found experimentally in fish (Kotrschal et al., 2019). It is shown that the evolutionary patterns of an animal's life history are not consistent across vertebrate taxa, and that differences between bony and cartilaginous fish species should be accounted for when examining the relationship of brain size and lifespan among classes of the same taxa. Thus, this research suggests that across macroecological scales the high metabolic cost of producing neural tissue may surpass the cognitive benefits of evolving a larger brain, resulting in a negative pattern among cartilaginous fish.

CONFLICT OF INTEREST

There are no competing interests for the author of this manuscript.

DATA ACCESSIBILITY STATEMENT

All data used in the analyses and the associated metadata are available in Supporting Information Appendix S1. All codes used in the analyses are available in Supporting Information Appendix S2.

Supporting information

Appendix S1. Supporting Information

Appendix S2. Supporting Information

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

I am grateful to Rachel Schwarz Stark, Simon Jamison and Daniel Pincheira‐Donoso for constructive discussion. I am also enormously grateful to Naomi Paz for English editing on an earlier draft of the manuscript.

Stark, G. (2022). Large and expensive brain comes with a short lifespan: The relationship between brain size and longevity among fish taxa. Journal of Fish Biology, 101(1), 92–99. 10.1111/jfb.15074

REFERENCES

  1. Allman, J. , McLaughlin, T. , & Hakeem, A. (1993). Brain weight and life‐span in primate species. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 90(1), 118–122. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Amiel, J. J. , Tingley, R. , & Shine, R. (2011). Smart moves: Effects of relative brain size on establishment success of invasive amphibians and reptiles. PLoS One, 6(4), e18277. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bakewell, A. T. , Davis, K. E. , Freckleton, R. P. , Isaac, N. J. , & Mayhew, P. J. (2020). Comparing life histories across taxonomic groups in multiple dimensions: How mammal‐like are insects? The American Naturalist, 195(1), 70–81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Barrickman, N. L. , Bastian, M. L. , Isler, K. , & van Schaik, C. P. (2008). Life history costs and benefits of encephalization: A comparative test using data from long‐term studies of primates in the wild. Journal of Human Evolution, 54, 568–590. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Barton, R. A. , & Capellini, I. (2011). Maternal investment, life histories, and the costs of brain growth in mammals. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 108, 6169–6174. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Benson‐Amram, S. , Dantzer, B. , Stricker, G. , Swanson, E. M. , & Holekamp, K. E. (2016). Brain size predicts problem‐solving ability in mammalian carnivores. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113(9), 2532–2537. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bielby, J. , Mace, G. M. , Bininda‐Emonds, O. R. , Cardillo, M. , Gittleman, J. L. , Jones, K. E. , … Purvis, A. (2007). The fast‐slow continuum in mammalian life history: An empirical reevaluation. The American Naturalist, 169(6), 748–757. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Cailliet, G. M. , Andrews, A. H. , Burton, E. J. , Watters, D. L. , Kline, D. E. , & Ferry‐Graham, L. A. (2001). Age determination and validation studies of marine fishes: Do deep‐dwellers live longer? Experimental Gerontology, 36(4–6), 739–764. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Carey, J. R. (2003). Longevity: The biology and demography of life span. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. [Google Scholar]
  10. De Magalhaes, J. P. , & Costa, J. (2009). A database of vertebrate longevity records and their relation to other life‐history traits. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 22(8), 1770–1774. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Dunbar, R. I. M. , & Shultz, S. (2007). Evolution in the social brain. Science, 317, 1344–1347. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Font, E. , García‐Roa, R. , Pincheira‐Donoso, D. , & Carazo, P. (2019). Rethinking the effects of body size on the study of brain size evolution. Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 93(4), 182–195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Forsgren, K. , McBride, R. S. , Nakatsubo, T. , Thys, T. M. , Carson, C. D. , Tholke, E. K. , … Potter, I. (2020). Reproductive biology of the ocean sunfishes. The ocean sunfishes: Evolution, biology and conservation (pp. 87–104). Boca Raton, FL, USA: CRC Press. [Google Scholar]
  14. Fowler, S. L. , & Cavanagh, R. D. (2005). Sharks, rays and chimaeras: The status of the Chondrichthyan fishes: Status survey (Vol. 63). Gland, Switzerland and Cambridge, UK: IUCN; [Google Scholar]
  15. Freckleton, R. P. , Harvey, P. H. , & Pagel, M. (2002). Phylogenetic analysis and comparative data: A test and review of evidence. The American Naturalist, 160, 712–726. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Galván, I. , & Møller, A. P. (2011). Brain size and the expression of pheomelanin‐based colour in birds. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 24(5), 999–1006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Gillooly, J. F. , & McCoy, M. W. (2014). Brain size varies with temperature in vertebrates. PeerJ, 2, e301. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. González‐Lagos, C. , Sol, D. , & Reader, S. M. (2010). Large‐brained mammals live longer. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 23, 1064–1074. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Harraan, D. (1955). Aging: A theory based on free radical and radiation chemistry. Journal of Gerontology, 11, 298–300. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Healy, K. , Ezard, T. H. , Jones, O. R. , Salguero‐Gómez, R. , & Buckley, Y. M. (2019). Animal life history is shaped by the pace of life and the distribution of age‐specific mortality and reproduction. Nature ecology & evolution, 3(8), 1217–1224. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Hofman, M. A. (1983). Energy metabolism, brain size and longevity in mammals. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 58, 495–512. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Holekamp, K. E. , & Benson‐Amram, S. (2017). The evolution of intelligence in mammalian carnivores. Interface Focus, 7(3), 20160108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Isler, K. , & van Schaik, C. P. (2009). The expensive brain: A framework for explaining evolutionary changes in brain size. Journal of Human Evolution, 57(4), 392–400. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Jiménez‐Ortega, D. , Kolm, N. , Immler, S. , Maklakov, A. A. , & Gonzalez‐Voyer, A. (2020). Long life evolves in large‐brained bird lineages. Evolution, 74(12), 2617–2628. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Kirkwood, T. B. (2017). The disposable soma theory. The evolution of senescence in the tree of life , 23–39.
  26. Kirkwood, T. B. L. (1992). The disposable soma theory: Evidence and implications. Netherlands Journal of Zoology, 43, 359–363. [Google Scholar]
  27. Kotrschal, A. , Corral‐Lopez, A. , & Kolm, N. (2019). Large brains, short life: Selection on brain size impacts intrinsic lifespan. Biology Letters, 15(5), 20190137. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Kotrschal, A. , Rogell, B. , Bundsen, A. , Svensson, B. , Zajitschek, S. , Brännström, I. , … Kolm, N. (2013). The benefit of evolving a larger brain: Big‐brained guppies perform better in a cognitive task. Animal Behaviour, 86, e4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Lefebvre, L. , & Sol, D. (2008). Brains, lifestyles and cognition: Are there general trends? Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 72(2), 135–144. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Lefebvre, L. , Whittle, P. , Lascaris, E. , & Finkelstein, A. (1997). Feeding innovations and forebrain size in birds. Animal Behaviour, 53, 549–560. [Google Scholar]
  31. Liao, W. B., Lou, S. L., Zeng, Y., & Kotrschal, A. (2016). Large Brains, Small Guts: The Expensive Tissue Hypothesis Supported within Anurans. The American Naturalist, 188(6), 693–700. 10.1086/688894 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Magalhães, J. P. D. , Costa, J. , & Church, G. M. (2007). An analysis of the relationship between metabolism, developmental schedules, and longevity using phylogenetic independent contrasts. The Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 62(2), 149–160. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Martin, R. D. (1981). Relative brain size and basal metabolic rate in terrestrial vertebrates. Nature, 293(5827), 57–60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Milton, D. A. , Blaber, S. J. M. , & Rawlinson, N. J. F. (1993). Age and growth of three species of clupeids from Kiribati, tropical central South Pacific. Journal of Fish Biology, 43(1), 89–108. [Google Scholar]
  35. Minias, P. , & Podlaszczuk, P. (2017). Longevity is associated with relative brain size in birds. Ecology and Evolution, 7, 3558–3566. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Mink, J. W. , Blumenschine, R. J. , & Adams, D. B. (1981). Ratio of central nervous system to body metabolism in vertebrates: Its constancy and functional basis. American Journal of Physiology‐Regulatory, Integrative and Comparative Physiology, 241, R203–R212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Mull, C. G. , Yopak, K. E. , & Dulvy, N. K. (2020). Maternal investment, ecological lifestyle, and brain evolution in sharks and rays. The American Naturalist, 195(6), 1056–1069. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Nielsen, J. , Hedeholm, R. B. , Heinemeier, J. , Bushnell, P. G. , Christiansen, J. S. , Olsen, J. , … Steffensen, J. F. (2016). Eye lens radiocarbon reveals centuries of longevity in the Greenland shark (Somniosus microcephalus). Science, 353(6300), 702–704. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. O'Brien, R. M. (2007). A caution regarding rules of thumb for variance inflation factors. Quality & Quantity, 41, 673–690. [Google Scholar]
  40. Orme, D. , Freckleton, R. , Thomas, G. , Petzoldt, T. , Fritz, S. , & Isaac, N. (2013). The caper package: Comparative analysis of phylogenetics and evolution in R. R package Version, 5, 1–36. [Google Scholar]
  41. Paradis, E. , Claude, J. , & Strimmer, K. (2004). APE: An R package for analyses of phylogenetics and evolution. Bioinformatics, 20(2), 289–290. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Rabosky, D. L. , Santini, F. , Eastman, J. , Smith, S. A. , Sidlauskas, B. , Chang, J. , & Alfaro, M. E. (2013). Rates of speciation and morphological evolution are correlated across the largest vertebrate radiation. Nature Communications, 4(1), 1–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Rochet, M. J. , Cornillon, P. A. , Sabatier, R. , & Pontier, D. (2000). Comparative analysis of phylogenetic and fishing effects in life history patterns of teleost fishes. Oikos, 91(2), 255–270. [Google Scholar]
  44. Scharf, I. , Feldman, A. , Novosolov, M. , Pincheira‐Donoso, D. , Das, I. , Böhm, M. , … Meiri, S. (2015). Late bloomers and baby boomers: Ecological drivers of longevity in squamates and the tuatara. Global Ecology and Biogeography, 24(4), 396–405. [Google Scholar]
  45. Sol, D. (2009). Revisiting the cognitive buffer hypothesis for the evolution of large brains. Biology Letters, 5(1), 130–133. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Sol, D. , Bacher, S. , Reader, S. M. , & Lefebvre, L. (2008). Brain size predicts the success of mammal species introduced into novel environments. The American Naturalist, 172(S1), S63–S71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Sol, D. , Sayol, F. , Ducatez, S. , & Lefebvre, L. (2016). The life‐history basis for behavioural innovations. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 371, 20150187. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Sol, D. , Székely, T. , Liker, A. , & Lefebvre, L. (2007). Big‐brained birds survive better in nature. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 274, 763–769. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Stark, G. , & Meiri, S. (2018). Cold and dark captivity: Drivers of amphibian longevity. Global Ecology and Biogeography, 27, 1384–1397. [Google Scholar]
  50. Stark, G. , & Pincheira‐Donoso, D. (2022). The evolution of brain size in ectothermic Tetrapods: Large brain mass trades‐off with lifespan in reptiles. Evolutionary Biology, 1‐9. 10.1007/s11692-022-09562-4. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  51. Stark, G. , Schwarz, R. , & Meiri, S. (2020a). Does nocturnal activity prolong gecko longevity? Israel Journal of Ecology and Evolution, 1(aop), 1–8. [Google Scholar]
  52. Stark, G. , Pincheira‐Donoso, D. , & Meiri, S. (2020b). No evidence for the ‘rate‐of‐living’ theory across the tetrapod tree of life. Global Ecology and Biogeography, 29, 857–884. [Google Scholar]
  53. Stark, G. , Tamar, K. , Itescu, Y. , Feldman, A. , & Meiri, S. (2018). Cold and isolated ectotherms: Drivers of reptilian longevity. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 125, 730–740. [Google Scholar]
  54. Stein, R. W. , Mull, C. G. , Kuhn, T. S. , Aschliman, N. C. , Davidson, L. N. , Joy, J. B. , … Mooers, A. O. (2018). Global priorities for conserving the evolutionary history of sharks, rays and chimaeras. Nature Ecology & Evolution, 2(2), 288–298. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Striedter, G. F. (2005). Principles of brain evolution. Sunderland, U.K.: Sinauer Associates. [Google Scholar]
  56. Tsuboi, M., Husby, A., Kotrschal, A., Hayward, A., Buechel, S. D., Zidar, J., Løvlie, H., & Kolm, N. (2014). Comparative support for the expensive tissue hypothesis: Big brains are correlated with smaller gut and greater parental investment in Lake Tanganyika cichlids. Evolution, 69(1), 190–200. Portico. 10.1111/evo.12556Tsuboi [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Tsuboi, M. , Lim, A. C. O. , Ooi, B. L. , Yip, M. Y. , Chong, V. C. , Ahnesjö, I. , & Kolm, N. (2017). Brain size evolution in pipefishes and seahorses: The role of feeding ecology, life history and sexual selection. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 30(1), 150–160. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Tsuboi, M. , van der Bijl, W. , Kopperud, B. T. , Erritzøe, J. , Voje, K. L. , Kotrschal, A. , … Kolm, N. (2018). Breakdown of brain–body allometry and the encephalization of birds and mammals. Nature Ecology & Evolution, 2, 1492–1500. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Van Eenennaam, J. P. , Doroshov, S. I. , Moberg, G. P. , Watson, J. G. , Moore, D. S. , & Linares, J. (1996). Reproductive conditions of the Atlantic sturgeon (Acipenser oxyrinchus) in the Hudson River. Estuaries, 19(4), 769. [Google Scholar]
  60. Vonk, J. (2016). Bigger brains may make better problem‐solving carnivores. Learning & Behavior, 44(2), 99–100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Wilkinson, G. S. , & Adams, D. M. (2019). Recurrent evolution of extreme longevity in bats. Biology Letters, 15(4), 20180860. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Yu, X. , Zhong, M. J. , Li, D. Y. , Jin, L. , Liao, W. B. , & Kotrschal, A. (2018). Large‐brained frogs mature later and live longer. Evolution, 72(5), 1174–1183. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Appendix S1. Supporting Information

Appendix S2. Supporting Information

Data Availability Statement

All data used in the analyses and the associated metadata are available in Supporting Information Appendix S1. All codes used in the analyses are available in Supporting Information Appendix S2.


Articles from Journal of Fish Biology are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES