ABSTRACT
This review describes the advantages of adopting a molluscan complementary model, the freshwater snail Lymnaea stagnalis, to study the neural basis of learning and memory in appetitive and avoidance classical conditioning; as well as operant conditioning of its aerial respiratory and escape behaviour. We firstly explored ‘what we can teach Lymnaea’ by discussing a variety of sensitive, solid, easily reproducible and simple behavioural tests that have been used to uncover the memory abilities of this model system. Answering this question will allow us to open new frontiers in neuroscience and behavioural research to enhance our understanding of how the nervous system mediates learning and memory. In fact, from a translational perspective, Lymnaea and its nervous system can help to understand the neural transformation pathways from behavioural output to sensory coding in more complex systems like the mammalian brain. Moving on to the second question: ‘what can Lymnaea teach us?’, it is now known that Lymnaea shares important associative learning characteristics with vertebrates, including stimulus generalization, generalization of extinction and discriminative learning, opening the possibility to use snails as animal models for neuroscience translational research.
Keywords: associative learning, behaviour, brain–behaviour, complementary models, invertebrates, L. stagnalis, Mollusca, memory, neuroscience translational research, psychiatric disorders
I. INTRODUCTION
Learning about the relationships between stimuli (i.e. classical conditioning) and the consequences of one's own behaviour (i.e. operant conditioning) is part of the ‘survival‐pack’ that animals use to adapt to an ever‐changing environment (Abel & Lattal, 2001). It would be tremendously ineffective if each experience encountered was considered unique, because this would require that learning must take place de novo each time a situation is encountered. For that reason, the ability to encode, store, and retrieve information from previous experiences is necessary for making predictions, recognizing time‐based patterns and generating appropriate behaviours (Abel & Lattal, 2001).
Based on its temporal persistence, memory can be categorized as: short‐term memory (STM; persisting for minutes), intermediate‐term memory (ITM; lasting 2–3 h), and long‐term memory (LTM; persisting >18 h) (Rosenzweig et al., 1993; Abel & Lattal, 2001). The process by which the transient STM is converted to a more stable LTM is generally referred to as consolidation and is dependent on altered gene activity and new protein synthesis (Squire et al., 2015). However, LTM is far from being ‘fixed’. In fact, to respond in a flexible and adaptive manner to continuously changing environments, the stored memory can be weakened, disrupted, or enhanced (Alberini & Ledoux, 2013). For example, following memory retrieval, the memory reverts to a labile state and must undergo a reconsolidation process for it to be retained. However, the memory trace may be altered during the reconsolidation process in response to changes in the environment (Agren, 2014).
Over the last century, a vast repertoire of classical and operant conditioning procedures has provided a solid foundation to explain various aspects of learning and memory (Brembs, 2003; Byrne & Hawkins, 2015). The core of classical conditioning (i.e. Pavlovian conditioning) lies in the temporal‐contingent association between two stimuli: an initially neutral stimulus (the conditional stimulus, CS) and a biologically relevant stimulus (the unconditional stimulus, US). By the temporal and forward pairing of the CS with the US, the CS comes to evoke a response that is similar to the response (i.e. behaviour) that the US evoked (Pavlov, 1927; Walters, Carew & Kandel, 1979).
In operant conditioning, the frequency of a behaviour is increased or decreased (depending on the reinforcer used) by the consequences of the behaviour. Reinforcement can either be negative, leading to a decrease in the frequency of the behaviour, or positive, resulting in an increase (Brembs, 2003). Thus, operant conditioning is concerned with an association between the behaviour of an organism and its environment.
II. LYMNAEA STAGNALIS IN NEUROSCIENCE AND BEHAVIOURAL RESEARCH
One of the most remarkable discoveries by Charles Darwin is that evolution is conservative (Griffiths et al., 1999). When a mechanism is successful, natural selection tends to retain it, and it is transmitted to subsequent generations. This has taken place with cellular processes underlying learning and memory (Kandel & Schwartz, 1982). Although there are large phylogenetic differences and extensive variability in neural organization in the animal kingdom; the cellular and molecular basis of learning and memory is conserved (Byrne & Hawkins, 2015). Thus, a focus of neuroscientists is the development of translational approaches to study the conserved mechanisms underlying learning and memory (Willner, 1986; Rodgers et al., 1997). In this regard, it is of great importance to identify and select the most appropriate animal models, focussing on homologous/analogous behaviours, neuronal circuits, and molecular cascades (Byrne & Hawkins, 2015).
In this context, invertebrates offer several experimental advantages over other possible model systems. Chief among these is that they possess a relatively simple nervous system compared to mammals that mediates relatively simple behaviours that are tractable (Tascedda et al., 2015). Although invertebrates are often identified as ‘alternative models’, the term ‘complementary models’ might be more appropriate. That is, they can and should be used in addition to, and not as an alternative, to classic mammalian models. Invertebrate model systems are also, compared to mammalian systems, relatively inexpensive and they allow fine dissection of the neuronal circuits and underlying molecular pathways of memory and learning (Rivi et al., 2020). Among a wide variety of invertebrate models, the freshwater pond snail Lymnaea stagnalis (henceforth Lymnaea), has been widely recognized as useful model for the study of the behavioural, cellular, and molecular mechanisms underlying learning and memory (Willner, 1986; Rodgers et al., 1997). Lymnaea has a wide Holarctic distribution, mainly inhabiting ponds and lakes (Kemenes & Benjamin 2009; Fodor et al., 2020a ). The rich behavioural repertoire that these snails use to survive and adapt to their natural environment makes Lymnaea a remarkable model system with which to study associative learning and the neuronal and molecular mechanisms of memory formation (Rivi et al., 2020). Lymnaea possess relatively simple but important homeostatic behaviours whose underlying neuronal circuitry has been well elucidated (Benjamin & Kemenes, 2010). Moreover, many of these behaviours are tractable and are relatively easy to train (Benjamin, Staras & Kemenes, 2000).
In this review, we highlight the most widely employed classical and operant conditioning procedures used in Lymnaea, offering the possibility to study both ‘why’ and ‘when’ associative learning occurs. In particular, by focusing on the animal's responses to internal and external stimuli at various times during behavioural training, it is possible to study the temporal dimension of memory formation and storage. Interestingly, studies from Lymnaea confirmed previous research conducted in Aplysia and other model systems demonstrating that ITM is an additional form of new‐protein‐dependent memory that persists for a few hours and is later transformed into LTM (Emptage & Carew, 1993; Lukowiak et al., 2000; Braun & Lukowiak, 2011). These studies add complexity to the standard dichotomy between STM and LTM and show that what we call ‘memory’ is a continuum in time and space, where the past, present, and future are acting almost in parallel (Rosenzweig et al., 1993).
At neuronal level, the nervous system of Lymnaea consists of about 20000 large (up to 150 μm in diameter) neurons, organized in a ring of interconnected ganglia, offering a relatively large amount of biological material that can be analysed molecularly, physiologically, and morphologically (Hawkins, Hon & Ren, 2010; Tascedda et al., 2015). The neurons can be easily removed and placed in culture, where they reform the appropriate synaptic connections (Syed, Bulloch & Lukowiak, 1990). Thus, single neurons can be identified and analysed as part of defined circuits, allowing electrophysiological dissection of the networks involved in relatively simple rhythmic behaviours, such as feeding and aerial respiration (ter Maat, 1992; Whelan & McCrohan, 1996; Jones, Kemenes & Benjamin, 2001; Jones et al., 2003; Feng et al., 2009). These rhythmic movements are induced by groups of central pattern‐generating neurons (CPGs) (Katz, 2016), whose characterization is critical for understanding where and how the nervous system controls these homeostatic behaviours and how the interplay between CPGs and external stimuli participates in the production of adaptive learned behaviours (Syed et al., 1990; Yeoman, Brierley & Benjamin, 1996; Spencer, Syed & Lukowiak, 1999; Spencer et al., 2002; Straub, 2004). These CPG circuits can be plastically reconfigured via environmental changes, experiences, and conditioning procedures to optimize the output to meet specific behavioural demands (Katz, 2016). Neuronal plasticity exhibited in the CPG circuits plays an important role in regulating the initiation and temporal output of behavioural rhythms in response to rewarding/aversive stimuli (as occurs in classical conditioning) and action–outcome contingencies (as occurs in operant conditioning) (Kojima et al., 1997). Using in vitro and semi‐intact preparations (which allow monitoring the behaviour and neural activity simultaneously), the CPGs controlling feeding and aerial respiration have been well studied in Lymnaea and learning‐induced changes elucidated with cellular precision (Lukowiak, 1991; Kemenes et al., 1997; Spencer et al., 1999, 2002; Lukowiak & Syed, 1999; McComb, 2005). Studies such as these cannot easily be performed in most vertebrate preparations because their behaviours are more complex, and the underlying neuronal circuitries are more inaccessible to direct cellular and synaptic analyses (Kemenes, Staras & Benjamin, 1997).
An additional advantage is that molluscan neurons are unipolar and the single process (i.e. the primary neurite) emerging from the soma is the site where most synaptic interactions and normal neuronal activities and behaviours are mediated (Syed et al., 1990, 1992; Scheibenstock et al., 2002). Fascinating and noteworthy is the finding that the primary neurite can not only survive after the surgical ablation of the soma, but is also competent to synthesize new proteins without the soma being present (Spencer et al., 2000). For that reason, it has been demonstrated that following removal of the soma ITM can occur but not LTM (Scheibenstock et al., 2002). These studies have allowed researchers to distinguish between the sites (i.e. neurites and soma) in which memories are processed (Scheibenstock et al., 2002). The characteristics of molluscan neurons differ from most vertebrate and mammal preparations, where disruption of the neuronal soma usually causes death of the entire cell (Saleuddin & Mukai, 2017).
Finally, quantitative changes in gene expression induced by conditioning can be studied at the level of single neurons, helping us to elucidate which molecules participate in the dialogue between the synapse and the nucleus and vice versa during memory and learning (Rivi et al., 2020).
The molecular mechanisms of LTM involve highly conserved signalling pathways [such as cyclic AMP‐dependent protein kinase A (cAMP/PKA), nitric oxide/cyclic GMP (NO/cGMP), mitogen‐activated protein kinase (MAPK), N‐methyl‐D‐aspartate (NMDA) receptors, and Ca2+/calmodulin‐dependent protein kinase II (CaMKII)], transcriptional regulation of gene expression by cAMP response element‐binding protein (CREB) and CCAAT‐enhancer‐binding proteins (C/EBPs), and new protein synthesis (Kemenes, 2008; Rosenegger, Wright & Lukowiak, 2010; Korneev et al., 2018; Rivi et al., 2020). Moreover, studies from Lymnaea confirmed that the induction of ITM is protein synthesis‐dependent but RNA synthesis‐independent, whereas STM requires neither protein nor RNA synthesis and LTM requires both (reviewed in Rivi et al., 2020).
III. CLASSICAL CONDITIONING IN LYMNAEA STAGNALIS
Feeding is a rhythmic behaviour that has proved remarkably useful to investigate both reward (Alexander, Audesirk & Audesirk, 1984; Kemenes & Benjamin, 1989) and aversive classical conditioning (Kojima et al., 1996). Feeding behaviour in Lymnaea shares important aspects with vertebrates, such as a strong dependence on external and internal variables and stimulus generalization and discrimination (Kemenes & Benjamin, 2009).
The well‐characterized CPG controlling feeding in Lymnaea has been a major focus of learning and memory studies, allowing investigators to correlate conditioning‐induced changes at the behavioural level with neuronal modifications in the CPG feeding circuit. In this circuit, neuron 1 medial (N1M), neuron 2 (N2), and neuron 3 tonic (N3t) cells form part of the feeding CPG (Elliott & Benjamin, 1985; Yeoman et al., 1995; Brierley, Staras & Benjamin, 1997; Straub et al., 2002; Braak et al., 2013) and their activity is regulated by higher order interneurons, termed cerebral giant cells (CGCs) and cerebro‐buccal interneurons (CBIs) (McCrohan, 1984). When food is present, the feeding response is generated by a cascade of depolarization through the sensory neurons (SNs) to the CBIs that excite N1M which, in turn, activates motor neurons triggering feeding behaviour. In the absence of food and in satiated snails, tonic inhibition of the feeding network is mediated by N3t which has inhibitory monosynaptic connections with N1M (Yeoman et al., 1996). Because sucrose enhances feeding behaviour in Lymnaea (Kemenes & Benjamin, 2009), it can be used as a rewarding stimulus to evoke a feeding response in food‐reward conditioning (Sadamoto et al., 2010; Ito, Totani & Oike, 2017). On the other hand, potassium chloride (KCl) elicits escape behaviour, shutting down the feeding response; thus KCl can act as an aversive stimulus in conditioned taste aversion (Kojima et al., 1996).
(1). Classical food‐reward conditioning
Classical appetitive food‐reward conditioning involves a temporal‐contingent repeated presentation of a neutral CS with a US that elicits feeding. In Lymnaea, the US results in a sequence of rhythmic and stereotyped feeding movements consisting of opening the mouth, rasping with the radula, and closing the mouth (Kemenes & Benjamin, 1989). A sucrose solution is typically used as the US, but tactile, chemical, or visual cues can all be used as a CS (Kemenes & Benjamin, 1989, 1994; Staras, Kemenes & Benjamin, 1998). In one of the first associative learning LTM experiments using Lymnaea (see online Supporting Information, Appendix S1 for full details of experimental procedures used in conditioning experiments with Lymnaea), investigators employed a single‐trial learning procedure consisting of a single pairing of amyl acetate, which typically does not elicit feeding, as the CS, with sucrose used as the US (Fig. S1). After the single‐trial training session (i.e. CS–US pairing), application of the CS alone in the memory test induced feeding behaviour, whereas feeding behaviour in response to the CS was not observed in any of the control groups, demonstrating associative learning (Alexander et al., 1984). This single CS–US pairing was sufficient to create a LTM trace that persisted for at least 15 days (Alexander et al., 1984).
Single‐trial conditioning of feeding using a visual cue (see Appendix S1 for methodological details) is also possible by adding the US (sucrose) to a black panel (the CS) (Andrew & Savage, 2000), which snails perceive using a lens capable of forming an image on the retina underwater (Seyer, 1992). After conditioning, approach to the black panel elicited more rasping movements in trained snails. It was also demonstrated that snails can learn to discriminate the black panel (CS) from a grey pattern of equal luminance, exhibiting the behavioural response only with the CS (Andrew & Savage, 2000).
Using appetitive classical conditioning, researchers were able to elucidate sites in the snails’ central nervous system (CNS) involved in learning this task and the mechanisms used to store memory‐related representations. In particular, some hours after training, conditioning leads to a reduction in spike activity in N3t that, in turn, makes N1M more likely to respond to the SNs (Marra et al., 2010). Thus, there is a switch of the network from an inactive to a CS‐evoked rhythmically active state (Staras et al., 2003). At the same time, the conditioning procedure induces delayed but persistent depolarization of the CGCs, that facilitates inputs of the SN‐to‐CBI excitatory synapse (Kemenes et al., 2006). Because delayed depolarization of the CGCs is known to be correlated with the establishment and the duration of LTM, it has been assumed that CGCs are involved in the maintenance of the late phase of LTM (Nikitin et al., 2008).
Memory consolidation after single‐trial chemical appetitive classical conditioning represents a dynamic process that offers the opportunity to study both at the behavioural and neurophysiological level so‐called ‘memory lapses’. During memory consolidation, multiple learning events often occur in rapid succession and competition between the various consolidating memories can emerge, resulting in memory lapses (Marra et al., 2013). Thus, single‐trial food‐reward memory could result in the erasure of newly acquired information (retroactive interference) or previous learning can affect the success of the acquisition of a second memory (proactive interference) (Crossley et al., 2019). Whether proactive or retroactive interference is activated depends on the timing of the second training and the underlying neuronal mechanisms. In particular, it was demonstrated that when new learning takes place during a stable period of the original memory, proactive interference only occurs if the two consolidating memories engage the same circuit mechanisms. On the other hand, when different circuits are used, both memories survive. New learning occurring during a labile period of consolidation instead promotes retroactive interference and the acquisition of the new memory.
The success of classical conditioning training depends on both internal (e.g. food deprivation/satiety) and external variables (e.g. water conditions) (Kemenes & Benjamin, 1994; Murakami et al., 2013). Regarding external variables, Kemenes & Benjamin (1994) showed that snails trained in a novel environment had better appetitive learning performance compared to animals trained in the water of their home tanks (i.e. a familiar environment). Moreover, the learning‐stimulating effect of the novel environment was enhanced if snails were food‐deprived before testing, whereas it was strongly suppressed in snails fed ad libitum, suggesting a reciprocal interaction between internal and external variables (Kemenes & Benjamin, 1994).
In pivotal studies of reward conditioning, Audesirk et al. (1982) demonstrated that although both ‘young’ and ‘old’ starved snails acquired the association between food and the chemo‐stimulus, ‘old’ snails had worse memory performance compared to ‘young’ ones and required multi‐trial experiments. ‘Old’ snails fed ad libitum before training failed to learn, while their younger counterparts acquired the association, but expressed the learned response only after a period of food deprivation (Audesirk et al., 1982). Further studies demonstrated that the acquisition of appetitive memory was not affected by ageing, but that memory retention and consolidation become progressively impaired with advancing age (Hermann et al., 2007). Using appetitive classical conditioning, age‐associated learning and memory deficits were associated with declining electrophysiological excitability of the CGCs, most likely reflecting a selective deficit in the activity‐dependent regulation of gene transcription, possibly through CREB‐dependent mechanisms, as demonstrated in other models (Hermann et al., 2007; Pirger et al., 2010; Scutt et al., 2015). Memory decline in aged Lymnaea is consistent with the age‐dependent impairment in learning and memory functions observed in many animal species, including humans, where the molecular, cellular, and neural network functions show a decline of neuronal experience‐dependent plasticity (Hermann et al., 2007). Together, these results indicate that the Lymnaea feeding system is a powerful tool for investigators attempting to understand the cellular and molecular correlates of age‐related declines in cognitive ability.
(2). Aversive classical conditioning of feeding behaviour
Learning what to eat, and what not to eat, is fundamental for well‐being and survival (Curtis, de Barra & Aunger, 2011). Thus, animals evolved food‐avoidance behaviours to prevent ingestion of toxic substances (Lin, Arthurs & Reilly, 2017). Because this behaviour can be conditioned by internal cues that occur after ingestion of an aversive substance [e.g. the Garcia effect (Garcia, Hankins & Rusiniak, 1974; Garcia et al., 1985)], observing the aversive reaction of a peer (Chambers, 2018), or a bitter taste (Sugai et al., 2006), it is often termed conditioned taste aversion (CTA).
Lymnaea can show CTA following pairing of an appetitive CS (e.g. sucrose or carrot juice) with an aversive US (e.g. KCl; Sugai et al., 2007). These procedures can involve either single‐trial training (i.e. single pairing of the CS–US; Fig. S2A) or multi‐trial training (i.e. multiple CS–US pairings with a short inter‐trial interval interposed between each pairing; Fig. S2B). CTA can be acquired rapidly, and can persist for up to 1 month after single‐trial conditioning (Kojima et al., 1996; Kawai et al., 2004).
Changes in activity of CPG neurons mediating feeding induced by CTA conditioning have been extensively studied in Lymnaea (Kojima et al., 1997; Ito et al., 2012b ). Before CTA training, SNs responsive to sucrose excite the CBIs of the feeding CPG to induce a feeding response, whereas the SNs activated by KCl depolarize motor neurons involved in the defensive withdrawal pathway, which in turn inhibits the feeding response (Kojima et al., 1997). Activity of the higher‐order modulatory neurons (the CGCs) is altered by the KCl‐induced input and ultimately exerts a strong polysynaptic inhibitory influence on N1M both directly and indirectly via N3t, thereby blocking the feeding response (Kojima et al., 1997).
Additionally, it has been demonstrated that after ablation of the CGC soma, the functional primary neurite continues to be a necessary participant in the mediation of CTA learning. Following soma ablation LTM cannot be formed, as it is dependent on altered gene activity that cannot take place as ablation of the soma also removes the nucleus. However, when the CGC soma is ablated after memory consolidation, snails can still retrieve the memory, indicating that while the CGC must be present for LTM formation, it is not essential for its recall (Sunada, Lukowiak & Ito, 2017).
Whether CTA learning and subsequent memory formation occur is dependent on the snail's internal state. The strongest CTA occurs when snails are trained after 1 day of food deprivation (referred to as Day‐1 snails), suggesting that a modest level of food deprivation acts as motivating factor for memory formation (Ito et al., 2015). On the other hand, when snails were subjected to severe food deprivation for 5 days (Day‐5 snails) motivation from hunger was stronger than the conditioned memory and animals continued to respond to the CS (Ito et al., 2015). The memory phenotype of Day‐5 snails was carefully investigated: after training CTA LTM was formed but its recall was prevented by the effects of food deprivation. Day‐5 snails must eat in order to survive, even if the sucrose had become a predictor of an aversive event (Ito et al., 2015). Subsequently allowing Day‐5 snails ad libitum access to food for 7 days after CTA training was not sufficient to induce memory retrieval, ruling out the possibility that very hungry snails could not access memory. Memory retrieval in Day‐5 snails was present after ad libitum access to food if they were food deprived for 1 day before the memory test (Day‐13 snails). What enables memory recall in Day‐13 snails thus is the creation of a condition similar to that of Day‐1 snails (Ito et al., 2015). Recently Totani et al. (2020) demonstrated that both Day‐1 and Day‐13 snails exhibit CTA memory due to the presence of an ‘optimal central state’ in the snail related to a critical level of insulin; the insulinergic central state of Day‐5 snails did not allow LTM recall. This optimal central state can be re‐established by an injection of insulin, which allowed memory to be retrieved (Totani et al., 2020).
That insulin plays a role in memory formation following CTA training in Lymnaea was convincingly demonstrated in an important series of papers (Kojima et al., 1996; Sugai et al., 2006; Ito et al., 2012a , 2015; Murakami et al., 2013). In both Lymnaea and mammals, insulin, in addition to its classical role in energy metabolism, plays a role in learning and memory. Insulin is known to be beneficial in modifying the pathophysiology of Alzheimer's disease and in maintaining cognitive functions (Zhao et al., 1999; Dou et al., 2005; Smith et al., 2010; Mita et al., 2014).
CTA LTM is robust and is very resistant to a memory extinction procedure consisting of the presentation of the CS alone three times at 10 min intervals at the end of CTA training (Sugai et al., 2006). In each session of this extinction procedure, and in the post‐extinction session, the number of bites elicited by the CS remained significantly low (Sugai et al., 2006).
Extinction is not ‘unlearning’ (indeed, the original memory can be spontaneously recovered) and therefore does not erase the response learned previously. On the contrary, there is evidence that extinction training results in a new memory that may compete with the original memory (Sangha et al., 2003c ; Sugai et al., 2006). The mechanism(s) that allow one memory to dominate over the other remain unclear, however there is a direct proportionality between the robustness of the original memory and its resistance to extinction (Sugai et al., 2006), confirming that CTA memories are strong. A possible explanation for the robustness of CTA memories is that the new CS evokes a conditioned fear response. This is supported by observations of changes to heart rate in conditioned snails (Kita et al., 2011). Future work should explore the neural and molecular basis of this switch from aversive to fear conditioning and the consequent robust and long‐lasting memory. We will return to this theme when we discuss configural learning (Swinton et al., 2019) when an appetitive food is simultaneously experienced with a stimulus that signals the presence of a predator (see Section IV.1).
Using a CTA training procedure, it was also shown that snails can distinguish between appetitive stimuli during CTA and can even acquire second‐order conditioning (Fig. S2C; Sugai et al., 2006). In these experiments, two appetitive CSs (sucrose and carrot juice) and an aversive US (KCl) were used in a two‐phase training procedure. In the first phase snails were conditioned to avoid one of the appetitive food substances (CS1–US). In the second phase, the second CS (CS2) was paired 10 times with CS1, thus serving as the US. After this second training presentation (CS2–CS1), CS2 no longer acted as an appetitive stimulus, but instead acquired the ability to evoke CTA, even though it was never paired with the US (Fig. S2C; Sugai et al., 2006). This procedure showed the presence in the Lymnaea model system of higher‐order conditioning previously found only in vertebrates (Gewirtz, 2000).
CTA studies are extremely useful in terms of translational medicine. Food avoidance, fear conditioning, and resistance to extinction are all hallmarks of CTA in mammals. Moreover, in rodents and humans, CTA has been associated with digestive dysfunction and has been viewed as allowing rapid learning of food toxicity that is then maintained as LTM (Garcia et al., 1974). However, while one‐trial training is sufficient to establish CTA in mammals, in Lymnaea learning occurs after a single‐training procedure in only about 40% of the snails (Sugai et al., 2007), suggesting that some snails can acquire CTA faster than others, despite being reared under the same conditions. CTA can be established in almost all snails with multi‐trial training (Sugai et al., 2006, 2007).
Recently Nakai et al. (2020) demonstrated that in Lymnaea CTA is consolidated to LTM via protein synthesis‐dependent processes, and showed that memory consolidation begins during the period of CTA training (10 CS–US pairings). Future studies of Lymnaea could allow us to accumulate new insights into the molecular events associated with LTM.
IV. OPERANT CONDITIONING IN LYMNAEA STAGNALIS
Compared with vertebrates, relatively few studies of operant conditioning have been performed on invertebrates, and in the vast majority of cases the behaviours studied are relatively simple and reflexive (Carew, 1996; Lukowiak et al., 1996). Operant conditioning of aerial respiration in Lymnaea is a fascinating exception that has provided insights into basic and advanced cognition (Dickinson, 1987).
(1). Operant conditioning of aerial respiration behaviour
Lymnaea is a bimodal breather: under normoxic (i.e. eumoxia) conditions, gaseous exchange occurs via transpiration across the skin (cutaneous exchange). In hypoxic conditions [as often arise in their pond‐water environment], snails come to the water surface for aerial respiration, consisting of rhythmic opening and closing of the pneumostome (Boycott, 1936; Jones, 1961). Aerial respiratory behaviour can thus be used in operant conditioning without affecting the survival of the animal, as they can obtain oxygen via cutaneous exchange (Lukowiak et al., 1996). In a ground‐breaking series of experiments the neuronal basis of the CPG that underlies aerial respiration in Lymnaea was elucidated (Syed et al., 1990, 1992), showing that a three‐neuron network was both sufficient and necessary for aerial respiratory behaviour. Few, if any, other neuronal circuits underlying an easily observable and tractable behaviour have been described, and this system has been used to investigate the neuronal mechanisms of learning and LTM.
Lukowiak et al. (1996) demonstrated that the aerial respiration behaviour can be operantly conditioned by applying a relatively weak tactile stimulus to the pneumostome each time snails attempt to open it. This negative reinforcement causes the snail to close its pneumostome but does not cause the animal to withdraw its foot and mantle area (i.e. the whole‐animal withdrawal response). This learning undergoes consolidation into either ITM or LTM (Lukowiak et al., 2000).
ITM, and LTM can be differentially produced by altering the duration, number and intervals used in training protocols for this behaviour (Lukowiak et al., 2000). For example, an operant training procedure of two 45‐min training sessions with a 1‐h interval between training sessions, followed by a third 45‐min training session 18 h later was sufficient to produce an LTM persisting for at least 5 days (McComb et al., 2002); five training sessions of 30 min with an inter‐session interval of at least 1 h over the course of 2.5 days was required for the establishment of a LTM that persisted for at least 1 week; eight training sessions repeated over a four‐week period resulted in a LTM that persisted for at least 1 month (Lukowiak et al., 1998). A LTM lasting for 48 h can be evoked with a single day of training, by exposing snails to three series of 15‐min training sessions with an inter‐training interval of at least 1 h (Fig. S3A), whilst a 30‐min interval between sessions resulted in associative learning but memory persistence for only ~3 h, designated as ITM by Lukowiak et al. (2000) (Fig. S3B) (Lukowiak et al., 2000; Smyth, Sangha & Lukowiak 2002). Moreover, Smyth, Sangha & Lukowiak (2002) demonstrated that if snails received the ITM training procedure followed by 5 h in their home aquaria, subsequent training with the LTM procedure led to a LTM that persisted for at least 72 h; 24 h longer than usually produced by this LTM training procedure on its own.
It was proposed that the ITM training procedure initiates the translation of pre‐existing mRNA into proteins that bring about the neuronal changes necessary for memory. Although 5 h after the ITM training session memory can no longer be demonstrated behaviourally, it appears that at a neuronal level, some changes persisted for longer constituting a ‘trace’ of memory that was sufficient to enhance LTM formation and maintenance. This mechanism was described as “gone but not forgotten” by Smyth et al. (2002). If LTM training occurred 8 h after ITM training, LTM augmentation did not occur, suggesting that this inter‐training interval was too long for the memory trace to be maintained (Smyth et al., 2002). These results indicate that if the ITM trace is still present during LTM conditioning, then the memory trace can be reconstructed from a pre‐formed framework, enabling it to last for longer, whereas if LTM training occurred after the ITM trace had disappeared then the neurons must build a new memory framework (Smyth et al., 2002).
It was also demonstrated that extinction training after ITM training did not increase memory persistence following subsequent LTM training (Smyth et al., 2002). Extinction of the ITM was achieved by placing ITM‐trained snails in the same hypoxic environment for 90 min without a reinforcing stimulus. The snails were then immediately subjected to LTM training, but the memory phenotype did not persist for longer than 48 h (Smyth et al., 2002). Because extinction is viewed as a form of learning that co‐exists with the previously learned behaviour, it was assumed that ‘ITM protein’ was used up or replaced with ‘extinction protein’, resulting in no LTM enhancement (Smyth et al., 2002).
Operant conditioning of aerial respiration clearly represents a versatile behavioural procedure that can be used to elucidate what Smyth et al. (2002) defined as the “lingering effects” of ITM on LTM persistence in the title of their paper. Future studies using this system could investigate the neuronal and molecular changes involved in LTM enhancement and whether these are conserved in invertebrate and vertebrate models.
Both ITM and LTM can be extinguished (McComb et al., 2002). In this study, snails received three training sessions followed by extinction training in the same temporal sequence as the operant conditioning training but without a reinforcing stimulus. LTM was not observed when tested the following day (McComb et al., 2002).
From a neuronal perspective, the CPG driving aerial respiration behaviour consists of three interneurons: right pedal dorsal 1 (RPeD1), visceral dorsal 4 (VD4) and (wide‐acting) input 3 (IP3). Spiking activity in RPeD1 initiates rhythmic patterns of spiking activity of IP3 that results in pneumostome opening through monosynaptic excitatory synaptic connections to visceral J (VJ) pneumostome opener motor neurons. On the contrary, monosynaptic excitatory connections exist from VD4 to VK pneumostome closer motor neurons (Syed et al., 1990; Spencer et al., 1999). After conditioning, the excitatory input from RPeD1 to IP3 is weakened, resulting in reduction of the rhythmic activity of the pneumostome (Spencer et al., 1999).
Direct evidence for the role of RPeD1 in operant conditioning comes from soma‐ablated snails: when the soma of RPeD1 was ablated before training, the CPG is only capable of mediating ITM, with LTM is no longer observed (Scheibenstock et al., 2002; Sangha, McComb & Lukowiak, 2003a ). These data indicate that ITM and LTM are not only distinct in a chronological and transcriptional manner, but are also different at the neuronal level (Braun & Lukowiak, 2011). Because ITM is dependent on new protein synthesis but not altered gene activity (Scheibenstock et al., 2002), it seems that the remaining functional neurites translate the pre‐existing messenger RNA (mRNA) into new proteins (Braun & Lukowiak, 2011). However, following ablation of the soma of RPeD1 after consolidation, snails could still access or retrieve a previously encoded memory, indicating that the soma was not essential for memory retention (Lukowiak et al., 2003; Sangha, Scheibenstock & Lukowiak, 2003b ). Thus, studies in Lymnaea demonstrate for the first time that a single neuron can be a site (although not necessarily the only site) for LTM formation and storage, enabling direct investigations of interactions between a single cell and a neuronal network that are necessary for memory formation, reconsolidation and extinction (Sangha et al., 2003a , b , c ).
Because extinction is due to new memory, ablation of the soma of RPeD1 after LTM formation could potentially prevent memory erasure (Sangha et al., 2003a , 2005). Moreover, because ablating the soma of RPeD1 disrupts the reconsolidation of a LTM, the critical molecular processes occurring during both consolidation and reconsolidation not only require both new RNA and protein synthesis, but also that these take place in the same cell in Lymnaea. These data are consistent with studies in mammals, where both memory formation and reconsolidation use similar pathways and occur in the same brain regions as the original learning (Nader, Schafe & Le Doux, 2000; Abel & Lattal, 2001). Sangha et al. (2005) showed that ‘forgetting’ was an active process requiring altered gene activity and new protein synthesis. Ablation of the soma of RPeD1 after LTM consolidation resulted in snails that did not forget (LTM persisted for significantly longer).
Operant conditioning of Lymnaea aerial respiratory behaviour represents a useful tool to explore how internal and external variables affect learning and to elucidate how the context in which the snails are trained and tested interferes with associative learning (Fig. S4; Haney & Lukowiak, 2001). Haney & Lukowiak (2001) thereby demonstrated the context specificity of learning and LTM in Lymnaea. By training snails in a hypoxic environment with a food odorant (carrot odour), LTM was demonstrated only if carrot odour was present also at the time of the memory test (Haney & Lukowiak, 2001). Interestingly, in Lymnaea not only learning, but also extinction is context dependent. In particular, if extinction training was given in a different context from that in which the learning and memory occurred (e.g. hypoxic pond water versus food‐odorant context), then extinction did not occur (McComb et al., 2002). These data confirm that extinction is itself a form of learning that occludes the original learned behaviour.
Interestingly, Lukowiak et al. (2003) demonstrated that snails trained in one context (context 1) but experiencing a new context (context 2) during the reconsolidation period (i.e. during the time when memory is labile) showed memory for context 2. This was defined by the authors as “memory infidelity”, where “infidelity” means that snails develop a memory for something they did not undergo training for (Fig. S5). When snails were exposed to context 2 only during the reconsolidation period they incorporated that new context into LTM, even though it was not part of their training (i.e. memory infidelity). Although control snails experienced context 2 for the same interval as the experimental group, because the exposure occurred after the reconsolidation period, memory infidelity did not occur. This approach identified ‘unfaithful’ memory for the first time in a molluscan model system. Moreover, it was the first time that this process had been observed in a non‐declarative memory in any model system (Lukowiak et al., 2007). Considering that it is relatively easy to modify a memory after its reactivation, future studies could attempt to use this system to gain new insights into how memories are made, maintained and modulated.
The operant conditioning paradigm of aerial respiration has also been used to explore how memory formation in Lymnaea is affected by environmental stress. Environmental stressors can have a strong, and sometimes unpredictable, modulatory effect on learning and memory formation depending on the nature of the stressor and when it is encountered relative to a period of learning. In Lymnaea, as in vertebrates, stressors may block or enhance learning and memory formation (Lukowiak et al., 2014).
We previously explored how different periods of food deprivation could alter the CTA LTM (Ito et al., 2015). Studies using aerial operant conditioning also demonstrate a better ability to form memory in snails trained in the presence of kairomones of their crayfish predator (Cambarus spp.) (i.e. the crayfish effluent) (Forest et al., 2016; see Fig. S6). Note, however, that the snails used by Forest et al. (2016) were first trained as juveniles, which typically are not capable of forming LTM (McComb, Varshney & Lukowiak, 2005). Interestingly, when those snails were allowed to mature into adults during 4 weeks in their home aquaria and were then trained using the ITM procedure in pond water they formed a LTM. Thus, training the juvenile snails in the presence of crayfish effluent enabled formation of LTM in response to the ITM procedure (Forest et al., 2016).
Adult snails trained with the LTM procedure in the presence of crayfish effluent exhibited increased persistence of LTM, that lasted for at least 8 h, suggesting that predator‐augmented memory formation allows the memory to be recalled under a broader range of challenges (Forest et al., 2016). Interestingly, this memory enhancement is associated with long‐lasting changes in RPeD1 (Orr & Lukowiak, 2008), suggesting that interspecific chemical communication can augment memory formation, inducing long‐lasting changes in this single neuron necessary for LTM formation. This research opens the possibility to investigate how ecologically relevant stressful stimuli can alter behaviour, learning and memory at the level of a single neuron (Dalesman & Lukowiak, 2012). Enhanced LTM formation after training in the presence of crayfish effluent is not observed if snails are exposed to the serotonin blockers mianserin and methysergide, meaning that the perception of risk activates a neuronal mechanism that enhances memory formation through serotonergic modulation (Forest et al., 2016). These data are consistent with previous studies in humans correlating increased serotoninergic tone with enhanced responses to anxiety‐related stimuli and memory consolidation (Inoue, Koyama & Yamashita, 1993; Ji & Suga, 2007). Stress‐mediated memory enhancement may not necessarily be beneficial and can be exacerbated in pathologies such as post‐traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and panic attacks (Lancaster et al., 2016). Thus, Lymnaea represents a valid model system for elucidating how behaviourally relevant stressors can alter LTM formation and/or its persistence and may guide researchers towards possible therapeutic strategies for these psychiatric disorders.
In a recent study, Swinton et al. (2019) demonstrated that snails can exhibit configural learning, a higher‐order form of learning and memory involving the ability to assign importance to different stimuli. In their protocol, snails simultaneously experienced the odours of crayfish effluent and carrot. Afterwards, animals were transferred for 2 h into their home aquarium where neither odour was present. Snails were then trained with a single training session lasting 30 min (ITM procedure) in hypoxic pond water containing carrot odour. Memory was tested 24 h later in hypoxic pond water and it was found that LTM was formed (Fig. S7). Enhanced LTM formation confirmed that the simultaneous exposure to the two stimuli together led carrot odour, which typically elicits feeding behaviour (Sugai et al., 2006), to acquire the properties of the predator kairomone in enhancing memory‐forming ability (Orr et al., 2007). This result did not occur when carrot odour was paired with boiled crayfish effluent, as boiling inactivated its predator‐signalling properties (Orr et al., 2007), nor with simultaneous exposure to both odours for only 10 min. Finally, enhanced LTM formation was not observed if training occurred in pond water in the absence of carrot odour. These data show that snails not only possess the ability to consider the individual components of a stimulus but also can form a relationship between the two components that is treated differently from the sum of these stimuli. Interestingly, simultaneous exposure for 45 min to both odours was shown to alter the feeding responses that typically occur when snails are exposed to carrot odour (Swinton et al., 2019). At the end of the conditioning procedure carrot odour no longer elicited a significant increase in the feeding response, suggesting that after pairing of the two odours snails can assign a new meaning to carrot odour that now signals ‘fear’ rather than ‘food’. In other words, carrot odour acquired a new motivational state (i.e. fear) as opposed to its intrinsic motivational state (i.e. enhanced rasping).
Experience with predators not only shapes learning to guide value‐based behaviours, but that value can also be transferred between stimuli that perceptually or conceptually resemble one another (Orr et al., 2007). Such stimulus generalization confers important adaptive advantages allowing the animal to decipher the meaning of important stimuli and to generalize the context of a predator encounter to other environmental conditions. Thus, it is not surprising that this learning process has been conserved across taxa (Swinton et al., 2019).
The experiments and results described above highlight the power of behavioural training in Lymnaea to elucidate the neuronal–behavioural crosstalk involved in learning and to study the role of context, stress and internal variables on memory formation and retrieval. The ability of snails to undergo configural learning confirms their potential in neuroscience and behavioural research.
(2). Operant conditioning of escape behaviour
Escape behaviour is the result of a complex integration of information from sensory systems, internal states and expectations, and represents an interesting tool to investigate the molecular and behavioural mechanisms by which organisms acquire, store, and make use of their past experiences (Kobayashi et al., 1998; Evans et al., 2019). Benatti et al. (2020) demonstrated that the escape behaviour in Lymnaea (Fig. S8) can be operantly conditioned by repeatedly exposing snails to negative reinforcement (KCl) (see Appendix S1 for full details). From the fourth day of training snails trained with KCl attempted to escape significantly less often in both the pre‐ and post‐test session (i.e. when KCl was not present), indicating that a memory had been formed. This effect became more pronounced over time and was accompanied by a significant increase in the latency to first escape (Benatti et al., 2020).
These results suggest that retrieval of the previously acquired memory allows snails to predict and avoid noxious stimuli. Interestingly, this behavioural protocol was used to compare the performance of adult and ‘old’ snails, demonstrating that ageing did not affect the acquisition of escape memory, but its consolidation in aged snails required more time (Benatti et al., 2020). Lymnaea, unlike other well‐characterized invertebrate models such as Drosophila melanogaster and Caenorhabditis elegans, has a relatively long lifespan, allowing the identification of distinct age categories that could be linked with age‐related modifications to genetic, molecular, and cellular mechanisms, which often require time to manifest their effects (Rivi et al., 2020). Future studies could investigate the impact of age‐related changes in electrophysiological activity, motor and/or chemosensory functions, and the functionality of biochemical components of memory formation on escape behaviour in ageing snails (Fodor et al., 2020b ). We also suggest that operant conditioning of escape behaviour, rarely employed in the last 20 years, could be used to unravel a variety of sophisticated cognitive phenomena that were previously thought to be restricted to vertebrates or humans, such as goal‐directed decision‐making.
V. CONCLUSIONS
This review firstly explored ‘what we can teach Lymnaea’, by discussing a variety of sensitive, solid, easily reproducible, and simple behavioural tests that allow us to investigate how snails modify their behaviours to survive and adapt in an ever‐changing environment. Although these conditioning paradigms are based on a restricted set of learned behaviours that snails perform to receive rewards or avoid negative reinforcers, there are other learned behaviours in Lymnaea and further studies will allow greater insights into the memory abilities of this model system. Learned behaviours reflect plastic alterations at both the neuronal and molecular level and are dependent on various factors including external stimuli, past experiences, and changes in internal homeostasis. From a translational perspective, Lymnaea and its nervous system, can help to understand the flow of neural transformation from behavioural output to sensory coding in more complex systems like the mammalian brain.
So ‘what can Lymnaea teach us’? Classical and operant conditioning of its feeding and respiratory behaviour have allowed to explore the behavioural, neuronal and molecular mechanisms of memory consolidation, reconsolidation, and extinction revealing that Lymnaea shares important characteristics with vertebrates related to associative learning, such as stimulus generalization, generalization of extinction and discriminative learning, and opening the possibility that Lymnaea can be used as animal models in translational research.
Lymnaea can form LTMs not only after multi‐trial conditioning but also after single‐trial conditioning. Moreover, Lymnaea show ‘higher forms’ of learning, such as second‐order conditioning, contingency learning, and configural learning. These forms of learning also have been identified in other molluscs, including Limax (Sahley, Rudy & Gelperin, 1981) and Aplysia (Hawkins, Greene & Kandel, 1998), but in Lymnaea the neural circuits can be analysed relatively easily. Unlike other invertebrate models (with the exception of bees), Lymnaea shares with vertebrates the capacity to acquire configural learning, being able to make associations regarding predation risk and making appropriate responses in similar situations (Giurfa, 2017; Swinton et al. 2019).
Lymnaea is also ‘teaching us’ the role of context in memory formation and consolidation. Typically, snails trained in one context only showed memory if memory testing was performed in the same context, similar to results obtained for other invertebrate models such as Aplysia, C. elegans, honeybees, Drosophila and ants, and in studies on mammals (Ardiel & Rankin, 2010; Bos, Guerrieri & d'Ettorre, 2010; Kahsai & Zars, 2011; Fujinaka et al., 2016; Panoz‐Brown et al., 2016; Giurfa, 2017). However, context generalization was reported in predator‐experienced Lymnaea.
Changing the context in which memory is reconsolidated may have consequences on the accuracy of memory, resulting in ‘memory infidelity’. As in ‘higher’ organisms, including humans, activated memories in Lymnaea can re‐enter a labile state in which they can be modified or changed during the reconsolidation process, before reconsolidating into a stable, permanent state (Lukowiak et al., 2007). These findings on memory reconsolidation have triggered great interest among mental health professionals who treat disorders based on pathological memories (Alberini & Ledoux, 2013). In recent years, classical and operant conditioning procedures have been translated into preclinical and clinical practice (Bisaz, Travaglia & Alberini, 2014). In particular, memory reconsolidation has been adopted in therapeutic settings to make learning and memory creation more efficient and adaptive, to prevent or rescue memory impairments, and to ameliorate maladaptive memories linked to psychopathologies, such as those associated with PTSD and addiction (Bisaz et al., 2014).
Although many such studies have focused on humans and rodents, a translational approach based on Lymnaea may be a rapid and cost‐effective option for elucidating the causal, neuronal and molecular changes underlying memory at the level of a single cell.
Supporting information
VI. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
The authors acknowledge the great body of data on various aspects of L. stagnalis learning and memory produced by numerous colleagues over decades. We are sincerely grateful to them for their fruitful research. Open access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL.
REFERENCES
- Abel, T. & Lattal, K. M. (2001). Molecular mechanisms of memory acquisition, consolidation and retrieval. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 11, 180–187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Agren, T. (2014). Human reconsolidation: a reactivation and update. Brain Research Bulletin 105, 70–82. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alberini, C. M. & Ledoux, J. E. (2013). Memory reconsolidation. Current Biology: CB 23, R746–R750. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alexander, J. , Audesirk, T. E. & Audesirk, G. J. (1984). One‐trial reward learning in the snail Lymnaea stagnalis . Journal of Neurobiology 15, 67–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andrew, R. J. & Savage, H. (2000). Appetitive learning using visual conditioned stimuli in the pond snail, Lymnaea . Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 73, 258–273. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ardiel, E. L. & Rankin, C. H. (2010). An elegant mind: learning and memory in Caenorhabditis elegans . Learning & Memory 17, 191–201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Audesirk, T. E. , Alexander, J. E. , Audesirk, G. J. & Moyer, C. M. (1982). Rapid, nonaversive conditioning in a freshwater gastropod: I. Effects of age and motivation. Behavioral and Neural Biology 36, 379–390. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Benatti, C. , Rivi, V. , Colliva, C. , Radighieri, G. , Tascedda, F. & Blom, J. M. C. (2020). Redefining operant conditioning of escape behaviour in Lymnaea stagnalis . Invertebrate Survival Journal 17, 129–137. [Google Scholar]
- Benjamin, P. & Kemenes, G. (2010). Lymnaea learning and memory. Scholarpedia 5, 4247. [Google Scholar]
- Benjamin, P. R. , Staras, K. & Kemenes, G. (2000). A systems approach to the cellular analysis of associative learning in the pond snail Lymnaea . Learning & Memory (Cold Spring Harbor, N.Y.) 7, 124–131. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bisaz, R. , Travaglia, A. & Alberini, C. M. (2014). The neurobiological bases of memory formation: from physiological conditions to psychopathology. Psychopathology 47, 347–356. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bos, N. , Guerrieri, F. J. & d'Ettorre, P. (2010). Significance of chemical recognition cues is context dependent in ants. Animal Behaviour 80, 839–844. [Google Scholar]
- Boycott, A. E. (1936). The habitats of fresh‐water Mollusca in Britain. The Journal of Animal Ecology 5, 116. [Google Scholar]
- Braak, H. , Zetterberg, H. , del Tredici, K. & Blennow, K. (2013). Intraneuronal tau aggregation precedes diffuse plaque deposition, but amyloid‐β changes occur before increases of tau in cerebrospinal fluid. Acta Neuropathologica 126, 631–641. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Braun, M. H. & Lukowiak, K. (2011). Intermediate and long‐term memory are different at the neuronal level in Lymnaea stagnalis . Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 96, 403–416. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brembs, B. (2003). Operant conditioning in invertebrates. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 13, 710–717. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brierley, M. J. , Staras, K. & Benjamin, P. R. (1997). Behavioral function of glutamatergic interneurons in the feeding system of Lymnaea: plateauing properties and synaptic connections with motor neurons. Journal of Neurophysiology 78, 3386–3395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Byrne, J. H. & Hawkins, R. D. (2015). Nonassociative learning in invertebrates. Cold Spring Harbor Perspectives in Biology 7, a021675. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carew, T. J. (1996). Molecular enhancement of memory formation. Neuron 16, 5–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chambers, K. C. (2018). Conditioned taste aversions. World Journal of Otorhinolaryngology ‐ Head and Neck Surgery 4, 92–100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crossley, M. , Lorenzetti, F. D. , Naskar, S. , O'Shea, M. , Kemenes, G. , Benjamin, P. R. & Kemenes, I. (2019). Proactive and retroactive interference with associative memory consolidation in the snail Lymnaea is time and circuit dependent. Communications Biology 2, 1–11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Curtis, V. , de Barra, M. & Aunger, R. (2011). Disgust as an adaptive system for disease avoidance behaviour. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 366, 389–401. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dalesman, S. & Lukowiak, K. (2012). How stress alters memory in ‘smart’ snails. PLoS One 7, e32334. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dickinson, A. (1987). Animal conditioning and learning theory. In Theoretical Foundations of Behavior Therapy (eds Eysenck H. J. and Martin I.), pp. 57–79.Springer US, Boston. [Google Scholar]
- Dou, J.‐T. , Chen, M. , Dufour, F. , Alkon, D. L. & Zhao, W.‐Q. (2005). Insulin receptor signaling in long‐term memory consolidation following spatial learning. Learning & Memory (Cold Spring Harbor, N.Y.) 12, 646–655. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elliott, C. J. & Benjamin, P. R. (1985). Interactions of pattern‐generating interneurons controlling feeding in Lymnaea stagnalis . Journal of Neurophysiology 54, 1396–1411. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Emptage, N. J. & Carew, T. J. (1993). Long‐term synaptic facilitation in the absence of short‐term facilitation in Aplysia neurons. Science (New York, N.Y.) 262, 253–256. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Evans, D. A. , Stempel, A. V. , Vale, R. & Branco, T. (2019). Cognitive control of escape behaviour. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 23, 334–348. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feng, Z.‐P. , Zhang, Z. , van Kesteren, R. E. , Straub, V. A. , van Nierop, P. , Jin, K. , Nejatbakhsh, N. , Goldberg, J. I. , Spencer, G. E. , Yeoman, M. S. , Wildering, W. , Coorssen, J. R. , Croll, R. P. , Buck, L. T. , Syed, N. I. , et al. (2009). Transcriptome analysis of the central nervous system of the mollusc Lymnaea stagnalis . BMC Genomics 10, 451. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fodor, I. , Hussein, A. A. , Benjamin, P. R. , Koene, J. M. & Pirger, Z. (2020a). The unlimited potential of the great pond snail, Lymnaea stagnalis . eLife 9, e56962. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fodor, I. , Urbán, P. , Kemenes, G. , Koene, J. M. & Pirger, Z. (2020b). Aging and disease‐relevant gene products in the neuronal transcriptome of the great pond snail (Lymnaea stagnalis): a potential model of aging, age‐related memory loss, and neurodegenerative diseases. Invertebrate Neuroscience 20, 9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Forest, J. , Sunada, H. , Dodd, S. & Lukowiak, K. (2016). Training Lymnaea in the presence of a predator scent results in a long‐lasting ability to form enhanced long‐term memory. Journal of Comparative Physiology A 202, 399–409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fujinaka, A. , Li, R. , Hayashi, M. , Kumar, D. , Changarathil, G. , Naito, K. , Miki, K. , Nishiyama, T. , Lazarus, M. , Sakurai, T. , Kee, N. , Nakajima, S. , Wang, S.‐H. & Sakaguchi, M. (2016). Effect of context exposure after fear learning on memory generalization in mice. Molecular Brain 9, 2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garcia, J. , Hankins, W. G. & Rusiniak, K. W. (1974). Behavioral regulation of the milieu interne in man and rat. Science 185, 824–831. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garcia, J. , Lasiter, P. S. , Bermudez‐Rattoni, F. & Deems, D. A. (1985). A general theory of aversion learning. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 443, 8–21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gewirtz, J. C. (2000). Using Pavlovian higher‐order conditioning paradigms to investigate the neural substrates of emotional learning and memory. Learning & Memory 7, 257–266. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Giurfa, M. (2017). Behavioral analysis of learning and memory in honeybees. In Learning and Memory: A Comprehensive Reference (ed. Byrne J. H.), pp. 407–426.Academic Press, Cambridge. [Google Scholar]
- Griffiths, A. J. , Gelbart, W. M. , Miller, J. H. & Lewontin, R. C. (1999). Darwin's Revolution. Modern Genetic Analysis. W. H. Freeman, New York. [Google Scholar]
- Haney, J. & Lukowiak, K. (2001). Context learning and the effect of context on memory retrieval in Lymnaea . Learning & Memory (Cold Spring Harbor, N.Y.) 8, 35–43. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hawkins, R. D. , Greene, W. & Kandel, E. R. (1998). Classical conditioning, differential cond*itioning, and second‐order conditioning of the Aplysia gill‐withdrawal reflex in a simplified mantle organ preparation. Behavioral Neuroscience 112, 636–645. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hawkins, R. D. , Hon, G. C. & Ren, B. (2010). Next‐generation genomics: an integrative approach. Nature Reviews. Genetics 11, 476–486. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hermann, P. M. , Lee, A. , Hulliger, S. , Minvielle, M. , Ma, B. & Wildering, W. C. (2007). Impairment of long‐term associative memory in aging snails (Lymnaea stagnalis). Behavioral Neuroscience 121, 1400–1414. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Inoue, T. , Koyama, T. & Yamashita, I. (1993). Effect of conditioned fear stress on serotonin metabolism in the rat brain. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior 44, 371–374. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ito, E. , Okada, R. , Sakamoto, Y. , Otshuka, E. , Mita, K. , Okuta, A. , Sunada, H. & Sakakibara, M. (2012a). Insulin and memory in Lymnaea . Acta Biologica Hungarica 63(Suppl 2), 194–201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ito, E. , Otsuka, E. , Hama, N. , Aonuma, H. , Okada, R. , Hatakeyama, D. , Fujito, Y. & Kobayashi, S. (2012b). Memory trace in feeding neural circuitry underlying conditioned taste aversion in Lymnaea . PLoS One 7, e43151. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ito, E. , Totani, Y. & Oike, A. (2017). Necessity knows no law in a snail. The European Zoological Journal 84, 457–464. [Google Scholar]
- Ito, E. , Yamagishi, M. , Hatakeyama, D. , Watanabe, T. , Fujito, Y. , Dyakonova, V. & Lukowiak, K. (2015). Memory block: a consequence of conflict resolution. Journal of Experimental Biology 218, 1699–1704. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ji, W. & Suga, N. (2007). Serotonergic modulation of plasticity of the auditory cortex elicited by fear conditioning. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience 27, 4910–4918. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jones, J. D. (1961). Aspects of respiration in Planorbis corneus and Lymnaea stagnalis (Gastropoda: Pulmonata). Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology 4, 1–29. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jones, N. , Kemenes, G. & Benjamin, P. R. (2001). Selective expression of electrical correlates of differential appetitive classical conditioning in a feeding network. Journal of Neurophysiology 85, 89–97. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jones, N. G. , Kemenes, I. , Kemenes, G. & Benjamin, P. R. (2003). A persistent cellular change in a single modulatory neuron contributes to associative long‐term memory. Current Biology: CB 13, 1064–1069. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kahsai, L. & Zars, T. (2011). Learning and memory in Drosophila: behavior, genetics, and neural systems. In International Review of Neurobiology (ed. Atkinson N.), pp. 139–167.Academic Press, Cambridge. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kandel, E. & Schwartz, J. (1982). Molecular biology of learning: modulation of transmitter release. Science 218, 433–443. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Katz, P. S. (2016). Evolution of central pattern generators and rhythmic behaviours. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 371, 20150057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kawai, R. , Sunada, H. , Horikoshi, T. & Sakakibara, M. (2004). Conditioned taste aversion with sucrose and tactile stimuli in the pond snail Lymnaea stagnalis . Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 82, 164–168. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kemenes, G. (2008). Molecular mechanism of associative learning in Lymnaea . In Learning and Memory: A Comprehensive Reference (Volume 4, ed. Byrne J. H.), pp. 133–148.Elsevier, Oxford. [Google Scholar]
- Kemenes, G. & Benjamin, P. R. (1989). Appetitive learning in snails shows characteristics of conditioning in vertebrates. Brain Research 489, 163–166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kemenes, G. & Benjamin, P. R. (1994). Training in a novel environment improves the appetitive learning performance of the snail, Lymnaea stagnalis . Behavioral and Neural Biology 61, 139–149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kemenes, G. & Benjamin, P. R. (2009). Lymnaea. Current Biology 19, R9–R11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kemenes, G. , Staras, K. & Benjamin, P. R. (1997). In vitro appetitive classical conditioning of the feeding response in the pond snail Lymnaea stagnalis . Journal of Neurophysiology 78, 2351–2362. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kemenes, I. , Kemenes, G. , Andrew, R. J. , Benjamin, P. R. & O'Shea, M. (2002). Critical time‐window for NO‐cGMP‐dependent long‐term memory formation after one‐trial appetitive conditioning. The Journal of Neuroscience 22, 1414–1425. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kemenes, I. , Straub, V. A. , Nikitin, E. S. , Staras, K. , O'Shea, M. , Kemenes, G. & Benjamin, P. R. (2006). Role of delayed nonsynaptic neuronal plasticity in long‐term associative memory. Current Biology 16, 1269–1279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kita, S. , Hashiba, R. , Ueki, S. , Kimoto, Y. , Abe, Y. , Gotoda, Y. , Suzuki, R. , Uraki, E. , Nara, N. , Kanazawa, A. , Hatakeyama, D. , Kawai, R. , Fujito, Y. , Lukowiak, K. & Ito, E. (2011). Does conditioned taste aversion learning in the pond snail Lymnaea stagnalis produce conditioned fear? The Biological Bulletin 220, 71–81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kobayashi, S. , Kojima, S. , Yamanaka, M. , Sadamoto, H. , Nakamura, H. , Fujito, Y. , Kawai, R. , Sakakibara, M. & Ito, E. (1998). Operant conditioning of escape behavior in the pond snail, Lymnaea stagnalis . Zoological Science 15, 683–690. [Google Scholar]
- Kojima, S. , Nanakamura, H. , Nagayama, S. , Fujito, Y. & Ito, E. (1997). Enhancement of an inhibitory input to the feeding central pattern generator in Lymnaea stagnalis during conditioned taste‐aversion learning. Neuroscience Letters 230, 179–182. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kojima, S. , Yamanaka, M. , Fujito, Y. & Ito, E. (1996). Differential neuroethological effects of aversive and appetitive reinforcing stimuli on associative learning in Lymnaea stagnalis . Zoological Science 13, 803–812. [Google Scholar]
- Korneev, S. A. , Vavoulis, D. V. , Naskar, S. , Dyakonova, V. E. , Kemenes, I. & Kemenes, G. (2018). A CREB2‐targeting microRNA is required for long‐term memory after single‐trial learning. Scientific Reports 8, 1–12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lancaster, C. , Teeters, J. , Gros, D. & Back, S. (2016). Posttraumatic stress disorder: overview of evidence‐based assessment and treatment. Journal of Clinical Medicine 5, 105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lin, J.‐Y. , Arthurs, J. & Reilly, S. (2017). Conditioned taste aversions: from poisons to pain to drugs of abuse. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 24, 335–351. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lukowiak, K. (1991). Experimental reconstruction of neuronal pattern generators. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 1, 577–582. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lukowiak, K. , Adatia, N. , Krygier, D. & Syed, N. (2000). Operant conditioning in Lymnaea: evidence for intermediate‐ and long‐term memory. Learning & Memory (Cold Spring Harbor, N.Y.) 7, 140–150. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lukowiak, K. , Cotter, R. , Rose, J. , Ringseis, E. & Spencer, G. (1998). Long‐term memory of an operantly conditioned respiratory behaviour pattern in Lymnaea stagnalis . The Journal of Experimental Biology 201, 877–882. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lukowiak, K. , Fras, M. , Smyth, K. , Wong, C. & Hittel, K. (2007). Reconsolidation and memory infidelity in Lymnaea . Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 87, 547–560. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lukowiak, K. , Ringseis, E. , Spencer, G. , Wildering, W. & Syed, N. (1996). Operant conditioning of aerial respiratory behaviour in Lymnaea stagnalis . The Journal of Experimental Biology 199, 683–691. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lukowiak, K. , Sangha, S. , Scheibenstock, A. , Parvez, K. , McComb, C. , Rosenegger, D. , Varshney, N. & Sadamoto, H. (2003). A molluscan model system in the search for the engram. Journal of Physiology, Paris 97, 69–76. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lukowiak, K. , Sunada, H. , Teskey, M. , Lukowiak, K. & Dalesman, S. (2014). Environmentally relevant stressors alter memory formation in the pond snail Lymnaea . Journal of Experimental Biology 217, 76–83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lukowiak, K. & Syed, N. (1999). Learning, memory and a respiratory central pattern generator. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology ‐ Part A, Molecular & Integrative Physiology 124, 265–274. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marra, V. , Kemenes, I. , Vavoulis, D. , Feng, J. , O'Shea, M. & Benjamin, P. R. (2010). Role of tonic inhibition in associative reward conditioning in Lymnaea . Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 4, 161. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marra, V. , O'Shea, M. , Benjamin, P. R. & Kemenes, I. (2013). Susceptibility of memory consolidation during lapses in recall. Nature Communications 4, 1578. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McComb, C. (2005). Juvenile Lymnaea ventilate, learn and remember differently than do adult Lymnaea . Journal of Experimental Biology 208, 1459–1467. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McComb, C. , Sangha, S. , Qadry, S. , Yue, J. , Scheibenstock, A. & Lukowiak, K. (2002). Context extinction and associative learning in Lymnaea . Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 78, 23–34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McComb, C. , Varshney, N. & Lukowiak, K. (2005). Juvenile Lymnaea ventilate, learn and remember differently than do adult Lymnaea . The Journal of Experimental Biology 208, 1459–1467. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCrohan, C. R. (1984). Properties of ventral cerebral neurones involved in the feeding system of the snail, Lymnaea stagnalis . Journal of Experimental Biology 108, 257–272. [Google Scholar]
- Mita, K. , Yamagishi, M. , Fujito, Y. , Lukowiak, K. & Ito, E. (2014). An increase in insulin is important for the acquisition conditioned taste aversion in Lymnaea . Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 116, 132–138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moroz, L. , Hasan, S. , Bulloch, A. , Lukowiak, K. & Syed, N. (1993). Behavioral analysis of respiration in Lymnaea stagnalis . Society for Neuroscience – Abstracts 23, 183. [Google Scholar]
- Murakami, J. , Okada, R. , Sadamoto, H. , Kobayashi, S. , Mita, K. , Sakamoto, Y. , Yamagishi, M. , Hatakeyama, D. , Otsuka, E. , Okuta, A. , Sunada, H. , Takigami, S. , Sakakibara, M. , Fujito, Y. , Awaji, M. , Moriyama, S. , Lukowiak, K. & Ito, E. (2013). Involvement of insulin‐like peptide in long‐term synaptic plasticity and long‐term memory of the pond snail Lymnaea stagnalis . The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience 33, 371–383. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nader, K. , Schafe, G. E. & Le Doux, J. E. (2000). Fear memories require protein synthesis in the amygdala for reconsolidation after retrieval. Nature 406, 722–726. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nakai, J. , Totani, Y. , Kojima, S. , Sakakibara, M. & Ito, E. (2020). Features of behavioral changes underlying conditioned taste aversion in the pond snail Lymnaea stagnalis . Invertebrate Neuroscience 20, 8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nikitin, E. S. , Vavoulis, D. V. , Kemenes, I. , Marra, V. , Pirger, Z. , Michel, M. , Feng, J. , O'Shea, M. , Benjamin, P. R. & Kemenes, G. (2008). Persistent sodium current is a nonsynaptic substrate for long‐term associative memory. Current Biology 18, 1221–1226. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Orr, M. V. , El‐Bekai, M. , Lui, M. , Watson, K. & Lukowiak, K. (2007). Predator detection in Lymnaea stagnalis . Journal of Experimental Biology 210, 4150–4158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Orr, M. V. & Lukowiak, K. (2008). Electrophysiological and behavioral evidence demonstrating that predator detection alters adaptive behaviors in the snail Lymnaea . The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience 28, 2726–2734. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Panoz‐Brown, D. , Corbin, H. E. , Dalecki, S. J. , Gentry, M. , Brotheridge, S. , Sluka, C. M. , Wu, J.‐E. & Crystal, J. D. (2016). Rats remember items in context using episodic memory. Current Biology: CB 26, 2821–2826. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pavlov, I. P. (1927). Conditioned Reflexes: An Investigation of the Physiological Activity of the Cerebral Cortex. Oxford University Press, Oxford. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pirger, Z. , László, Z. , Kemenes, I. , Tóth, G. , Reglődi, D. & Kemenes, G. (2010). A homolog of the vertebrate pituitary adenylate cyclase‐activating polypeptide is both necessary and instructive for the rapid formation of associative memory in an invertebrate. Journal of Neuroscience 30, 13766–13773. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rivi, V. , Benatti, C. , Colliva, C. , Radighieri, G. , Brunello, N. , Tascedda, F. & Blom, J. M. C. (2020). Lymnaea stagnalis as model for translational neuroscience research: from pond to bench. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 108, 602–616. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rodgers, R. J. , Cao, B. J. , Dalvi, A. & Holmes, A. (1997). Animal models of anxiety: an ethological perspective. Brazilian Journal of Medical and Biological Research = Revista Brasileira De Pesquisas Medicas E Biologicas 30, 289–304. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosenegger, D. , Wright, C. & Lukowiak, K. (2010). A quantitative proteomic analysis of long‐term memory. Molecular Brain 3, 9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosenzweig, M. R. , Bennett, E. L. , Colombo, P. J. , Lee, D. W. & Serrano, P. A. (1993). Short‐term, intermediate‐term, and long‐term memories. Behavioural Brain Research 57, 193–198. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sadamoto, H. , Kitahashi, T. , Fujito, Y. & Ito, E. (2010). Learning‐dependent gene expression of CREB1 isoforms in the Molluscan brain. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 4, 25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sahley, C. , Rudy, J. W. & Gelperin, A. (1981). An analysis of associative learning in a terrestrial mollusc. Journal of Comparative Physiology 144, 1–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saleuddin, S. & Mukai, S. (2017). Physiology of Molluscs: A Collection of Selected Reviews, Two‐Volume Set. CRC Press, Boca Raton. [Google Scholar]
- Sangha, S. , McComb, C. & Lukowiak, K. (2003a). Forgetting and the extension of memory in Lymnaea . The Journal of Experimental Biology 206, 71–77. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sangha, S. , Scheibenstock, A. & Lukowiak, K. (2003b). Reconsolidation of a long‐term memory in Lymnaea requires new protein and RNA synthesis and the soma of right pedal dorsal 1. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience 23, 8034–8040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sangha, S. , Scheibenstock, A. , Martens, K. , Varshney, N. , Cooke, R. & Lukowiak, K. (2005). Impairing forgetting by preventing new learning and memory. Behavioral Neuroscience 119, 787–796. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sangha, S. , Scheibenstock, A. , Morrow, R. & Lukowiak, K. (2003c). Extinction requires new RNA and protein synthesis and the soma of the cell right pedal dorsal 1 in Lymnaea stagnalis . Journal of Neuroscience 23, 9842–9851. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scheibenstock, A. , Krygier, D. , Haque, Z. , Syed, N. & Lukowiak, K. (2002). The Soma of RPeD1 must be present for long‐term memory formation of associative learning in Lymnaea . Journal of Neurophysiology 88, 1584–1591. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scutt, G. , Allen, M. , Kemenes, G. & Yeoman, M. (2015). A switch in the mode of the sodium/calcium exchanger underlies an age‐related increase in the slow afterhyperpolarization. Neurobiology of Aging 36, 2838–2849. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seyer, J.‐O. (1992). Resolution and sensitivity in the eye of the winkle Littorina littorea . Journal of Experimental Biology 170, 57–69. [Google Scholar]
- Smith, M. , Riby, L. , Eekelen, J. & Foster, J. (2010). Glucose enhancement of human memory: a comprehensive research review of the glucose memory facilitation effect. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 35, 770–783. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smyth, K. , Sangha, S. & Lukowiak, K. (2002). Gone but not forgotten: the lingering effects of intermediate‐term memory on the persistence of long‐term memory. Journal of Experimental Biology 205, 131–140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spencer, G. E. , Kazmi, M. H. , Syed, N. I. & Lukowiak, K. (2002). Changes in the activity of a CpG neuron after the reinforcement of an operantly conditioned behavior in Lymnaea . Journal of Neurophysiology 88, 1915–1923. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spencer, G. E. , Syed, N. I. , van Kesteren, E. , Lukowiak, K. , Geraerts, W. P. & van Minnen, J. (2000). Synthesis and functional integration of a neurotransmitter receptor in isolated invertebrate axons. Journal of Neurobiology 44, 72–81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spencer, G. E. , Syed, N. I. & Lukowiak, K. (1999). Neural changes after operant conditioning of the aerial respiratory behavior in Lymnaea stagnalis . The Journal of Neuroscience 19, 1836–1843. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Squire, L. R. , Genzel, L. , Wixted, J. T. & Morris, R. G. (2015). Memory consolidation. Cold Spring Harbor Perspectives in Biology 7(8), a021766. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Staras, K. , Kemenes, G. & Benjamin, P. R. (1998). Pattern‐generating role for motoneurons in a rhythmically active neuronal network. Journal of Neuroscience 18, 3669–3688. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Staras, K. , Kemenes, G. & Benjamin, P. R. (1999). Cellular traces of behavioral classical conditioning can be recorded at several specific sites in a simple nervous system. Journal of Neuroscience 19, 347–357. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Staras, K. , Kemenes, I. , Benjamin, P. R. & Kemenes, G. (2003). Loss of self‐inhibition is a cellular mechanism for episodic rhythmic behavior. Current Biology 13, 116–124. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Straub, V. A. (2004). Central localization of plasticity involved in appetitive conditioning in Lymnaea . Learning & Memory 11, 787–793. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Straub, V. A. , Staras, K. , Kemenes, G. & Benjamin, P. R. (2002). Endogenous and network properties of Lymnaea feeding central pattern generator interneurons. Journal of Neurophysiology 88, 1569–1583. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sugai, R. , Azami, S. , Shiga, H. , Watanabe, T. , Sadamoto, H. , Kobayashi, S. , Hatakeyama, D. , Fujito, Y. , Lukowiak, K. & Ito, E. (2007). One‐trial conditioned taste aversion in Lymnaea: good and poor performers in long‐term memory acquisition. The Journal of Experimental Biology 210, 1225–1237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sugai, R. , Shiga, H. , Azami, S. , Watanabe, T. , Sadamoto, H. , Fujito, Y. , Lukowiak, K. & Ito, E. (2006). Taste discrimination in conditioned taste aversion of the pond snail Lymnaea stagnalis . The Journal of Experimental Biology 209, 826–833. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sunada, H. , Lukowiak, K. & Ito, E. (2017). Cerebral giant cells are necessary for the formation and recall of memory of conditioned taste aversion in Lymnaea . Zoological Science 34, 72–80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swinton, C. , Swinton, E. , Shymansky, T. , Hughes, E. , Zhang, J. , Rothwell, C. , Kakadiya, M. & Lukowiak, K. (2019). Configural learning: a higher form of learning in Lymnaea . The Journal of Experimental Biology 222, jeb190405. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Syed, N. I. , Bulloch, A. G. & Lukowiak, K. (1990). In vitro reconstruction of the respiratory central pattern generator of the mollusk Lymnaea . Science (New York, N.Y.) 250, 282–285. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Syed, N. I. , Ridgway, R. L. , Lukowiak, K. & Bulloch, A. G. M. (1992). Transplantation and functional integration of an identified respiratory interneuron in Lymnaea stagnalis . Neuron 8, 767–774. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tascedda, F. , Malagoli, D. , Accorsi, A. , Rigillo, G. , Blom, J. M. C. & Ottaviani, E. (2015). Molluscs as models for translational medicine. Medical Science Monitor Basic Research 21, 96–99. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- ter Maat, A. (1992). Egg laying in the hermaphrodite pond snail Lymnaea stagnalis . Progress in Brain Research 92, 345–360. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Totani, Y. , Nakai, J. , Dyakonova, V. E. , Lukowiak, K. , Sakakibara, M. & Ito, E. (2020). Induction of LTM following an insulin injection. eNeuro 7, ENEURO.0088‐20.2020. https://doi/org.10.1523/ENEURO.0088-20.2020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walters, E. T. , Carew, T. J. & Kandel, E. R. (1979). Classical conditioning in Aplysia californica . Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 76, 6675–6679. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whelan, H. A. & Mccrohan, C. R. (1996). Food‐related conditioning and neuronal correlates in the freshwater snail Lymnaea stagnalis . Journal of Molluscan Studies 62, 483–494. [Google Scholar]
- Willner, P. (1986). Validation criteria for animal models of human mental disorders: learned helplessness as a paradigm case. Progress in Neuro‐Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry 10, 677–690. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yeoman, M. S. , Brierley, M. J. & Benjamin, P. R. (1996). Central pattern generator interneurons are targets for the modulatory serotonergic cerebral giant cells in the feeding system of Lymnaea . Journal of Neurophysiology 75, 11–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yeoman, M. S. , Vehovszky, A. , Kemenes, G. , Elliott, C. J. & Benjamin, P. R. (1995). Novel interneuron having hybrid modulatory‐central pattern generator properties in the feeding system of the snail, Lymnaea stagnalis . Journal of Neurophysiology 73, 112–124. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhao, W. , Chen, H. , Xu, H. , Moore, E. , Meiri, N. , Quon, M. J. & Alkon, D. L. (1999). Brain insulin receptors and spatial memory. Correlated changes in gene expression, tyrosine phosphorylation, and signalling molecules in the hippocampus of water maze trained rats. The Journal of Biological Chemistry 274, 34893–34902. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.