Skip to main content
Wiley Open Access Collection logoLink to Wiley Open Access Collection
. 2022 Jun 5;43(4):653–676. doi: 10.1002/imhj.21995

Latin American Attachment studies: A narrative review

Katherine Fourment 1,2,, Camila Espinoza 1, Ana Carla Lima Ribeiro 3, Judi Mesman 1
PMCID: PMC9546405  PMID: 35661377

Abstract

Attachment theory´s core hypotheses (universality, normativity, sensitivity, and competence) are assumed to be applicable worldwide. However, the majority of studies on attachment theory have been conducted in Western countries, and the extent to which these core hypotheses are supported by research conducted in Latin America has never been systematically addressed. The purpose of this systematic narrative literature review is to provide an integrative discussion of the current body of empirical studies concerning attachment theory conducted in Latin American countries. For that purpose, a search was conducted in four electronic databases (Web of Science, PsycInfo, SciELO, and Redalyc) and 82 publications on attachment and/or sensitivity met inclusion criteria. None of the studies reported cases in which an attachment relationship was absent, and a predominance of secure attachment patterns was found, mainly for non‐risk samples (NRS). Sensitivity levels were generally deemed adequate in NRS, and related to attachment quality. Attachment security and caregivers’ sensitivity were positively associated with child outcomes. Attachment‐based intervention studies mostly showed efficacy. In conclusion, Latin American research supports the key theoretical assumptions of attachment theory, mainly in samples of urban middle‐class NRS. However, the field of attachment‐related research would be enriched by also investing in Latin American studies on caregiving rooted in local concepts and theories.

Keywords: attachment; attachment theory; Latin America; parenting; sensitivity; América Latina; teoría de la afectividad; afectividad; sensibilidad; crianza; Mots clés: Amérique Latin; théorie de l'attachement; attachement; sensibilité; parentage; Schlüsselwörter: Lateinamerika; Bindungstheorie; Bindung; Sensibilität; Elternschaft; キーワード:ラテンアメリカ、愛着理論、アタッチメント、感受性、子育て; 关键词:拉丁美洲, 依恋理论, 依恋, 敏感性, 育儿; الكلمات الرئيسية: أمريكا اللاتينية ، نظرية التعلق ، التعلق ، الحساسية ، التربية

1. INTRODUCTION

More than 600 million people (nearly 9% of world population) live in the 20 countries that make up Latin America. Yet the Latin American literature on parenting and child development is often not considered in the international scientific discourse about attachment, even though it has seen substantial growth in the past decades. This way, in order to better understand attachment theory, which claims to reflect universal processes, it is necessary to determine whether its core hypotheses, in terms of the predominance of secure attachment (van IJzendoorn, 1990), sensitive parenting as a predictor of secure attachment (Ainsworth et al., 1974; Bakermans‐Kranenburg et al., 2003; De Wolff & van IJzendoorn, 1997), and positive child outcomes of attachment (Thompson, 2016), are also confirmed in these populations. Although these hypotheses are thought to be universally applicable, most attachment research comes from Western, Anglo‐Saxon countries, (Mesman, van IJzendoorn, & Sagi‐Schwartz, 2016); and the debate on the universality versus culture‐specificity of caregiver sensitivity is far from over (Keller et al., 2018; Mesman, 2018; Mesman et al., 2018). In this narrative literature review, we present and discuss empirical studies on attachment and/or sensitivity in Latin American countries to provide a comprehensive overview and integration of the available evidence for the core hypotheses of attachment theory in this region.

Humans establish attachment relationships throughout their lives, and the earliest attachment relationship is the emotional bond between infants and their main caregivers that is thought to reflect a universal human mechanism based on ethological and evolutionary considerations (Bowlby, 1969/1982, 1979). Attachment is considered “secure” if there is a balance between attachment behaviors (seeking proximity and comfort when distressed) and exploration behaviors (engaging with the environment when it is safe) (Ainsworth, 1989; Bowlby, 1969/1982). A core parenting variable in attachment theory is sensitive responsiveness, which refers to the ability of the caregiver to notice and interpret children´s signals accurately and to respond to those signals promptly and appropriately, which is defined as fitting the nature of the child´s communications (Ainsworth et al., 1978). The proposed universality of the main attachment mechanisms has been captured in four core hypotheses (van IJzendoorn, 1990): (1) The universality hypothesis assumes that given the opportunities, and in absence of neurodevelopmental issues, all children will become attached to a caregiver; (2) the normativity hypothesis states that in a non‐life‐threatening context, most children will be securely attached to their caregiver; (3) the sensitivity hypothesis refers to the assumption that children will be securely attached depending on the caregiving features, in which sensitivity is central; and (4) the competence hypothesis assumes that infants with a secure attachment will develop higher levels of social‐cognitive competence than children with an insecure attachment relationship. Further, relevant additions to the original formulations of the third and fourth hypotheses include the assumption that sensitivity is universal (although not uniform) (Mesman et al., 2018), and is related to positive child outcomes (Feeney & Woodhouse, 2016). Moreover, attachment theory research has led to the development of many intervention programs (Berlin et al., 2016), with many focusing on improving caregivers’ sensitivity as a means of fostering positive child development in different domains of functioning (Bakermans‐Kranenburg et al., 2003).

KEY FINDINGS

  1. A total of 82 publications, between 1988 and 2020, representing 8 Latin American countries were identified, mostly on urban middle‐class non‐risk samples of mother‐child dyads.

  2. Core theoretical assumptions (universality, normativity, sensitivity, and competence) were supported; and attachment‐based interventions proved some level of efficacy.

  3. Despite an increase in Latin American attachment studies in the last decades, the continent is still underrepresented in the international scientific discourse on attachment which would benefit from including truly local insights in caregiving practices.

RELEVANCE TO THE FIELD OF MENTAL HEALTH

The relevance of attachment theory concepts to infant and child mental health development is undeniable, as evidenced by over 750 papers published in Infant Mental Health Journal since the year 2000. However, despite the theory's universality claims, systematic investigations of evidence for its hypotheses outside of the Western world are scarce. This paper provides a comprehensive summary of attachment research in Latin American countries to contribute to inclusive perspectives on attachment theory.

DIVERSITY AND ANTI‐RACIST STATEMENT

This literature review in itself represents an effort to mark the relevance of studying the empirical evidence base for a key developmental theory in a region usually ignored in mainstream research. Highlighting research from the Global South not necessarily written in hegemonic English promotes equity and inclusiveness in this particular scientific field. Further, any theory that supposes universality can only be taken seriously by paying attention to populations and studies from all possible corners of the globe to address potential normative biases. All of the authors identify as people of color, and three of them as ethnically Latin American (from Peru, Brazil, and Chile). All authors of the paper are involved in research on marginalized populations, addressing Eurocentric normativity and exclusion in mainstream theories and scientific practices.

Even though there is growing evidence that attachment theory's core principles are applicable outside of the Western world, the overwhelming majority of studies on attachment and sensitivity have been conducted in western, educated, industrialized, rich, and democratic (WEIRD; Henrich et al., 2010) societies (Mesman, van IJzendoorn, & Sagi‐Schwartz, 2016). This is one of the circumstances that prompted the cultural debate on attachment theory, along with the fact that parenting is known to be influenced by urban versus rural residence (Keller et al., 2006). Based on this and other relevant differences between Western and non‐Western countries, the cultural debate has raised criticism and questioned the universality of the attachment theory assumptions construct (Keller, 2013, 2021; LeVine, 2004; Weisner, 2015). Authors from the culture‐specificity perspective questioned the effect of culture on the applicability of the attachment theory´s core hypotheses and argued that definitions of those hypotheses and attachment‐related constructs are biased, focusing on individualistic cultural contexts (Keller, 2013; Rothbaum et al., 2000; Quinn & Mageo, 2013), not considering cultural‐specific parenting patterns (Dawson, 2018, Kärtner et al., 2010). For instance, they criticize the assumption that the infant is the central agent of the interaction, that infants are assumed to take the lead, and that dyadic interactions are expected to be turn‐taking and well‐rounded. Finally, they also question the focus on dyadic exchanges rather than (simultaneous) interactions with multiple caregivers that are common in many cultures (Keller et al., 2018).

Regarding the study of the attachment hypotheses in non‐Western countries, a cross‐cultural review including the case of Asia reported a total of 18 studies from six countries (Mesman, van IJzendoorn, & Sagi‐Schwartz, 2016). More recently, a review of research in Africa over the past 50 years identified only nine studies that assessed infant attachment in only five African countries (Voges et al., 2019). In both reviews, the fact that children were observed to be attached to a preferred caregiver, and a predominance of secure attachment patterns—similar to the Western findings—support the universality and normativity hypotheses. Only a handful of studies examined and (partly) supported the sensitivity and competence hypotheses outside of the West (Mesman, van IJzendoorn, & Sagi‐Schwartz, 2016).

Although research about attachment theory in Latin America has steadily evolved from a nascent to a fruitful field during the last two decades, it has not yet bridged the large gap separating Latin American research from the international discourse on attachment (Causadias et al., 2011). Challenges that may contribute to this gap include practical barriers to the use of attachment standardized research methods, the lack of facilities and financial resources for this type of research, the expenses of training (often only available in English) for key observational measures (Mesman, van IJzendoorn, & Sagi‐Schwartz, 2016), limited access to international peer‐reviewed publications on the state of the art of attachment research (Causadias & Posada, 2013), as well as the need to include large sample sizes that enable the use of parametric statistics. In addition, the work that is done in the region is often published only in Spanish or Portuguese and does not find its way to the international research community. It is not surprising, then, that attachment‐related studies in Latin America are scarce, and those that exist are overlooked. In the nonsystematic overview of relevant studies in non‐Western context by Mesman, van IJzendoorn, and Sagi‐Schwartz (2016), only seven studies from Latin America were included.

Despite these problems, clear progress has been made in the past decades. A group mainly of Latin American professionals and researchers established the Ibero‐American Attachment Network in 2009 (Red Iberoamericana de Apego, RIA) (Causadias et al., 2011), aimed at enhancing the interest, knowledge, and international collaboration in attachment research in the region. The network intends to provide training opportunities, hold a biennial Latin American attachment conference, and foster collaboration between teams from different countries. As a result, more than a decade after the foundation of RIA, the body of attachment literature in the area and the collaboration among local researchers has grown. However, many studies are still published in Spanish or Portuguese, and/or published in journals that are not indexed in the most current databases, so that they are easily segregated from the international literature. Therefore, the actual extent to which the attachment hypotheses are supported by research in Latin America has never been systematically addressed.

Given the specific cultural characteristics of Latin American countries, we cannot just assume that the principles of attachment theory apply here exactly as they do in Western countries. In the specific case of Latin American countries, familism—that refers to the support, loyalty and commitment offered to family members—is an important element of family cultural conceptions (Coohey, 2001), and Latin American parents and caregivers tend to present higher levels of emotional support and protectiveness than those in other cultures (Domenech et al., 2009; Harwood et al., 2002). Regarding parenting practices, a more controlling parental style characterized by respect and obedience is common (Dixon et al., 2008), but it is combined with traditional childrearing patterns characterized by physical affection and close family bonds (López et al., 2000). These particular manifestations of parental care do not seem conducive to sensitive responsiveness to children´s needs, or to child secure attachment and positive developmental outcomes. Overprotection or controlling strategies tend to be at odds with fostering secure base behaviors such as experiencing the mother as a safe haven (DeKlyen & Greenberg, 2016).

The aim of this systematic narrative literature review is to provide an integrative discussion of the current body of empirical studies concerning attachment theory in Latin American countries. Most of attachment research focuses on early childhood and the relationship between children and their caregivers, and observational measures commonly use a macro‐coding approach that refers to procedures in which caregivers´ behaviors are assessed based on a holistic observation of an entire interaction period, yielding a global score, in contrast to micro‐coding approach in which interactions are coded frame by frame (Mesman, 2010). For this reason, this narrative literature review will focus on children up to 6 years of age and their caregivers, and when using observational techniques will only include macro‐coding measures. The review is organized according to three themes: (1) the publication characteristics in terms of language, country, authors, publication year, and main caregiver considered; (2) the four core attachment hypotheses and the two stated additions, regarding the principles of (a) the universality of attachment, (b) the normativity of secure attachment, (c) caregiver sensitivity predicting secure attachment and additionally, the universality of caregiver sensitivity, and (d) secure attachment fostering social‐cognitive competence and other positive child outcomes, and—added for the purpose of this review—caregiver sensitivity fostering positive child outcomes across domains; and (3) the content and effectiveness of attachment‐based intervention studies in the Latin American context. We conclude by describing the main lessons from this review in terms of substantive insights, limitations of the current body of empirical research, and directions for future studies in the region. It has to be noted that this narrative literature review is theory‐driven, and focuses on the pre‐defined (Western) concepts and instruments from the field of attachment theory (etic approach), and does not include studies from other literatures of more locally developed caregiving concepts (emic approach) that may also provide valuable contributions to our understanding of the bond between caregivers and children. We will consider the potential added value of the latter in relation to our findings in the discussion section.

2. METHOD

2.1. Search strategy

The literature search focused on empirical studies about child attachment and parental sensitivity in Latin America published by August 22nd of 2018, using four electronic databases: Web of Science (WoS), PsycInfo, SciELO, and Redalyc. Prior to the database exploration, search terms that best represented the review aim were discussed and defined by the first and last authors.

The search keywords defined for WoS and PsycInfo were: TOPIC: (“maternal sensitiv*” OR “maternal responsive*” OR “paternal sensitiv*” OR “paternal responsive*” OR “mother* sensitiv*” OR “mother* responsive*” OR “father* sensitiv*” OR “father* responsive” OR “parent* sensitive*” OR “parent* responsive*” OR “sensitive parenting” OR attachment* OR “attachment representation*”) AND TOPIC: (infant* OR child* OR toddler OR baby OR babies OR preschooler) AND TOPIC: (“latin america*” OR latinoamerica* OR Argentin* OR Bolivia* OR Brasil* OR Brazil* OR Chile* OR Colombia* OR “Costa Rica*” OR “Costa Riq*” OR Cuba* OR Equador* OR Ecuador* OR “El Salvador*” OR Guatemal* OR Haiti* OR Hondur* OR Mexic* OR Nicaragu* OR Panam* OR Paragua* OR Peru* OR “República Dominicana” OR Dominican* OR Uruguay* OR Uruguai* OR Venezuela*OR “across the globe*” OR cultur*).

The search terms were adapted for the databases containing Spanish publications (SciELO and Redalyc) and defined as follows: “sensibilidad* materna* OR responsividad* materna* OR sensitividad* materna* OR sensibilidad* paterna* OR responsividad* paterna* OR sensitividad* paterna* OR sensibilidad* parental* OR responsividad* parental* OR sensitividad* parental* OR sensib* madre* OR sensib* padre* OR crianza sensi*OR apego* OR representaciones de apego AND Infan* OR niñ* OR hij* OR bebe* OR preescolar.” For all databases, the search terms were requested in the “Topic” field, matching on title, abstract, keywords and/or content of the publications. The range of time of publication was 1988–2018. Additionally, due to the production time of this review, a supplemental search was conducted on October 16th 2020. Disciplines unrelated to the field of this study (the broad behavioral and social sciences) were excluded.

2.2. Selection criteria

Studies had to meet all of the following inclusion criteria: (a) empirical article; (b) language of publication Spanish, Portuguese, or English; (c) when using observational techniques studies that include macro‐coding measures of attachment and/or sensitivity; (d) focus on human infants and children up to the age of 6 years; and (e) the focus children were born and living in Latin American countries (Argentina, Bolivia, Brazil, Chile, Colombia, Costa Rica, Cuba, Ecuador, El Salvador, Guatemala, Haiti, Honduras, Mexico, Nicaragua, Panama, Paraguay, Peru, Dominican Republic, Uruguay, and Venezuela).

The selection of studies for this review is summarized in Figure 1, following the PRISMA guidelines for systematic reviews and meta‐analyses (Moher et al., 2009), and shows the results of the main search conducted in 2018, and the supplemental search conducted in 2020. These searches combined yielded 82 publications, between 1988 and 2020, to be included in this narrative review. Two randomly selected publications in English and two in Spanish were used to pilot the coding process by the first and the second authors, and results were discussed with the last author. The remaining publications were randomly assigned to trained coders. Studies published in Spanish or Portuguese were only assigned to native speakers of the respective languages. For each publication, coders performed a full‐text reading to identify: main research question, research design, presence of intervention (yes/no), groups/conditions, country, age of children, special characteristics of the population, main variables, attachment theory hypotheses tested in the study, measures and instruments, use of observation (yes/no), main results, and other coder observations (if applicable).

FIGURE 1.

FIGURE 1

PRISMA flow diagram for the search strategy

3. RESULTS

3.1. Publication characteristics

Eighty‐two publications related to attachment theory hypotheses and/or interventions conducted in Latin America were identified. Approximately three‐quarters of them were published in the past 10 years, showing that the number of studies from this region has increased just recently. Two of these publications were written in Portuguese, 31 in Spanish, 42 in English, and the remaining seven in Spanish and English, which is a recent practice of some Latin American journals. More than a third of the publications (n = 32) have co‐authors from institutions from outside of Latin America, and almost all these publications were issued in English.

Eight out of twenty Latin American countries were represented in these publications: Argentina, Brazil, Chile, Colombia, Cuba, Ecuador, Mexico, and Peru. The vast majority of publications (n = 68) presented results based on a sample from one country. Fourteen publications presented data on more than one country, but just three of these presented samples from more than one Latin American country. The remaining eleven were comparisons mainly with the United States or European countries. Chilean samples were by far the most frequently represented, being present in more than half of identified publications (n = 45), followed by publications from Colombia (n = 12), Peru (n = 9), Mexico (n = 8), and Brazil (n = 6). Argentina, Ecuador, and Cuba were represented only once or twice each. Figure 2 shows the frequency of samples represented by years and countries.

FIGURE 2.

FIGURE 2

Frequency of samples per country and year

Finally, in most of the publications (n = 62) mothers were included as the main caregiver in relation to whom child attachment and caregiver sensitivity was assessed. Additional professional caregivers were considered in twelve publications, while fathers and grandmothers were only considered in three and one publication, respectively. One small‐scale study included three same‐sex couples as the main caregivers in relation to whom variables were assessed. In 11 publications no specific caregiver was reported since attachment was assessed at a representational level.

3.2. Attachment theory core hypotheses

In this section, studies reporting results that relate to four core hypotheses of attachment theory and defined additions are revised. This includes studies that report statistics on (a) the presence and quality of child attachment—covering the universality and normativity hypotheses; (b) the level of caregiver sensitivity, and/or the association between sensitivity and attachment quality—covering sensitivity hypothesis; and (c) the association between attachment quality or caregiver sensitivity and child social‐cognitive competence or other child outcomes—covering the competence hypothesis. Five of the identified publications (Fresno & Spencer, 2011; Garcia Quiroga & Hamilton‐Giachritsis, 2017; Posada et al., 2013; Santelices et al., 2015; Woldarsky et al., 2019) did not report individual statistics results or it was not possible to confirm that samples were also considered in other publications, therefore, are not included in the descriptions below.

Groups and subgroups of participants were categorized as either non‐risk samples (NRS) or high‐risk samples (HRS), based on the assumptions in the publication in question. Samples were labeled as HRS when the publication mentioned that specific characteristics of this sample were likely to negatively affect child attachment and/or caregiver´s sensitivity. These characteristics could include child, caregiver, and/or family risk factors. In four studies data from rural groups were considered, and even though in some cases it was mentioned it was a possible risk, we chose to not stigmatize these groups by labeling them as HRS. Finally, we decided not to report specific socioeconomic status (SES) level of groups and subgroups, because this information was often lacking, and when present reported in many different ways that cannot be compared between studies.

3.2.1. Universality and normativity hypotheses

Forty‐eight publications that measured child attachment were found, of which 45 reported statistical results on the presence and/or quality of attachment, reflecting 39 unique samples (see Table 1 for details). Secure attachment was assessed with six different instruments, of which the Attachment Behavior Q‐Sort (AQS; Waters, 1995) and the Massie‐Campbell´s Attachment During Stress Scale (ADS; Massie & Campbell, 1992) are the only ones with psychometric studies in Latin American samples (Nóblega, Conde et al., 2019; Salinas‐Quiroz et al., 2014). Most of the publications using other instruments presented below did report relevant information about the measures they used for their specific samples; and described their training processes, along with information about the achieved intercoder reliability at training or when coding, that were adequate in all publications. However, in eight cases no appropriate information about reliability was reported.

TABLE 1.

Overview of publications reporting on child attachment quality and attachment correlates

Publication Country Type of sample n Children's age (in months) Predominantly secure Attachment‐outcome relation
The Strange Situation Procedure (SSP)
Valenzuela (1990) Chile NRS and HRS 85 17 to 21

NRS: Yes

HRS: No

Yes
Lecannelier et al. (2008) Chile NRS 130 11 to 19 Yes NA
Mesa et al. (2009) Colombia HRS 4 12 to 18 No NA
Santelices et al. (2010)a Chile NRS 72 M = 12 Yes NA
Quezada and Santelices (2010) Chile NRS 72 11 to 15 Yes NA
Gojman et al. (2012) Mexico NRS, urban (U) and rural (R) 66 10 to 26

U: Yes

R: No

NA
Saur et al. (2018) Brazil NRS 50 12 to 25 Yes Yes
Lecannelier et al. (2019) Chile NRS, study 1 (S1) and study 2 (S2) 233

S1: M = 13.30

S2: M = 18.00

S1: Yes

S2: Yes

NA
Fuertes et al. (2020) Brazil NRS 26 M = 12 Yes NA
The Attachment Behavior Q‐Sort (AQS)
Posada et al. (1995) Colombia NRS 31 30 to 55 No NA
Posada et al. (1999) Colombia NRS and HRS 84

NRS = 8 to 19

HRS = 12 to 60

NRS: Yes

HRS: No

NA
Posada et al. (2002) Colombia NRS 61 8 to 19 Yes NA
Carrillo et al. (2004) Colombia NRS, with mother (M) and grandmother (GM) 30 18 to 42

M: Yes

GM: Yes

NA
Posada et al. (2004) Colombia NRS 30 6 to 13 Yes NA
Ortiz et al. (2006) Colombia NRS and HRS 40 10 to 30

NRS: No

HRS: No

NA
Wachs et al. (2011)b Peru NRS, at 12 (T1) and 18 (T2) months 177 12 and 18

T1: Yes

T2: Yes

Yes
Carbonell et al. (2015) Colombia HRS 10 30 to 66 NA No
Salinas‐Quiroz (2015) Mexico NRS 34 20 to 36 No Yes
Nóblega et al. (2016) Peru NRS 32 8 to 10 No NA
Posada et al. (2016)

Colombia (C)

Peru (P)

NRS 115

C = 39 to 48

P = 45 to 72

C: Yes

P: No

NA
Díaz et al. (2018) Ecuador NRS 16 36 to 71 Yes NA
Salinas‐Quiroz et al. (2018) Mexico NRS 8 M = 27.3 NRS: Yes NA
Bortolini and Piccinini (2018) Brazil NRS and HRS 48 M = 28.1

NRS: Yes

HRS: No

NA
Nóblega, Bárrig & Fourment (2019) Peru NRS 56 30 to 72 No NA
The Attachment Story Completion Task (ASCT)
Riquelme et al. (2003) Chile NRS 60 36 to 72 Yes Yes
Pierrehumbert et al. (2009) Chile NRS, girls (G) and boys (B) 45 M = 54

G: Yes

B: No

NA
Pérez et al. (2013) Chile NRS 137 37 to 49 Yes NA
Fresno et al. (2014) Chile NRS and HRS 66 M = 70.8

NRS: Yes

HRS: No

NA
Villachan‐Lyra et al. (2015) Brazil NRS 40 36 to 48 NA Yes
García Quiroga et al. (2017) Chile NRS and HRS 77

NRS: M = 53.63

HRS: M = 63.78

HRS: M = 64.71

NRS: Yes

HRS: No

HRS: No

NA
Pérez et al. (2017) Chile NRS 46 M = 54.26 No NA
Fresno et al. (2018) Chile NRS and HRS 96 M = 72

NRS: Yes

HRS: No

NA
Nóblega, Bárrig‐Jó et al. (2019)

Mexico (M)

Peru (P)

NRS 94

M = 36 to 78

P = 41 to 72

M: Yes

P: Yes

Yes
Massie‐Campbell`s Attachment During Stress Scale (ADS)
Lecannelier et al. (2009)a Chile NRS 17 M = 44 Yes NA
Figueroa et al. (2012)a Chile NRS 9 5 to 12 No NA
Lecannelier et al. (2014)a Chile HRS 62 2 to 12 No Yes
Cárcamo et al. (2016) Chile NRS, at Time 1 (T1) and Time 2 (T2) 110

T1: M = 6.4

T2: M = 14.85

T1: Yes

T2: Yes

NA
Farkas et al. (2017) Chile NRS, urban/non‐Mapuche (NM) and rural/Mapuche (M) 34 M = 11.74

NM: Yes

M: No

NA
Cárcamo et al. (2019) Chile NRS 25 10 to 14 Yes NA
Lecannelier et al. (2019) Chile NRS and HRS 253

NRS: M = 17.21

HRS: M = 12.50

HRS: M = 7.51

NRS: Yes

HRS: No

HRS: No

NA
Parenting‐Child Reunion Inventory (PCRI)
Sotgiu et al. (2011) Cuba NRS and HRS 22 48 to 132

NRS: Yes

HRS: Yes

NA
Family Drawing Test
Lara et al. (1994) Mexico NRS, working (W) and non‐working (NW) mothers 211 60 to 83

W: No

NW: No

Yes

Note: n = number of participants being considered in reported results from publication; NRS = Non‐risk sample; HRS = High‐risk sample. For AQS, samples with similar or higher scores as/than the mean security score from the met‐analysis (.35; Cadman et al., 2017) were classified as predominantly secure.

aAttachment‐based intervention publication: only control group or pre‐intervention results are reported.

bOnly 30 items from AQS were used to describe attachment behavior.

In relation to the universality hypothesis, it should be noted that none of the studies that assessed child attachment reported cases in which it was not possible to evaluate an attachment relationship. Additionally, Table 1 shows that the AQS, considered as a gold standard of attachment assessment, was the most used measure in this review with 14 publications, reporting on 19 unique samples. Average secure base behavior scores in all samples were positive, ranging from .19 (SD = .30) to .58 (SD = .19). Comparing sample averages with the mean security score of .35 (95% CI [.34,.37]) reported in the most recent meta‐analysis (average of 186 samples; Cadman et al., 2017), reveals a mix of Latin American samples scoring similarly (k = 3), scoring higher (k = 7), and scoring lower (k = 9). Samples with similar or higher scores as/than the mean security score from the meta‐analysis were classified as predominately secure.

According to attachment theory, normativity hypothesis can only be expected to be met among populations without conditions that would hamper the formation of a secure attachment relationship, therefore, results based on HRS should be cautiously used when testing this hypothesis. The AQS average scores, as describing the level of similarity with a securely attached child, provide some indirect information that also gives some support for this hypothesis. Additionally, the remaining five instruments reported on the quality of attachment. Table 1 shows that the Strange Situation Procedure (SSP; Ainsworth et al., 1978)—considered to be the gold standard of assessing attachment quality—was only used in nine publications with 12 unique samples, consisting mostly of NRS of typically developing children (TDC) from Chile. In one sample, almost 50%, and in eight samples, more than 50% of children were classified as securely attached, which is in line with the normative (modal) tendency reported cross‐culturally (Solomon & George, 2016). Lower percentages of security were reported in publications on high‐risk and rural samples. This pattern reflects mixed support for the normativity hypothesis, with the majority of (but not all) studies in NRS showing predominant secure attachment. The lower rate of security in at‐risk samples is consistent with attachment theory which assumes lower security in challenging circumstances.

The Attachment Story Completion Task (ASCT; Bretherton et al., 1990) assesses children attachment representations through narratives to identify secure base scripts. Eight publications, reporting on 14 unique samples, mainly from Chile, included children from 3 to 6 years old. For eight samples, a predominance of secure attachment was reported, with more than 50% of children classified as secure or children obtaining scores reflecting secure attachment according to their specific coding system. The remaining four out of six samples with predominant insecure attachment were reported for high‐risk groups. The ADS is a widely used instrument in Chilean public health care, and we found seven publications using the ADS reporting on 10 unique samples, five of which showed a predominance of secure attachment, with more than 50% of children classified as secure or with scores that indicated the predominance of secure behaviors. Three out of the remaining five samples with predominant insecurity were HRS and one was a rural/Mapuche sample. The Parent–Child Reunion Inventory (PCRI self‐report; Marcus, 1988) and the Family Drawing Test (FDT; based on child drawings) were each used in one publication with two samples. Attachment security was the predominant pattern for the samples on the PCRI publication, but not for the samples on the FDT publication.

3.2.2. Sensitivity hypotheses and quality of caregiving

Fifty‐three publications that measured caregiver sensitive behavior were found, of which 48 reported statistical results on the quality of sensitivity and/or the association between caregiver sensitivity and child attachment quality, reflecting 44 unique samples (see Table 2 for details).

TABLE 2.

Overview of publications reporting on caregiver´s sensitive quality and sensitivity correlates

Publication Country Type of sample n Children's age (in months) Predominantly sensitive Sensitivity hypothesis confirmed Sensitivity‐outcome relation
Ainsworth's Maternal Sensitivity vs. Insensitivity scale
Valenzuela (1997) Chile NRS and HRS 85 17 to 21

NRS: Yes

HRS: No

Yes Yes
Gojman et al. (2012) Mexico NRS, urban secure (US), urban insecure group (UI) and rural secure (RS), rural insecure group (RI) 66 9 to 26

US: Yes

UI: No

RS: Yes

RI: No

Yes NA
Fourment et al. (2021) Peru NRS 12 4 to 21 Yes NA NA
Ribeiro et al. (2021) Brazil NRS 22 M = 2 Yes NA NA
Experimental Index of Child‐Adult Relationships (CARE‐Index)
Santelices et al. (2009) Chile NRS, primary caregiver (PC) and care centre staff (CCS) 185 8 to 24

PC: Yes

CCS: Yes

NA Yes
Tenorio De Aguiar et al. (2009) Chile NRS 40 3 to 9 Yes NA Yes
Olhaberry (2011) Chile NRS, day care (DC) and without day care (WDC) 80 4 to 15

DC: Yes

WDC: Yes

NA Yes
Olhaberry (2012) Chile NRS, girls (G) and boys (B) 80 4 to 15

G: Yes

B: Yes

NA Yes
Olhaberry and Santelices (2013) Chile NRS, single mother (SF) and nuclear families (NF) 80 4 to 17

SF: Yes

NF: Yes

NA Yes
Santelices and Pérez (2013) Chile NRS, at Time 1 (T1), Time 2 (T2), and Time 3 (T3) 43 8 to 24

T1: Yes

T2: Yes

T3: Yes

NA NA
Santelices (2014) Chile NRS 69 12 to 24 Yes NA NA
Olhaberry, Escobar, Mena et al. (2015) a Chile HRS 134 2 to 3 No NA NA
Olhaberry, Escobar, Morales et al. (2015) Chile HRS 10 4 to 16.2 No NA Yes
Olhaberry, León et al. (2015) a Chile HRS 61 8.4 to 18.8 No NA NA
Farkas et al. (2017) Chile NRS, urban/non‐Mapuche (NM) and rural/Mapuche (M) 34 M = 11.74

NM: Yes

M: Yes

NA NA
Santelices et al. (2017) a Chile NRS, control (CG) and experimental group (EG) 53 0 to 24

CG: Yes

EG: Yes

NA NA
Binda et al. (2019) Chile HRS 177 2 to 12 Yes NA NA
Fuertes et al. (2020) Brazil NRS, clustered as secure (S) and insecure (I) 26 M = 9

S: Yes

I: Yes

No NA
Maternal Behavior Q‐Set (MBQS) and Maternal Behavior for Preschoolers Q‐Set (MBPQS)
Posada et al. (1999) Colombia NRS and HRS 84

NRS = 8 to 19

HRS = 12 to 60

NRS: Yes

HRS: No

Yes NA
Posada et al. (2002) Colombia NRS 61 8 to 19

Yes

Yes NA
Posada et al. (2004) Colombia NRS 30 6 to 13 Yes Yes NA
Ortiz et al. (2006) Colombia NRS and HRS 40 10 to 30

NRS: No

HRS: Yes

Yes NA
Carbonell et al. (2010) Colombia NRS and HRS 93 3 to 7

NRS: Yes

HRS: Yes

NA
Carbonell et al. (2015) Colombia HRS 10 30 to 66 NA

No

Yes
Salinas‐Quiroz (2015) Mexico NRS 34 20 to 60

Yes

No

No
Nóblega et al. (2016) Peru NRS 32 8 to 10

Yes

Yes NA
Posada et al. (2016)

Colombia (C)

Peru (P)

NRS 115

C = 39 to 48

P = 45 to 72

C: Yes

P: No

Yes NA
Díaz et al. (2018) Ecuador NRS 16 36 to 71

No

Yes NA
Salinas‐Quiroz et al. (2018) Mexico NRS 8 M = 27.3 Yes NA NA
Márquez et al. (2019) Mexico NRS and HRS 40

NRS = 61.2

HRS = 60

NRS: Yes

HRS: Yes

NA NA
Nóblega, Bárrig & Fourment (2019) Peru NRS 56 30 to 72 No Yes NA
Barone et al. (2021) a Colombia NRS, control (CG) and experimental group (EG) 25 16 to 36

CG: Yes

EG: Yes

NA NA
Adult Sensitivity Scale (E.S.A.)
Farkas et al. (2015) Chile NRS, mothers (MO) and educators (ED) 226 10 to 15

MO: Yes

ED: Yes

NA NA
Farkas and Rodríguez (2017) Chile NRS 90 10 to 15 Yes NA No
Gálvez and Farkas (2017) Chile NRS 105 10 to 14 Yes NA Yes
Kast et al. (2017) Chile NRS 19 10 to 15 Yes NA
Cuellar and Farkas (2018) Chile NRS 78 10 to 15 Yes NA No
Muñoz and Farkas (2018) Chile NRS 12 12 to 14 NA NA Yes
Farkas et al. (2020) Chile NRS, at Time 1 (T1) and Time 2 (T2) 90

T1: M = 12.0

T2: M = 29.31

T1: Yes

T2: Yes

NA NA
Ramos et al. (2020) Chile NRS 91 10 to 15 Yes NA Yes
Emotional Availability Scales: Infancy to Early Childhood Version (EAS)
Bornstein et al. (2008) Argentina NRS 70 M = 20.22 Yes NA NA
Fonseca et al. (2010) Brazil NRS and HRS 131 M = 4.00

NRS: Yes

HRS: Yes

NA NA
Bornstein et al. (2012) Argentina NRS 70 M = 5.27 Yes NA NA
Gil‐Rodríguez et al. (2018) Mexico NRS 60 M = 51.96 No NA Yes
The Attachment During Stress Scale (ADS)
Cárcamo et al. (2016) Chile NRS, at Time 1 (T1) and Time 2 (T2) 110

T1: M = 6.4

T2: M = 14.85

T1: Yes

T2: Yes

Yes NA

Note: n = number of participants being considered in reported results from publication; NRS = Non‐risk sample; HRS = High‐risk sample.

a

Attachment‐based intervention publication: only control group or pre‐intervention results are reported.

Sensitivity was assessed with seven different instruments, of which the Adult Sensitivity Scale (E.S.A; Santelices et al., 2012) is the only one that has been developed in Latin America, and has been shown to have adequate validity and reliability for its use in different contexts in Chile (Santelices et al., 2012). Additionally, the maternal Q‐sorts are the only ones with psychometric studies in Latin American samples (Bárrig‐Jó et al., 2020; Díaz & Nóblega 2020; Salinas‐Quiroz et al., 2014). Almost all of the publications using other instruments presented here did report information about the measures they used for their specific samples, and described their training processes, along with information about the achieved intercoder reliability at training or when coding, which was adequate in all cases. In two cases no information about reliability for the Maternal Behavior Q‐Set (MBQS; Pederson & Moran, 1995) and the Emotional Availability Scales: Infancy to Early Childhood Version (EA Scales; 3rd ed.; Biringen et al., 1998) was reported.

Table 2 shows that Ainsworth's Maternal Sensitivity versus Insensitivity to Infant Signals and Communications observational scale (Ainsworth et al., 1974)—considered as the gold standard of sensitive caregiver behavior assessment—was only used in four publications with eight unique samples. Five of eight samples had average sensitivity scores considered as sensitive, that is, scores 5 or higher. Lower average sensitivity levels were reported for a high‐risk sample and mothers with insecure attachment representations. The Experimental Index of Child‐Adult Relationships (CARE‐Index, Crittenden, 2006) was the most used measure in this review with 14 publications, reporting on 21 unique samples. Considering average scores rounded up to one digit, 18 samples were in the “adequate” category (scores from 7 to 10 out of 14) and the remaining three in the “risk/inept” category (scores from 0 to 6). Lower average scores of sensitive behaviors were reported in publications on HRS. The other most used instrument was the MBQS and Maternal Behavior for Preschoolers Q‐Set (MBPQS; Posada et al., 2007) also with 14 publications, reporting on 19 unique samples, mainly with NRS of TDC. Average caregivers´ sensitive behavior scores were positive in all subsamples, ranging from .20 (SD = .44) to .74 (SD = .25). More than two‐thirds of these samples (k = 14) reached average scores close to .50 or higher. Only one out of the remaining five samples with lower average scores can be considered an at‐risk sample.

The E.S.A was the only measure developed in Latin America, with eight publications, reporting on eight unique samples, all of them NRS of TDC from Chile. In all eight samples average caregiver´s sensitive behavior scores were in the “adequate” category, with the majority of caregivers classified as “adequate” too. The EA Scales and the Massie‐Campbell´s Attachment During Stress Scale (ADS; Massie & Campbell, 1992) were used four times and once, respectively, and sensitive behavior was the predominant pattern among five out of the six samples they reported.

The sensitivity hypothesis, that is, the relation between caregiver´s sensitive behavior and child secure attachment, was tested in fourteen publications, and was confirmed in 11 of those publications (see penultimate column of Table 2), most of which were NRS.

3.2.3. Competence hypotheses and sensitivity correlates

The final column of Table 1 shows that the competence hypothesis and its extension to other child outcomes were tested in only a few Latin American publications, but when studied, most of the evidence supports it. Nine out of ten relevant publications indicated that secure attachment was related to social‐cognitive competence and more positive functioning in children across different domains. Attachment security was associated with a higher socio‐cognitive development and higher social competence; and more competent behavior in terms of social orientation, object orientation, and reactivity. Additionally, attachment security was found to be related with a higher cognitive and language development level; a stronger theory of mind; and a better nutritional status. Only one study found no relation between attachment security and child development such as gross and fine motor, hearing and language, and personal and social competence.

Although caregiver´s sensitive responsiveness is not generally represented in descriptions of the competence hypothesis, it is interesting to see if sensitivity—as a key attachment process variable—also relates to positive outcomes in the Latin American context as it does in Western samples. The final column of Table 2 shows that 15 publications reported testing associations between sensitivity and child developmental outcomes, 12 of which turned out to be significant. Sensitive behavior was associated with a better nutritional status, a higher level of cooperativeness (and lower level of passivity), a better development of gross and fine motor skills, hearing and language, and personal and social skills, a better socio‐emotional developmental level, a higher dyadic level of commitment, and lower emotional and behavioral problems. Three publications found no significant relations between sensitivity and child socio‐cognitive or socio‐emotional development, nor with child language ability.

3.3. Effectiveness of attachment‐based interventions

Nine publications aimed to evaluate the effectiveness of attachment‐based interventions (see Table 3 for details). Almost all of them were conducted in Chile, and only the one conducted in Colombia used an internationally known program, the Video‐feedback Intervention to promote Positive Parenting and Sensitive Discipline (VIPP‐SD; Juffer et al., 2008). Local programs used in Chile differed in each study, and only two of them had specific names: Play with Our Children (POC; Brahm et al., 2016) and Promoting secure attachment (Santelices et al., 2010). Studies were experimental or quasi‐experimental, and seven out of the nine studies worked with a control group to compare the results of the intervention programs.

TABLE 3.

Overview of publications reporting on attachment‐based interventions

Lecannelier et al. (2009)
Country Chile
Sample N = 55 typically developing children (age 2–4 months)
Intervention program Two intervention groups: (1) Attachment workshop group: six one‐and‐a‐half‐hour group session intervention with the objective of providing tools and knowledge to promote secure attachment through the enhancement of maternal sensitivity and mentalization skills; (2) Massage workshop group: one one‐and‐a‐half‐hour and seven twenty‐minutes group session training on infant massage combined with maternal sensitivity
Study design Randomized control trial with Pre‐ to Posttest
Measures Massie‐Campbell´s Attachment During Stress Scale for infant attachment, Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale for maternal depressive symptomatology
Main results Considering both attachment workshop and massage workshop as one group, secure attachment rate significantly increased from pre‐ to posttest. The rate of depressive symptomatology only significantly decreased from pre‐ to posttest in the attachment workshop group
Santelices et al. (2010)
Country Chile
Sample N = 72 typically developing children (mean age 12 months)
Intervention program Promoting secure attachment intervention with ten (pre‐ and post‐natal) group sessions aimed at promoting maternal sensitivity, to modify mother´s mental representations, and to promote the development of a secure bond between the dyad. Techniques included group discussions, educational videos, and didactical exercises were used to approach topics like attachment, pregnancy, maternal representations, and discussing fantasies related to the baby
Study design Randomized control trial with Posttest only
Measures Strange Situation Procedure for infant attachment
Main results The proportion of secure attached children increased from pre‐ to posttest in the intervention groups, but not significantly more than in the control group (n.s.)
Figueroa et al. (2012)
Country Chile
Sample N = 9 children (average age 7 months) with early indicators of insecure attachment to mothers
Intervention program Four weekly two‐hours group sessions with the educational objectives of clarifying the "attachment" concept, some parenting guidelines, and tools to address stressful situations. Skills associated with parental sensitivity, child rearing, and child development were discussed
Study design Pre‐to Posttest, No control group
Measures Massie‐Campbell´s Attachment During Stress Scale for infant attachment, Qualitative interviews for adult's evaluation
Main results Quantitatively, the number of securely attached children increased from four pre‐intervention to seven post‐intervention (n.s.). Qualitatively, participants were satisfied with the intervention, highlighted that it helped them to connect better with their other children, and that during the workshops they were able to share their experiences, emotions, and fears with other caregivers
Lecannelier et al. (2014)
Country Chile
Sample N = 41 institutionalized children (age 2–12 months) and their caregivers
Intervention program One‐day face‐to‐face training followed but permanent supervision with the Attachment sensitivity manual aimed at developing and promote of skills, knowledge, and attitudes adequate to understand, manage, and assess the infant's competencies and development. The manual is divided in two: (1) Basic aspects: the minimum competencies for interacting with the infants and include the promotion of physical contact, visual contact, and vocalization; and (2) Specific aspects: a more complex type of activity related to the promotion of interactive play, the detection and regulation of temperament, and the detection and regulation of attachment styles
Study design Pre‐to Posttest, No control group
Measures Massie‐Campbell´s Attachment During Stress Scale for infant attachment
Main results

No effects of the intervention on attachment security was found

Olhaberry, Escobar, Mena et al. (2015)
Country Chile
Sample N = 134 typically developing children (age 2–3 months) and their mothers with history of depression
Intervention program Five 1.5‐hr group sessions aimed at reducing maternal depression and promote a positive mother‐infant bond from pregnancy to the child's first year of life. Theoretically based on attachment theory and cognitive behavioral principals, topics related to how participants feel, how to identify and solve problems, how the pregnancy is being lived, the mother they had and the mother they want to be, and getting ready for the arrival of the baby we treated in each session
Study design Pre‐to Posttest, with control group
Measures Experimental Index of Child‐Adult Relationships for maternal sensitivity, Beck Depression Inventory for maternal depressive symptomatology
Main results Intervention group showed significantly higher scores in maternal sensitivity compared to control group at posttest, and a significant reduction of depressive symptoms from pre‐ to posttest compared to control group
Olhaberry, León et al. (2015)
Country Chile
Sample N = 61 typically developing children (age 8.4–18.8 months) and their mothers receiving treatment for depressive symptomatology
Intervention program Four sixty‐minutes video‐feedback‐sessions that included speaking for the child, questions (related to observed, its relation with other interactions, about the child, and themselves), information about the child`s development period, identification of sensitive interaction chains, giving support to the mother, exploration of internal states that underlie behaviors, reflection, and creating new meanings
Study design Pre‐to Posttest, with control group
Measures Experimental Index of Child‐Adult Relationships for maternal sensitivity, Beck Depression Inventory for maternal depressive symptomatology
Main results Intervention group showed significantly higher level of maternal sensitivity compared to control group from pre‐ to posttest. A descriptive reduction in maternal depressive symptoms was observed for both groups, difference was not statistically significant
Brahm et al. (2016)
Country Chile
Sample N = 102 typically developing children (age 2–23 months)
Intervention program Play with Our Children (POC; UC–Christus Health Network dependent on the Family Medicine Department of the Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile) with weekly group‐sessions, home visits, and interviews aimed at strengthening dyadic interaction, promote comprehensive development of children from 0 to 48 months old, enhance parenting skills, and strengthen networks among caregivers. Intervention is mainly based on attachment theory, a psychoanalytic approach, and community orientation.
Study design Posttest only, with control group
Measures Maternal Behavior Q‐set for maternal sensitivity, Patient Health Questionnaire for maternal depressive symptoms, Parenting stress index
Main results At posttest, sensitivity was significantly higher in intervention mothers than control‐group mothers of children older than 12 months, but not in the group of children younger than 12 months. Parental stress was significantly lower in mothers of children younger than 12 months. A decrease in maternal depressive symptoms was observed for both age groups, but was not statistically significant
Santelices et al. (2017)
Country Chile
Sample N = 53 typically developing children (age 0–24 months) and their nursery schools` caregivers (teachers and assistants)
Intervention program Eight monthly four‐hour group workshop and field supervision program to promote sensitivity, based on the needs of the individuals involved. Workshops aimed at developing a more sensitive response and an improved reflective or mentalizing capacity in the relationships with children, and field supervision aimed at addressing the school caregiver's anxiety and supporting their work with their managers
Study design Pre‐to Posttest, with control group
Measures Experimental Index of Child‐Adult Relationships for caregiver sensitivity
Main results Intervention group showed significantly higher sensitivity levels than the control group from pre‐ to posttest
Barone et al. (2021)
Country Colombia
Sample N = 25 typically developing children (age 16–36 months) and their mothers, from a low socioeconomic status rural area
Intervention program Video‐feedback Intervention to promote Positive Parenting and Sensitive Discipline (VIPP‐SD; Juffer et al., 2008) standardized protocol of six home visits, with defined themes, tips, and exercises for each visit, according to the specific maternal profile defined at baseline evaluation
Study design Randomized control trial with Pre‐ to Posttest
Measures Maternal Behavior Q‐Set for maternal sensitivity and Knowledge, Attitude, and Practices survey for food habits
Main results VIPP‐SD enhanced sensitivity at post‐intervention and changes remained stable at 6‐month follow up. Intervention group showed significantly higher positive food habits than the control group, food habits improved from pre‐ to post‐intervention and from pre‐intervention to follow‐up, and VIPP‐SD enhanced food habits at post‐intervention and changes remained stable at 6‐month follow up

Table 3 shows that three interventions were tested with TDC and their caregivers. In two of these cases, dyads were parents and children from low to middle‐low SES homes and some evidence on the effectiveness of their interventions was found, based on a significant increase in sensitivity or secure attachment in the intervention group either from pre‐ to posttest or compared to the control group, or both. In the case of the low‐risk or non‐risk dyads, results did not demonstrate any significant effectiveness. In contrast, a study with TDC and their nursery school´s caregivers, teachers and assistants, showed significant effectiveness of the intervention to improve caregivers` sensitivity in the intervention group compared to the control group from pre‐ to posttest. From the remaining five publications, which included dyads considered to be at risk or disadvantaged, only three showed evidence for the effectiveness of the interventions. This was the case for two Chilean samples of TDC children with their mothers with history of depression and receiving treatment for depressive symptomatology; and one Colombian sample of TDC from a low socioeconomic status rural area. In all three cases, effectiveness meant a significant increase in sensitivity in the intervention group compared to the control group from pre‐ to posttest. From these publications, it can be concluded that there is some evidence for the effectiveness of attachment‐based interventions in Latin American samples.

4. DISCUSSION

The present systematic narrative literature review gives information about the main characteristics of Latin American publications on attachment theory; additionally, it provides some support for each of the core hypotheses of attachment theory, however, some specificities need to be highlighted; and some evidence on the effectiveness of attachment‐based interventions in the Latin American contexts is also provided. Nevertheless, some important gaps and limitations in the available literature need to be addressed.

Looking at the characteristics of all the studies reviewed in terms of country, authors, publication language, sample characteristic, and caregivers considered, some interesting patterns emerge. For one, only a few Latin American countries produce the majority of publications (12 out of 20 countries not represented at all), with Chile being the country with the highest production on publications related to attachment theory by far, particularly regarding attachment‐based intervention publications that were almost exclusively from Chile. This is likely to be related to a recent public health policy shift in Chile focusing on promoting children´s social and emotional development (Cárcamo et al., 2014), which might draw more attention from scholars and grant higher societal relevance to attachment‐driven research. With considerably less production, but still second on the list is Colombia, that together with Chile is among the top five countries with the highest scientific production in Latin America in general (Ibánez, 2018). Interestingly, most of the researchers conducting attachment studies in Latin America received their graduate training in the United States and Europe (Causadias & Posada, 2013). In addition, a significant percentage of the English publications in the current review have co‐authors from institutions from United States and Europe. This is likely to indicate that Latin American researchers are collaborating to make use of the extensive expertise in this field in other regions of the world. However, a potential downside is that this could also reflect dependence on Western researchers. More generally, the mechanisms that favor Western theories and English publications as focal points in scholarship from the Global South could play a role here, given that it facilitates international visibility and recognition, but can also marginalize local perspectives (Collyer, 2016).

In terms of study characteristics, most samples consisted of NRS of TDC, and most included only the mother as the main caregiver. Non‐maternal caregivers were only represented in a few publications. The near absence of fathers is in line with the international literature, in which fathers are usually left out (Cabrera et al., 2018; Palm, 2014). Remarkably, one small‐scale study included same‐sex parents, which is clearly an innovative inclusion in the field. Further, it has to be noted that few psychometric studies of the key attachment‐related instruments have been conducted with Latin American groups, and study samples are usually small (in comparison to Western samples). The often‐limited resources available for academic research in most Latin American countries are likely to be responsible for these issues.

Furthermore, the results of the present study reveal some support for the universality and normativity hypotheses. Regarding universality hypothesis, from the AQS publications and the fact that scores tend to be positive it is possible to presume the existence of an attachment bond; while from the other instruments measuring the quality of attachment, the possibility of classifying children as secure or insecure allowed us to presumed the existence of an attachment bond. It is also important to note that in all these cases Western‐based instruments to measure attachment were used. The Latin‐American studies described here support Bowlby´s (1969/1982) idea of attachment as a universal (and evolutionary) phenomenon.

Regarding the normativity hypothesis, results showed that the majority of NRS, mainly consisting of TDC, were securely attached or had secure attachment representations. However, this predominance was not the pattern in most of the HRS (12 out of 13), characterized by at‐risk family circumstances, such as chronically underweight children, or children with history of maltreatment, including abuse, children institutionalized or in alternative care, and dyads in prison. As previously mentioned, normativity hypothesis as such, can only be expected to be met among populations without conditions that entail risk for typical development, and the aforementioned results support this idea. Moreover, when reviewing AQS results, most of NRS had average scores similar to or higher than the mean security score reported in the most recent meta‐analysis (Cadman et al., 2017), and when this was not the case low scores were assumed to be due to the fact that they were based on mothers´ reports, that the interaction was assessed with secondary caregivers, that the samples consisted of very young children, or that the dyads were characterized by low‐SES backgrounds. However, in those cases in which reasons were not totally clear, a qualitative approach that allows for a better understanding of the potential cultural‐specific patterns needs to be conducted.

The findings from samples at risk underline the importance of socioeconomic characteristics in shaping parenting and child development (Conger & Donnellan, 2007; Emmen et al., 2013; Mesman et al., 2012). This is a particularly relevant issue considering that 72 million children (around 37%) live in moderate or extreme poverty in Latin American and the Caribbean, and that poverty is higher among children who live in remote rural areas and peri‐urban setting or among indigenous or afro‐descendant population (Unicef, 2021). The question is then, what is normative in this broader socio‐economic context? More variety in study populations would help us answer that question. Most of the participants in studies reviewed are not poor. Even though a few publications included extremely poor dyads or population from rural areas, the majority of studies are conducted with middle class urban samples, mainly due to the accessibility and practical reasons. Unfortunately, as it was not possible to report specific SES levels of all samples, due to their lack or the diversity of ways in which it was reported, we acknowledge the disadvantage this represents to provide a broader understanding of the impact of socioeconomic background in child development.

The universality of sensitivity and the association between sensitivity and child secure attachment, that is to say sensitivity hypothesis, were also well‐supported. Firstly, according to the criteria set by the instruments in question, most of the NRS showed adequate levels of sensitive responsiveness, whereas lower average sensitivity scores were observed mainly in at‐risk caregivers. In addition, support for the presence and relevance of the idea of sensitive behavior within Latin American population has been provided by a study that reported convergence between maternal beliefs of the ideal sensitive mother and attachment theory's description of the sensitive mother in seven cultural groups of four Latin American countries (Mesman, van IJzendoorn, Behrens et al., 2016). Secondly, the significant association between sensitivity and attachment security was supported in almost all samples in which it was tested (11 out of 14).

Finally, some support was also found for the competence hypothesis, along with some support for the significant relation of secure attachment and caregiver sensitivity with other positive child outcomes, although these were addressed in only a few studies. Interestingly, there were more studies testing the relation between caregivers’ sensitivity and positive child outcomes than studies testing the relation between child attachment quality and social‐cognitive competence and other child outcomes. Most of the studies testing these relations proved that both child attachment security and caregivers’ sensitivity were relevant antecedents for a more positive developmental functioning of children. The low number of this type of studies is not specific to Latin America (Mesman, van IJzendoorn, & Sagi‐Schwartz, 2016), and this scarcity is striking considering that the claim that early child attachment security is vital for promoting positive development across the life span is central to attachment theory (Bowlby, 1969/1982). At best, this signals an urgent need for further research to empirically clarify the nature and strength of the contribution of caregiver‐child attachment and caregiver´s sensitive behavior to children's development. At worst, it raises suspicions about non‐significant results in file drawers, casting doubt on the actual predictive value of attachment security for child developmental outcomes.

Attachment‐based intervention studies were by far the least represented in this review and only one of the included programs (VIPP‐SD; Juffer et al., 2008) was on the list of those identified as having the strongest evidence for effectiveness in Western samples (Berlin et al., 2016). The lack of this type of studies could be related to the relative complexity of conducting such research, given that they require more involvement, and longer commitment from participants, as repeated visits are required. Challenges such as cultural differences, limited funding for enough participants, insufficient familiarity of participants with this type of program, and lack of workplace support hinder the implementation of interventions in low‐SES countries (Hailemariam et al., 2019). All of the attachment‐based intervention studies were published in the last decade, and eight out of nine were conducted in Chile, showing that this type of studies is a novelty in Latin America attachment research, in contrast to the international literature in which this type of studies has been produced over three decades (Woodhouse, 2018). Regarding the performance of attachment‐based interventions, most of the publications reported some level of effective functioning, generally showing an increase in caregivers´ sensitive behavior and to a lesser extent in child secure attachment. Interestingly, all nine studies used a different intervention program, and none of them were used more than once, suggesting that the theoretical focus rather than the precise content could contribute to their success.

5. CONCLUSIONS

This narrative literature review represents a first step in the process of systematically collecting and describing studies rooted in attachment theory in Latin America. However, some additional steps need to be taken. Future reviews could include studies on older children, adolescents, or adults, as well as other assessment approaches, in order to gradually achieve a better and broader insight into attachment research in Latin American countries. But more importantly, acknowledging that there is still a debate on the universality versus culture‐specificity of attachment theory and its applicability in non‐Western countries, reviews of Latin American studies related to parenting and caregiver‐child interactions rooted in local concepts and theoretical points of view need to be conducted. The scholarly literature on parenting would benefit from combining insights from etic and emic approaches (Corona & Maldonado, 2018; Helfrich, 1999) that allow for the recognition and understanding of cultural patterns and practices related to the concepts of sensitivity and attachment in the Latin American region and beyond.

Fourment, K. , Espinoza, C. , Ribeiro, A. C. L. , & Mesman, J. (2022). Latin American Attachment studies: A narrative review. Infant Mental Health Journal, 43, 653–676. 10.1002/imhj.21995

REFERENCES

  1. Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1989). Attachments beyond infancy. American Psychologist, 44(4), 709–716. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Ainsworth, M. D. S. , Bell, S. M. , & Stayton, D. J. (1974). Infant–mother attachment and social development: ‘‘Socialization’’ as a product of reciprocal responsiveness to signals. In Richards M.P.M. (Ed.), The integration of a child into a social world (pp. 99–135). Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  3. Ainsworth, M. D. S. , Blehar, M. C. , Waters, E. , & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the strange situation. Erlbaum. 10.4324/9781315802428 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bakermans‐Kranenburg, M. J. , van IJzendoorn, M. H. , & Juffer, F. (2003). Less is more: Meta‐analyses of sensitivity and attachment interventions in early childhood. Psychological Bulletin, 129(2), 195–215. 10.1037/0033-2909.129.2.195 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Barone, L. , Carone, N. , Salazar, J. , & Ortiz, J. (2021). Enhancing food habits via sensitivity in rural low‐SES mothers of children aged 1–3 living in Colombia: A randomized controlled trial using video‐feedback intervention. Attachment and Human Development, 23(6), 831–852. 10.1080/14616734.2020.1784243 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bárrig‐Jó, P. , Nóblega, M. , & Ugarte, A. (2020). Evidencias de Validez Convergente y de Criterio del Maternal Behavior Q‐Sort (MBQS) 3.1 en Madres Peruanas [Convergent and Criterion Validity of the Maternal Behavior Q‐Sort 3.1 in a Sample of Peruvian Mothers]. Revista Iberoamericana de Diagnóstico y Evaluación, 56(3), 117–128. 10.21865/RIDEP56.3.09 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  7. Berlin, L. J. , Zeanah, C. H. , & Lieberman, A. F. (2016). Prevention and Intervention programs to support early attachment security. A move to the level of the community. In Cassidy J. & Shaver P. R. (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (3rd ed.), (pp. 739–758). Guilford. [Google Scholar]
  8. Binda, V. , Figueroa‐Leigh, F. , & Olhaberry, M. (2019). Antenatal and postnatal depressive symptoms: association with quality of mother‐infant interaction. Infant Behavior and Development, 57, 1–12. 10.1016/j.infbeh.2019.101386 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Biringen, Z. , Robinson, J. L. , & Emde, R. N. (1998). Appendix B: The Emotional Availability Scales (3rd ed.; an abridged infancy/early childhood version). Attachment and Human Development, 2(2), 256–270. 10.1080/14616730050085626 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bornstein, M. H. , Putnick, D. L. , Heslington, M. , Gini, M. , Suwalsky, J. T. D. , Venuti, P. , de Falco, S. , Giusti, Z. , & de Galperín, C. Z. (2008). Mother‐child emotional availability in ecological perspective: Three countries, two regions, two genders. Developmental Psychology, 44(3), 666–680. 10.1037/0012-1649.44.3.666 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bornstein, M. H. , Putnick, D. L. , Suwalsky, J. T. D. , Venuti, P. , de Falco, S. , de Galperín, C. Z. , Gini, M. , & Marianne Heslington Tichovolsky . (2012). Emotional relationships in mothers and infants: Culture‐common and community‐specific Characteristics of dyads from rural and metropolitan settings in Argentina, Italy, and the United States. Journal of Cross‐Cultural Psychology, 43(2), 171–197. 10.1177/0022022110388563 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Bortolini, M. , & Piccinini, C. A. (2018). Mothers’ experiences with their own caregivers and child's behaviors attachment. Paidéia, 28, Article e2837. 10.1590/1982-4327e2837 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  13. Bowlby, J. (1969/1982). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment. Basic Books. [Google Scholar]
  14. Bowlby, J. (1979). The making and breaking of affectional bonds. Tavistock Publications. [Google Scholar]
  15. Brahm, P. , Cortázar, A. , Fillol, M. P. , Mingo, M. V. , Vielma, C. , & Aranguiz, M. C. (2016). Maternal sensitivity and mental health: Does an early childhood intervention programme have an impact? Family Practice, 33(3), 226–232. 10.1093/fampra/cmv071 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Bretherton, I. , Ridgeway, D. , & Cassidy, J. (1990). Assessing internal working models of the attachment relationship: An attachment story completion task for 3‐year‐olds. In Greenberg M., Cicchetti D., & Cummings E. M. (Eds.), Attachment in the pre‐school years: Theory, research and intervention (pp. 273–308). University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
  17. Cabrera, N. , Volling, B. L. , & Barr, R. (2018). Fathers are parents, too! Widening the lens on parenting for children´s development. Child Development Perspectives, 12(3), 152–157. 10.1111/cdep.12275 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  18. Cadman, T. , Diamond, P. R. , & Fearon, P. (2017). Reassessing the validity of the attachment Q‐sort: An updated meta‐analysis. Infant and Child Development, 27(1), 1–54. 10.1002/icd.2034 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  19. Carbonell, O. A. , Plata, S. J. , Bermúdez, M. E. , Suárez, L. C. , Peña, P. A. , & Villanueva, C. (2015). Caracterización de prácticas de cuidado en familias colombianas con niñas y niños en primera infancia en situación de desplazamiento forzado [Characteristics of Caregiving Practices in Colombian Families with Children in Early Childhood in Forcibly Displaced Situation]. Universitas Psychologica, 14(1), 15–28. 10.11144/Javeriana.upsy14-1.cpcf [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  20. Carbonell, O. A. , Plata, S. J. , Peña, P. A. , Cristo, M. , & Posada, G. (2010). Calidad de cuidado materno: una comparación entre bebés prematuros en cuidado madre canguro y bebés a término en cuidado regular [Quality of Maternal Care: A Comparison of Preterm Infants in Kangaroo Mother Care and Full‐Term Infants in Regular Care]. Universitas Psychologica, 9(3), 773–785. Retrieved from http://www.scielo.org.co/scielo.php?script=sci_arttext&pid=S1657‐92672010000300014&lang=es [Google Scholar]
  21. Cárcamo, R. A. , Lagos, S. , García‐Riquelme, A. , & Gómez, E. (2019). Apego en niños con asistencia temprana a salas cuna en la ciudad de Punta Arenas (Chile): un estudio piloto [Attachment of children who experience early entry to daycare in Punta Arenas, Chile: a pilot study]. Revista Latinoamericana de Ciencias Sociales, Niñez y Juventud, 17(2), 1–21. 10.11600/1692715x.17210 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  22. Cárcamo, R. A. , van der Veer, R. , Vermeer, H. J. , & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2014). From foundling homes to day care: A historical review of childcare in Chile. Cadernos De Saúde Pública, 30(3), 461–472. 10.1590/0102-311x00060613 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Cárcamo, R. A. , Vermeer, H. J. , van der Veer, R. , & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2016). Early full‐time day care, mother–child attachment, and quality of the home environment in Chile: Preliminary findings. Early Education and Development, 27(4), 457–477. 10.1080/10409289.2016.1091971 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  24. Carrillo, S. , Maldonado, C. , Saldarriaga, L. M. , Vega, L. , & Díaz, S. (2004). Patrones de apego en familias de tres generaciones: abuela, madre adolescente, hijo [Attachment patterns in three‐generation families: Grandmother, teenage mother, son]. Revista Latinoamericana de Psicología, 36(3), 409–430. Retrieved from https://www.redalyc.org/articulo.oa?id=80536304 [Google Scholar]
  25. Causadias, J. M. , & Posada, G. (2013). The relevance of cross‐national studies on early attachment: Research advances in Latin America. Bulletin of the International Society for the Study of Behavioural Development, 63(1), 18–21. [Google Scholar]
  26. Causadias, J. M. , Sroufe, L. A. , & Herreros, F. (2011). The establishment of an attachment research network in Latin America: Goals, accomplishments, and challenges. Attachment & Human Development, 13(2), 193–198. 10.1080/14616734.2011.554008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Collyer, F. M. (2016). Global patterns in the publishing of academic knowledge: Global North, global South. Current Sociology, 66(1), 56–73. 10.1177/0011392116680020 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  28. Conger, R. D. , & Donnellan, M. B. (2007). An interactionist perspective on the socioeconomic context of human development. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 175–199. 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085551 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Corona, J. L. , & Maldonado, J. F. (2018). Investigación Cualitativa: Enfoque Emic‐Etic. [Qualitative Research: Emic‐Etic]. Approach Revista Cubana de Investigaciones Biomédicas, 37(4), 1–4. Retrieved from http://scielo.sld.cu/scielo.php?script=sci_arttext&pid=S0864‐03002018000400022&lang=es [Google Scholar]
  30. Coohey, C. (2001). The relationship between familism and child maltreatment in Latino and Anglo families. Child Maltreatment, 6(2), 130–141. 10.1177/1077559501006002005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Crittenden, P. (2006). Care‐Index Para Infantes (Nacimiento‐15 Meses): Manual de Codificación [CARE‐Index for infants (born to 15 months). Coding Manual] . Family Relations Institute. [Google Scholar]
  32. Cuellar, M. P. , & Farkas, C. (2018). Sensibilidad y mentalización de las educadoras de párvulos. Predictores del lenguaje infantil a los 30 meses [Sensitivity and mentalization of teaching staff. Predictors of infant language at 30 months]. Perfiles educativos, 40(160), 64–82. 10.1080/10409289.2017.1287994 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  33. Dawson, N. (2018). From Uganda to Baltimore to Alexandra township: How far can Ainsworth's theory stretch. South African Journal of Psychiatry, 24(1), 1–8. 10.4102/sajpsychiatry.v24i0.1137 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. DeKlyen, M. , & Greenberg, M. T. (2016). Attachment and Psychopathology in Childhood. In Cassidy J. & Shaver P. R. (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (3rd ed., pp. 639–666). Guilford. [Google Scholar]
  35. De Wolff, M. S. , & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (1997). Sensitivity and Attachment: A meta‐analysis on parental antecedents of infant attachment. Child Development, 68(4), 571–591. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1997.tb04218.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Díaz, E. , Andrade, I. , Espinosa, E. , Nóblega, M. , & Núñez del Prado, J. (2018). Cuidado sensible y seguridad del apego en preescolares [Sensitive caregiving and attachment security in preschoolers]. Ciencias Psicológicas, 12(1), 97–107. 10.22235/cp.v12i1.1600 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  37. Díaz, E. , & Nóblega, M. (2020). MBPQS in Ecuador: Exploring its content validity for its application with caregivers of children with ASD [MBPQS in Ecuador: Exploring its content validity for its application with caregivers of children with ASD]. Ciencias Psicológicas, 14(1), Article e2063. 10.22235/cp.v14i1.2063 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  38. Dixon, S. , Graber, J. , & Brooks‐Gunn, J. (2008). The roles of respect for parental authority and parenting practices in parent‐child conflict among African American, Latino and European American families. Journal of Family Psychology, 22(1), 1–10. 10.1037/0893-3200.22.1.1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Domenech, M. , Donovick, M. , & Crowley, S. (2009). Parenting styles in a cultural context: Observations of “protective parenting” in first‐generation Latinos. Family Process, 48(2), 1–18. 10.1111/j.1545-5300.2009.01277.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Emmen, R. A. G. , Malda, M. , Mesman, J. , van IJzendoorn, M. H. , Prevoo, M. J. L. , & Yeniad, N. (2013). Socioeconomic status and parenting in ethnic minority families: testing a minority Family Stress model. Journal of Family Psychology, 27(6), 896–904. 10.1037/a0034693 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Farkas, C. , Álvarez, C. , Cuellar, M. P. , Avello, E. , Gómez, D. M. , & Pereira, P. (2020). Mothers’ competence profiles and their relation to language and socioemotional development in Chilean children at 12 and 30 months. Infant Behavior and Development, 59, 101443. 10.1016/j.infbeh.2020.101443 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Farkas, C. , Carvacho, C. , Galleguillos, F. , Montoya, F. , León, F. , Santelices, M. P. , & Himmel, E. (2015). Estudio comparativo de la sensibilidad entre madres y personal educativo en interacción con niños y niñas de un año de edad. [Comparative study of sensitivity between mothers and educational staff with children up to one year old]. Perfiles Educativos, 37(148), 16–33. 10.1016/j.pe.2015.11.005 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  43. Farkas, C. , Olhaberry, M. , Santelices, M. P. , & Cordella, P. (2017). Interculturality and early attachment: A comparison of urban/non‐Mapuche and rural/Mapuche mother‐baby dyads in Chile. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 26, 205–216. 10.1007/s10826-016-0530-6 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  44. Farkas, C. , & Rodríguez, K. A. (2017). Percepción materna del desarrollo socioemocional infantil: relación con temperamento infantil y sensibilidad maternal. [Maternal perception of infant socio‐emotional development: Relationship with infant temperament and maternal sensitivity]. Acta de Investigación Psicológica ‐ Psychological Research Records, 7(2), 2735–2746. 10.1016/j.aipprr.2017.06.003 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  45. Feeney, B. C. , & Woodhouse, S. S. (2016). Caregiving. In Cassidy J. & Shaver P. R. (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (3rd ed., pp. 827–851). Guilford. [Google Scholar]
  46. Figueroa, F. , Binda, V. , & Santelices, M. P. (2012). Enhancing early attachment: Design and pilot study of an intervention for primary health care dyads. Journal of Child Health Care, 17(1), 82–91. 10.1177/1367493512447533 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Fonseca, V. R. J. R. M. , Silva, G. , & Otta, E. (2010). Relação entre depressão pós‐parto e disponibilidade emocional maternal [The relationship between postpartum depression and maternal emotional availability]. Cadernos de Saúde Pública, 26(4), 738–746. 10.1590/S0102-311x2010000400016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Fourment, K. , Nóblega, M. , Conde, G. , Núñez, J. , & Mesman, J. (2021). Maternal sensitivity in rural Andean and Amazonian Peru. Attachment and Human Development, 23(2), 134–149. 10.1080/14616734.2020.1828515 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Fresno, A. , & Spencer, R. (2011). Efecto del Maltrato Físico en la Calidad de las Representaciones de Apego Infantil en Chile: Resultados Preliminares. [The Effect of Physical Abuse on The Quality of Infant Attachment Representations in Chile: Preliminary Results]. Terapia Psicológica, 29(2), 191–211. Retrieved from http://www.redalyc.org/articulo.oa?id=78520905006 [Google Scholar]
  50. Fresno, A. , Spencer, R. , & Espinoza, C. (2018). Does the type of abuse matter? Study on the quality of child attachment narratives in a sample of abused children. Journal of Child and Adolescent Trauma, 11(4), 421–430. 10.1007/s40653-017-0182-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Fresno, A. , Spencer, R. , Ramos, N. , & Pierrehumbert, B. (2014). The Effect of sexual abuse on children's attachment representations in Chile. Journal of Child Sexual Abuse, 23(2), 128–145. 10.1080/10538712.2014.870949 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Fuertes, M. , Ribeiro, C. , Gonçalves, J. , Rodrigues, C. , Beeghly, M. , Lopes‐dos‐Santos, P. , & Lamônica, D. A. C. (2020). Maternal perinatal representations and their associations with mother–infant interaction and attachment: A longitudinal comparison of Portuguese and Brazilian dyads. International Journal of Psychology, 55(2), 224–233. 10.1002/ijop.12577 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Gálvez, A. P. , & Farkas, C. (2017). Relación entre mentalización y sensibilidad de madres de infantes de un año de edad y su efecto en su desarrollo socioemocional [Mentalization‐Sensitivity Relationship in Mothers of One‐Year‐Old Infants and its Effect on Their Social‐Emotional Development]. Psykhe, 26(1), 1–14. 10.7764/psykhe.26.1.879 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  54. Garcia Quiroga, M. , & Hamilton‐Giachritsis, C. (2017). The crucial role of the micro caregiving environment: Factors associated with attachment styles in alternative care in Chile. Child Abuse & Neglect, 70, 169–179. 10.1016/j.chiabu.2017.05.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Garcia Quiroga, M. , Hamilton‐Giachritsis, C. , & Ibañez Fanés, M. (2017). Attachment representations and socio‐emotional difficulties in alternative care: A comparison between residential, foster and family‐based children in Chile. Child Abuse & Neglect, 70, 180–189. 10.1016/j.chiabu.2017.05.021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Gil‐Rodríguez, L. , Lucio Gómez Maqueo, E. , & Forns I Santacana, M. (2018). Patrones de disponibilidad emocional y los problemas de salud mental del pre‐escolar. [Patterns of emotional availability and preschool mental health problems]. Acta de Investigación Psicológica, 8(1), 37–48. 10.22201/fpsi.20074719E.2018.1.04 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  57. Gojman, S. , Millan, S. , Carlson, E. , Sánchez, G. , Rodarte, A. , González, P. , & Hernández, G. (2012). Intergenerational relations of attachment: a research synthesis of urban/rural Mexican samples. Attachment & Human Development, 14(6), 553–566. 10.1080/14616734.2012.727255 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Hailemariam, M. , Bustos, T. , Montgomery, B. , Barajas, R. , Evans, L. B. , & Drahota, A. (2019). Evidence‐based intervention sustainability strategies: A systematic review. Implementation Science, 14(1), 57. 10.1186/s13012-019-0910-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Harwood, R. , Leyendecker, B. , Carlson, V. , Asencio, M. , & Miller, A. (2002). Parenting among Latino Families in the U.S. In Bornstein M. (Ed.), Handbook of parenting (Vol. IV, pp. 21–47). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [Google Scholar]
  60. Helfrich, H. (1999). Beyond the dilemma of cross‐cultural psychology: Resolving the tension between etic and emic approaches. Culture & Psychology, 5(2), 131–153. 10.1177/1354067x9952002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  61. Henrich, J. , Heine, S. J. , & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2‐3), 61–135. 10.1017/S0140525x0999152X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Ibánez, J. J. (2018). La ciencia en Latinoamérica: Tendencias y patrones [Science in Latin America: Trends and patterns]. Revista de la Facultad de Ciencias, 7(1), 23–39. 10.15446/rev.fac.cienc.v7n1.69409 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  63. Juffer, F. , Bakermans‐Kranenburg, M. J. , & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (Eds.). (2008). Promoting positive parenting: An attachment‐based intervention. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [Google Scholar]
  64. Kast, M. J. , Farkas, C. , & Vallotton, C. D. (2017). Diferencias en sensibilidad parental entre madres y padres de Chile y Estados Unidos [Parental sensitivity differences between mothers and fathers from Chile and USA]. Psicoperspectivas, 16(3), 137–146. 10.5027/psicoperspectivas-Vol16-Issue3-fulltext-950 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  65. Kärtner, J. , Keller, H. , & Yovsi, R. D. (2010). Mother–infant interaction during the first 3 months: The emergence of culture‐specific contingency patterns. Child Development, 81(2), 540–554. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01414.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Keller, H. (2013). Attachment and culture. Journal of Cross‐Cultural Psychology, 44(2), 175–194. 10.1177/0022022112472253 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  67. Keller, H. (2021). Attachment theory. Fact or fancy? In Thompson R. A., Simpson J. A., & Berlin L. J. (Eds.), Attachment. The fundamental questions (pp. 229–236). Guilford. [Google Scholar]
  68. Keller, H. , Bard, K. , Morelli, G. , Chaudhary, N. , Vicedo, M. , Rosabal‐Coto, M. , Scheidecker, G. , Murray, M. , & Gottlieb, A. (2018). The myth of universal sensitive responsiveness: Comment on Mesman et al. (2017). Child Development, 89(5), 1921–1928. 10.1111/cdev.13031 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Keller, H. , Lamm, B. , Abels, M. , Yovsi, R. , Borke, J. , Jensen, H. , Papaligoura, Z. , Holub, C. , Lo, W. , Tomiyama, A. J. , Su, Y. , Wang, Y. , & Chaudhary, N. (2006). Cultural models, socializations goals and parenting ethnotheories. A multicultural analysis. Journal of Cross‐Cultural Psychology, 37(2), 155–172. 10.1177/0022022105284494 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  70. Lara, M. A. , Acevedo, M. , López, E. K. , & Fernández, M. (1994). La conducta de apego en niños de 5 y 6 años: influencia de la ocupación materna fuera del hogar [Attachment behavior in children of 5 and 6 years: influence of maternal occupation outside the home]. Revista Latinoamericana de Psicología, 26(2), 283–313. Retrieved from https://www.redalyc.org/articulo.oa?id=80526206 [Google Scholar]
  71. Lecannelier, F. , Kimelman, M. , González, L. , Nuñez, C. , & Hoffmann, M. (2008). Evaluación de patrones de apego en infantes durante su segundo año en dos centros de atención de Santiago de Chile [Assessment of infant attachment patterns during their second year in two attention centers from Santiago de Chile]. Revista Argentina de Clínica Psicológica, 17(3), 197–207. Retrieved from https://www.redalyc.org/articulo.oa?id=281921795001 [Google Scholar]
  72. Lecannelier, F. , Monje, G. , & Guajardo, H. (2019). Patterns of attachment in early infancy in normative samples, alternative caregiving systems, and high risk infants. Revista Chilena de Pediatría, 90(5), 515–521. 10.32641/rchped.v90i5.1037 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Lecannelier, F. , Silva, J. R. , Hoffmann, M. , Melo, R. , & Morales, R. (2014). Effects of an intervention to promote socioemotional development in terms of attachment security: A study in early institutionalization in Chile. Infant Mental Health Journal, 35(2), 151–159. 10.1002/imhj.21436 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Lecannelier, F. , Undurraga, V. , Olivares, A. M. , Rodriguez, J. , Nuñez, J. C. , Hoffmann, M. , Goldstein, N. , & Larraín, C. (2009). Estudio de eficacia sobre dos intervenciones basadas en el fomento del apego temprano en díadas madre‐bebé provenientes de la región metropolitana en Santiago de Chile [Efficacy Study on two interventions based on promoting early attachment with mother‐baby dyads from the Metropolitan región in Santiago de Chile]. Revista Argentina de Clínica Psicológica, 18(2), 143–155. Retrieved from https://www.redalyc.org/articulo.oa?id=281921792005 [Google Scholar]
  75. LeVine, R. A. (2004). Challenging expert knowledge: Findings from an African study of infant care and development. In Gielen U. P. & Roopnarine J. (Eds.), Childhood and adolescence: Cross‐cultural perspectives and applications (pp. 149–165). Praeger. [Google Scholar]
  76. López, C. , Lara, M. G. , Herrera‐Amighetti, L. D. , Wissow, L. S. , Gutierrez, M. I. , Levav, I. , & Maddaleno, M. (2000). Parenting and physical punishment: Primary care interventions in Latin America. Pan American Journal of Public Health, 8(4), 257–267. 10.1590/S1020-49892000000900005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Marcus, R. F. (1988). Parent/child reunion inventory. Institute for Child Study, University of Maryland. [Google Scholar]
  78. Márquez, C. , Nicolini, H. , Crowley, M. J. , & Solís‐Vivanco, R. (2019). Early processing (N170) of infant faces in mothers of children with autism spectrum disorder and its association with maternal sensitivity. Autism Research, 12(5), 744–758. 10.1002/aur.2102 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Massie, H. , & Campbell, K. (1992). The Massie‐Campbell scale of mother‐infant attachment indicators during stress: For use during the pediatric examination and other childcare situations. Instruction Guide. [Google Scholar]
  80. Mesa, A. M. , Estrada, L. F. , Bahamón, A. L. , & Perea, D. (2009). Experiencias de maltrato infantil y transmisión intergeneracional de patrones de apego madre‐infante [Child abuse experiences and intergenerational transmission of mother‐infant attachment patterns]. Pensamiento Psicológico, 6(13), 127–151. Retrieved from https://www.redalyc.org/articulo.oa?id=801/80112469010 [Google Scholar]
  81. Mesman, J. (2010). Maternal responsiveness to infants: Comparing micro‐and macro‐level measures. Attachment & Human Development, 12(1), 143–149. 10.1080/14616730903484763 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Mesman, J. (2018). Sense and sensitivity: A response to the commentary by Keller et al. (2018). Child Development, 89(5), 1929–1931. 10.1111/cdev.13030 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Mesman, J. , Minter, T. , Angnged, A. , Cissé, I. , Salali, G. G. , & Migliano, A. B. (2018). Universality without uniformity: A culturally inclusive approach to sensitive responsiveness in infant caregiving. Child Development, 89(3), 837–850. 10.1111/cdev.12795 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Mesman, J. , van IJzendoorn, M. H. , & Bakermans‐Kranenburg, M. J. (2012). Unequal in opportunity, equal in process: Parental sensitivity promotes positive child development in ethnic minority families. Child Development Perspectives, 6, 239–250. 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2011.00223.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  85. Mesman, J. , van IJzendoorn, M. H. , & Sagi‐Schwartz, A. (2016). Cross‐cultural patterns of attachment: Universal and contextual dimensions. In Cassidy J. & Shaver P. R. (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (3rd ed., pp. 852–877). Guilford. [Google Scholar]
  86. Mesman, J. , van IJzendoorn, M. , Behrens, K. , Carbonell, O. A. , Cárcamo, R. , Cohen‐Paraira, I. , de la Harpe, C. , Ekmekçi, H. , Emmen, R. , Heidar, J. , Kondo‐Ikemura, K. , Mels, C. , Mooya, H. , Murtisari, S. , Nóblega, M. , Amanda Ortiz, J. , Sagi‐Schwartz, A. , Sichimba, F. , Soares, I. , … Zreik, G. (2016). Is the ideal mother a sensitive mother? Beliefs about early child‐hood parenting in mothers across the globe. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 40(5), 385–397. 10.1177/0165025415594030 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  87. Moher, D. , Liberati, A. , Tetzlaff, J. , Altman, D. G. , & The PRISMA Group . (2009). Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta‐Analyses: The PRISMA Statement. PLoS Med, 6(7), e1000097. 10.7326/0003-4819-151-4-200908180-00135 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Muñoz, E. , & Farkas, Ch. (2018). Relación entre atención compartida madre‐hijo, sensibilidad materna y expresión emocional gestual infantil [Relationship between mother‐child shared attention, maternal sensitivity and infant emotional gestural expresión]. Acta Colombiana de Psicología, 21(2), 131–155. 10.14718/acp.2018.21.2.7 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  89. Nóblega, M. , Bárrig, P. , Conde, L. C. , Nuñez del Prado, J. , Carbonell, O. A. , de Litvan, M. A. , González, E. , Sasson, E. , de Perkal, A. W. , & Bauer, M. (2016). Cuidado materno y seguridad del apego antes del primer año de vida [Maternal Care and Attachment Security in the First Year of Life]. Universitas Psychologica, 15(1), 15–30. 10.11144/Javeriana.upsy15-1.cmsa [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  90. Nóblega, M. , Bárrig, P. , & Fourment, K. (2019). Cuidado materno y seguridad del apego del niño preescolar [Maternal Care and Attachment Security in Preschool Children]. Psicologia: teoria e pesquisa, 35, Article e3534. 10.1590/0102.3772e3534 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  91. Nóblega, M. , Bárrig‐Jó, P. , Gonzalez, L. , Fourment, K. , Salinas‐Quiroz, F. , Vizuet, A. , & Posada, G. (2019). Secure base scripted knowledge and preschoolers’ social competence in samples from Mexico and Peru. Attachment and Human Development, 21(3), 253–264. 10.1080/14616734.2019.1575548 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Nóblega, M. , Conde, G. , Núñez, J. , Bárrig, P. , Marinelli, F. , Alcántara, N. , & Cárcamo, R. (2019). Evidencias de validez de constructo y de criterio de la Escala Massie‐Campbell de Apego Durante Estrés (ADS) [Construct and criterion validity evidences of the Massie‐Campbell Scale of Attachment During Stress (ADS)]. Acta Colombiana de Psicología, 22(1), 129–140. 10.14718/ACP.2019.22.1.7 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  93. Olhaberry, M. (2011). Calidad de la interacción madre‐hijo(a), asistencia a salas cuna y la importancia de la edad de ingreso: Estudio comparativo en familias monoparentales chilenas [Quality of mother‐child interaction, attendance at nurseries and the importance of the age of admission: Comparative study in Chilean single‐parent families]. Revista Argentina de Clínica Psicológica, 20(2), 161–173. Retrieved from https://www.redalyc.org/articulo.oa?id=281922823007 [Google Scholar]
  94. Olhaberry, M. (2012). Interacciones tempranas y género infantil en familias monoparentales chilenas [Early interactions and child gender in chilean single‐mothers families]. Revista Latinoamericana de Psicología, 44(2), 75–86. Retrieved from http://www.scielo.org.co/scielo.php?script=sci_arttext&pid=S0120‐05342012000200007&lang=es [Google Scholar]
  95. Olhaberry, M. , Escobar, M. , Mena, C. , Santelices, M. P. , Morales‐Reyes, I. , Rojas, G. , & Martínez, V. (2015). Intervención grupal para reducir la sintomatología depresiva y promover la sensibilidad materna en embarazadas chilenas [Group intervention for reducing depressive symptoms and promoting maternal sensitivity in pregnant Chilean women]. Suma Psicológica, 22(2), 93–101. 10.1016/j.sumpsi.2015.08.002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  96. Olhaberry, M. , Escobar, M. , Morales, I. , Cierpka, M. , Frey, B. , Eickhorst, A. , & Sidor, A. (2015). Díadas madre adolescente‐bebé chilenas y alemanas institucionalizadas: Estudio comparativo sobre depresión, calidad vincular, desarrollo infantil y variables culturales [Chileans and Germans institutionalized teen mother ‐baby dyads: Comparative Study about depression, bonding quality, development child and cultural variables]. Revista Argentina de Clínica Psicológica, 24(1), 79–92. Retrieved from https://www.redalyc.org/articulo.oa?id=281944843009 [Google Scholar]
  97. Olhaberry, M. , León, M. J. , Seguel, M. , & Mena, C. (2015). Video‐feedback intervention in mother‐baby dyads with depressive symptomatology and relationship difficulties. Research in Psychotherapy: Psychopathology, Process and Outcome, 18(2), 82–92. 10.4081/ripppo.2015.177 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  98. Olhaberry, M. , & Santelices, M. P. (2013). Presencia del padre y calidad de la interacción madre‐hijo: un estudio comparativo en familias chilenas nucleares y monoparentales [Father's Presence and the Quality of Mother‐Child Interaction: A Comparative Study on Chilean Nuclear and Single‐Mother Families]. Universitas Psychologica, 12(3), 833–843. 10.11144/Javeriana.UPSY12-3.ppci [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  99. Ortiz, J. A. , Borré, A. , Carrillo, S. , & Gutiérrez, G. (2006). Relación de apego en madres adolescentes y sus bebés canguro [Attachment relationship in teenage mothers and their kangaroo babies]. Revista Latinoamericana de Psicología, 38(1), 71–86. Retrieved from http://www.scielo.org.co/scielo.php?script=sci_arttext&pid=S0120‐05342006000100005&lang=es [Google Scholar]
  100. Palm, G. (2014). Attachment theory and fathers: Moving from “being there” to “being with.” Journal of Family Theory and Review, 6(4), 282–297. 10.1111/jftr.12045 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  101. Pederson, D. R. , & Moran, G. (1995). A categorical description of infant–mother relationships in the home and its relation to Q‐sort measures of infant–mother interaction. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 60(2–3), 111–132. 10.2307/1166174 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  102. Pérez, F. , Moessner, M. , & Santelices, M. P. (2017). Beyond the dyad: The relationship between preschoolers’ attachment representations and family triadic interactions. Infant Mental Health Journal, 38(2), 198–209. 10.1002/imhj.21633 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Pérez, F. , Santelices, M. P. , Rivera, C. , & Farkas, Ch. (2013). Preschoolers' attachment representations and maternal psychosocial characteristics in Santiago, Chile. Mental Health and Prevention, 1(1), 33–43. 10.1016/j.mhp.2013.10.002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  104. Pierrehumbert, B. , Santelices, M. P. , Ibáñez, M. , Alberdi, M. , Ongari, B. , Roskam, I. , Stievenart, M. , Spencer, R. , Fresno Rodríguez, A. , & Borghini, A. (2009). Gender and attachment representations in the preschool years: Comparisons between five countries. Journal of Cross‐Cultural Psychology, 40(4), 543–566. 10.1177/0022022109335181 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  105. Posada, G. , Carbonell, O. A. , Alzate, G. , & Plata, S. J. (2004). Through Colombian lenses: Ethnographic and conventional analyses of maternal care and their associations with secure base behavior. Developmental Psychology, 40(4), 508–518. 10.1037/0012-1649.40.4.508 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Posada, G. , Gao, Y. , Wu, F. , Posada, R. , Tascon, M. , Schöelmerich, A. , Sagi, A. , Kondo‐Ikemura, K. , Haaland, W. , & Synnevaag, B. (1995). The secure‐base phenomenon across cultures: Children's behavior, mother's preferences, and experts' concepts. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 60(2/3), 27–48. 10.2307/1166169 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  107. Posada, G. , Jacobs, A. , Carbonell, O. A. , Alzate, G. , Bustamante, M. R. , & Arenas, A. (1999). Maternal care and attachment security in ordinary and emergency contexts. Developmental Psychology, 35(6), 1379–1388. 10.1037/0012-1649.35.6.1379 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Posada, G. , Jacobs, A. , Richmond, M. K. , Carbonell, O. A. , Alzate, G. , Bustamante, M. R. , & Quiceno, J. (2002). Maternal caregiving and infant security in two cultures. Developmental Psychology, 38(1), 67–78. 10.1037/0012-1649.38.1.67 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Posada, G. , Kaloustian, G. , Richmond, M. , & Moreno, A. (2007). Maternal secure base support and preschoolers´ secure base behavior in natural environments. Attachment & Human Development, 9, 393–411. 10.1080/14616730701712316 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Posada, G. , Trumbell, J. , Noblega, M. , Plata, S. , Peña, P. , Carbonell, O. A. , & Lu, T. (2016). Maternal sensitivity and child secure base use in early childhood: Studies in different cultural contexts. Child Development, 87(1), 297–311. 10.1111/cdev.12454 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Posada, G. , Lu, T. , Trumbell, J. , Kaloustian, G. , Trudel, M. , Plata, S. J. , Peña, P. P. , Perez, J. , Tereno, S. , Dugravier, R. , Coppola, G. , Constantini, A. , Cassibba, R. , Kondo‐Ikemura, K. , Nóblega, M. , Haya, I. M. , Pedraglio, C. , Verissimo, M. , Santos, A. J. , … Lay, K. L. (2013). Is the secure base phenomenon evident here, there, and anywhere? A cross‐cultural study of child behavior and experts' definitions. Child Development, 84(6), 1896–1905. 10.1111/cdev.12084 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Quezada, V. , & Santelices, M. P. (2010). Apego y psicopatología materna: relación con el estilo de apego del bebé al año de vida [Attachment and mother's psychopathology: relation with baby attachment style in the first year of life]. Revista Latinoamericana de Psicología, 42(1), 53–61. Retrieved from http://www.scielo.org.co/scielo.php?script=sci_arttext&pid=S0120‐05342010000100005&lang=es [Google Scholar]
  113. Quinn, N. , & Mageo, J. (2013). Attachment and culture: An introduction. In Quinn N. & Mageo J. (Eds.), Attachment reconsidered: Cultural perspectives on a Western theory (pp. 3–32). SPA Palgrave. [Google Scholar]
  114. Ramos, R. , Yávar, V. , Del Río, A. , Schettino, J. , Bresciani, V. , Gómez, D. , Álvarez, C. , & Farkas, Ch. (2020). Mode of delivery and maternal sensitivity: Effects on the socioemotional development of children at one year of age. Acta Colombiana de Psicología, 23(2), 241–253. 10.14718/ACP.2020.23.2.10 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  115. Ribeiro, A. C. , Seidl‐de‐Moura, M. L. , Leal Fernandes Mendes, D. M. , & Mesman, J. (2021). Maternal sensitivity in interactions with their 2‐month‐old infants in Rio de Janeiro ‐ Brazil. Attachment and Human Development, 23(2), 188–198. 10.1080/14616734.2020.1828543 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Riquelme, E. H. , Henríquez, C. , & Álvarez, B. (2003). Relación entre estilos de apego y teoría de la mente [Relationship Between Attachment Styles and Theory of Mind]. Psykhe, 12(1), 73–81. Retrieved from http://www.psykhe.cl/index.php/psykhe/article/view/342/322 [Google Scholar]
  117. Rothbaum, F. , Weisz, J. , Pott, M. , Miyake, K. , & Morelli, G. (2000). Attachment and culture: Security in the United States and Japan. American Psychologist, 55(10), 1093–1104. 10.1037//0003-066X.55.10.1093 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Salinas‐Quiroz, F. (2015). Sensibilidad, comportamiento de base segura y desarrollo sociocognitivo en centros de educación inicial en México [Sensitivity, Secure Base Behavior and Socio‐Cognitive Development in Mexican Childcare Settings]. Universitas Psychologica, 14(3), 1033–1044. 10.11144/Javeriana.upsy14-3.scbs [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  119. Salinas‐Quiroz, F. , Morales, F. , Cruz, L. M. , Posada, G. , & Carbonell, O. A. (2014). Aproximación Psicométrica al Uso de los Q‐Sorts en Contextos de Educación Inicial. [Psychometric Evaluation of the Q‐sorts in Initial Education Contexts]. Acta De Investigación Psicológica, 4(2), 1595–1611. 10.1016/S2007-4719(14)70396-5 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  120. Salinas‐Quiroz, F. , Rodríguez‐Sánchez, R. , Costa, P. A. , Rosales, M. , Silva, P. , & Cambón, V. (2018). Can children have ordinary expectable caregiving environments in unconventional contexts? Quality of care organization in three mexican same‐sex planned families. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 2349. 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02349 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Santelices, M. P. (2014). Stress and caregiver sensitivity among childcare staff in Santiago, Chile. European Early Childhood Education Research Journal, 22(2), 271–285. 10.1080/1350293X.2014.883723 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  122. Santelices, M. P. , Carvacho, C. , Farkas, C. , León, F. , Galleguillos, F. , & Himmel, E. (2012). Medición de la sensibilidad del adulto con niños de 6 a 36 meses de edad: Construcción y análisis preliminares de la escala de sensibilidad del adulto, E.S.A. [Adult sensitivity assessment in Interaction with 6–36 Months old Children: Design and Preliminary Analyses of Adult sensitivity scale]. Terapia Psicológica, 30(3), 19–29. 10.4067/S0718-48082012000300003 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  123. Santelices, M. P. , Farkas, C. , & Aracena, M. (2017). Evaluation of the effectiveness of a pilot program that promotes sensitive response in the educational staff of Chilean nursery schools. Children and Youth Services Review, 75, 87–95. 10.1016/j.childyouth.2017.02.023 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  124. Santelices, M. P. , Farkas, C. , Montoya, M. F. , Galleguillos, F. , Carvacho, C. , Fernández, A. , Morales, L. , Taboada, C. , & Himmel, E. (2015). Factores predictivos de sensibilidad materna en infancia temprana [Predictive factors of mother's sensitivity in early childhood]. Psicoperspectivas, 14(1), 66–76. 10.5027/psicoperspectivas-Vol14-Issue1-fulltext-441 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  125. Santelices, M. P. , Guzmán, G. , Aracena, M. , Farkas, C. , Armijo, I. , Pérez‐Salas, C. P. , & Borghini, A. (2010). Promoting secure attachment: Evaluation of the effectiveness of an early intervention pilot programme with mother–infant dyads in Santiago, Chile. Child: Care, Health and Development, 37(2), 203–210. 10.1111/j.1365-2214.2010.01161.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Santelices, M. P. , Olhaberry, M. , Pérez‐Salas, C. , & Carvacho, C. (2009). Comparative study of early interactions in mother‐child dyads and care centre staff child within the context of Chilean crèches. Child: Care, Health and Development, 36(2), 255–264. 10.1111/j.1365-2214.2009.01032.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Santelices, M. P. , & Pérez, F. (2013). Evolución del vínculo que establece el niño con el personal educativo en salas cuna chilenas [Children's Attachment Evolution with the Educational Staff in Chilean Child Care Centers]. Universitas Psychologica, 12(3), 821–832. 10.11144/Javeriana.UPSY12-3.even [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  128. Saur, B. , Bruck, I. , Antoniuk, S. A. , & Jaworski de Sá Riechi, T. I. (2018). Relação entre vínculo de apego e desenvolvimento cognitivo, linguístico e motor [Relationship between attachment bond and cognitive, language and motor development]. Psico (Porto Alegre), 49(3), 257–265. 10.15448/1980-8623.2018.3.27248 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  129. Solomon, J. , & George, C. (2016). The Measurement of Attachment Security and Related Constructs in Infancy and Early Childhood. In Cassidy J. & Shaver P. R. (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (3rd ed.) (pp. 366–396). Guilford. [Google Scholar]
  130. Sotgiu, I. , Galati, D. , Manzano, M. , Gandione, M. , Gomez, K. , Romero, Y. , & Rigardetto, R. (2011). Parental attitudes, attachment styles, social networks, and psychological processes in autism spectrum disorders: A cross‐cultural perspective. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 172(4), 353–375. 10.1080/00221325.2010.544342 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Tenorio De Aguiar, S. , Santelices, M. P. , & Pérez, J. C. (2009). Apego, sensibilidad paterna y patrón de interacción del padre con su primer bebé [Attachment, paternal sensitivity and interaction pattern of the father with his first baby.]. Revista Argentina de Clínica Psicológica, 18(1), 51–58. Retrieved from https://www.redalyc.org/articulo.oa?id=281921800005 [Google Scholar]
  132. Thompson, R. (2016). Early attachment and later development: Reframing the questions. In Cassidy J. & Shaver P. R. (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (3rd ed., pp. 330–348). Guilford. [Google Scholar]
  133. Unicef , (2021). Social Policy: Equitable opportunities for every child . Promoting strategies to reduce child poverty. Retreived April 8, 2021 from https://www.unicef.org/lac/en/social‐policy‐equitable‐opportunities‐every‐child [Google Scholar]
  134. van IJzendoorn, M. H. (1990). Developments in cross‐cultural research on attachment: Some methodological notes. Human Development, 33, 3–9. 10.1159/000276498 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  135. Valenzuela, M. (1990). Attachment in chronically underweight young children. Child Development, 61(6), 1984–1996. 10.2307/1130852 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  136. Valenzuela, M. (1997). Maternal sensitivity in a developing society: The context of urban poverty and infant chronic undernutrition. Developmental Psychology, 33(5), 845–855. 10.1037//0012-1649.33.5.845 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  137. Villachan‐Lyra, P. , Almeida, E. , Hazin, I. , & Maranhão, S. (2015). Styles of attachment and acquisition of the Theory of Mind. Estudos de Psicologia, 20(3), 139–150. 10.5935/1678-4669.20150016 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  138. Voges, J. , Berg, A. , & Niehaus, D. J. H. (2019). Revisiting the African origins of attachment research—50 years on from Ainsworth: A descriptive review. Infant Mental Health Journal, 40(6), 799–816. 10.1002/imhj.21821 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  139. Wachs, T. D. , Posada, G. , Carbonell, O. A. , Creed‐Kanashiro, H. , & Gurkas, P. (2011). Infant nutrition and 12 and 18 months secure base behavior: An exploratory study. Infancy, 16(1), 91–111. https://doi‐org.ezproxy.leidenuniv.nl:2443/10.1111/j.1532‐7078.2010.00040.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  140. Waters, E. (1995). Appendix A: The attachment Q Set (version 3.0) Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 60(2/3), 234–246. 10.2307/1166181 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  141. Weisner, T. S. (2015). The socialization of trust: Plural caregiving and diverse pathways in human development across cultures. In Otto H. & Keller H. (Eds.), Different faces of attachment (pp. 263–277). Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  142. Woldarsky, V. , Urzúa, C. , Farkas, C. , & Vallotton, C. (2019). Differences in Chilean and USA Mothers' sensitivity considering child gender and temperament. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 28(7), 1937–1947. 10.1007/s10826-019-01419-3 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  143. Woodhouse, S. S. (2018). Attachment‐based interventions for families with young children. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 74(8), 1296–1299. 10.1002/jclp.22640 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Infant Mental Health Journal are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES