Skip to main content
Frontiers in Molecular Neuroscience logoLink to Frontiers in Molecular Neuroscience
. 2022 Sep 27;15:972025. doi: 10.3389/fnmol.2022.972025

General anesthesia in children and long-term neurodevelopmental deficits: A systematic review

Aoyi Xiao 1,, Yingying Feng 1,, Shan Yu 2, Chunli Xu 2, Jianghai Chen 3, Tingting Wang 2,*, Weimin Xiao 2,*
PMCID: PMC9551616  PMID: 36238262

Abstract

Background

Millions of children experienced surgery procedures requiring general anesthesia (GA). Any potential neurodevelopmental risks of pediatric anesthesia can be a serious public health issue. Various animal studies have provided evidence that commonly used GA induced a variety of morphofunctional alterations in the developing brain of juvenile animals.

Methods

We conducted a systematic review to provide a brief overview of preclinical studies and summarize the existing clinical studies. Comprehensive literature searches of PubMed, EMBASE, CINAHL, OVID Medline, Web of Science, and the Cochrane Library were conducted using the relevant search terms “general anesthesia,” “neurocognitive outcome,” and “children.” We included studies investigating children who were exposed to single or multiple GA before 18, with long-term neurodevelopment outcomes evaluated after the exposure(s).

Results

Seventy-two clinical studies originating from 18 different countries published from 2000 to 2022 are included in this review, most of which are retrospective studies (n = 58). Two-thirds of studies (n = 48) provide evidence of negative neurocognitive effects after GA exposure in children. Neurodevelopmental outcomes are categorized into six domains: academics/achievement, cognition, development/behavior, diagnosis, brain studies, and others. Most studies focusing on children <7 years detected adverse neurocognitive effects following GA exposure, but not all studies consistently supported the prevailing view that younger children were at greater risk than senior ones. More times and longer duration of exposures to GA, and major surgeries may indicate a higher risk of negative outcomes.

Conclusion

Based on current studies, it is necessary to endeavor to limit the duration and numbers of anesthesia and the dose of anesthetic agents. For future studies, we require cohort studies with rich sources of data and appropriate outcome measures, and carefully designed and adequately powered clinical trials testing plausible interventions in relevant patient populations.

Keywords: general anesthesia, children, pediatrics, neurodevelopment, neurotoxicity

Introduction

On average 3.9 million surgeries are performed on children between 0 and 17 years of age in America each year, with 4.7% of children undergoing a surgical procedure. Any potential neurocognitive risks of pediatric anesthesia can be a serious public health issue (Rabbitts and Groenewald, 2020). A common parental concern is whether general anesthesia (GA) has potentially adverse effects on children's developing brains. But this topic remains controversial.

A multitude of preclinical evidence derived from in vitro and in vivo animal studies has shown that commonly used general anesthetics induce a variety of morphofunctional alterations in the developing brain of juvenile animals. In addition, evidence that long-term and prolonged exposure may cause greater harm to neuro-cognitive development than short-term exposure in various animals has been demonstrated in some studies. These effects are thought to be mediated through two principal mechanisms: (1) an increase in inhibition via γ-aminobutyric acid (GABA-A) receptors (e.g., benzodiazepines, barbiturates, propofol, etomidate, isoflurane, enflurane, sevoflurane, and halothane) (Franks and Lieb, 1994), and (2) a decrease in excitation through N-methyl-aspartate (NMDA) receptors [e.g., ketamine, nitrous oxide (N2O), and xenon] (Lodge and Anis, 1982; Franks et al., 1998; Jevtović-Todorović et al., 1998; Mennerick et al., 1998).

In December 2016, the U.S. Food and Drug Administration (FDA) approved label changes for use of all commonly used anesthetics that bind to GABA and NMDA receptors in young children including a new warning stating that exposure to these medicines for lengthy periods or over multiple surgeries or procedures may negatively affect brain development in children younger than 3 years (Food and Drug Administration Drug Safety Communication, 2017; FDA Drug Safety Communication, 2022). This update appears to have been based largely on the preclinical non-human primate data, particularly regarding the duration of anesthesia and the risk of multiple anesthetic exposures (Andropoulos and Greene, 2017). Though regarding the heterogeneity between animal models and humans, the lack of surgical stimulus, resulting inflammation in experimental animals, and appropriate outcomes translatable to children, whether the extrapolation of animal data to humans is feasible remains unclear, the overwhelming evidence does provide biological and theoretical plausibility to the hypothesis that exposure to anesthesia agents in children may cause long-term neurocognitive effect.

However, recent clinical studies have demonstrated disputable results with the adoption of various databases, ages at exposure, surgeries, anesthetic agents, and outcome measures. Some studies observed no correlation between multiple anesthesia exposures and neurodevelopmental deficits (Graham et al., 2016) while others reported that even a single exposure could increase the risk of deficits (Ing et al., 2012). It is difficult to elucidate whether this inconsistency of results is due to the choice of outcomes, the difference of vulnerability among age groups, the duration or frequency of GA exposure, the degree of surgical stimulus, and neurotoxicity of various anesthetic agents or other unidentified confounders. Thus, human studies to date have not adequately answered the above-raised question.

The purpose of this systematic review is to provide a brief overview of pre-clinical studies, summarize the existing clinical studies focusing on a few providing the most robust evidence, and discuss the remaining problems and future research design.

We discuss questions as follows:

  1. What long-term neurocognitive outcomes can be induced by GA exposure at a young age?

  2. If GA exposure can cause negative neurological effects in children, is the younger age at exposure correlated with a higher vulnerability?

  3. If GA exposure can cause negative neurological effects in children, is the frequency or duration of anesthesia procedures correlated with higher risk?

  4. If GA exposure can cause negative neurological effects in children, do specific types of surgical procedures or anesthetic agents account for the detrimental effect?

  5. If GA exposure can cause negative neurological effects in children, are anesthesia procedures per se causal?

Methods

The systematic review was conducted and reported according to preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses (PRISMA) guidelines.

Search strategy

Comprehensive literature searches of PubMed, EMBASE, CINAHL, OVID Medline, Web of Science, and the Cochrane Library were conducted using the relevant search terms “general anesthesia,” “neurocognitive outcome,” and “children” (last search on May 1, 2022). The exact search for the respective databases is available as Supplementary material 1. In total, 73,644 papers were identified. Two reviewers independently screened titles and abstracts, eliminating duplicates and irrelevant items. Finally, a total of 72 studies were included in this review. The study flow chart is shown in Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

PRISMA flow diagram of literature review.

Study selection

To be eligible for this review, studies must report on:

  • Age at exposure ≤ 18 years.

  • Exposure to single/multiple GA or surgery (it is assumed that surgery is only delivered with a concomitant GA).

  • Evaluation of long-term neurodevelopment after the exposure(s) (The evaluation of neurodevelopment includes academics/achievement, cognition, development/behavior, diagnosis, brain studies, and others).

  • Between 2000 and 2022.

Studies must be published in peer-reviewed journals, identified by experts within the field, or have been cited by peer-reviewed papers.

We excluded studies that were not reported in English, publications before 2000, not evaluated neurodevelopment, and evaluated short-term neurodevelopment. Also excluded were studies on sedation performed in the intensive care unit, drug toxicities, prescription medications, substance abuse, and fetal (obstetric), adult, and geriatric populations. Besides, we also excluded the studies on children who had shown signs of impaired cognitive development before exposure to anesthetics.

Data extraction

We recorded the following variables if available: study ID, country of origin, publication, study design, data source, number of exposed/non-exposed study population, age at exposure, age at follow-up, the reason for anesthesia, anesthesia procedure (type, dosage, and duration), and outcome measures.

Results

Pre-Clinical evidence

Since 2003, a multitude of preclinical evidence derived from in vitro and in vivo animal studies has shown that commonly used GA induced a variety of morphofunctional alterations in the developing brain of juvenile animals (Deng et al., 2014). In addition, evidence that long-term and prolonged exposure may cause greater harm to neuro-cognitive development than short-term exposure in various animals has been interpreted in some studies. Several rodent models have provided evidence that the use of anesthetic drugs during the period of rapid brain growth or synaptogenesis, which in humans is generally considered to be before 3 years of age, results in widespread neuronal cell loss in the developing brain, and alterations in synaptic morphology and neurogenesis (Andropoulos and Greene, 2017), like rapidly increasing dendritic spine density (Briner et al., 2010), and causes diversified neurofunctional deficits including learning deficit (Jevtovic-Todorovic et al., 2003), abnormal social behaviors, or deficits in fear conditioning (Satomoto et al., 2009). Molecular and pathophysiological mechanisms have also been interpreted encompassing signaling in neuroinflammatory pathways (Soriano et al., 2010), the KCC2-dependent developmental increase in the efficacy of GABA-mediated inhibition (Puskarjov et al., 2017), epigenetic phenome (Dalla Massara et al., 2016), microglial activation, disruption of hippocampal P2RX2/CaMKII/NF-κB signaling (Zhao et al., 2022), or suppression of VEGFR2 signaling pathway (Zuo et al., 2022). A dose–response relationship has been detected in some studies as well (Liu et al., 2016).

More significant data can be obtained from juvenile non-human primate (NHP) models, among which rhesus macaques are most commonly applied, regarding their resemblance to humans in physiology, neuroanatomy, development, cognition, and behavior, allow for adequate monitoring of physiological parameters, and use of anesthetic equipment and techniques similar to those used for human children, augmenting the reliability of evidence, because it is possible that the adverse impacts attributed to GA do not occur when physiological parameters are monitored and controlled more effectively (Phillips et al., 2014). Extensive negative outcomes have been adopted and detected in NHP models to measure the impairment of the nervous system, ranging from neuronal or oligodendrocyte apoptosis response, cognition, and behavior to MRI results (Paule et al., 2011; Brambrink et al., 2012; Creeley et al., 2013; Raper et al., 2015, 2018; Alvarado et al., 2017; Coleman et al., 2017; Schenning et al., 2017; Talpos et al., 2019; Neudecker et al., 2021; Young et al., 2021).

Despite the heterogeneity between animal models and humans, the lack of surgical stimulus and resulting inflammation in experimental animals, appropriate outcomes translatable to children, a longer anesthetic duration in experimental animals (commonly over 4 h), whether the extrapolation of animal data to humans is feasible remains unclear, the overwhelming preclinical evidence does provide biological and theoretical plausibility to the hypothesis that exposure to anesthetic agents in children may cause long-term neurocognitive effect.

Clinical evidence

Seventy-two clinical studies originating from 18 different countries published from 2000 to 2022 are included in this review (Table 1). Two-thirds of studies (n = 48) provide evidence of negative neurocognitive effects after GA exposure in children. The majority are retrospective studies (n = 58), with the number of exposed individuals ranging from n = 15 to n = 42,684. The age of outcome assessment differentiates from 3 months postoperatively to 28 years.

Table 1.

Summarized characteristics of studies included in the current review.

Study ID Study design Study subjects Age at exposure Anesthesia protocol stated Yes/No Type of surgery stated Yes/No Outcome accessed
Multi-Exp. Single-Exp. Control
Andropoulos et al. (2014) Retrospective cohort study 0 59 0 <30 days Yes Yes Development/behavior
Brain studies
Håkanson et al. (2020) Retrospective cohort study 485 4,835 <1 yr Yes Yes Diagnosis, others
Backeljauw et al. (2015) Retrospective matched-cohort study 13 40 53 <4 yr Yes Yes Academics/achievement
Cognition brain studies
Bakri et al. (2015) Matched-Cohort study 35 0 35 1.5 ~ 5 yr Yes Yes Development/behavior
Diagnosis
Banerjee et al. (2019) Retrospective observational study 212 0 0 Mean 6.66 yr Yes No Cognition
Development/behavior
Brain studies
Bartels et al. (2009) Register based national cohort study 1,143 1,143 <12 yr No Yes Academics/achievement
Cognition
Development/behavior
Birajdar et al. (2017) Retrospective cohort study 0 33 0 1–22 days Yes Yes Development/behavior
Diagnosis
Others
Block et al. (2017) Retrospective cohort study 17 17 <1 yr Yes Yes Brain studies
Bong et al. (2013) Retrospective observational cohort study 100 106 <1 yr Yes Yes Academics/achievement
Diagnosis
Brévaut-Malaty et al. (2022) Cross-Sectional observational study 85 to sevoflurane; 18 without sevoflurane Neonatal period Yes Yes Cognition
Development/behavior
Diagnosis
Ing et al. (2017a) Retrospective cohort study 21 127 0 <3 yr Yes Yes Cognition
Development/behavior
Ing et al. (2020) Retrospective propensity score-matched cohort study 0 42,687 213,435 <5 yr No Yes Diagnosis
Clausen et al. (2017) Nationwide register-based follow-up study 509 14,677 <22 months No Yes Academics/achievement
Conrad et al. (2017) Retrospective cohort study 87 160 <7 yr No Yes Cognition
Brain studies
Davidson et al. (2016) International assessor-masked randomized controlled trial LA: 238; GA: 294 Up to 60 weeks post-menstrual age Yes Yes Cognition
Development/behavior
de Heer et al. (2017) Retrospective cohort study 415 3,026 <5 yr Yes No Cognition
Diaz et al. (2016) Bidirectional cohort study 96 0 <6 months Yes Yes Cognition
DiMaggio et al. (2011) Retrospective sibling-matched birth cohort study 304 10,146 <3 yr No Yes Development/behavior Diagnosis
DiMaggio et al. (2009) Retrospective cohort study 383 5,050 12–48 months No Yes Development/behavior Diagnosis
Djurhuus et al. (2016) Retrospective Cohort study 549 15,106 Mean 9.4 yr No Yes Academics/achievement
Doberschuetz et al. (2016) Ambidirectional matched cohort study 40 40 <28 days No Yes Development/behavior
Fan et al. (2013) Single-Center prospective study 0 72 0 4 ~ 7 yr Yes Yes Cognition
Feng et al. (2020) Retrospective national population-based cohort study 5,576 5,881 22,914 <2 yr Yes Yes Development/behavior
Flick et al. (2011) Population based, matched cohort study 64 286 700 <2 yr Yes Yes Academics/achievement
Cognition
Development/behavior
Diagnosis
Gano et al. (2015) Prospective cohort study 18 25 94 Before term equivalent age Yes Yes Cognition
Garcia Guerra et al. (2014) Prospective follow-up study 122 0 ≤ 6 weeks Yes Yes
Cognition
Development/behavior
Glatz et al. (2017) Nationwide cohort study 3,640 33,514 159,619 <4 yr No Yes
Academics/achievement
cognition
Graham et al. (2016) Retrospective matched cohort study 620 3,850 13,586 <4 yr No Yes Development/behavior
Green et al. (2015) Retrospective autopsy-based study 19 7 20 Exp.: 23 days
Non-exp.: 9 days
Yes No Brain studies
Hansen et al. (2015) Nationwide unselected register-based follow-up study 228 14,698 Infant No Yes Academics/achievement
Others
Hansen et al. (2013) Nationwide unselected register-based follow-up study 0 2,689 14,575 <1 yr No Yes Academics/achievement
Hansen et al. (2011) Retrospective nationwide unselected register-based follow-up study 779 14,665 <3months No Yes Academics/
achievement
Hu et al. (2017) Retrospective cohort study 116 457 463 <3 yr Yes Yes Academics/achievement
Development/behavior
diagnosis
Ing et al. (2012) Retrospective cohort study 52 206 1,523 <3 yr No Yes Cognition
Development/behavior
Ing et al. (2014) Retrospective cohort study 112 669 <3 yr No Yes Academics/
achievement
Cognition
Development/behavior
Diagnosis
Ing et al. (2017c) Retrospective cohort study 34 122 963 <3 yr No Yes Cognition
Development/behavior
Ing et al. (2017b) Observational cohort study 0 38,943 192,465 <5 yr No Yes Development/behavior
Diagnosis
Jacola et al. (2020) Prospective study 101 0 0 10.1 yr Yes Yes
Academics/achievement
Cognition
Kalkman et al. (2009) Retrospective cohort study 0 243 0 <6 yr Yes Yes Development/behavior
Kayaalp et al. (2006) Retrospective cohort study 23 10 10 4–18 yr Yes Yes Development/behavior
Diagnosis
Ko et al. (2015) Nationwide retrospective matched cohort study 5,197 20,788 <2 yr Yes Yes
Diagnosis
Ko et al. (2014) Nationwide retrospective matched cohort study 1,274 2,019 13,172 <3 yr No No
Diagnosis
Kobayashi et al. (2020) Retrospective cohort study 161 746 63,232 <1 yr No No Development/behavior
Lap et al. (2017) Retrospective cohort study 9 7 32 Newborn No Yes Cognition
Development/behavior
Laporta et al. (2021) Retrospective population-based birth cohort study 499ASD (132 exposure; 367 control);
998 controls (155 exposure; 843 control)
<3 yr Yes Yes
Diagnosis
Sun et al. (2016) Sibling-matched cohort study 0 105 105 <3 yr Yes Yes Cognition
Development/behavior
McCann et al. (2019) International assessor-masked randomized controlled trial GA:242; LA:205 0 <60 weeks' post-menstrual age Yes Yes Academics/achievement
Cognition
Development/behavior
Diagnosis, others
Morriss et al. (2014) Retrospective cohort study GA:2186;
LA:784;
multi-exp.:1080;
single-exp.:1890
9,141 <22 months No No
Development/behavior
Others
Nestor et al. (2017) Retrospective cohort study 64 234 129 <1 yr Yes Yes
Development/behavior
Diagnosis
Brain studies
O'Leary et al. (2016) Population-Based retrospective cohort study 28,366 55,910 <6 yr No Yes
Development/behavior
O'Leary et al. (2019) Retrospective sibling-matched cohort study 591 2,489 18,714
<2 yr: 43.6%
>2 yr: 56.4%
No Yes
Development/behavior
Partanen et al. (2021) Prospective longitudinal study 107 0 0 Mean 10.2 yr Yes Yes
Cognition
Petráčková et al. (2015) Retrospective cohort study 0 3 0 3–10 months Yes Yes
Cognition
Development/behavior
Poor Zamany Nejat Kermany et al. (2016) Retrospective cohort study 46 22 47 <3 yr Yes Yes
Cognition
Schneuer et al. (2018) Retrospective population-based record-linkage cohort study 37,880 197,301 <4 yr No Yes
Academics/achievement
Development/behavior
Schüttler et al. (2021) Controlled cross-sectional study 93 347 67 <3 yr Yes No
Cognition
Seltzer et al. (2016) Observational cohort study 21 0 <30 days Yes Yes
Development/behavior
Shi et al. (2022) Prospective longitudinal study 23 38 0 2.5–6 yr Yes Yes
Cognition
Development/behavior
Sprung et al. (2012) Retrospective cohort study 64 286 5,007 <2 yr Yes Yes
Diagnosis
Taghon et al. (2015) Bidirectional cohort study 15 Not stated <2 yr Yes Yes Development/behavior
Brain studies
Terushkin et al. (2017) Retrospective single-center cohort study 33 0 0 <4 yr Yes Yes Academics/achievement
Cognition
Development/behavior Diagnosis
Walkden et al. (2020) Retrospective population-based birth cohort study 212 1,110 12,111 <4 yr No No Academics/achievement
Cognition
Development/behavior
Walsh et al. (2021) Prospective longitudinal observational cohort study 25 59 27 weeks No No Development/behavior
Brain studies
Warner et al. (2021) Retrospective cohort study 206 380 411 <3 yr Yes Yes Cognition
Development/behavior
Warner et al. (2018) Retrospective cohort study 0 1,054 5,339 <3 yr Yes Yes Development/behavior
Diagnosis
Wilder et al. (2009) Population-Based retrospective cohort study 144 449 4,764 <4 yr Yes Yes Diagnosis
Yang et al. (2021) Retrospective nationwide cohort study 2,261 4,522 <3 yr Yes No Cognition
Development/behavior
Diagnosis
Yazar et al. (2016) Retrospective cohort study 33 94 707 <3 yr Yes Yes Others
Yin et al. (2014) Observational prospective study (single center) Propofol:30;
sevoflurane:30
0 7–13 yr Yes Yes Cognition
Zaccariello et al. (2019) Population-Based retrospective cohort study ClusterA:106; ClusterB:557; ClusterC:334 <3 yr Yes Yes Cognition
Development/behavior
Diagnosis
Zhang et al. (2017) Prospective cohort study <1 h:49;
1–3 h:51;
>3 h:79
30 6–12 yr No Yes Cognition
Zhou et al. (2021) Prospective, equivalence, controlled trial GA:129;
LA:144
0 <7 yr Yes Yes Cognition

Outcome

Various neuropsychological tests, batteries, academic performance, and imaging tests were applied to measure the neurocognitive long-term outcome potentially induced by GA. Detailed information on psychometric tests and batteries is displayed in Table 2. For simplicity, outcomes were categorized into six domains: academics/achievement, cognition, development/behavior, diagnosis, brain studies, and others. Multiple domains were measured in over half of the studies (n = 42).

Table 2.

List of miscellaneous neurocognitive tests utilized to assess neurodevelopment of children exposed to general anesthesia (alphabetic order) and the domain to which the test was assigned.

Psychometric test Domain of testing
ABAS-II Adaptive Behavior Assessment System-2nd Edition Development/behavior
ASQ Ages & Stages Questionnaire Development/behavior
AVLT Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test Cognition
BDS Backward digit span Cognition
Beery Visual Perception Cognition
Beery Motor Coordination Development/behavior
BNT Boston Naming Test Development/behavior
BRIEF Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function Development/behavior
BSID-II Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development-2nd Edition Cognition
Development/behavior
BSID-III The Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development-III Cognition
Development/behavior
CAT California Achievement Test Academics/achievement
CBCL Child Behavior Checklist Development Development/behavior
CDI Child Depression Inventory Diagnosis
CELF Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals Development/behavior
CHQ50 The Child Health Questionnaire 50 Development/behavior
CLDQ Colorado Learning Difficulties Questionnaire Diagnosis
CPM Raven's Colored Progressive Matrices Cognition
CPT-II/CCPT-II (Conners') Continuous Performance Test II Diagnosis
CTOPP Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing Development/behavior
CTRS-R Conners' teacher Rating Scale-Revised Cognition
Development/behavior
DKEFS Delis-Kaplan Executive Function Systems/Trail Making Subtests Development/behavior
DSM-IV Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-4th Edition Diagnosis
EDI Early Development Instrument Development/behavior
FDS Forward digit span test Cognition
GMDS Griffiths Mental Development Scale Development/behavior
GMFCS General Motor Function Classification Score Development/behavior
GPT Grooved Pegboard Test Development/behavior
ICD-9 International classification of Diseased-9th Edition Diagnosis
IEP-EBD Individualized Educational Program-Disorders of Emotion and Behavior Development/behavior
IEP-SL Individualized Educational Program-Speech and Language Development/behavior
KABC-II Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children—Second Edition Cognition
MacArthur-Bates CDI The MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventory: Words and Sentences Development/behavior
MAND McCarron Assessment of Neuromuscular Development Development/behavior
MSEL Mullen Scales of Early Learning Development/behavior
NEPSY Developmental NEuroPSYchological Assessment Development/behavior
NEPSY-2-NL Developmental Neuropsychological Assessment Battery, Second Edition, Dutch version Development/behavior
OLSAT OtiseLennon School Ability test Academics/achievement
OWLS Oral and Written Language Scales Cognition
PANDA PANDA neuropsychological battery Cognition
PPT Purdue pegboard test Development/behavior
PPVT Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test Cognition
PSLE Primary School Leaving Examination Academics/achievement
PVF phonemic verbal fluency test Cognition
RCPM Raven's Colored Progressive Matrices Cognition
RSPM The Raven's Standard Progressive Matrices Cognition
SDMT Symbol Digit Modality Test Cognition
SON-R Hogrefe/Snijders-Oomen Non-Verbal Intelligence Test-Revised Cognition
SVF Semantic verbal fluency Cognition
TCS Total Cognitive Skills Cognition
TEA-Ch NL Test of Everyday Attention for Children, Dutch version Development/behavior
Touwen neurological examination Development/behavior
TOWRE-2 Test of Word Reading Efficiency Development/behavior
VABS-II The Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scale, 2nd Edition Development/behavior
VMI Berry Visual Motor Integration Development/behavior
WASME Western Australian Monitoring Standards in Education Academics/achievement
WASI Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence Cognition
WIAT-II The Wechsler Individual Achievement Test Second Edition Academics/achievement
WISC-III Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-3rd Version Cognition
WISC-III-NL Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-3rd Version, Dutch version Cognition
WISC-IV Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-4th Edition Cognition
WJ-III-ACH The Woodcock Johnson Tests of Academic Achievement, Third Edition Academics/achievement
WJ-III-COG Woodcock Johnson Tests of Cognitive Abilities-Third Edition Cognition
WMS The Wechsler Memory Scale- Chinese Revision Cognition
Wisconsin Card Sorting Development/behavior
WOLD Wechsler Objective Language Dimensions Development/behavior
WPPSI-III The Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence Third Edition Full Scale Intelligence Quotient score Cognition
WPPSI-IV The Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence Fourth Edition Full Scale Intelligence Quotient score Cognition
WRAML-2 The Wide Range Assessment of Memory and Learning-Second Edition Development/behavior

Assessment of academics or school achievement was applied in 17 studies, among which 10 studies reported poorer academic results, especially for multiple or prolonged exposures to GA. Most were assessed after the 10th birthday except one with a mean age of 7.6-years at follow-up.

Thirty-four studies tested cognition including intelligence quotient (IQ), memory reasoning, spatial ability, processing speed, etc., with 20 studies evaluating IQ by adopting batteries encompassing PANDA, WASI, WISC, WPPSI-III, KABC-II, and SON-R. Twenty of these found a decline in cognitive function after exposure to GA.

Tests evaluating development or behavior were used in 42 studies. This domain not only covered any mental disorder, specifically developmental delays, such as reading and language disorders, and the need for special educational programs or therapy but also language and motor function, weight or height, behavior, executive ability, inattention, etc. CBCL, a battery primarily assessing children's behavioral, emotional, and social ability, was the most frequently used measure. An increased risk of developmental or behavioral disorders as the consequence of surgeries requiring GA was observed in 32 of these studies. Diagnostic results covering learning disability (LD), attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), autistic spectrum disorder (ASD), cerebral palsy, and other mental diseases, were used as outcome measures following diagnostic criteria of ICD-9, ICD-10, etc. in 24 studies. LD, ADHD, and ASD were the most commonly observed phenotypes, higher morbidity of which was reported in 15 studies.

Imaging tests and biopsy results (Green et al., 2015) in nine studies, despite some short-term results (Andropoulos et al., 2014; Green et al., 2015), all provided evidence of structural or functional abnormality after GA exposure.

Results from miscellaneous outcomes, including visual or auditory defect, educational level, income (Håkanson et al., 2020), and mortality (Morriss et al., 2014; Hansen et al., 2015), were unanimous. The two mortality studies both found significant ascent, while the other four studies obtained equivalence between unexposed and GA-exposed groups.

Age at exposure

Over 90% of the studies (n = 66) recruited children who were exposed to GA before the age of 7 years, among which neonates and infants were assessed in 11 and 12 studies, respectively. For the child group younger than 7 years, 65.2% of studies (n = 43) detected adverse neurocognitive effects after GA exposure, while the neonate and infant groups reported the proportion of 72.7% (n = 8) and 58.3% (n = 7), respectively. The 3rd birthday was mostly chosen as the demarcation point (n = 17), probably out of the consideration to provide a balance between a presumed window of vulnerability and including sufficient numbers of children for statistical analysis. Six studies adopted relatively senior samples ranging from 7 to 18 years. 83.33% (n = 5) of these demonstrated a neurological impairment. Age-related subgroups were subdivided into 18 studies to further elucidate whether a younger age at exposure was more vulnerable. Four provided evidence supporting this concern, while 11 studies indicated no significant distinction among age subgroups regardless of tantamount or negative outcomes after exposure. Three studies even showed contrary results that the elder the GA exposure, the greater the likelihood of long-term neurocognitive risk (Graham et al., 2016; O'Leary et al., 2016; Schneuer et al., 2018).

Anesthetic agents, frequency, and duration of GA exposure

The vast majority of studies utilized anesthetic agents with either GABA-A receptor-enhancing (sevoflurane, isoflurane, desflurane, halothane, propofol, thiopental, midazolam, etc.) or NMDA receptor-blocking (nitrous oxide, ketamine, etc.) properties, while only five mentioned the usage of opioids for analgesia. Most studies induced or maintained a surgical plane of anesthesia with the application of multiple types of anesthetic drugs in combination. The sole individually studied drug was sevoflurane, with six studies concluding no negative impact.

Nine studies recruited children with solely single GA exposure, with 2/3 showing no evidence of a correlation between exposure and adverse effect. Correspondingly, five of the six studies including only multiple-exposure individuals observed a significant decline in manifold neurological functions. The concern of whether more exposures were associated with a higher risk was discussed in 27 studies. Fourteen studies found an equivalence between the result of multiple and single exposure, eight reported neurocognitive detriments, and six showed non-effect. In the other 13 studies, however, repeated exposures were proved to generate severer harm. Seven studies provided evidence that children with multiple, but not single exposures were more likely to develop more serious adverse outcomes.

Detailed mean cumulative duration of GA was recorded in 27 studies, ranging diffusely from <45 min to 21.1 h (Poor Zamany Nejat Kermany et al., 2016; Jacola et al., 2020). Seventeen studies referred to a relatively short or moderate length shorter than 2 h. Eight of these provided evidence supporting malign results induced by GA under such circumstances, while the other nine gave opposite findings. Fifteen papers included cases with prolonged anesthetic procedures over 2 h, whereas the results appeared more consistent, with 13 of these observing a significant relationship between exposures and long-term diminution of neurocognitive functions (Taghon et al., 2015; Zhou et al., 2021). Moreover, Ing et al. classified the duration into 25, >25–35, >35–60, and >60 min and found even a duration over 35 min could contribute to lower total and expressive language scores (Ing et al., 2017a). Eighteen studies focused on the discrepancy among distinct duration groups, 11 of which found worse outcomes were directly proportional to the accumulated duration, while the others observed no inter-group gap.

Surgery procedure

The majority of studies included mixed cases of surgeries covering the most common pediatric operations, such as otorhinolaryngologic-, neurologic-, urologic-, gastrointestinal-, orthopedic-, plastics-, cardiovascular-, ocular-, dental-, and some other surgeries. Twenty-nine studies focused on one specific system or type of surgery. Eight studies found children undergoing inguinal hernia repair or ocular surgeries had no statistically significant variation in neurocognitive functions. All but one referred to laser surgery for vascular anomalies, and four cardiovascular surgery studies supported negative outcomes. Five studies proved teenage medulloblastoma or cleft-lip patients were more vulnerable, while the prognosis of gastrointestinal surgeries was ambiguous.

Studies comparing GA and LA

Local anesthesia (LA) was applied in four studies to further interpret the neurocognitive effect of GA per se with surgical factors eliminated. Three of these demonstrated equivalence between GA and LA, one of which was an international assessor-masked randomized controlled trial (GAS).

More robust studies and meta-analysis evidence

Most of the studies included have been retrospective in nature, in which various confounders can hardly be evited. Performing a randomized controlled trial is the ideal method, by which a causal relationship between an exposure and an outcome can be established. However, considering the difficulty and complexity of design and conduct, only one such study has been executed: The General Anesthesia or Awake-regional Anesthesia in Infancy (GAS) study. We also consider two bidirectional trials MASK (The Mayo Anesthesia Safety in Kids study) and PANDA (the Pediatric Anesthesia Neurodevelopment Assessment project) study, in which a retrospective cohort is developed and then prospectively evaluated, as more robust ones because of their strict match of study groups. Several meta-analysis evidence has been included as well, which synthesize homogeneous studies and provide more reliable conclusions. Thus in this review, we highlight these important studies to demonstrate the neurodevelopmental effect of GA.

GAS study

The GAS trial randomized a total of 722 infants <60 weeks post-menstrual age from 28 hospitals in seven different countries to receive either awake-regional anesthesia or sevoflurane-based general anesthesia for inguinal hernia repair. Randomization was stratified by site and gestational age at birth. The average duration of anesthesia was 54 min approximately.

The primary outcome was the full-scale IQ at 5 years of age measured by the WPPSI-III. It showed no significant difference between the two groups. The secondary outcome was the composite cognitive score of BSID-III, assessed at 2 years. Strong evidence of equivalence was found between the two groups.

No bias was introduced by the 19% failure rate for awake regional techniques by comparing intention-to-trea t and as-per-protocol analyses. Multiple imputations and complete case analyses showed no bias caused by the 15% loss to follow-up. However, this study cannot access the possible increased neurotoxicity caused by multiple exposures (Davidson et al., 2016; Shukla and Chowdhary, 2019).

MASK study

The MASK study, a propensity-matched cohort study, enrolled 998 children born in Olmsted County, Minnesota, born from 1994 to 2007. The children were divided into three groups according to their exposure to anesthesia before 3 years of age: 411 unexposed, 380 singly exposed, and 206 multiply exposed. The mean cumulative duration of anesthesia was 45 and 187 min in singly and multiply exposed children tested, respectively. Otorhinolaryngologic procedures were the most common and the most common anesthetic agents used were nitrous oxide and sevoflurane.

The children underwent neuropsychological testing at ages 8–12 or 15–20 years. The primary outcome was the full-scale IQ standard score of WASI, which showed no significant difference. The secondary outcomes, including a comprehensive neuropsychological assessment, such as WRAML-2, DKEFS, etc., and parent reports, did not differ significantly between singly exposed and unexposed children, while only the reading skills and the fine motor study composite differed significantly between multiply exposed and unexposed (Warner et al., 2018).

PANDA study

The PANDA study was a bidirectional sibling-matched study conducted between 2009 and 2015, which enrolled 105 sibling pairs within 36 months of age and is currently 8–15 years old, with one of the siblings exposed to general anesthesia for inguinal hernia surgery. The average duration of anesthesia exposure was 80 min. All exposed children received inhaled anesthetic agents (sevoflurane or isoflurane), but some of them also received intravenous agents (opioids, caudal anesthesia, or midazolam premedication).

The primary outcome was global cognitive function (IQ) and there was no evidence of any significant difference. The secondary outcomes included domain-specific neurocognitive functions and behavior, measured by CBCL and ABAS-II and the study still found no differences for children with and without a single anesthetic exposure.

Sibling controls worked effectively in reducing confounding influences of genetic background and family economic status. However, this study did not take the neurocognitive risks of multiple anesthesia exposures into consideration (Sun et al., 2016).

Evidence from meta-analysis

Four meta-analyses from different periods were included. In 2014, Wang et al. analyzed seven studies from 2009 to 2012 covering a total of 44,143 children under the age of 4, 5,546 of which had experienced GA due to surgical procedures. Educational achievement, cognition, development/behavior, and LD were assessed. Modestly elevated risk of adverse neurodevelopmental outcomes was observed in this study, especially for those with multiple times of exposure (Wang et al., 2014). A similarly designed meta-analysis from 2015 also suggested a modestly elevated risk of neurodevelopmental disorders exists in children near 3 years of age and a single GA is relatively safe after 3 years, as the outcome is very close before 3 and 4 years old (Zhang et al., 2015). In a study from 2020 that combined results encompassing the three above-mentioned clinical evidence (MASK, PANDA, and GAS) and others utilizing prospectively collected outcomes, single GA-exposed children were at a higher risk of behavioral problems but displayed no difference in general intelligence (Ing et al., 2021). One year later Sun and colleagues included seven studies adopting solely ADHD as an outcome measure (Sun et al., 2021). This meta-analysis indicated that the effect of GA on the risk of ADHD is dose- or duration dependent.

Discussion

Since 2000, numerous clinical studies tried to find out the association between GA and long-term neurocognitive deficits in children, notably after the publication of the FDA concern in 2016, probably out of eager demand for assertive evidence. Though the findings were mixed, overall, more studies found an increased risk after GA exposure. However, the heterogeneity in study design, sample size, demographic information of the studied children (gender, ethnicity, nationality, family income, parent's education level, etc.), the comparability between the exposed and unexposed group, preexisting medical conditions, characteristics of experienced surgical procedures and anesthetic agents (frequency, duration, and types of surgeries and drugs) age at GA exposure and follow-up, loss to follow-up, and outcome measures hindered the synthesis and replication of findings and prohibited reaching a decisive conclusion. Moreover, most studies were retrospectively designed in nature with only a few measuring relevant outcomes before exposure, leaving a lack of strong evidence for the validity of measuring neurodevelopmental changes due to anesthesia, probably because considering the complexity of its design, the difficulty of its implementation, the large human and financial resources required, and the associated ethical issues, it would be very difficult to implement a GAS-like randomized controlled trial. The studies examining the effects of anesthesia on neurodevelopment are usually cohort studies, in which confounding factors are intrinsically hard to avert. Confounding occurred when some potential factors affected both the possibility of anesthetic or surgical need and neurological damage, such as comorbid conditions, varying surgical procedures, the potentially different neurotoxicity of anesthetic agents, etc. These factors could either aggravate or alleviate the observed outcome, reducing the reliability of the studies. Although various methods including propensity-matched design, sibling-matched cohort study design, etc. were applied as an attempt to ameliorate the effect of confounding factors, such an approach might still fail to fully eliminate all relevant confounders. Loss to follow-up and some missing data also remained a concern, as we could not gauge whether these lead us to overestimate or underestimate the true impact.

Outcome

Investigators have applied a multitude of measures including batteries, diagnosis, brain studies, and academic tests to assess the long-term neurological outcome following GA; however, which phenotype can most precisely weigh this association is still currently under debate. The six outcome domains we categorize reflect different perspectives of neurocognitive damage but still share some overlap. Generally, more studies provided evidence of negative outcomes following GA exposure in six domains, respectively.

Several studies assessed academic achievement scores, such as average 9th-grade marks and Western Australian Monitoring Standards in Education (WAMSE). Previous studies have proved that IQ and academic achievement were correlatives (Barton et al., 1972; Laidra et al., 2007). School performance is now widely accepted to be a synthesis of intelligence, attention, behavior, and learning ability, so it can be a pragmatic and accessible method to describe the long-term effect of GA, in which parents are more interested (Cattell et al., 1972). However, academic achievement can be skewed by many confounding factors unrelated to GA exposure. For example, Glatz and colleagues found the overall difference in mean school grades at 16 years between GA-exposed and -unexposed cohorts was markedly less than the differences associated with sex, maternal educational level, or month of birth during the same year (Glatz et al., 2017). Clausen's team found the type of oral cleft as well as a more important factor than exposures (Clausen et al., 2017).

Cognition, commonly considered a comprehensive embodiment of intelligence, memory, abstract reasoning, spatial ability, and processing speed, was assessed in various studies, including MASK, PANDA, and GAS. Several former studies have demonstrated its relative stability and constancy throughout life (Deary et al., 2000, 2010). For example, in the Vietnam Experience Study, the cognitive function of a cohort of soldiers involved in military service abroad was tested during adolescence and in middle age (Association American Psychiatric, 2013). The cognition in young- and middle-age shared remarkable similarities. However, currently, we can hardly define which test or battery can most precisely reflect the change of cognition in practice, so despite its stability, it is hard to recommend cognition to be the most suitable outcome. Besides, cognitive assessment is not feasible until the child has achieved some basic cognitive skills. Hence, regarding the reliability of results, assessment is recommended to be practiced after school age (Clausen et al., 2018).

Development and behavior, as the most frequently used neurological index, not only cover the domain of any mental disorder, specifically developmental delay and need for special education program or therapy, but also language and motor function, behavior, executive ability, inattention, etc. Most studies adopted developmental batteries including CBCL, EDI, BSID, BRIEF, and others, reckoning the development of infants follows specific patterns which can be measured by sensorimotor and executive functions, language, and behavior at a certain age. Following this norm, the developmental state is appraised depending on whether the child reaches predefined “milestones.” Deviation from the regulated performance or delay in the progression indicates neurodevelopmental abnormalities. Resembling academic performance, development and behavior can be influenced by various confounding factors (i.e., socioeconomic status, diseases, and interaction with parents), thus compromising its stability as an outcome measure. Compared with cognition, development can be assessed even during the neonatal period, which allows for the assessment in very young cohorts.

A diagnosis, such as LD, ADHD, ASD, cerebral palsy, sleep disturbances, anxiety, and obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD), was assessed in several studies, in which the first three were most used. Diagnosis of diseases can provide direct and practical evidence to answer parents' concerns; however, its robustness as neurocognitive outcomes remains disputed. For example, their pathogenesis can be multifactoral, the heterogeneous clinical presentations may fluctuate over time, and such diseases are influenced by various underlying innate or environmental factors. Furthermore, a consensus on diagnostic criteria of these diseases is hardly available, some including IQ and actual school performance or certain clinical presentations, some incorporating specific questionnaires from parents and teachers, some using various batteries, and some strictly following the ICD-9/10 criteria. Moreover, a child with diseases like LD may at some point have a change in achievement placing him or her back in the normal range, thus the case cannot be detected (Ing et al., 2022).

Brain studies seemed to be the most sensitive outcome measure, with all nine studies directly displaying evidence of cerebral structural transformation after GA. In contrast to cognition, development/behavior, or diagnosis, imaging studies can be readily practiced and provide intuitional insight into brain injury. Meanwhile, brain structural changes were testified to correlate to other neurocognitive outcomes in some studies. Banerjee and colleagues found a significant correlation between processing speed and corpus callosum diffusivity (Banerjee et al., 2019). Other studies that reported decreased performance IQ and language comprehension were associated with lower gray matter density in the occipital cortex and cerebellum (Backeljauw et al., 2015), and increased frontal lobe volume accounted for a decline in verbal IQ (Conrad et al., 2017). However, whether a slight variation in brain structure can precisely predict certain neurological phenotypes remains controversial. Currently, we cannot definitively specify the extent to which structure shapes function within human brain networks until the brain structural networks are fully characterized at both micro and macro scales. Computational modeling and network approaches will be indispensable in the future search for structural-functional relationships across the multiscale architecture of the human brain (Honey et al., 2010).

For miscellaneous outcomes, visual acuity is an outcome of the proper functioning of the cornea, lens, retina, optic nerve, and higher cortical aspects from the parietal/temporal/occipital lobe (Yazar et al., 2016). Assessment of deafness likewise reflected the functioning of the auditory conduction pathway. Higher mortalities can indicate a worse developmental progression or prognosis to a certain extent. However, the two studies referring to mortalities included very low-birth-weight preterm infants (Morriss et al., 2014) or neurosurgical procedures, which may compromise the strength of the correlation between GA and this outcome.

Age at exposure

This review includes a wide range of ages at GA exposure from the neonate period to the age of 18. Most studies focusing on children <7 years detected adverse neurocognitive effects after GA exposure, while the neonate and infant group was at an even greater risk. The period of rapid brain growth or synaptogenesis and vulnerability is generally considered to be prior to 3 years of age in humans, thus the 3rd birthday was mostly adopted demarcation point. Paradoxically, not all studies consistently supported the prevailing view that younger children were at greater risk than senior ones. Findings in three studies observed negative outcomes in older groups, whereas no detrimental effect was detected in younger children, which refuted the previously held assumption that younger age indicated a more vulnerable period.

Anesthetic agents, frequency, and duration of exposure

The neurodevelopmental effects of GA agents are commonly thought to be induced through actions at the GABA-A and/or NMDA receptors, which are the main mechanisms of most currently used GA agents. The GABA system is the main inhibitory neurotransmitter pathway in the central nervous system (CNS) of the mammalian brain, and one-third of all synapses are GABAergic. GABA-A agonists, such as sevoflurane, propofol, benzodiazepines, barbiturates, isoflurane, and halothane, can enhance the fast inhibitory signals through rapid postsynaptic membrane hyperpolarization mediated by GABA-A receptor, which is a ligand-gated chloride channel. Preclinical studies have found the KCC2-dependent developmental increase in the efficacy of GABA-A-mediated inhibition is a major determinant of the age-dependent actions of propofol on dendritic spinogenesis (Puskarjov et al., 2017). Sevoflurane is proven to increase the affinity of GABA to the GABA-AR, and induce a picrotoxin-like open-channel block at the GABA-AR. The reversal of the open-channel block elicits a delayed GABA response (Hapfelmeier et al., 2001). Whereas, the metabotropic GABA-B receptor produces slow and prolonged inhibitory signals as a result of an increase in K+ conductance, or a decrease in voltage-dependent Ca2+ currents or via G proteins and second messengers. It may provide targets for pharmaceutical intervention in areas, such as drug addiction, nociception, and absence of seizures (Ong and Kerr, 2000), while the application of GABA-B inhibitor as an anesthetic is rare. NMDA antagonists bind to NMDA receptors and prevent the binding of glutamate, thereby preventing the release of calcium into the nerve cells. For example, Orser and colleagues found that ketamine blocks the open NMDA channel, thereby reducing channel mean open time, and decreasing the frequency of channel opening by an allosteric mechanism (Orser et al., 1997).

However, in current studies, the lack of detailed information on types and doses of drugs, and the shortage of single-drug studies hinder the investigation into which anesthetics can mostly diminish the potential negative effect. The sole individually studied drug is sevoflurane, which is the most commonly used anesthetic in pediatric surgeries. The six studies utilizing sevoflurane as the sole anesthetic found no negative effect on neurodevelopment, possibly related to the types of surgeries, with five studies including cases with minor surgeries for inguinal hernia, glaucoma, strabismus, and some dental diseases like cleft lip, while only one including multiple types of surgeries. This result might indicate the safety of sevoflurane in children undergoing minor surgeries.

Generally, most studies found a single GA exposure was safe. In the contrast, the majority of studies that included multiple anesthetics found significant changes with multiple exposures, two of which were relatively robust evidence (MASK and PANDA). Though the result in children with short and moderate cumulative duration is ambiguous, it's consistently certified in most studies covering prolonged exposure that a longer duration contributes to a higher risk of long-term diminution of neurocognitive function. However, longer cumulative duration and multiple exposures may be closely linked. Whether the frequency (discrete number of times of exposure) or the duration (the length of a single procedure) leads to the change remains to be further explored.

Surgery procedure

Discrepant surgical procedures not only indicate the comorbidities of children, but also epitomize perioperative physiologic disturbances (i.e., hypotension, hypoxia, and hypercapnia), operational injuries, inflammation, psychologic stresses, the duration of anesthesia, and the prognosis of surgeries. For example, cardiac and neurological surgeries can be associated with severe congenital abnormalities, primary neurocognitive impairment by diseases requiring surgeries, longer duration of the procedure, and worse perioperative conditions, thus resulting in a higher risk of residual cognitive damage. Hearing deficits requiring myringotomy may contribute to LD. Medulloblastoma patients require multi-modal therapy that includes surgery, risk-adapted craniospinal irradiation, and adjuvant chemotherapy. The cumulative anesthetic duration is commonly over 20 h. The combination of such factors can greatly increase the detrimental outcomes. Conversely, shorter and safer procedures like inguinal herniorrhaphy and ocular and dental surgeries have an overtly better neurological prognosis.

Is anesthesia per se the cause of bad neurocognitive outcomes?

As an indispensable part of surgery procedures, assessing the effect of GA independent of surgeries is hard to attain in humans. However, children exposed to LA which is widely considered to be safe for neurological functions can be an ideal control group. Most studies demonstrated equivalence between GA and LA including the GAS study, which suggests that long-term adverse neurocognitive outcomes may not be the consequence of anesthetic agents but rather surgical procedures or other factors.

Future studies

It is still difficult to draw a definitive conclusion about the relationship between anesthesia and neurodevelopment based on the available studies. Large and well-designed cohort studies, with single and multiple anesthesia exposures, consistency in study design, rigorous control for confounders, and strict follow-up, are needed to provide robust evidence and facilitate the synthesis and replication of studies. In the aspect of outcome measure, we need further exploration into a sensitive and stable outcome to estimate the neurocognitive harm in children. Ages should be subdivided into future studies to further detect a potentially existing window of vulnerability. We need more studies comparing the effect of single and repeated exposures, and short and long cumulative duration. Studies involving a single type of surgery, anesthetic drugs, and a better consistency of the surgical procedure are recommended to eliminate underlying confounders (Disma et al., 2018). Patients exposed to LA can be a suitable control group as an attempt to demonstrate whether anesthesia per se is causal. Regarding the limitation of cohort studies, randomized controlled trials like the GAS study are the accepted gold standard for studying causality (Davidson and Sun, 2018). TREX (Trial Remifentanil and dEXmedetomidine), a new multicenter randomized controlled trial, has now started enrollment of infants before their 2nd birthday, who are scheduled to undergo anesthetic exposure of more than 3 h to customary doses of sevoflurane or a dexmedetomidine/remifentanil/low-dose sevoflurane technique, in Europe, Australia, and North America (Soriano and McCann, 2020). At the age of 3, the children will have a neurodevelopmental test to access a mitigation strategy for anesthesia neurotoxicity. This trial may shed light on the remaining questions.

Conclusion

Through a review of available literature, we do find more studies demonstrate multiple anesthetic exposures in early childhood accompanied by an increased risk of neurodevelopmental impairment. However, most of these only show a moderate risk of negative outcomes, with a hazard ratio of no more than 2. In addition, three large-scale studies (GAS, MASK, and PANDA) provide strong evidence that a single exposure of <1 h is not associated with long-term neurodevelopmental abnormalities. Although the current results are reassuring, the potential neurotoxicity of anesthesia procedures cannot be neglected without a decisive conclusion (Hansen, 2015). Out of this concern, for clinical practice, if anesthesia exposure is inevitable, it is necessary to endeavor to limit the duration and number of anesthesia and the dose of anesthetic agents. It is also feasible to consider alternative and mitigating treatments. For future studies, we require cohort studies with rich sources of data and appropriate outcome measures, and carefully designed and adequately powered clinical trials testing plausible interventions in relevant patient populations.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author contributions

AX and YF designed the study, drafted, prepared the tables and figures, and wrote the manuscript. SY, CX, and JC reviewed the articles and assisted in the literature review and preparation of tables and figures. TW and WX revised the manuscript and supervised each step involved in the preparation of the manuscript. All authors have read and agreed to the content of the manuscript.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnmol.2022.972025/full#supplementary-material

References

  1. Alvarado M. C., Murphy K. L., Baxter M. G. (2017). Visual recognition memory is impaired in rhesus monkeys repeatedly exposed to sevoflurane in infancy. Br. J. Anaesth. 119, 517–523. 10.1093/bja/aew473 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Andropoulos D. B., Ahmad H. B., Haq T., Brady K., Stayer S. A., Meador M. R., et al. (2014). The association between brain injury, perioperative anesthetic exposure, and 12-month neurodevelopmental outcomes after neonatal cardiac surgery: a retrospective cohort study. Paediatr. Anaesth. 24, 266–274. 10.1111/pan.12350 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Andropoulos D. B., Greene M. F. (2017). Anesthesia and developing brains - implications of the FDA warning. N. Engl. J. Med. 376, 905–907. 10.1056/NEJMp1700196 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Association American Psychiatric (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. Arlington: American Psychiatric Publishing. [Google Scholar]
  5. Backeljauw B., Holland S. K., Altaye M., Loepke A. W. (2015). Cognition and brain structure following early childhood surgery with anesthesia. Pediatrics 136, e1–e12. 10.1542/peds.2014-3526 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bakri M. H., Ismail E. A., Ali M. S., Elsedfy G. O., Sayed T. A., Ibrahim A. (2015). Behavioral and emotional effects of repeated general anesthesia in young children. Saudi J. Anaesth. 9, 161–166. 10.4103/1658-354X.152843 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Banerjee P., Rossi M. G., Anghelescu D. L., Liu W., Breazeale A. M., Reddick W. E., et al. (2019). Association between anesthesia exposure and neurocognitive and neuroimaging outcomes in long-term survivors of childhood acute lymphoblastic leukemia. JAMA Oncol. 5, 1456–1463. 10.1001/jamaoncol.2019.1094 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bartels M., Althoff R. R., Boomsma D. I. (2009). Anesthesia and cognitive performance in children: no evidence for a causal relationship. Win Res Hum Genet. 12, 246–253. 10.1375/twin.12.3.246 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Barton K., Dielman T. E., Cattell R. B. (1972). Personality and IQ measures as predictors of school achievement. J. Educ. Psychol. 63, 398–404. 10.1037/h0033573 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Birajdar S., Rao S., McMichael J. (2017). Neurodevelopmental outcomes of neonates undergoing surgery under general anesthesia for malrotation of intestines. Early Hum. Dev. 109, 32–36. 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2017.04.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Block R. I., Magnotta V. A., Bayman E. O., Choi J. Y., Thomas J. J., Kimble K. K. (2017). Are anesthesia and surgery during infancy associated with decreased white matter integrity and volume during childhood? Anesthesiology 127, 788–799. 10.1097/ALN.0000000000001808 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Bong C. L., Allen J. C., Kim J. T. (2013). The effects of exposure to general anesthesia in infancy on academic performance at age 12. Anesth Analg. 117, 1419–1428. 10.1213/ANE.0b013e318299a7c2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Brambrink A. M., Evers A. S., Avidan M. S., Farber N. B., Smith D. J., Martin L. D., et al. (2012). Ketamine-induced neuroapoptosis in the fetal and neonatal rhesus macaque brain. Anesthesiology 116, 372–384. 10.1097/ALN.0b013e318242b2cd [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Brévaut-Malaty V., Resseguier N., Garbi A., Tosello B., Thomachot L., Vialet R., et al. (2022). Long term neurodevelopmental outcomes after sevoflurane neonatal exposure of extremely preterm children: a cross-sectional observationnal study. Children 9, 548. 10.3390/children9040548 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Briner A., De Roo M., Dayer A., Muller D., Habre W., Vutskits L. (2010). Volatile anesthetics rapidly increase dendritic spine density in the rat medial prefrontal cortex during synaptogenesis. Anesthesiology 112, 546–556. 10.1097/ALN.0b013e3181cd7942 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Cattell R. B., Barton K., Dielman T. E. (1972). Prediction of school achievement from motivation, personality, and ability measures. Psychol. Rep. 30, 35–43. 10.2466/pr0.1972.30.1.35 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Clausen N. G., Kähler S., Hansen T. G. (2018). Systematic review of the neurocognitive outcomes used in studies of paediatric anaesthesia neurotoxicity. Br. J. Anaesth. 120, 1255–1273. 10.1016/j.bja.2017.11.107 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Clausen N. G., Pedersen D. A., Pedersen J. K., Møller S. E., Grosen D., Wehby G. L., et al. (2017). Oral clefts and academic performance in adolescence: the impact of anesthesia-related neurotoxicity, timing of surgery, and type of oral clefts. Cleft Palate Craniofac. J. 54, 371–380. 10.1597/15-185 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Coleman K., Robertson N. D., Dissen G. A., Neuringer M. D., Martin L. D., Cuzon Carlson V. C., et al. (2017). Isoflurane anesthesia has long-term consequences on motor and behavioral development in infant rhesus macaques. Anesthesiology 126, 74–84. 10.1097/ALN.0000000000001383 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Conrad A. L., Goodwin J. W., Choi J., Block R. I., Nopoulos P. (2017). The relationship of exposure to anesthesia on outcomes in children with isolated oral clefts. J. Child Neurol. 32, 308–315. 10.1177/0883073816681257 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Creeley C., Dikranian K., Dissen G., Martin L., Olney J., Brambrink A. (2013). Propofol-induced apoptosis of neurones and oligodendrocytes in fetal and neonatal rhesus macaque brain. Br. J. Anaesth. 110 (Suppl 1), i29–i38. 10.1093/bja/aet173 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Dalla Massara L., Osuru H. P., Oklopcic A., Milanovic D., Joksimovic S. M., Caputo V., et al. (2016). General anesthesia causes epigenetic histone modulation of c-fos and brain-derived neurotrophic factor, target genes important for neuronal development in the immature rat hippocampus. Anesthesiology 124, 1311–1327. 10.1097/ALN.0000000000001111 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Davidson A. J., Disma N., de Graaff J. C., Withington D. E., Dorris L., Bell G., et al. (2016). Neurodevelopmental outcome at 2 years of age after general anaesthesia and awake-regional anaesthesia in infancy (GAS): an international multicentre, randomised controlled trial. Lancet 387, 239–250. 10.1016/S0140-6736(15)00608-X [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Davidson A. J., Sun L. S. (2018). Clinical evidence for any effect of anesthesia on the developing brain. Anesthesiology 128, 840–853. 10.1097/ALN.0000000000001972 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. de Heer I. J., Tiemeier H., Hoeks S. E., Weber F. (2017). Intelligence quotient scores at the age of 6 years in children anaesthetised before the age of 5 years. Anaesthesia 72, 57–62. 10.1111/anae.13687 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Deary I. J., Penke L., Johnson W. (2010). The neuroscience of human intelligence differences. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 201–211. 10.1038/nrn2793 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Deary I. J., Whalley L. J., Lemmon H., Crawford J. R., Starr J. M. (2000). The stability of individual differences in mental ability from childhood to old age: follow-up of the 1932 Scottish mental survey. Intelligence 28, 49–55. 10.1016/S0160-2896(99)00031-8 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  28. Deng M., Hofacer R. D., Jiang C., Joseph B., Hughes E. A., Jia B., et al. (2014). Brain regional vulnerability to anaesthesia-induced neuroapoptosis shifts with age at exposure and extends into adulthood for some regions. Br. J. Anaesth. 113, 443–451. 10.1093/bja/aet469 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Diaz L. K., Gaynor J. W., Koh S. J., Ittenbach R. F., Gerdes M., Bernbaum J. C., et al. (2016). Increasing cumulative exposure to volatile anesthetic agents is associated with poorer neurodevelopmental outcomes in children with hypoplastic left heart syndrome. J. Thorac. Cardiovasc. Surg. 152, 482–489. 10.1016/j.jtcvs.2016.03.095 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. DiMaggio C., Sun L. S., Kakavouli A., Byrne M. W., Li G. (2009). A retrospective cohort study of the association of anesthesia and hernia repair surgery with behavioral and developmental disorders in young children. Neurosurg. Anesthesiol. 21, 286–291. 10.1097/ANA.0b013e3181a71f11 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. DiMaggio C., Sun L. S., Li G. (2011). Early childhood exposure to anesthesia and risk of developmental and behavioral disorders in a sibling birth cohort. Anesth Analg. 113, 1143–1151. 10.1213/ANE.0b013e3182147f42 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Disma N., O'Leary J. D., Loepke A. W., Brambrink A. M., Becke K., Clausen N. G., et al. (2018). Anesthesia and the developing brain: a way forward for laboratory and clinical research. Paediatr. Anaesth. 28, 758–763. 10.1111/pan.13455 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Djurhuus B. D., Hansen T. G., Pedersen J. K., Faber C. E., Christensen K. (2016). School performance in cholesteatoma-operated children in Denmark: a nationwide population-based register-study. Acta Otolaryngol. 136, 663–668. 10.3109/00016489.2016.1146412 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Doberschuetz N., Dewitz R., Rolle U., Schlösser R., Allendorf A. (2016). Follow-Up of children with gastrointestinal malformations and postnatal surgery and anesthesia: evaluation at two years of age. Neonatology. 110, 8–13. 10.1159/000443873 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Fan Q., Cai Y., Chen K., Li W. (2013). Prognostic study of sevoflurane-based general anesthesia on cognitive function in children. J. Anesth. 27, 493–499. 10.1007/s00540-013-1566-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. FDA Drug Safety Communication. (2022). FDA Review Results in New Warnings About Using General Anesthetics and Sedation Drugs in Young Children and Pregnant Women. FDA Drug Safety Communication.Available online at: https://www.fda.gov/drugs/drug-safety-and-availability/fda-drug-safety-communication-fda-review-results-new-warnings-about-using-general-anesthetics-and [Google Scholar]
  37. Feng Y. P., Yang T. S., Chung C. H., Chien W. C., Wong C. S. (2020). Early childhood general anesthesia exposure associated with later developmental delay: a national population-based cohort study. PLoS ONE 15, e0238289. 10.1371/journal.pone.0238289 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Flick R. P., Katusic S. K., Colligan R. C., Wilder R. T., Voigt R. G., Olson M. D., et al. (2011). Cognitive and behavioral outcomes after early exposure to anesthesia and surgery. Pediatrics 128, e1053–e1061. 10.1542/peds.2011-0351 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Food and Drug Administration Drug Safety Communication (2017). FDA Approves Label Changes for Use of General Anesthetic and Sedation Drugs in Young Children. Food and Drug Administration Drug Safety Communication. Available online at: https://www.fda.gov/Drugs/DrugSafety/ucm554634.htm (accessed November 1, 2017).
  40. Franks N. P., Dickinson R., de Sousa S. L., Hall A. C., Lieb W. R. (1998). How does xenon produce anaesthesia? Nature 396, 324. 10.1038/24525 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Franks N. P., Lieb W. R. (1994). Molecular and cellular mechanisms of general anaesthesia. Nature 367, 607–614. 10.1038/367607a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Gano D., Andersen S. K., Glass H. C., Rogers E. E., Glidden D. V., Barkovich A. J., et al. (2015). Impaired cognitive performance in premature newborns with two or more surgeries prior to term-equivalent age. Pediatr. Res. 78, 323–329. 10.1038/pr.2015.106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Garcia Guerra G., Robertson C. M., Alton G. Y., Joffe A. R., Cave D. A., Yasmin F., et al. (2014). Neurotoxicity of sedative and analgesia drugs in young infants with congenital heart disease: 4-year follow-up. Paediatr. Anaesth. 24, 257–265. 10.1111/pan.12257 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Glatz P., Sandin R. H., Pedersen N. L., Bonamy A. K., Eriksson L. I., Granath F. (2017). Association of anesthesia and surgery during childhood with long-term academic performance. JAMA Pediatr. 171, e163470. 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2016.3470 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Graham M. R., Brownell M., Chateau D. G., Dragan R. D., Burchill C., Fransoo R. R. (2016). Neurodevelopmental assessment in kindergarten in children exposed to general anesthesia before the age of 4 years: a retrospective matched cohort study. Anesthesiology 125, 667–677. 10.1097/ALN.0000000000001245 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Green M. S., Aman M. M., Stevens L., Voralu K., Saththasivam P., Mychaskiw G., et al. (2015). Histopathological observations in the brains of children exposed to inhalational anesthetic agents: a retrospective autopsy-based study. Min. Anestesiol. 81, 1329–1337. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Håkanson C. A., Fredriksson F., Lilja H. E. (2020). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and educational level in adolescent and adult individuals after anesthesia and abdominal surgery during infancy. PLoS ONE 15, e0240891. 10.1371/journal.pone.0240891 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Hansen T. G. (2015). Anesthesia-related neurotoxicity and the developing animal brain is not a significant problem in children. Paediatr. Anaesth. 25, 65–72. 10.1111/pan.12548 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Hansen T. G., Pedersen J. K., Henneberg S. W., Morton N. S., Christensen K. (2013). Educational outcome in adolescence following pyloric stenosis repair before 3 months of age: a nationwide cohort study. Paediatr. Anaesth. 23, 883–890. 10.1111/pan.12225 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Hansen T. G., Pedersen J. K., Henneberg S. W., Morton N. S., Christensen K. (2015). Neurosurgical conditions and procedures in infancy are associated with mortality and academic performances in adolescence: a nationwide cohort study. Paediatr. Anaesth. 25, 186–192. 10.1111/pan.12533 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Hansen T. G., Pedersen J. K., Henneberg S. W., Pedersen D. A., Murray J. C., Morton N. S., et al. (2011). Academic performance in adolescence after inguinal hernia repair in infancy: a nationwide cohort study. Anesthesiology. 114, 1076–1085. 10.1097/ALN.0b013e31820e77a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Hapfelmeier G., Schneck H., Kochs E. (2001). Sevoflurane potentiates and blocks GABA-induced currents through recombinant alpha1beta2gamma2 GABAA receptors: implications for an enhanced GABAergic transmission. Eur. J. Anaesthesiol. 18, 377–383. 10.1046/j.0265-0215.2001.00848.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Honey C. J., Thivierge J. P., Sporns O. (2010). Can structure predict function in the human brain? Neuroimage 52, 766–776. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.071 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Hu D., Flick R. P., Zaccariello M. J., Colligan R. C., Katusic S. K., Schroeder D. R., et al. (2017). Association between exposure of young children to procedures requiring general anesthesia and learning and behavioral outcomes in a population-based birth cohort. Anesthesiology 127, 227–240. 10.1097/ALN.0000000000001735 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Ing C., DiMaggio C., Whitehouse A., Hegarty M. K., Brady J., von Ungern-Sternberg B. S., et al. (2012). Long-term differences in language and cognitive function after childhood exposure to anesthesia. Pediatrics 130, e476–e485. 10.1542/peds.2011-3822 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Ing C., Hegarty M. K., Perkins J. W., Whitehouse A., DiMaggio C. J., Sun M., et al. (2017a). Duration of general anaesthetic exposure in early childhood and long-term language and cognitive ability. Br. J. Anaesth. 119, 532–540. 10.1093/bja/aew413 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Ing C., Jackson W. M., Zaccariello M. J., Goldberg T. E., McCann M. E., Grobler A., et al. (2021). Prospectively assessed neurodevelopmental outcomes in studies of anaesthetic neurotoxicity in children: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Br. J. Anaesth. 126, 433–444. 10.1016/j.bja.2020.10.022 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Ing C., Ma X., Sun M., Lu Y., Wall M. M., Olfson M., et al. (2020). Exposure to surgery and anesthesia in early childhood and subsequent use of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder medications. Anesth Analg. 131, 723–733. 10.1213/ANE.0000000000004619 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Ing C., Sun M., Olfson M., DiMaggio C. J., Sun L. S., Wall M. M., et al. (2017b). Age at exposure to surgery and anesthesia in children and association with mental disorder diagnosis. Anesth Analg. 125, 1988–1998. 10.1213/ANE.0000000000002423 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Ing C., Wall M. M., DiMaggio C. J., Whitehouse A., Hegarty M. K., Sun M., et al. (2017c). Latent class analysis of neurodevelopmental deficit after exposure to anesthesia in early childhood. Neurosurg. Anesthesiol. 29, 264–273. 10.1097/ANA.0000000000000303 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Ing C., Warner D. O., Sun L. S., Flick R. P., Davidson A. J., Vutskits L., et al. (2022). Anesthesia and developing brains: unanswered questions and proposed paths forward. Anesthesiology 136, 500–512. 10.1097/ALN.0000000000004116 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Ing C. H., DiMaggio C. J., Malacova E., Whitehouse A. J., Hegarty M. K., Feng T., et al. (2014). Comparative analysis of outcome measures used in examining neurodevelopmental effects of early childhood anesthesia exposure. Anesthesiology 120, 1319–1332. 10.1097/ALN.0000000000000248 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Jacola L. M., Anghelescu D. L., Hall L., Russell K., Zhang H., Wang F., et al. (2020). Anesthesia exposure during therapy predicts neurocognitive outcomes in survivors of childhood medulloblastoma. J. Pediatr. 223, 141–147.e4. 10.1016/j.jpeds.2020.04.039 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Jevtovic-Todorovic V., Hartman R. E., Izumi Y., Benshoff N. D., Dikranian K., Zorumski C. F., et al. (2003). Early exposure to common anesthetic agents causes widespread neurodegeneration in the developing rat brain and persistent learning deficits. J. Neurosci. 23, 876–882. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-03-00876.2003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Jevtović-Todorović V., Todorović S. M., Mennerick S., Powell S., Dikranian K., Benshoff N., et al. (1998). Nitrous oxide (laughing gas) is an NMDA antagonist, neuroprotectant and neurotoxin. Nat. Med. 4, 460–463. 10.1038/nm0498-460 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Kalkman C. J., Peelen L., Moons K. G., Veenhuizen M., Bruens M., Sinnema G., et al. (2009). Behavior and development in children and age at the time of first anesthetic exposure. Anesthesiology 110, 805–812. 10.1097/ALN.0b013e31819c7124 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Kayaalp L., Bozkurt P., Odabasi G., Dogangun B., Cavusoglu P., Bolat N., et al. (2006). Psychological effects of repeated general anesthesia in children. Paediatr. Anaesth. 16, 822–827. 10.1111/j.1460-9592.2006.01867.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Ko W. R., Huang J. Y., Chiang Y. C., Nfor O. N., Ko P. C., Jan S. R., et al. (2015). Risk of autistic disorder after exposure to general anaesthesia and surgery: a nationwide, retrospective matched cohort study. Eur. J. Anaesthesiol. 32, 303–310. 10.1097/EJA.0000000000000130 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Ko W. R., Liaw Y. P., Huang J. Y., Zhao D. H., Chang H. C., Ko P. C., et al. (2014). Exposure to general anesthesia in early life and the risk of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder development: a nationwide, retrospective matched-cohort study. Paediatr. Anaesth. 24, 741–748. 10.1111/pan.12371 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Kobayashi Y., Tokuda N., Adachi S., Takeshima Y., Hirose M., Shima M., et al. (2020). Association between surgical procedures under general anesthesia in infancy and developmental outcomes at 1 year: the Japan environment and children's study. Environ. Health Prev. Med. 25, 32. 10.1186/s12199-020-00873-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Laidra K., Pullmann H., Allik J. (2007). Personality and intelligence as predictors of academic achievement: a cross-sectional study from elementary to secondary school. Pers. Individ. Dif. 42, 441–451. 10.1016/j.paid.2006.08.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  72. Lap C. C., Bolhuis S. W., Van Braeckel K. N., Reijneveld S. A., Manten G. T., Bos A. F., et al. (2017). Functional outcome at school age of children born with gastroschisis. Early Hum. Dev. 106–107, 47–52. 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2017.01.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Laporta M. L., Sprung J., Fejedelem C. A., Henning D. T., Weaver A. L., Hanson A. C., et al. (2021). Association between exposure of children to general anesthesia and autism spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 10.1007/s10803-021-05305-0. [Epub ahead of print]. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Liu J. R., Yuki K., Baek C., Han X. H., Soriano S. G. (2016). Dexmedetomidine-Induced neuroapoptosis is dependent on its cumulative dose. Anesth Analg. 123, 1008–1017. 10.1213/ANE.0000000000001527 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Lodge D., Anis N. A. (1982). Effects of phencyclidine on excitatory amino acid activation of spinal interneurones in the cat. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 77, 203–204. 10.1016/0014-2999(82)90022-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. McCann M. E., de Graaff J. C., Dorris L., Disma N., Withington D., Bell G., et al. (2019). Neurodevelopmental outcome at 5 years of age after general anaesthesia or awake-regional anaesthesia in infancy (GAS): An international, multicentre, randomised, controlled equivalence trial. Lancet. 393, 664–677. 10.1016/S0140-6736(18)32485-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Mennerick S., Jevtovic-Todorovic V., Todorovic S. M., Shen W., Olney J. W., Zorumski C. F. (1998). Effect of nitrous oxide on excitatory and inhibitory synaptic transmission in hippocampal cultures. J. Neurosci. 18, 9716–9726. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-23-09716.1998 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Morriss F. H., Jr., Saha S., Bell E. F., Colaizy T. T., Stoll B. J., Hintz S. R., et al.(2014). Surgery and neurodevelopmental outcome of very low-birth-weight infants. JAMA Pediatr. 168, 746–754. 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2014.307 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Nestor K. A., Zeidan M., Boncore E., Richardson A., Alex G., Weiss M., et al. (2017). Neurodevelopmental outcomes in infants undergoing general anesthesia. J. Pediatr. Surg. 52, 895–900. 10.1016/j.jpedsurg.2017.03.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Neudecker V., Perez-Zoghbi J. F., Coleman K., Neuringer M., Robertson N., Bemis A., et al. (2021). Infant isoflurane exposure affects social behaviours, but does not impair specific cognitive domains in juvenile non-human primates. Br. J. Anaesth. 126, 486–499. 10.1016/j.bja.2020.10.015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. O'Leary J. D., Janus M., Duku E., Wijeysundera D. N., To T., Li P., et al. (2016). A population-based study evaluating the association between surgery in early life and child development at primary school entry. Anesthesiology 125, 272–279. 10.1097/ALN.0000000000001200 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. O'Leary J. D., Janus M., Duku E., Wijeysundera D. N., To T., Li P., et al. (2019). Influence of surgical procedures and general anesthesia on child development before primary school entry among matched sibling Pairs. JAMA Pediatr. 173, 29–36. 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2018.3662 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Ong J., Kerr D. I. (2000). Recent advances in GABAB receptors: from pharmacology to molecular biology. Acta Pharmacol. Sin. 21, 111–123. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Orser B. A., Pennefather P. S., MacDonald J. F. (1997). Multiple mechanisms of ketamine blockade of N-methyl-D-aspartate receptors. Anesthesiology 86, 903–917. 10.1097/00000542-199704000-00021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Partanen M., Anghelescu D. L., Hall L., Schreiber J. E., Rossi M., Gajjar A., et al. (2021). Longitudinal associations between exposure to anesthesia and neurocognitive functioning in pediatric medulloblastoma. Eur. J. Cancer. 148, 103–111. 10.1016/j.ejca.2021.02.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Paule M. G., Li M., Allen R. R., Liu F., Zou X., Hotchkiss C., et al. (2011). Ketamine anesthesia during the first week of life can cause long-lasting cognitive deficits in rhesus monkeys. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 33, 220–230. 10.1016/j.ntt.2011.01.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Petráčková I., Zach J., Borský J., Cerný M., Hacklová R., Tvrdek M., et al. (2015). Early and late operation of cleft lip and intelligence quotient and psychosocial development in 3-7 years. Early Hum. Dev. 91, 149–152. 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2014.12.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Phillips K. A., Bales K. L., Capitanio J. P., Conley A., Czoty P. W., 't Hart B. A., et al. (2014). Why primate models matter. Am. J. Primatol. 76, 801–827. 10.1002/ajp.22281 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Poor Zamany Nejat Kermany M., Roodneshin F., Ahmadi Dizgah N., Gerami E., Riahi E. (2016). Early childhood exposure to short periods of sevoflurane is not associated with later, lasting cognitive deficits. Paediatr. Anaesth. 26, 1018–1025. 10.1111/pan.12969 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Puskarjov M., Fiumelli H., Briner A., Bodogan T., Demeter K., Lacoh C. M., et al. (2017). K-Cl Cotransporter 2-mediated cl- extrusion determines developmental stage-dependent impact of propofol anesthesia on dendritic spines. Anesthesiology 126, 855–867. 10.1097/ALN.0000000000001587 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Rabbitts J. A., Groenewald C. B. (2020). Epidemiology of pediatric surgery in the United States. Paediatr. Anaesth. 30, 1083–1090. 10.1111/pan.13993 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Raper J., Alvarado M. C., Murphy K. L., Baxter M. G. (2015). Multiple anesthetic exposure in infant monkeys alters emotional reactivity to an acute stressor. Anesthesiology 123, 1084–1092. 10.1097/ALN.0000000000000851 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Raper J., De Biasio J. C., Murphy K. L., Alvarado M. C., Baxter M. G. (2018). Persistent alteration in behavioural reactivity to a mild social stressor in rhesus monkeys repeatedly exposed to sevoflurane in infancy. Br. J. Anaesth. 120, 761–767. 10.1016/j.bja.2018.01.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Satomoto M., Satoh Y., Terui K., Miyao H., Takishima K., Ito M., et al. (2009). Neonatal exposure to sevoflurane induces abnormal social behaviors and deficits in fear conditioning in mice. Anesthesiology 110, 628–637. 10.1097/ALN.0b013e3181974fa2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Schenning K. J., Noguchi K. K., Martin L. D., Manzella F. M., Cabrera O. H., Dissen G. A., et al. (2017). Isoflurane exposure leads to apoptosis of neurons and oligodendrocytes in 20- and 40-day old rhesus macaques. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 60, 63–68. 10.1016/j.ntt.2016.11.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Schneuer F. J., Bentley J. P., Davidson A. J., Holland A. J., Badawi N., Martin A. J., et al. (2018). The impact of general anesthesia on child development and school performance: a population-based study. Paediatr. Anaesth. 28, 528–536. 10.1111/pan.13390 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Schüttler C., Münster T., Gall C., Trollmann R., Schüttler J. (2021). General Anesthesia in the first 36 months of life. Dtsch. Arztebl. Int. 118, 835–841. 10.3238/arztebl.m2021.0355 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Seltzer L., Swartz M. F., Kwon J., Burchfiel J., Cholette J. M., Wang H., et al. (2016). Neurodevelopmental outcomes after neonatal cardiac surgery: role of cortical isoelectric activity. J. Thorac. Cardiovasc. Surg. 151, 1137–1142. 10.1016/j.jtcvs.2015.10.065 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Shi Y., Hanson A. C., Schroeder D. R., Haines K. M., Kirsch A. C., Macoun S., et al. (2022). Longitudinal assessment of cognitive function in young children undergoing general anaesthesia. Br. J. Anaesth. 128, 294–300. 10.1016/j.bja.2021.11.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Shukla A., Chowdhary V. (2019). Neurodevelopmental outcome at 5 years of age after general anaesthesia or awake-regional anaesthesia in infancy (GAS): an international, multicentre, randomised, controlled equivalence trial. Acta Paediatr. 108, 2115–2116. 10.1111/apa.14943 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Soriano S. G., Liu Q., Li J., Liu J. R., Han X. H., Kanter J. L., et al. (2010). Ketamine activates cell cycle signaling and apoptosis in the neonatal rat brain. Anesthesiology 112, 1155–1163. 10.1097/ALN.0b013e3181d3e0c2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Soriano S. G., McCann M. E. (2020). Is anesthesia bad for the brain? Current knowledge on the impact of anesthetics on the developing brain. Anesthesiol Clin. 38, 477–492. 10.1016/j.anclin.2020.05.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Sprung J., Flick R. P., Katusic S. K., Colligan R. C., Barbaresi W. J., Bojani,ć K., et al. (2012). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder after early exposure to procedures requiring general anesthesia. Mayo Clin. Proc. 87, 120–129. 10.1016/j.mayocp.2011.11.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Sun J. J., Zhu C. Y., Jiang H. Y. (2021). Exposure to general anaesthesia in childhood and the subsequent risk of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analysis of cohort studies. Asian J. Psychiatr. 62, 102708. 10.1016/j.ajp.2021.102708 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Sun L. S., Li G., Miller T. L., Salorio C., Byrne M. W., Bellinger D. C., et al. (2016). Association Between a single general anesthesia exposure before age 36 months and neurocognitive outcomes in later childhood. JAMA 315, 2312–2320. 10.1001/jama.2016.6967 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Taghon T. A., Masunga A. N., Small R. H., Kashou N. H. (2015). A comparison of functional magnetic resonance imaging findings in children with and without a history of early exposure to general anesthesia. Paediatr. Anaesth. 25, 239–246. 10.1111/pan.12606 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Talpos J. C., Chelonis J. J., Li M., Hanig J. P., Paule M. G. (2019). Early life exposure to extended general anesthesia with isoflurane and nitrous oxide reduces responsivity on a cognitive test battery in the nonhuman primate. Neurotoxicology 70, 80–90. 10.1016/j.neuro.2018.11.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Terushkin V., Brauer J., Bernstein L., Geronemus R. (2017). Effect of general anesthesia on neurodevelopmental abnormalities in children undergoing treatment of vascular anomalies with laser surgery: a retrospective review. Dermatol. Surg. 43, 534–540. 10.1097/DSS.0000000000001003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Walkden G. J., Gill H., Davies N. M., Peters A. E., Wright I., Pickering A. E. (2020). Early childhood general anesthesia and neurodevelopmental outcomes in the avon longitudinal study of parents and children birth cohort. Anesthesiology 133, 1007–1020. 10.1097/ALN.0000000000003522 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Walsh B. H., Paul R. A., Inder T. E., Shimony J. S., Smyser C. D., Rogers C. E. (2021). Surgery requiring general anesthesia in preterm infants is associated with altered brain volumes at term equivalent age and neurodevelopmental impairment. Pediatr. Res. 89, 1200–1207. 10.1038/s41390-020-1030-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Wang X., Xu Z., Miao C. H. (2014). Current clinical evidence on the effect of general anesthesia on neurodevelopment in children: an updated systematic review with meta-regression. PLoS ONE 9, e85760. 10.1371/journal.pone.0085760 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Warner D. O., Hu D., Zaccariello M. J., Schroeder D. R., Hanson A. C., Wilder R. T., et al. (2021). association between behavioral and learning outcomes and single exposures to procedures requiring general anesthesia before age 3: secondary analysis of data from Olmsted County, MN. Anesth Analg. 133, 160–167. 10.1213/ANE.0000000000005180 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Warner D. O., Zaccariello M. J., Katusic S. K., Schroeder D. R., Hanson A. C., Schulte P. J., et al. (2018). Neuropsychological and behavioral outcomes after exposure of young children to procedures requiring general anesthesia: the mayo anesthesia safety in kids (MASK) study. Anesthesiology 129, 89–105. 10.1097/ALN.0000000000002232 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Wilder R. T., Flick R. P., Sprung J., Katusic S. K., Barbaresi W. J., Mickelson C., et al. (2009). Early exposure to anesthesia and learning disabilities in a population-based birth cohort. Anesthesiology 110, 796–804. 10.1097/01.anes.0000344728.34332.5d [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Yang Y. L., Wang L. J., Chang J. C., Ho S. C., Kuo H. C. (2021). A national population cohort study showed that exposure to general anesthesia in early childhood is associated with an increase in the risk of developmental delay. Children 8, 840. 10.3390/children8100840 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Yazar S., Hewitt A. W., Forward H., Jacques A., Ing C., von Ungern-Sternberg B. S., et al. (2016). Early anesthesia exposure and the effect on visual acuity, refractive error, and retinal nerve fiber layer thickness of young adults. J. Pediatr. 169, 256–259.e1. 10.1016/j.jpeds.2015.10.048 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Yin J., Wang S. L., Liu X. B. (2014). The effects of general anaesthesia on memory in children: a comparison between propofol and sevoflurane. Anaesthesia 69, 118–123. 10.1111/anae.12504 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Young J. T., Vlasova R. M., Howell B. R., Knickmeyer R. C., Morin E., Kuitchoua K. I., et al. (2021). General anaesthesia during infancy reduces white matter micro-organisation in developing rhesus monkeys. Br. J. Anaesth. 126, 845–853. 10.1016/j.bja.2020.12.029 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Zaccariello M. J., Frank R. D., Lee M., Kirsch A. C., Schroeder D. R., Hanson A. C., et al. (2019). Patterns of neuropsychological changes after general anaesthesia in young children: secondary analysis of the mayo anesthesia safety in kids study. Br. J. Anaesth. 122, 671–681. 10.1016/j.bja.2019.01.022 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Zhang H., Du L., Du Z., Jiang H., Han D., Li Q. (2015). Association between childhood exposure to single general anesthesia and neurodevelopment: a systematic review and meta-analysis of cohort study. J. Anesth. 29, 749–757. 10.1007/s00540-015-2030-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Zhang Q., Peng Y., Wang Y. (2017). Long-duration general anesthesia influences the intelligence of school age children. BMC Anesthesiol. 17, 170. 10.1186/s12871-017-0462-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Zhao W., Song S., Chu W., Li Y., Chen S., Ji Y., et al. (2022). Disruption of hippocampal P2RX2/CaMKII/NF-κB signaling contributes to learning and memory impairment in C57BL/6 mice induced by surgery plus anesthesia in neonatal period. Biomed. Pharmacother. 149, 112897. 10.1016/j.biopha.2022.112897 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Zhou P., Zhang C., Huang G., Hu Y., Ma W., Yu C. (2021). The effect of sevoflurane anesthesia for dental procedure on neurocognition in children: a prospective, equivalence, controlled trial. BMC Pediatr. 21, 177. 10.1186/s12887-021-02649-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Zuo C., Ma J., Pan Y., Zheng D., Chen C., Ruan N., et al. (2022). Isoflurane and sevoflurane induce cognitive impairment in neonatal rats by inhibiting neural stem cell development through microglial activation, neuroinflammation, and suppression of VEGFR2 signaling pathway. Neurotox. Res. 40, 775–790. 10.1007/s12640-022-00511-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.


Articles from Frontiers in Molecular Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Frontiers Media SA

RESOURCES