Abstract
Long-lasting fear-related disorders depend on the excessive retention of traumatic fear memory. We previously showed that the palmitoylation-dependent removal of synaptic α-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazole propionate (AMPA) receptors prevents hyperexcitation-based epileptic seizures and that AMPA receptor palmitoylation maintains neural network stability. In this study, AMPA receptor subunit GluA1 C-terminal palmitoylation-deficient (GluA1C811S) mice were subjected to comprehensive behavioral battery tests to further examine whether the mutation causes other neuropsychiatric disease-like symptoms. The behavioral analyses revealed that palmitoylation-deficiency in GluA1 is responsible for characteristic prolonged contextual fear memory formation, whereas GluA1C811S mice showed no impairment of anxiety-like behaviors at the basal state. In addition, fear generalization gradually increased in these mutant mice without affecting their cued fear. Furthermore, fear extinction training by repeated exposure of mice to conditioned stimuli had little effect on GluA1C811S mice, which is in line with augmentation of synaptic transmission in pyramidal neurons in the basolateral amygdala. In contrast, locomotion, sociability, depression-related behaviors, and spatial learning and memory were unaffected by the GluA1 non-palmitoylation mutation. These results indicate that impairment of AMPA receptor palmitoylation specifically causes posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD)-like symptoms.
Subject terms: Post-traumatic stress disorder, Trauma
Introduction
While fear memory is effective in avoiding dangerous situations, which promotes survival in complex and dynamically changing environments, fear extinction processes need to occur in parallel to suppress excessive fear and anxiety [1–3]. Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is a mental and behavioral disorder that is triggered by either experiencing or witnessing a traumatic event. Recollection of terrifying fear usually occurs when patients are exposed to incentives. Failure of appropriate fear reduction leads to PTSD and afflicts a person throughout their life span. Uncontrollable augmentation of synaptic transmission in neuronal fear pathways has been hypothesized to be involved in anxiety-related disorders including PTSD and impair the regulation of excitatory synapses in fear-related amygdala regions; their interactions with the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex (PFC) may play a role in the pathophysiology of PTSD [4, 5].
Glutamate is the major excitatory neurotransmitter in the mammalian central nervous system. The expression of postsynaptic α-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazole propionate (AMPA)-type ionotropic glutamate receptors (AMPA receptors) is closely linked to excitatory synaptic strength [6, 7]. Therefore, the quantitative control of synaptic AMPA receptor numbers is critical for basal synaptic transmission, synaptic plasticity, and higher brain function [7–10]. Among the four AMPA receptor subunits (GluA1, 2, 3, and 4, also known as GluR1-4, GluRA-D, or GluRα1–4), GluA1 has a dominant role during activity-dependent AMPA receptor insertion into synapses [11]. AMPA receptor trafficking to and from synapses is dynamically regulated by post-translational protein modifications such as phosphorylation [11–13]. In these processes, AMPA receptor phosphorylation reversibly modulates the properties of AMPA receptor ion channels and membrane trafficking of AMPA receptors to the postsynaptic membrane [7, 14–16]. Previous studies have shown that phosphorylation of AMPA receptors regulates various forms of fear memory [17, 18].
Another key modification of AMPA receptors is reversible S-palmitoylation, the covalent attachment of palmitic acid to intracellular cysteine residues via thioester bonds [19–22]. Generally, palmitoylation acts as a sticky tag that can direct proteins, including many neuronal receptors and ion channels, to specific regions on the plasma membrane or specific intracellular membranes or vesicles [23–25]. We have previously reported that palmitoylation regulates the synaptic expression of AMPA receptors [16, 26–30]. All mammalian AMPA receptor subunits, GluA1-4, are palmitoylated at their C-terminal conserved region in an activity-dependent manner [24, 26, 31, 32]. Palmitoylation inhibits GluA1 interaction with the postsynaptic 4.1N protein, which stabilizes synaptic AMPA receptor expression in long-term potentiation (LTP) [26, 27, 33]. We recently generated mice lacking the palmitoylation site of GluA1 at Cys811 by substituting with Ser (GluA1C811S) and demonstrated that a deficiency in GluA1 palmitoylation enhanced seizure susceptibility and robust LTP-induced spine enlargement without affecting gross brain structure and normal excitatory synaptic transmission [34, 35]. Furthermore, the mutation at the GluA1 palmitoylation site induces hyperexcitation-based epileptic seizures, and the anticonvulsive effects of clinically used antiepileptic drugs were reduced, which suppressed excess excitation [36]. Our findings indicate that an abnormality in palmitoylation-dependent regulation of the AMPA receptor may lead to hyperexcitability, which weakens the maintenance of network stability throughout the brain. In summary, palmitoylation appears uniquely suited to create dynamic quantitative control of synaptic receptor numbers and intracellular trafficking of AMPA receptors, which are associated with complex neuronal events [37].
Here, we further analyzed GluA1C811S knock-in mice on a pure C57BL/6N genetic background to examine whether the palmitoylation-deficient mutation causes other neuropsychiatric disease-like symptoms. The effects of palmitoylation site ablation on behavior were examined using a comprehensive behavioral test battery. The results revealed several characteristic features of GluA1C811S mice, including prolonged fear memory, whereas they showed no significant alteration of anxiety-like behaviors at the basal state.
Materials and methods
Animals and design of behavioral experiments
GluA1C811S mutant mice were backcrossed into the C57BL/6N strain (Charles River Laboratories Japan, Inc.) at least five times. The GluA1C811S allele was identified by PCR, as previously described [34]. The intercross of heterozygotes resulted in the production of wild-type (wt), heterozygous, and homozygous offspring at the expected 1:2:1 Mendelian ratio. Only male mice were used for the subsequent behavioral analyses.
We prepared two independent groups of mice for behavioral battery testing. All behavioral tests were carried out with male mice that were 10–12 (first group) or 28–34 (second group) weeks old at the start of testing. Dubious differences observed in the first group were double-checked using the second group. Different age groups were used to confirm that these behavioral changes were induced by GluA1 palmitoylation deficiency regardless of age. Male mice were housed in groups of four (two pairs of wt and GluA1C811S knock-in mice) per standard animal cage in a room under a 12-h light/dark cycle with access to standard laboratory chow and water ad libitum. All experimental procedures, except measurements of body weight and body temperature, were performed in a soundproof room. Prior to all experiments, the mice were left undisturbed in the testing room for at least 30 min to allow for acclimation. The order of the tests is listed in Table 1. Our serial behavioral tests have been designed from least to most invasive and from less to most burdened with recovery time between tests to decrease the chance that behavioral responses are influenced by prior test history [38–43]. Each behavioral test was separated from the next one by at least 1 day. After each test, the entire apparatus was cleaned with a diluted sodium hypochlorite solution to prevent bias due to olfactory cues. All behavioral tests were conducted as previously described [44–47]. Even a little suspicious phenotypes observed in first group were reconfirmed by testing in second group.
Table 1.
1st group (wild-type, n = 20; GluA1C811S, n = 20) | 2nd group (wild-type, n = 17; GluA1C811S, n = 19) | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Test | Age (weeks old) | Days | Results | Test | Age (weeks old) | Days | Results |
GHNS | 10–12 | 1–2 | GHNS | 28–34 | 1–2 | Table 2 | |
LD | 10–12 | 3 | LD | 28–34 | 3 | Fig. 1A | |
OF | 11–13 | 7 |
Fig. 1B Supplementary Fig. 2A |
HP | 28–34 | 4 | Table 2 |
EP | 11–13 | 8 | Fig. 1C | RR | 28–35 | 8 | Supplementary Fig. 1A |
HP | 11–13 | 9 | PPI | 29–36 | 14 | Table 2 | |
SI | 11–13 | 10 | Supplementary Fig. 3A | PS | 29–36 | 15–16 | Supplementary Fig. 4A |
RR | 12–14 | 15 | |||||
CSI | 13–15 | 21 | Supplementary Fig. 3B | ||||
PS | 13–15 | 24 | |||||
GA | 14–16 | 30 | Supplementary Fig. 1C | ||||
HP (2nd) | 14–16 | 31 | |||||
BM | 16–23 | 46–88 | Supplementary Fig. 5A | ||||
NIH | 23–25 | 102 | Fig. 1D | ||||
BT | 24–26 | 110 | Supplementary Fig. 1B | ||||
TM-SA | 25–27 | 117 | Supplementary Fig. 5B | ||||
PaS | 26–28 | 122 | Supplementary Fig. 5C | ||||
TS | 26–28 | 123 | Supplementary Fig. 4B | ||||
Obj Res | 27–29 | 128 | |||||
Obj Rec | 28–30 | 133 | |||||
PPI | 28–30 | 134–135 | |||||
FZ | 28–35 | 136–170 | Fig. 2AB | ||||
FE | 35–51 | 186–275 | Fig. 2CD Supplementary Fig. 6 | ||||
OF (2nd) | 38–40 | 196 | |||||
HCSI | 38–41 | 199–207 | Supplementary Fig. 3C |
The order of tests was as follows: first group (wild-type, n = 20; GluA1C811S, n = 20): general health and neurological screen (GH), neuromuscular strength examination (NS), light/dark transition test (LD), open field test (OF), elevated plus maze test (EP), hot plate test (HP), social interaction test in a novel environment (SI), rotarod test (RR), Crawley’s sociability and preference for social novelty (three-chamber) test (CSI), Porsolt forced swim test (PS), gait analysis (GA), 2nd hot plate test (HP), Barnes maze test (BM), Novelty-induced hypophagia test (NIH), beam test (BT), T-maze spontaneous alteration test (TM-SA), pattern separation test (PaS), tail suspension test (TS), object reaction response test (Obj Res), object recognition and object recency test (Obj Rec), startle response/prepulse inhibition test (PPI), contextual and cued fear conditioning test (FZ), fear erase test (FE), 2nd open field test (OF), and social interaction in home cage (HCSI); second group (wild-type, n = 17; GluA1C811S, n = 19): general health and neurological screen (GH), neuromuscular strength examination (NS), light/dark transition test (LD), hot plate test (HP), rotarod test (RR), startle response/prepulse inhibition test (PPI), and Porsolt forced swim test (PS).
All animal care procedures and experiments were performed in accordance with the regulations and institutional guidelines of the National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry (NCNP), National Institute for Physiological Sciences (NIPS), and National Institute of Advanced Industrial Science and Technology (AIST). The technical protocols for animal experiments in this study were approved by the Animal Care and Use Committees of NCNP, NIPS, and AIST. Raw data from the behavioral tests, the date on which each experiment was performed, and the age of each mouse at the time of the experiment are available from the Mouse Phenotype Database (http://www.mouse-phenotype.org/). The detailed protocols of the comprehensive behavioral battery tests are provided in the Supplementary Information.
Electrophysiology
Preparation of basolateral amygdala (BLA) slices and whole-cell recordings were performed as described previously with minor modifications [34, 35]. AMPA/NMDA ratios were calculated as the ratio of the peak AMPAR-current at −70 mV to the NMDAR-current 80 ms after stimulus onset at +40 mV. The detailed protocols are provided in the Supplementary Information.
Biochemical analysis
Palmitoylation of GluA1 protein was assessed using the acyl-biotinyl exchange (ABE) method as described previously [34]. The detailed protocols are provided in the Supplementary Information.
Statistical analysis
The analysis was conducted using StatView (SAS Institute, Cary, NC, USA) or SPSS (IBM, Chicago, IL, USA). Data were analyzed by one-way ANOVA followed by Tukey’s test, two-way repeated measures ANOVA followed by Fisher’s LSD test, ANCOVA, MANOVA, Student’s t-tests, paired t-tests, Mann-Whitney’s U test, or log-rank test. Statistical significance was set at p < 0.05.
Results
Normal appearance of GluA1C811S mutant mice
We subjected homozygotes of GluA1C811S mice and their wt littermates to a comprehensive battery of behavioral tests to evaluate the behavioral effects of deficiency of C-terminal palmitoylation of GluA1 (Table 1) [48]. As we previously reported [34], GluA1C811S mice appeared healthy and showed no obvious differences in their physical characteristics (Table 2). There were no significant differences between the genotypes in body weight, neuromuscular strength, startle response, prepulse inhibition, or pain sensitivity (Table 2). Non-palmitoylation C811S mutation in GluA1 did not seriously affect locomotor ability (Supplementary Fig. 1A, B), with mild changes in bowlegged-walking habits in GluA1C811S mice (Supplementary Fig. 1C). In accordance with our previous observation [34], there were also no significant differences between wt and GluA1C811S mice at the basal level without intense shock. Furthermore, GluA1C811S mice showed normal sociability (Supplementary Fig. 3, see details below), normal spatial memory, and normal pattern separation (Supplementary Fig. 5, see details below).
Table 2.
Test | wild-type | GluA1C811S | p value | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Physical characterization | body weight (g) | 43.6 ± 1.1 | 43.4 ± 0.9 | 0.91 | ||
rectal temperature (°C) | 34.3 ± 0.2 | 34.1 ± 0.2 | 0.46 | |||
Neuromuscular strength | grip strength (N) | 0.68 ± 0.03 | 0.63 ± 0.03 | 0.26 | ||
wire hang (s) | 4.84 ± 0.54 | 4.02 ± 1.04 | 0.50 | |||
Sensory function | acoustic startle response (a.u.) | 110 dB | 0.76 ± 0.10 | 0.95 ± 0.13 | 0.50 | |
120 dB | 1.01 ± 0.12 | 1.05 ± 0.13 | ||||
prepulse inhibition (%) | 74–110 dB | 41.0 ± 8.0 | 42.7 ± 6.6 | 0.83 | ||
78–110 dB | 60.0 ± 5.3 | 61.5 ± 3.8 | ||||
74–120 dB | 31.4 ± 6.2 | 26.5 ± 7.9 | 0.51 | |||
78–120 dB | 55.2 ± 4.8 | 49.9 ± 5.4 | ||||
Hot plate | latency to avoid (s) | 7.24 ± 0.58 | 7.94 ± 0.64 | 0.43 |
The p values represent the genotype effect in the ANOVA. All values are represented as the mean ± SEM.
Unaffected anxiety-like behaviors in GluA1C811S mutant mice
We then performed a series of tests on anxiety-like behaviors at the basal level [49, 50]. In the light/dark transition test, there were no significant differences between the genotypes in distance traveled (Fig. 1A1; F1, 34 = 0.546, p = 0.4651), time spent in the light chamber (Fig. 1A2; F1, 34 = 0.019, p = 0.8914), number of transitions between chambers (Fig. 1A3; F1, 34 = 0.717, p = 0.4031), and first latency to enter the light chamber (Fig. 1A4; F1, 34 = 0.931, p = 0.3413). Spontaneous locomotor activity was examined using an open field test (Fig. 1B). No obvious differences were observed between genotypes in horizontal activity (Fig. 1B1; F1, 38 = 0.001, p = 0.9781), time spent in the center area (Fig. 1B2; F1, 38 = 1.107, p = 0.2994), vertical activity (Supplementary Fig. 2A1; F1, 38 = 0.169, p = 0.683), and stereotypic behaviors (Supplementary Fig. 2A2; F1, 38 = 0.137, p = 0.713). In the elevated plus maze test, the GluA1C811S mice’s behavior was similar to that of their wt littermates in number of entries into the arms (Fig. 1C1; F1, 38 = 1.251, p = 0.2703), percentage of entries into the open arms (Fig. 1C2; F1, 38 = 2.000, p = 0.1655), distance traveled (Fig. 1C3; F1, 38 = 0.814, p = 0.3725), and percentage of time spent in the open arms (Fig. 1C4; F1, 38 = 0.736, p = 0.3964). In the novelty-induced hypophagia test, the latency to begin drinking water in a novel cage was greater than that in the home cage in both genotypes (Fig. 1D1; control: p = 0.0346, mutant: p = 0.0054), and the fold change in consumption was larger in GluA1C811S mice than in wt mice (Fig. 1D2; p = 0.00156 and Fig. 1D3; p = 0.0235, log-rank test). Consumption in the novel cage was lower than that in the home cage in both genotypes (Fig. 1D4; control: p = 0.0001, mutant: p < 0.0001), but no significant difference was observed in the fold change of consumption (Fig. 1D5; p = 0.5967). These results suggest that anxiety-like behavior is not affected by Cys to Ser non-palmitoylation mutation in GluA1 in daily activities.
Normal social behaviors in GluA1C811S mutant mice
In the social interaction test conducted in a novel environment, the total duration of contacts (Supplementary Fig. 3A1; F1, 18 = 0.246, p = 0.6261), number of contacts (Supplementary Fig. 3A2; F1, 18 = 0.002, p = 0.962), total duration of active contacts (Supplementary Fig. 3A3; F1, 18 = 0.005, p = 0.9417), mean duration per contact (Supplementary Fig. 3A4; F1, 18 = 0.991, p = 0.3327), and distance traveled (Supplementary Fig. 3A5; F1, 18 = 0.112, p = 0.7415) did not differ between genotypes. In Crawley’s sociability and preference for social novelty test (three-chamber test), we did not find any differences between the genotypes in the sociability indices (Supplementary Fig. 3B1 left: ratio of stay time; F1, 38 = 0.045, p = 0.8339, right: distance traveled; F1, 38 = 0.872, p = 0.3563), and the social novelty preference test (Supplementary Fig. 3B2 left: ratio of stay time; F1, 38 = 0.026, p = 0.8718, right: distance traveled; F1, 38 = 0.021, p = 0.886). We also monitored social interactions in the home cage under familiar conditions over a 7-day period. In the social interaction test in the home cage, time spent separated usually increases when mice are active and decreases when mice are sleeping. There were no significant differences between the genotypes in the mean number of detected particles (Supplementary Fig. 3C1 top; night period: F1, 15 = 0.565, p = 0.464, day period: F1, 15 = 0.02, p = 0.8897, total: F1, 15 = 0.234, p = 0.6359), indicating that GluA1C811S mice displayed normal social interaction behavior in their home cages. Locomotor activity in the home cage did also not differ between the genotypes (Supplementary Fig. 3C1 bottom; night period: F1, 15 = 0.009, p = 0.9274; day period: F1, 15 = 0.033, p = 0.8573, total: F1, 15 = 0.017, p = 0.8967). The average 3-day moving pattern (days 3–5), is depicted in Supplementary Fig. 3C2. There were no significant differences between the genotypes in the mean number of particles (Supplementary Fig. 3C2 top; night period: F1, 15 = 1.181, p = 0.2943, day period: F1, 15 = 0.154, p = 0.7003, total: F1, 15 = 0.189, p = 0.6698) or locomotor activity in the home cage (Supplementary Fig. 3C2 bottom; night period: F1, 15 = 0.11, p = 0. 7444; day period: F1, 15 = 0.308, p = 0.5869, total: F1, 15 = 0.004, p = 0.951).
Unaffected depression-related behaviors in GluA1C811S mutant mice
Two types of experiments related to depression-related behaviors were conducted. GluA1C811S mice showed immobility similar to wt mice in inescapable stressful environments (Supplementary Fig. 4). In the Porsolt forced swim test, there were no significant differences between the genotypes in immobility (Supplementary Fig. 4A, top: F1, 34 = 0.121, p = 0.7299 on the first day; F1, 34 = 1.791, p = 0.1896 on the second day) or distance traveled (Supplementary Fig. 4A, bottom; F1, 34 = 0.927, p = 0.3424 on the first day; F1, 34 = 0.731, p = 0.3984 on the second day). In the tail suspension test, GluA1C811S mice showed immobility results that were similar to those of their wt littermates (Supplementary Fig. 4B; F1, 37 = 0.788, p = 0.3805).
Normal spatial learning and memory in GluA1C811S mutant mice
Concerning learning and memory, we first examined spatial reference memory using the Barnes maze test and spatial working memory using the T-maze test (Supplementary Fig. 5A, B). In the Barnes circular maze, there was no significant effect of genotype on the number of search errors made during acquisition (Supplementary Fig. 5A1 left; F1, 38 = 2.004, p = 0.1651) or the latency to find the target hole (Supplementary Fig. 5A1 right; F1, 38 = 0.13, p = 0.7208), indicating normal acquisition of spatial reference memory in GluA1C811S mice. Probe trials in which the escape box was removed were performed 1 day (1st test) and 30 days (2nd test) after the last day of training. During the probe trial, both genotypes showed a significant effect of hole location both in the 1st and 2nd tests, indicating that both genotypes recalled the location of the target. There were no significant differences between the genotypes in the time spent around the target during the 1st (Supplementary Fig. 5A2; F1, 38 = 4.27, p = 0.0457) and 2nd tests (Supplementary Fig. 5A3; F1, 38 = 2.738, p = 0.1062). The results of the probe trials suggest that GluA1C811S mice have intact consolidation or retention of spatial reference memory.
We then examined behavioral flexibility using reversal tasks. The mice were trained for an additional 4 days after the 2nd probe test. The target was then moved to the opposite site. During the reversal training, there was no significant difference in the number of errors (Supplementary Fig. 5A4 left; F1, 38 = 0.339, p = 0.5636), whereas the latency to find the target hole was slightly larger in GluA1C811S mice than in wt mice (Supplementary Fig. 5A4 right; F1, 38 = 1.909, p = 0.1752). In the probe test after the reversal training, both wt and GluA1C811S mice spent a similar time around the target hole (Supplementary Fig. 5A5; p = 0.1468, one-way ANOVA). Thus, GluA1C811S mice exhibited comparable behavioral flexibility to wt mice.
In the T-maze spontaneous alternation task, both wt and GluA1C811S mice showed a similar performance (Supplementary Fig. 5B; F1, 38 = 0.416, p = 0.5229).
Pattern separation ability was also examined using the non-associative place-learning test. Both genotypes showed significantly reduced motility in the combination of the pattern (Supplementary Fig. 5C; F3, 36 = 10.458, p < 0.0001 for wt, F3, 36 = 6.439, p = 0.0005 for GluA1C811S), while there was no significant reduction in the different-combination groups (Supplementary Fig. 5C; p = 0.1116 for wt, p = 0.1353 for GluA1C811S). The results suggest that both genotypes can similarly distinguish pattern differences.
Enhanced acquisition of contextual, but not cued, fear memory long-lasting in GluA1C811S mutant mice
Finally, we examined the fear memory of GluA1C811S mice using contextual and cued fear conditioning tests. During the conditioning period, freezing behavior before the first presentation of cue-shock pairings was minimal and did not differ between wt and GluA1C811S mice. After footshocks, freezing responses of both genotypes were similarly increased (genotype effect, F1, 38 = 1.835, p = 0.1836; genotype × time effect, F7, 266 = 2.528, p = 0.0156) (Fig. 2A, left). Correspondingly, both genotypes showed similar moving patterns after each shock (Fig. 2B) and comparable pain sensitivity (Table 2, hot plate test). One day after conditioning, the freezing level of GluA1C811S mice was significantly higher than that exhibited by wt mice in the contextual test (Fig. 2A, middle; genotype effect, F1, 38 = 5.532, p = 0.024). In contrast, there were no differences between genotypes in cue (tone)-dependent (auditory) fear conditioning (Fig. 2A, right; genotype effect, F1, 38 = 0.052, p = 0.8213, 1–3 min; genotype effect, F1, 38 = 0.54, p = 0.4668, 4–6 min).
Thirty-five or 49 days after conditioning, contextual fear memory scores were still significantly higher in GluA1C811S mice than in wt mice (genotype effect, F1, 38 = 13.644, p = 0.0007 at 35 days, F1, 38 = 30.952, p < 0.0001 at 49 days). The contextual fear enhancement in GluA1C811S mice was still observed 4 months later, even after standard fear extinction re-exposure training (genotype effect, F1, 38 = 18.872, p = 0.0001 at 125 days) (Fig. 2C). Concerning cued fear memory, GluA1C811S mice showed an enhancement of freezing responses at 35 days, just induced by transferring them from the home cage, even though fear acquisition was not influenced by GluA1 C-terminal palmitoylation. Interestingly, this generalized fear response sustained for 140 days, even after fear extinction training (Fig. 2D and Supplementary Fig. 6). These results demonstrate the strong formation of fear generalization in GluA1C811S mice [51, 52].
Impaired extinction and elevated excitation in the BLA of GluA1C811S mutant mice
After a comprehensive battery of behavioral tests, we further investigated the basis of the poor extinction of contextual fear memory in GluA1C811S mice [53–55]. Repeated exposure of wt mice to the conditioned chamber for 10 min at 24, 48, and 72 h after the footshock gradually decreased freezing rates, but these extinction procedures had less effect on GluA1C811S mice (Fig. 3A). The differences were remarkable during the first 5 min. Shorter exposure to the conditioned chamber for 3 min had little effect on either genotype (Fig. 3B).
As for the contextual fear memory-related neural circuit, the C811S non-palmitoylation mutation in GluA1 led to increase the ratio of AMPA receptor- to NMDA receptor-mediated synaptic currents in BLA pyramidal neurons (Fig. 3C). We then biochemically confirmed the mutation of the palmitoylation site by an acyl-biotinyl exchange (ABE) assay using anti-GluA1 antibodies. Decreased levels of GluA1 palmitoylation were found in the amygdala slices from GluA1C811S mice (42.2 ± 11.4%, compared to wt control, n = 3, respectively; p < 0.01; t-test; Supplementary Fig. 7A). The residual signals likely represented the palmitoylation at another site, Cys585 on transmembrane domain (TMD) 2, which regulates the AMPA receptor localization in the Golgi apparatus, not synaptic membrane trafficking, and is intact in GluA1C811S mice [26, 31]. Along with that, GluA1 expression in postsynaptic density (PSD) fraction was enhanced in the amygdala of GluA1C811S mice (148.0 ± 6.1%, compared to wt control, n = 4, respectively; p < 0.01; t test; Supplementary Fig. 7B), whereas total GluA1 protein amount showed similar level with wt mice (Supplementary Fig. 7B).
Discussion
Although the AMPA receptor GluA1 subunit is widely expressed in excitatory synapses throughout the brain, we found the influence of its C811S non-palmitoylation mutation on behavior after intense shock to be quite limited. Our comprehensive behavioral battery tests show that general health, sensitivity, locomotion, sociability related to autism spectrum disorder, depression-related behaviors, and spatial learning and memory are unaffected by the C811S mutation in GluA1. On the other hand, increased formation (Fig. 2A) and poor extinction (Fig. 2C) of contextual fear and enhancement of fear generalization (Fig. 2D) were observed in GluA1C811S mice despite their normal anxiety-like behaviors at the basal level (Fig. 1). In contrast to contextual fear, cued fear was not influenced by GluA1 palmitoylation (Fig. 2A, D). Previous behavioral study revealed that both contextual and auditory cued fear conditioning are especially resistant to test order [38]. Decreased palmitoylation of GluA1 in the amygdala of GluA1C811S mice corresponds exactly to our behavioral results (Supplementary Fig. 7A). Enormous cellular heterogeneity is known in complicated structure of amygdala, which comprises 13 or more subnuclei including the basal and lateral subregions, known as the BLA [56–58]. In addition to amygdala, fear memory is regulated by its excitatory and inhibitory connections among PFC, hippocampus, and thalamus [53, 59–61]. In the current study, we performed slice patch-clamp recordings from BLA pyramidal neurons and found hyperexcitability of glutamatergic synapses in the BLA of GluA1C811S mice (Fig. 3C). The BLA is mainly involved in the fear extinction pathway that originates from the infralimbic cortex in the medial PFC [62–65]. These augmentations suggest that contextual fear extinction is notably regulated by GluA1 palmitoylation in the postsynapses in BLA [56]. In contrast, we have previously shown that there is no significant difference in the ratio of AMPA receptor- to NMDA receptor-mediated synaptic currents in hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons between wt and GluA1C811S mice [34]. Our results revealed a specific role of the AMPA receptor palmitoylation-mediated reduction of contextual fear in BLA. Moreover, biochemical results of reduced GluA1 palmitoylation and increased synaptic expression of GluA1 in PSD of the amygdala (Supplementary Fig. 7A, B), which should enhance AMPA receptor synaptic retention as well as synaptic plasticity [26, 27, 32, 34], explain behavioral alterations well. Cue (tone)-dependent (auditory) fear conditioning is mediated by the potentiation of glutamatergic synaptic transmission in the lateral amygdala [66, 67]. Fear generalization is mediated by coordinated actions of the PFC, hippocampus, amygdala, and thalamus [52]. Freezing response was increased in palmitoylation-deficient GluA1C811S mice even by transferring from home cage to extraordinary chamber without being affected by tone cue. This phenomenon suggests that mice choose safer way to avoid potential threats in AMPA receptor palmitoylation-dependent manner, which seems to be related to primate fear generalization based on negative experience [51].
PTSD is triggered by brief re-exposure to sights, sounds, smells, or thoughts which remind patient of the traumatic event. Previous studies showed that PTSD involves an impairment of fear extinction [4, 68]. Actual or imaginal prolonged exposure to traumatic cues is employed to induce habituation in the psychotherapy for PTSD, known as exposure therapy and cognitive behavioral therapy [69–71]. Epidemiological investigations show that females are more likely to be affected by PTSD than males [72–74]. Influence of AMPA receptor palmitoylation deficiency on long-lasting fear, which was experimentally observed even in palmitoylation-deficient male mice as mentioned above, may be more serious in female. The sexual differences in AMPA receptor palmitoylation-related fear should be further investigated in the future. Even though GluA1C811S mice were repeatedly re-exposed to the contextual environments, such extinction training had little effect on contextual fear in the case of late-start intervention (Fig. 2C). These results indicate that palmitoylation of the AMPA receptor is indispensable to reduce contextual fear at a very early stage. Extinction training just after fear memory formation is thought to be crucial for effective treatment of human PTSD [70, 71]. In the current study, similar decreases in freezing rates were observed only in wt, not in GluA1C811S, mice after long exposure every day (Fig. 3A). Consistent with human therapeutic accumulations and our previous reports [54, 55], shorter exposure had little effect (Fig. 3B). Taken together, appropriate regulation of GluA1 palmitoylation in pyramidal neurons in the BLA soon after initial traumatic event is necessary to suppress long-term excessive fear, which may play an important role in preventing PTSD.
Supplementary information
Acknowledgements
We are grateful to Ms. K. Takamatsu, S. Furukawa, M. Hyodo, and A. Hayashi for excellent administrative and technical assistance in NIPS and thank colleagues in NCNP and AIST, Dr. K. Yamamoto and Ms. M. Date for animal care, Ms. A. Takayama, J. Sakawa, A. Tsuzuki, A. Yanai and N. Kawakami for excellent administrative assistance.
Author contributions
KT, MS, HO, TM, and TH designed research and experiments; AI, DY, MI, YK, MA, RN, MK, TA, TK, NS, and TH performed experiments and analyzed data; KT, MS, HO, TM, and TH wrote the manuscript with contributions from all of the other authors. KS, KW, and MM were involved with project conceptualization.
Funding
This work was supported in part by the Grants-in-Aid from the Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology of Japan (MEXT)/Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (JSPS) (Grant numbers 16K07078 to TH; 16H06276 Platform of Advanced Animal Model Support (AdAMS) to TM and KT; 15K06730, 17K10286 to MS; 15H04258, 18H05127, 19H03328, 20H05068 to HO), RRIME and FORCE from Japan Agency for Medical Research and Development (AMED) (Grant number JP18gm5910009, JP20gm4010004 to TH), the Takeda Science Foundation (TH), the Mitsubishi Foundation (TH), the Brain Science Foundation (TH), and the Astellas Foundation for Research on Metabolic Disorders (TH).
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41386-022-01347-9.
References
- 1.Johansen JP, Cain CK, Ostroff LE, LeDoux JE. Molecular mechanisms of fear learning and memory. Cell. 2011;147:509–24. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2011.10.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Maren S, Phan KL, Liberzon I. The contextual brain: implications for fear conditioning, extinction and psychopathology. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2013;14:417–28. doi: 10.1038/nrn3492. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Izquierdo I, Furini CR, Myskiw JC. Fear memory. Physiol Rev. 2016;96:695–750. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00018.2015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Rauch SL, Shin LM, Phelps EA. Neurocircuitry models of posttraumatic stress disorder and extinction: human neuroimaging research-past, present, and future. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;60:376–82. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.06.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Johnson LR, McGuire J, Lazarus R, Palmer AA. Pavlovian fear memory circuits and phenotype models of PTSD. Neuropharmacology. 2012;62:638–46. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2011.07.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Heine M, Thoumine O, Mondin M, Tessier B, Giannone G, Choquet D. Activity-independent and subunit-specific recruitment of functional AMPA receptors at neurexin/neuroligin contacts. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2008;105:20947–52. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0804007106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Shepherd JD, Huganir RL. The cell biology of synaptic plasticity: AMPA receptor trafficking. Annu Rev Cell Dev Biol. 2007;23:613–43. doi: 10.1146/annurev.cellbio.23.090506.123516. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Collingridge GL, Isaac JT, Wang YT. Receptor trafficking and synaptic plasticity. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2004;5:952–62. doi: 10.1038/nrn1556. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Huganir RL, Nicoll RA. AMPARs and synaptic plasticity: the last 25 years. Neuron. 2013;80:704–17. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.10.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Kessels HW, Malinow R. Synaptic AMPA receptor plasticity and behavior. Neuron. 2009;61:340–50. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.01.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Anggono V, Huganir RL. Regulation of AMPA receptor trafficking and synaptic plasticity. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2012;22:461–9. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2011.12.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Jiang J, Suppiramaniam V, Wooten MW. Posttranslational modifications and receptor-associated proteins in AMPA receptor trafficking and synaptic plasticity. Neurosignals. 2006;15:266–82. doi: 10.1159/000105517. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Lussier MP, Sanz-Clemente A, Roche KW. Dynamic regulation of N-Methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) and alpha-Amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolepropionic acid (AMPA) receptors by posttranslational modifications. J Biol Chem. 2015;290:28596–603. doi: 10.1074/jbc.R115.652750. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Derkach VA, Oh MC, Guire ES, Soderling TR. Regulatory mechanisms of AMPA receptors in synaptic plasticity. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2007;8:101–13. doi: 10.1038/nrn2055. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Malinow R, Malenka RC. AMPA receptor trafficking and synaptic plasticity. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2002;25:103–26. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.25.112701.142758. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Diering GH, Huganir RL. The AMPA Receptor code of synaptic plasticity. Neuron. 2018;100:314–29. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.10.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Clem RL, Huganir RL. Calcium-permeable AMPA receptor dynamics mediate fear memory erasure. Science. 2010;330:1108–12. doi: 10.1126/science.1195298. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Rao-Ruiz P, Rotaru DC, van der Loo RJ, Mansvelder HD, Stiedl O, Smit AB, et al. Retrieval-specific endocytosis of GluA2-AMPARs underlies adaptive reconsolidation of contextual fear. Nat Neurosci. 2011;14:1302–8. doi: 10.1038/nn.2907. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Fukata Y, Fukata M. Protein palmitoylation in neuronal development and synaptic plasticity. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2010;11:161–75. doi: 10.1038/nrn2788. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Linder ME, Deschenes RJ. Palmitoylation: policing protein stability and traffic. Nat Rev Mol Cell Biol. 2007;8:74–84. doi: 10.1038/nrm2084. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Resh MD. Palmitoylation of ligands, receptors, and intracellular signaling molecules. Sci STKE. 2006;2006:re14. doi: 10.1126/stke.3592006re14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Matt L, Kim K, Chowdhury D, Hell JW. Role of palmitoylation of postsynaptic proteins in promoting synaptic plasticity. Front Mol Neurosci. 2019;12:8. doi: 10.3389/fnmol.2019.00008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Borroni MV, Valles AS, Barrantes FJ. The lipid habitats of neurotransmitter receptors in brain. Biochim Biophys Acta. 2016;1858:2662–70. doi: 10.1016/j.bbamem.2016.07.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Kang R, Wan J, Arstikaitis P, Takahashi H, Huang K, Bailey AO, et al. Neural palmitoyl-proteomics reveals dynamic synaptic palmitoylation. Nature. 2008;456:904–9. doi: 10.1038/nature07605. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Shipston MJ. Ion channel regulation by protein palmitoylation. J Biol Chem. 2011;286:8709–16. doi: 10.1074/jbc.R110.210005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Hayashi T, Rumbaugh G, Huganir RL. Differential regulation of AMPA receptor subunit trafficking by palmitoylation of two distinct sites. Neuron. 2005;47:709–23. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.06.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Lin DT, Makino Y, Sharma K, Hayashi T, Neve R, Takamiya K, et al. Regulation of AMPA receptor extrasynaptic insertion by 4.1N, phosphorylation and palmitoylation. Nat Neurosci. 2009;12:879–87. doi: 10.1038/nn.2351. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Thomas GM, Hayashi T. Smarter neuronal signaling complexes from existing components: how regulatory modifications were acquired during animal evolution: evolution of palmitoylation-dependent regulation of AMPA-type ionotropic glutamate receptors. Bioessays. 2013;35:929–39. doi: 10.1002/bies.201300076. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Thomas GM, Hayashi T, Chiu SL, Chen CM, Huganir RL. Palmitoylation by DHHC5/8 targets GRIP1 to dendritic endosomes to regulate AMPA-R trafficking. Neuron. 2012;73:482–96. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.11.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Thomas GM, Hayashi T, Huganir RL, Linden DJ. DHHC8-dependent PICK1 palmitoylation is required for induction of cerebellar long-term synaptic depression. J Neurosci. 2013;33:15401–7. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1283-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Hayashi T. Evolutionarily conserved palmitoylation-dependent regulation of ionotropic glutamate receptors in vertebrates. Neurotransmitter. 2014;1:e388. [Google Scholar]
- 32.Hayashi T. Evolutionarily established palmitoylation-dependent regulatory mechanisms of the vertebrate glutamatergic synapse and diseases caused by their disruption. Front Mol Neurosci. 2021;14:796912. doi: 10.3389/fnmol.2021.796912. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Shen L, Liang F, Walensky LD, Huganir RL. Regulation of AMPA receptor GluR1 subunit surface expression by a 4. 1N-linked actin cytoskeletal association. J Neurosci. 2000;20:7932–40. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-21-07932.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Itoh M, Yamashita M, Kaneko M, Okuno H, Abe M, Yamazaki M, et al. Deficiency of AMPA receptor-palmitoylation aggravates seizure susceptibility. J Neurosci. 2018;38:10220–35. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1590-18.2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Itoh M, Okuno H, Yamada D, Yamashita M, Abe M, Natsume R, et al. Perturbed expression pattern of the immediate early gene Arc in the dentate gyrus of GluA1 C-terminal palmitoylation-deficient mice. Neuropsychopharmacol Rep. 2019;39:61–66. doi: 10.1002/npr2.12044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Iizumi M, Oota-Ishigaki A, Yamashita M, Hayashi T. Reduced effect of anticonvulsants on AMPA receptor palmitoylation-deficient mice. Front Pharmacol. 2021;12:711737. doi: 10.3389/fphar.2021.711737. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Hayashi T. Post-translational palmitoylation of ionotropic glutamate receptors in excitatory synaptic functions. Br J Pharmacol. 2021;178:784–97. doi: 10.1111/bph.15050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.McIlwain KL, Merriweather MY, Yuva-Paylor LA, Paylor R. The use of behavioral test batteries: effects of training history. Physiol Behav. 2001;73:705–17. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(01)00528-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Takao K, Miyakawa T. Investigating gene-to-behavior pathways in psychiatric disorders: the use of a comprehensive behavioral test battery on genetically engineered mice. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2006;1086:144–59. doi: 10.1196/annals.1377.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Mandillo S, Tucci V, Holter SM, Meziane H, Banchaabouchi MA, Kallnik M, et al. Reliability, robustness, and reproducibility in mouse behavioral phenotyping: a cross-laboratory study. Physiol Genomics. 2008;34:243–55. doi: 10.1152/physiolgenomics.90207.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Takao K, Miyakawa T. Intrauterine environment-genome interaction and children’s development (4): Brain-behavior phenotypying of genetically-engineered mice using a comprehensive behavioral test battery on research of neuropsychiatric disorders. J Toxicol Sci. 2009;34(Suppl 2):SP293–305. doi: 10.2131/jts.34.SP293. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Lad HV, Liu L, Paya-Cano JL, Parsons MJ, Kember R, Fernandes C, et al. Behavioural battery testing: evaluation and behavioural outcomes in 8 inbred mouse strains. Physiol Behav. 2010;99:301–16. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.11.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Sare RM, Lemons A, Smith CB. Behavior testing in rodents: highlighting potential confounds affecting variability and reproducibility. Brain Sci. 2021;11:522. doi: 10.3390/brainsci11040522. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Arron JR, Winslow MM, Polleri A, Chang CP, Wu H, Gao X, et al. NFAT dysregulation by increased dosage of DSCR1 and DYRK1A on chromosome 21. Nature. 2006;441:595–600. doi: 10.1038/nature04678. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Ihara M, Yamasaki N, Hagiwara A, Tanigaki A, Kitano A, Hikawa R, et al. Sept4, a component of presynaptic scaffold and Lewy bodies, is required for the suppression of alpha-synuclein neurotoxicity. Neuron. 2007;53:519–33. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.01.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Miyakawa T, Yamada M, Duttaroy A, Wess J. Hyperactivity and intact hippocampus-dependent learning in mice lacking the M1 muscarinic acetylcholine receptor. J Neurosci. 2001;21:5239–50. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-14-05239.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Miyakawa T, Yared E, Pak JH, Huang FL, Huang KP, Crawley JN. Neurogranin null mutant mice display performance deficits on spatial learning tasks with anxiety related components. Hippocampus. 2001;11:763–75. doi: 10.1002/hipo.1092. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Takao K, Yamasaki N, Miyakawa T. Impact of brain-behavior phenotypying of genetically-engineered mice on research of neuropsychiatric disorders. Neurosci Res. 2007;58:124–32. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2007.02.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Pellow S, Chopin P, File SE, Briley M. Validation of open:closed arm entries in an elevated plus-maze as a measure of anxiety in the rat. J Neurosci Methods. 1985;14:149–67. doi: 10.1016/0165-0270(85)90031-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Fernandes C, File SE. The influence of open arm ledges and maze experience in the elevated plus-maze. Pharm Biochem Behav. 1996;54:31–40. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(95)02171-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Resnik J, Paz R. Fear generalization in the primate amygdala. Nat Neurosci. 2015;18:188–90. doi: 10.1038/nn.3900. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Asok A, Kandel ER, Rayman JB. The neurobiology of fear generalization. Front Behav Neurosci. 2018;12:329. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00329. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Quirk GJ, Mueller D. Neural mechanisms of extinction learning and retrieval. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33:56–72. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301555. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Yamada D, Wada K, Sekiguchi M. Facilitating actions of an AMPA receptor potentiator upon extinction of contextually conditioned fear response in stressed mice. Neurosci Lett. 2011;488:242–6. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.11.038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Yamada D, Zushida K, Wada K, Sekiguchi M. Pharmacological discrimination of extinction and reconsolidation of contextual fear memory by a potentiator of AMPA receptors. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2009;34:2574–84. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.86. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Sah P, Faber ES, Lopez De Armentia M, Power J. The amygdaloid complex: anatomy and physiology. Physiol Rev. 2003;83:803–34. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00002.2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Reppucci CJ, Petrovich GD. Organization of connections between the amygdala, medial prefrontal cortex, and lateral hypothalamus: a single and double retrograde tracing study in rats. Brain Struct Funct. 2016;221:2937–62. doi: 10.1007/s00429-015-1081-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Aerts T, Seuntjens E. Novel perspectives on the development of the amygdala in rodents. Front Neuroanat. 2021;15:786679. doi: 10.3389/fnana.2021.786679. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Likhtik E, Popa D, Apergis-Schoute J, Fidacaro GA, Pare D. Amygdala intercalated neurons are required for expression of fear extinction. Nature. 2008;454:642–5. doi: 10.1038/nature07167. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Lee S, Kim SJ, Kwon OB, Lee JH, Kim JH. Inhibitory networks of the amygdala for emotional memory. Front Neural Circuits. 2013;7:129. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2013.00129. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Zhang WH, Zhang JY, Holmes A, Pan BX. Amygdala circuit substrates for stress adaptation and adversity. Biol Psychiatry. 2021;89:847–56. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2020.12.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Cho JH, Deisseroth K, Bolshakov VY. Synaptic encoding of fear extinction in mPFC-amygdala circuits. Neuron. 2013;80:1491–507. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.09.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Gass JT, Chandler LJ. The plasticity of extinction: contribution of the prefrontal cortex in treating addiction through inhibitory learning. Front Psychiatry. 2013;4:46. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Marek R, Jin J, Goode TD, Giustino TF, Wang Q, Acca GM, et al. Hippocampus-driven feed-forward inhibition of the prefrontal cortex mediates relapse of extinguished fear. Nat Neurosci. 2018;21:384–92. doi: 10.1038/s41593-018-0073-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Marek R, Xu L, Sullivan RKP, Sah P. Excitatory connections between the prelimbic and infralimbic medial prefrontal cortex show a role for the prelimbic cortex in fear extinction. Nat Neurosci. 2018;21:654–8. doi: 10.1038/s41593-018-0137-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.McKernan MG, Shinnick-Gallagher P. Fear conditioning induces a lasting potentiation of synaptic currents in vitro. Nature. 1997;390:607–11. doi: 10.1038/37605. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Rogan MT, Staubli UV, LeDoux JE. Fear conditioning induces associative long-term potentiation in the amygdala. Nature. 1997;390:604–7. doi: 10.1038/37601. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Milad MR, Rauch SL, Pitman RK, Quirk GJ. Fear extinction in rats: implications for human brain imaging and anxiety disorders. Biol Psychol. 2006;73:61–71. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.01.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Paunovic N, Ost LG. Cognitive-behavior therapy vs exposure therapy in the treatment of PTSD in refugees. Behav Res Ther. 2001;39:1183–97. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(00)00093-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Kaczkurkin AN, Foa EB. Cognitive-behavioral therapy for anxiety disorders: an update on the empirical evidence. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2015;17:337–46. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2015.17.3/akaczkurkin. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Cooper AA, Clifton EG, Feeny NC. An empirical review of potential mediators and mechanisms of prolonged exposure therapy. Clin Psychol Rev. 2017;56:106–21. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2017.07.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Kessler RC, Berglund P, Demler O, Jin R, Merikangas KR, Walters EE. Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the national comorbidity survey replication. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2005;62:593–602. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.593. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Harpaz-Rotem I, Rosenheck RA, Mohamed S, Desai RA. Pharmacologic treatment of posttraumatic stress disorder among privately insured Americans. Psychiatr Serv. 2008;59:1184–90. doi: 10.1176/ps.2008.59.10.1184. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Christiansen DM, Berke ET. Gender- and sex-based contributors to sex differences in PTSD. Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2020;22:19. doi: 10.1007/s11920-020-1140-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.