Skip to main content
Alzheimer's Research & Therapy logoLink to Alzheimer's Research & Therapy
. 2022 Oct 14;14:156. doi: 10.1186/s13195-022-01098-1

Reading activities compensate for low education-related cognitive deficits

Yue Wang 1,2,3, Shinan Wang 4, Wanlin Zhu 2,3, Na Liang 1,2,3, Chen Zhang 1,2,3, Yuankun Pei 1,2,3, Qing Wang 1,2,3, Shiping Li 1,2,3,, Jiong Shi 1,2,3,
PMCID: PMC9563722  PMID: 36242017

Abstract

Background

The incidence of cognitive impairment is increasing with an aging population. Developing effective strategies is essential to prevent dementia. Higher education level is associated with better baseline cognitive performance, and reading activities can slow down cognitive decline. However, it is unclear whether education and reading activities are synergistic or independent contributors to cognitive performance.

Methods

This was a sub-study of an ongoing prospective community cohort of China National Clinical Research Center Alzheimer’s Disease and Neurodegenerative Disorder Research (CANDOR). Demographic and clinical information, educational levels, and reading activities were collected. All participants finished neuropsychological testing batteries and brain MRIs. We analyzed cognitive performance and brain structures with education and reading activities.

Results

Four hundred fifty-nine subjectively cognitively normal participants were enrolled in the study. One hundred sixty-nine (36.82%) of them had regular reading activities. Participants in the reading group had better performance in all cognitive tests compared with those in the non-reading group, but no difference in brain MRI variables. Participants with higher education levels (more than 13 years) had better cognitive performance and higher hippocampal volumes. In low education groups (less than 12 years), more reading activities were associated with better cognitive test scores.

Conclusions

Both education and reading activities are important and synergistic for baseline cognitive function. Higher education level is associated with larger hippocampal volumes. Education may stimulate the growth and development of the hippocampus. Reading activities help to maintain and improve cognitive function in people with low levels of education.

Trial registration

NCT04320368.

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1186/s13195-022-01098-1.

Keywords: Reading activities, Education, Cognition

Introduction

Aging is the most important risk factor for dementia. With an aging population, dementia has cast an enormous social and economic burden around the world [1, 2]. Developing effective strategies is essential to prevent dementia [3]. It has been reported there are modifiable risk factors for dementia and modifying 12 of them may prevent or delay up to 40% of dementia [4]. The Finnish Geriatric Intervention Study to Prevent Cognitive Impairment and Disability (FINGER), a multicenter randomized controlled trial, reported beneficial effects on cognition through multimodal intervention including cognitive training [5]. Reading activities and other mental stimulation help to slow down cognitive decline [6, 7].

High education level is associated with better cognitive performance and lower likelihood to have Alzheimer’s disease (AD) [8, 9]. High education level may delay cognitive decline in individuals with subjective cognitive decline [10, 11]. However, although education is associated with baseline cognitive performance, it doesn’t affect the rate of cognitive decline [12], nor does it affect the neuropathological changes related to dementia, such as amyloid plaques and tangles [13].

Previous research compared the influence of reading activities and education on cognition and found that reading activities were associated with a lower risk of dementia even in late life, independent of education and other related factors [6, 7], while another study demonstrated that reading activities have a stronger relationship than education with executive function tests [14].

It is inconclusive whether reading activities and education are synergistic or independent contributors to cognitive performance. In this prospective community-based cohort study, we try to answer the following questions. First, what are the relationships of education and reading activities with cognitive performance on domain-specific tests? Second, are education and/or reading activities associated with brain structure? Third, can reading activities compensate for lower levels of education?

Methods

Study design and participants

This study was a sub-study of an ongoing prospective community-based cohort study of the China National Clinical Research Center Alzheimer's Disease and Neurodegenerative Disorder Research (CANDOR). CANDOR was started in July 2019 and planned to enroll one thousand and five hundred participants, including individuals with normal cognition (NC), mild cognitive impairment (MCI), and dementia. Demographic information and past medical history were collected. All participants were required to have a study partner to provide an independent evaluation of daily and social functions. They underwent detailed assessments for cognition and functional abilities, a comprehensive neuropsychological battery (described below in “Neuropsychological assessment”) including the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR), and brain MRIs. All enrolled participants for this study (1) were subjectively cognitively normal; (2) aged 40–100 years old; (3) had at least 3 years of education; (4) had no condition known to affect cognitive function, such as Alzheimer’s Disease, vascular dementia, Lewy body dementia, frontotemportal dementia, Parkinson’s disease, epilepsy, stroke, hydrocephalus, multiple sclerosis, traumatic brain injuries, genetic disorders affecting cognition, alcoholism, uncontrolled depression, or other psychiatric disorders; (5) had no uncontrolled neoplasia, or severe pulmonary, cardiovascular, metabolic, infectious, inflammatory, or endocrine diseases. We excluded individuals with less than 3 years of education because people started to learn how to read and write in the third year of elementary school in China. Therefore, people who have less than three years of education will have difficulties in reading.

To assess the relationship between education and leisure reading activities, we defined regular reading activities as reading at least one book per month on average for at least one year. We divided the participants as follows. First, participants were divided into 2 groups based on their reading activities. Reading activities were detailed, including (1) reading materials, such as paper books, e-books, and audio-books; (2) reading content, such as literature books, and non-literature books; (3) the total number of books, which was calculated as the average number of books read per month ×12 months × years of reading. In participants with reading habits, we divided them further into several groups based on reading years, reading content and reading materials. Second, participants were divided based on their education. Previous studies analyzed education by ≤9, 10–12, and ≥13 years [15, 16]. In our study, the average education years of all participants were 12.12 years. Therefore, we used a 12-year cut-off to divide participants into two groups: low education (≤12 years, high school education or below, under the average education level) and high education (≥13 years, college education or above, over the average education level). Third, participants were divided into four groups based on education years and reading activities: low education (educational years ≤12) with and without reading activities (groups 1 and 2), and high education (educational years ≥13) with and without reading activities (groups 3 and 4).

Standard protocol approvals, registrations, and patient consents

This protocol was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Beijing Tiantan Hospital (approval number: KY 2019-004-007) and was in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations. Written informed consent was obtained from each participant.

Neuropsychological assessment

Thirteen neuropsychological tests were completed at the visit, including (1) tests for overall cognitive performance: Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA), and Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR) with global scores; (2) Tests for specific cognitive domain: Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT) [17], Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test (ROCF) [18], Stroop Color-Word Test-Victoria version [19], Trail Making Test-A (TMT- A) and Trail Making Test B (TMT -B) [20], clock drawing test (CDT), Boston Naming Test (BNT), Digit Span Test (DST), and Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT); and (3) Neuropsychiatry Inventory (NPI). These tests were administered by experienced neuropsychologists who were blinded to group assignment.

MRI assessment

All participants completed the brain MRI to exclude other demonstrable neurological diseases. Quantitative measures of signal-to-noise ratio, uniformity, and geometric distortion were conducted in each research center. 3.0 T-MRI was used with the scanning thickness not exceeding 1.5mm. The three-dimensional T1 weighted images were corrected for intensity non-uniformity with the N4 algorithm. Brain surface was reconstructed using FreeSurfer (version 7.2.0, http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/) recon-all pipeline. The cortical thickness and volume of the total brain, nuclei, gray matter white matter, and white matter lesion were all obtained with this pipeline. Regional cortical thickness was obtained and statistical analysis was performed.

Statistical analysis

The analysis was conducted with SPSS 24.0. Continuous variables were characterized as mean plus and/or minus standard deviations (SD). T-test or nonparametric test was used by the characteristic of the distribution. Categorical variables were analyzed by Pearson’s χ2 tests. We performed logistic regression analysis to evaluate the association between reading and CDR (CDR=0 or >0), linear regression analysis for the association of reading and neuropsychological assessment, and linear regression analysis for education and brain structure. The regression analyses were independent of age and sex in Table 2 model 1 and Table 5. The regression analyses were independent of age, sex, and education in Table 2 model 2. We also performed the collinearity analysis in every linear regression analysis, and all the results showed no collinearity between every included independent variable.

Table 2.

The logistic and linear regression analysis of reading in all cognitive tests

Logistic regression Model 1, OR, 95% CI P Model 2, OR, 95% CI P
Global CDR score (=0), 330 (71.9%) 2.012, 1.258–3.218 0.004 1.416, 0.848–2.365 0.210
Linear regression Model 1, Beta, 95%CI P Model 2, Beta, 95%CI P
MMSE 2.193 [1.463, 2.923] <0.001 1.044 [0.302, 1.787] 0.006
MOCA 3.342 [2.486, 4.198] <0.001 1.546 [0.744, 2.348] <0.001
DST total 1.240 [0.749, 1.731] <0.001 0.496 [0.003, 0.988] 0.048
RAVLT learn total 4.162 [2.208, 6.121] <0.001 1.235 [−0.758, 3.227] 0.224
RAVLT long-delayed recall 1.185 [0.544, 1.826] <0.001 0.241 [−0.426, 0.907] 0.478
ROCF copy 3.800 [1.487, 6.114] 0.001 1.296 [−0.998, 3.590] 0.267
ROCF delayed recall 5.359 [3.138, 7.580] <0.001 3.103 [0.892, 5.314] 0.006
Stroop D time −5.162 [−7.712, −2.611] <0.001 −3.080 [−5.789, −0.371] 0.026
Stroop W time −4.802 [−7.074, −2.530] <0.001 −3.304 [−5.694, −0.914] 0.007
TMT-A time −14.499 [−19.792, −9.206] <0.001 −8.246 [−13.798, −2.694] 0.004
TMT-B time −26.290 [−40.525, −12.055] <0.001 −7.465 [−22.449–7.519] 0.328
BNT 2.961 [2.252, 3.669] <0.001 1.761 [1.048, 2.474] <0.001
SDMT 7.385 [5.233, 9.538] <0.001 2.719 [0.693, 4.746] 0.009
CDT 0.703 [0.284, 1.121] 0.001 0.219 [−0.218, 0.656] 0.325

Model 1 logistic or linear regression included age and sex

Model 2 logistic or linear regression included age, sex, and years of education

Abbreviations: OR odds ratio for logistic regression, CI confidence interval, CDR Clinical Dementia Rating, MMSE Mini-Mental State Examination, MoCA Montreal Cognitive Assessment, DST Digit Span Test, RAVLT Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test, ROCF Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test, TMT Trail Making Test, BNT Boston Naming Test, SDMT Symbol Digit Modalities Test, CDT clock drawing test, NPI Neuropsychiatry Inventory

Table 5.

The linear regression of education and hippocampal volume

Model 1, beta, 95% CI P Model 2, beta, 95% CI P
Years of education 14.999, [4.906, 25.092] 0.004 15.816, [4.949, 26.683] 0.004
Education ≥13 23.020, [3.868, 42,172] 0.019 22.114, [1.476, 42.753] 0.036

Model 1, data of left hippocampal volume were analyzed as results, age, and sex were in linear regression

Model 2, data of right hippocampal volume were analyzed as results, age, and sex were in linear regression

Abbreviations: CI confidence interval

Results

From July 31, 2019, to August 1, 2021, 694 individuals were screened from communities in Beijing, Shijiazhuang, and Langfang, all in northern China. They completed standard baseline assessments. 459 were enrolled in the study who had both valid brain MRI examination and cognitive evaluation (Fig. 1). Among them, 169 (36.82%) had regular reading activities. There was no significant difference in age, sex, medical history, and mood assessment (NPI) between the two groups (Table 1). The reading group had more years of education and better cognitive performance than the non-reading group, including CDR, MMSE, MoCA, DST, RAVLT, ROCF, Stroop D and W time, TMT-A and B, BNT, SDMT, and CDT. However, there was no difference in cortical thickness and hippocampal volume in either hemisphere between the two groups.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Study flowchart. Shown is the flowchart of the study enrollment

Table 1.

Demographic, clinical information, cognitive test scores, and MRI variables in reading and non-reading groups

Reading
n=169
Non-reading
n=290
All patients
n=459
P value
Average age 60.33±9.04 60.03±8.5 60.14±8.69 0.727
Sex female, (n, %) 89, 52.7% 171, 59.0% 260, 56.6% 0.189
Years of education 13.88±3.3 11.1±3.52 12.12±3.69 <0.001
Hypertension (n, %) 57, 33.7% 96, 33.1% 153, 33.3% 0.918
Diabetes (n, %) 19, 11.2% 34, 11.7% 53, 11.5% 0.882
Stroke or TIA (n, %) 11, 6.5% 21, 7.2% 32, 7.0% 0.851
Coronary heart disease (n, %) 14, 8.3% 18, 6.2% 32, 7.0% 0.449
Global CDR score 0.11±0.21 0.22±0.41 0.18±0.36 0.001
MMSE 26.15±2.85 24.04±4.82 24.82±4.32 <0.001
MoCA 23.38±3.82 20.13±5.64 21.33±5.29 <0.001
DST total 12.3±2.34 11.09±2.93 11.54±2.79 <0.001
RAVLT total learning 39.98±10.06 36.26±12.68 37.63±11.91 0.001
RAVLT long-delayed recall 8.01±3.44 6.85±3.85 7.28±3.74 0.001
ROCF copy 32.34±6.91 28.48±10 30.36±8.83 0.001
ROCF long-delayed recall 16.5±7.82 11.03±9.01 13.69±8.87 <0.001
Stroop D time 16.86±6.13 21.8±16.31 19.97±13.67 <0.001
Stroop W time 22.32±8.05 26.77±14.46 25.12±12.65 <0.001
TMT-A time 44.18±21.57 58.17±35.98 52.98±32.11 <0.001
TMT-B time 101.55±71.89 125.32±87.41 116.51±82.72 0.002
BNT 25.02±3.4 21.99±4.37 23.11±4.29 <0.001
SDMT 39.57±13.39 32.71±14.89 35.25±14.72 <0.001
CDT 8.79±1.79 8.09±2.42 8.35±2.23 <0.001
NPI 1.03±3.17 1.83±6.01 1.54±5.15 0.064
Brain structure
 Left hippocampal volume, mm2 3496.15±427.05 3436.04±451.65 3458.79±442.97 0.170
 Left amygdala volume, mm2 1463.18±355.93 1482.53±324.29 1475.21±336.34 0.561
 Left thalamus volume, mm2 6879.88±904.81 6876.82±939.2 6877.98±925.29 0.973
 Left caudate volume, mm2 3316.52±565.36 3297.41±581.55 3304.64±574.9 0.737
 Left putamen volume, mm2 4632.86±662.24 4613.26±719.29 4620.68±697.55 0.776
 Left pallidum volume, mm2 1940±269.78 1916.5±238.56 1925.39±250.78 0.343
 Left cortex volume, mm2 213,629.66±24,614.53 215,780.91±24,480.75 214,966.79±24,525.43 0.375
 Left cerebral white matter volume, mm2 221,073.68±32,180.9 222,387.88±29,129.85 221,890.53±30,290.75 0.661
 Left mean cortical thickness, mm 2.37±0.11 2.38±0.12 2.37±0.11 0.516
 Right hippocampal volume, mm2 3605.87±487.08 3571.79±460.36 3584.69±470.39 0.464
 Right amygdala volume, mm2 1643.52±356.73 1664.16±329.83 1656.35±340 0.539
 Right thalamus volume, mm2 6643.18±897.79 6669.33±823.35 6659.44±851.36 0.756
 Right caudate volume, mm2 3366.34±617.01 3358.4±559.61 3361.41±581.31 0.869
 Right putamen volume, mm2 4714.73±647.17 4743.82±707.15 4732.81±684.46 0.667
 Right pallidum volume, mm2 1937.36±281.85 1920.2±253.02 1926.7±264.11 0.511
 Right cortex volume, mm2 212,619.37±25,297.4 215,773.47±24,519.75 214,579.83±24,835.2 0.199
 Right cerebral white matter volume, mm2 219,923.7±31,876.29 221,611.7±28,804.31 220,972.9±29,978.85 0.569
 Right mean cortical thickness, mm 2.36±0.11 2.37±0.12 2.37±0.11 0.151
 Cortex volume, mm2 426,249.03±49,571.32 431,554.38±48,668.99 429,546.62±49,023.28 0.274
 Subcortex gray volume, mm2 54,301.14±6096.21 54,392.28±6088.1 54,357.79±6084.32 0.88
 Total gray volume, mm2 440,997.39±63,865.8 443,999.58±57,770.04 442,863.43±60,094.68 0.613
 Cerebral white matter volume, mm2 3788.28±5597.58 2941.34±4143.9 3261.86±4758.19 0.093
 WM hyperintensities volume, mm2 578,959.97±62,023.02 584,269.48±59,903.83 582,260.15±60,698.27 0.376
 Brain segmentation volume, mm2 1,077,237.37±118,406.82 1,084,041.34±113,069.4 1,081,466.44±115,030.46 0.488
 eTIV, mm2 1,430,023.25±162,478.34 1,438,793.2±151,423.56 1,435,474.3±155,572.46 0.499
 Brain segmentation volume to eTIV, % 75.59±5.81 75.55±5.38 75.56±5.54 0.941

Abbreviations: CDR Clinical Dementia Rating, MMSE Mini-Mental State Examination, MoCA Montreal Cognitive Assessment, DST Digit Span Test, RAVLT Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test, ROCF Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test, TMT Trail Making Test, BNT Boston Naming Test, SDMT Symbol Digit Modalities Test, CDT clock drawing test, NPI Neuropsychiatry Inventory, WM white matter, eTIV estimated total intracranial volume

Logistic and linear regression were used to assess confounding factors (Table 2). Reading activities were associated with better cognitive performance, such as MMSE (beta 2.193, 95%CI: 1.463–2.923, P<0.001), independent of age and sex in model 1. In model 2, when education was taken into account, reading activities showed similar effects in MoCA and Boston Naming; significant but less effects in MMSE, DST, ROCF delayed recall, Stroop D and W time, TMT-A and SDMT, but no effects in CDR, RAVLT, ROCF copy, TMT-B, and CDT.

Education had a remarkable effect on cognitive performance (Table 3). Participants with high education scored higher in all cognitive tests than those with low education. They also have higher hippocampal volumes on both sides (Fig. 2).

Table 3.

Cognitive performance and brain structure at different education levels

Education ≤12 years
n=294
Education ≥13 years
n=165
P
Average age 61.66±8.28 57.43±8.78 <0.001
Sex female (n, %) 177, 60.2% 83, 50.3% 0.040
Years of education 9.79±2.34 16.29±0.95 <0.001
Global CDR score 0.10±0.24 0.22±0.40 <0.001
MMSE 23.74±4.65 26.73±2.79 <0.001
MoCA 19.47±5.18 24.62±3.61 <0.001
DST total 10.72±2.51 12.98±2.68 <0.001
RAVLT total learning 8.93±3.32 11.07±2.79 <0.001
RAVLT long-delayed recall 6.04±3.7 8.65±3.53 <0.001
ROCF copy 33.18±6.55 28.8±9.53 <0.001
ROCF long-delayed recall 17.23±7.96 11.74±8.76 <0.001
Stroop D time 22.07±16.02 16.25±6.55 <0.001
Stroop W time 27.72±13.85 20.54±8.46 <0.001
TMT-A time 59.73±32.17 41.09±28.42 <0.001
TMT-B time 136.48±91.03 81.31±48.78 <0.001
BNT 21.78±4.47 25.44±2.69 <0.001
SDMT 30.22±13.05 44.06±13.285 <0.001
CDT 8.00±2.39 8.96±1.76 <0.001
NPI 1.75±5.86 1.16±3.55 0.242
Brain structure
 Left hippocampal volume, mm2 3386.57±435.93 3584.61±428.01 <0.001
 Left amygdala volume, mm2 1634.78±220.55 1709.65±215.06 0.145
 Left thalamus volume, mm2 6883.74±897.91 6867.95±973.97 0.864
 Left caudate volume, mm2 3313.07±580.16 3289.96±567.13 0.697
 Left putamen volume, mm2 4629.44±710.25 4605.42±676.78 0.73
 Left pallidum volume, mm2 1931.57±251.23 1914.63±250.43 0.498
 Left cortex volume, mm2 216,139.25±23,700.57 212,924.21±25,849.25 0.188
 Left cerebral white matter volume, mm2 223,576.87±29,777.23 218,952.71±31,041.08 0.125
 Left mean cortical thickness, mm 2.37±0.12 2.38±0.10 0.678
 Right hippocampal volume, mm2 3512.3±476.23 3710.8±433.37 <0.001
 Right amygdala volume, mm2 4691.13±49,986.08 1777.05±252.54 0.563
 Right thalamus volume, mm2 6696.93±830.19 6594.12±885.91 0.225
 Right caudate volume, mm2 3373.49±565.75 3340.36±608.73 0.474
 Right putamen volume, mm2 4743.54±678.03 4714.12±697.27 0.666
 Right pallidum volume, mm2 1928.99±259.36 1922.71±272.97 0.812
 Right cortex volume, mm2 216,046.23±23,917.16 212,025.16±26,239.96 0.104
 Right cerebral white matter volume, mm2 222,783.05±29,373.45 217,819.36±30,845.32 0.096
 Right mean cortical thickness, mm 2.37±0.12 2.37±0.11 0.800
 Cortex volume, mm2 432,185.48±47,269.6 424,949.37±51,768.69 0.138
 Subcortex gray volume, mm2 54,526.69±6033.69 54,063.54±6179.62 0.445
 Total gray volume, mm2 585,324.16±59,164.42 576,922.21±63,115.96 0.164
 Cerebral white matter volume, mm2 446,359.92±58,982.67 436,772.07±61,698.97 0.109
 WM hyperintensities volume, mm2 3219.53±4544.25 3335.6±5123.64 0.510
 Brain segmentation volume, mm2 1,089,199.77±112,197.33 1,067,993.91±118,965.54 0.110
 eTIV, mm2 1,443,152.53±151,357.93 1,422,097.74±162,271.98 0.199
 Brain segmentation volume to eTIV, % 75.67±5.19 75.37±6.11 0.590

Abbreviations: CDR Clinical Dementia Rating, MMSE Mini-Mental State Examination, MoCA Montreal Cognitive Assessment, DST Digit Span Test, RAVLT Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test, ROCF Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test, TMT Trail Making Test, BNT Boston Naming Test, SDMT Symbol Digit Modalities Test, CDT clock drawing test, NPI Neuropsychiatry Inventory, WM white matter, eTIV estimated total intracranial volume

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Hippocampal volumes at different education levels. Shown is the right (open box) and left (closed dot) hippocampal volume associated with different education levels

Reading years and reading content had little impact on cognitive performance and brain structure (Supplemental Tables 1 and 2). Reading e-books showed no obvious cognitive benefits than paper books, and listening to audio-books performed better in MoCA, BNT, and SDMT (Supplemental Table 3).

To assess if reading activities have a compensatory effect for low education, we divided participants into four groups: low education (educational years ≤12) with and without reading activities (groups 1 and 2), and high education (educational years ≥13) with and without reading activities (groups 3 and 4). Reading activities improved most cognitive tests (except RAVLT, ROCF copy, and TMT-B) in the low education group (group 1 better than group 2, Table 4). By reading more books, participants with low education could achieve similar or even better cognitive scores than those with high education in MMSE, MoCA, DST, and BNT (Fig. 3). In the high education groups, reading activities showed few effects probably due to ceiling effects (group 3 similar to group 4, Table 4).

Table 4.

Cognitive performance and brain structure comparison by education level and reading activities

Group 1
n=71
Group 2
n=223
P Group 3
n=98
Group 4
n=67
P
Age 62.66±9.79 61.34±7.73 0.299 58.63±8.09 55.67±9.5 0.825
Gender female (n, %) 39, 54.9% 138, 61.9% 0.297 50, 51.0% 33, 49.3% 0.824
Education years 10.44±1.95 9.58±2.42 0.007 16.37±1.08 16.18±0.72 0.180
Global CDR score 0.14±0.23 0.24±0.44 0.323 0.08±0.20 0.13±0.28 0.236
MMSE 25.04±3.13 23.33±4.97 0.001 26.94±2.35 26.42±3.34 0.241
MoCA 21.26±4.05 18.9±5.37 0.001 24.9±2.82 24.22±4.52 0.280
DST total 11.59±2.27 10.45±2.53 0.001 12.82±2.26 13.21±3.19 0.387
RAVLT total learning 35.97±9.55 33.81±11.85 0.123 42.85±9.45 44.44±12.02 0.345
RAVLT long-delayed recall 6.07±3.43 6.03±3.79 0.931 8.73±3.29 8.53±3.88 0.726
ROCF copy 30.36±8.25 27.99±10.07 0.167 33.96±5.08 30.84±9.53 0.186
ROCF long-delayed recall 14.33±8.32 10.39±8.73 0.012 18.31±6.95 14.05±9.92 0.096
Stroop D time 18.19±7.84 23.31±17.69 0.001 15.91±4.35 16.75±8.88 0.419
Stroop W time 24.97±9.69 28.59±14.85 0.019 20.42±6 20.72±11.21 0.844
TMT-A time 50.89±25.93 62.56±33.49 0.003 39.39±16.34 43.62±40.19 0.419
TMT-B time 128.57±91.74 139.01±90.87 0.405 82.26±44.79 79.91±54.49 0.764
BNT 23.97±3.87 21.08±4.43 <0.001 25.77±2.81 24.96±2.45 0.057
SDMT 33.07±12.71 29.31±13.05 0.035 44.21±11.89 43.84±15.18 0.864
CDT 8.59±1.92 7.82±2.50 0.008 8.94±1.69 8.98±1.87 0.870
NPI 1.31±4.25 1.89±6.29 0.478 0.83±2.09 1.65±4.97 0.206
Brain structure
 Left hippocampal volume, mm2 3391.25±442.13 3372.73±419.86 0.759 3580.93±455.11 3587.09±411.2 0.93
 Left amygdala volume, mm2 1487.93±400.96 1494.7±333.99 0.889 1444.95±319.72 1443.14±289.65 0.971
 Left thalamus volume, mm2 6927.86±912.39 6868.82±894.71 0.635 6844.53±902.39 6902.71±1077.97 0.713
 Left caudate volume, mm2 3274.19±590.97 3326.22±577.32 0.518 3347.72±546.78 3204.22±589.96 0.118
 Left putamen volume, mm2 4613.36±742.16 4634.88±700.9 0.827 4647.23±600.29 4543.36±777.51 0.344
 Left pallidum volume, mm2 1958.16±291.24 1922.58±236.29 0.307 1926.63±253.55 1896.81±246.62 0.463
 Left cortex volume, mm2 215,000.93±24,471.4 216,524.19±23,482.27 0.643 212,619.25±24,800.12 213,376.89±27,526.83 0.857
 Left cerebral white matter volume, mm2 225,861.59±35,115.03 222,804.26±27,795.56 0.51 217,545.76±29,530.28 221,041.16±33,286.15 0.488
 Left mean cortical thickness, mm 2.37±0.12 2.36±0.11 0.34 2.38±0.11 2.38±0.1 0.796
 Right hippocampal volume, mm2 3533.42±467.04 3449.84±500.66 0.205 3695.9±417.79 3720.84±445.47 0.723
 Right amygdala volume, mm2 1638.97±387.11 1671.78±333.05 0.495 1646.86±334.66 1639.52±320.52 0.89
 Right thalamus volume, mm2 6739.93±910.74 6682.39±802.96 0.617 6571.9±886.17 6627.11±891.5 0.701
 Right caudate volume, mm2 3326.03±602.96 3389.53±553.2 0.418 3396.05±628.68 3257.71±572.69 0.161
 Right putamen volume, mm2 4702.98±639.94 4757.25±691.4 0.564 4723.38±655.7 4700.38±759.94 0.839
 Right pallidum volume, mm2 1939.62±285.7 1925.39±250.47 0.692 1935.71±280.49 1903.42±262.42 0.466
 Right cortex volume, mm2 214,709.43±24,120.85 216,498.29±23,889.7 0.589 211,079.33±26,149.49 213,429.13±26,517.63 0.581
 Right cerebral white matter volume, mm2 225,512.97±34,160.89 221,859.89±27,599.24 0.42 215,805.29±29,593.22 220,808.98±32,622.88 0.317
 Right mean cortical thickness, mm 2.37±0.12 2.35±0.11 0.142 2.38±0.11 2.37±0.11 0.427
 Cortex volume, mm2 429,710.36±48,310.18 433,022.48±47,001.73 0.613 423,698.58±50,582.34 426,806.01±53,831.66 0.712
Subcortex gray volume, mm2 54,504.63±6480.09 54,534.15±5891.59 0.972 54,151.2±5827.67 53,933.42±6713.47 0.828
 Total gray volume, mm2 582,730.42±62,282.68 586,201.27±58,202.35 0.672 576,181.75±62,013.7 578,021.34±65,196.6 0.858
 Cerebral white matter volume, mm2 451,374.56±69,166.21 444,664.14±55,202.29 0.463 433,351.05±58,867.31 441,850.14±65,823.54 0.396
 WM hyperintensities volume, mm2 3602.52±4963.72 3090.02±4398.62 0.416 3925.16±6044.47 2460.48±3165.75 0.048
 Brain segmentation volume, mm2 1,093,169.74±123,689.54 1,087,857.27±108,324.4 0.733 1,065,497.73±113,587.95 1,071,699.19±127,357.94 0.748
 eTIV, mm2 1,440,816.13±159,459 1,443,942.62±148,911.25 0.882 1,422,070.61±165,054.25 1,422,138.01±159,346.57 0.998
 Brain segmentation volume to eTIV, % 76.03±5.26 75.55±5.18 0.507 75.26±6.19 75.54±6.03 0.783

Four groups: low education (educational years ≤12) with and without reading activities (groups 1 and 2) and high education (educational years ≥13) with and without reading activities (groups 3 and 4)

Abbreviations: CDR Clinical Dementia Rating, MMSE Mini-Mental State Examination, MoCA Montreal Cognitive Assessment, DST Digit Span Test, RAVLT Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test, ROCF Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test, TMT Trail Making Test, BNT Boston Naming Test, SDMT Symbol Digit Modalities Test, CDT clock drawing test, NPI Neuropsychiatry Inventory, WM white matter, eTIV estimated total intracranial volume

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3

Cognitive performance associated with reading activities at different education levels. Shown is that by reading more books, participants with low education (blue line) could achieve similar or even better cognitive scores than those with high education (red line) in a MMSE, b MoCA, c DST, and d BNT. a MMSE (education ≤12: beta, 95% CI 0.1035 [0.0009, 0.0198], P=0.0316; education ≥13: beta, 95% CI 0.0012 [−0.0035, 0.0059], P=0.6113); b MoCA (education ≤12: beta, 95% CI 0.0136 [0.0031, 0.0241], P=0.0112; education ≥13: beta, 95% CI 0.0040 [−0.0020, 0.0100], P=0. 1856); c DST (education ≤12: beta, 95% CI 0.0086 [0.0035, 0.0136], P=0.0009; education ≥13: beta, 95% CI 0.0008 [−0.0037, 0.0053], P=0.7199); d BNT (education ≤12: beta, 95% CI 0.0166 [0.0076, 0.0256], P=0.0003; education ≥13: beta, 95% CI 0.0012 [0.0012, 0.0100], P=0.0371)

The linear regression related to hippocampal volume on either side showed that years of education influenced hippocampal volume with beta1 14.999 [4.906, 25.092], P=0.004 and beta2 15.816 [4.949, 26.683], P=0.004 (Table 5), regardless of age and sex.

Discussion

In this community-based subjectively cognitively normal population, participants with regular reading activities showed better cognitive performance in overall cognitive abilities, attention, memory, language, visuospatial and executive function. This effect is independent of brain volume, especially hippocampal volume. A prospective cohort study showed that increased participation in cognitive activities (including reading) was associated with better memory [21]. Although reading activities involve multiple brain areas, subgroup analysis of the FINGER study has shown that the multi-domain intervention has no effects on brain volume, cortical thickness, and white matter lesion [22].

Education was related to cognition across all tested domains. Reading is associated with all tested domains controlled with age and sex. However, when education was included in the analysis, the effect of reading on cognitive assessment weakened, indicating a stronger correlation between education level and cognition. Reading is a complex task that involves various brain areas, including the insular and frontal opercular cortex, lateral temporal cortex, and early auditory cortex with the positive reaction and inferior temporal and motor cortex with the negative reaction [23]. However, we did not see a difference in the cortical thickness and the hippocampus between reading and non-reading groups. This suggests that reading activities may help to improve cognitive function in participants with low education level (≤12 years) independent of brain volume. In some cognitive domains, the cognitive performance gap caused by education level is decreased with the increase in reading activities. Reading is a good way to fill the cognitive gap brought about by lack of education, especially in language, non-verbal memory, and executive function.

In this study, participants with high education level had higher hippocampal volume. Larger hippocampal volumes may be associated with higher intelligence quotient (IQ), practice in hippocampus-related function (e.g., learning and memory), lifestyle, and medial/historical factors (neurotoxic effects of obesity, diabetes mellitus, hypertension, hypoxic brain injury, obstructive sleep apnea, bipolar disorder, clinical depression, and head trauma) [24]. Higher education level is favorable to the neurological task performance [10, 12, 2527], but not to AD-related pathology [28].

MMSE and MoCA are screening tests for cognition. Their cut-off scores are based on education levels. In this study, participants with low education but reading more books showed no difference in MMSE and MoCA compared with participants with high education level. It suggests that people with low education but who read a lot probably should be screened at the same level as those who are more educated.

Audio devices are a new form of reading activities and have become popular. It is suggested that audiobooks are probably better than non-audio books at improving cognitive function. Young children learned more words from the e-book and from the audio narrator than print books [29]. Different types of books may influence the ability to retrieve information. Listening to audiobooks may stimulate more brain areas to have positive effects on cognition, especially memory and executive function. Since poor vision is not uncommon in the elderly, audiobooks are a better tool for old people to enjoy reading activities.

The strength of this study is a large community-based cohort with detailed neuropsychological testing batteries and brain MRI analysis. But the study has several limitations. First, this is an observational, cross-sectional study. Correlation does not imply causation. To study the causative effect of reading activities on cognitive function, a randomized clinical trial is warranted. Participants with certain education levels would be assigned with different reading activities. Other intellectual activities besides leisure reading would also be taken into account. Second, we enrolled participants with subjectively normal cognitive function to represent community-based cohorts. The average CDR was 0.18 although a few participants with a CDR more than 0.5. Ongoing longitudinal follow-ups will allow us to assess the relationship between risk/protective factors and the conversion to dementia. Third, all participants were enrolled from northern China. There is likely a difference in culture, education, and environmental factors among different regions in China. To expand population sampling is needed in future studies. Fourth, higher education level is associated with larger hippocampal volumes. One explanation is that education may stimulate the growth and development of the hippocampus. Alternatively, people with larger hippocampal volume may have a better chance to acquire higher education. Fifth, the study may have a recall bias since reading activities were recorded by self-reported questionnaires. People might under- or overestimate the books they read. Objective measures (e.g., a shopping receipt of purchased books) may help to validate the finding. Finally, reading activities as measured by reading books are mainly leisure reading. It does not take into account of all activities related to intellectual activities. Individuals who do a lot of reading or research at work but have little time in reading books outside of work may be underestimated in reading activities.

Conclusions

Participants in reading groups with less education (educational years ≤12) had better cognitive performance than the ones in non-reading groups. Education affects more than reading habits in every cognitive domain and in hippocampal volume.

Supplementary Information

13195_2022_1098_MOESM1_ESM.docx (20.8KB, docx)

Additional file 1: Supplemental Table 1. Cognitive performance of participants having different reading years.

13195_2022_1098_MOESM2_ESM.docx (22.4KB, docx)

Additional file 2: Supplemental Table 2. Cognitive performance of participants reading different content.

13195_2022_1098_MOESM3_ESM.docx (17.9KB, docx)

Additional file 3: Supplemental Table 3. Cognitive performance of participants using different reading materials.

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank the participants and their caregivers who participated in the study.

Abbreviations

CANDOR

China National Clinical Research Center Alzheimer's Disease and Neurodegenerative Disorder Research

FINGER

Finnish Geriatric Intervention Study to Prevent Cognitive Impairment and Disability

AD

Alzheimer’s disease

MRI

Magnetic resonance imaging

NC

Normal cognition

MCI

Mild cognitive impairment

MMSE

Mini-Mental State Examination

CDR

Clinical Dementia Rating

MoCA

Montreal Cognitive Assessment

RAVLT

Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test

ROCF

Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test

TMT

Trail Making Test

CDT

Clock drawing test

BNT

Boston Naming Test

DST

Digit Span Test

SDMT

Symbol Digit Modalities Test

NPI

Neuropsychiatry Inventory

SD

Standard deviations

OR

Odds ratio

CI

Confidence interval

WM

White matter

eTIV

Estimated total intracranial volume

Authors’ contributions

SL and JS designed the study. YW, SL, and JS did the scientific literature search. YW, SW, SL, NL, CZ, YP, and QW collected data. YW, WZ, SL, and JS analyzed data. YW and SL created the tables. YW, SL, and JS wrote and all authors edited the report. All authors read and approve the manuscript.

Funding

This study is supported by the Beijing Municipal Science & Technology Commission (Grant No. Z181100001518005) and the Strategic Priority Research Program of the Chinese Academy of Sciences (XDB39000000).

Availability of data and materials

SL and JS had full access to all of the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.

Declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

This protocol was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Beijing Tiantan Hospital (approval number: KY 2019-004-007) and was in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations. Written informed consent was obtained from each participant.

Consent for publication

Not involving any person’s data in any form.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Contributor Information

Shiping Li, Email: drlishiping@163.com.

Jiong Shi, Email: jiongshi@ncrcnd.org.cn.

References

  • 1.Patnode CD, Perdue LA, Rossom RC, Rushkin MC, Redmond N, Thomas RG, et al. Screening for Cognitive Impairment in Older Adults: Updated Evidence Report and Systematic Review for the US Preventive Services Task Force. JAMA. 2020;323(8):764–785. doi: 10.1001/jama.2019.22258. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Chan KY, Wang W, Wu JJ, Liu L, Theodoratou E, Car J, et al. Epidemiology of Alzheimer's disease and other forms of dementia in China, 1990–2010: a systematic review and analysis. Lancet. 2013;381(9882):2016–2023. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(13)60221-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Lisko I, Kulmala J, Annetorp M, Ngandu T, Mangialasche F, Kivipelto M. How can dementia and disability be prevented in older adults: where are we today and where are we going? J Intern Med. 2021;289(6):807–830. doi: 10.1111/joim.13227. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Livingston G, Huntley J, Sommerlad A, Ames D, Ballard C, Banerjee S, et al. Dementia prevention, intervention, and care: 2020 report of the Lancet Commission. Lancet. 2020;396(10248):413–446. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30367-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Ngandu T, Lehtisalo J, Solomon A, Levälahti E, Ahtiluoto S, Antikainen R, et al. A 2 year multidomain intervention of diet, exercise, cognitive training, and vascular risk monitoring versus control to prevent cognitive decline in at-risk elderly people (FINGER): a randomised controlled trial. Lancet. 2015;385(9984):2255–2263. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(15)60461-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Lee ATC, Richards M, Chan WC, Chiu HFK, Lee RSY, Lam LCW. Association of Daily Intellectual Activities With Lower Risk of Incident Dementia Among Older Chinese Adults. JAMA Psychiatry. 2018;75(7):697. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2018.0657. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Wilson RS, Boyle PA, Yu L, Barnes LL, Schneider JA, Bennett DA. Life-span cognitive activity, neuropathologic burden, and cognitive aging. Neurology. 2013;81(4):314–321. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31829c5e8a. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Hall K, Gao S, Unverzagt F, Hendrie H. Low education and childhood rural residence: risk for Alzheimer's disease in African Americans. Neurology. 2000;54(1):95–99. doi: 10.1212/wnl.54.1.95. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Valenzuela M, Sachdev P. Brain reserve and cognitive decline: a non-parametric systematic review. Psychol Med. 2006;36(8):1065–1073. doi: 10.1017/s0033291706007744. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Chen G, Zhao M, Yang K, Lin H, Han C, Wang X, et al. Education Exerts Different Effects on Cognition in Individuals with Subjective Cognitive Decline and Cognitive Impairment: A Population-Based Study. J Alzheimers Dis. 2021;79(2):653–661. doi: 10.3233/JAD-201170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Bento-Torres NV, Bento-Torres J, Tomas AM, Costa VO, Correa PG, Costa CN, et al. Influence of schooling and age on cognitive performance in healthy older adults. Braz J Med Biol Res. 2017;50(4):e5892. doi: 10.1590/1414-431X20165892. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Schneider AL, Sharrett AR, Patel MD, Alonso A, Coresh J, Mosley T, et al. Education and cognitive change over 15 years: the atherosclerosis risk in communities study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2012;60(10):1847–1853. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2012.04164.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Wilson RS, Yu L, Lamar M, Schneider JA, Boyle PA, Bennett DA. Education and cognitive reserve in old age. Neurology. 2019;92(10):e1041–e1e50. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000007036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Johnson AS, Flicker LJ, Lichtenberg PA. Reading ability mediates the relationship between education and executive function tasks. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2006;12(1):64–71. doi: 10.1017/S1355617706060073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.O'Carroll R, Ebmeier K. Education and prevalence of Alzheimer's disease and vascular dementia. Premorbid ability influences measures used to identify dementia. BMJ. 1995;311(6997):125–126. doi: 10.1136/bmj.311.6997.125b. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Mougias A, Christidi F, Kaldi M, Kerossi MI, Athanasouli P, Politis A. Mini-Mental State Examination: Greek Normative Data Stratified by Age and Education in a Large Sample of 925 Community-Dwelling Healthy Participants. Adv Exp Med Biol. 2020;1196:93–102. doi: 10.1007/978-3-030-32637-1_9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Lezak MD. Neuropsychological Assessment, (2nd ed) New York: Oxford University Press; 1983. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Zhang X, Lv L, Min G, Wang Q, Zhao Y, Li Y. Overview of the Complex Figure Test and Its Clinical Application in Neuropsychiatric Disorders, Including Copying and Recall. Front Neurol. 2021;12:680474. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2021.680474. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Spreen O, Strauss E. A compendium of neuropsychological tests: administration, norms, and commentary. 2nd ed. New York: Oxford University Press; 1998
  • 20.Lezak MDHD, Loring DW, Hannay HJ, Fischer JS. Neuropsychological Assessment. 4th ed. New York: Oxford University Press; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Verghese J, Lipton RB, Katz MJ, Hall CB, Derby CA, Kuslansky G, et al. Leisure activities and the risk of dementia in the elderly. N Engl J Med. 2003;348(25):2508–2516. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa022252. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Stephen R, Liu Y, Ngandu T, Antikainen R, Hulkkonen J, Koikkalainen J, et al. Brain volumes and cortical thickness on MRI in the Finnish Geriatric Intervention Study to Prevent Cognitive Impairment and Disability (FINGER) Alzheimers Res Ther. 2019;11(1):53. doi: 10.1186/s13195-019-0506-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Kristanto D, Liu M, Liu X, Sommer W, Zhou C. Predicting reading ability from brain anatomy and function: From areas to connections. Neuroimage. 2020;218:116966. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116966. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Fotuhi M, Do D, Jack C. Modifiable factors that alter the size of the hippocampus with ageing. Nat Rev Neurol. 2012;8(4):189–202. doi: 10.1038/nrneurol.2012.27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Bruandet A, Richard F, Bombois S, Maurage CA, Masse I, Amouyel P, et al. Cognitive decline and survival in Alzheimer's disease according to education level. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2008;25(1):74–80. doi: 10.1159/000111693. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Cabeza R, Albert M, Belleville S, Craik F, Duarte A, Grady C, et al. Maintenance, reserve and compensation: the cognitive neuroscience of healthy ageing. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2018;19(11):701–710. doi: 10.1038/s41583-018-0068-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Stern Y, Arenaza-Urquijo E, Bartrés-Faz D, Belleville S, Cantilon M, Chetelat G, et al. Whitepaper: Defining and investigating cognitive reserve, brain reserve, and brain maintenance. Alzheimers Dementia. 2020;16(9):1305–1311. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2018.07.219. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Richards M, Deary IJ. A life course approach to cognitive reserve: a model for cognitive aging and development? Ann Neurol. 2005;58(4):617–622. doi: 10.1002/ana.20637. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.O'Toole KJ, Kannass KN. Emergent literacy in print and electronic contexts: The influence of book type, narration source, and attention. J Exp Child Psychol. 2018;173:100–115. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2018.03.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

13195_2022_1098_MOESM1_ESM.docx (20.8KB, docx)

Additional file 1: Supplemental Table 1. Cognitive performance of participants having different reading years.

13195_2022_1098_MOESM2_ESM.docx (22.4KB, docx)

Additional file 2: Supplemental Table 2. Cognitive performance of participants reading different content.

13195_2022_1098_MOESM3_ESM.docx (17.9KB, docx)

Additional file 3: Supplemental Table 3. Cognitive performance of participants using different reading materials.

Data Availability Statement

SL and JS had full access to all of the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.


Articles from Alzheimer's Research & Therapy are provided here courtesy of BMC

RESOURCES