Abstract
This work aims to address whether knowing what dark tourism is (or not) impacts rumination on sadness, self-hatred, hostility, psychological vulnerability, and tourist wellbeing, as well as practices and motivations for dark tourism. A quantitative approach, based on a survey of 993 respondents, reveals that women and more educated participants know more about dark tourism; people who know what dark tourism is have visited more Holocaust museums, sites of human tragedy and natural disasters, concentration camps, and prisons; show more curiosity, need to learn and understand, and need to see morbid things. A model was found showing that gender, age, know/do not know dark tourism, and motivations (curiosity, the need to learn, the need to understand, and pleasure) explained 38.1% of a dark tourism practice index. Most findings also indicate that rumination on sadness, self-hatred, hostility, and psychological vulnerability are associated with darker practices. Greater wellbeing was not found in participants who knew in advance what dark tourism was. Interestingly, participants who visit tragic human sites present higher values in hostility and tourist wellbeing than those who do not. In summary, people who visit more dark places and score higher on negative personality characteristics have higher values of tourist wellbeing.
Keywords: dark tourism, motivations, practices, dark tourist, tourist profile, tourism wellbeing
1. Introduction
Many people are increasingly looking for new and unique touristic experiences to satisfy a wide range of motivations. That has driven the segmentation and the emergence of increasingly specific typologies, such as dark tourism, that, in contrast with mass tourism, are characterized by a high degree of diversification and individualization. Dark tourism comprises visiting real or recreated places related with death, suffering, disgrace, or the macabre [1,2]. From the perspective of dark tourism places, it is important to understand what drives people to visit them to design satisfying experiences. We may think of death as an obvious motivation, often part of the site’s history, but it is not always the primary or explicitly recognized motivation for a visit. Sharpley and Stone [3] admitted that the field of motivation to visit dark tourism destinations remains an understudied area, although recent literature has provided an increasing number of empirical studies about the reasons for visiting those sites [4,5].
This research intends to contribute to the dark tourism literature by seeking to understand whether people know what dark tourism is and identify a differentiated sociodemographic, motivational, and tourist practice profile between people who know and do not know what dark tourism is. In addition, it aims to understand if dark tourists’ motivations for visiting dark tourism destinations explain their practices. The research approach relies on empirically exploring the motivations, practices, and sociodemographic characteristics of a sample of 933 people that participated in a survey held in Portugal.
The remainder of the text is organized as follows: firstly, a brief theoretical background is put forward, focused on the dark tourism concept and dark tourists’ motivations and practices; then, the quantitative study’s applied methods and obtained results are described; finally, the results are discussed, and conclusions and implications are drawn.
2. Theoretical Background
Despite the fact that some authors consider it one of the older forms of tourism, it has gained great popularity amongst academics from the 1990s onwards [3], confirmed by the significant volume of literature published ever since [4,5,6]. However, understanding the demand for this type of tourism persists as poorly defined and theoretically fragile [3,4,7,8]. For a long time, places that have been the scene of wars, disasters, deaths, and atrocities have always fascinated people, motivating them to travel [3,9]. Sharpley and Stone [3] often use the term dark tourism as the type of tourism that encompasses traveling to sites related to death, suffering, and macabre—a globally accepted definition. However, Tarlow [10] implies the phenomenon is complex by describing it as “visits to places where noteworthy historical tragedies or deaths have occurred that continue to impact our lives”, which raises the question about the inherent motives to consume dark tourism.
2.1. Dark Tourists and Their Motivation to Dark Tourism Consumption
Stone’s (2006) idea of dark tourism goes far beyond related attractions. From this standpoint, diverse well-visited tourist sites may become places of dark tourism due to their history linked with death—e.g., suicides in the Eiffel Tower, tombs in the pyramids of Egypt, the Valley of the Kings, and the Taj Mahal, funeral art at the Cairo Museum, and terrorist attacks in Ground Zero [11]. Ashworth and Isaac [12] also suggest that all tourist places have a greater or lesser potential of being perceived as “dark.” Accordingly, the same dark tourism place can evoke different experiences in different visitors (i.e., a site one visitor sees as “dark” may not be for another); thus, the authors argue that no site is intrinsically, automatically, and universally “dark,” as, even they may be labeled as dark, they are not always perceived as such by all visitors.
Walter [13] states that most dark tourism is not specifically motivated, comprising only parallel visits inserted in a trip of a wider reach. Nonetheless, the literature indicates that tourists who visit dark places are not a homogeneous group, and neither the factors inherent to the visitation are the same. Moreover, the “darker” motivation can undertake distinctive levels of intensity. Consequently, in addition to the fascination and interest in death [12,14,15], the visit to this type of place is also motivated by personal, cultural, and psychological reasons [4] or driven by entertainment purposes [7,16].
The literature indicates numerous reasons to visit dark tourism sites: educational experience, desire to learn and understand past events, and historical interest [7,17,18,19,20], as self-discovery purposes [17], identity [7], memory, remembrance, celebration, nostalgia, empathy, contemplation, and homage [10,17,20], curiosity [17,19,20,21], the search for novelty, authenticity, and adventure [2,20], convenience when visiting other places [19], and also status, prestige, affirmation, and recognition that these visits provide [22]. To a lesser extent, the literature also mentions religious and pilgrimage reasons, feelings of guilt, a search for social responsibility, or heritage experience.
The desire to learn and understand stands out as a motive associated with sites of death and/or heritage. Whereas some visitors exhibit a considerable need for emotional experience and connection to their heritage, engaging, as Slade puts it [23], in a “profound heritage experience”, and emotionally to the “dark” space influence [24], other visitors may be knowledge-seekers, who are more interested in a knowledge-enriching experience [25] than an emotional one and look for gaining a deeper understanding. Isaac et al. [20] found that memory, gaining knowledge and awareness, and exclusivity were important motivations for dark tourists; also, “(…), consuming dark tourism may allow the individual a sense of meaning and understanding of past disaster and macabre events that have perturbed life projects” [2]. Tourists’ interest in places associated with death and tragedy may also be related to educational goals [9].
Curiosity and the need to learn and understand are entwined. Dark tourism develops curiosity and satisfies the desire for knowledge of past suffering and pain [26]. Ashworth (2004) and Ashworth and Hartmann [27] suggested three main reasons for visiting dark sites: curiosity about the unusual, attraction to horror, and a desire for empathy or identification with the victims of atrocity. Yan, Zhang, Zhang, Lu and Guo [24] refer to the curious type of dark tourist who engages cognitively by learning about the issue. From another perspective, dark tourists may feel motivated by morbid tourism [28] and show interest in specific macabre exhibitions and museums [29] and fascination with evil [30], given the morbid nature of dark tourism [31]. Other authors present yet other motives: secular pilgrimage; a desire for inner purification; schadenfreude or malicious joy; “ghoulish titillation”; a search for the otherness of death; an interest in personal genealogy and family history; a search for “authentic” places in a commodified world; and a desire to encounter the pure/impure sacred [18]. Iliev [4] concludes that although tourists visit places related to death, they may not necessarily be considered dark tourists; as already acknowledged, those sites may not be experienced as “dark” by each visitor. It is, therefore, imperative that the so-called dark tourists are considered as such based on their experience.
2.2. Dark Tourist Personality
Some authors who study dark tourism have tried to relate dark tourist practice with personality characteristics, namely with the dark triad—psychoticism, narcissism, and Machiavellianism [16,32,33,34]. However, the nature of dark tourism, especially that related to the Holocaust, can be so complex that the personality characteristics that motivate it may be less central, so we decided to study the following characteristics: rumination in sadness, self-hatred, hostility, and psychological vulnerability.
Rumination about sadness includes “repetitive thoughts concerning one’s present distress and the circumstances surrounding the sadness” [35]. These thoughts are related to the nature of one’s negative affect, are not goal-directed nor lead to plans for solutional action [36], and are not socially shared while the rumination occurs. Thus, rumination on sadness presents a negative content, “does not facilitate problem resolution, is a solitary activity, and is intrusive if the person is pursuing either self-or situationally imposed task-oriented goals” [35].
Nolen-Hoeksema and Morrow’s [36] measure of rumination focuses on ideation, contrary to expression or disclosure, but it also includes disclosing feelings to others and emotional expressiveness as components of rumination. According to Nolen-Hoeksema and Morrow [36], ruminative responses are different from structured problem-solving because people only think or talk about how “unmotivated, sad, and lethargic they feel” (p. 569). Despite that, Nolen-Hoeksema and Morrow’s [36] stated that ruminative responses include telling others how badly one feels. Although rumination has negative consequences, disclosure may have positive effects [37]; also, some forms of emotional expressiveness, a component of disclosure, seem beneficial [38].
Self-hatred is an “enduring dysfunctional and destructive self-evaluation, characterized by attributions of undesirable and defective qualities, and failure to meet perceived standards and values leading to feelings of inadequacy, incompetency, and worthlessness” [39]. High self-hatred is related to low self-esteem, shame, self-blame or guilt, and a mental state of agitation, raising an experience of psychological and emotional turmoil [39].
According to Derogatis and Melisaratos [40], hostility captures thoughts, feelings, and actions associated with hostile behavior. Although the hostility scale measures perceived levels of expressed hostility rather than actual levels of outwardly expressed hostility, the hostility scale is significantly associated with anger [41], and high anger is related to outward, uncontrolled, and negative expressions of anger [42].
Psychological vulnerability is the “individual’s capacity to deal with mechanisms of maintaining emotional strength, in case of a pessimistic point of view, due to the lack of social support” [43]. Psychological vulnerability is a pattern of cognitive beliefs translating to “a dependence on achievement or external sources of affirmation for one’s sense of self-worth” [44]. Psychological vulnerability is negatively associated with positive affect, self-efficacy, and social support and positively associated with negative affect, perceived powerlessness, and maladaptive coping behavior [43,44]. Dark tourists are subjects situated in emotionally sensitive spaces [45] that can trigger their psychological vulnerability.
2.3. Research Questions
Although research on dark tourism has increased in recent years, there are not enough studies exploring if people’s knowledge of this phenomenon and their personality traits lead to distinctive dark tourists’ motivations and behaviors. Taking into account the aforementioned motivations to visit dark tourism places, the present study intends to empirically explore if dark tourists’ personality characteristics and sociodemographic variables impact such motivations and dark tourists’ practices and wellbeing (the latter, measured as a dark tourism practice index, given the diversity of known dark tourism practices). Specifically, our research questions are: Do rumination on sadness, self-hatred, hostility, and psychological vulnerability explain the practices and motivations for dark tourism and thus explain tourist wellbeing? Does knowing what dark tourism is (or not) impact rumination on sadness, self-hatred, hostility, and psychological vulnerability, as well as practices and motivations for dark tourism and tourist wellbeing?
3. Materials and Methods
Given the research questions, the aims of the present study are as follows: (1) to find the sociodemographic differences in touristic practices and motivations for dark tourism according to two groups (those who knew what dark tourism is and those who did not know); (2) to assess the fit of the rumination on the sadness scale, self-hatred scale, hostility scale, psychological vulnerability scale, and tourism wellbeing scale; (3) to determine the differences in rumination on sadness, self-hatred, hostility, psychological vulnerability, and tourism wellbeing according to two groups (those who knew what dark tourism is and those who didn’t know); (4) to find the differences in rumination on sadness, self-hatred, hostility, psychological vulnerability, and tourism wellbeing according to practices and motivations for dark tourism; and (5) to determine variables that contribute to the dark tourism practice index. Accordingly, we hypothesize:
H1.
Participants who know what dark tourism is are younger and have more education than those who do not.
H2.
Participants who know what dark tourism is are more motivated and visit more places associated with dark tourism than those who do not.
H3.
All measures show a good fit for the sample.
H4.
Differences in rumination on sadness, self-hatred, hostility, psychological vulnerability, and tourism wellbeing according to two groups (those who knew what dark tourism is and those who did not know) will be found.
H5.
Differences in rumination on sadness, self-hatred, hostility, psychological vulnerability, and tourism wellbeing according to practices and motivations for dark tourism will be found.
H6.
Gender, age, to know/know not dark tourism, and the motivations of curiosity, need to learn, need to understand, and pleasure will contribute to explaining dark tourism practice.
3.1. Procedures
All procedures followed the Declaration of Helsinki and later amendments or comparable ethical standards. The investigation protocol included informed consent, and confidentiality and anonymity of the data were guaranteed. The research protocol was applied in person to a random sample of participants between 18 October and 17 December 2021. The participants were informed about the study’s purpose and were ensured confidentiality and anonymity of the data; they also signed informed consent. The inclusion criteria consisted of being over 18 years old, Portuguese, and having touristic experiences. The respondents were approached by two researchers and five MSc students on the University’s campuses and within their informal networks, with the questionnaire being self-administered.
3.2. Instruments
The instruments that were not validated for the Portuguese population—the Rumination on Sadness Scale (RSS) and the Self-Hatred Scale (SHS)—were first translated from English to Portuguese by two bilingual translators, one from and another not from the field of psychology. Then, a third bilingual translator from the field of psychology provided a reconciliation of the two translations. Next, a native English speaker not from the psychology field independently performed the reconciled version’s back-translation. Finally, the first translator reviewed the back-translated version of the scale and compared it with the original English version to ensure linguistic and cultural equivalence consistency.
Sociodemographic questionnaire
The sociodemographic questionnaire included questions related to gender (feminine—0; masculine—1), age, education (no education–0; primary education—1; secondary education—2; higher education—3), marital status (no relationship-single, divorced, separated, widowed–0; in a relationship-boyfriends, married, de facto union—1), and employment status (inactive—unemployed, retired, on sick leave–0; active-student, employee, housewife, caregivers—1).
Questionnaire about dark tourism’s practices
The questionnaire on dark tourism practices includes a question about knowledge of dark tourism (or not). In addition, it also asked participants about their tourist practices related to dark tourism (Have you ever visited…? cemeteries; holocaust museums; sites of human tragedy; concentration camps; prisons; sites of war; sites of natural disasters; stop to see accidents). All these questions are answered dichotomously (no—0; yes—1).
Questionnaire about dark tourism´s motivations
This questionnaire includes the presentation of several reasons to visit a dark place: curiosity, the need to learn, the need to see, the need to understand, pleasure, and the need to see morbid things. All these questions are answered dichotomously (no—0; yes—1).
Rumination on Sadness Scale (RSS)
The Rumination on Sadness Scale, an individual-difference measure of rumination on sadness, was developed by Conway et al. [35] as an alternative to the Ruminative Responses Scale of the Response Styles Questionnaire (RRRSQ; [36]). It is a unifactorial scale with 13 items. Higher ratings indicate higher levels of rumination on sadness. Cronbach’s alpha, the internal reliability coefficient, was 0.91 in the original version. Since there is no Portuguese version of this scale, it will be validated in this study.
Self-Hatred Scale (SHS)
The Self-Hatred Scale was developed by Turnell et al. [39] to assess individuals’ levels of self-hatred. Since self-hatred is a significant predictor of suicidal ideation, this scale has the potential to be helpful in suicide risk assessment. Higher ratings indicate higher levels of self-hatred. Cronbach’s alpha was 0.95 in the original version. There is no Portuguese version of this scale, so it will also be validated in this study.
BSI Hostility Scale (HSS)
BSI Hostility Scale (HS) is a subscale of the Brief Symptoms Inventory [BSI; [40]], whose Portuguese version is from Canavarro [46]. BSI is a 53-item measure to identify self-reported clinically relevant psychological symptoms in adolescents and adults. The BSI covers nine symptom dimensions: Somatization, Obsession-Compulsion, Interpersonal Sensitivity, Depression, Anxiety, Hostility, Phobic Anxiety, Paranoid Ideation, and Psychoticism; and three global indices of distress: Global Severity Index, Positive Symptom Distress Index, and Positive Symptom Total. The Hostility subscale includes five items, and higher ratings indicate higher levels of hostility. In the original version, the alpha coefficients for the nine dimensions of the scale ranged from 0.64 in the Psychoticism dimension to 0.81 in the Somatization dimension. In the Portuguese version, the alpha coefficients ranged from 0.71 in the Psychoticism dimension to 0.85 in the Depression dimension.
Psychological Vulnerability Scale (PVS)
The Psychological Vulnerability Scale (PVS) was designed to obtain information about maladaptive cognitive patterns, such as dependence, perfectionism, need for external sources of approval, and generalized negative attributions. The PVS is a six-item scale with higher scores indicating greater psychological vulnerability. In the original version [44], Cronbach’s α coefficient ranged from 0.71 to 0.87 for different samples; in the Portuguese version [47], Cronbach’s α coefficient was 0.73.
Tourism Wellbeing Scale (TWS)
The Tourism Wellbeing Scale (TWS) was developed by [48] Garcês et al. (2018 [49]); it aims to evaluate tourism wellbeing in each destination, having been built from positive psychology variables, namely, wellbeing, creativity, optimism, and spirituality. It is a unifactorial scale with eight items. Higher ratings indicate higher levels of tourism wellbeing. Cronbach’s alpha was 0.97 in the original version.
3.3. Data Analysis
Prior to analysis, the normality of items was examined by skewness (SI) and kurtosis (KI) indexes; absolute values of SI less than 3 and KI less than 10 indicate a normal distribution of the data. [50]. All the instruments were subject to a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) procedure with maximum likelihood estimation (MLE). The model fit evaluation was based on test statistics and approximate fit indexes, following the thresholds presented in Kline [50]. Thus, a non-significant model chi-square statistic, χ2, states that the model fits the data acceptably in the population; the higher the probability related to χ2, the closer the fit to the perfect fit. A value of the parsimony-corrected index Steiger–Lind root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) close to 0 represents a good fit; RMSEA ≤ 0.05 may indicate a good fit, but the upper bound of the 90% confidence interval exceeding 0.10 may indicate poor fit; also, this test should be non-significant at the 0.05 level. Values of incremental fit index (IFI), Tucker–Lewis index (TLI), and the Bentler incremental comparative fit index (CFI), close to 1 (0.95 or better), are indicators of best fit; also, the standardized root mean square residual (SRMR), a statistic related to the correlation residuals (SRMR over 0.10 suggests fit problems) was used; the smallest the values, the most parsimonious is the model.
Besides goodness-of-fit index evaluation, model re-specification involved analyzing path estimates, standardized residuals of items, and modification indices for all non-estimated parameters. The modifications indices (MI) provide information about potential cross-loadings and error term correlations not specified in the model and the expected change in the chi-square value for each fixed parameter if it were to be freed. Only modifications theoretically meaningful and MI > 11 were considered. Finally, Cronbach’s alpha coefficients were calculated to ascertain the model’s reliability.
Group differences were analyzed. The independent t-test was applied to compare the means of the two groups. In addition, chi-squared was used to compare distributions’ differences and Mann–Whitney test to compare ordinal data. Three measures of the effect size, Cohen’s d, the eta squared, phi, and rank biserial correlation were used according to the variables’ measurement level; interpretation followed Cohen’s [51] guidelines; the statistical significance level was set at 0.05. Statistical analysis was performed using SPSS version 28 and AMOS version 28.
4. Results
The sample includes 993 participants, mainly female, in a romantic relationship, with secondary or university education, and active; the mean age is around 31 years. Statistically significant differences were found concerning age and education between the sample that had already heard about dark tourism and knew what it was and the sample that had not yet heard about it. Participants who had heard about dark tourism were significantly younger and more educated than those who had not (Table 1).
Table 1.
Sample sociodemographic characteristics.
Sociodemographic Variables | N (%) | n (%) | n (%) | χ2 | p | Φ | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Total | Know Not Dark Tourism |
Know Dark Tourism |
|||||
Sample | 993 (100.0) | 600 (60.4) | 393 (39.6) | ||||
Gender | Female | 574 (57.8) | 343 (57.2) | 231 (68.8) | 0.253 | 0.615 | −0.016 |
Male | 419 (42.2) | 257 (42.8) | 162 (41.2) | ||||
Marital status | No relation | 377 (38.0) | 224 (37.3) | 153 (38.9) | 0.257 | 0.612 | −0.016 |
In a relation | 616 (62.0) | 376 (62.7) | 240 (61.1) | ||||
Education | No education | 9 (0.9) | 6 (1.0) | 3 (0.8) | 18.955 | <0.001 | 0.139 |
Primary | 77 (7.8) | 63 (10.5) | 14 (3,6) | ||||
Secondary | 437 (44.0) | 268 (44.7) | 169 (43.0) | ||||
University | 470 (47.3) | 263 (43.8) | 207 (52.7) | ||||
Professional status | Inactive | 110 (11.1) | 72 (12.0) | 38 (9.7) | 1.310 | 0.252 | 0.036 |
Active | 883 (88.9) | 528 (88.0) | 355 (90.3) | ||||
t | p | d | |||||
Age | M ± SD; Min–Max | 31.28 ± 14.45; 18–87 | 32.41 ± 15.07; 18–87 | 29.56 ± 13.29; 18–79 | 3.136 | 0.002 | 0.198 |
Notes: N = frequencies; % = percentage; M = mean; SD = standard deviation; χ2 = qui-squared test; Φ = Phi size effect; t = t-test; Cohen’s d = size effect; p = p-value. In bold: statistically significant values.
Concerning the total sample and dark tourism practices, most people have visited cemeteries, and about a third of the sample stopped to see accidents. On the other hand, about a quarter of the sample already had other practices, except for a visit to concentration camps, which was only carried out by about 14% of the total sample. The same trend remains in the sample that has not yet heard about dark tourism and the sample that has. However, there are statistically significant differences between these two samples regarding practices related to dark tourism, being that the sample that has already heard about dark tourism visits many more Holocaust museums, sites of human tragedy, concentration camps, prisons, and sites of natural disasters than the sample that has not yet heard about dark tourism (Table 2).
Table 2.
Dark tourism practices.
Total | Know Not Dark Tourism |
Know Dark Tourism |
||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Sample | N | % | n | % | n | % | χ2 | p | Φ | |
Have you ever visited…? | 993 | 100 | 600 | 60.4 | 393 | 39.6 | ||||
Cemeteries | No | 291 | 29.3 | 178 | 29.7 | 113 | 28.8 | 0.096 | 0.057 | 0.010 |
Yes | 702 | 70.7 | 422 | 70.3 | 280 | 71.2 | ||||
Holocaust Museums | No | 762 | 76.7 | 480 | 80.0 | 282 | 71.8 | 9.041 | 0.003 | 0.095 |
Yes | 231 | 23.3 | 120 | 20.0 | 111 | 28.2 | ||||
Sites of Human Tragedy | No | 768 | 77.3 | 490 | 81.7 | 278 | 70.7 | 16.184 | <0.001 | 0.128 |
Yes | 225 | 22.7 | 110 | 18.3 | 115 | 29.3 | ||||
Concentration Camps | No | 855 | 86.1 | 531 | 88.5 | 324 | 82.4 | 7.281 | 0.007 | 0.086 |
Yes | 138 | 13.9 | 69 | 11.5 | 69 | 17.6 | ||||
Prisons | No | 748 | 75.3 | 479 | 79.8 | 269 | 68.4 | 16.563 | <0.001 | 0.129 |
Yes | 245 | 24.7 | 121 | 20.2 | 124 | 31.6 | ||||
Sites of War | No | 783 | 78.9 | 485 | 80.8 | 298 | 75.8 | 3.569 | 0.059 | 0.060 |
Yes | 210 | 21.1 | 115 | 19.2 | 95 | 24.2 | ||||
Sites of Natural Disasters | No | 769 | 77.4 | 485 | 80.8 | 284 | 72.3 | 9.980 | 0.002 | 0.100 |
Yes | 224 | 22.6 | 115 | 19.2 | 109 | 27.7 | ||||
Stop to see accidents | No | 622 | 62.6 | 380 | 63.3 | 242 | 61.6 | 0.313 | 0.576 | 0.018 |
Yes | 371 | 37.4 | 220 | 36.7 | 151 | 38.4 |
Notes: N = frequencies; % = percentage; χ2 = qui-squared test; Φ = Phi size effect; p = p-value. In bold: statistically significant values.
As for the reasons behind the desire to visit dark places, curiosity stands out in the total sample, with the least chosen reason being the need to see morbid things. The same trend can be seen in the two subsamples. However, there are statistically significant differences between these two samples regarding motives to visit dark places, being that the sample that has already heard about dark tourism presents higher values in the motives related to curiosity, the need to learn and understand, and the need to see morbid things than the sample that has not yet heard about dark tourism (Table 3).
Table 3.
Dark tourism motives.
Total | Know Not Dark Tourism |
Know Dark Tourism |
||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Sample | N | % | n | % | n | % | χ2 | p | Φ | |
Motives to visit | 993 | 100 | 600 | 60.4 | 393 | 39.6 | ||||
Curiosity | No | 417 | 42.0 | 273 | 45.5 | 144 | 36.6 | 7.605 | 0.006 | 0.088 |
Yes | 576 | 58.0 | 327 | 54.5 | 249 | 63.4 | ||||
Need to learn | No | 675 | 68.0 | 433 | 72.2 | 242 | 61.6 | 12.231 | <0.001 | 0.111 |
Yes | 318 | 32.0 | 167 | 27.8 | 151 | 38.4 | ||||
Need to see | No | 636 | 64.0 | 392 | 65.3 | 244 | 62.1 | 1.087 | 0.297 | 0.033 |
Yes | 357 | 36.0 | 208 | 34.7 | 149 | 37.9 | ||||
Need to understand | No | 591 | 59.5 | 390 | 65.0 | 201 | 51.1 | 18.919 | <0.001 | 0.138 |
Yes | 402 | 40.5 | 210 | 35.0 | 192 | 48.9 | ||||
Pleasure | No | 913 | 91.9 | 557 | 92.8 | 356 | 90.6 | 1.620 | 0.203 | 0.040 |
Yes | 80 | 8.1 | 43 | 7.2 | 37 | 9.4 | ||||
Need to see morbid things | No | 941 | 94.8 | 576 | 96.0 | 365 | 92.9 | 4.672 | 0.031 | 0.069 |
Yes | 52 | 5.2 | 24 | 4.0 | 28 | 7.1 |
Notes: N = frequencies; % = percentage; χ2 = qui-squared test; Φ = Phi size effect; p = p-value. In bold: statistically significant values.
Table 4 shows the descriptive statistics related to the items of the instruments used in this study: the rumination on sadness, tourism wellbeing, self-hatred, hostility, and psychological vulnerability. The skewness and kurtosis values are all within the normative values, ensuring the normality of the distribution, except for item SHS3 whose values are slightly above the recommended one.
Table 4.
Items’ frequencies.
Minimum | Maximum | Mean | Standard Deviation |
Skewness | Kurtosis | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Rumination on sadness (RSS) |
||||||
RSS1 | 1 | 5 | 2.31 | 1.18 | 0.62 | −0.46 |
RSS2 | 1 | 5 | 2.68 | 1.28 | 0.30 | −0.98 |
RSS3 | 1 | 5 | 2.62 | 1.30 | 0.32 | −0.99 |
RSS4 | 1 | 5 | 2.26 | 1.20 | 0.69 | −0.52 |
RSS5 | 1 | 5 | 2.11 | 1.28 | 0.92 | −0.34 |
RSS6 | 1 | 5 | 1.85 | 1.16 | 1.28 | 0.68 |
RSS7 | 1 | 5 | 2.25 | 1.30 | 0.70 | −0.69 |
RSS8 | 1 | 5 | 1.54 | 0.96 | 1.94 | 3.31 |
RSS9 | 1 | 5 | 1.93 | 1.17 | 1.13 | 0.29 |
RSS10 | 1 | 5 | 2.21 | 1.17 | 0.72 | −0.38 |
RSS11 | 1 | 5 | 2.19 | 1.23 | 0.68 | −0.65 |
RSS12 | 1 | 5 | 2.94 | 1.28 | 0.02 | −1.06 |
RSS13 | 1 | 5 | 2.00 | 1.25 | 1.06 | −0.04 |
Self-hatred (SHS) | ||||||
SHS1 | 1 | 7 | 1.52 | 1.12 | 2.37 | 5.14 |
SHS2 | 1 | 7 | 1.73 | 1.28 | 1.95 | 3.23 |
SHS3 | 1 | 7 | 1.31 | 0.88 | 3.59 | 13.83 |
SHS4 | 1 | 7 | 1.65 | 1.24 | 2.10 | 3.76 |
SHS5 | 1 | 7 | 1.52 | 1.14 | 2.56 | 6.40 |
SHS6 | 1 | 7 | 1.79 | 1.50 | 2.05 | 3.29 |
SHS7 | 1 | 7 | 1.94 | 1.53 | 1.73 | 2.18 |
Hostility (HSS) | ||||||
BSI_HSS1 | 0 | 4 | 2.33 | 1.23 | −0.17 | −1.02 |
BSI_HSS2 | 0 | 4 | 1.31 | 1.22 | 0.70 | −0.43 |
BSI_HSS3 | 0 | 4 | 0.85 | 1.01 | 1.32 | 1.34 |
BSI_HSS4 | 0 | 4 | 0.98 | 1.13 | 1.18 | 0.70 |
BSI_HSS5 | 0 | 4 | 1.57 | 1.27 | 0.46 | −0.85 |
Psychological vulnerability (PVS) |
||||||
PVS1 | 1 | 5 | 2.85 | 1.37 | 0.19 | −1.18 |
PVS2 | 1 | 5 | 2.73 | 1.36 | 0.27 | −1.12 |
PVS3 | 1 | 5 | 2.02 | 1.28 | 1.03 | −0.16 |
PVS4 | 1 | 5 | 2.00 | 1.19 | 1.04 | 0.07 |
PVS5 | 1 | 5 | 2.53 | 1.36 | 0.44 | −1.04 |
PVS6 | 1 | 5 | 3.19 | 1.44 | −0.16 | −1.33 |
Tourism wellbeing (TWB) | ||||||
TWBS1 | 1 | 7 | 4.50 | 1.78 | −0.43 | −0.69 |
TWBS2 | 1 | 7 | 5.74 | 1.54 | −1.31 | 1.06 |
TWBS3 | 1 | 7 | 4.89 | 1.57 | −0.54 | −0.39 |
TWBS4 | 1 | 7 | 5.41 | 1.66 | −0.97 | 0.11 |
TWBS5 | 1 | 7 | 4.30 | 1.92 | −0.28 | −1.05 |
TWBS6 | 1 | 7 | 4.18 | 2.00 | −0.17 | −1.13 |
TWBS7 | 1 | 7 | 3.48 | 2.03 | 0.23 | −1.21 |
TWBS8 | 1 | 7 | 5.06 | 1.76 | −0.75 | −0.28 |
A confirmatory factorial analysis of the rumination on sadness scale was carried out to confirm the authors’ model [χ2(46) = 4.121; CFI = 0.977; TLI = 0.961; IFI = 0.977; RMSEA = 0.056; PCLOSE = 0.107: SMRM = 0.028]; however, to achieve this model fit, some correlations between errors were established (Figure 1).
Figure 1.
Model fit of Rumination on Sadness Scale.
Confirmatory factorial analysis of the self-hatred scale [χ2(11) = 5.118; CFI = 0.992; TLI = 0.984; IFI = 0.992; RMSEA = 0.064; PCLOSE = 0.069: SMRM = 0.015] (Figure 2), hostility scale [χ2(2) = 4.216; CFI = 0.995; TLI = 0.976; IFI = 0.995; RMSEA = 0.057; PCLOSE = 0.317: SMRM = 0.012] (Figure 3), psychological vulnerability scale [χ2(7) = 2.886; CFI = 0.992; TLI = 0.983; IFI = 0.992; RMSEA = 0.044; PCLOSE = 0.644; SMRM = 0.018] (Figure 4), and tourism wellbeing scale [χ2(16) = 3.787; CFI = 0.979; TLI = 0.964; IFI = 0.980; RMSEA = 0.053; PCLOSE = 0.339: SMRM = 0.029] (Figure 5) were carried out to assess the models’ adjustments. Despite finding good fits for all models, some correlations between errors were established to achieve such fits. Thus, hypothesis H3 is confirmed.
Figure 2.
Model fit of Self-hatred Scale.
Figure 3.
Model fit of Hostility Scale.
Figure 4.
Model fit of Psychological Vulnerability Scale.
Figure 5.
Model fit of Tourism Wellbeing Scale.
There are no differences in the values of rumination on sadness, self-hatred, hostility, psychological vulnerability, and tourism wellbeing concerning knowing what dark tourism is or not (Table 5).
Table 5.
Rumination on sadness (RSS), self-hatred (SHS), hostility (HSS), psychological vulnerability (PVS), and tourism wellbeing (TWBS) frequencies and differences between those who know dark tourism and those who do not.
Total | Know Not Dark Tourism |
Know Dark Tourism |
||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
α | M | SD | MR | MR | U | p | r | |
Sample | 993 | 100 | 600 | 393 | ||||
RSS Total | 0.916 | 2.22 | 0.86 | 498.37 | 494.91 | 117,077.500 | 0.852 | −0.0059 |
SHS Total | 0.931 | 1.64 | 1.06 | 504.99 | 484.80 | 113,106.500 | 0.245 | −0.0369 |
HSS Total | 0.790 | 1.41 | 0.87 | 498.43 | 4.94.82 | 117,044.500 | 0.846 | −0.0062 |
PVS Total | 0.788 | 2.55 | 0.93 | 508.70 | 479.13 | 110,878.500 | 0.112 | −0.0505 |
TWBS Total | 0.818 | 4.69 | 1.19 | 486.39 | 513.19 | 111,536.500 | 0.150 | −0.0457 |
Notes: α = Cronbach’s alpha; M = mean; SD = standard deviation; MR–mean rank; U = Mann–Whitney test; p = p-value; r = rank-biserial correlation.
Differences were assessed regarding the values of rumination on sadness, self-hatred, hostility, psychological vulnerability, and tourism wellbeing according to dark tourism practices. Being that only statistically significant results are presented, it was found that participants who visit cemeteries have significantly lower values of self-hatred and psychological vulnerability than participants who report not visiting cemeteries (Table 6). Furthermore, those who visit tragic human sites present higher values in hostility and tourism wellbeing than those who do not. Those who visit sites of war present higher values in self-hatred than those who did not. Those who visit site of natural tragedies also present higher values in hostility and tourism wellbeing. Lastly, those who stop to see accidents present higher values in rumination on sadness, self-hatred, hostility, psychological vulnerability, and tourism wellbeing than those who do not stop (Table 6).
Table 6.
Rumination on sadness (RSS), self-hatred (SHS), hostility (HSS), psychological vulnerability (PVS) and tourism wellbeing (TWBS) frequencies and differences according to dark tourism practices.
No Visit | Visit | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Sample | MR | MR | U | p | r |
Cemeteries | |||||
SHS Total | 550.65 | 474.76 | 86,528.500 | <0.001 | −0.1291 |
PVS Total | 592.94 | 483.34 | 92,554.500 | 0.020 | −0–0741 |
Sites of war | |||||
SHS Total | 485.08 | 541.46 | 72,879.000 | 0.007 | −0.0861 |
Sites of Natural Disasters | |||||
HSS Total | 479.27 | 557.88 | 72,491.500 | <0.001 | −0.1148 |
TWBS Total | 483.55 | 543.17 | 75,786.000 | 0.006 | −0.0869 |
Stop to see accidents | |||||
RSS Total | 463.43 | 553.28 | 94,502.500 | <0.001 | −0.1516 |
SHS Total | 471.10 | 540.42 | 99,273.500 | <0.001 | −0.1253 |
HSS Total | 461.28 | 556.89 | 93,161.000 | <0.001 | −0.1605 |
PVS Total | 457.84 | 562.66 | 91,021.500 | <0.001 | −0.1771 |
TWBS Total | 475.07 | 533.77 | 101,739.000 | 0.002 | −0.0991 |
Notes: α = Cronbach’s alpha; M = mean; SD = standard deviation; MR–mean rank; U = Mann–Whitney test; p = p-value; r = rank-biserial correlation. In bold: statistically significant values.
Differences were also assessed concerning the values of rumination on sadness, self-hatred, hostility, psychological vulnerability, and tourism wellbeing according to dark tourism motives. Those participants who identified curiosity, need to see, and need to understand as reasons to visit dark places in the context of tourism presented higher values in rumination on sadness, self-hatred, hostility, psychological vulnerability, and tourism wellbeing than those who did not identify curiosity as a motive (Table 7). Concerning the motive “need to learn”, it was found to be a statistically significant difference in tourism wellbeing, being that those who identified the need to learn as a motive to visit dark places in the context of tourism present higher values in tourism wellbeing and self-hatred than those who did not. Those participants who identified the need to see as a reason to visit dark places in the context of tourism presented higher values in rumination on sadness, self-hatred, hostility, and psychological vulnerability than those who did not identify the need to see as a motive (Table 7). Those participants who recognized the need to understand as a reason to visit dark places in the context of tourism present higher values in rumination on sadness, hostility, psychological vulnerability, and tourism wellbeing than those who did not identify the need to understand as a motive (Table 7). Concerning the motive “pleasure”, it was found a statistically significant difference in tourism wellbeing; those who recognized pleasure as a motive to visit dark places presented higher values in tourism wellbeing than those who did not. Lastly, those participants who identified the need to see morbid things as a reason to visit dark places presented higher values in rumination on sadness, self-hatred, hostility, and psychological vulnerability than those who did not identify the need to see morbid things as a motive (Table 7).
Table 7.
Rumination on sadness (RSS), self-hatred (SHS), hostility (HSS), psychological vulnerability (PVS), and tourism wellbeing (TWBS) frequencies and differences according to dark tourism motives.
No | Yes | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Sample | MR | MR | U | p | r |
Curiosity | |||||
RSS Total | 466.19 | 519.31 | 107,248.000 | 0.004 | −0.0915 |
SHS Total | 454.48 | 527.78 | 102,364.000 | <0.001 | −0.1353 |
HSS Total | 457.02 | 525.95 | 103,423.000 | <0.001 | −0.1188 |
PVS Total | 449.50 | 531.39 | 100,288.000 | <0.001 | −0.1411 |
TWBS Total | 468.48 | 517.65 | 108,203.000 | 0.008 | −0.0847 |
Need to learn | |||||
SHS Total | 480.57 | 531.88 | 96,232.000 | 0.005 | −0.0895 |
TWBS Total | 481.97 | 528.91 | 97,177.500 | 0.016 | −0.0764 |
Need to see | |||||
RSS Total | 468.92 | 547.02 | 95,668.500 | <0.001 | −0.1307 |
SHS Total | 479.58 | 528.04 | 102,446.000 | 0.006 | −0.0869 |
HSS Total | 469.66 | 545.71 | 96,137.000 | <0.001 | −0.1276 |
PVS Total | 477.48 | 431.78 | 101,110.500 | 0.004 | −0.0910 |
TWBS Total | 482.84 | 522.22 | 104,523.000 | 0.038 | −0.0659 |
Need to understand | |||||
RSS Total | 462.18 | 548.20 | 98,210.500 | <0.001 | −0.1473 |
SHS Total | 479.19 | 523.19 | 108,263.000 | 0.011 | −0.0807 |
HSS Total | 476.29 | 527.45 | 106,551.500 | 0.006 | −0.0878 |
PVS Total | 476.42 | 527.26 | 106,625.500 | 0.006 | −0.0871 |
TWBS Total | 479.01 | 523.45 | 108,160.000 | 0.016 | −0.0761 |
Pleasure | |||||
TWBS Total | 491.22 | 562.92 | 31,246.500 | 0.032 | −0.0681 |
Need to see morbid things | |||||
RSS Total | 489.97 | 624.13 | 17,855.000 | 0.001 | −0.1042 |
SHS Total | 490.72 | 610.68 | 18,554.500 | 0.020 | −0.0999 |
HSS Total | 492.32 | 581.65 | 20,064.000 | 0.028 | −0.0696 |
PVS Total | 491.89 | 589.43 | 19,659.500 | 0.017 | −0.0759 |
Notes: α = Cronbach’s alpha; M = mean; SD = standard deviation; MR–mean rank; U = Mann–Whitney test; p = p-value; r = rank-biserial correlation. In bold: statistically significant values.
After creating a new variable, an index about practices related to dark tourism, based on the individual items, we carried out a multiple linear regression in which the dependent variable is the index, and the independent variables are the motivations, with the intent to find the variables that explain the touristic practice. It was found that gender, age, know/know not dark tourism, and motives (curiosity, need to learn, need to understand, and pleasure) explain 38% of the touristic practice (Table 8).
Table 8.
Variables that contribute to the dark tourism practice index.
Model 1 | Model 2 | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
B | EP B | β | B | EP B | β | |
Gender | 0.031 | 0.015 | 0.066 | 0.028 | 0.012 | 0.060 |
Age | 0.001 | 0.001 | 0.052 | 0.001 | 0.000 | 0.076 |
Know/know not dark tourism | 0.069 | 0.015 | 0.147 | 0.030 | 0.012 | 0.064 |
Curiosity | 0.130 | 0.013 | 0.280 | |||
Need to learn | 0.107 | 0.015 | 0.217 | |||
Need to understand | 0.107 | 0.014 | 0.229 | |||
Pleasure | 0.108 | 0.021 | 0.128 | |||
R2 (R2 Adj.) | 0.027 (0.024) | 0.385 (0.381) | ||||
F for change in R2 | 9.291 ** | 143.202 ** |
Notes: R2 = R squared; R2 Adj. = R squared adjusted; B = unstandardized regression coefficients; EP B = unstandardized error of B; β = standardized regression coefficients; ** p < 0.001.
5. Discussion
The aims of the present study were to find the sociodemographic differences in touristic practices and motivations for dark tourism according to two groups (those who knew what dark tourism is and those who did not know); to determine the differences in rumination on sadness, self-hatred, hostility, psychological vulnerability, and tourism wellbeing according to two groups; to find the differences in rumination on sadness, self-hatred, hostility, psychological vulnerability, and tourism wellbeing according to practices and motivations for dark tourism; and, at last, to determine variables that contribute to a dark tourism practice index. To this end, we carried out a cross-sectional study that included questionnaires related to sociodemographic aspects, motivations to visit dark tourism places, practices of dark tourism, the rumination on the sadness scale, the self-hatred scale, the hostility scale, the psychological vulnerability scale, and the tourism wellbeing scale.
Concerning the participants’ profiles, those who had heard about dark tourism were significantly younger and more educated than those who had not. These results confirm hypothesis H1. These results corroborate those of Millán, et al. [52] who found a profile of dark tourists in Cordoba between 26 and 40 years old and having university studies. Dark tourism is a niche market [53] and also is itself a trend [54], and young people are more available and attentive to new trends [55]. In addition, more educated people seek more information and have superior technological skills [56]. Significant differences between the two samples regarding practices related to dark tourism were found, being that the sample that has already heard about dark tourism visits much more Holocaust museums, sites of human tragedy, concentration camps, prisons, and sites of natural disasters than the sample that has not yet heard about dark tourism. These results confirm hypothesis H2. According to Iliev [4], “if tourists do not experience a site as dark, then they cannot be called dark tourists”, so the author proposed a more apparent distinction of the “dark tourists” based on experience. Ashworth and Isaac (2015) also stated that any tourist site has a greater or lesser potential of being perceived as “dark.” Besides, “darkness cannot be viewed as an objective fact because it is subjectively and socially constructed since (different) people in various (cultural or social) contexts understand and experience dark tourism in different ways” [57]. In fact, we may ask “who makes the association of ‘darkness’ to a place? Is the label ‘dark tourism’ applied by those offering (and commoditizing) the visitor experience? Alternatively, is any “dark” significance to be evaluated and decided upon by the tourists themselves?” [58]. “Dark tourism consumption can no longer be derived as an ordinary activity where humans might engage in for “fun”, but rather as part of a quest for a deeper experience, especially in our inherent fear of death” [4].
The subsample that has already heard about dark tourism presents higher values in the curiosity, the need to learn and understand, and the need to see morbid things motives than the sample that has not yet heard about dark tourism. These results also confirm hypothesis H2. In fact, dark tourists are very interested in understanding historical events; they are psychologically moved by the need to be in contact with authentic experiences by looking at the other’s death as if it were their own death [59]. One of the motivations that drive dark tourists is the possibility of re-creating the same emotions victims experienced, followed by the authenticity issue [60]. “Many dark tourists are motivated by the desire and interest in cultural heritage, learning, education, understanding about what happened at the dark site” [4].
There are no differences in the values of rumination on sadness, self-hatred, hostility, psychological vulnerability, and tourism wellbeing concerning knowing what dark tourism is or not. Therefore, hypothesis H4 cannot be confirmed. These results apparently seem to contradict the relationship between the dark triad of the personality (narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy) and the practice of dark tourism [16,32,33,34]. That relationship, studied by those authors, reflects the practice of dark tourism and not the knowledge about it (which is the subject of our study), although there is hardly any knowledge without practice. Concerning tourism wellbeing, these results may question Kidron [61] who said that dark tourism generates wellbeing and thus assume that dark tourists show wellbeing despite dark practices. However, our results do not show greater wellbeing in the participants who knew in advance what dark tourism was in relation to the others.
Participants who visit cemeteries have significantly lower values of self-hatred and psychological vulnerability than participants who report not visiting cemeteries. Visiting a cemetery can fulfill different functions, such as visiting a dark place or the social and cultural function of honoring the dead. Probably, our results reflect this last function to the detriment of the first and this conformity to cultural and social practices is in accordance with lower values of psychopathology [62], namely rumination on sadness, self-hatred, hostility, and psychological vulnerability. This result partially confirms hypothesis 5.
Those who visit sites of war present higher levels of self-hatred than those who did not. Furthermore, those who visit natural tragedies sites present higher values in hostility and tourism wellbeing than those who do not. This result reflects the relationship of this tourist practice with the above-mentioned dark triad [16,32,33,34] and is in line with Kidron [61], who suggested wellbeing in dark tourists. At last, those who stop to see accidents present higher values in rumination on sadness, self-hatred, hostility, psychological vulnerability, and tourism wellbeing than those who do not stop. Again, this result reveals the relationship between psychopathology and tourist wellbeing that needs to be further explained, although some authors suggest that psychopathology leads to less tourism wellbeing [63]. This result partially confirms hypothesis 5.
Participants who identified curiosity as a reason to visit dark places in the context of tourism presented higher values in rumination on sadness, self-hatred, hostility, psychological vulnerability, and tourism wellbeing than those who did not identify curiosity as a motive. Curiosity has been a central reason pointed out in the literature for tourism in general [64] and, specifically, for dark tourism [15,17,19,20,21,65,66]. Curiosity is a complex construct, which can be seen as something positive, but it can also contain darker aspects of the personality, namely morbid curiosity, and this fact explains its relationship with, on the one hand, wellbeing, and, on the other hand, with rumination on sadness, self-hatred, hostility, and psychological vulnerability. This result partially confirms hypothesis 5.
The participants who identified the need to learn, the need to understand as motives to visit dark places in the context of tourism present higher values in tourism wellbeing and self-hatred than those who did not. The need to learn and understand are also central reasons for tourism in general and their relationship with wellbeing does not seem specific to dark tourism [67]. This result partially confirms hypothesis 5.
The participants who identified the need to see as a reason to visit dark places in the context of tourism presented higher values in rumination on sadness, self-hatred, hostility, psychological vulnerability, and tourism wellbeing. This result partially confirms hypothesis 5. Similarly to the need to learn, the need to see correlates with wellbeing but with psychopathology. Perhaps this need to learn motivation is correlated with the touristic practice of seeing morbid things [68].
The participants who recognized pleasure as a motive to visit dark places presented higher values in tourism wellbeing than those who did not. This result partially confirms hypothesis 5. Dark tourism conforms with the pleasure of tourism in general (Yanjun et al., 2015); wellbeing derives from the emotional experience of dark tourism as a motor for transforming the self [69].
The participants who identified the need to see morbid things as a drive to visit dark places presented higher values in rumination on sadness, self-hatred, hostility, and psychological vulnerability. The need to see morbid things may be a specific motivation for dark tourism [1,70] and not tourism in general. To that extent, the relationship between this motivation and rumination on sadness, self-hatred, hostility, and psychological vulnerability is justified. This result partially confirms Hypothesis 5.
The reasons to visit dark places-curiosity, the need to see, the need to understand, and pleasure are positively and significantly correlated with all places associated with dark tourism. Gender, age, know/know not dark tourism, and motives (curiosity, the need to learn, the need to understand, and pleasure) explained 38.1% of the practice index variance, thus confirming H6. These results mean that motivations to visit dark places are associated with the touristic activity itself and may contradict those of Buda [71], that claims more emotional and psychoanalytical explorations through the concepts of the death drive [71], desire [72], and unconsciousness and voyeurism [73]. In fact, dark tourists are not altruistic persons [14,60]. Moreover, Jovanovic, Mijatov, and Šuligoj [32] found that Machiavellianism was related to the preference for dark exhibitions, psychopathy to the preference for visiting conflict/battle sites, and sadism was negatively related to the preference for fun factories and dark tourism sites. However, the “darker” motivation may present different levels of intensity; besides the fascination and interest in death [15], these visits are also motivated by personal, cultural, and psychological reasons [4] and/or by entertainment purposes such as entertainment-based museums of torture [7,16]. One of the most curious outcomes of this study is the association of motivations to visit dark tourist sites and self-hatred; the fact that the authors have not found any study that could explain such a result suggests this association exists in the context of dark tourism and not of tourism in general. The dark nature of this type of tourism can be attractive to tourists with less positive personality traits such as self-hatred.
6. Conclusions
The results of this study add new knowledge to this area of expertise as it allows us to understand the association between motivations and practices related to dark tourism. This study also identified the main motivations to visit dark places-curiosity, the need to see, the need to understand, and pleasure, being, interestingly, all internal motivations and, thus, contradicting the literature that, in addition to these motivations, also identifies external motivations. Most findings also indicate that the rumination on sadness, self-hatred, hostility, and psychological vulnerability personality dimensions are associated with dark practices (e.g., the need to see morbid things). Lastly, people who visit more dark places and score higher on negative personality characteristics have higher values of tourism wellbeing. These findings are in line with the literature, which suggests that dark tourism generates negative and positive wellbeing (or even ambivalence). As such, dark tourists, even presenting negative personality characteristics, and also because of them, show tourism wellbeing in their practices and motivations.
The fact that this study was held in a specific sample in Portugal may be considered a limitation; future lines of research could extend it to other countries and age segments.
Author Contributions
Conceptualization, J.M., J.A.F.-B. and Â.L.; methodology, J.M.; formal analysis, J.M. and Â.L.; writing—original draft preparation, J.M.; writing—review and editing, J.M., J.A.F.-B. and Â.L. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
Institutional Review Board Statement
Ethical review and approval were waived for this study, as no medical research involving human subjects has been carried out, including research on identifiable human material and data, as indicated by the terms of the Declaration of Helsinki.
Informed Consent Statement
Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.
Data Availability Statement
Datasets are available upon request to the authors.
Conflicts of Interest
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
Funding Statement
This research received no external funding.
Footnotes
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Stone P.R. A dark tourism spectrum: Towards a typology of death and macabre related tourist sites, attractions and exhibitions. Tour. Int. Interdiscip. J. 2006;54:145–160. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Farmaki A. Dark tourism revisited: A supply/demand conceptualisation. Int. J. Cult. Tour. Hosp. Res. 2013;7:281–292. doi: 10.1108/IJCTHR-05-2012-0030. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Sharpley R., Stone P.R. The Darker Side of Travel: The Theory and Practice of Dark Tourism. Channel View Publications; Bristol, UK: 2009. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Iliev D. Consumption, motivation and experience in dark tourism: A conceptual and critical analysis. Tour. Geogr. 2020;23:963–984. doi: 10.1080/14616688.2020.1722215. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Martini A., Buda D.M. Dark tourism and affect: Framing places of death and disaster. Curr. Issues Tour. 2020;23:679–692. doi: 10.1080/13683500.2018.1518972. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Hartmann R., Lennon J., Reynolds D.P., Rice A., Rosenbaum A.T., Stone P.R. The history of dark tourism. J. Tour. Hist. 2018;10:269–295. doi: 10.1080/1755182X.2018.1545394. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Biran A., Poria Y., Oren G. Sought experiences at (dark) heritage sites. Ann. Tour. Res. 2011;38:820–841. doi: 10.1016/j.annals.2010.12.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Buda D.M. Affective Tourism: Dark Routes in Conflict. 1st ed. Routledge; London, UK: 2015. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Cohen E.H. Educational dark tourism at an in populo site: The Holocaust Museum in Jerusalem. Ann. Tour. Res. 2011;38:193–209. doi: 10.1016/j.annals.2010.08.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Tarlow P. Dark tourism: The appealing ‘dark’side of tourism and more. In: Novelli M., editor. Niche Tourism: Contemporary Issues, Trends and Cases. 1st ed. Routledge; London, UK: 2005. pp. 61–72. [Google Scholar]
- 11.Coutinho B., Baptista M.M. Há morte nas catacumbas? Perceções de visitantes de uma atração de turismo negro. Rev. Tur. Desenvolv. 2014;4:493–503. doi: 10.34624/rtd.v4i21/22.12441. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Ashworth G.J., Isaac R.K. Have we illuminated the dark? Shifting perspectives on ‘dark’ tourism. Tour. Recreat. Res. 2015;40:316–325. doi: 10.1080/02508281.2015.1075726. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Walter T. The Darker Side of Travel. Channel View Publications; Bristol, UK: 2009. Dark tourism: Mediating between the dead and the living; pp. 39–55. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Seaton A.V. Guided by the dark: From thanatopsis to thanatourism. Int. J. Herit. Stud. 1996;2:234–244. doi: 10.1080/13527259608722178. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Biran A., Liu W., Li G., Eichhorn V. Consuming post-disaster destinations: The case of Sichuan, China. Ann. Tour. Res. 2014;47:1–17. doi: 10.1016/j.annals.2014.03.004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Best M. Norfolk Island: Thanatourism, history and visitor emotions. Shima Int. J. Res. Isl. Cult. 2007;1:30–48. [Google Scholar]
- 17.Isaac R.K., Çakmak E. Understanding visitor’s motivation at sites of death and disaster: The case of former transit camp Westerbork, the Netherlands. Curr. Issues Tour. 2014;17:164–179. doi: 10.1080/13683500.2013.776021. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Light D. Progress in dark tourism and thanatourism research: An uneasy relationship with heritage tourism. Tour. Manag. 2017;61:275–301. doi: 10.1016/j.tourman.2017.01.011. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Nhlabathi S.S., Maharaj B. The dark tourism discipline: A creative brand in a competitive academic environment? Curr. Issues Tour. 2020;23:2428–2439. doi: 10.1080/13683500.2019.1636770. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Isaac R.K., Nawijn J., van Liempt A., Gridnevskiy K. Understanding Dutch visitors’ motivations to concentration camp memorials. Curr. Issues Tour. 2019;22:747–762. doi: 10.1080/13683500.2017.1310190. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Ashworth G.J. Tourism and the heritage of atrocity: Managing the heritage of South African apartheid for entertainment. In: Singh T.V., editor. New Horizons in Tourism: Strange Experiences and Stranger Practices. CABI Publishing; Wallingford, CT, USA: 2004. pp. 95–108. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Podoshen J.S. Dark tourism in an increasingly violent world. In: Stone P.R., Hartmann R., Seaton T., Sharpley R., White L., editors. The Palgrave Handbook of Dark Tourism Studies. Springer; Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany: 2018. pp. 173–187. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Slade P. Gallipoli thanatourism: The meaning of ANZAC. Ann. Tour. Res. 2003;30:779–794. doi: 10.1016/S0160-7383(03)00025-2. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Yan B.-J., Zhang J., Zhang H.-L., Lu S.-J., Guo Y.-R. Investigating the motivation–experience relationship in a dark tourism space: A case study of the Beichuan earthquake relics, China. Tour. Manag. 2016;53:108–121. doi: 10.1016/j.tourman.2015.09.014. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Prentice R., Andersen V. Interpreting heritage essentialisms: Familiarity and felt history. Tour. Manag. 2007;28:661–676. doi: 10.1016/j.tourman.2006.02.008. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Sharpley R., Stone P., editors. Contemporary Tourism Experience. Volume 96. Routledge; New York, NY, USA: 2012. p. 109. [Google Scholar]
- 27.Ashworth G., Hartmann R. Horror and Human Tragedy Revisited: The Management of Sites of Atrocities for Tourism. Cognizant Communication Corporation; Elmsford, NY, USA: 2005. [Google Scholar]
- 28.Blom T. Morbid tourism-a postmodern market niche with an example from Althorp. Nor. Geogr. Tidsskr. 2000;54:29–36. doi: 10.1080/002919500423564. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Johnston J.L. Branded entertainment: The old is new again and more complicated than ever. J. Spons. 2009;2:170–175. [Google Scholar]
- 30.Lennon J.J., Foley M. Dark tourism: The attraction of death and disaster. Ann. Tour. Res. 2002;29:1188–1189. [Google Scholar]
- 31.Çoban F. Political Reflections on dark tourism: The case of Turkey. In: Korstanje M., George B., editors. Virtual Traumascapes and Exploring the Roots of Dark Tourism. IGI Global; Hershey, PA, USA: 2018. pp. 99–125. [Google Scholar]
- 32.Jovanovic T., Mijatov M., Šuligoj M. Does a visitor of dark tourism sites have a dark personality? A study of potential visitors of dark tourism sites in Serbia. Primenj. Psihol. 2021;14:407–442. doi: 10.19090/pp.2021.4.407-442. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Nørfelt A., Kock F., Karpen I.O., Josiassen A. Pleasure through Pain: An Empirical Examination of Benign Masochism in Tourism. J. Travel Res. 2022 doi: 10.1177/00472875211067550. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Christou P.A. Philosophies of Hospitality and Tourism: Giving and Receiving. Channel View Publications; Bristol, UK: 2020. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Conway M., Csank P.A., Holm S.L., Blake C.K. On assessing individual differences in rumination on sadness. J. Pers. Assess. 2000;75:404–425. doi: 10.1207/S15327752JPA7503_04. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Nolen-Hoeksema S., Morrow J. A prospective study of depression and posttraumatic stress symptoms after a natural disaster: The 1989 Loma Prieta Earthquake. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1991;61:115. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.61.1.115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Derlega V.J., Metts S., Petronio S., Margulis S.T. Self-Disclosure. Sage Publications, Inc.; Newbury Park, CA, USA: 1993. [Google Scholar]
- 38.Stanton A.L., Danoff-Burg S., Cameron C.L., Ellis A.P. Coping through emotional approach: Problems of conceptualizaton and confounding. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1994;66:350. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.66.2.350. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Turnell A.I., Fassnacht D.B., Batterham P.J., Calear A.L., Kyrios M. The Self-Hate Scale: Development and validation of a brief measure and its relationship to suicidal ideation. J. Affect. Disord. 2019;245:779–787. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2018.11.047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Derogatis L.R., Melisaratos N. The brief symptom inventory: An introductory report. Psychol. Med. 1983;13:595–605. doi: 10.1017/S0033291700048017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Spielberger C. State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory. Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.; Odessa, FL, USA: 1988. [Google Scholar]
- 42.Deffenbacher J.L., Oetting E.R., Thwaites G.A., Lynch R.S., Baker D.A., Stark R.S., Thacker S., Eiswerth-Cox L. State–trait anger theory and the utility of the trait anger scale. J. Couns. Psychol. 1996;43:131. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.43.2.131. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Sinclair V.G., Wallston K.A. Psychological vulnerability predicts increases in depressive symptoms in individuals with rheumatoid arthritis. Nurs. Res. 2010;59:140–146. doi: 10.1097/NNR.0b013e3181d1a6f6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Sinclair V.G., Wallston K.A. The development and validation of the Psychological Vulnerability Scale. Cogn. Ther. Res. 1999;23:119–129. doi: 10.1023/A:1018770926615. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Sharma N., Rickly J.M. Self-care for the researcher: Dark tourism in Varanasi, India. J. Teach. Travel Tour. 2018;18:41–57. doi: 10.1080/15313220.2017.1403801. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Canavarro M.C. Inventário de Sintomas Psicopatológicos (BSI): Uma revisão crítica dos estudos realizados em Portugal. [Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI): A critical review of studies carried out in Portugal] Avaliação Psicológica Instrum. Validados Popul. Port. 2007;3:305–331. [Google Scholar]
- 47.Nogueira M.J., Barros L., Sequeira C. Psychometric properties of the psychological vulnerability scale in higher education students. J. Am. Psychiatr. Nurses Assoc. 2017;23:215–222. doi: 10.1177/1078390317695261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Garcês S., Pocinho M., Jesus S. Development of the Tourism Wellbeing Scale. Rev. Divulg. Científica-AICA. 2018;10:55–63. [Google Scholar]
- 49.Garcês S., Pocinho M., Jesus S.N. Psychometric Analysis of the Tourism Wellbeing Scale (TWS): A multidisciplinary approach. An. Acad. Bras. Ciênc. 2020;92:e20190232. doi: 10.1590/0001-3765202020190232. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Kline R.B. Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling. Guilford Publications; New York, NY, USA: 2015. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Cohen J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. 2nd ed. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers; Hillsdale, NJ, USA: 1988. [Google Scholar]
- 52.Millán G.D., Rojas R.D.H., García J.S.-R. Analysis of the demand of dark tourism: A case study in Córdoba (Spain) Mediterr. J. Soc. Sci. 2019;10:161. doi: 10.2478/mjss-2019-0015. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Proos E., Hattingh J. Dark tourism: Growth potential of niche tourism in the Free State Province, South Africa. Dev. South. Afr. 2022;39:303–320. doi: 10.1080/0376835X.2020.1847636. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Collins-Kreiner N. Dark tourism as/is pilgrimage. Curr. Issues Tour. 2016;19:1185–1189. doi: 10.1080/13683500.2015.1078299. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Bonnie R.J., Stroud C., Breiner H. Investing in the Health and Well-Being of Young Adults. National Academies Press; Washington, DC, USA: 2014. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Bond M., Marín V.I., Dolch C., Bedenlier S., Zawacki-Richter O. Digital transformation in German higher education: Student and teacher perceptions and usage of digital media. Int. J. Educ. Technol. High. Educ. 2018;15:48. doi: 10.1186/s41239-018-0130-1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Sigala M., Steriopoulos E. Does emotional engagement matter in dark tourism? Implications drawn from a reflective approach. J. Herit. Tour. 2021;16:412–432. doi: 10.1080/1743873X.2020.1851697. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Morten R., Stone P.R., Jarratt D. Dark tourism as psychogeography: An initial exploration. In: Stone P.R., Hartmann R., Seaton T., Sharpley R., White L., editors. The Palgrave Handbook of Dark Tourism Studies. Springer; Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany: 2018. pp. 227–255. [Google Scholar]
- 59.Korstanje M.E. The dark tourist: Consuming dark spaces in the periphery. In: Korstanje M.E., Seraphin H., editors. Tourism, Terrorism and Security. Emerald Publishing Limited; Bingley, UK: 2020. pp. 135–150. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Stone P., Sharpley R. Consuming dark tourism: A thanatological perspective. Ann. Tour. Res. 2008;35:574–595. doi: 10.1016/j.annals.2008.02.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Kidron C.A. Being there together: Dark family tourism and the emotive experience of co-presence in the holocaust past. Ann. Tour. Res. 2013;41:175–194. doi: 10.1016/j.annals.2012.12.009. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Narvaez D., Wang L., Cheng Y. The evolved developmental niche in childhood: Relation to adult psychopathology and morality. Appl. Dev. Sci. 2016;20:294–309. doi: 10.1080/10888691.2015.1128835. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Genç R. Subjective aspects of tourists’ quality-of-life (QOL) In: Uysal M., Perdue R.R., Sirgy M.J., editors. Handbook of Tourism and Quality-of-Life Research. Springer; Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany: 2012. pp. 149–167. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Totsune T., Matsudaira I., Taki Y. Curiosity–tourism interaction promotes subjective wellbeing among older adults in Japan. Humanit. Soc. Sci. Commun. 2021;8:69. doi: 10.1057/s41599-021-00748-3. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Johnston T. The geographies of thanatourism. Geography. 2015;100:20–27. doi: 10.1080/00167487.2015.12093950. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Seaton A.V., Lennon J.J. Thanatourism in the early 21st century: Moral panics, ulterior motives and alterior desires. In: Singh T.V., editor. New Horizons in Tourism: Strange Experiences and Stranger Practices. CABI Publishing; Wallingford, CT, USA: 2004. pp. 63–82. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Heitmann S. Tourist behaviour and tourism motivation. In: Robinson P., Heitmann S., Dieke P., editors. Research Themes for Tourism. CABI Publishing; Wallingford, CT, USA: 2011. pp. 31–44. [Google Scholar]
- 68.Cutting J. Morbid objectivization in psychopathology. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 1999;99:30–33. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1999.tb05980.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Nawijn J., Biran A. Negative emotions in tourism: A meaningful analysis. Curr. Issues Tour. 2019;22:2386–2398. doi: 10.1080/13683500.2018.1451495. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Sharpley R. Travels to the edge of darkness: Towards a typology of “dark tourism”. In: Ryan C., Page S.J., Aicken M., editors. Taking Tourism to the Limits. Routledge; London, UK: 2006. pp. 239–250. [Google Scholar]
- 71.Buda D.M. The death drive in tourism studies. Ann. Tour. Res. 2015;50:39–51. doi: 10.1016/j.annals.2014.10.008. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Buda D.M., Shim D. Desiring the dark: A taste for the unusual’in North Korean tourism? Curr. Issues Tour. 2015;18:1–6. doi: 10.1080/13683500.2014.948813. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Buda D.M., McIntosh A.J. Dark tourism and voyeurism: Tourist arrested for “spying” in Iran. Int. J. Cult. Tour. Hosp. Res. 2013;7:214–226. doi: 10.1108/IJCTHR-07-2012-0059. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
Datasets are available upon request to the authors.