Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2023 Jul 10.
Published in final edited form as: Pediatr Res. 2022 Apr 20;93(7):2051–2060. doi: 10.1038/s41390-022-02051-6

Early-life gut microbiota and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in preadolescents

Andrea E Cassidy-Bushrow 1,2,*, Alexandra R Sitarik 1, Christine Cole Johnson 1,2, Tisa M Johnson-Hooper 3,4, Zeinab Kassem 1, Albert M Levin 1, Susan V Lynch 5, Dennis R Ownby 6, Jannel M Phillips 4,7, Germaine JM Yong 5, Ganesa Wegienka 1,2, Jennifer K Straughen 1,2
PMCID: PMC9582043  NIHMSID: NIHMS1792597  PMID: 35440767

Abstract

Gut microbiota maturation coincides with nervous system development. Cross-sectional data suggest gut microbiota of individuals with and without attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) differs. We hypothesized that infant gut microbiota composition is associated with later ADHD development in our on-going birth cohort study, WHEALS. Gut microbiota was profiled using 16S ribosomal RNA and the internal transcribed spacer region 2 (ITS2) sequencing in stool samples from 1 month and 6 months of age. ADHD was defined by parent-reported or medical record doctor diagnosis at age 10. A total of 314 children had gut microbiota and ADHD data; 59 (18.8%) had ADHD. After covariate adjustment, bacterial phylogenetic diversity (p = 0.017) and bacterial composition (unweighted UniFrac p = 0.006, R2= 0.9%) at age 6 months were associated with development of ADHD. At 1 month of age, 18 bacterial and 3 fungal OTUs were associated with ADHD development. At 6 months of age, 51 bacterial OTUs were associated with ADHD; 14 of the order Lactobacillales. Three fungal OTUs at 6 months of age were associated with ADHD development. Infant gut microbiota is associated with ADHD development in pre-adolescents. Further studies replicating these findings and evaluating potential mechanisms of the association are needed.

Introduction

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a complex neurodevelopmental disorder hallmarked by hyperactivity, inattention, and impulsivity at a developmentally inappropriate level.1 Studies of the gut-brain axis suggest the gut microbiome influences neurodevelopment.24 The gut microbiome develops rapidly over the first 3 years of life concurrently with synaptogenesis, myelination and synaptic refinement of the central nervous system, and produces a vast range of bioactive metabolites that could influence neurodevelopment.2, 57 Studies of the role of the gut microbiome in neurodevelopment could enhance our understanding of the etiology of neurodevelopmental disorders as well as identify targets for intervention.

Most studies of gut microbiota and ADHD have been relatively small, and cross-sectional, precluding investigation of a temporal relationship. In a study of 19 adolescents and adults with ADHD and 77 healthy controls, ADHD was associated with higher abundance of Bifidobacterium.8 In a cross-sectional study, children with ADHD had lower levels of Faecalibacterium, Dialister and Sutterella than controls.9 Children with ADHD had relatively lower abundance of Bacteroides coprocola and higher abundances of B. uniformis, B. ovatus and Sutterella stercoricanis in a cross-sectional study of 30 Taiwanese children with ADHD and 30 healthy controls (mean age 8.4 years).10 Finally, in Chinese children aged 6–12 years (17 cases with ADHD and 17 controls), cases had significantly lower abundance of Faecalibacterium and Veillonellaceae and significantly higher levels of Odoribacter and Enterococcus.11 Less is known, however, about the infant gut microbiome and subsequent risk of childhood ADHD. In a randomized trial of Lactobacillus rhamnosus GG supplementation during the first 6 months of life, no supplemented children developed ADHD by age 13 years.12 In this same trial, Bifidobacterium species were lower in children who later developed ADHD.12

Given that neurodevelopment and gut microbiome development are rapid during early life, studies of the association of the infant gut microbiota with neurodevelopmental disorders in childhood are needed. This study sought to examine if early-life gut microbiota composition (at 1 month and 6 months of age) is associated with ADHD in preadolescence.

Methods

Study Population

The Wayne County Health, Environment, Allergy and Asthma Longitudinal Study (WHEALS) recruited pregnant women aged 21–49 years, second trimester or later, with due dates from September 2003 through December 2007, who were seeing a practitioner at any of 5 clinics in the Henry Ford Health System, and were living in a predefined geographic area that included the city of Detroit and surrounding suburban areas to establish a birth cohort.13, 14 Mothers were interviewed in the clinic during pregnancy and postpartum interviewer-administered questionnaires were completed at child age 1, 6, 12, and 24 months. Research clinic visits were conducted at 2 and 10 years of age. Study protocols were approved by the Henry Ford Health System Institutional Review Board. The mother provided written, informed consent at the prenatal visit, and at the age 10 year visit the guardian (primarily the mother) provided written, informed consent and the child provided written, informed assent.

Definition of ADHD and Neurotypical Development

At the age 10-year visit, the caregiver reported if the child had ever been diagnosed with ADHD, autism spectrum disorder, Asperger’s syndrome, or sensory processing disorder. Medical chart abstraction was also completed to obtain these diagnoses. We previously reported near perfect agreement between caregiver-reported and medical record ADHD diagnosis in WHEALS,15 thus children were considered to have ADHD if either source indicated the child had ADHD. Children were considered neurotypical (NT) if they did not have a caregiver-reported or medical record identified autism spectrum disorder, Asperger’s, ADHD, or sensory processing disorder diagnosis.

Stool Specimens

At infant ages 1 and 6 months, stool samples were collected from soiled diapers and stored at −80°C. Detailed information on DNA extraction methods are presented elsewhere.16

Polymerase Chain Reaction Conditions and Library Preparation for Sequencing

For bacterial sequencing, the V4 region of the 16S ribosomal RNA gene was amplified using Illumina Nextseq (San Diego, CA).17 For fungal sequencing, the internal transcribed spacer region (ITS) 2 of the ribosomal RNA gene was amplified using the primer pair fITS7 (5’-GTGARTCATCGAATCTTTG-3’) and ITS4 (5’-TCCTCCGCTTATT GATATGC-3’) and processed using Illumina MiSeq according to previously published protocols.18

Sequence Data Processing and Quality Control

Bacterial paired-end sequences were assembled using FLASH v1.2.7 requiring a minimum base pair overlap of 25bp and demultiplexed by barcode using QIIME (Quantitative Insights Into Microbial Ecology, v1.8). Quality filtering was performed using USEARCH v8.0.1623 to remove reads with > 2 expected errors. Quality reads were dereplicated at 100% sequence identity, clustered at 97% sequence identity into operational taxonomic units (OTUs), filtered of chimeric sequences by UCHIME,19 and mapped back to resulting OTUs using UPARSE;20 sequence reads that failed to cluster with a reference sequence were clustered de novo. Taxonomy was assigned to the OTUs using the Greengenes database v13_5.21 Sequences were aligned using PyNAST,22 and FastTree 2.1.323 was used to build a phylogenetic tree. Resulting sequence reads were normalized by multiply rarefying to 60,000 reads per sample as described previously24 to assure reduced data were representative of the fuller data for each sample. The rarefied OTU table was used for all subsequent analyses.

Fungal sequences were quality trimmed (Q score, < 25) and removed of adaptor sequences using cutadapt.25 Paired-end sequences were assembled, demultiplexed by barcode, clustered into OTUs at 97% identity and filtered of chimeras using similar methods as described for 16S amplicons. Taxonomy was assigned using UNITE v7.0.26 Resulting sequence reads were normalized by multiply rarefying to 1,000 reads per sample to assure reduced data were representative of the fuller data for each sample.

A total of 580 children had at least 1 stool sample in the final rarefied OTU table; of these, 323 (56%) had data to classify neurodevelopment. We excluded 9 children with autism spectrum disorder, Asperger’s syndrome, or sensory processing disorder. A total of 431 stool samples (229 1-month and 202 6-month) from 314 children had bacterial gut microbiota measured; only 209 of these (107 1-month and 102 6-months) produced an ITS2 amplicon permitting fungal identification.

In the analytical dataset, stool specimens from the 1- and 6-month visits were collected at a mean ± standard deviation of 39 ± 19 days and 205 ± 32 days, respectively. Throughout, “1 month” and “6 months” are used as labels of the intended time period of sample collection.

Covariates

During the prenatal interview, the mother self-reported race, date of birth, marital status, household income, education, parity, smoking history and exposure to environmental tobacco smoke. Maternal medical records were abstracted to obtain maternal height and weight, prescription antibiotic and antifungal use, delivery mode, birth weight, and gestational age at delivery.27

Statistical Analysis

Basic parametric (independent samples t-test and chi-square test) and non-parametric (Fisher’s exact test and Kruskal-Wallis test) tests were used to compare maternal and child characteristics between children with and without ADHD. Linear regression was used to test for the association of alpha diversity metrics (richness, Pielou’s evenness, Faith’s phylogenetic diversity [bacterial only], and Shannon’s diversity) with ADHD. The association of infant gut microbiota composition with ADHD was evaluated using permutational multivariate analysis of variance (PERMANOVA) with the R package vegan (R Foundation, Vienna, Austria).28, 29 Unweighted and weighted UniFrac were tested for bacterial composition and Canberra and Bray-Curtis for fungal composition.30, 31 Because of the small number of children with ADHD and the unbalanced design (large number of NT children), we were concerned about violating the homogeneity of dispersions assumption inherent to PERMANOVA. Homogeneity of dispersion by group was evaluated using the betadisper function in vegan. Differential abundance of OTUs between ADHD and NT children was tested using zero-inflated negative binomial models (pscl package)32 or negative binomial models (MASS package)33 in cases of non-convergence or model-fitting issues. Only OTUs detected in ≥ 5% of samples were included for testing. P-values were corrected using the false discovery rate;34 false discovery rate corrected p < 0.05 was considered significant. All models were fit unadjusted and adjusted for a pre-specified set of potential confounders (exact age at stool sample collection, maternal body mass index, prenatal antifungal use, maternal smoking during pregnancy, child sex, mode of delivery, gestational age at delivery, parity, and breastfeeding status). Sex-specific effects were tested using interaction terms (interaction p < 0.1 was considered significant).35 For all main effect analyses, p<0.05 was considered significant.

Results

Among the 314 WHEALS children with gut microbiota and neurodevelopment data, 59 (18.8%) had ADHD. Boys were more likely to develop ADHD (Table 1; p = 0.001). Mothers of children who developed ADHD were more likely during pregnancy to have used antifungals (p = 0.001), to have exposure to environmental tobacco smoke (p = 0.048), to be younger (p = 0.031) and to have a larger body mass index (p = 0.023) (Table 1).

Table 1.

Descriptive characteristics of children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) or neurotypical (NT) development through age 10 years and early-life gut microbiota measurements.

Covariate NT (N=255) ADHD (N=59) P

Maternal Characteristic N (%), Mean ± SD, or Median [Q1; Q3]

Age at delivery (years) 30.6 ± 5.34 28.8 ± 5.64 0.031a
Race 0.749b
 White 65 (25.5%) 13 (22.0%)
 Black 155 (60.8%) 36 (61.0%)
 Mixed/Other 35 (13.7%) 10 (16.9%)
Marital status 0.113b
 Unmarried 86 (33.7%) 27 (45.8%)
 Married 169 (66.3%) 32 (54.2%)
Household income 0.243c
 <$20,000 26 (10.2%) 11 (18.6%)
 $20,000–<$40,000 54 (21.2%) 13 (22.0%)
 $40,000–<$80,000 58 (22.7%) 12 (20.3%)
 $80,000–<$100,000 36 (14.1%) 9 (15.3%)
 ≥$100,000 44 (17.3%) 4 (6.78%)
 Refused to answer 37 (14.5%) 10 (16.9%)
Education 0.127c
 <High school diploma 10 (3.92%) 1 (1.69%)
 High school diploma 36 (14.1%) 11 (18.6%)
 Some college 112 (43.9%) 33 (55.9%)
 ≥Bachelor’s degree 97 (38.0%) 14 (23.7%)
Smoked during pregnancy 0.794b
 No 235 (92.2%) 54 (91.5%)
 Yes 20 (7.84%) 5 (8.47%)
Prenatal ETS exposure 0.048b
 No 202 (79.2%) 39 (66.1%)
 Yes 53 (20.8%) 20 (33.9%)
Prenatal antibiotic use 1.000b
 No 101 (41.2%) 23 (41.1%)
 Yes 144 (58.8%) 33 (58.9%)
Prenatal antifungal use 0.001b
 No 203 (82.9%) 35 (62.5%)
 Yes 42 (17.1%) 21 (37.5%)
BMI from first prenatal visit (kg/m2)d 30.2 ± 7.59 (N=253) 33.0 ± 8.67 (N=58) 0.023a
Prenatal indoor dog(s) 0.864b
 No 187 (73.3%) 42 (71.2%)
 Yes 68 (26.7%) 17 (28.8%)
Prenatal indoor cat(s) 0.805b
 No 210 (82.4%) 50 (84.7%)
 Yes 45 (17.6%) 9 (15.3%)

Child Characteristic

Mode of delivery 0.370b
 Vaginal 169 (66.5%) 35 (59.3%)
 C-section 85 (33.5%) 24 (40.7%)
Firstborn 0.139b
 No 159 (62.4%) 30 (50.8%)
 Yes 96 (37.6%) 29 (49.2%)
Gestational age at delivery (weeks)d 39.0 [38.0; 40.0] (N=254) 39.0 [38.0; 40.0] (N=58) 0.238e
Preterm birth (gestational age <37 weeks) 0.780b
 No 236 (92.9%) 53 (91.4%)
 Yes 18 (7.09%) 5 (8.62%)
Birthweight (g)d 3388 ± 607 (N=243) 3308 ± 613 (N=52) 0.391a
Low birth weight (<2,500 g) 0.776c
 No 224 (92.2%) 49 (94.2%)
 Yes 19 (7.82%) 3 (5.77%)
Child sex 0.001b
 Male 126 (49.4%) 44 (74.6%)
 Female 129 (50.6%) 15 (25.4%)
Breastfeeding status at 1-month 0.629b
 Formula feeding 54 (21.3%) 12 (21.1%)
 Mixed feeding 156 (61.4%) 38 (66.7%)
 Breastfeeding 44 (17.3%) 7 (12.3%)
Breastfeeding status at 6-months 0.971c
 Formula feeding 54 (21.3%) 12 (21.1%)
 Mixed feeding 178 (70.1%) 41 (71.9%)
 Breastfeeding 22 (8.66%) 4 (7.02%)
Exact age at 1-month stool sample collection (months)d 1.09 [0.89; 1.45] (N=190) 1.12 [0.86; 1.56] (N=39) 0.666e
Exact age at 6-month stool sample collection (months)d 6.32 [5.99; 7.11] (N=164) 6.68 [6.00; 7.33] (N=38) 0.498e
Child age at 10-year visit (years)d 9.96 ± 0.75 (N=255) 9.97 ± 0.74 (N=50) 0.940a

BMI, body mass index; ETS, environmental tobacco smoke; Q1, 1st quartile; Q3, 3rd quartile; SD, standard deviation.

a

Calculated by independent samples t-test.

b

Calculated by the chi-square test.

c

Calculated by Fisher’s exact test.

d

Continuous covariate with missing data; sample size is presented.

e

Calculated by the Kruskal-Wallis test.

Early-Life Bacterial and Fungal Alpha Diversity Metrics and ADHD

Bacterial alpha diversity metrics in 1-month samples were not associated with ADHD (Figure 1, Supplemental Table 1; all p ≥ 0.57). At 6 months of age, both before and after covariate adjustment, Faith’s phylogenetic diversity was higher in children who developed ADHD (Figure 1, Supplemental Table 1; both p = 0.017). No other bacterial alpha diversity metrics in 6-month samples were associated with ADHD (Figure 1, Supplemental Table 1; all p ≥ 0.17).

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Gut bacterial microbiota alpha diversity metrics at 1 and 6 months of age in children who later developed Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) or were neurotypical (NT). Data are mean±standard deviation. Unadjusted (punadj) and adjusted (padj) p-values are presented; adjusted analyses included exact age at stool sample collection, maternal prenatal body mass index, prenatal antifungal use, maternal smoking during pregnancy, child sex, mode of delivery, gestational age at delivery, parity, and breastfeeding status.

Twenty-three (59%) children with ADHD and 84 (44%) NT children had 1-month fungal data (i.e., fungal ITS2 sequences). The fungal amplification rate was not statistically significantly different between the two groups (p = 0.13). Similarly, in 6-month stool samples, 23 (61%) children with ADHD had fungal data compared to 79 (48%) NT children (p = 0.23). Prior to covariate adjustment, fungal richness was higher (p = 0.046) at 6 months of age in children who developed ADHD; after covariate adjustment, this was slightly attenuated and no longer statistically significant (Figure 2, Supplemental Table 1; p = 0.083).

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Gut fungal microbiota alpha diversity metrics at 1 and 6 months of age in children who later developed Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) or were neurotypical (NT). Data are mean±standard deviation. Unadjusted (punadj) and adjusted (padj) p-values are presented; adjusted analyses included exact age at stool sample collection, maternal prenatal body mass index, prenatal antifungal use, maternal smoking during pregnancy, child sex, mode of delivery, gestational age at delivery, parity, and breastfeeding status.

The association between all alpha diversity (both bacterial and fungal) metrics and ADHD did not differ by child sex (all interaction p ≥ 0.17).

Early-Life Gut Microbiota Compositional Differences and ADHD

Bacterial microbiota composition was associated with development of ADHD (Table 2; Supplemental Figure 1). Bacterial composition at 6 months of age reached statistical significance before covariate adjustment (PERMANOVA; p = 0.012 and p = 0.034, for unweighted and weighted UniFrac, respectively); after covariate adjustment, only the unweighted UniFrac remained statistically significant (PERMANOVA; p = 0.006), explaining 0.9% of the variability. When dispersions of unweighted UniFrac at 6 months were compared between groups, the null hypothesis of homogeneous dispersions was rejected (p=0.036), with dispersions being smaller in ADHD relative to NT children (average distance to median=0.38, 0.40, respectively). The association between gut microbiota composition and ADHD did not differ by child sex (all interaction p ≥ 0.12).

Table 2.

Difference in stool beta diversity comparing children with ADHD to NT children.

Time Metric Unadjusted Adjusteda
N p-value R2 N p-value R2

Bacterial

1-month Unweighted UniFrac 229 0.50 0.004 216 0.27 0.005
Weighted UniFrac 229 0.79 0.002 216 0.59 0.003
6-months Unweighted UniFrac 202 0.012 0.009 190 0.006 0.009
Weighted UniFrac 202 0.034 0.012 190 0.057 0.012

Fungal

1-month Canberra 107 0.76 0.008 102 0.76 0.008
Bray-Curtis 107 0.61 0.007 102 0.67 0.007
6-months Canberra 102 0.22 0.011 96 0.36 0.011
Bray-Curtis 102 0.57 0.007 96 0.53 0.008

ADHD, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder; NT, neurotypical.

a

Adjusted for exact age at stool sample collection, maternal body mass index, prenatal antifungal use, maternal smoking during pregnancy, child sex, mode of delivery, gestational age at delivery, parity, and breastfeeding status.

Specific bacterial and fungal OTUs at 1 and 6 months of age associated with ADHD development after covariate adjustment are presented in Table 3 (unadjusted results are presented in Supplemental Table 2). At 1 month of age, 18 bacterial OTUs reached statistical significance after covariate adjustment and false discovery rate correction: 6 with lower abundance (including 2 Lachnospiraceae OTUs), and 12 with higher abundance in children who developed ADHD (including Collinsella, Akkermansia, and Dorea OTUs). At 6 months of age, 51 bacterial OTUs reached significance; Enterococcus and Ruminococcus OTUs were relatively depleted in children who developed ADHD, while Dorea and Blautia OTUs were relatively enriched. At the order level, 14 of these 51 OTUs were Lactobacillales (lactic acid bacteria), all but one of which was significantly depleted in children who developed ADHD.

Table 3:

Bacterial and fungal OTUs significantly associated with ADHD by age at time of stool sample.a

OTU Taxonomyb Estimatec p-value FDR corrected p-value

1-month bacterial
OTU_1685 Dorea −16.4 4.10E-06 5.24E-04
OTU_68 Pseudoramibacter_Eubacterium. −11.9 1.17E-10 5.95E-08
OTU_104 Lachnospiraceae −9.7 7.12E-04 2.27E-02
OTU_806 Lachnospiraceae −7.6 4.33E-07 1.11E-04
OTU_241 Prevotella −5.7 1.10E-05 6.04E-04
OTU_152 Veillonella −4.8 9.37E-07 1.60E-04
OTU_1347 Megasphaera 1.5 1.18E-05 6.04E-04
OTU_130 Clostridiaceae 3.1 7.61E-04 2.29E-02
OTU_45 Lactococcus 3.5 1.90E-04 6.93E-03
OTU_1329 Bacteroides 3.5 1.50E-03 4.25E-02
OTU_138 Streptococcus 3.7 2.81E-04 9.58E-03
OTU_523 Dorea 4.2 1.16E-05 6.04E-04
OTU_949 Erysipelotrichaceae 4.3 1.87E-04 6.93E-03
OTU_509 Dorea 5.3 8.98E-06 6.04E-04
OTU_391 Akkermansia muciniphila 6.8 3.69E-05 1.71E-03
OTU_292 Corynebacterium kroppenstedtii 7.5 6.99E-06 5.98E-04
OTU_242 Comamonadaceae 9.9 1.53E-04 6.50E-03
OTU_1492 Collinsella aerofaciens 13.8 7.02E-06 5.98E-04

6-months bacterial

OTU_57 Lachnospiraceae −8.8 2.51E-05 1.02E-03
OTU_144 Ruminococcus −7.4 2.66E-04 6.33E-03
OTU_1430 Bacteroides −5.7 1.88E-04 5.76E-03
OTU_318 Lachnospiraceae −5.3 1.69E-03 2.62E-02
OTU_533 Lachnospiraceae −4.9 4.02E-11 4.55E-09
OTU_1563 Enterococcus −3.7 4.69E-07 2.90E-05
OTU_1443 Streptococcus −3.5 2.06E-03 3.05E-02
OTU_636 Lachnospiraceae −3.3 8.88E-04 1.66E-02
OTU_128 Ruminococcus gnavus −2.2 8.19E-07 4.64E-05
OTU_986 Streptococcus −2.2 2.29E-04 6.08E-03
OTU_1134 Enterococcus −2.2 2.28E-03 3.30E-02
OTU_437 Enterococcaceae −2.1 1.26E-05 6.12E-04
OTU_360 Peptostreptococcaceae −2.1 2.70E-04 6.33E-03
OTU_1604 Enterococcaceae −2.1 2.40E-03 3.33E-02
OTU_1659 Enterococcus −2.1 2.51E-03 3.35E-02
OTU_1278 Enterococcus −2 1.69E-03 2.62E-02
OTU_923 Enterococcus −1.9 2.04E-04 5.78E-03
OTU_772 Enterococcus −1.9 2.64E-04 6.33E-03
OTU_1238 Lactobacillales −1.9 1.56E-03 2.55E-02
OTU_157 Clostridiales −1.8 2.32E-05 1.02E-03
OTU_1149 Enterobacteriaceae −1.7 9.05E-04 1.66E-02
OTU_395 Bifidobacterium −1.7 2.47E-03 3.35E-02
OTU_37 Staphylococcus −1.6 6.97E-04 1.39E-02
OTU_41 Ruminococcus −1.5 3.95E-04 8.95E-03
OTU_1161 Enterococcaceae −1.5 1.45E-03 2.47E-02
OTU_950 Ruminococcus gnavus −1.5 2.65E-03 3.47E-02
OTU_208 Erysipelotrichaceae −1.4 2.01E-03 3.03E-02
OTU_9 Enterococcus −1.3 7.21E-04 1.40E-02
OTU_77 Coprobacillus −1.2 1.57E-03 2.55E-02
OTU_1067 Clostridiales 1.1 1.16E-03 2.06E-02
OTU_1540 Enterobacteriaceae 1.3 5.35E-04 1.10E-02
OTU_810 Serratia marcescens 1.6 4.78E-06 2.50E-04
OTU_756 Dorea 1.7 3.06E-03 3.78E-02
OTU_781 Streptococcus 1.9 5.09E-04 1.08E-02
OTU_883 Blautia 2.4 4.28E-04 9.39E-03
OTU_1343 Peptostreptococcaceae 2.7 1.64E-09 1.60E-07
OTU_1411 Clostridiales 3.5 3.81E-15 5.18E-13
OTU_1089 Clostridiales 3.6 2.76E-03 3.55E-02
OTU_791 Lachnospiraceae 3.8 1.52E-04 4.92E-03
OTU_167 Lachnospiraceae 3.8 1.95E-04 5.76E-03
OTU_1587 Lachnospiraceae 3.8 2.35E-03 3.32E-02
OTU_356 Lachnospiraceae 4 2.47E-07 1.68E-05
OTU_520 Ruminococcus 4 2.88E-03 3.63E-02
OTU_1259 Ruminococcaceae 4.9 2.54E-05 1.02E-03
OTU_429 Peptoniphilus 5.3 9.09E-05 3.09E-03
OTU_189 Lachnospiraceae 5.5 8.08E-05 2.89E-03
OTU_1490 Dialister 5.7 1.18E-03 2.06E-02
OTU_410 Ruminococcaceae 5.8 3.10E-15 5.18E-13
OTU_76 Erysipelotrichaceae 6.4 4.00E-29 1.36E-26
OTU_82 Dorea 8.4 5.58E-05 2.11E-03
OTU_47 Bacteroides 10.7 2.33E-04 6.08E-03

1-month fungal

OTU_7 Rhodotorula mucilaginosa −4.4 4.81E-04 1.25E-02
OTU_411 Candida orthopsilosis −8.8 1.13E-03 1.47E-02
OTU_13 Malassezia globosa −1.9 2.05E-03 1.78E-02

6-months fungal

OTU_1093 Candida −3.5 1.93E-05 3.09E-04
OTU_1375 Saccharomyces −3.1 1.87E-04 1.50E-03
OTU_9 Trichosporon 11.0 6.38E-03 3.40E-02

ADHD, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder; FDR, false discovery rate; OTU, operational taxonomic units.

a

No overlapping OTUs in 1- and 6-month samples.

b

Most specific taxonomic rank available is noted.

c

Estimate from zero-inflated negative binomial or standard negative binomial model, after adjusting for exact age at stool sample collection, maternal body mass index, prenatal antifungal use, maternal smoking during pregnancy, child sex, mode of delivery, gestational age at delivery, parity, and breastfeeding status. Interpreted as the difference in the log OTU abundance, comparing children with ADHD to neurotypical children (i.e., positive values imply higher abundance in children with ADHD, while negative values imply lower abundance in children with ADHD).

After covariate adjustment, a total of 6 fungal OTUs were associated with ADHD development, 3 at each time point (Table 3; unadjusted results are presented in Supplemental Table 2). Specifically, all three significant fungal OTUs at 1 month (Rhodotorula mucilaginosa, Candida orthopsilosis, and Malassezia globose) were in lower abundance in children who developed ADHD. At 6-months of age, 2 fungal OTUs (1 Candida and 1 Saccharomyces OTU) were in lower abundance in children who developed ADHD while 1 (Trichosporon OTU) was relatively enriched.

Discussion

Our results indicate that the early-life gut microbiota is associated with ADHD at age 10 years. Specifically, children with ADHD had significantly higher phylogenetic diversity and distinct bacterial compositions at 6 months of age, characterized by a relative depletion of lactic acid bacteria, including Enterococcaceae and Lactobacillales.

Lactic acid bacteria are a diverse group of bacteria characterized by the primary end-product of carbohydrate metabolism being lactic acid36 and impact health through metabolism regulation, defense against infection, and immune activation.37 In a mouse model of dextran sulfate sodium-treated mice, administration of Enterococcus faecalis 2001 reversed neuroinflammation in the hippocampus and depressive symptoms.38 Neuroinflammation is a potential mechanistic factor in ADHD.39 Additionally, some Lactobacillus species are capable of producing γ-Aminobutyric Acid (GABA)40; lower GABA levels are associated with ADHD.41 Thus, the relative depletion of lactic acid bacteria in the gastrointestinal tracts of children at 6 months of age who later developed ADHD could be related to neuroinflammatory effects or reductions in GABA; however, this requires further study.

To our knowledge, no other study has examined the early-life gut microbiome with later development of ADHD, making direct comparisons to other studies difficult. Children with ADHD in the current study had significantly higher bacterial phylogenetic diversity (measured as Faith’s diversity) than NT children at 6 months of age. Previous studies of cross-sectional associations of gut microbiota alpha diversity metrics and ADHD have been mixed; 3 studies showed no differences in alpha diversity, another showed decreased Shannon diversity, and another study showed higher Shannon diversity and Chao index values, but lower Simpson index values, in children with ADHD compared to controls.811 Diversity in the infant gut increases with age.42 It is possible, although not part of the current analysis, that the association we found between higher bacterial diversity at 6 months of age and ADHD in the current study may actually reflect that children with ADHD experienced precocious gut maturation. Of note, in a study of Bangladeshi infants through age 2 years, two Dorea species were part of the 24 age-discriminant taxa identified;43 Dorea OTUs at both 1 month and 6 months of age were associated with ADHD development in the current study, and thus may suggest that aberrant gut microbiota maturation in early life may influence later ADHD development. Future studies that specifically investigate early maturation of the gut microbiota with neurodevelopment are needed.

Compositionally, previous cross-sectional studies have implicated Bacteroides, Bifidobacterium, Dialister, Enterococcus, Faecalibacterium, Odoribacter, Sutterella, and Veillonellaceae, as being in differential abundance comparing those with ADHD to healthy controls.811 In the current study, the early life abundance of Campylobacteraceae, Enterococcaceae, Eubacteriaceae, Ruminococcaceae, and Staphylococcaceae was associated with ADHD in preadolescence. The inconsistent findings across these studies and our own likely relate to the study design (cross-sectional vs. longitudinal) and the timing of gut microbiota measurement (mean age of stool collection ranged from 8 to 27 years, compared to 1 and 6 months of age in the current study).

Several mechanisms by which the gut microbiome could impact neurodevelopment have been postulated, including upregulation of proinflammatory cytokines, altered mitochondrial function, blood-brain barrier or gut permeability changes, and stimulation of afferent vagal nerves by peptides produced by gut microbiota.3 The gut microbiome may also influence available nutrients essential for the developing nervous system.44 Cowan et al suggested there are critical windows of gut microbiome development that correspond with neurodevelopment.44 In our study, the association of the gut bacterial microbiota with ADHD was more consistent at 6 months of age than 1 month of age, suggesting that exposures in later infancy (e.g., complementary food introduction) may influence or enhance risk of disease. Further study of the gut microbiota over more frequent time points in early life will be needed to identify specific critical windows of development.

A small body of literature in humans supports the role of the early-life microbiome in various aspects of neurodevelopment, including achievement of developmental milestones and variations in brain structure and function. Sordillo et al measured the infant gut microbiome (at 3 to 6 months of age) and the Ages and Stages Questionnaire, 3rd edition, at age 3 years in children participating in the VDAART trial; 4 bacterial co-abundance groups were identified.45 Clostridiales-dominated co-abundance scores were significantly associated with greater odds of communication skills and personal and social skills delays at 3 years of age whereas Bacteriodetes-dominated co-abundance scores were associated with greater odds of fine motor skills delays.45 Children with ADHD have weaker social skills46 and fine motor control,47 thus, these findings are consistent with an early influence of the gut microbiota on ADHD risk. In a study of 39 1-year-olds, higher Faith’s phylogenetic diversity was statistically significantly associated with functional connectivity in several brain regions.48 A number of studies have shown altered functional connectivity between various brain regions in those with and without ADHD.49, 50 In a study of 89 healthy children, gut microbiota was measured at age 1 year and the Mullen Scales of Early Learning was administered along with structural brain magnetic resonance imagining at ages 1 and 2 years.51 Higher alpha diversity at age 1 year was associated with an overall lower Mullen cognitive score as well as lower scores on the visual reception and expressive language scales at 2 years of age and with higher brain volumes in the precentral gyrus, left amygdala and right angular gyrus.51 Children with ADHD have deficits in language, including expressive language.52 Together, these data support a role for the gut microbiota in altering specific aspects of early development and brain structure and function that may precede diagnosis of ADHD.

Little is known about fungal microbiota and ADHD, with the exception of a case study suggesting that Candida infection worsened ADHD symptoms.53 A challenge in profiling the fungal microbiome is that DNA extraction steps need to be modified due to difficulties with fungal cell lysis.54 Although successful fungal amplification rates were relatively modest in our study despite making such modifications, we did detect fungal OTU differences in children with and without ADHD. Children with ADHD had a significant depletion of 3 fungal OTUs at 1 month (R. mucilaginosa, C. orthopsilosis, and M. globosa) while at age 6 months 2 fungal OTUs were depleted (a Candida and a Saccharomyces OTU) and 1 was enriched (Trichosporon OTU.). The relative depletion of some Candida in the early life gut of children with ADHD was unexpected, given that Candida overgrowth, particularly C. albicans, is associated with other neurodevelopmental disorders, including autism spectrum disorder.55, 56 Of note, Rhodotorula may strengthen intestinal barrier function and provide a probiotic effect,57 thus its relative depletion in early life may plausibly influence neurodevelopment. Additional studies are needed to confirm these findings.

Our findings may have implications for prevention of ADHD beginning in very early life. In a randomized trial, Pärtty et al found that among children supplemented with L. rhamnosus GG during the first 6 months of life, none developed ADHD by age 13 years.12 However, there is a paucity of randomized trial data on this topic.58 Observational study data, such as ours, may be critical to identify potential targets for intervention. In the current study we utilized 16S technology to identify potential compositional differences in the early-life gut microbiota of children who later developed ADHD. This does not provide information on the function of these organisms, which may be the best targets for intervention (e.g. production of short chain fatty acids, metabolism of omega-3 polyunsaturated fatty acids59). Future studies that utilize technologies such as metagenomics, metabolomics and/or metatranscriptomics59, 60 to understand the functional changes in the early-life gut are needed to design potential interventional trials for prevention of ADHD.

Our definition of ADHD was based on medical record or caregiver-reported doctor diagnosis. While we have previously shown high agreement between medical record and caregiver-reported ADHD diagnosis,15 we do not have data on ADHD subtype (inattentive, hyperactive, or combined), severity, or age at diagnosis, which could be differentially related to microbiota. We were able to adjust for a number of potential confounding factors (e.g., breastfeeding and mode of delivery). Some of these factors, however, may be on the causal pathway, thus models may be over-adjusted.61 ADHD is heritable,62 but we do not have data on family history of ADHD. It is possible we are missing an important confounding or effect modifying variable. Because we only have antibiotic information in early life on a subset of children, we did not include that as a covariate. In a previous publication of factors impacting early life microbiota in WHEALS children16, we failed to detect an association with infant antibiotic use, which was likely due to the small sample size of children with this covariate. While antibiotic use is known to impact gut microbiota, several studies suggest antibiotic use in early life is not associated with ADHD,63, 64 thus it is unlikely we missed an important confounder in this analysis. However, future studies with more complete data on both prenatal and early-life antibiotic use should examine these relationships further, including careful investigation by antibiotic timing and class. Although our measurement of the infant gut microbiota at 1 and 6 months of age aligns with the shift from predominantly a facultative anaerobe composition in the first few weeks of life to a strict anaerobe composition at around 6 months of age,65 even with two early life measurements, we may be missing key time points and inferences about key developmental windows should not be made from our study. However, the current study does provide compelling evidence suggesting the need for future studies with multiple stool sample collection over infancy/early childhood. In their recent review on gut microbiota and ADHD, Checa-Ros et al noted the considerable differences in associations of the gut microbiota and ADHD across studies;59 we also found some inconsistent findings between our work and previous studies. Previous studies have relied on cross-sectional study designs, and thus alterations in the gut microbiota in those studies could reflect differences due to ADHD treatment, differences in dietary patterns between children with and without ADHD, or reverse causality (changes in the gut microbiota caused by ADHD).66 We found evidence that the assumption of homogenous dispersions was not met. However, for unbalanced designs, PERMANOVA tests are typically too conservative if the larger group has greater dispersion,67 so despite not meeting the homogeneity assumption, results are unlikely to be a false positive.

A strength of the current study is the longitudinal design, with measurement of the gut microbiota preceding ADHD diagnosis by years, and thus differences in the gut microbiota found in our study are not due to ADHD treatment. As described in Checa-Ros et al, future studies with standardized protocols leveraging high-resolution techniques such as shotgun metagenomics are needed to reduce heterogeneity in results across different studies.59

In summary, the early-life gut microbiota is associated with ADHD in preadolescence. Future studies replicating these findings and evaluating potential functional differences are needed.

Supplementary Material

1792597_Sup_Fig
1792597_Sup_Tab_1
1792597_Sup_Tab_2

Impact:

  • Cross-sectional studies suggest the gut microbiota of individuals with and without ADHD differ

  • We found evidence that the bacterial gut microbiota of infants at ages 1 month and 6 months of age is associated with ADHD at age 10 years

  • We also found novel evidence that the fungal gut microbiota in infancy (ages 1 month and 6 months) is associated with ADHD at age 10 years

  • This study addresses a gap in the literature in providing longitudinal evidence for an association of the infant gut microbiota with later ADHD development

Funding:

This study was supported by the National Institutes of Health (R01 AI050681, R01 HL113010, R01 HD082147, and P01 AI089473) and the Fund for Henry Ford Hospital. The Funders had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

Footnotes

Disclosures: Dr. Lynch is a board member and consultant for Siolta Therapeutics Inc. All other authors declare no conflict of interest.

Consent: Parental written informed consent and pre-adolescent written informed assent was obtained for the conduct of the study. Specific patient consent was not required for the current manuscript.

Category of Study: Population Study.

References

  • 1.Tarver J, Daley D & Sayal K Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): an updated review of the essential facts. Child Care Health Dev. 40, 762–774 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Borre YE, et al. Microbiota and neurodevelopmental windows: implications for brain disorders. Trends Mol Med. 20, 509–518 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Principi N & Esposito S Gut microbiota and central nervous system development. J Infect. 73, 536–546 (2016). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.McDonald D, et al. Towards large-cohort comparative studies to define the factors influencing the gut microbial community structure of ASD patients. Microb Ecol Health Dis. 26, 26555 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Gilbert JA, et al. Current understanding of the human microbiome. Nat Med. 24, 392–400 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Ly V, et al. Elimination diets’ efficacy and mechanisms in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and autism spectrum disorder. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 26, 1067–1079 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Evans JM, Morris LS & Marchesi JR The gut microbiome: the role of a virtual organ in the endocrinology of the host. J Endocrinol. 218, R37–47 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Aarts E, et al. Gut microbiome in ADHD and its relation to neural reward anticipation. PLoS One. 12, e0183509 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Jiang HY, et al. Gut microbiota profiles in treatment-naive children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Behav Brain Res. 347, 408–413 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Wang LJ, et al. Gut microbiota and dietary patterns in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 29, 287–297 (2020). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Wan L, et al. Case-control study of the effects of gut microbiota composition on neurotransmitter metabolic pathways in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Front Neurosci. 14, 127 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Partty A, Kalliomaki M, Wacklin P, Salminen S & Isolauri E A possible link between early probiotic intervention and the risk of neuropsychiatric disorders later in childhood: a randomized trial. Pediatr Res. 77, 823–828 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Havstad S, et al. Effect of prenatal indoor pet exposure on the trajectory of total IgE levels in early childhood. J Allergy Clin Immunol. 128, 880–885 e884 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Wegienka G, et al. Racial disparities in allergic outcomes in African Americans emerge as early as age 2 years. Clin Exp Allergy. 42, 909–917 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Cassidy-Bushrow AE, et al. Prenatal pet keeping and caregiver-reported attention deficit hyperactivity disorder through preadolescence in a United States birth cohort. BMC Pediatr. 19, 390 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Levin AM, et al. Joint effects of pregnancy, sociocultural, and environmental factors on early life gut microbiome structure and diversity. Sci Rep. 6, 31775 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Caporaso JG, et al. Ultra-high-throughput microbial community analysis on the Illumina HiSeq and MiSeq platforms. ISME J. 6, 1621–1624 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Sitarik AR, et al. Fetal and early postnatal lead exposure measured in teeth associates with infant gut microbiota. Environ Int. 144, 106062 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Edgar RC, Haas BJ, Clemente JC, Quince C & Knight R UCHIME improves sensitivity and speed of chimera detection. Bioinformatics. 27, 2194–2200 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Edgar RC UPARSE: highly accurate OTU sequences from microbial amplicon reads. Nat Methods. 10, 996–998 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.McDonald D, et al. An improved Greengenes taxonomy with explicit ranks for ecological and evolutionary analyses of bacteria and archaea. ISME J. 6, 610–618 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Caporaso JG, et al. PyNAST: a flexible tool for aligning sequences to a template alignment. Bioinformatics. 26, 266–267 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Price MN, Dehal PS & Arkin AP FastTree: computing large minimum evolution trees with profiles instead of a distance matrix. Mol Biol Evol. 26, 1641–1650 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Fujimura KE, et al. Neonatal gut microbiota associates with childhood multisensitized atopy and T cell differentiation. Nat Med. 22, 1187–1191 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Martin M Cutadapt removes adapter sequences from high-throughput sequencing reads. EMBnet.journal. 17, 3 (2011). [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Koljalg U, et al. Towards a unified paradigm for sequence-based identification of fungi. Mol Ecol. 22, 5271–5277 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Wegienka G, et al. Combined effects of prenatal medication use and delivery type are associated with eczema at age 2 years. Clin Exp Allergy. 45, 660–668 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Oksanen J, et al. 2019. vegan: Community Ecology Package. R package version 2.5–6. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=vegan.
  • 29.Oksanen J, et al. 2012. Community Ecology Package. R Version 2.0–5. http://cran.r-project.org, http://vegan.r-forge.r-project.org/.
  • 30.Lozupone C & Knight R UniFrac: a new phylogenetic method for comparing microbial communities. Appl Environ Microbiol. 71, 8228–8235 (2005). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Legendre P & De Cáceres M Beta diversity as the variance of community data: dissimilarity coefficients and partitioning. Ecol Lett. 16, 951–963 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Zeileis A, Kleiber C & Jackman S Regression Models for Count Data in R. J Stat Softw 27, 1–25 (2008). [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Venables WN & Ripley BD Modern Applied Statistics with S Plus. 4th edn. (Springer, New York, 2002). [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Benjamini Y & Hochberg Y Controlling the False Discovery Rate: A Practical and Powerful Approach to Multiple Testing. J R Stat Soc Ser B Methodol. 57, 289–300 (1995). [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Selvin S Statistical Analysis of Epidemiologic Data, 2nd edn. (Oxford University Press, New York, 1996). [Google Scholar]
  • 36.George F, et al. Occurrence and Dynamism of Lactic Acid Bacteria in Distinct Ecological Niches: A Multifaceted Functional Health Perspective. Front Microbiol. 9, 2899 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Pessione E Lactic acid bacteria contribution to gut microbiota complexity: lights and shadows. Front Cell Infect Microbiol. 2, 86 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Takahashi K, et al. Effect of Enterococcus faecalis 2001 on colitis and depressive-like behavior in dextran sulfate sodium-treated mice: involvement of the brain-gut axis. J Neuroinflammation. 16, 201 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Dunn GA, Nigg JT & Sullivan EL Neuroinflammation as a risk factor for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 182, 22–34 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Yogeswara IBA, Maneerat S & Haltrich D Glutamate Decarboxylase from Lactic Acid Bacteria-A Key Enzyme in GABA Synthesis. Microorganisms. 8, 1923 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Mamiya PC, Arnett AB & Stein MA Precision Medicine Care in ADHD: The Case for Neural Excitation and Inhibition. Brain Sci. 11, 91 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Tanaka M & Nakayama J Development of the gut microbiota in infancy and its impact on health in later life. Allergol Int. 66, 515–522 (2017). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Subramanian S, et al. Persistent gut microbiota immaturity in malnourished Bangladeshi children. Nature. 510, 417–421 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Cowan CSM, Dinan TG & Cryan JF Annual Research Review: Critical windows - the microbiota-gut-brain axis in neurocognitive development. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 61, 353–371 (2020). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Sordillo JE, et al. Association of the Infant Gut Microbiome With Early Childhood Neurodevelopmental Outcomes: An Ancillary Study to the VDAART Randomized Clinical Trial. JAMA Netw Open. 2, e190905 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Ros R & Graziano PA Social Functioning in Children With or At Risk for Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: A Meta-Analytic Review. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 47, 213–235 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Kaiser ML, Schoemaker MM, Albaret JM & Geuze RH What is the evidence of impaired motor skills and motor control among children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)? Systematic review of the literature. Res Dev Disabil 36c, 338–357 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Gao W, et al. Gut microbiome and brain functional connectivity in infants-a preliminary study focusing on the amygdala. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 236, 1641–1651 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Castellanos FX & Aoki Y Intrinsic Functional Connectivity in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: A Science in Development. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. 1, 253–261 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Uddin LQ, Dajani DR, Voorhies W, Bednarz H & Kana RK Progress and roadblocks in the search for brain-based biomarkers of autism and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Transl Psychiatry. 7, e1218 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Carlson AL, et al. Infant Gut Microbiome Associated With Cognitive Development. Biol Psychiatry. 83, 148–159 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Korrel H, Mueller KL, Silk T, Anderson V & Sciberras E Research Review: Language problems in children with Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder - a systematic meta-analytic review. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 58, 640–654 (2017). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Rucklidge JJ Could yeast infections impair recovery from mental illness? A case study using micronutrients and olive leaf extract for the treatment of ADHD and depression. Adv Mind Body Med. 27, 14–18 (2013). [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Forbes JD, Bernstein CN, Tremlett H, Van Domselaar G & Knox NC A fungal world: could the gut mycobiome be involved in neurological disease? Front Microbiol. 9, 3249 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Iovene MR, et al. Intestinal Dysbiosis and Yeast Isolation in Stool of Subjects with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Mycopathologia. 182, 349–363 (2017). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Strati F, et al. New evidences on the altered gut microbiota in autism spectrum disorders. Microbiome. 5, 24 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Hof H Rhodotorula spp. in the gut - foe or friend? GMS Infect Dis. 7, Doc02 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Rianda D, Agustina R, Setiawan EA & Manikam NRM Effect of probiotic supplementation on cognitive function in children and adolescents: a systematic review of randomised trials. Benef Microbes. 10, 873–882 (2019). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Checa-Ros A, Jeréz-Calero A, Molina-Carballo A, Campoy C & Muñoz-Hoyos A Current Evidence on the Role of the Gut Microbiome in ADHD Pathophysiology and Therapeutic Implications. Nutrients. 13, 249 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Rojo D, et al. Exploring the human microbiome from multiple perspectives: factors altering its composition and function. FEMS Microbiol Rev. 41, 453–478 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Schisterman EF, Cole SR & Platt RW Overadjustment bias and unnecessary adjustment in epidemiologic studies. Epidemiology. 20, 488–495 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Biederman J & Faraone SV Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Lancet. 366, 237–248 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Hamad AF, Alessi-Severini S, Mahmud SM, Brownell M & Kuo IF Antibiotic Exposure in the First Year of Life and the Risk of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: A Population-Based Cohort Study. Am J Epidemiol. 188, 1923–1931 (2019). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Axelsson PB, et al. Investigating the effects of cesarean delivery and antibiotic use in early childhood on risk of later attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 60, 151–159 (2019). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Jena A, et al. Gut-Brain Axis in the Early Postnatal Years of Life: A Developmental Perspective. Front Integr Neurosci. 14, 44 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Chou WJ, et al. Dietary and nutrient status of children with attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disorder: a case-control study. Asia Pac J Clin Nutr. 27, 1325–1331 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Anderson MJ & Walsh DCI PERMANOVA, ANOSIM, and the Mantel test in the face of heterogeneous dispersions: What null hypothesis are you testing? Ecol Monogr. 83, 557–574 (2013). [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

1792597_Sup_Fig
1792597_Sup_Tab_1
1792597_Sup_Tab_2

RESOURCES