Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2022 Oct 12;11:e74270. doi: 10.7554/eLife.74270

KLC4 shapes axon arbors during development and mediates adult behavior

Elizabeth M Haynes 1,2,3,4, Korri H Burnett 1,3, Jiaye He 4,5, Marcel W Jean-Pierre 1,3, Martin Jarzyna 1,3, Kevin W Eliceiri 2,4, Jan Huisken 1,4,6, Mary C Halloran 1,3,
Editors: Fabienne E Poulain7, Didier YR Stainier8
PMCID: PMC9596160  PMID: 36222498

Abstract

Development of elaborate and polarized neuronal morphology requires precisely regulated transport of cellular cargos by motor proteins such as kinesin-1. Kinesin-1 has numerous cellular cargos which must be delivered to unique neuronal compartments. The process by which this motor selectively transports and delivers cargo to regulate neuronal morphogenesis is poorly understood, although the cargo-binding kinesin light chain (KLC) subunits contribute to specificity. Our work implicates one such subunit, KLC4, as an essential regulator of axon branching and arborization pattern of sensory neurons during development. Using live imaging approaches in klc4 mutant zebrafish, we show that KLC4 is required for stabilization of nascent axon branches, proper microtubule (MT) dynamics, and endosomal transport. Furthermore, KLC4 is required for proper tiling of peripheral axon arbors: in klc4 mutants, peripheral axons showed abnormal fasciculation, a behavior characteristic of central axons. This result suggests that KLC4 patterns axonal compartments and helps establish molecular differences between central and peripheral axons. Finally, we find that klc4 mutant larva are hypersensitive to touch and adults show anxiety-like behavior in a novel tank test, implicating klc4 as a new gene involved in stress response circuits.

Research organism: Zebrafish

Introduction

Proper development of complex neuronal morphology and assembly of neural circuits requires tight regulation of axon growth and branching. Precise control over axon branch formation is essential not only for development but also for plasticity and regeneration after injury. Branching allows one neuron to innervate numerous targets, and in many cases, neurons extend multiple axons or branches that navigate to distinct targets. Many molecular signaling pathways can influence branching (Gallo, 2011; Kalil and Dent, 2014), although the mechanisms that control branch formation are still not fully understood. Highly arborized vertebrate sensory neurons, including Rohon-Beard (RB) neurons, serve as an ideal model to study mechanisms of branching and neuronal morphogenesis. Sensory neurons must extend separate axon branches to targets in the periphery and to the CNS to relay sensory information. Peripheral and central axons have different capacity for branching (Andersen et al., 2011), and behave differently when contacting other axons—central axons fasciculate with one another while peripheral axons show contact repulsion (Liu and Halloran, 2005; Sagasti et al., 2005). Contact repulsion of peripheral axons is necessary for establishment of proper axon arbor patterns that ‘tile’ with arbors from other neurons to effectively innervate the sensory field. Despite the importance of establishing axons with distinct character and branching patterns, the mechanisms regulating molecular compartmentalization of separate axons from one neuron are poorly understood.

Molecular compartmentalization of neurons depends on precisely controlled transport of vesicle and organelle cargos to specific cell locations. Cargos are transported by motor proteins that move in a directional manner along MT tracks. Kinesin-1, the founding member of the kinesin superfamily of motor proteins, is a heterotetramer composed of homodimers of kinesin heavy chains (KHCs), which contain the MT binding and ATPase motor domains, and of kinesin light chains (KLCs), which interact with cargos. Kinesin-1 can mediate long-distance, anterograde transport of multiple cargos including mitochondria, synaptic vesicle precursors, endosomes, lysosomes, and RNA granules (Hirokawa et al., 2010; Morfini et al., 2016). Kinesin-1 also has roles in organizing MT tracks. The forces it exerts can physically influence the MT lattice (Shima et al., 2018; Triclin et al., 2021). It can also cross-link MTs by combined interactions of the motor domain and a second MT binding site in the KHC tail, and slide MTs along one another, thereby influencing cell shape (Lu and Gelfand, 2017). While it is clear that kinesin-1 is essential for proper development of the nervous system, the mechanisms by which it achieves cell-context specificity of its multiple functions and how its cargo-binding and MT-organizing functions are regulated have yet to be fully defined.

The specific activities of kinesin-1 in a given context are likely governed in part by KLCs. KLCs are known to mediate binding between the kinesin motor and cellular cargos, and also to activate the motor by releasing KHC autoinhibition (Morfini et al., 2016; Wong et al., 2010; Woźniak and Allan, 2006). Vertebrates have four klc genes (klc1-4), with klc1, 2 and 4 expressed in the nervous system, but our knowledge of the different roles played by individual KLCs in neurons remains very limited. In Drosophila, mutants for the single klc gene are lethal in larval stages and have defects in neural development (Gindhart et al., 1998; Mochizuki et al., 2011), indicating essential neural functions for KLC. Growing evidence supports the idea that molecular functions are divided among vertebrate KLCs and that each has unique roles. Structural and biochemical studies show that KLC1 and KLC2 have differing affinities for cargo adaptor proteins such as JIP1 and JIP3 (Cockburn et al., 2018; Pernigo et al., 2018; Zhu et al., 2012). Research to date has begun to define distinct neurophysiological functions of KLC1 and KLC2. Studies of mouse klc1 mutants revealed functions for KLC1 in regulating motor behavior (Rahman et al., 1999), transport of amyloid precursor protein into axons (Kamal et al., 2000), phagosome transport in retinal pigment epithelium (Jiang et al., 2015), and axon guidance and cannabinoid recepter transport during neural development (Saez et al., 2020). Mouse klc2 mutants have hearing loss, cochlear hair cell loss and mislocalized mitochondria in hair cells (Fu et al., 2021). A study using a peptide inhibitor of KLC2 phosphorylation showed that KLC2 regulates synaptic plasticity, AMPAR trafficking and mood associated behavior (Du et al., 2010). Mutations in human klc2 can cause SPOAN syndrome, which is characterized by progressive spastic paraplegia, optic atrophy and neuropathy with onset in infancy (Bazvand et al., 2021; Melo et al., 2015). On the whole, previous work supports the idea that individual KLCs have unique, indispensable functions, but has only begun to reveal the specific cellular functions for individual KLCs.

In contrast to KLC1 and KLC2, KLC4 has been the subject of few studies. Mutation of KLC4 in humans causes a type of hereditary spastic paraplegia (HSP) that manifests in early childhood (Bayrakli et al., 2015), indicating essential developmental functions. To date, the only cellular function shown for KLC4 is that it supports mitochondrial function and confers resistance to radiation in cancer cells (Baek et al., 2018). A clear cellular function for KLC4 in neurons and therefore insight into the KLC4 mutation-related pathology is lacking.

In this study, we reveal critical functions for KLC4 in sensory axon branching and axon arbor patterning during development, and in regulation of animal behavior. Using live imaging in klc4 mutant zebrafish, we found that KLC4 is required for stabilization of nascent peripheral sensory axon branches. The branching defects do not reflect a general growth defect, as axons grew faster in klc4 mutants and did not show signs of early degeneration. Live imaging also showed that klc4 mutant peripheral axons have defects in mutual repulsion and instead often fasciculate with one another, a behavior characteristic of central axons, suggesting KLC4 may contribute to the molecular compartmentalization that defines differential behavior of these axons. Using high-speed, in vivo imaging of endosomal vesicle transport and MT dynamics, we showed that klc4 mutant axons have altered vesicle transport and MT dynamics. Nascent branches in klc4 mutants have less acetylated tubulin, suggesting less stable MTs. Finally, we investigated the ramifications of KLC4 loss on larval and adult behavior. Larval klc4 mutant zebrafish show hyperactivity in response to touch, implying heightened sensitivity. Adult klc4 mutants exhibit increased anxiety-like behavior, indicating essential roles for KLC4 in neural circuit formation and/or function in adults.

Results

klc4 is expressed during early development

klc genes encode proteins with conserved functional domains including the heptad repeat region that mediates binding to KHC, six cargo-binding tetratricopeptide repeats (TPRs), a C-terminal lipid-binding amphipathic helix, and an LFP motif that mediates KLC auto-inhibition (Figure 1A; Antón et al., 2021; Cockburn et al., 2018; Morfini et al., 2016; Pernigo et al., 2018; Zhu et al., 2012). As a first step toward understanding developmental functions of KLC4, we investigated the expression of zebrafish klc4 at different stages of development. Zebrafish klc4 has two alternative splice isoforms, one long and one short, both of which are also present in other vertebrates including mice and humans. In zebrafish, the long klc4 transcript encodes a full length protein of 620 amino acids while the short isoform is predicted to encode a 185 amino acid protein, ending just before the LFP motif (Figure 1—figure supplement 1A). To determine whether both transcripts are expressed during development, we performed non-quantitative reverse transcriptase-PCR with transcript-specific primers on RNA extracted from embryos during early development (Figure 1B, Figure 1—figure supplement 1A). We found that both isoforms are expressed throughout early development, with expression detectable at 2 hours post fertilization (hpf) and continuing through the initial period of neuronal axon outgrowth, pathfinding and branching, between 16 and 28 hpf (Figure 1B). We next used in situ hybridization to determine the spatial expression pattern of klc4 during development, using a riboprobe that binds both transcripts. At 24 hpf, klc4 is expressed in clusters of neurons in the telencephalon, epiphysis, diencephalon, ventral midbrain and hindbrain, and in the trigeminal and the posterior lateral line (pLL) ganglia (Figure 1C and D). In the spinal cord, klc4 is strongly expressed in the Rohon-Beard sensory neurons, and at lower levels in other neuron populations (Figure 1E). At 48 hpf, klc4 continues to be expressed broadly in the nervous system with high expression levels in the trigeminal ganglia, pLL ganglia, and RB neurons (Figure 1F, G and H). A riboprobe that only binds the long isoform shows a similar expression pattern to the probe that recognizes both isoforms (Figure 1I, J and K), indicating the full-length klc4 mRNA is present in developing neurons. The early expression of klc4 in diverse neural populations in both the central and peripheral nervous system suggests it has functions in neural development.

Figure 1. klc4 mRNA is expressed in developing zebrafish embryos.

(A) Diagram of the highly conserved domains of KLC4. Arrows indicate where mutation occurs in humans and in our zebrafish mutant. (B) Qualitative RT-PCR showing both long and short klc4 isoforms are expressed throughout development. (C–D) In situ hybridization for klc4 mRNA shown in lateral view (C) or dorsal view (D) of the head in wild type 24 hpf embryos. (E) Lateral view of trunk showing klc4 expression in Rohon-Beard sensory neurons in a 24 hpf wild type embryo. (F–G) klc4 expression in the lateral view (F) or dorsal view (G) of the head of a 48 hpf wild type embryo. (H) Lateral view of trunk showing klc4 expression in the spinal cord of a 48 hpf wild type embryo. (I–K) Lateral (I,K) and dorsal (J) views of klc4 long isoform specific in situ hybridization in wild type 24 hpf embryos. Lateral (L,N) and dorsal (M) views of klc4 in situ hybridization in 24 hpf klc4uw314 mutant embryo head (L,M) and trunk (N) showing little expression of klc4. Te = telencephalon, dien = diencephalon, mb = midbrain, hb = hindbrain, tg = trigeminal ganglion, pllg = posterior lateral line ganglion, rb = rohon beard neuron.

Figure 1—source data 1. Original.tiff and.scn file for the gel of RT-PCR results showing presence of klc4 isoforms.

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. PCR strategy for the detection of long and short klc4 isoforms, and sequence alignment of wild type and klc4uw314 mutant.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

(A) Diagram of the primer design for the RT-PCR of the long and short isoforms of klc4. For the long isoform, the primers were designed for regions of exon 8 (fwd) and exon 12 (rev), which are not contained in the short isoform. The amplified product is 421 bp. For the short isoform, the primers were designed against exon 2 (contained in long isoform) and the short isoform 3’UTR (not contained in long isoform). The amplified product is 523 bp. (B) The sequence alignment around the 25 bp deletion within exon 3 contained in klc4uw314 mutants.

Generation of klc4 mutants

To assess the role of KLC4 during neural development, we used CRISPR/Cas9 to generate klc4 mutants. We used a guide RNA targeting exon 3 of klc4, which encodes a part of the N-terminal heptad repeat region. One resulting allele, klc4uw314, has a 25 bp deletion in exon 3 from 404bp to 428bp in the heptad repeat region that leads to an early stop after 9 missense amino acids (Figure 1A, Figure 1—figure supplement 1B). Heterozygous klc4uw314 mutants were outcrossed to wild type (AB strain) fish for three generations to remove any off-target mutations from the background. In situ hybridization analysis of homozygous klc4uw314 mutant embryos showed virtually no klc4 mRNA expression, suggesting that the mutant transcript undergoes nonsense-mediated decay (Figure 1L, M and N).

klc4uw314 mutants show reduced branching of RB peripheral sensory axons

Because klc4 is highly expressed in spinal RB sensory neurons, we first analyzed the effects of klc4 mutation on their axon growth and morphology. We crossed the klc4uw314 mutants with a transgenic line in which RB neurons are labeled with membrane-targeted GFP [Tg(–3.1ngn1:gfp-caax)] (Andersen et al., 2011) and used light sheet microscopy to image RB axon development across the whole embryo (Figure 2A and B). Inspection of mutant embryos revealed that RB peripheral axon branches appeared less dense and displayed less overall innervation of the sensory field compared to wild type (Figure 2A and B, Figure 2—videos 1 and 2). To quantify these differences we labeled sensory neurons with anti-HNK1 antibody at 24 hpf (Figure 2C, D and E). We counted the number of axons that crossed eight regions of interest drawn at standardized intervals from the central axon fascicle, proceeding from dorsal to ventral (Figure 2—figure supplement 1). Consistent with our visual impression of reduced branching, mutant embryos had consistently fewer axons at each ROI (Figure 2F). The reduction in branches at the most dorsal levels in the mutant embryos may indicate an effect on initiation of the peripheral axon, which most often forms as a branch from the central axon near the cell body (Andersen et al., 2011).

Figure 2. klc4uw314 mutants have reduced Rohon-Beard peripheral axon branching.

(A–B) Stills taken at approximately 24 hpf from long term light sheet movies of wild type (A) or klc4uw314 mutant (B) embryo development Scale bar = 150µm. (C) Diagram of a 24 hpf embryo indicating the area over the yolk tube extension imaged for axon analysis, with the standard region used for analysis outlined by the white dashed rectangle. (D–E) Representative examples of HNK-1 stained embryos showing peripheral axon branching in wild type (D) and klc4uw314 I embryos. (F–G) Quantification of axon branching across three technical replicates in wild type (n=32) and klc4uw314 mutant (n=32) embryos (F) and DMSO (n=12) and kinesore (n=17) treated embryos (G). Error bars = SEM. Statistical significance was measured by comparing the area under the curve for (F–G) using t-test. For (F), *p=0.0378 for entire graph. For (G), *p=0.0484 for shaded region. (H–I) Representative examples of HNK-1 stained embryos showing peripheral axon branching in DMSO (H) and kinesore treated (I) embryos. Scale bars for D,E,H,I = 50 µm.

Figure 2—source data 1. The number of branches crossing a selected region of interest (1-8) for wild type and klc4uw314 mutant embryos.
Figure 2—source data 2. The number of branches crossing a selected region of interest (1-8) for wild type embryos treated with either 2% DMSO or 100 µM Kinesore.

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. Schematic describing our branch analysis method.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

A standard sized ROI (ROItrunk) from the trunk over the yolk tube extension is selected with the top of the ROItrunk aligned to the bottom of the central axon fascicle. Eight 3 µm ROIs (white and green, ROIn, n=1–8) are selected at regularly spaced intervals along ROItrunk. Each ROIn is duplicated and then thresholded into a binary image. This binary image is run through Particle Analysis in ImageJ, which counts the number of particles present with gating for size and shape characteristics. We use the number of particles counted to represent the number of branches passing through ROIn.
Figure 2—video 1. A wild type Tg(–3.1ngn1:gfp-caax) embryo imaged by light sheet microscopy over 11.6 hr to show sensory axon development, from approximately 18 to 29.6 hpf.
Download video file (8.5MB, mp4)
Anterior is to the left.
Figure 2—video 2. A klc4uw314 mutant Tg(–3.1ngn1:gfp-caax) embryo imaged by light sheet microscopy over 8.6 hr to show sensory axon development, from approximately 19 to 27.6 hpf.
Download video file (6MB, mp4)
Anterior is to the left.

As an alternative approach to understand the role of KLCs in axon branching, and to develop a more time-resolved means to disrupt KLC function, we took a pharmacological approach. Specifically, embryos were treated with Kinesore, a small molecule that has been shown to interfere with the interaction between the KLC TPR repeat domain and cargo adaptor proteins, while also relieving KLC auto-inhibition and activating kinesin-1’s ability to cross-link MTs and regulate MT dynamics (Randall et al., 2017). We found that treatment of wild type embryos with 100 µM Kinesore during early stages of peripheral axon arborization, from 17 hpf to 24 hpf, caused a reduction in axon branch number in ventral regions at 24 hpf of a similar magnitude to that seen in klc4uw314 mutants (Figure 2G-I), although more dorsal branching appears unaffected. These results give further evidence that properly regulated KLC-cargo interactions or kinesin-1-MT interactions during axon outgrowth are important for successful axon branching.

KLC4 is required for stabilization of axon branches

To investigate which specific dynamic processes during branching are affected by klc4 loss of function, we used live confocal imaging. Zebrafish RB axons branch by two mechanisms: growth cone bifurcation (GCB) and interstitial back branching (IB) (Gibson and Ma, 2011). To determine if GCB, IB or both were defective in mutants, we used the Tg(–3.1ngn1:gfp-caax) transgenic line (Andersen et al., 2011). Z-stacks of RB peripheral axon arbors were collected every 1–1.5 min for 2–4 hr beginning from approximately 18 hpf. The number of branch initiation and retraction events during a 2-hr time window were counted and categorized as either a GCB (Figure 3A, white arrows) or IB event (Figure 3A, magenta arrows). Surprisingly, we found no difference in the rate of branch initiation between klc4uw314 mutant and wild-type embryos (Figure 3C). In both conditions, new branches were initiated via GCB more frequently than IB, but there was no difference in the proportion of GCB versus IB branching events between wild-type and klc4uw314 mutants.

Figure 3. Branches initiate but do not stabilize in klc4uw314 mutants.

(A) A wild type axon forms stable branches by growth cone bifurcation (GCB, white arrows) and interstitial branching (IB, magenta arrows) over the course of 38’ min. (B) A klc4uw314 mutant axon initiates a branch via GCB (0minutes, white arrow), but one branch begins to retract and is nearly fully gone after 66’ min (panel 3, white arrow). 20 µm scale bar. (C) Quantification of axon branch initiations per neuron per hour in wild type and mutant embryos wild type GCB mean = 2.22 branches/neuron/hr, N=11 neurons from 4 embryos. Klc4uw314 GCB = 2.13 branches/neuron/hr, N=14 neurons from 4 embryos. Wild type IB=0.99 branches/neuron/hr, N=10 neurons from 4 embryos. Klc4uw314 IB = 0.65 branches/neuron/hr, N=8 neurons from 4 embryos. Error bars = SEM. There was no significant difference in branch initiation between wild type and mutant axons. GCB P=0.73, IB P=0.15, Mann–Whitney test. (D) Quantification of branches that retracted after initiation in wild type and klc4uw314 embryos. Data are displayed as a branch loss ratio (branches retracted/total branching events). Branches created by either GCB or IB were both more likely to retract in klc4uw314 embryos (wild type GCB mean = 0.13, N=11 neurons from 4 embryos; klc4uw314 GCB = 0.58, N=14 neurons from 4 embryos; wild type IB = 0.10, N=10 neurons from 4 embryos; klc4uw314 IB = 0.62, N=8 neurons from 4 embryos). **p=0.0055, ****p<0.0001, Mann–Whitney test. Error bars = SEM. (E) Quantification of axon growth velocity. Axons in klc4uw314 mutants grow faster than in wild type. Wild type N=19 axons from 3 embryos. Klc4uw314 N=22 axons from 4 embryos. ***P=0.0001, Mann-Whitney test. Error bars = SEM.

Figure 3—source data 1. The number of branches initiated per neuron per hour separated by type of branch (growth cone bifurcation = GCB, Interstitial Branch = IB).
Figure 3—source data 2. The number of branches of a category (growth cone bifurcation = GCB, Interstitial Branch = IB) that retracted divided by the total number of branching events of that category.
Figure 3—source data 3. The growth speed of wild type and klc4uw314 mutant embryos.

Figure 3.

Figure 3—video 1. Branches in a wild type Tg(-3.1ngn1:gfp-caax) embryo growing and spreading over 15 min.
Download video file (341.7KB, mp4)
Figure 3—video 2. An example of a growth cone bifurcation in a klc4uw314 mutant Tg(-3.1ngn1:gfp-caax) embryo where the left branch begins to retract at approximately 23 min.
Download video file (432KB, mp4)

Nevertheless, the live imaging immediately suggested an explanation for the branch number deficit: failure of branches to persist. That is, following formation, while most of the branches in wild type animals were relatively stable and continued growing (Figure 3—video 1), those in the mutants often retracted after formation (Figure 3B, Figure 3—video 2). Quantification showed that in klc4uw314 mutants, 57.7% of GCB branches and 61.9% of IB branches retracted after initiating, compared to 14.2% of GCB and 11.8% of IB branches in wild type (Figure 3D). Mutants showed no significant directional bias to the retracted branches (on average, 52.5% of branches lost per neuron were anteriorly-directed, and 47.2% were posteriorly-directed. p=0.6588). This phenotype cannot be explained as a general axon growth defect, as klc4uw314 mutant axons grew at a faster rate than wild type axons (Figure 3E). Thus, these data suggest that KLC4 is required specifically for stabilization of axon branches.

Sensory axon arborization pattern is altered in klc4uw314 mutants

Peripheral sensory axons develop a distinctive arborization pattern to successfully innervate the sensory field. During this process, axons mutually repel each other upon contact (Sagasti et al., 2005; Liu and Halloran, 2005). Precise regulation of contact repulsion as well as branching are critical for effective tiling of the entire skin. To determine whether mutants have altered arborization and to quantify branching pattern in an unbiased manner, we developed an image analysis pipeline to measure axon directional orientation and overall axon abundance (Figure 4). We used OrientationJ (Rezakhaniha et al., 2011) to measure the fluorescence signal of aligned pixels at each orientation (Figure 4A and B) and created an axon directionality “profile” that represents the average signal at each 1.0 degree angle interval across multiple embryos (Figure 4C). In wild-type embryos, most axons are oriented in ventral-posterior and ventral-anterior directions, with a higher proportion of axon signal in posteriorly directed angles, peaking approximately between –50° and –85° (Figure 4A and C, Figure 4—figure supplement 1A). Little signal was detected in the horizontal (0 degrees) orientation. In contrast, klc4uw314 mutants had reduced overall axon signal compared to their wild-type cousins (Figure 4B and C, Figure 4—figure supplement 1B), consistent with the reduced branch density. Mutants showed an overall directional profile similar to wild type (Figure 4B and C). However, because differences in axon orientation between wild type and mutants may be masked by the overall difference in signal, we normalized the data and displayed it as a percentage of total signal at each angle. We found a small but significant increase in axons oriented in the posterior direction in klc4uw314 mutants, specifically those oriented between –20.5° and –50.5° (Figure 4D). There was no difference in the proportion of anterior-directed axons between wild type and klc4uw314 mutants (Figure 4—figure supplement 1C).

Figure 4. Axon orientation profiling reveals increased posterior directionality of peripheral axons in klc4uw314 mutants.

(A–B) A representative region of axon analysis for a 24 hpf wild type (A) and klc4uw314 (B) embryo. The axons are color coded for directionality according to the legend in the upper left corner of (A). (C) Plot of axon directionality in 24 hpf wild type (green) and klc4uw314 mutants (magenta). (D) Axon directionality data was normalized to total signal and posterior angles are displayed to compare directional bias. Area under the curve was measured for shaded area and tested for statistical significance using a t-test. **P=0.0047. For (C–D), error bars = 95% CI. Wild type N=40 embryos, klc4uw314 N=23 embryos.

Figure 4—source data 1. Histogram of the axon signal at each angle for wild type and klc4uw314 mutant axons, used to generate directional distribution plots.
Figure 4—source data 2. Histogram data in Source Data 1 that has been normalized to the total axon signal present in the sample.
Presented as a percentage of total signal.

Figure 4.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1. Additional data for branch directionality profiling.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1.

(A) All individual data points for the 40 wild-type embryos analyzed (dots), with the mean overlayed (dark green line). (B) All individual data points for the 23 klc4uw314 mutant embryos analyzed (dots), with the mean overlayed (magenta line). (C) Axon directionality data normalized to total signal with posterior and anterior data displayed. Error bars = 95% CI.

To investigate what changes in axon behavior could generate the posterior orientation bias in mutants, we turned to live imaging. We analyzed the growth direction of actively extending axons by measuring the orientation angle of the distal-most axon shaft at each time point over 2–4 hr movies (Figure 5A) and displaying the distribution of growth angles as rose plots. We found klc4uw314 mutant axons had a preference for posterior growth paths, while wild-type axons had no anterior-posterior bias (Figure 5B and C). We then measured the angles between newly bifurcating sister branches. Prior to bifurcation, axons commonly grow in a posterior direction in both wild type and mutant embryos (Figure 5D). As growth cones bifurcate in wild-type embryos, the sister branches grow away from each other (Figure 5D’ and E, Figure 5—video 1), resulting in an average branch angle of 96.5 degrees 15 min after branch formation, and 91.7 degrees 75 min after branch formation (Figure 5E and G). This behavior results in axon arbors with an even distribution of axon orientation angles in wild-type embryos (Figure 4C). In klc4uw314 mutants, the newly formed branches did not separate as much, resulting in a shallower average angle of 79.7 degrees between sister branches 15 min after branch formation, and narrowing to 60.2 degrees after 75 min (Figure 5F and G, Figure 5—video 2). In fact, branches in klc4uw314 mutants at times failed to repel at all, instead appearing to collapse onto one another (see Figure 6B). These results suggest that KLC4 may be involved in mediating mutual repulsion between sister axon branches. Moreover, the posterior growth bias detected by automated orientation analysis in klc4uw314 mutant embryos (Figure 4C) is likely the result of branch separation failure. Because axons initially tend toward posterior growth, reduced branch separation or collapse of anteriorly directed branches onto the more posterior axon shaft leads to axon arbors with an overall increase in posterior orientation angles. Thus, our automated, population-scale method of axon orientation profiling led to the discovery of a branch separation defect, demonstrating the utility of the profiling method for revealing alterations in axon arbor pattern.

Figure 5. Live imaging shows that klc4uw314 mutants have a bias toward posterior growth direction and defective sister branch separation.

(A) Illustration of how axon growth direction was measured. The angle between an axon’s position in each frame and the frame preceding it was measured. (B–C) These angles were plotted as rose plots to show growth direction distributions. Wild type axons grow evenly between anterior and posterior directions, while klc4uw314 mutant axons show a preference for posterior directed growth. Wild type N=19 axons from 3 embryos. Klc4uw314 N=22 axons from 4 embryos. (D) Stills from a live capture of wild type axon outgrowth, showing that axons often grow posteriorly, but anterior directed branches form over time (D’, white arrows). (E) As a branch forms in a wild type peripheral axon, the angle between sister branches remains consistent as axons grow apart. (F) In klc4uw314 mutants, sister branches do not grow as far apart and the sister branch angle narrows over time compared to wild type. All scale bars = 20 µm. (G) Quantification of the angle between newly formed sister branches at 15 and 75 min after branch creation. Wild type 15’ N=37 branches, klc4uw314 15’ N=36 branches, Wild type 75’ N=25 branches, klc4uw314 75’ N=18 branches. 4 embryos per genotype. *p=0.021, ***p=0.0003, One-way ANOVA. Error bars = 5-95th percentile.

Figure 5—source data 1. The rose plot data generated with the Chemotaxis Tool for wild type axon tracking data.
Figure 5—source data 2. The rose plot data generated with the Chemotaxis Tool for klc4uw314 mutant axon tracking data.
Figure 5—source data 3. The angle between sister branches, measured at 15 min after branch formation and 75 min after branch formation.
If a branch retracted or the 75-min time point fell out of recorded range, no data was recorded for the 75-min point.

Figure 5.

Figure 5—video 1. Example of a wild type peripheral axon growing out while maintaining a wide branch angle between sister branches.
Download video file (1.9MB, mp4)
Figure 5—video 2. Peripheral axon growth in a klc4uw314 mutant embryo demonstrating both fasciculation between axons from the same neuron (self-fasciculation) and the narrowing of the angle between sister axons over time.
Download video file (2.5MB, mp4)

Figure 6. Loss of KLC4 results in aberrant axon fasciculation.

(A) In wild type embryos, the most common response to self or non-self contact between axons is retraction. Two neighboring axons in a wild type embryo make contact (time 0’) and within 30 min, one of the axons has retracted. (B–C) When axons make contact in klc4uw314 embryos, they often fasciculate instead of retracting. This happens with both self-contacts (B–C, sf) and non-self-contacts (C), nsf. All scale bars = 20 µm. (D) Fasciculations per neuron, separated by self or non-self, as well as all fasciculations combined. Wild type N=35 neurons in 4 embryos. Klc4uw314 N=37 neurons in 5 embryos. Error bars = SEM. (E) Fasciculation length was measured from the last frame of each 2 hr imaging period. Wild type N=3 fasciculations, klc4uw314 N=19 fasciculations. Mean with SEM displayed over individual data points. Because fasciculation events are rare in wild-type embryos and a length measurement is conditional on the event occurring, we could not apply a statistical test to assess the significance of the difference in fasciculation length.

Figure 6—source data 1. Fasciculations per neuron, separated as self or non-self, and combined (all).
Figure 6—source data 2. Fasciculation length measured in microns.

Figure 6.

Figure 6—video 1. A peripheral axon in a klc4uw314 mutant embryo contacts a neighboring peripheral axon and fasciculates with it.
Download video file (1.4MB, mp4)
Figure 6—video 2. Tight fasciculation is revealed in a klc4uw314 mutant peripheral axon as a gap opens between the two axons as an interstitial branch forms.
Download video file (1.2MB, mp4)

Peripheral axons in klc4uw314 mutants exhibit abnormal fasciculation behavior

Live imaging also revealed a broader defect in axon repulsion: normally, in wild type embryos when a peripheral axon growth cone contacts another axon (either a branch of the same neuron or of another neuron) it retracts or changes direction to grow away from the other axon, thus minimizing contact between axons (Sagasti et al., 2005; Liu and Halloran, 2005; Figure 6A). However, in klc4uw314 mutants, axons often fasciculate after such contacts and grow together for substantial distances (Figure 6B and C, Figure 6—video 1). In some cases, axons were fasciculated so tightly that they only became distinguishable as two axons when pulled apart by the force of a developing branch (Figure 6C, arrows, Figure 6—video 1, minutes 41–47, Figure 6—video 2, minutes 24–44). In wild-type embryos, all observed fasciculations (n=3) resolved by retraction or defasciculation, but only 46.5% of fasciculations (n=19) in klc4uw314 mutants were resolved in this manner. Fasciculation behavior was quantified by measuring the frequency and length of fasciculation events occurring during our live imaging period. We found that fasciculation events were rare in wild type embryos but occurred relatively frequently in mutants (Figure 6D). Fasciculations ranged from 7.5 to 60.5 µm in length in klc4uw314 mutants (Figure 6E). Together, these results show that KLC4 is important for the mutual repulsion needed to shape sensory arbors. The mutual repulsion normally exhibited by peripheral axons is in contrast to the fasciculation behavior of the central axons from the same neuron. The failure of klc4uw314 mutant peripheral axons to show repulsion suggests that KLC4 may be involved in the localization of factors controlling central versus peripheral axon behavior.

RB neurons have aberrant apical axons in klc4uw314 mutants

RB neurons in klc4uw314 mutants also showed abnormal morphology suggestive of altered cell polarity. We found that some RB neurons in Tg(–3.1ngn1:gfp-caax);klc4uw314 mutant embryos and Kinesore-treated embryos projected a process from the apical cell body that crossed the midline within the dorsal spinal cord (Figure 7A and B, Figure 7—videos 1 and 2). Small apical protrusions sometimes form on RB neurons, especially in the anterior portion of the spinal cord, though they can be found nearly anywhere along the spinal cord. These processes are typically short, dynamic, and are most common early in development (18–24 hpf; Figure 7—video 3). In wild-type embryos, they rarely cross the midline of the spinal cord. We counted the number of midline crossing apical processes from images of anti-HNK-1-labeled embryos captured in the spinal cord overlying the yolk extension and found that 4% of neurons (n=1,509 neurons in 51 embryos) in klc4uw314 mutants had midline crossing processes compared to 1% in wild type embryos (n=608 neurons in 21 embryos) (Figure 7C). Similar results were obtained following treatment with Kinesore from 18 to 24 hpf, with 7% of Kinesore-treated neurons (n=433 neurons in 17 embryos) extending an apical protrusion that crossed the midline, compared to 2% of neurons in vehicle-treated control (n=436 neurons in 15 embryos) (Figure 7C). To further assess neuronal morphology and the pathways of the aberrant projections, we sparsely labeled individual RB neurons by injecting DNA encoding TagRFP-caax driven by the –3.1ngn1 promoter at the one-cell stage. This mosaic cell labeling showed that the apical processes grew to and fasciculated with the contralateral central axon fascicle in both klc4uw314 mutant and kinesore-treated embryos (Figure 7D and F), suggesting they may respond to cues directing central axon behavior despite emerging from an ectopic apical location. These results suggest that KLCs have a role in determining the location of axon emergence from the cell body.

Figure 7. Midline crossing of apical protrusions occurs more frequently when KLC function is disrupted.

(A–B) Confocal projections (dorsal views) of partial z-stacks containing only the optical sections in the plane of the RB cell bodies within the spinal cord in a wild-type Tg(–3.1ngn1:gfp-caax) embryo (A), or a klc4uw314 Tg(–3.1ngn1:gfp-caax) embryo (B), with an apical protrusion from a neuron crossing the midline and reaching the contralateral side (white arrowhead). Scale bars = 20 µm. (C) The percentage of neurons per embryo that had an apical protrusion crossing the midline of the spinal cord was quantified. **p<0.0052, Mann–Whitney test. Mean with SEM displayed over individual data points. Wild type N=608 neurons from 21 embryos, klc4uw314 N=1496 neurons from 47 embryos, 2% DMSO N=429 neurons from 15 embryos, Kinesore N=595 neurons from 21 embryos. (D–F) Mosaic labeling of individual neurons by injection with –3.1ngn1:gfp-caax DNA into klc4uw314 mutants (D), DMSO-treated embryos I or kinesore-treated embryos (F) showing that apical protrusions (magenta arrowheads) can fasciculate onto the contralateral central axon.

Figure 7—source data 1. The percentage of neurons per embryo that had an apical protrusion that fully crossed the midline of the spinal cord.

Figure 7.

Figure 7—video 1. Light sheet microscopy of an embryo treated with 100µM Kinesore, showing what appear to be apical protrusions crossing the midline of the spinal cord.
Download video file (5.3MB, mp4)
This behavior was seen in both Kinesore treated and klc4uw314 mutant embryos. Anterior is to the right.
Figure 7—video 2. High-speed, high-magnification light sheet microscopy of a klc4uw314 mutant embryo showing an apical protrusion growing across the midline with a dynamic tip that shows growth-cone like behavior.
Download video file (5MB, mp4)
Anterior is to the top right corner.
Figure 7—video 3. A cropped portion of Figure 2—video 1 showing normal behavior of apical protrusions in the spinal cord of a wild-type embryo.
Download video file (2.4MB, mp4)
Video has been rotated to show the spinal cord in a more horizontal position. Anterior to the left.

Endosomal vesicle transport is altered in klc4uw314 mutants

To begin exploring the mechanisms by which KLC4 affects axon arbor development, we imaged endosomal vesicle transport. We previously found that a properly functioning endosomal trafficking system is necessary for RB axon branching (Ponomareva et al., 2014; Ponomareva et al., 2016). Moreover, we showed that the kinesin adaptor Clstn1 is required for branching and that it functions in part by trafficking Rab5-labeled endosomal vesicles to branch points (Ponomareva et al., 2014). We used a previously described zebrafish Rab5c construct (Clark et al., 2011) to label Rab5-containing endosomes in vivo. We expressed GFP-Rab5c in individual RB neurons by injecting ngn1:GFP-Rab5c DNA into embryos at the one-cell stage (Figure 8A). We imaged vesicle movement using a high-speed swept field confocal microscope that allows imaging of rapid events in 3D. We captured z-stacks of RB axon arbors at 2–5 s intervals for 7–10 min periods and used kymography to quantify endosome dynamics (Figure 8A’). We found some alterations in transport. In klc4uw314 mutants, a smaller proportion of vesicles moved in the anterograde direction compared to wild type (Figure 8B), and the maximum anterograde vesicle run length was reduced (Figure 8C), suggesting that some aspects of Rab5 vesicle transport or a subset of Rab5 vesicles rely on KLC4 function. However, there was no significant difference in velocity of the Rab5 vesicles that were moving, suggesting these vesicles can attach to the motor without KLC4 (Figure 8D).

Figure 8. Endosomal trafficking dynamics are disrupted in klc4314 mutants.

(A) Live RB neuron expressing eGFP-Rab5c. Scale bar = 10 μm. (A’) Representative kymograph of axon segment boxed in magenta in (A).(B) Percentage of anterogradely moving vesicles out of all motile Rab5 vesicles in peripheral axons. The proportion of anterogradely moving vesicles is decreased in klc4314 mutants, *p=0.0372 by Mann-Whitney test; error bars = mean with SD. Wild type N=15 embryos, klc4314 N=15 embryos. (C) Average Rab5 vesicle run length and maximum anterograde vesicle run length in peripheral axons. *p=0.0358, ns p=0.5359, Mann-Whitney test. (D) Rab5 vesicle velocity in peripheral axons. NS by Mann-Whitney test (anterograde p=0.6505, retrograde p=0.0792), wild type N=12 embryos, klc4314 N=11 embryos. For all data, one neuron per embryo was analyzed. Error bars in box and whisker plots represent 5-95th percentile.

Figure 8—source data 1. The percentage of motile vesicles per cell that were traveling anterogradely in peripheral RB axon branches.
Figure 8—source data 2. The distance traveled by Rab5 comets, including both average and maximum run length.
Figure 8—source data 3. The measured velocity of Rab5 comets in peripheral axons.

Figure 8.

Figure 8—figure supplement 1. Vesicles labeled with photoactivatable Rab5 accumulate normally at branch points.

Figure 8—figure supplement 1.

(A) Representative image series of PA-GFP-Rab5 photoactivation. A region outlining the cell body is selected and photoactivated using a 405 nm laser (middle panel). Rab5 vesicles can then be visualized moving into the axon and into branch points (lower panel). (B) We quantified the accumulation of Rab5 at branch points by dividing the acquired images into 4 time periods (t1–t4) comprised of 7–8 min intervals and measuring the signal at branch points from maximum intensity projections of each time period (t1–t4). Signal was normalized to the signal at t1. There was no significant difference in the area under the curve between klc4+/+ and klc4uw314 mutants (p=0.1319, t-test, klc4+/+ N = 42 branches from 20 embryos, klc4uw314 N=18 branches from 8 embryos). Error bars = SEM.
Figure 8—video 1. An RB neuron expressing photoactivatable Rab5 (green) and membrane-bound TagRFP (magenta).
Download video file (2.2MB, mp4)
Upon photoactivation, vesicles can be seen traveling into the peripheral axons. The second half of the movie shows the maximum intensity projections of time intervals (T1-4) used to analyze vesicle accumulation at branch points. Each time interval represents a maximum intensity projection of 6.5 minutes of time. T0 represents the frame prior to photoactivation.

To determine whether KLC4 regulates Rab5 vesicular transport from the cell body specifically to axon branch points, we labeled Rab5c with photoactivatable GFP (PA-GFP). We co-injected ngn1:PA-GFP-Rab5c and ngn1:TagRFP-caax to label individual RB neurons. We photoactivated the GFP in the cell body during stages when peripheral axons are actively growing and branching, and captured images at ~1–2 min intervals for 30–40 min after photoactivation (Figure 8—figure supplement 1A). We previously reported that in wild type embryos, PA-GFP-Rab5c vesicles originating in the cell body appeared to accumulate at the peripheral axon initiation point and in secondary branch points within minutes of photoactivation (Ponomareva et al., 2014). To quantify vesicle accumulations at branch points over time, we divided the movies into 4 time periods (t1-t4), made maximum intensity projections of the z-stacks for t1-t4, and measured the fluorescent signal at branch points as a percentage of the signal at the first time period (t1). In both wild type and klc4 mutant neurons, the intensity increased similarly during this period, suggesting KLC4 does not play a significant role in vesicle delivery from the cell body to branch points (Figure 8—figure supplement 1B).

Microtubule dynamics are altered in klc4uw314 mutants

Because local changes in MT dynamics are associated with axon branch formation, and MT invasion of nascent axon branches is required for branches to be stabilized and maintained (Yu et al., 1994; Dent and Kalil, 2001; Yu et al., 2008; Gallo, 2011; Armijo-Weingart and Gallo, 2017), we assessed whether the MT cytoskeleton is affected in klc4uw314 mutants. To visualize MT dynamics in vivo, we used the MT plus-tip binding protein EB3 fused to GFP (Stepanova et al., 2003). EB3-GFP binds to the plus ends of actively polymerizing MTs, which appear as moving GFP puncta or ‘comets’. We expressed EB3-GFP in RB neurons by injecting ngn1:EB3-GFP DNA, and imaged with a swept field confocal microscope. We captured z-stacks at 2–5 s intervals for periods ranging from 7 to 10 min and analyzed microtubule dynamics using kymography (Figure 9A, Figure 9—video 1). In axons, the majority of comets move in the anterograde direction, reflecting the plus-end-distal organization of MTs. We found no change in the percentage of retrograde comets between wild type and klc4uw314 mutants, suggesting KLC4 is not necessary for organization of MT polarity (Figure 9B and C). However, we found a significant increase in the frequency of anterograde comets in peripheral axons, but not central axons, in klc4uw314 mutant RB neurons (Figure 9B and D, Figure 9—figure supplement 1A), suggesting increased MT dynamics in peripheral axons. In addition, we found that the velocity of EB3-GFP comets, both anterograde and retrograde, was increased in peripheral and central axons in klc4uw314 mutants (Figure 9E, Figure 9—figure supplement 1B). There was no difference in the distance traveled by EB3-GFP comets in any condition (Figure 9—figure supplement 1C,D), although duration of anterograde comet runs was reduced in peripheral axons, consistent with their increased velocity (Figure 9—figure supplement 1E,F). Thus, the length of new MT polymer growth at plus ends is not affected in mutants, but mutant MTs polymerize faster, suggesting a role for KLC4 in regulating the growth rate of MT plus ends.

Figure 9. Microtubule dynamics are increased in klc4uw314 mutants.

(A-A”) Live RB neuron expressing EB3-GFP, scale bar = 10 μm. Magenta box in (A) indicates axon segment selected for kymograph in (A’). (A”) EB3 comets identified as anterogradely (green) or retrogradely (magenta) directed based on their angle. (B) Representative kymographs from peripheral axon segments of wildtype (top) or klc4uw314 (bottom). (C) Quantification of the percentage of retrograde comets out of all comets imaged for both peripheral and central axons. NS by Mann-Whitney test. (D) Peripheral axon comet frequency. ***p=0.0003, Mann-Whitney test. (E) Peripheral axon comet velocity. ***p=0.0004, *p=0.042, Mann-Whitney test. For all data, error bars = SEM. Wild type central N=8 segments, klc4uw314 central N=15 segments, Wild type peripheral N=26 segments, klc4uw314 peripheral N=48 segments. Wild type data from 10 neurons in 7 embryos. Klc4uw314 data from 13 neurons in 8 embryos. All comets in a kymograph were measured.

Figure 9—source data 1. Source Data 1: All measurements of EB3-GFP comets in wild type and klc4uw314 mutants, averaged per measured segment.

Figure 9.

Figure 9—figure supplement 1. Additional quantification of microtubule dynamics in wild type and klc4 mutant axons.

Figure 9—figure supplement 1.

(A) Central axon comet frequency. NS by Mann-Whitney test. (anterograde p=0.4664, retrograde p=0.7268) (B) Central axon comet velocity. *p<0.05, Mann-Whitney test. (C) Average distance traveled by peripheral axon comets. NS by Mann-Whitney test (anterograde p=0.1476, retrograde p=0.9288) (D) Average distance traveled by central axon comets. NS by Mann-Whitney test (anterograde p=0.3738, retrograde p=0.8836) (E) Average comet run time for peripheral axon comets. NS by Mann-Whitney test (***p=0.0001, NS p=0.1389) (F) Average comet run time for central axon comets. NS by Mann-Whitney test (anterograde p=0.3363, retrograde p=0.3196) For all data, error bars = SEM. Wild type central N=8 segments, klc4uw314 central N=15 segments, Wild type peripheral N=26 segments, klc4uw314 peripheral N=48 segments. Wild type data from 10 neurons in 7 embryos. Klc4uw314 data from 13 neurons in 8 embryos.
Figure 9—video 1. An RB neuron expressing EB3-GFP to tag plus ends of microtubules.
Download video file (7.1MB, mp4)
Plus end comets can be seen traveling in a predominantly anterograde direction.

MTs in nascent branches are less stable in klc4uw314 mutants

To ask whether MT dynamics are altered in newly forming branches, we co-injected ngn:EB3-GFP DNA with ngn:TagRFP-caax DNA to label cell membranes, and imaged EB3-GFP comets in nascent branches (Figure 10A). We found no difference in the frequency of comets in nascent branches between wild type and mutants (Figure 10B). EB3 comet frequency also did not differ between mutant and wild type when separated by branch type (IB vs GCB, data not shown, p>0.7 by t-test). However, because EB3-GFP comet frequency in peripheral axons overall is increased in klc4 uw314 mutants, the equivalent comet frequencies between mutant and wild type in nascent branches may represent a net reduction in MT polymerization into nascent branches in mutants.

Figure 10. Nascent RB axon branches show reduced microtubule stabilization in klc4uw314 mutants.

Figure 10.

(A) Still image of nascent RB branches expressing EB3-GFP in a live wild type embryo at 23hpf. (A’) Zoom in of the branches outlined in white in (A).(A”) EB3-GFP signal in nascent branches in (A’). (A’’’) Representative kymograph generated from nascent branch outlined in magenta in (A’). (B) Quantification of peripheral axon comet frequency. NS by Mann-Whitney test, p=0.8410. Wild type N=22 nascent branches from 9 embryos, klc4uw314 N=25 nascent branches from 8 embryos. (C-D’’) Representative examples of HNK-1 and acetylated tubulin stained embryos at 24hpf. White arrows in (C’-C’’) indicate nascent branches with tubulin acetylation. Magenta arrows in (D’-D’’) indicate nascent branches lacking tubulin acetylation. (E) Percentage of nascent branches containing acetylated tubulin out of all nascent branches. Fewer nascent branches contained acetylated tubulin in klc4uw314 mutants. ****p<0.0001 by Mann-Whitney test. Wild type N=132 nascent branches from 15 embryos, and klc4uw314 N=143 nascent branches from 16 embryos. Scale bars = 20 μm. Error bars in box and whisker plots represent 5-95th percentile.

Figure 10—source data 1. Source Data 1: The EB3 comet frequency in nascent axons.
Figure 10—source data 2. The percentage of nascent branches with discernable acetylated tubulin signal out of all nascent branches observed.

As another approach to assess MT stability in new branches, we fixed embryos at a stage of active branching (24 hpf) and double-labeled neurons with anti-HNK1 and an antibody to acetylated tubulin, a modification found on stable MTs (Webster and Borisy, 1989; Figure 10C and D). We analyzed terminal branches up to 10 μm in length formed by interstitial branching or bifurcation, then scored those with acetylated tubulin signal. We found a significant reduction in the percentage of nascent branches with acetylated tubulin in klc4uw314 mutants (Figure 10E). This finding suggests that KLC4 is required for MTs to stabilize in newly formed branches.

Larval and adult klc4uw314 fish show behavioral defects

The formation of functional neural circuits ultimately depends on the precise orchestration of cell biological processes during neural development. To ask whether KLC4 loss affects circuit function, we analyzed larval escape behavior in response to touch, a behavior mediated by sensory neurons. Larva at 3 days post-fertilization (dpf) were given a light tail touch, evoking a swim response (Figure 11A). The time of the swim response and number of swim bouts completed was measured for each larva. Klc4uw314 mutant larva swam for a longer total time (wild type median = 0.65 s, klc4uw314 1.49 s) and performed more swim bouts (wild type median = 1 bout, klc4uw314 mutants = 2 bouts) after touch stimulus than wild type larva (Figure 11B and C), suggesting KLC4 loss leads to hypersensitivity to touch.

Figure 11. Larval and adult klc4uw314 mutants have behavioral defects (A) Example of touch assay administration.

A 3 dpf larva is touched on the tail using a gel loading tip, provoking a swim response. (B) Larval response time was measured starting from beginning of swim to cessation of motion. (C) The number of swim bouts performed by each larva during total post-touch swim time was counted. For B-C, each point represents an individual larva. ***p=0.0005, Mann-Whitney test. Error bars = SEM. Wild type N=45, klc4uw314 N=41. 55 total larva were tested for each genotype, but larva that did not respond were not included in these data. (D) Schematic of the novel tank test design, with upper and lower zones demarcated by the center dotted line. (E) Percentage of fish per experiment that entered the upper zone of the tank during a 3-min exposure to a novel tank. Error bars = SEM. Each point represents one experiment day: wild type N=3 experiments with 30 total fish, klc4uw314 N=3 experiments with 39 total fish. (F) The time it took for individual fish to first cross into the upper zone during a 3-min exposure to a novel tank. Points represent individual fish, error bars = SEM. Wild type N=24, klc4uw314 N=8. Fish that did not cross into the upper zone are not represented in this data. NS by t-test, p=0.7766. (G) The total time fish spent in the upper zone of the novel tank during a 3-min exposure. Points represent individual fish, ***p=0.0002 by t-test, error bars = SEM. Wild type N=30, klc4uw314 N=39. (H) Percentage of fish to freeze per experiment during a 3-min exposure to the novel tank. Error bars = SEM. Each point represents one experiment day: wild type N=3 experiments with 30 total fish, klc4uw314 N=3 experiments with 39 total fish. (I) The total time fish spent frozen during a 3-min exposure to a novel tank. Points represent individual fish, ****p<0.0001 by Mann-Whitney test, error bars = SEM. Wild type N=30, klc4uw314 N=39.

Figure 11—source data 1. The length of time each individual larva spent swimming after a touch stimulus (non-responding fish removed from graph).
Figure 11—source data 2. The number of swim bouts performed by each individual larva after a touch stimulus (non-responding fish removed from graph).
Figure 11—source data 3. The average percentage of fish that entered the upper zone of the tank per experimental trial.
Figure 11—source data 4. The time it took an individual fish to first cross into the upper zone of the tank.
Only individuals that crossed into the upper zone are represented in this dataset.
Figure 11—source data 5. The total time spent by each individual fish in the upper zone throughout the experimental time period.
Figure 11—source data 6. The average percentage of fish that performed one or more freezing bouts per experimental trial.
Figure 11—source data 7. The total time spent frozen by each individual fish throughout the experimental time period.

Figure 11.

Figure 11—figure supplement 1. Loss of klc4 results in reduced size and ineffective feeding in adult zebrafish.

Figure 11—figure supplement 1.

(A) Age-matched adult wild type and klc4uw314 mutant fish. Ruler tick marks represent 1 mm. (B) Body length measurements for age matched adult fish at 7 weeks and 9 weeks old. ****p<0.0001, **p=0.0024, *p=0.013, t-test. N is given in figure as individual fish. (C) Percentage of larvae consumed over an hour. **p=0.0090, t-test. Wild type N=31, klc4uw314 N=30.

The cell biological defects found during development in klc4uw314 mutants seemed likely to lead to deficits in adult fish as well. Indeed, we found that klc4uw314 mutants are adult viable and fertile but are smaller in size than their wild-type siblings (Figure 11—figure supplement 1A). To measure body size in a controlled manner, we in-crossed F4 homozygous klc4uw314 mutants from both AB and mixed AB/TL backgrounds and separately in-crossed their wild-type (AB) siblings. The F5 embryos were born on the same day, raised side-by-side in an identical environment, and fed the same diet. We measured the body length of these fish at 7 weeks and at 9 weeks (Figure 11—figure supplement 1B). klc4uw314 mutants were on average 83.3% the size of their wild-type cousins at 7 weeks. This difference remained at 9 weeks, despite both the wild type and klc4uw314 mutant fish increasing in size, with klc4uw314 mutant fish measured at 84.6% the length of their wild-type cousins. Smaller body size has also been reported in klc1 mutant mice (Rahman et al., 1999), supporting the idea that KLCs are important for post-embryonic growth of the animal.

Multiple neural systems are affected in the human disease caused by KLC4 mutation; patients show defects in vision, hearing, movement and cognition (Bayrakli et al., 2015). Thus, we analyzed adult behavior to more broadly investigate KLC4’s roles in neural circuit function. To ask if the mutant’s small size reflected a defect in feeding behavior, we assayed feeding ability (Howe et al., 2018). Adult zebrafish were isolated in individual 1 L tanks, then 50 larval zebrafish were added to each tank. The fish were allowed to feed on the larvae for one hour, then removed from the tank to allow counting of the remaining larvae. Wild type fish were effective feeders, eating on average 90% of available zebrafish larvae (Figure 11—figure supplement 1C). In comparison, klc4uw314 mutants ate on average only 60% of the larvae available. We noted that the mutant fish that performed the worst in the feeding assay underwent bouts of freezing behavior during the assay. Upon introduction of the larvae to the tank, the fish ceased movement for extended periods.

Freezing behavior in zebrafish in response to a novel environment has been previously described and interpreted as increased anxiety (Wong et al., 2010; Maximino et al., 2010; Ziv et al., 2013; Khan et al., 2017). To test whether klc4uw314 mutant fish show anxiety-like behavior, we used a novel tank diving test, which is similar in principle to the open field test used in rodents (Wong et al., 2010; Maximino et al., 2010). In this test, fish are introduced to an unfamiliar tank and allowed to explore. Zebrafish typically prefer to stay in the lower zone of the tank initially, and once they become acclimated, they begin to explore the upper regions of the tank (Figure 11D). We introduced adult animals (7 months old) to a novel tank and filmed their behavior for 3 min. Analysis revealed that 90.9% of wild type fish explored the upper zone of the tank during the 3-min window, compared to only 18.8% of the klc4uw314 mutant fish (Figure 11E). Of the animals that did explore the upper zone, both wild type and klc4uw314 mutant animals took an average of 100 s before their first cross into the upper zone (Figure 11F). Wild type animals spent an average of 14 s in the upper region of the tank, compared to just 3.26 s for klc4uw314 mutants (Figure 11G). Notably, freezing behavior, defined for this experiment as cessation of all movement except for gills for at least 3 s, was observed in an average of 43% of mutant fish, and only 3% of wild type fish (Figure 11H). 15.4% of klc4uw314 mutant fish, but no wild type animals, froze for the full 3-min duration of the experiment (Figure 11I).

Studies in both zebrafish and rodents have shown that repeated exposure to the unfamiliar environment leads to habituation of the anxiety behavior. In zebrafish, habituation manifests as increased exploration of the upper zone and reduced freezing behavior. Fish that do not habituate to the novel environment or that worsen over multiple exposures are thought to demonstrate a depression-like phenotype (Ziv et al., 2013). Behaviors driven by increased anxiety are expected to lessen with repeat exposures. We tested the ability of klc4uw314 mutants to habituate to a novel environment after multiple exposures. In addition, because KLC4 and other kinesin-1 subunits have been implicated in neurodegenerative diseases, we sought to determine whether the anxiety-like behavior worsened with age. We challenged 1.25–1.5 year old klc4uw314 mutants and their wild type cousins by repeated exposure to a novel tank for 10 min, twice a week for 3 weeks in succession. The first exposure allowed us to test the effects of age. We found that increased age improved freezing response of klc4uw314 mutants, with only 29.2% (8 out of a total of 23) of individuals tested freezing for more than 3 s during first 3 min of exposure to the novel tank (Figure 12A). Aged wild-type animals worsened in performance, with 13.3% (3 out of 23) undergoing a bout of freezing behavior during first exposure. These numbers remained similar when the entire 10 min was analyzed (wt = 16.7%, mut = 29.2%, Figure 12B, Figure 12—figure supplement 1A). Mutant fish spent a longer period of time frozen than wild-type fish and underwent more total bouts of freezing per fish (Figure 12C, Figure 12—figure supplement 1B). Upon subsequent exposures to the same tank, klc4uw314 mutant freezing behavior worsened (Figure 12B, Figure 12—figure supplement 1A, B), with 50% of klc4uw314 mutants freezing by the sixth exposure, indicating that mutants fail to habituate freezing behavior. Mutants consistently froze for longer periods of time than wild type animals throughout all exposures (Figure 12B and C). Wild type animals performed approximately equally through subsequent exposures, undergoing less frequent freezing bouts and spending less time frozen than mutants (Figure 12B and C, Figure 12—figure supplement 1A, B). Wild-type fish also swam faster than klc4uw314 mutants and showed similar swim velocities when their total swim velocity (including pauses) was compared to their mobile swim velocity (with pauses in swimming subtracted) (Figure 12D). klc4uw314 swim mobile swim velocity was faster than their total swim velocity, reflecting that they spent a greater amount of time immobile (Figure 12D).

Figure 12. Adult klc4uw314 mutant anxiety-like behavior does not fully habituate after repeat exposure to a novel environment.

(A) Percentage of fish per experiment that froze during the first 3 min of a 10-min initial exposure to a novel tank. (B) Plot of the percentage of fish that froze during the course of six 10-min exposures to the same novel tank. (C) Plot of the average time spent frozen during the course of six 10-min exposures to the same novel tank. (D) Plot of the average total and mobile-only swim velocities of fish during each of six 10-min exposures to the same novel tank. Mobile-only velocity excludes periods of time the fish spent at a velocity of 0 cm/s (frozen). (E) Plot of the percentage of time that fish spent in the upper zone of the tank during the course of six 10-min exposures to the same novel tank. (E) Plot of the average time it took for a fish to first enter the upper zone of the tank during the course of six 10-min exposures to the same novel tank. For all data, N=4 groups per genotype, 23 total fish per genotype. Error bars = SEM. For figures D-F, videos that could not be automatically tracked were omitted (for WT, 6/138, for klc4uw314, 4/138).

Figure 12—source data 1. The percentage of fish per group that froze during the first 3 min of exposure 1 of the habituation series.
Figure 12—source data 2. The percentage of fish per group that froze for each exposure in the habituation series.
Figure 12—source data 3. The total time spent frozen by each individual fish throughout the experimental time period for each exposure in the habituation series.
Figure 12—source data 4. The mobile and total swim velocities for each individual fish throughout the experimental time period for each exposure in the habituation series.
Figure 12—source data 5. The percentage of fish per group that entered the upper zone of the tank at least once during each exposure in the habituation series.
Figure 12—source data 6. The time in seconds it took each individual fish to initially cross over into the upper zone of the tank for each exposure in the habituation series.

Figure 12.

Figure 12—figure supplement 1. Additional data for novel tank habituation experiments.

Figure 12—figure supplement 1.

(A) The average number of freezing bouts per fish over the course of 6 exposures. (B) The number of fish that froze out of the total number of fish in the experiment. * indicates data for 3 fish lost from one exposure due to video equipment malfunction. These data could not be generated again since fish had already been exposed to the tank, so the group N for this exposure is 20 instead of 23.

Interestingly, both wild type and klc4uw314 mutants did show habituation as measured by the percentage of time spent in the upper zone of the tank (Figure 12E). During the first exposure, mutant animals only spent 8.7% of the ten minute experiment window in the upper zone and took on average 170.2 s before exploring the upper zone. Wild-type animals explored the upper zone after an average of 80.0 s and spent 17.6% of the first exposure period in the upper zone (Figure 12E and F). At the time of the sixth exposure, however, both wild type and klc4uw314 mutants increased their time spent in the upper zone, and mutants entered the upper zone faster than in their first exposure (Figure 12E and F). Wild type animals took on average 79.7 s to explore the upper zone and spent an average of 25.4% of the experiment period in the upper zone. klc4uw314 mutants performed similarly, spending 23.1% of the experiment period in the upper zone, but taking on average 129.0 s to explore the upper zone (Figure 12E and F). Thus, the failure of klc4uw314 mutants to habituate freezing behavior is specific rather than reflecting a general deficit in habituation.

Discussion

The association of KLC4 with human disease demonstrates its critical importance, yet its cellular and developmental functions are poorly understood. In this study, we used automated analysis of sensory axon arbors and live, in vivo imaging of axon behavior and intracellular MT dynamics to define new roles for KLC4 in shaping neuronal morphology. We found that KLC4 is required for sensory axon branching and for repulsion between peripheral axons, behaviors which are important for proper innervation and tiling of the sensory field. Klc4 mutant neurons do not have a general growth defect, in that their axons grow faster than wild type axons. In addition, klc4 mutants are adult viable and show no signs of early neurodegeneration. These results suggest that KLC4 has a specific function in patterning developing neurons rather than simply being required for general neuronal health. Our finding that the mutant larvae are hypersensitive to touch stimuli suggests that the altered sensory arborization pattern affects sensory circuits. At least two axon defects could contribute to the increased response. Fasciculation of peripheral axons from neighboring neurons might lead to an increase in the number of neurons activated by a localized touch stimulus. Also, the aberrant midline crossing of axons in mutants could cause inappropriate activation of contralateral neurons. Behavioral analysis also showed that klc4 mutant adults exhibit increased anxiety-like behavior, indicating roles for KLC4 in adult circuit function and identifying KLC4 as a novel gene associated with anxiety behavior. This work sheds light on potential mechanisms of human HSPs and provides a new animal model uniquely suited to further investigation of the cell biological and circuit mechanisms underlying disease.

Precise regulation of axon branching is critical for neurons to develop diverse and complex architecture. We identified KLC4 as a novel regulator of axon branching and show that it is required for stabilization of nascent branches. Axon branching requires extensive cytoskeletal rearrangements (Kalil and Dent, 2014; Gallo, 2011; Winkle et al., 2016). Branches begin as actin-based filopodial protrusions, which must be invaded by MTs to mature into branches. MT severing by proteins such as katanin and spastin is required to facilitate invasion (Qiang et al., 2010; Yu et al., 1994; Yu et al., 2008) and branch points are hot spots of increased MT dynamics (Dent and Kalil, 2001). Local unbundling of MTs in the axon shaft occurs at sites of interstitial branch formation (Kalil and Dent, 2014; Ketschek et al., 2015). Kinesin-1 can bind MTs via the KHC C-terminal tail and crosslink or bundle MTs (Seeger and Rice, 2010; Navone et al., 1992). Binding of KLC to KHC releases the autoinhibition of KHC and can tip the balance between kinesin-1’s cargo transport activities and the ability of the KHC tail region to bind MTs (Wong and Rice, 2010; Cai et al., 2007; Chiba et al., 2021). Our finding that MT dynamics are affected in klc4 mutants is consistent with a model in which KLC4 promotes the localized MT dynamics or severing that allows MTs to invade new branches, either by transporting/localizing MT regulators or by facilitating MT unbundling. The increase in MT velocity could suggest an increase in MT sliding or altered regulation or localization of polymerization-promoting MAPs such as CRMP2 (Nakamura et al., 2020). CRMP2 has been reported to influence MT polymerization and stabilization and has been found in complex with KLC4, supporting its potential role in KLC4-mediated effects on MTs (Liz et al., 2014; Koh et al., 2021). The model of KLC4 as a mediator of microtubule dynamics is also supported by the similarities in phenotype between klc4 mutants and kinesore treatment. Kinesore blocks binding between KLCs and cargo adaptors, and also releases KLC autoinhibition and drives KHC toward increased MT bundling function (Randall et al., 2017). MTs that invade nascent branches in klc4 mutants have less acetylation, suggesting KLC4 may influence post-translational modifications of MTs and thereby affect stabilization of new branches.

In addition to cytoskeletal remodeling, regulated organelle/cargo transport and membrane remodeling are important for axon branching (Winkle et al., 2016), although little is known about how individual regulators of trafficking contribute to branching patterns. We previously found that a properly functioning endosomal trafficking system is necessary for RB axon branching (Ponomareva et al., 2014; Ponomareva et al., 2016). Moreover, we showed that the kinesin adaptor Clstn1 is required for branching of peripheral RB axons and that it functions in part by trafficking endosomal vesicles to branch points (Ponomareva et al., 2014). KLC4 loss has a relatively minor effect on Rab5 endosome transport, which is not likely to fully explain the branch stabilization phenotype. KLC4 could potentially influence transport or localization of other necessary organelle cargos, such as mitochondria, which must be localized to branch points to support branch growth (Courchet et al., 2013; Spillane et al., 2013; Tao et al., 2014; Wong et al., 2017), or organelles that support mitochondria, such as ER, which is a critical regulator of mitochondrial function (Csordás et al., 2018; Rowland and Voeltz, 2012; Krols et al., 2016). An important future research goal is to determine whether KLC4 localizes specific organelles to branch points.

To maximize coverage of an innervation field, the axonal or dendritic arbors of neuronal populations that share a similar function must repel one another on contact to ‘tile’ the field (Grueber and Sagasti, 2010; Spead and Poulain, 2020). Our results show that KLC4 is an important player in repulsion between sensory axons, and to our knowledge KLC4 is the first motor protein shown to be involved in tiling. Several cell surface or secreted proteins that meditate dendritic tiling have been identified, including protocadherins (Kostadinov and Sanes, 2015; Lefebvre et al., 2012), DSCAM (Matthews et al., 2007; Soba et al., 2007), netrin (Smith et al., 2012), and slit/robo signaling (Gibson et al., 2014). Less is known about the mechanisms of axon tiling, although similar molecules appear to be involved. For example, in Drosophila, DSCAM mediates sister branch separation in mushroom body axons (Wang et al., 2002), and Dscam2 mediates repulsion between axons in the visual system (Millard et al., 2007). An atypical cadherin, Flamingo, is required for spacing of photoreceptor axon arbors (Senti et al., 2003), and functions together with the transmembrane protein Golden goal (Tomasi et al., 2008; Hakeda-Suzuki et al., 2011). More recent studies have shown that the protocadherin PcdhaC2 mediates repulsion between axons of serotonergic neurons in the mammalian brain (Katori et al., 2017; Chen et al., 2017). Thus, the field has seen multiple advances in knowledge of the membrane proteins that mediate repulsion. However, the mechanisms that regulate their localization to the correct cellular compartments are unknown. Our work can drive new exploration into such mechanisms. For example, it will be interesting to determine whether KLC4 mediates transport of contact repulsion molecules and their specific targeting to peripheral versus central sensory axons.

The formation of polarized neuronal structure is a critical component of proper neural circuit development. An important early step in neuronal polarity development is determination of the location of axon emergence from the cell body. The ectopic apical axons we see in klc4 mutants suggest that KLC4 also has roles in the polarity of axon initiation location. RB neuron axons normally emerge from basal edge of cell body (Andersen et al., 2011), and their emergence correlates with centrosome movement to the basal region of the cell (Andersen and Halloran, 2012). We previously found that laminin-1 mutants also have ectopic apical axons in RB neurons and these ectopic axons correlate with apically displaced centrosomes (Andersen and Halloran, 2012). Laminin has been shown to influence the location of axon emergence from the cell body in zebrafish RGCs (Randlett et al., 2011), and can direct the positioning of the centrosome in neurons in vitro and in vivo (Randlett et al., 2011; Gupta et al., 2010). KLC4 may play a role in coordinating the response to extracellular signals such as laminin that help define the site of axon formation.

Overall, the developmental phenotypes seen in klc4uw314 mutants are striking in that they appear to reflect problems with specific patterning of neuronal structure rather than problems in bulk cargo transport. Neurons appear healthy in the mutants, show no signs of axon degeneration during development, and in fact axons grow faster in mutants than in wild type embryos. Moreover, the klc4uw314 mutants are adult viable. Interestingly, klc1 knockout mice are also adult viable and show specific defects in transport of cannabinoid receptors in developing axons (Saez et al., 2020). Thus our findings may reflect a general theme, that the function of the KLC subunits of the kinesin-1 motor are to provide specificity to the process of localizing cargos to particular cell regions. Our results also give insight into mechanisms of the human disease caused by KLC4 mutation, which may be caused by defects in developmental patterning. It will be important to fully define the unique cellular functions of individual KLCs in order to better understand mechanisms of disease.

Adult circuit function can be affected by both defects in circuit development and/or later defects in the function of neurons within circuits. Our finding that klc4uw314 mutant adults show freezing and reduced exploration in the novel tank assay implicates KLC4 in the circuits that regulate stress response and that are affected in anxiety and depression disorders. The hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis is an important system that mediates stress responses, and zebrafish mutants with disruptions to the HPA also exhibit anxiety behavior in the novel tank test. Mutation of zebrafish glucocorticoid receptor, a target of the stress hormone cortisol, results in increased freezing and decreased exploratory behavior. Like klc4uw314 mutants, glucocorticoid receptor mutants fail to habituate freezing behavior after repeated exposures to the novel tank (Ziv et al., 2013). However, unlike klc4uw314 mutants, glucocorticoid receptor mutants do not habituate exploratory behavior over multiple exposures. This suggests that freezing behavior and exploratory behavior are controlled by different mechanisms. Interestingly, zebrafish with mutation in a chemokine gene expressed in multiple brain regions, sam2, freeze during the novel tank test, but show normal tank exploration (Choi et al., 2018), providing additional evidence that these two behaviors can be uncoupled. The sam2 mutants have disruptions in corticotropin releasing hormone neurotransmission, linking the mutation to HPA disruption, but not excluding contribution from other circuits. Our klc4uw314 mutants provide another tool to further investigate the circuit changes underlying anxiety disorders.

Whether developmental defects in RB neurons contribute to adult circuit function is unknown. RB neurons are traditionally thought to be a transient cell population that undergoes programmed cell death during larval stages. However recent evidence has shown that 40% of RBs are maintained for at least the first 2 weeks of development, suggesting they may be retained (Williams and Ribera, 2020). Moreover, RB neurons have been shown to play significant roles in regulating larval behaviors (Knafo et al., 2017). Hypersensitivity to touch is a common trait of autism spectrum disorder (ASD), although the relationship of somatosensory neurons to states like anxiety, depression, and hyperstimulation is not well defined. Interestingly, deletion of the ASD risk genes Mecp2 and Gabrb3 in mouse primary somatosensory neurons during development led to a reduction in exploratory behavior in the open-field test, while deletion of these genes only during adulthood caused no such impairment or anxiety behavior (Orefice et al., 2016). In addition to sensory neurons, klc4 is expressed in many cell types in the developing nervous system and thus likely has important roles in development and function of other neurons, which may include neurons that are directly involved in circuits mediating adult behavior. A future research goal is to test the functions of KLC4 in neurons of stress response circuits, such as the HPA axis.

Materials and methods

Key resources table.

Reagent type (species) or resource Designation Source or reference Identifiers Additional information
Gene (Danio rerio) klc4 Ensembl ENSDARG00000086985
Strain, strain background (Danio rerio) AB ZIRC ZL1 Wildtype Strain of Zebrafish
Strain, strain background (Danio rerio) TL ZIRC ZL86 Wildtype Strain of Zebrafish
Genetic reagent (Danio rerio) Tg(–3.1ngn1:gfp-caax) Andersen et al., 2011 Tg(ngn:gfp-caax) Transgenic line labelling sensory axons, AB/TL background
Recombinant DNA reagent 3.1ngn1:trfp-caax Andersen et al., 2011 Tg(ngn:trfp-caax) Plasmid for labelling sensory axons
Recombinant DNA reagent 3.1ngn1-mcherry-caax Andersen et al., 2011 Tg(ngn:mcherry-caax) Plasmid for labelling sensory axons
Recombinant DNA reagent 3.1ngn1:eb3-caax Stepanova et al., 2003; Ponomareva et al., 2014 Tg(ngn:eb3-gfp) Plasmid for labelling microtubule plus ends
Recombinant DNA reagent 3.1ngn1:GFP-rab5c Clark et al., 2011; Ponomareva et al., 2014 Tg(ngn:GFP-rab5c) Plasmid for labelling Rab5c vesicles
Recombinant DNA reagent 3.1ngn1:PA-gfp-Rab5c Ponomareva et al., 2014 Tg(ngn:PA-gfp-Rab5c) Plasmid for labelling Rab5c vesicles
Sequence-based reagent klc4 in situ probe This paper Sequences listed in methods
Sequence-based reagent klc4 long isoform specific in situ probe This paper Sequences listed in methods
Sequence-based reagent short isoform RT primers This paper Sequences listed in methods
Sequence-based reagent long isoform RT primers This paper Sequences listed in methods
Sequence-based reagent klc4 gRNA This paper Sequence listed in methods
Peptide, recombinant protein Cas9 protein with NLS PNA Bio NC0789474
Chemical compound, drug Trizol Thermo-Fisher 15596026 For RNA extraction
Chemical compound, drug Kinesore Chembridge Hit2Lead library #6233307 Kinesin1 targeting small molecule
Commercial assay or kit Superscript III Invitrogen 18080093
Commercial assay or kit DIG RNA labelling kit Roche 11175025910
Chemical compound, drug Oligo dT Invitrogen 18418020
Chemical compound, drug Random Hexamers Invitrogen N8080127
Antibody anti-HNK1 (mouse monoclonal) ZIRC ZN-12 IF(1:250)
Antibody anti-acetylated tubulin (Mouse monoclonal) Sigma T6793 IF(1:1000)
Antibody anti-mouse IgG AlexaFluor 488 (goat polyclonal) Invitrogen A-21121 IF(1:1000)
Antibody Goat anti-mouse IgG2b AlexaFluor 647 (goat polyclonal) Invitrogen A-21141 IF(1:1000)
Software, algorithm CRISPRscan Moreno-Mateos et al., 2015 https://www.crisprscan.org/
Software, algorithm CRISP-ID Dehairs et al., 2016 http://crispid.gbiomed.kuleuven.be/
Software, algorithm ToxTrac Rodriguez et al., 2018 Animal Tracking Freeware
Software, algorithm OrientationJ Rezakhaniha et al., 2011 Used to quantify orientation of axons
Software, algorithm Graphpad Prism Graphpad Graph making software
Software, algorithm Chemotaxis Tool Ibidi https://ibidi.com/chemotaxis-analysis/171-chemotaxis-and-migration-tool.html

Animals

Adult zebrafish (Danio rerio) were kept in a 14/10 hr light/dark cycle. Embryos were maintained at 28.5 °C and staged as hours postfertilization (hpf) as described previously (Kimmel et al., 1995). Wild type AB strain or transgenic Tg(−3.1ngn1:GFP-CAAX) (Andersen et al., 2011) (AB/TL background) embryos were used for all experiments. In experiments using the Tg(−3.1ngn1:GFP-CAAX) line as a background, wild type is used to refer to Tg(−3.1ngn1:GFP-CAAX), klc4+/+ animals. All animals in these studies were handled in accordance with the National Institutes of Health Guide for the care and use of laboratory animals, and the University of Wisconsin Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC). These studies were approved by the University of Wisconsin IACUC (protocols L005692 and L005704).

CRISPR/Cas9 mutagenesis

A guide RNA targeting zebrafish klc4 (5’-GAGCGTGGCACAGCTGGAGGAGG-3’) was designed by using CRISPRScan (crisprscan.org) and synthesized as previously described (Shah et al., 2015; Moreno-Mateos et al., 2015). To generate klc4uw314 mutants, embryos at the very early one-cell stage were injected with 374.5 ng/µl of the guide RNA targeted to zebrafish klc4 complexed with 800 ng/µl of Cas9 protein with NLS (PNA Bio). Injection volume was approximately 1 nL. F0 adult fish were pair-mated to determine individuals carrying germ line mutations. A subset of individuals with germ line mutations were outcrossed to wild type AB fish. F1 fish were raised and genotyped by PCR and sequencing, using CRISP-ID (http://crispid.gbiomed.kuleuven.be/) to assist in sequence unmixing (Dehairs et al., 2016). The klc4uw314 allele was found to have a 25 bp deletion. Fish heterozygous for klc4uw314 were outcrossed to wild type AB fish until the F3 generation. F3 klc4uw314 heterozygotes were in-crossed to generate F4 klc4uw314 homozygotes. All homozygous mutants in this study were F5-F6 generation mutants from homozygous parents, unless otherwise specified. Adults and embryos described as “wild type cousins” of klc4uw314 mutants were generated by crossing F4 klc4+/+ siblings from the F3 klc4uw314/+ in-cross.

RT-PCR

RNA was extracted from zebrafish embryos at specified ages with Trizol (Thermo-Fisher), followed by chloroform extraction and isopropanol precipitation. cDNA was generated from embryo-derived RNA using the Superscript III reverse transcriptase kit and a 50:50 mixture of oligo dT and random hexamer primers (Invitrogen). We designed PCR primers to amplify specific klc4 isoforms. A reverse primer unique to the short isoform’s 3’ UTR was designed for the klc4 short isoform, with the forward primer in exon 2 (Fwd: 5’-GCGCAAATCAGTGGAGATGATAGAGC-3’, Rev: 5’-GCATAGTGGCGCGGTAGGTA-3’). A set of primers for exons exclusive to the long isoform of klc4 (spanning exons 7 through 12) was designed for the long isoform (Fwd: 5’- CAGAGAGAAGGTGCTGGG –3’, Rev: 5’- GGTGGTGTTCACTGTTGG-3’). PCR was then run to test for presence of the isoforms in cDNA from each embryonic age.

In situ hybridization

A digoxigenin-labeled riboprobe complementary to a region spanning exons 2–3 of the klc4 mRNA, which are contained in both long and short isoform mRNAs, was synthesized by in vitro transcription using T7 RNA polymerase with a DIG RNA labeling kit (Roche). The template for transcription was made using PCR on zebrafish cDNA with the following primers: Fwd: 5’-GCGCAAATCAGTGGAGATGATAGAGC-3’, Rev: 5’-TAATACGACTCACTATAGCTCCAGATGTTTCTTCTCCTCC-3’. A second probe recognizing a region in exons 8–11, found only in the long isoform, was synthesized using PCR on zebrafish cDNA with the following primers: Fwd: 5’-CAG AGA GAA GGT GCT GGG-3’, Rev: 5’- TAATACGACTCACTATAG GGTGGTGTTCACTGTTGG-3’. Whole-mount in situ hybridization was performed as previously described (Halloran et al., 1999).

Immunohistochemistry

Embryos were fixed overnight at room temperature in 4% paraformaldehyde in 0.1 M PO4 buffer, then rinsed with PBS/0.1% Tween-20 (PBST) and blocked with incubation buffer (IB: 2 mg/mL BSA, 1% Sheep Serum, 1% DMSO, 0.5% Triton X-100 in PBS) for at least 1 hr. Embryos were incubated overnight at 4 °C with monoclonal anti-HNK1 antibody in IB (ZN-12, 1:250; Zebrafish International Resource Center), rinsed in PBST, then incubated with anti-mouse AlexaFluor 488 secondary antibody (1:1000; Invitrogen) for at least 2 hr at room temperature or overnight at 4 °C. For double labeling with anti-acetylated tubulin (T6793, 1:1000, Sigma), the IgG subtype-specific secondary antibodies anti-mouse IgG1 AlexaFluor 488 and anti-mouse IgG2b AlexaFluor 647 (1:1000, Invitrogen) were used.

Microinjection and Mosaic Expression

DNA expression constructs were made using the Multisite Gateway Cloning System (Invitrogen) into Tol2 vectors (Kwan et al., 2007). Zebrafish embryos at the one-cell stage were injected with ~10 pg –3.1ngn1:EB3-GFP DNA (Lee et al., 2017) to visualize microtubule dynamics. For experiments to image individual neuron morphology, embryos were injected with ~12 pg 3.1ngn1:mCherry-CAAX DNA (Andersen et al., 2011).

Kinesore treatment

Kinesore was acquired from the Hit-2-Lead library (Chembridge, compound #6233307) and suspended in DMSO for a stock concentration of 100 mM. Embryos treated with Kinesore were dechorionated at 17 hpf and placed in E3 containing 150 µM Kinesore and 2% DMSO. Controls were treated with 2% DMSO in E3 for the same time period.

Live Imaging

Light sheet imaging was performed on a custom multi-view light sheet microscope as described previously (Daetwyler et al., 2019). The detection objective was either an Olympus UMPLFLN 10 x W (NA 0.3) or a Nikon CFI75 Apochromat 25xC W (NA 1.1) with a 400 mm tube lens (TL400-A) for 50 x total magnification. Briefly, the Tg(3.1ngn1:GFP-CAAX) wild type or mutant embryos were mounted in an FEP tube (0.8 mm inner diameter, 1.2 outer diameter, Bola) suspended in 0.02% tricaine in E3 with an agarose plug to prevent leakage.

For live confocal imaging, embryos were anesthetized in 0.02% tricaine and mounted in 1% low melting agarose in 10 mM HEPES E3 medium as described previously (Andersen et al., 2011). Axon outgrowth was imaged in Tg(3.1ngn1:GFP-CAAX) embryos with either an Olympus FV1000 laser-scanning confocal microscope using a UplanSApo 20 x Oil objective (NA 0.85) or an Opterra Swept-Field confocal (Bruker Fluorescence Microscopy) with a Nikon S Fluor 40 x Oil immersion objective (NA 1.30). Imaging was started between 18 and 19 hpf, when peripheral RB axons are extending out of the spinal cord. Optical sections (1–1.25 µm) covering a 30–36 µm Z depth were captured every 60–90 s for 2–4 hr.

High speed imaging of EB3-GFP comets was performed with the Opterra Swept-Field confocal microscope equipped with a Nikon Plan Apo VC 100 x oil immersion objective (NA 1.40). Embryos were imaged at stages between 18 and 26 hpf, during peripheral axon outgrowth. EB3-GFP comets in nascent branches were imaged at stages between 22 and 25 hpf with a Nikon Plan Apo 60 x oil immersion objective (NA 1.40). Z-stacks of 5–30 1 μm optical sections were captured at 2–5 s time intervals, for total durations between 7 and 10 min. High speed imaging of Rab5 vesicle dynamics was done with the Opterra Swept-Field confocal and the Nikon Plan Apo 100 x oil objective (NA 1.40). Embryos were imaged between 22 and 26 hpf. Z-stacks of 5–30 μm optical sections were captured at 2–5 s time intervals for a total duration of 7–10 min. Photoactivation and imaging of PA-GFP-Rab5 were performed on the Olympus FV1000 confocal using a 60 X UplanSApo oil immersion objective (NA 1.35). A region of interest encompassing the cell body was activated with the 405 nm laser during the scan of one Z-plane in the cell body. Z-stacks of 1 µm optical sections were captured every 30–170 s for durations ranging from 26 to 34 minutes (average 29.22 minutes, median 28.84 minutes).

Axon outgrowth analysis

To measure the speed of axon outgrowth, the area immediately behind the growth cone was measured using the Manual Tracking plugin in FIJI (Fabrice Cordelières). The generated data was loaded into the Chemotaxis and Migration Tool (Ibidi, https://ibidi.com/chemotaxis-analysis/171-chemotaxis-and-migration-tool.html) to generate velocity measurements and rose plot histograms. Rose plots were re-plotted in MATLAB (MathWorks) using secplot (Matthew Foster, https://www.mathworks.com/matlabcentral/fileexchange/14174-secplot).

Branch analysis

Custom macros for FIJI (Schindelin et al., 2012) were written to automate branch analysis. Briefly, stacks were processed to be maximum intensity projections and oriented with anterior to the left. A rectangular ROI was selected over the posterior region of the yolk tube extension such that the top of the selection was aligned with the bottom of the central RB axons. A tubeness filter was applied to reduce non-axonal background. A histogram of axon directionality was generated for the ROI using OrientationJ (Rezakhaniha et al., 2011). To measure differences in axon directional distribution, we computed the area under the curve (AUC) for the regions to be compared. We then performed a t-test using the resulting mean and standard error to determine significance. To measure axon branch density, eight ROIs measuring 3 µm in height were generated at regularly spaced intervals from dorsal to ventral (Figure 2—figure supplement 1). These selections were individually thresholded, binarized, and the number of objects contained in each ROI was determined using the Analyze Particles function. To analyze acetylated tubulin signal in nascent branches, terminal branches up to 10 µm in length were scored based on the presence or absence of visible acetylated tubulin signal. Total proportion of nascent branches with signal was compared between wild type and mutant embryos.

Kymography

Hyperstacks of EB3-GFP or GFP-Rab5c images were assembled using FIJI (Schindelin et al., 2012) and segments of axons were traced for kymography using the segmented line tool. For peripheral axons, a segment was defined as an area of axon between two branch nodes, or between the first visible portion of axon and the first node of branching. Growth cones and filopodia were excluded from analysis. For analysis of EB3-GFP in nascent branches, only branches < 10 µm long were analyzed. For unbranched central axons, a segment was defined as the entire visible length of either the ascending or descending central axon within each field of view. Kymographs were made using the Multi Kymograph plugin (developed by J. Rietdorf and A. Seitz, European Molecular Biology Laboratory, 2004). Comet or vesicle parameters were measured from the resulting kymographs. To measure Rab5 accumulation at branch points, movies were divided into 4 time periods (t1-t4), each containing 7–8 min. Maximum intensity projections were made for t1-t4 and the fluorescent signal at branch points was measured for each maximum intensity projection. Background signal from the branch point prior to activation was subtracted from measured t1-t4 values, and then all values were normalized to the signal at the t1 period. Comparisons between experimental conditions were made using the Mann-Whitney test for non-parametric data.

Adult Body Size Measurements

Zebrafish were raised for this experiment by in-crossing wildtype or klc4uw314 homozygous siblings. Larva were placed in 3 L tanks side-by-side and subjected to the same light cycle and feeding schedule during rearing. At 7 weeks and 9 weeks, fish were briefly anesthetized with 0.02% tricaine, placed on white waterproof surface, measured with a ruler, and photographed.

Larval touch assay

To assess responsiveness to touch, 3 dpf zebrafish larva were individually placed in a 6 cm dish and allowed to acclimate for 2 min. A tail touch was administered to the end of the tail using a gel loading tip attached to a long applicator stick. The tail touch was given from a side angle to avoid looming stimulus. A high-speed camera was used to acquire 50 frames per second for a total of 1050 frames (21 s). The response time of each larva was measured from the first movement after a touch to the cessation of movement for more than 15 frames (inter-bout pauses only lasted ~7–8 frames). Some wild type and klc4 mutant larva did not respond to an initial touch (10 wild type, 14 mutant), and these samples were excluded from analysis.

Feeding experiment

Feeding experiments were conducted as in Howe et al., 2018. Briefly, adult zebrafish were isolated in individual 1 L tanks without food for 48 hr, then 50 larval zebrafish were added to each tank. The fish were allowed to feed on the larvae for one hour, then removed from the tank to allow counting of the remaining larvae. All tested animals were of AB background.

Novel tank diving test

The evening before an experiment, fish were moved to the behavior experiment room and allowed to acclimate overnight. Exposure to novel tank was carried out in the morning, and the order of which genotype received exposure first was randomized. The novel tank had 3 opaque sides to block potential views of other fish and one clear side for imaging. The water in the novel tank was changed between groups. Fish were netted from their 3 L home tank, released into the novel tank, and allowed to explore for 3 min before being removed and placed into a recovery tank. All tested animals were of AB background.

Habituation experiment

Two weeks prior to an experiment, adults were sorted into separate 3 L tanks with 5–6 individuals of the same genotype in each. Experiment was conducted in a similar manner as described above, but fish were allowed to explore for 10 min before removal. Novel tank exposure was repeated on the same groups twice a week for 3 weeks. Exposures were conducted a minimum of two days apart. The experiment was repeated for another 3 weeks with an additional two groups of 6 fish per genotype. All tested animals were of AB background. When possible, groups were divided into similar ratios of male and female fish for a total of 11 females and 12 males within each genotype. The group compositions were as follows: wild type: group 1 (3 females [F], 2 males [M]) group 2, (3 F, 3 M), group 3 (3 F, 3 M), group 4 (2 F, 4 M), klc4uw314: group 1 (2 F, 3 M), group 2, (3 F, 3 M), group 3 (3 F, 3 M), group 4 (3 F, 3 M).

Automated tracking of adults

For the habituation experiment, automated tracking of zebrafish through ToxTrac (Rodriguez et al., 2018) was used to quantify swim velocity and percent of time spent in the upper zone. When necessary for accurate tracking, background subtraction was performed on movies prior to using ToxTrac. This was accomplished by subtracting the first frame of each movie (showing only the empty tank) from subsequent frames in the movie via FIJI (Schindelin et al., 2012). Operations were performed using Clij2 where possible to speed up processing time (Haase et al., 2019).

Statistics

Statistical analyses and graphs were generated with Graphpad Prism. See figure legends for details on sample sizes, specific statistical tests used, error bars and p values. Data were checked for normality using Anderson-Darling test, D’Agostino & Pearson test, and Shapiro-Wilk test before deciding on a statistical test. Non-normal data were tested for significance using a Mann-Whitney test. In some cases, the N values were too low to perform a statistical test (Figure 6E, Figure 11E and H, Figure 12A). In Figure 6E, the N for wild type is low because fasciculation events are very rare in wild type and obtaining a value for fasciculation length is contingent on the event occurring. For the behavioral data shown in Figure 11E and H and Figure 12A, multiple animals were tested in each trial, but N is the number of individual behavioral trial days (N=3, which is too low to test for normality). In all these cases, individual data points are shown for comparison. Data tested using the Mann-Whitney test were independent of each other and comprised ordinal or continuous numbers.

Acknowledgements

The authors thank the members of the Huisken lab for technical expertise and intellectual contributions, particularly Kurt Weiss, Joe Li, and Alyssa Graves. We are grateful to Katie Drerup and Bill Bement for critical reading and feedback on this manuscript, which greatly improved the final work. We also give our heartfelt thanks to the current and former members of the Halloran lab for their support of and contributions to this work, especially Conlin Bass for valuable work during the initial characterization of klc4 expression pattern, and Daniel North for fish care and maintenance. This work was supported by funds to Mary Halloran from the National Institutes of Health (R01 NS086934, R21 NS116326) and funds to Elizabeth Haynes from the National Institutes of Health (F32 NS098689) and the Morgridge Institute for Research. The Morgridge Institute for Research provided support for the Huisken lab and the light sheet microscope. The Center for Quantitative Cell Imaging provided support for the Opterra Confocal.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Mary C Halloran, Email: mchalloran@wisc.edu.

Fabienne E Poulain, University of South Carolina, United States.

Didier YR Stainier, Max Planck Institute for Heart and Lung Research, Germany.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • National Institutes of Health R01 NS086934 to Mary C Halloran.

  • National Institutes of Health R21 NS116326 to Mary C Halloran.

  • National Institutes of Health F32 NS098689 to Elizabeth M Haynes.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

No competing interests declared.

is a consultant for Bruker, the manufacturer of the Opterra swept field confocal used in this work.

Author contributions

Conceptualization, Software, Formal analysis, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review and editing.

Formal analysis, Investigation, Writing – review and editing.

Conceptualization, Resources, Software, Supervision, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review and editing.

Conceptualization, Software, Formal analysis, Supervision, Investigation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review and editing.

Conceptualization, Software, Investigation.

Resources, Supervision, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review and editing.

Resources, Methodology, Writing – review and editing.

Conceptualization, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review and editing.

Ethics

This study was performed in accordance with the recommendations in the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals of the National Institutes of Health. Animals were handled according to approved institutional animal care and use committee protocols of the University of Wisconsin (protocols L005692 and L005704).

Additional files

Transparent reporting form

Data availability

All data generated or analyzed in this study are included in the manuscript and supporting files. Source Data files have been provided for Figures 1-12.

References

  1. Andersen EF, Asuri NS, Halloran MC. In vivo imaging of cell behaviors and F-actin reveals LIM-HD transcription factor regulation of peripheral versus central sensory axon development. Neural Development. 2011;6:27. doi: 10.1186/1749-8104-6-27. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Andersen EF, Halloran MC. Centrosome movements in vivo correlate with specific neurite formation downstream of LIM homeodomain transcription factor activity. Development. 2012;139:3590–3599. doi: 10.1242/dev.081513. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Antón Z, Weijman JF, Williams C, Moody ERR, Mantell J, Yip YY, Cross JA, Williams TA, Steiner RA, Crump M, Woolfson DN, Dodding MP. Molecular Mechanism for Kinesin-1 Direct Membrane Recognition. bioRxiv. 2021 doi: 10.1101/2021.01.20.427326. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  4. Armijo-Weingart L, Gallo G. It takes a village to raise a branch: cellular mechanisms of the initiation of axon collateral branches. Molecular and Cellular Neurosciences. 2017;84:36–47. doi: 10.1016/j.mcn.2017.03.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Baek JH, Lee J, Yun HS, Lee CW, Song JY, Um HD, Park JK, Park IC, Kim JS, Kim EH, Hwang SG. Kinesin light chain-4 depletion induces apoptosis of radioresistant cancer cells by mitochondrial dysfunction via calcium ion influx. Cell Death & Disease. 2018;9:496. doi: 10.1038/s41419-018-0549-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bayrakli F, Poyrazoglu HG, Yuksel S, Yakicier C, Erguner B, Sagiroglu MS, Yuceturk B, Ozer B, Doganay S, Tanrikulu B, Seker A, Akbulut F, Ozen A, Per H, Kumandas S, Altuner Torun Y, Bayri Y, Sakar M, Dagcinar A, Ziyal I. Hereditary spastic paraplegia with recessive trait caused by mutation in KLC4 gene. Journal of Human Genetics. 2015;60:763–768. doi: 10.1038/jhg.2015.109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bazvand F, Keramatipour M, Riazi-Esfahani H, Mahmoudi A. SPOAN syndrome: a novel mutation and new ocular findings; a case report. BMC Neurology. 2021;21:24. doi: 10.1186/s12883-021-02051-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Cai D, Hoppe AD, Swanson JA, Verhey KJ. Kinesin-1 structural organization and conformational changes revealed by FRET stoichiometry in live cells. The Journal of Cell Biology. 2007;176:51–63. doi: 10.1083/jcb.200605097. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Chen WV, Nwakeze CL, Denny CA, O’Keeffe S, Rieger MA, Mountoufaris G, Kirner A, Dougherty JD, Hen R, Wu Q, Maniatis T. Pcdhαc2 is required for axonal tiling and assembly of serotonergic circuitries in mice. Science. 2017;356:406–411. doi: 10.1126/science.aal3231. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Chiba K, Ori-McKenney KM, Niwa S, McKenney RJ. Reconstitution of Kinesin-1 Activation. bioRxiv. 2021 doi: 10.1101/2021.03.12.434960. [DOI]
  11. Choi JH, Jeong YM, Kim S, Lee B, Ariyasiri K, Kim HT, Jung SH, Hwang KS, Choi TI, Park CO, Huh WK, Carl M, Rosenfeld JA, Raskin S, Ma A, Gecz J, Kim HG, Kim JS, Shin HC, Park DS, Gerlai R, Jamieson BB, Kim JS, Iremonger KJ, Lee SH, Shin HS, Kim CH. Targeted knockout of a chemokine-like gene increases anxiety and fear responses. PNAS. 2018;115:E1041–E1050. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1707663115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Clark BS, Winter M, Cohen AR, Link BA. Generation of rab-based transgenic lines for in vivo studies of endosome biology in zebrafish. Developmental Dynamics. 2011;240:2452–2465. doi: 10.1002/dvdy.22758. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Cockburn JJB, Hesketh SJ, Mulhair P, Thomsen M, O’Connell MJ, Way M. Insights into kinesin-1 activation from the crystal structure of KLC2 bound to JIP3. Structure. 2018;26:1486–1498. doi: 10.1016/j.str.2018.07.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Courchet J, Lewis TL, Lee S, Courchet V, Liou DY, Aizawa S, Polleux F. Terminal axon branching is regulated by the LKB1-NUAK1 kinase pathway via presynaptic mitochondrial capture. Cell. 2013;153:1510–1525. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2013.05.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Csordás G, Weaver D, Hajnóczky G. Endoplasmic reticulum-mitochondrial contactology: structure and signaling functions. Trends in Cell Biology. 2018;28:523–540. doi: 10.1016/j.tcb.2018.02.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Daetwyler S, Günther U, Modes CD, Harrington K, Huisken J. Multi-sample SPIM image acquisition, processing and analysis of vascular growth in zebrafish. Development. 2019;146:6. doi: 10.1242/dev.173757. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Dehairs J, Talebi A, Cherifi Y, Swinnen JV. CRISP-ID: decoding CRISPR mediated indels by sanger sequencing. Scientific Reports. 2016;6:1–5. doi: 10.1038/srep28973. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Dent EW, Kalil K. Axon branching requires interactions between dynamic microtubules and actin filaments. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2001;21:9757–9769. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-24-09757.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Du J, Wei Y, Liu L, Wang Y, Khairova R, Blumenthal R, Tragon T, Hunsberger JG, Machado-Vieira R, Drevets W, Wang YT, Manji HK. A kinesin signaling complex mediates the ability of GSK-3beta to affect mood-associated behaviors. PNAS. 2010;107:11573–11578. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0913138107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Fu X, An Y, Wang H, Li P, Lin J, Yuan J, Yue R, Jin Y, Gao J, Chai R. Deficiency of klc2 induces low-frequency sensorineural hearing loss in C57BL/6 J mice and human. Molecular Neurobiology. 2021;58:4376–4391. doi: 10.1007/s12035-021-02422-w. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Gallo G. The cytoskeletal and signaling mechanisms of axon collateral branching. Developmental Neurobiology. 2011;71:201–220. doi: 10.1002/dneu.20852. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Gibson DA, Ma L. Developmental regulation of axon branching in the vertebrate nervous system. Development. 2011;138:183–195. doi: 10.1242/dev.046441. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Gibson DA, Tymanskyj S, Yuan RC, Leung HC, Lefebvre JL, Sanes JR, Chédotal A, Ma L. Dendrite self-avoidance requires cell-autonomous slit/robo signaling in cerebellar purkinje cells. Neuron. 2014;81:1040–1056. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.01.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Gindhart JG, Desai CJ, Beushausen S, Zinn K, Goldstein LS. Kinesin light chains are essential for axonal transport in Drosophila. The Journal of Cell Biology. 1998;141:443–454. doi: 10.1083/jcb.141.2.443. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Grueber WB, Sagasti A. Self-avoidance and tiling: mechanisms of dendrite and axon spacing. Cold Spring Harbor Perspectives in Biology. 2010;2:a001750. doi: 10.1101/cshperspect.a001750. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Gupta SK, Meiri KF, Mahfooz K, Bharti U, Mani S. Coordination between extrinsic extracellular matrix cues and intrinsic responses to orient the centrosome in polarizing cerebellar granule neurons. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30:2755–2766. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4218-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Haase R, Royer LA, Steinbach P, Schmidt D, Dibrov A, Schmidt U, Weigert M, Maghelli N, Tomancak P, Jug F, Myers EW. CLIJ: GPU-accelerated image processing for everyone. Nature Methods. 2019;17:5–6. doi: 10.1038/s41592-019-0650-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Hakeda-Suzuki S, Berger-Müller S, Tomasi T, Usui T, Horiuchi SY, Uemura T, Suzuki T. Golden goal collaborates with flamingo in conferring synaptic-layer specificity in the visual system. Nature Neuroscience. 2011;14:314–323. doi: 10.1038/nn.2756. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Halloran MC, Severance SM, Yee CS, Gemza DL, Raper JA, Kuwada JY. Analysis of a zebrafish semaphorin reveals potential functions in vivo. Developmental Dynamics. 1999;214:13–25. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0177(199901)214:1&#x0003c;13::AID-DVDY2&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Hirokawa N, Niwa S, Tanaka Y. Molecular motors in neurons: transport mechanisms and roles in brain function, development, and disease. Neuron. 2010;68:610–638. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.09.039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Howe HB, McIntyre PB, Wolman MA. Adult zebrafish primarily use vision to guide piscivorous foraging behavior. Behavioural Processes. 2018;157:230–237. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2018.10.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Jiang M, Esteve-Rudd J, Lopes VS, Diemer T, Lillo C, Rump A, Williams DS. Microtubule motors transport phagosomes in the RPE, and lack of KLC1 leads to AMD-like pathogenesis. The Journal of Cell Biology. 2015;210:595–611. doi: 10.1083/jcb.201410112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Kalil K, Dent EW. Branch management: mechanisms of axon branching in the developing vertebrate CNS. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience. 2014;15:7–18. doi: 10.1038/nrn3650. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Kamal A, Stokin GB, Yang Z, Xia CH, Goldstein LS. Axonal transport of amyloid precursor protein is mediated by direct binding to the kinesin light chain subunit of kinesin-I. Neuron. 2000;28:449–459. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)00124-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Katori S, Noguchi-Katori Y, Okayama A, Kawamura Y, Luo W, Sakimura K, Hirabayashi T, Iwasato T, Yagi T. Protocadherin-αC2 is required for diffuse projections of serotonergic axons. Scientific Reports. 2017;7:15908. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-16120-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Ketschek A, Jones S, Spillane M, Korobova F, Svitkina T, Gallo G. Nerve growth factor promotes reorganization of the axonal microtubule array at sites of axon collateral branching. Developmental Neurobiology. 2015;75:1441–1461. doi: 10.1002/dneu.22294. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Khan KM, Collier AD, Meshalkina DA, Kysil EV, Khatsko SL, Kolesnikova T, Morzherin YY, Warnick JE, Kalueff AV, Echevarria DJ. Zebrafish models in neuropsychopharmacology and CNS drug discovery. British Journal of Pharmacology. 2017;174:1925–1944. doi: 10.1111/bph.13754. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Kimmel CB, Ballard WW, Kimmel SR, Ullmann B, Schilling TF. Stages of embryonic development of the zebrafish. Developmental Dynamics. 1995;203:253–310. doi: 10.1002/aja.1002030302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Knafo S, Fidelin K, Prendergast A, Tseng PEB, Parrin A, Dickey C, Böhm UL, Figueiredo SN, Thouvenin O, Pascal-Moussellard H, Wyart C. Mechanosensory neurons control the timing of spinal microcircuit selection during locomotion. eLife. 2017;6:e25260. doi: 10.7554/eLife.25260. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Koh SY, Cain JT, Magee H, White KA, Rechtzigel M, Meyerink B, Leppert H, Timm DJ, Morgan JP, Johnson TB, Grove B, Khanna R, Hensley K, Brudvig J, Weimer JM. A CLN6-CRMP2-KLC4 Complex Regulates Anterograde ER-Derived Vesicle Trafficking in Cortical Neurites. bioRxiv. 2021 doi: 10.1101/2021.09.16.460653. [DOI]
  41. Kostadinov D, Sanes JR. Protocadherin-dependent dendritic self-avoidance regulates neural connectivity and circuit function. eLife. 2015;4:e964. doi: 10.7554/eLife.08964. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Krols M, Bultynck G, Janssens S. ER-mitochondria contact sites: a new regulator of cellular calcium flux comes into play. The Journal of Cell Biology. 2016;214:367–370. doi: 10.1083/jcb.201607124. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Kwan KM, Fujimoto E, Grabher C, Mangum BD, Hardy ME, Campbell DS, Parant JM, Yost HJ, Kanki JP, Chien CB. The tol2kit: a multisite gateway-based construction kit for tol2 transposon transgenesis constructs. Developmental Dynamics. 2007;236:3088–3099. doi: 10.1002/dvdy.21343. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Lee TJ, Lee JW, Haynes EM, Eliceiri KW, Halloran MC. The kinesin adaptor calsyntenin-1 organizes microtubule polarity and regulates dynamics during sensory axon arbor development. Frontiers in Cellular Neuroscience. 2017;11:107. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2017.00107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Lefebvre JL, Kostadinov D, Chen WV, Maniatis T, Sanes JR. Protocadherins mediate dendritic self-avoidance in the mammalian nervous system. Nature. 2012;488:517–521. doi: 10.1038/nature11305. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Liu Y, Halloran MC. Central and peripheral axon branches from one neuron are guided differentially by semaphorin3d and transient axonal glycoprotein-1. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2005;25:10556–10563. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2710-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Liz MA, Mar FM, Santos TE, Pimentel HI, Marques AM, Morgado MM, Vieira S, Sousa VF, Pemble H, Wittmann T, Sutherland C, Woodgett JR, Sousa MM. Neuronal deletion of GSK3β increases microtubule speed in the growth cone and enhances axon regeneration via CRMP-2 and independently of MAP1B and CLASP2. BMC Biology. 2014;12:1–19. doi: 10.1186/1741-7007-12-47. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Lu W, Gelfand VI. Moonlighting motors: kinesin, dynein, and cell polarity. Trends in Cell Biology. 2017;27:505–514. doi: 10.1016/j.tcb.2017.02.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Matthews BJ, Kim ME, Flanagan JJ, Hattori D, Clemens JC, Zipursky SL, Grueber WB. Dendrite self-avoidance is controlled by dscam. Cell. 2007;129:593–604. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2007.04.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Maximino C, de Brito TM, da Silva Batista AW, Herculano AM, Morato S, Gouveia A. Measuring anxiety in zebrafish: a critical review. Behavioural Brain Research. 2010;214:157–171. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.05.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Melo US, Macedo-Souza LI, Figueiredo T, Muotri AR, Gleeson JG, Coux G, Armas P, Calcaterra NB, Kitajima JP, Amorim S, Olávio TR, Griesi-Oliveira K, Coatti GC, Rocha CRR, Martins-Pinheiro M, Menck CFM, Zaki MS, Kok F, Zatz M, Santos S. Overexpression of KLC2 due to a homozygous deletion in the non-coding region causes SPOAN syndrome. Human Molecular Genetics. 2015;24:6877–6885. doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddv388. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Millard SS, Flanagan JJ, Pappu KS, Wu W, Zipursky SL. Dscam2 mediates axonal tiling in the Drosophila visual system. Nature. 2007;447:720–724. doi: 10.1038/nature05855. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Mochizuki H, Toda H, Ando M, Kurusu M, Tomoda T, Furukubo-Tokunaga K. Unc-51/ATG1 controls axonal and dendritic development via kinesin-mediated vesicle transport in the Drosophila brain. PLOS ONE. 2011;6:e19632. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0019632. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Moreno-Mateos MA, Vejnar CE, Beaudoin JD, Fernandez JP, Mis EK, Khokha MK, Giraldez AJ. CRISPRscan: designing highly efficient sgrnas for CRISPR-cas9 targeting in vivo. Nature Methods. 2015;12:982–988. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.3543. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Morfini G, Schmidt N, Weissmann C, Pigino G, Kins S. Conventional kinesin: biochemical heterogeneity and functional implications in health and disease. Brain Research Bulletin. 2016;126:347–353. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2016.06.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Nakamura F, Ohshima T, Goshima Y. Collapsin response mediator proteins: their biological functions and pathophysiology in neuronal development and regeneration. Frontiers in Cellular Neuroscience. 2020;14:188. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2020.00188. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Navone F, Niclas J, Hom-Booher N, Sparks L, Bernstein HD, McCaffrey G, Vale RD. Cloning and expression of a human kinesin heavy chain gene: interaction of the COOH-terminal domain with cytoplasmic microtubules in transfected CV-1 cells. The Journal of Cell Biology. 1992;117:1263–1275. doi: 10.1083/jcb.117.6.1263. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Orefice LL, Zimmerman AL, Chirila AM, Sleboda SJ, Head JP, Ginty DD. Peripheral mechanosensory neuron dysfunction underlies tactile and behavioral deficits in mouse models of asds. Cell. 2016;166:299–313. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2016.05.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Pernigo S, Chegkazi MS, Yip YY, Treacy C, Glorani G, Hansen K, Politis A, Bui S, Dodding MP, Steiner RA. Structural basis for isoform-specific kinesin-1 recognition of Y-acidic cargo adaptors. eLife. 2018;7:e38362. doi: 10.7554/eLife.38362. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Ponomareva OY, Holmen IC, Sperry AJ, Eliceiri KW, Halloran MC. Calsyntenin-1 regulates axon branching and endosomal trafficking during sensory neuron development in vivo. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2014;34:9235–9248. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0561-14.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Ponomareva OY, Eliceiri KW, Halloran MC. Charcot-marie-tooth 2B associated rab7 mutations cause axon growth and guidance defects during vertebrate sensory neuron development. Neural Development. 2016;11:2. doi: 10.1186/s13064-016-0058-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Qiang L, Yu W, Liu M, Solowska JM, Baas PW. Basic fibroblast growth factor elicits formation of interstitial axonal branches via enhanced severing of microtubules. Molecular Biology of the Cell. 2010;21:334–344. doi: 10.1091/mbc.E09-09-0834. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Rahman A, Kamal A, Roberts EA, Goldstein LS. Defective kinesin heavy chain behavior in mouse kinesin light chain mutants. The Journal of Cell Biology. 1999;146:1277–1288. doi: 10.1083/jcb.146.6.1277. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Randall TS, Yip YY, Wallock-Richards DJ, Pfisterer K, Sanger A, Ficek W, Steiner RA, Beavil AJ, Parsons M, Dodding MP. A small-molecule activator of kinesin-1 drives remodeling of the microtubule network. PNAS. 2017;114:13738–13743. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1715115115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Randlett O, Poggi L, Zolessi FR, Harris WA. The oriented emergence of axons from retinal ganglion cells is directed by laminin contact in vivo. Neuron. 2011;70:266–280. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.03.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Rezakhaniha R, Agianniotis A, Schrauwen JTC, Griffa A, Sage D, Bouten CVC, van de Vosse FN, Unser M, Stergiopulos N. Experimental investigation of collagen waviness and orientation in the arterial adventitia using confocal laser scanning microscopy. Biomechanics and Modeling in Mechanobiology. 2011;11:461–473. doi: 10.1007/s10237-011-0325-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Rodriguez A, Zhang H, Klaminder J, Brodin T, Andersson PL, Andersson M, Freckleton R. Toxtrac: a fast and robust software for tracking organisms. Methods in Ecology and Evolution. 2018;9:460–464. doi: 10.1111/2041-210X.12874. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  68. Rowland AA, Voeltz GK. Endoplasmic reticulum-mitochondria contacts: function of the junction. Nature Reviews. Molecular Cell Biology. 2012;13:607–625. doi: 10.1038/nrm3440. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Saez TMM, Fernandez Bessone I, Rodriguez MS, Alloatti M, Otero MG, Cromberg LE, Pozo Devoto VM, Oubiña G, Sosa L, Buffone MG, Gelman DM, Falzone TL. Kinesin-1-mediated axonal transport of CB1 receptors is required for cannabinoid-dependent axonal growth and guidance. Development. 2020;147:8. doi: 10.1242/dev.184069. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Sagasti A, Guido MR, Raible DW, Schier AF. Repulsive interactions shape the morphologies and functional arrangement of zebrafish peripheral sensory arbors. Current Biology. 2005;15:804–814. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2005.03.048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Schindelin J, Arganda-Carreras I, Frise E, Kaynig V, Longair M, Pietzsch T, Preibisch S, Rueden C, Saalfeld S, Schmid B, Tinevez JY, White DJ, Hartenstein V, Eliceiri K, Tomancak P, Cardona A. Fiji: an open-source platform for biological-image analysis. Nature Methods. 2012;9:676–682. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Seeger MA, Rice SE. Microtubule-associated protein-like binding of the kinesin-1 tail to microtubules. The Journal of Biological Chemistry. 2010;285:8155–8162. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M109.068247. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Senti KA, Usui T, Boucke K, Greber U, Uemura T, Dickson BJ. Flamingo regulates R8 axon-axon and axon-target interactions in the Drosophila visual system. Current Biology. 2003;13:828–832. doi: 10.1016/s0960-9822(03)00291-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Shah AN, Davey CF, Whitebirch AC, Miller AC, Moens CB. Rapid reverse genetic screening using CRISPR in zebrafish. Nature Methods. 2015;12:535–540. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.3360. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Shima T, Morikawa M, Kaneshiro J, Kambara T, Kamimura S, Yagi T, Iwamoto H, Uemura S, Shigematsu H, Shirouzu M, Ichimura T, Watanabe TM, Nitta R, Okada Y, Hirokawa N. Kinesin-binding-triggered conformation switching of microtubules contributes to polarized transport. The Journal of Cell Biology. 2018;217:4164–4183. doi: 10.1083/jcb.201711178. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Smith CJ, Watson JD, VanHoven MK, Colón-Ramos DA, Miller DM. Netrin (UNC-6) mediates dendritic self-avoidance. Nature Neuroscience. 2012;15:731–737. doi: 10.1038/nn.3065. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Soba P, Zhu S, Emoto K, Younger S, Yang SJ, Yu HH, Lee T, Jan LY, Jan YN. Drosophila sensory neurons require dscam for dendritic self-avoidance and proper dendritic field organization. Neuron. 2007;54:403–416. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.03.029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Spead O, Poulain FE. Trans-axonal signaling in neural circuit wiring. International Journal of Molecular Sciences. 2020;21:E5170. doi: 10.3390/ijms21145170. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Spillane M, Ketschek A, Merianda TT, Twiss JL, Gallo G. Mitochondria coordinate sites of axon branching through localized intra-axonal protein synthesis. Cell Reports. 2013;5:1564–1575. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2013.11.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Stepanova T, Slemmer J, Hoogenraad CC, Lansbergen G, Dortland B, De Zeeuw CI, Grosveld F, van Cappellen G, Akhmanova A, Galjart N. Visualization of microtubule growth in cultured neurons via the use of EB3-GFP (end-binding protein 3-green fluorescent protein) The Journal of Neuroscience. 2003;23:2655–2664. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-07-02655.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Tao K, Matsuki N, Koyama R. AMP-activated protein kinase mediates activity-dependent axon branching by recruiting mitochondria to axon. Developmental Neurobiology. 2014;74:557–573. doi: 10.1002/dneu.22149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Tomasi T, Hakeda-Suzuki S, Ohler S, Schleiffer A, Suzuki T. The transmembrane protein golden goal regulates R8 photoreceptor axon-axon and axon-target interactions. Neuron. 2008;57:691–704. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.01.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Triclin S, Inoue D, Gaillard J, Htet ZM, DeSantis ME, Portran D, Derivery E, Aumeier C, Schaedel L, John K, Leterrier C, Reck-Peterson SL, Blanchoin L, Théry M. Self-repair protects microtubules from destruction by molecular motors. Nature Materials. 2021;20:883–891. doi: 10.1038/s41563-020-00905-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Wang J, Zugates CT, Liang IH, Lee CHJ, Lee T. Drosophila dscam is required for divergent segregation of sister branches and suppresses ectopic bifurcation of axons. Neuron. 2002;33:559–571. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00570-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Webster DR, Borisy GG. Microtubules are acetylated in domains that turn over slowly. Journal of Cell Science. 1989;92 ( Pt 1):57–65. doi: 10.1242/jcs.92.1.57. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Williams K, Ribera AB. Long-lived zebrafish rohon-beard cells. Developmental Biology. 2020;464:45–52. doi: 10.1016/j.ydbio.2020.05.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Winkle CC, Taylor KL, Dent EW, Gallo G, Greif KF, Gupton SL. Beyond the cytoskeleton: the emerging role of organelles and membrane remodeling in the regulation of axon collateral branches. Developmental Neurobiology. 2016;76:1293–1307. doi: 10.1002/dneu.22398. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Wong K, Elegante M, Bartels B, Elkhayat S, Tien D, Roy S, Goodspeed J, Suciu C, Tan J, Grimes C, Chung A, Rosenberg M, Gaikwad S, Denmark A, Jackson A, Kadri F, Chung KM, Stewart A, Gilder T, Beeson E, Zapolsky I, Wu N, Cachat J, Kalueff AV. Analyzing habituation responses to novelty in zebrafish (Danio rerio) Behavioural Brain Research. 2010;208:450–457. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.12.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Wong YL, Rice SE. Kinesin’s light chains inhibit the head- and microtubule-binding activity of its tail. PNAS. 2010;107:11781–11786. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1005854107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Wong HHW, Lin JQ, Ströhl F, Roque CG, Cioni JM, Cagnetta R, Turner-Bridger B, Laine RF, Harris WA, Kaminski CF, Holt CE. RNA docking and local translation regulate site-specific axon remodeling in vivo. Neuron. 2017;95:852–868. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.07.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Woźniak MJ, Allan VJ. Cargo selection by specific kinesin light chain 1 isoforms. The EMBO Journal. 2006;25:5457–5468. doi: 10.1038/sj.emboj.7601427. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Yu W, Ahmad FJ, Baas PW. Microtubule fragmentation and partitioning in the axon during collateral branch formation. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1994;14:5872–5884. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.14-10-05872.1994. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Yu W, Qiang L, Solowska JM, Karabay A, Korulu S, Baas PW. The microtubule-severing proteins spastin and katanin participate differently in the formation of axonal branches. Molecular Biology of the Cell. 2008;19:1485–1498. doi: 10.1091/mbc.E07-09-0878. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Zhu H, Lee HY, Tong Y, Hong BS, Kim KP, Shen Y, Lim KJ, Mackenzie F, Tempel W, Park HW. Crystal structures of the tetratricopeptide repeat domains of kinesin light chains: insight into cargo recognition mechanisms. PLOS ONE. 2012;7:e33943. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0033943. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Ziv L, Muto A, Schoonheim PJ, Meijsing SH, Strasser D, Ingraham HA, Schaaf MJM, Yamamoto KR, Baier H. An affective disorder in zebrafish with mutation of the glucocorticoid receptor. Molecular Psychiatry. 2013;18:681–691. doi: 10.1038/mp.2012.64. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Editor's evaluation

Fabienne E Poulain 1

Using zebrafish as an in vivo model, this study reveals for the first time that mutations in the kinesin light chain gene klc4, which are known to cause a form of early onset hereditary spastic paraplegia in human, affect specific aspects of neuronal development and nervous system functions. High-resolution movies of developing sensory neurons in vivo and behavioral assays support the key findings that the motor subunit Klc4 plays essential functions in the control of neuronal morphogenesis and compartmentalization as well as associated behaviors.

Decision letter

Editor: Fabienne E Poulain1
Reviewed by: Fabienne E Poulain2

Our editorial process produces two outputs: (i) public reviews designed to be posted alongside the preprint for the benefit of readers; (ii) feedback on the manuscript for the authors, including requests for revisions, shown below. We also include an acceptance summary that explains what the editors found interesting or important about the work.

Decision letter after peer review:

Thank you for submitting your article "KLC4 shapes axon arbors during development and mediates adult behavior" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by 3 peer reviewers, including Fabienne E. Poulain as Reviewing Editor and Reviewer #1, and the evaluation has been overseen by Didier Stainier as the Senior Editor.

After discussing their reviews with one another, and as you will see from the individual reviews below, all three reviewers agree that your study describes novel and interesting functions of Klc4 in sensory axon development and has therefore a potential high impact. However, reviewers share four major concerns that need to be fully addressed before your manuscript can be considered further for publication in eLife. Essential revisions can be summarized as follows:

1) The study lacks a detailed analysis of axonal phenotypes in klc4 mutants and would be strengthened by analyzing single arbor dynamics and/or using additional morphological criteria to define branches and analyze axonal density.

2) The study lacks mechanistic insight into the roles of KLC4 and the phenotypes observed. Reviewers agree that at least some mechanistic experiments are necessary to explain how klc4 mutations cause axonal phenotypes. These include analyzing the transport of specific cargoes during axonal development and microtubule behavior during axonal branching. The localization and cell-autonomous function of klc4 could also be analyzed.

3) There is a disconnect between the description of sensory axonal defects in embryos and the characterization of behavioral defects in adults. Behavioral assays that rely on functional sensory neurons instead (touch-response in larvae for example) would be more appropriate for this study.

4) As described in detail by reviewer 2 (see below), multiple concerns about statistical analyses were raised and need to be addressed.

Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

Overall, the manuscript is very well written and the conclusions are well supported by the data. The light sheet and swept confocal microscopy movies provide a unique view of sensory axon development in vivo and are beautiful! Below are a few questions / comments that I had while reading the manuscript:

1) The data in figures 3 and 5 show nicely that axon branches are not stabilized and that axonal growth is shifted posteriorly in klc4 mutants. Is the collapse of branches direction-dependent in the mutant? Is there a difference in branch stabilization/pruning depending on whether the branch elongates anteriorly or posteriorly?

2) The presence of apical protrusions crossing the midline in klc4 mutants suggests a defect in RB neuron polarity. Is the centrosome mislocalized in klc4 mutants?

3) The existence of both Klc4 isoforms is interesting, especially since corresponding transcripts seem to be differently regulated during development. Since the short isoform is not predicted to bind any cargo, is anything known about its functions? Are both isoforms expressed in RB neurons?

4) It would be interesting to analyze the localization of Klc4 in sensory neurons, although this might be difficult to do. Would it be possible to mosaically express low levels of tagged klc4 in mutant sensor neurons and analyze its distribution?

Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

The authors use Crisper technology to generate a zebrafish line that lacks expression of KLC4. This animal is then used throughout, along with pharmacological inhibition of KLC binding to adaptor proteins, to address the roles of KLC4 in the development of the morphology of Rohon-Beard (RB) sensory neurons in embryos and assess behavioral alterations in adult fish. Modern imaging methods are applied to study neuronal morphology in embryos and a battery of behavioral tests in adults. The study provides first line of inquiry assessment of the role of KLC4 in the development of RB morphology, focusing on the failure to establish proper axon arbors in the periphery. While each data set has intrinsic value and is novel, none is followed up to provide mechanistic insights into KLC4 functions in neurons. The value of the submission rests in providing a characterization of the effects of KLC4 knockout, but it does not further the understanding of the mechanistic contributions of KLC4 in the processes impacted by the genetic deletion of KLC4. While the significance of the question posed is well presented and clear, multiple issues regarding interpretation, presentation and analysis decrease enthusiasm for the submission in its current form.

For the authors' consideration:

1. The current in situ data uses a riboprobe that does not differentiate between the short and long forms of the KLC4 transcript. Given the large difference between short and long transcripts of KLC4, and expected differences in function of the resultant proteins, it would assist in the interpretation of the results to assess short and long transcript expression in RB neurons. Ideally, this would be followed up by specific knock down of the long form to dissect its contributions from the short form.

2. At first evaluation, it is not clear how the axon branching metric (presented in Supplemental Figure 2) is specific for axon branching. In the images provided there seem to be consistently fewer axons present as a function of distance from the central fasicle in the KLC mutant and kinesore treated embryos (e.g., Figure 2A,B,D,E,H,I; Figure 4A,B). How can the metric dissociate the number of main axons that arrived at the ROI from branches made from those axons?

Based on the high quality of the imaging, it seems that it would be possible to use morphological criteria to define axon branches and measure the density along individual axons. This form of assessment would be much more stringent than the ROI crossing method used. Indeed, this form of assessment is used in the live imaging studies addressing rates or branch formation and stability.

3. Considering the above comment (2) regarding the consistent observable decrease in the apparent number of axons and their lengths into the periphery noted above, it is surprising that an increase in the rate of axon extension was detected in the live imaging analysis (Figure 3E). It is suggested that the authors analyze the density of axons and their total length as a function of distance from the central axon fascicle, noting that the ROI based approach in supplemental Figure 2 should serve this purpose by simply changing the distance from the fascicle into the periphery at constant intervals (currently only 65 microns was addressed).

Based on the images presented I would expect that a decrease in axon density and length will be determined in the KLC4 mutant. If so, then these data would stand in conflict with the observed increase rate of axon extension, but may indicate that earlier aspects of axon formation were also impacted in the KLC4 mutant. The formation of axons from the cell body may share mechanistic similarity to the emergence of branches, and thus may be delayed in the mutant due to failure to stabilize the new axon at its early stages of formation which could then result in the difference suggested by the images provided without being confounded by the subsequent increase in axon extension rate. It would also be insightful to perform this analysis at multiple points in ontogenesis.

4. An interpretational caveat is that with both the genetic mutant and the drug treatment all cells are expected to be affected. From the in situ is it not clear to this reviewer if there is no expression of KLC4 in the environment the axons/branches are migrating through. Might this issue be resolved using zebrafish genetics with lines allowing for specific knockdown in RB neurons? or some other approach to address whether the effects are cell intrinsic.

5. Statistical analysis. There are multiple concerns regarding the statistical presentation and analysis.

The section in the methods requires much elaboration, starting with which test was used to determine the normalcy of data sets, and addressed by subsequent comments.

Figures4 and 10 require statistical analysis of the data. "Plots using 95% confidence intervals (CI) were considered to be significant when the 95% CI ranges did not overlap." Is not considered a stringent analytical method, although a valid "rule of thumb". The authors are urged to seek assistance from a statistician.

6E, no 95% CI shown as claimed in the legend. The mean is not likely to be significant as the WT only have 3 points and one is an outlier relative to the other 2. I also do not see how the mean in the +/+ could be as shown, as claimed in the legend, it seems to be the median. I would estimate the mean to be somewhere around 20 um. For the mutant data, the distribution does not strike me as normal. Were these data analyzed for normality? Also, there is no statistical analysis shown, appreciating that with an n=3 for the WT this is problematic and power would have to be increased through greater sampling.

The legend to Figure 8 states that t-test with Mann-Whitney correction are used. I am not familiar with this correction for a t-test and was not able to find references to such a correction for t-test. Was Welch correction intended? The statistical description in the method is very sparse as presented and details should be presented. How the basic assumptions of the rather limited t-test were met by data prior to selection of specific t-test should be clearly denoted.

Figure 9. In panel C, a t-test is used but (1) the data are bounded values, and (2) the distribution in WT is clearly non normal with many data points at the boundary (i.e., 100%). A t-test is not applicable as basic assumptions are broken, and do not appear to have been determined. E and H have no p values or test shown in legend and it is not clear why individual data points are not shown here as in other panels. F and G have no test denoted. The meaning of * and multiples thereof is only defined for one panel. Panel G has *** not defined at all.

6. Strictly speaking, the authors are inferring contact repulsion but do not use the imaging data sets to directly address it (i.e., through direct analysis of the consequences of contacts between the two sister branches after growth cone bifurcation). The observed changes in angles between sister branches could be due to a variety of other mechanisms altering the ability of sister branches to generate larger angles.

The self-contact examples in Figure 6 are not particularly compelling. Perhaps, presentation of all relevant frames in the timelapse with greater annotation and description would assist a reader.

In Figure 6B, in contrast to what the text claims, this reviewer does not see a clear growth cone bifurcation followed by fasiculation due to lack of self-repulsion. It appears a small back branch from the second panel from the left aligns with the main axon.

Also, the "appearing to collapse onto one another" (line 232) could be interpreted as a consequence of the prior analysis of branch stability.

7. Figure 8D,E. "Comet Frequency" in comets/um/min. It is not clear if this reflects new comets observed during the imaging or all observed comets/um/min.

The imaging sets would benefit from additional analysis to extract the duration of EB3 comets (i.e., summed period spent polymerizing per comet) and total distance from initiation to loss of the comet.

It is not clear why central axons are analyzed for only these data sets and not others. As the focus of the paper is on peripheral axons the suggested additional data sets for peripheral axons would suffice.

As a whole, I would suggest that the data in Figure 8 from EB3 imaging indicate increased polymerization of plus tips along axons in the mutant. This is not immediately reconcilable with decreases branch stability as one would presume that microtubules would be more prone to catastrophe and depolymerization in conjunction with decreased branch stability. The data are broadly consistent with the detected increase in axon extension rate. However, analysis of EB3 comets needs to be provided in branches (collateral and bifurcated) in order to be linked to the main observed phenotype of decreased branch stability.

8. While of general interest, the behavioral assays do not address RB neuron function or circuitry, which is embryonic. The fit of these data to the rest of the study is not clear. Could tail startle be assessed (Liu and Hale (2017) Curr Biol 27:697-704) for addressing RB/RB-related circuit function? Any RB function assays would provide a much better fit to the rest of the data presented.

Importantly, these behavioral deficits cannot be attributed to changes in circuit formation, branching, axon extension etc as the mutant is not neuron specific, the relevant circuitry has not been analyzed, and no aspect of CNS neurons or glia or anything else has been analyzed. The concern is compounded by the differences in overall growth of the fish.

9. One fundamental aspect of the study that is missing is consideration of KLC4 localization in the RB neurons. Whether it localizes to the RB axons remains unknown, or whether it ever targets to branches. One would presume so, but this needs to be assessed. It is hard to interpret results with any degree of mechanistic insight without knowledge of the localization of the molecule addressed.

10. Final considerations: Overall, this submission contains interesting and valuable data sets that are however preliminary in nature as none is followed up to achieve a deeper mechanistic understanding. This manuscript presents a characterization of phenotypes in KLC4 mutant zebrafish RB neurons but does not provide insights into how KLC4 is mechanistically contributing to the observed phenotypes, or whether it is acting cell intrinsically. Having said this, the discussion carefully considers the potential implications of the individual findings and notes relevant future directions.

Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

Further characterization of axonal defects:

– While the population-based methods for characterizing axonal defects are elegant and informative, these defects could potentially be further appreciated with single cell resolution, What do the arbors of individual neurons look like in these mutants? Does the mutant affect all sensory neurons equally, or is a sub-population of neurons more severely affected? It should be possible to analyze single neurons with transient transgenesis in the zebrafish model. This could be particularly helpful since aberrant fasciculation may make it difficult to distinguish multiple fasciculated axon branches when the whole population of neurons is labeled. Identifying defects at a single neuron level could also make it possible to carry out rescue experiments to address the cell autonomy of axon arborization defects.

Explanation of axonal defects mechanistically:

– The impact of this paper would be most enhanced by characterizing the axonal trafficking of cellular cargos in klc4 mutants, particularly if defects in specific trafficked cargoes could plausibly explain the axonal defects. In the discussion of the paper, the authors note that they have previously found that endosomal trafficking regulates branching. Characterizing endosomal trafficking, as well as trafficking of mitochondria, lysosomes, golgi, etc, thus has the potential to make this study substantially more mechanistically satisfying.

– The changes in microtubule dynamics of klc4 mutants are interesting, but how trafficking defects affect these dynamics, and how defects in MT dynamics affect branching, is unclear. To address how trafficking affects MTs, the authors could attempt to identify MTOCs in axons. For example, since golgi outposts are thought to be MTOCs, is their trafficking into axons affected by klc4 mutation? To address how MT defects affect branching, the authors could characterize MT dynamics during branching-for example, do mutants display any defects in the frequency of MT invasion into nascent branches?

Behavior:

– As mentioned above, the analyses of adult behavioral defects, while interesting and rigorously carried out, do not complement the morphological analysis in larval sensory neurons. It would be exciting if axonal arborization defects in a specific neuron type could be linked to behavioral defects. Could the authors attempt to characterize touch-response behaviors in larval fish? This would be a particularly exciting if it were possible to do cell-autonomous rescue experiments, thus directly testing if defects in sensory neuron morphology affect behavior.

eLife. 2022 Oct 12;11:e74270. doi: 10.7554/eLife.74270.sa2

Author response


Essential revisions

1) The study lacks a detailed analysis of axonal phenotypes in klc4 mutants and would be strengthened by analyzing single arbor dynamics and/or using additional morphological criteria to define branches and analyze axonal density.

To get more detailed information about axon density, we increased our image sampling by using a grid ROI instead of a single line ROI. This analysis approach provides information about axon density as a function of dorsal-ventral location as suggested by Reviewer 2, and also gives information about both growth cone bifurcation and interstitial branching. Details are described in response to Reviewer 2, points 2 and 3 below, and data added to Figure 2F, G, and Supplemental Figure 2. Although all neurons were labeled in our live imaging of axon growth and branching experiments, we were able to distinguish axons from individual neurons, and the analysis of branch dynamics was done on individual neurons. Further description of our reasoning on this point is in our response to Reviewer 3, point 1.

2) The study lacks mechanistic insight into the roles of KLC4 and the phenotypes observed. Reviewers agree that at least some mechanistic experiments are necessary to explain how klc4 mutations cause axonal phenotypes. These include analyzing the transport of specific cargoes during axonal development and microtubule behavior during axonal branching. The localization and cell-autonomous function of klc4 could also be analyzed.

We added new experiments to image endosomal dynamics (new Figure 8), trafficking of Rab5-labeled endosomes from the cell body to branch points (new Figure 8 —figure supplement 1A, 1B), microtubule behavior during axon branching (new Figure 10), and acetylation of microtubules in nascent branches (new Figure 10). We chose Rab5-labeled endosomes because of our previous work showing a correlation between their transport and RB axon branching. Although the klc4 mutants did not have a decrease in endosome delivery to branch points, high-speed imaging of vesicle dynamics revealed some alterations in transport of these vesicles in klc4 mutants. These alterations in endosome transport are not likely to be the only effects of klc4 mutation on cargo dynamics or to fully explain the mechanism by which KLC promotes branch stabilization. However, an exhaustive analysis of all potential cargos would require many months to years of work and is outside the scope of this manuscript. Further description is in response to Reviewer 3, point 2.

We also added new experiments analyzing microtubules in nascent branches (new Figure 10). We used live imaging with EB3-GFP to measure frequency of comets entering nascent branches. Although comet frequency in established axons is increased in klc4 mutants, there is no difference in frequency of comets entering nascent branches, suggesting a relative decrease. We used anti-acetylated tubulin labeling to show that klc4 mutants have fewer nascent branches with stable, acetylated tubulin. Further description is in our response to Reviewer 2, point 7 and Reviewer 3, point 3.

As described in our response to Reviewer 2, point 4, we believe the effects of klc4 mutation are likely cell autonomous because klc4 is not expressed in the somites or the skin, which are the tissues in contact with RB peripheral axons, and RB neurons are the predominant cells in the dorsal spinal cord that express klc4 at the stages when we see the axon phenotypes.

As described in response to Reviewer 1, point 4 and Reviewer 2, point 9, there are major obstacles that prevent visualization of KLC4 protein localization within neurons. There are no available antibodies to zebrafish KLC4. We tried using expression of KLC4-GFP fusion constructs (as suggested by Reviewer 1), but expression leads to diffuse labeling throughout the cytoplasm. This problem has been previously reported by others attempting to visualize kinesin-1 heavy chains (Yang et al. 2019, Traffic 20:851-866). Because the vast majority of kinesin light chains and heavy chains exist in an auto-inhibited conformation that is freely diffusible, the diffuse signal resulting from expression of tagged proteins obscures visualization of motors bound to cargos. Thus, we are unfortunately not able to image KLC4 protein localization with current available approaches.

3) There is a disconnect between the description of sensory axonal defects in embryos and the characterization of behavioral defects in adults. Behavioral assays that rely on functional sensory neurons instead (touch-response in larvae for example) would be more appropriate for this study.

We added new experiments testing the larval touch response. We found that klc4 mutant larvae showed increased number of swim bouts and longer duration swim runs after a touch stimulus, suggesting the mutant larvae are hypersensitive to touch. We added these data to Figure 11. We moved the data on adult body size and feeding behavior to Figure 11 —figure supplement 1A-C. However, we kept the data on adult anxiety-like behavior in the main Figures 11 and 12. We believe these results will be of interest to the community and potentially useful as a new model to study stress response behaviors and circuits. Further explanation of our reasoning for keeping these data in the paper is in the response to Reviewer 2, point 8 and Reviewer 3, point 4.

4) As described in detail by reviewer 2 (see below), multiple concerns about statistical analyses were raised and need to be addressed.

We address each concern in the response to Reviewer 2, point 5.

Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

Overall, the manuscript is very well written and the conclusions are well supported by the data. The light sheet and swept confocal microscopy movies provide a unique view of sensory axon development in vivo and are beautiful! Below are a few questions / comments that I had while reading the manuscript:

1) The data in figures 3 and 5 show nicely that axon branches are not stabilized and that axonal growth is shifted posteriorly in klc4 mutants. Is the collapse of branches direction-dependent in the mutant? Is there a difference in branch stabilization/pruning depending on whether the branch elongates anteriorly or posteriorly?

We scored the directionality of the axon branches that failed to stabilize and did not find a significant difference in the percent that were anteriorly or posteriorly directed. We have added this result to the text of the Results.

2) The presence of apical protrusions crossing the midline in klc4 mutants suggests a defect in RB neuron polarity. Is the centrosome mislocalized in klc4 mutants?

This is an interesting question. We previously showed a correlation between mislocalized centrosomes and apical protrusions with two other manipulations that lead to apical protrusions in RB neurons: inhibition of LIM homeodomain transcription factors and lamininA1 mutation (Andersen and Halloran, 2012, Development 139:3590-3599). We expect that we would also see apical mislocalization of the centrosome in klc4 mutants. However, because showing a third condition under which these two events are correlated would be a relatively incremental conceptual advance, and because centrosome imaging is one of several time-intensive experiments requested by reviewers, we focused our efforts on the experiments deemed to be essential revisions (imaging of cargo transport and microtubule dynamics during branching, and larval behavior experiments).

3) The existence of both Klc4 isoforms is interesting, especially since corresponding transcripts seem to be differently regulated during development. Since the short isoform is not predicted to bind any cargo, is anything known about its functions? Are both isoforms expressed in RB neurons?

We made a riboprobe that recognizes sequence found only in the long isoform. We found that the expression pattern of the long isoform is very similar to that of the riboprobe that binds both isoforms, indicating that the long isoform is likely present in all klc4-expressing neurons, including RB neurons. We added those data in Figure 1. The short isoform does not have unique sequence that isn’t present in the long isoform, so we are not able to make a short isoform specific riboprobe. To our knowledge, nothing is known about the function of the short isoform.

4) It would be interesting to analyze the localization of Klc4 in sensory neurons, although this might be difficult to do. Would it be possible to mosaically express low levels of tagged klc4 in mutant sensor neurons and analyze its distribution?

We tried using expression of KLC4-GFP fusion constructs, but expression leads to diffuse labeling throughout the cytoplasm. This problem has been previously reported by others attempting to visualize kinesin-1 heavy chains (Yang et al. 2019, Traffic 20:851-866). Because the vast majority of kinesin light chains and heavy chains exist in an auto-inhibited conformation that is freely diffusible, the diffuse signal resulting from expression of tagged proteins obscures visualization of motors bound to cargos. Thus, we are unfortunately not able to image KLC4 protein localization with current available approaches.

Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

The authors use Crisper technology to generate a zebrafish line that lacks expression of KLC4. This animal is then used throughout, along with pharmacological inhibition of KLC binding to adaptor proteins, to address the roles of KLC4 in the development of the morphology of Rohon-Beard (RB) sensory neurons in embryos and assess behavioral alterations in adult fish. Modern imaging methods are applied to study neuronal morphology in embryos and a battery of behavioral tests in adults. The study provides first line of inquiry assessment of the role of KLC4 in the development of RB morphology, focusing on the failure to establish proper axon arbors in the periphery. While each data set has intrinsic value and is novel, none is followed up to provide mechanistic insights into KLC4 functions in neurons. The value of the submission rests in providing a characterization of the effects of KLC4 knockout, but it does not further the understanding of the mechanistic contributions of KLC4 in the processes impacted by the genetic deletion of KLC4. While the significance of the question posed is well presented and clear, multiple issues regarding interpretation, presentation and analysis decrease enthusiasm for the submission in its current form.

For the authors' consideration:

1. The current in situ data uses a riboprobe that does not differentiate between the short and long forms of the KLC4 transcript. Given the large difference between short and long transcripts of KLC4, and expected differences in function of the resultant proteins, it would assist in the interpretation of the results to assess short and long transcript expression in RB neurons. Ideally, this would be followed up by specific knock down of the long form to dissect its contributions from the short form.

We made a riboprobe that recognizes sequence found only in the long isoform. We found that the expression pattern of the long isoform is very similar to that of the riboprobe that binds both isoforms, indicating that the long isoform is likely present in all klc4-expressing neurons, including RB neurons. We added those data in Figure 1. The short isoform does not have unique sequence that isn’t present in the long isoform, so we are not able to make a short isoform specific riboprobe. We also do not have a means to specifically knock down the short isoform. Splice blocking morpholinos could potentially be used to knock down the long isoform, but given the issues with morpholinos and interpretation of morpholino phenotypes, this would not be a definitive experiment.

2. At first evaluation, it is not clear how the axon branching metric (presented in Supplemental Figure 2) is specific for axon branching. In the images provided there seem to be consistently fewer axons present as a function of distance from the central fasicle in the KLC mutant and kinesore treated embryos (e.g., Figure 2A,B,D,E,H,I; Figure 4A,B). How can the metric dissociate the number of main axons that arrived at the ROI from branches made from those axons?

The metric does not distinguish between primary peripheral axons and those arising from secondary, tertiary or higher order branches. Most RB peripheral axons form as a branch from the central axon close to the cell body, and often this primary branch bifurcates shortly after spinal cord exit. Thus, all the axon segments in the periphery should be considered branches. We modified our language in the manuscript to avoid use of the term “main axon” because there is no clear definition of this term.

Based on the high quality of the imaging, it seems that it would be possible to use morphological criteria to define axon branches and measure the density along individual axons. This form of assessment would be much more stringent than the ROI crossing method used. Indeed, this form of assessment is used in the live imaging studies addressing rates or branch formation and stability.

To get more detailed information about axon density, we increased our image sampling by using a grid ROI instead of a single line ROI. This analysis approach provides information about axon density as a function of dorsal-ventral location (as suggested by the reviewer in point 3 below). This approach also provides information about both growth cone bifurcation and interstitial branching. We added the new data to Figure 2F, G, and Figure 2 —figure supplement 1.

3. Considering the above comment (2) regarding the consistent observable decrease in the apparent number of axons and their lengths into the periphery noted above, it is surprising that an increase in the rate of axon extension was detected in the live imaging analysis (Figure 3E). It is suggested that the authors analyze the density of axons and their total length as a function of distance from the central axon fascicle, noting that the ROI based approach in supplemental Figure 2 should serve this purpose by simply changing the distance from the fascicle into the periphery at constant intervals (currently only 65 microns was addressed).

This is an excellent suggestion, and we now added this additional, multiple ROI analysis.

Based on the images presented I would expect that a decrease in axon density and length will be determined in the KLC4 mutant. If so, then these data would stand in conflict with the observed increase rate of axon extension, but may indicate that earlier aspects of axon formation were also impacted in the KLC4 mutant. The formation of axons from the cell body may share mechanistic similarity to the emergence of branches, and thus may be delayed in the mutant due to failure to stabilize the new axon at its early stages of formation which could then result in the difference suggested by the images provided without being confounded by the subsequent increase in axon extension rate. It would also be insightful to perform this analysis at multiple points in ontogenesis.

There are fewer axons present in the mutants because there is less branching. In both wild type and mutants, axons that are shorter at one snapshot in time likely formed more recently than the longer axons. Live imaging is required to make conclusions about extension rate, and our live imaging showed that in the mutants, the axons that do form grow faster than wild type axons. We previously showed that the peripheral axons most often arise as a branch off the central axon (Andersen et al. 2011, Neural Dev 6:27), and we do believe that process shares mechanistic similarity to the formation of subsequent branches. Interestingly, the grid ROI analysis showed fewer branches in the mutants at all dorsal-ventral levels, indicating that the initial formation of peripheral axons may also be affected in the mutants, as the reviewer suggests. We added discussion of our interpretation of these results to the manuscript.

4. An interpretational caveat is that with both the genetic mutant and the drug treatment all cells are expected to be affected. From the in situ is it not clear to this reviewer if there is no expression of KLC4 in the environment the axons/branches are migrating through. Might this issue be resolved using zebrafish genetics with lines allowing for specific knockdown in RB neurons? or some other approach to address whether the effects are cell intrinsic.

We believe the effects in the mutant are likely cell autonomous. While it may be difficult to see from images captured at one focal plane, we can clearly distinguish the different tissues when looking through the microscope. We see no klc4 expression in the somites or the skin cells, which are the tissues that RB peripheral axons contact while growing. In addition, the RB neurons are the predominant cell type in the dorsal spinal cord expressing klc4 at the stages we see the axon phenotypes.

5. Statistical analysis. There are multiple concerns regarding the statistical presentation and analysis.

The section in the methods requires much elaboration, starting with which test was used to determine the normalcy of data sets, and addressed by subsequent comments.

On every analyzed dataset tests of normality were run before a method to test significant differences was decided. We used Prism’s suite of normality tests, which include Anderson-Darling test, D’Agostino and Pearson test, and Shapiro-Wilk test. In nearly all cases these tests were in agreement about whether the data were normal or not normal. Occasionally, one test of the three would suggest data were non-normal when the other two tests would count them as normal. In these cases, the data were always considered to be non-normal. We have elaborated this process in the revised Methods section.

Figures4 and 10 require statistical analysis of the data. "Plots using 95% confidence intervals (CI) were considered to be significant when the 95% CI ranges did not overlap." Is not considered a stringent analytical method, although a valid "rule of thumb". The authors are urged to seek assistance from a statistician.

We consulted with a statistician, who advised us to calculate the area under the curve. For Figure 4, in the portion of the graph where the 95% confidence intervals do not overlap (posterior angles -20 to -45, area shaded in Figure 4D), we calculated the area under the curve, which gives us a mean and standard error of the mean and allows a t-test to be run on the data. The t-test showed that the difference in area under the curve was significant (p=0.0047). In Figure 12 (formerly Figure 10), we calculated the area under the curve in panels B-F and found all to be significant, with p values p=0.0005 or p<0.0001. We added these analyses to the Methods section and figure legends.

6E, no 95% CI shown as claimed in the legend. The mean is not likely to be significant as the WT only have 3 points and one is an outlier relative to the other 2. I also do not see how the mean in the +/+ could be as shown, as claimed in the legend, it seems to be the median. I would estimate the mean to be somewhere around 20 um. For the mutant data, the distribution does not strike me as normal. Were these data analyzed for normality? Also, there is no statistical analysis shown, appreciating that with an n=3 for the WT this is problematic and power would have to be increased through greater sampling.

We regret the inaccuracy of the legend which likely occurred during changes to that figure. The figure has been updated to show the mean with standard error of the mean. As the reviewer noted, these data were not analyzed for significance because of the paucity of fasciculation events in wildtype (all events are shown). We did not report the normality test because we were not running a statistical test on the data. However, a normality test showed that mutant distribution was normal by Anderson-Darling, D’Agostino and Pearson, and Shapiro-Wilk. Normality could not be assessed for wild type because of the small number of fasciculation events. Our statistical consultant confirmed that it would not be appropriate to run a statistical test on the data because the events are so rare in wild type. They advised us to note in the legend that length measurements are conditional on event occurring, which we have done. We believe that reporting these measurements is still informative to readers even if they can’t be tested for statistical significance.

The legend to Figure 8 states that t-test with Mann-Whitney correction are used. I am not familiar with this correction for a t-test and was not able to find references to such a correction for t-test. Was Welch correction intended? The statistical description in the method is very sparse as presented and details should be presented. How the basic assumptions of the rather limited t-test were met by data prior to selection of specific t-test should be clearly denoted.

We apologize for the inaccurate description. The Mann Whitney test is not a corrected t-test, but rather an alternative to the t-test for non-normal data. This test is the same as the Wilcoxon rank sum test. This does not change the results, as the Mann-Whitney test is still an appropriate test for these data. We added more description to the statistical section of the Methods. Before selection of a test, the data were tested for normality by the previously described methods. In most cases, data were not normally distributed in all samples, leading to use of the Mann-Whitney test. In cases where data were determined to be normal, a standard t-test was used. Data tested by Mann-Whitney or t-tests were independent of each other and comprised ordinal or continuous numbers. Data tested by t-test were confirmed to have no difference in variance by performing an F test to compare variances.

Figure 9. In panel C, a t-test is used but (1) the data are bounded values, and (2) the distribution in WT is clearly non normal with many data points at the boundary (i.e., 100%). A t-test is not applicable as basic assumptions are broken, and do not appear to have been determined. E and H have no p values or test shown in legend and it is not clear why individual data points are not shown here as in other panels. F and G have no test denoted. The meaning of * and multiples thereof is only defined for one panel. Panel G has *** not defined at all.

We regret that the legend for Figure 9, Panel C (now Figure 11 —figure supplement 1C) was incorrect as a Mann-Whitney test was performed and not a t-test. The Mann-Whitney test accepts ordinal values, which should include bounded variables. The Mann-Whitney test is also equivalent to the Wilcoxon rank sum test. To be thorough, we also performed a Kruskal-Wallis test, which also works on independent data that are non-normal and ordinal or continuous. We found that the Kruskal-Wallis test agreed with the Mann-Whitney test and found that there was a significant difference in the number of larvae eaten between wildtype and mutant fish, with p<0.0001.

For E and H (now Figure 11 E, H), these graphs represent 3 individual experiments. Multiple animals were tested in each trial, but the N is the number of individual behavioral trial days. N=3 is too low to test for normality, therefore it was not appropriate to perform a statistical analysis on the data. The Mann-Whitney test (used for non-normal data) fails with an N this low—if the N is less than 7, the Mann-Whitney will always give a p value greater than 0.05 no matter how much the groups differ. We were able to test significance of the total time spent in the upper zone (Figure 11G) and the time spent freezing (Figure 11I). We believe it is informative to also report the percent fish that entered the upper zone in each experiment (Figure 11E) and the percent that froze (Figure 11H), even though these can’t be tested for significance. We have updated the data display to show individual points for each experiment, along with mean and standard error of the mean. We have updated the legends to include all statistical test descriptions and p values.

6. Strictly speaking, the authors are inferring contact repulsion but do not use the imaging data sets to directly address it (i.e., through direct analysis of the consequences of contacts between the two sister branches after growth cone bifurcation). The observed changes in angles between sister branches could be due to a variety of other mechanisms altering the ability of sister branches to generate larger angles.

The reviewer’s point is well taken. We have modified our language in the text accordingly.

The self-contact examples in Figure 6 are not particularly compelling. Perhaps, presentation of all relevant frames in the timelapse with greater annotation and description would assist a reader.

We updated the images in Figure 6 to show a clearer example of self-contact. Space constraints prohibit including all relevant frames in the timelapse, but they can be viewed in the accompanying video.

In Figure 6B, in contrast to what the text claims, this reviewer does not see a clear growth cone bifurcation followed by fasiculation due to lack of self-repulsion. It appears a small back branch from the second panel from the left aligns with the main axon.

We have updated the images in Figure 6B to more clearly show branches collapsing onto each other.

Also, the "appearing to collapse onto one another" (line 232) could be interpreted as a consequence of the prior analysis of branch stability.

When following the dynamic axon behaviors in the movies, we can distinguish between branches that have retracted versus those that fasciculate onto other axons.

7. Figure 8D,E. "Comet Frequency" in comets/um/min. It is not clear if this reflects new comets observed during the imaging or all observed comets/um/min.

These data include all observed comets. We clarified this point in the figure legend (now Figure 9).

The imaging sets would benefit from additional analysis to extract the duration of EB3 comets (i.e., summed period spent polymerizing per comet) and total distance from initiation to loss of the comet.

We now include data on both distance (in microns) and duration (in seconds) of comet runs in Figure 9 —figure supplement 1C-F, and describe the results and interpretation in the Results section.

It is not clear why central axons are analyzed for only these data sets and not others. As the focus of the paper is on peripheral axons the suggested additional data sets for peripheral axons would suffice.

Because the focus is on peripheral axons, we moved the data on EB3-GFP comets in central axons to Figure 9 —figure supplement 1.

As a whole, I would suggest that the data in Figure 8 from EB3 imaging indicate increased polymerization of plus tips along axons in the mutant. This is not immediately reconcilable with decreases branch stability as one would presume that microtubules would be more prone to catastrophe and depolymerization in conjunction with decreased branch stability. The data are broadly consistent with the detected increase in axon extension rate. However, analysis of EB3 comets needs to be provided in branches (collateral and bifurcated) in order to be linked to the main observed phenotype of decreased branch stability.

We used two approaches to analyze microtubules in nascent branches (new Figure 10). First, we used EB3-GFP to image microtubule polymerization into nascent branches. Although comet frequency in established axons is increased in klc4 mutants, there is no difference in frequency of comets entering nascent branches, suggesting a relative decrease. Second, we used anti-acetylated tubulin labeling to show that nascent branches in klc4 mutants are less likely to have stable, acetylated tubulin. We added further interpretation of these data to the Discussion section.

8. While of general interest, the behavioral assays do not address RB neuron function or circuitry, which is embryonic. The fit of these data to the rest of the study is not clear. Could tail startle be assessed (Liu and Hale (2017) Curr Biol 27:697-704) for addressing RB/RB-related circuit function? Any RB function assays would provide a much better fit to the rest of the data presented.

Importantly, these behavioral deficits cannot be attributed to changes in circuit formation, branching, axon extension etc as the mutant is not neuron specific, the relevant circuitry has not been analyzed, and no aspect of CNS neurons or glia or anything else has been analyzed. The concern is compounded by the differences in overall growth of the fish.

We added new experiments testing the larval touch response at 3 dpf, a behavior that is mediated by sensory neurons. We found that klc4 mutant larvae showed increased number of swim bouts and longer duration swim runs after a touch stimulus, suggesting the mutant larvae are hypersensitive to touch. We added these data to Figure 10. We moved the data on adult body size and feeding behavior to Supplemental Figure 10. However, we kept the data on adult anxiety-like behavior in the main Figures 10 and 11. It is true that these behaviors cannot be linked directly to cell biological defects in RB neurons. We are not claiming a direct link. Instead we are making use of two separate approaches to address questions about KLC4 function: (1) We take advantage of the superficial position of the RB neurons, which makes them ideal for imaging, and their distinctly polarized morphology to uncover cell biological processes influenced by KLC4. (2) We use adult behavioral analysis to more broadly assess roles of KLC4 in other neural circuits. We believe that publication of the adult behavior phenotypes will benefit the scientific community and be useful as a new model to study stress response behaviors and circuits.

9. One fundamental aspect of the study that is missing is consideration of KLC4 localization in the RB neurons. Whether it localizes to the RB axons remains unknown, or whether it ever targets to branches. One would presume so, but this needs to be assessed. It is hard to interpret results with any degree of mechanistic insight without knowledge of the localization of the molecule addressed.

As mentioned in response to reviewer 1, we tried using expression of KLC4-GFP fusion constructs, but expression leads to diffuse labeling throughout the cytoplasm. This problem has been previously reported by others attempting to visualize kinesin-1 heavy chains (Yang et al. 2019, Traffic 20:851-866). Because the vast majority of kinesin light chains and heavy chains exist in an auto-inhibited conformation that is freely diffusible, the diffuse signal resulting from expression of tagged proteins obscures visualization of motors bound to cargos. Thus, we are unfortunately not able to image KLC4 protein localization with current available approaches.

10. Final considerations: Overall, this submission contains interesting and valuable data sets that are however preliminary in nature as none is followed up to achieve a deeper mechanistic understanding. This manuscript presents a characterization of phenotypes in KLC4 mutant zebrafish RB neurons but does not provide insights into how KLC4 is mechanistically contributing to the observed phenotypes, or whether it is acting cell intrinsically. Having said this, the discussion carefully considers the potential implications of the individual findings and notes relevant future directions.

Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

Further characterization of axonal defects:

– While the population-based methods for characterizing axonal defects are elegant and informative, these defects could potentially be further appreciated with single cell resolution, What do the arbors of individual neurons look like in these mutants? Does the mutant affect all sensory neurons equally, or is a sub-population of neurons more severely affected? It should be possible to analyze single neurons with transient transgenesis in the zebrafish model. This could be particularly helpful since aberrant fasciculation may make it difficult to distinguish multiple fasciculated axon branches when the whole population of neurons is labeled. Identifying defects at a single neuron level could also make it possible to carry out rescue experiments to address the cell autonomy of axon arborization defects.

We did analyze individual neurons labeled with transient transgenesis for the Rab5 and microtubule imaging experiments, and the single-cell labeling in Figure 7 to follow the trajectory of apical processes. As the reviewer points out, this is important because fasciculation of multiple axons would confound results. We used stable transgenics for the live imaging of axon growth, fasciculation and branch dynamics. This was absolutely necessary for the fasciculation analysis because all axons must be labeled to detect non-self-fasciculations. We are able to do the growth rate and branch dynamics analyses on individual neurons using the stable transgenics because we can easily resolve individual neurons by following videos of axons growing over time.

Explanation of axonal defects mechanistically:

– The impact of this paper would be most enhanced by characterizing the axonal trafficking of cellular cargos in klc4 mutants, particularly if defects in specific trafficked cargoes could plausibly explain the axonal defects. In the discussion of the paper, the authors note that they have previously found that endosomal trafficking regulates branching. Characterizing endosomal trafficking, as well as trafficking of mitochondria, lysosomes, golgi, etc, thus has the potential to make this study substantially more mechanistically satisfying.

We added new experiments to image endosomal dynamics (new Figure 8), trafficking of Rab5-labeled endosomes from the cell body to branch points (new Figure 8 —figure supplement 1), microtubule behavior during axon branching (new Figure 10), and acetylation of microtubules in nascent branches (new Figure 10). We chose Rab5-labeled endosomes because of our previous work showing a correlation between their transport and RB axon branching. Although the klc4 mutants did not have a decrease in endosome delivery to branch points, high-speed imaging of vesicle dynamics revealed some alterations in transport of these vesicles in klc4 mutants. These alterations in endosome transport are not likely to be the only effects of klc4 mutation on cargo dynamics or to fully explain the mechanism by which KLC promotes branch stabilization. However, an exhaustive analysis of all potential cargos would require many months to years of work and is outside the scope of this manuscript.

– The changes in microtubule dynamics of klc4 mutants are interesting, but how trafficking defects affect these dynamics, and how defects in MT dynamics affect branching, is unclear. To address how trafficking affects MTs, the authors could attempt to identify MTOCs in axons. For example, since golgi outposts are thought to be MTOCs, is their trafficking into axons affected by klc4 mutation? To address how MT defects affect branching, the authors could characterize MT dynamics during branching-for example, do mutants display any defects in the frequency of MT invasion into nascent branches?

We have tried to detect MTOCs using an antibody to γ tubulin, but that labeling in whole embryos has a very low signal to noise. It was not possible for us to detect signal above noise. We added new experiments analyzing microtubules in nascent branches (new Figure 10). We used live imaging with EB3-GFP to measure frequency of comets entering nascent branches. Although comet frequency in established axons is increased in klc4 mutants, there is no difference in frequency of comets entering nascent branches, suggesting a relative decrease. We used anti-acetylated tubulin labeling to show that klc4 mutants have fewer nascent branches with stable, acetylated tubulin.

Behavior:

– As mentioned above, the analyses of adult behavioral defects, while interesting and rigorously carried out, do not complement the morphological analysis in larval sensory neurons. It would be exciting if axonal arborization defects in a specific neuron type could be linked to behavioral defects. Could the authors attempt to characterize touch-response behaviors in larval fish? This would be a particularly exciting if it were possible to do cell-autonomous rescue experiments, thus directly testing if defects in sensory neuron morphology affect behavior.

This is an excellent suggestion and we have now added larval touch response assays (Figure 11). We found that klc4 mutant larvae showed increased number of swim bouts and longer duration swim runs after a touch stimulus, suggesting the mutant larvae are hypersensitive to touch. We added these data to Figure 11. We moved the data on adult body size and feeding behavior to Figure 11 —figure supplement 1. However, we kept the data on adult anxiety-like behavior in the main Figures 11 and 12. We believe these results will be of interest to the community and potentially useful as a new model to study stress response behaviors and circuits.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Figure 1—source data 1. Original.tiff and.scn file for the gel of RT-PCR results showing presence of klc4 isoforms.
    Figure 2—source data 1. The number of branches crossing a selected region of interest (1-8) for wild type and klc4uw314 mutant embryos.
    Figure 2—source data 2. The number of branches crossing a selected region of interest (1-8) for wild type embryos treated with either 2% DMSO or 100 µM Kinesore.
    Figure 3—source data 1. The number of branches initiated per neuron per hour separated by type of branch (growth cone bifurcation = GCB, Interstitial Branch = IB).
    Figure 3—source data 2. The number of branches of a category (growth cone bifurcation = GCB, Interstitial Branch = IB) that retracted divided by the total number of branching events of that category.
    Figure 3—source data 3. The growth speed of wild type and klc4uw314 mutant embryos.
    Figure 4—source data 1. Histogram of the axon signal at each angle for wild type and klc4uw314 mutant axons, used to generate directional distribution plots.
    Figure 4—source data 2. Histogram data in Source Data 1 that has been normalized to the total axon signal present in the sample.

    Presented as a percentage of total signal.

    Figure 5—source data 1. The rose plot data generated with the Chemotaxis Tool for wild type axon tracking data.
    Figure 5—source data 2. The rose plot data generated with the Chemotaxis Tool for klc4uw314 mutant axon tracking data.
    Figure 5—source data 3. The angle between sister branches, measured at 15 min after branch formation and 75 min after branch formation.

    If a branch retracted or the 75-min time point fell out of recorded range, no data was recorded for the 75-min point.

    Figure 6—source data 1. Fasciculations per neuron, separated as self or non-self, and combined (all).
    Figure 6—source data 2. Fasciculation length measured in microns.
    Figure 7—source data 1. The percentage of neurons per embryo that had an apical protrusion that fully crossed the midline of the spinal cord.
    Figure 8—source data 1. The percentage of motile vesicles per cell that were traveling anterogradely in peripheral RB axon branches.
    Figure 8—source data 2. The distance traveled by Rab5 comets, including both average and maximum run length.
    Figure 8—source data 3. The measured velocity of Rab5 comets in peripheral axons.
    Figure 9—source data 1. Source Data 1: All measurements of EB3-GFP comets in wild type and klc4uw314 mutants, averaged per measured segment.
    Figure 10—source data 1. Source Data 1: The EB3 comet frequency in nascent axons.
    Figure 10—source data 2. The percentage of nascent branches with discernable acetylated tubulin signal out of all nascent branches observed.
    Figure 11—source data 1. The length of time each individual larva spent swimming after a touch stimulus (non-responding fish removed from graph).
    Figure 11—source data 2. The number of swim bouts performed by each individual larva after a touch stimulus (non-responding fish removed from graph).
    Figure 11—source data 3. The average percentage of fish that entered the upper zone of the tank per experimental trial.
    Figure 11—source data 4. The time it took an individual fish to first cross into the upper zone of the tank.

    Only individuals that crossed into the upper zone are represented in this dataset.

    Figure 11—source data 5. The total time spent by each individual fish in the upper zone throughout the experimental time period.
    Figure 11—source data 6. The average percentage of fish that performed one or more freezing bouts per experimental trial.
    Figure 11—source data 7. The total time spent frozen by each individual fish throughout the experimental time period.
    Figure 12—source data 1. The percentage of fish per group that froze during the first 3 min of exposure 1 of the habituation series.
    Figure 12—source data 2. The percentage of fish per group that froze for each exposure in the habituation series.
    Figure 12—source data 3. The total time spent frozen by each individual fish throughout the experimental time period for each exposure in the habituation series.
    Figure 12—source data 4. The mobile and total swim velocities for each individual fish throughout the experimental time period for each exposure in the habituation series.
    Figure 12—source data 5. The percentage of fish per group that entered the upper zone of the tank at least once during each exposure in the habituation series.
    Figure 12—source data 6. The time in seconds it took each individual fish to initially cross over into the upper zone of the tank for each exposure in the habituation series.
    Transparent reporting form

    Data Availability Statement

    All data generated or analyzed in this study are included in the manuscript and supporting files. Source Data files have been provided for Figures 1-12.


    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES