Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2022 Oct 13;11:e81071. doi: 10.7554/eLife.81071

Postsynaptic burst reactivation of hippocampal neurons enables associative plasticity of temporally discontiguous inputs

Tanja Fuchsberger 1, Claudia Clopath 2,, Przemyslaw Jarzebowski 1,, Zuzanna Brzosko 1, Hongbing Wang 3, Ole Paulsen 1,
Editors: Marco Capogna4, Laura L Colgin5
PMCID: PMC9612916  PMID: 36226826

Abstract

A fundamental unresolved problem in neuroscience is how the brain associates in memory events that are separated in time. Here, we propose that reactivation-induced synaptic plasticity can solve this problem. Previously, we reported that the reinforcement signal dopamine converts hippocampal spike timing-dependent depression into potentiation during continued synaptic activity (Brzosko et al., 2015). Here, we report that postsynaptic bursts in the presence of dopamine produce input-specific LTP in mouse hippocampal synapses 10 min after they were primed with coincident pre- and post-synaptic activity (post-before-pre pairing; Δt = –20 ms). This priming activity induces synaptic depression and sets an NMDA receptor-dependent silent eligibility trace which, through the cAMP-PKA cascade, is rapidly converted into protein synthesis-dependent synaptic potentiation, mediated by a signaling pathway distinct from that of conventional LTP. This synaptic learning rule was incorporated into a computational model, and we found that it adds specificity to reinforcement learning by controlling memory allocation and enabling both ‘instructive’ and ‘supervised’ reinforcement learning. We predicted that this mechanism would make reactivated neurons activate more strongly and carry more spatial information than non-reactivated cells, which was confirmed in freely moving mice performing a reward-based navigation task.

Research organism: Mouse

Introduction

For an animal to successfully adjust its behavior to changing environmental demands, it needs to learn to associate a sequence of events or actions to a subsequent outcome, for example a reward, which may be experienced minutes or hours later. How this is achieved in the brain remains unknown. Even the longest timescale synaptic plasticity event reported so far, behavioral timescale synaptic plasticity (BTSP; Bittner et al., 2017; Milstein et al., 2021), cannot bridge this temporal gap. In machine learning, reinforcement learning algorithms solve this problem by storing a temporary record of the occurrence of an event, known as an eligibility trace, which can later undergo learning changes once the outcome is known (Sutton and Barto, 2018). In biology, dopamine (DA) is thought to represent such a reinforcement learning signal, converting a temporary eligibility trace into a lasting synaptic change (Ljungberg et al., 1992; Schultz et al., 1997; Frémaux and Gerstner, 2016; Gerstner et al., 2018). However, the use of a brief scalar signal to alter the relevant synaptic weights raises two fundamental problems, first, that of time scales, and second, that of specificity (or credit assignment). Whereas initial Pavlovian conditioning of reward-predicting stimuli has optimal stimulus-reward intervals of 1.5–3.0 s, other learning mechanisms occur in the second, minute, and even hour ranges (Schultz, 2007). Moreover, with a longer time delay, the relationship between the preceding behavior and the outcome is less clear, questioning what events or actions before the reward should be associated with the outcome. One possible mechanism that could link previous activity (e.g. exploration of an environment) with a specific outcome (e.g. finding a reward) is neuronal reactivation, or replay activity, which in the hippocampus is enhanced by reward (Singer and Frank, 2009; Ambrose et al., 2016).

The rodent hippocampus is important for spatial memories. Spatial representations are built during exploration of an environment, when the hippocampus shows theta activity, and is later reactivated or replayed both during sleep (Wilson and McNaughton, 1994; Nádasdy et al., 1999; Lee and Wilson, 2002) and in the awake state (Kudrimoti et al., 1999; Foster and Wilson, 2006; Csicsvari et al., 2007; Diba and Buzsáki, 2007). Reactivation occurs during sharp wave ripples (SWRs), when neurons typically fire action potentials in brief bursts (Buzsáki et al., 1992; Diba and Buzsáki, 2007). It has been reported that stimulation of dopaminergic input promotes reactivation of hippocampal cell assemblies and memory persistence (McNamara et al., 2014).

We investigated the effect of action potential bursts on synaptic plasticity in individual postsynaptic hippocampal CA1 neurons (SWR-associated ‘reactivation’) during dopaminergic modulation (‘reward signal’) after they had undergone a spike pairing protocol (prior exploration-based synaptic ‘priming’). We found that the pairing protocol set an NMDA receptor-dependent silent eligibility trace, which could be converted several minutes later by burst activity in the presence of DA into protein synthesis-dependent long-term potentiation (LTP) mediated by a signaling pathway distinct from that of conventional LTP. Using this synaptic learning rule in a computational model we show that reactivation-induced plasticity increases specificity to reinforcement learning, offering a candidate mechanism of credit assignment in neural networks. To investigate how reactivation affects the functional properties of neurons in vivo, we used chronic calcium imaging of the dorsal CA1 region of the hippocampus in freely moving mice performing a reward-location learning task. We defined neuronal reactivation as activity during immobility at the reward-location in neurons that were already previously active during the reward approach. Neurons that reactivated after finding the reward had increased calcium activity and place map peaks compared to non-reactivated neurons, suggestive of changes in synaptic weights in reactivated neurons.

Results

Reactivation during dopaminergic modulation induces LTP

To investigate the effect of burst reactivation of individual postsynaptic CA1 cells during dopaminergic modulation, we monitored the synaptic weights of afferent synapses that had previously undergone a spike timing-dependent synaptic priming protocol. For this, we used whole-cell recording of CA1 pyramidal neurons in mouse hippocampal slices (Figure 1A). To be able to distinguish between conventional LTP and reactivation-induced LTP, we used a priming protocol that induces synaptic depression (Andrade-Talavera et al., 2016). Single postsynaptic action potentials followed by presynaptic input stimulation (post-before-pre protocol, Δt = –20 ms) led to input-specific synaptic depression in the test pathway (t-LTD; 61% ± 11% vs 100%, t(9) = 3.7, p=0.005, n=10; Figure 1B and F). Application of DA alone, without resuming synaptic stimulation after this pairing protocol, did not affect the depression (53% ± 7% vs 100%, t(5) = 7.0, p=0.0009, n=6; Figure 1C and F blue trace). Strikingly, postsynaptic action potential bursts (5–6 APs) in the presence of DA, 10 min after the pairing protocol, triggered an immediate induction of synaptic potentiation (135% ± 14.9% vs 100%, t(8) = 2.4, p=0.044, n=9; Figure 1D and F red trace). Burst stimulation alone, in the absence of DA, did not prevent synaptic depression (72% ± 12% vs 100%, t(6) = 2.5, p=0.048, n=7; Figure 1E and F black trace). This result suggests that the pairing protocol sets an eligibility trace allowing activated synapses to be selectively altered minutes later by reactivation of the postsynaptic neuron in the presence of DA. To our knowledge, these are the longest-lasting synaptic eligibility traces reported in the brain.

Figure 1. Postsynaptic burst reactivation induces LTP in the presence of dopamine (DA).

Figure 1.

(A), Schematic of the experimental paradigm (top) and setup (bottom).=6 action potential bursts; Whole-cell recording in CA1 stratum pyramidale; electrical stimulation electrodes in stratum radiatum. Plasticity was induced in one pathway (Paired pathway), and a second pathway was used for stability control and for confirmation of input specificity (Control pathway). Normalized EPSP slopes were averaged and plotted as a function of time. (B), Post-before pre-pairing protocol leads to input-specific synaptic depression. Pairing protocol (Δt = –20 ms) induces t-LTD (black trace) and does not affect synaptic weights in control pathway (gray trace). (C), DA application after a post-before-pre pairing protocol (Δt = –20 ms) does not prevent t-LTD (+DA –Burst, blue trace) and does not affect synaptic weights in control pathway (gray trace). (D), Application of DA together with action potential bursts of the postsynaptic cell (indicated by black arrow) induces synaptic potentiation after a post-before-pre pairing protocol (Δt = –20 ms) (+DA +Burst, red trace) and does not affect synaptic weights in control pathway (gray trace). (E), The same protocol, without application of DA, leads to synaptic depression (–DA +Burst, black trace) and does not affect synaptic weights in control pathway (gray trace). (F), Summary of results. All traces show an EPSP before (1) and 40 min after pairing (2). Plots show averages of normalized EPSP slopes ± SEM.

Figure 1—source data 1. Normalized EPSP slopes of all recorded cells.

We next investigated the requirements for setting the eligibility trace. First, we found that synaptic potentiation was indeed not observed without prior spike pairing (93% ± 8% vs 100%, t(6) = 0.84, p=0.43, n=7; Figure 2A). Induction of hippocampal t-LTD requires metabotropic glutamate receptors (mGluRs; Andrade-Talavera et al., 2016). To investigate whether LTD or mGluR signaling is required for burst-induced potentiation, we applied the mGluR antagonist MPEP throughout the recording. This blocked t-LTD (94% ± 6% vs 100%, t(6) = 0.95, p=0.38, n=7; Figure 2B and C, blue; vs control t-LTD 69% ± 7%, n=7; t(11.5) = 2.67, p=0.02; Figure 2B and C, black) but burst-induced potentiation was intact (131% ± 10% vs 100%, t(6) = 3.07, p=0.021, n=7; Figure 2B and C, red), suggesting that setting the eligibility trace by spike pairing is distinct from the signaling mechanism that mediates t-LTD.

Figure 2. Setting of the eligibility trace is independent of LTD but requires postsynaptic NMDARs.

Figure 2.

(A), Application of DA and burst stimulation, but without pairing protocol, does not induce potentiation. (B), Post-before-pre pairing protocol (Δt = –20 ms) leads to synaptic depression (t-LTD) (black trace), which is blocked by MPEP (blue trace). MPEP does not block DA- and burst-induced potentiation (red trace). (C), Summary of results. (D), Application of AP5 during pairing blocks DA- and burst-induced potentiation. (E), Postsynaptic intracellular MK801 (iMK801) does not block t-LTD (blue trace) but blocks DA- and burst-induced potentiation (red trace). (F), Summary of results. All traces show an EPSP before (1) and 40 min after pairing (2). Plots show averages of normalized EPSP slopes ± SEM.

Figure 2—source data 1. Normalized EPSP slopes of all recorded cells.

Many forms of hippocampal plasticity require NMDA receptors (NMDARs; Shipton and Paulsen, 2014). We, therefore, asked if activation of NMDARs during the pairing protocol is required for the synaptic eligibility trace to be set. We found that application of the NMDAR antagonist d-AP5 during pairing (Δt = –20 ms) abolished both t-LTD and the subsequent burst-induced potentiation, resulting in no change of synaptic weights (100% ± 9% vs 100%, t(6) = 0.043, p=0.97, n=7; Figure 2D and F). We then investigated whether specifically postsynaptic NMDARs are required for the eligibility trace. Loading the postsynaptic cell with the NMDAR channel blocker MK801 through the recording pipette did not affect t-LTD (68% ± 7% vs 100%, t(5) = 4.77, p=0.005, n=6; Figure 2E and F) but completely abolished burst-induced potentiation, leaving synaptic depression instead (61% ± 10% vs 100%, t(6) = 3.85, p=0.0084, n=6; Figure 2E and F). These results show a double dissociation between t-LTD induced by mGluR and non-postsynaptic NMDAR signaling and the eligibility trace for reactivation-induced potentiation set by postsynaptic ionotropic NMDARs.

We next investigated the mechanism that induces synaptic potentiation during burst stimulation. It was previously shown that activation of NMDARs after pairing is necessary to induce DA-dependent potentiation with subthreshold synaptic stimulation (Brzosko et al., 2015). However, the NMDAR antagonist d-AP5, applied after the pairing protocol but before burst stimulation, did not affect burst-induced potentiation (127% ± 9%, t(5) = 2.94, p=0.032, n=6; Figure 3A and C). In contrast, voltage-gated calcium channels (VGCCs) are required as application of nimodipine, an L-type VGCC blocker, before the burst completely abolished LTP and left synaptic depression instead (72% ± 5% vs 100%, t(5) = 5.6, p=0.0026, n=6; Figure 3B and C) indicating that, as in some other forms of synaptic potentiation (Grover and Teyler, 1990), Ca2+ entry through VGCCs is required for burst reactivation to induce LTP.

Figure 3. Voltage gated calcium channels mediate DA and reactivation-induced plasticity during burst stimulation.

Figure 3.

Normalized EPSP slopes were averaged and plotted as a function of time. (A), Application of AP5 after pairing does not prevent DA- and burst-induced potentiation. (B), Application of nimodipine during the burst prevents DA- and burst-induced potentiation leaving synaptic depression. (C), Summary of results. All traces show an EPSP before (1) and 40 min after pairing (2). Plots show averages of normalized EPSP slopes ± SEM.

Figure 3—source data 1. Normalized EPSP slopes of all recorded cells.

Signaling pathway mediating reactivation-induced LTP

These findings suggest that coincident DA signaling and postsynaptic Ca2+ increase enable the potentiation of previously primed synapses. Searching for a potential coincidence detector for DA and intracellular Ca2+, we focused on adenylyl cyclases (ACs). They are activated by Gs-coupled dopamine D1/D5 receptor stimulation (Neve et al., 2004), and subtypes AC1 and AC8 are additionally Ca2+- stimulated (Wayman et al., 1994; Watson et al., 2000; Ferguson and Storm, 2004). To investigate whether AC subtypes AC1 and/or AC8 are involved in the form of plasticity described here, we tested the induction protocol in AC1/AC8 double knockout (AC DKO) mice (Wong et al., 1999). When postsynaptic burst stimulation in the presence of DA was applied after a negative pairing protocol (Δt = –20 ms; Figure 4Ai) in slices from AC DKO mice, the conversion to potentiation was absent and significantly different from DA- and burst-induced potentiation in slices from wildtype mice (AC DKO, 90% ± 8%, n=6 vs WT, 132% ± 11%, n=8; t(12) = 2.8, p=0.015; Figure 4Bi and Biii), revealing a role for AC1/AC8 as coincidence detector for DA- and Ca2+-induced potentiation. In contrast, conventional, DA-independent t-LTP induced by a pre-before-post pairing protocol (Δt = +10 ms; Figure 4Aii) showed significant potentiation comparable to that seen in wildtype mice (AC DKO, 150% ± 19% vs 100%, t(5) = 2.6, p=0.049, n=6; WT, 163% ± 14% vs 100%, t(9) = 4.4, p=0.0015, n=10; Figure 4Bii and Biii).

Figure 4. DA and reactivation-induced potentiation require AC1/AC8 and PKA.

Figure 4.

Schematics show the difference between the three induction protocols. Normalized EPSP slopes were averaged and plotted as a function of time. (Ai), DA and burst stimulation after a post-before-pre pairing protocol (Δt = –20 ms) induces potentiation. (Aii), A pre-before-post pairing protocol induces t-LTP (Δt = +10 ms). (B), AC DKO mice do not show DA-dependent plasticity with postsynaptic bursts (Bi) but shows conventional t-LTP (Bii). Summary of results (Biii). (C), Postsynaptic application of protein kinase inhibitor-(6-22)-amide (PKI) prevents DA-dependent plasticity with postsynaptic bursts (Ci) but leaves conventional t-LTP intact (Cii). Summary of results (Ciii). All traces show an EPSP before (1) and 40 minutes after pairing (2). Plots show averages of normalized EPSP slopes ± SEM.

Figure 4—source data 1. Normalized EPSP slopes of all recorded cells.

AC activation produces an increase in cyclic adenosine monophosphate (cAMP) which activates protein kinase A (PKA; Sassone-Corsi, 2012). To test whether this signaling cascade is required for reactivation-induced potentiation, we loaded the postsynaptic cell with a PKA blocker, protein kinase inhibitor-(6-22)-amide, through the recording pipette. In this configuration burst stimulation in the presence of DA after the post-before-pre protocol failed to induce synaptic potentiation (69% ± 13% vs 100%, t(5) = 2.4, p=0.064, n=6; Figure 4Ci and Ciii). In contrast, conventional pre-before-post pairing induced significant potentiation, albeit of somewhat reduced magnitude (137% ± 14% vs 100%, t(5) = 2.63, p=0.0463, n=6; Figure 4Cii and Ciii).

The requirement of DA and PKA for burst-induced potentiation is shared with late-phase LTP (Frey et al., 1990; Frey et al., 1993), which requires protein synthesis (Frey et al., 1988). We therefore investigated whether the burst-induced rapid potentiation also requires protein synthesis by delivering the protein synthesis inhibitor anisomycin (AM) to the postsynaptic cell through the recording pipette. We found that, with anisomycin, burst stimulation in the presence of DA no longer induced conversion to potentiation but left a synaptic depression instead, which was significantly different from vehicle control (AM, 52% ± 11% vs 100%, t(5) = 4.5, p=0.0062, n=6; Figure 5Ai and Aiii, red; vs vehicle, 128% ± 17%, n=5; t(9) = 3.9, p=0.0033; Figure 5Ai and Aiii, green). In contrast, conventional t-LTP induced by pre-before-post pairing was unaffected by anisomycin (161% ± 20% vs 100%, t(5) = 3.0, p=0.029, n=6; Figure 5Aii and Aiii, black). Furthermore, the post-before-pre pairing protocol induced t-LTD under these conditions (65% ± 6% vs 100%, t(5) = 5.9, p=0.0019, n=6; Figure 5Aii and Aiii, gray). We confirmed that postsynaptically applied anisomycin did not affect synaptic responses in baseline conditions (95% ± 9.7% vs 100%, t(6) = 0.56, p=0.59, n=7; Figure 5—figure supplement 1A). Furthermore, we compared action potential properties during pairing in cells with anisomycin to cells loaded with vehicle controls. Spike amplitude (AM 112 mV ± 3 mV, Vehicle 111 mV ± 3 mV) and spike width (AM 3.3 ms ± 0.2 ms, Vehicle 3.3 ms ± 0.2 ms) showed no significant differences (amplitude t(10) = 0.09050, p=0.92; width t(10) = 0.1134, p=0.91; Figure 5—figure supplement 1B, C).

Figure 5. DA and reactivation-induced plasticity requires protein synthesis.

(A), Postsynaptic anisomycin prevents DA-dependent plasticity with postsynaptic burst stimulation (Ai), but leaves conventional t-LTD (Aii, gray trace) and t-LTP (black trace) intact. Summary of results (Aiii). All traces show an EPSP before (1) and 40 min after (2) pairing. Plots show averages of normalized EPSP slopes ± SEM.

Figure 5—source data 1. Normalized EPSP slopes, spike amplitudes and spike width of all recorded cells.

Figure 5.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1. Postsynaptic anisomycin (AM) does not affect baseline synaptic responses and action potential properties.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1.

(A), Stable baseline in presence of postsynaptic anisomycin and summary of result. Traces show an EPSP at the start of the recording (1) and 60 min later (2). Plot shows averages of normalized EPSP slopes ± SEM. (B), No significant change in spike amplitude with anisomycin compared to vehicle control. (C), No significant change in spike width with anisomycin compared to vehicle control.

Burst-dependent plasticity increases specificity in reinforcement learning models

These experimental results show that, after a priming event, burst reactivation in the presence of DA induces a rapid form of protein synthesis-dependent LTP. This mechanism would ensure that only salient neuronal activity induces long-term changes in the network. We implemented this synaptic learning rule in a computational model to explore how such plasticity would control learning in a feedforward artificial neural network resembling hippocampal CA1. The learning rule states that the change in synaptic weights Δw between input and output neurons (inpo) depends on an eligibility trace e (set during the initial priming event), the reinforcement signal (dopamine d), and reactivation (bursting activity b).

winpo=αLTPedb (1)

The parameter αLTP is the learning rate. When there is no DA or bursts during the trial, the rule is updated to result in depression proportional to the eligibility trace with a learning rate αLTD (see Methods). When using a standard reinforcement learning (RL) rule, which does not depend on burst reactivation, all previously primed synapses are potentiated after receiving the DA signal (Figure 6A). Thus, the global neuromodulatory signal in traditional RL models provides limited information. In contrast, when applying the burst-dependent learning rule (Equation 1) to the network, in which potentiation of primed synapses depends on both the reward signal and reactivation, a selected subset of inputs becomes potentiated, while inputs on non-reactivated neurons remain depressed (gray) (Figure 6B). During DA modulation, information is allocated to primed synapses by reactivation of the postsynaptic neuron, and the broader computational implications of this learning rule depend on the control of postsynaptic neuronal bursting activity. First, it is possible that neurons are recruited to a new memory trace based on their relative neuronal excitability before the training session as suggested by the memory allocation hypothesis (Yiu et al., 2014). According to this scenario, the most excitable cells would be the most likely to show action potential bursts during reactivation and, therefore, show synaptic potentiation. Alternatively, there is evidence for replay of prioritized experience (Igata et al., 2021), suggesting that cells encoding the most salient events preceding the reward would reactivate, and thereby determine which set of cells would show potentiation during reward (Figure 6B). Assuming prioritized experience reflects experience relevant to the reward, this could help credit assignment in the network. In addition, because of the exclusive requirement of postsynaptic activity for potentiation to occur, this mechanism offers another intriguing possibility, namely that other inputs active at the reward location carries additional information about the nature of the reward, e.g., food, or the reward location, such as the presence of specific landmarks, which could elicit postsynaptic bursting activity. Under this hypothesis, neuronal reactivation would serve to associate a specific outcome to the priming event. When different instructive inputs induce bursting each in distinct subsets of neurons during reward, selective increases in synaptic weights would not only enable the encoding of reward but also distinguish between different rewards (Figure 7A). Finally, we explore how the learning rule performs in a network supervised by feedback synaptic input to strengthen synapses onto specific neurons encoding reward-related features. By allowing feedback input to assign which part of the network is responding, the burst-dependent learning rule enables the network to selectively learn relevant information (Figure 7B), resulting in potentiation of those synapses (magenta and cyan in Figure 7Biii) that are active temporally separated, but less when simultaneous active (Figure 7Biv). This provides a mechanism to associate temporally separated, reward-relevant information in a neuronal network. The burst-dependent learning rule provides the network with a gating mechanism for memory allocation to an engram. Thus, burst-induced plasticity reduces the number of neurons encoding the reward location and other reward-related information, increasing the specificity of synaptic memory in a neuronal network.

Figure 6. Dopamine-dependent burst-induced plasticity rule reduces the number of neurons in the network encoding a memory.

Figure 6.

(Ai-iii), Standard reinforcement learning rule shows reward associating inputs 1–10 (blue) with potentiation of all synaptic weights (blue). (Bi-iii), Burst-dependent potentiation reduces the number of neurons encoding the memory, leading to potentiation of synapses exclusively onto the most excitable burst-firing neurons 3, 6 (blue).

Figure 6—source data 1. Raster plot data and synaptic weights.

Figure 7. Dopamine-dependent burst-induced plasticity rule enables reinforcement learning (RL) models to encode a specific salient event.

Figure 7.

(Ai-iii), Instructive RL rule allows two inputs that code for different information to store the memory in separate sets of neurons, thus encoding not only the reward, but also other reward-relevant features 3, 6 (magenta, cyan). (Bi-ii), A supervised network enables burst-eliciting feedback synaptic input to assign credit to select synapses in the network to encode a desired reward identity. (Biii) Time-dependent lateral inhibition at the output neurons suppress non-relevant information. When only one of the inputs is active, the animal can learn two different memories over time in neurons 3, 6 (magenta, cyan). (Biv) When both inputs are active at the same time they compete with each other, and synapses onto these neurons (magenta, cyan) are less potentiated.

Figure 7—source data 1. Raster plot data and synaptic weights.

Increased calcium responses and spatial information in reactivated CA1 place cells

Based on these results we predicted that, when an animal navigates toward a reward, the previously reactivated hippocampal neurons would be more strongly activated than non-reactivated neurons. To test this prediction, we monitored calcium transients in hippocampal excitatory cells with a head-mounted microscope (Ghosh et al., 2011) while mice navigated on a ‘cheeseboard’ maze (Dupret et al., 2010) with two reward locations, one new to the animal and one previously learnt (Figure 8A). We defined neuronal reactivation as activity during immobility in neurons previously active during locomotion. Cells that were active when mice moved towards the reward locations were classified as reactivated at reward if they were active again during immobility when mice consumed reward (Foster and Wilson, 2006; O’Neill et al., 2006; Csicsvari et al., 2007) and non-reactivated if no further calcium event was detected after they had reached the reward. Of the cells that were active on the maze in a given trial, 44 ± 1% were reactivated at either or both of the reward locations. There was no detectable difference between the number of cells reactivating at the previously learnt or new reward location (Figure 8B). Less frequent were the reactivations during immobility at non-rewarded locations where 15 ± 2% of cells reactivated (Figure 8B).

Figure 8. Cells that reactivated at reward location have higher activity peaks than non-reactivated cells.

(A), Running path of a mouse (left) and calcium traces of example cells (right) in a representative single trial. Mice ran toward two reward locations: one previously learned (R1) and one new (R2). Calcium traces for two cells that reactivated at the reward location and two that did not. Arrows mark reactivations. Vertical line marks time of arrival at R2. (B), Percentage of cells that reactivated at one or both of the reward locations or other locations. Points mark percentage in single trials. Box-and-whisker plots: median, 25 and 75th percentile, and extreme values. n=444 trials. (C), Traces centered on activity peaks for two example cells: one that reactivated at the new reward location in trial 1 (top) and one that did not reactivate in any of the trials (bottom). (D), Histogram of activity peaks normalized to maximum AUC value compared between cells that reactivated for the first time in the preceding trial and cells that did not reactivate in any of the trials. Triangles mark mean values. Permutation tests for repeated measures ANOVA, model for reactivations at the reward locations: significant effect of trial (F(6, 96)=4.51, p<0.001) and reactivation (F(1,16) = 10.15, p=0.006), model for reactivations at non-rewarded locations: significant effect of trial (F(6, 96)=7.85, p=0.001) and non-significant of reactivation (F(1, 16)=0.56, p=0.47). n=6785 cell trials from 1405 cells. (E), The duration from activity during locomotion to the time of first reactivation did not correlate with activity peaks in the following trial, suggesting a long-lasting eligibility trace.(F), Mean calcium peak in each trial for cells that reactivated in trial 1 or trial 4 compared to cells that did not reactivate in any trial. Ribbons extend +/- 1 SEM. Cells that reactivated in trial 1 had significantly higher normalized calcium peaks in all trials. Cells that reactivated for the first time in trial 4 had significantly higher normalized calcium peaks in trials 4, 5, 7 and 8 but not in trial 6 and trials before the reactivation. Permutation t-tests with Benjamini-Hochberg correction for multiple comparisons and between-animal random effects. n=6721 cell trials from 681 reactivated in trial 1 cells, n=769 from 96 reactivated in trial 4 cells, and n=5980 from non-reactivated 607 cells. **p<0.01.

Figure 8.

Figure 8—figure supplement 1. Similar effect of the reactivation on the activity peaks in different cell groups.

Figure 8—figure supplement 1.

(A), Regardless of the cell event rates, the activity peaks were larger in the reactivated cells than in cells that did not reactivate that day. The comparison was performed on four cell groups defined by the quartile ranges (Q1–4) in the event rate distribution shown in the leftmost histogram. The histogram counts the trial event rates per cell. The four histograms to the right compare activity peaks within each quartile range. Calcium peaks in the cells reactivated for the first time in the previous trial are compared with the calcium peaks of non-reactivated cells, the quartiles are assigned based on the event rate in the trial prior. Triangles mark mean values. Permutation test for repeated measures ANOVA: effect of reactivation: F(1, 16)=7.54, p=0.02; effect of quartile: F(3, 48)=6.48, p=0.002. n=6785 cell trials from 1405 cells. (B) Percentage of cells that had a given calcium peak shown per trial. Cells are grouped by the trial of their first reactivation. In all cell groups, a large percentage of cells decreased calcium peaks in late trials. A larger percentage of the reactivated than non-reactivated cells maintained high peak values. (C), Histogram of normalized activity peaks in place cells and other cells compared between the cells reactivated for the first time in the preceding trial and the cells not reactivated in any of the trials. Triangles mark mean values. Permutation test for repeated measures ANOVA: non-significant interaction between cell type (place cell vs other cell) and reactivation (F(1,16) = 0.11, p=0.75, n=6785 cell trials from 1405 cells). (D), Activity peaks as a function of trial compared between place cells and other cells. Cells whose first reactivation happened in the same trial are grouped together. Ribbons extend +– 1 SEM. The effect of reactivation in trial 1 or in trial 4 was not significantly different for place cells and other cells in any of the trials after the reactivation (non-significant interaction between the cell type and reactivation). Permutation t-tests with Benjamini-Hochberg correction for multiple comparisons and between-animal random effects. n=4261 cell trials from place cells reactivated in trial 1 and n=2460 from other cells, n=507 from place cells reactivated in trial 4 and n=262 from other cells, n=2,409 for non-reactivated place cells and n=3571 from other cells.
Figure 8—figure supplement 2. Place cells during learning in reactivated and non-reactivated neurons.

Figure 8—figure supplement 2.

(A), Examples of CA1 place cells and changes in their activity from early (trials 1–4) to late learning trials (trials 5–8) after the cells were reactivated at the reward location in trial 4. Locations of calcium events marked with a red dot are overlaid over mouse movement paths; place maps are shown below. Reward locations are marked with grey circles. Gray background represents locations unsampled by the mouse. (B), Histogram of the ratios between place map peaks in the late over early learning trials. The ratios are compared between the cells reactivated for the first time in the trial 4 or later and the cells not reactivated at any of the trials. Triangles mark mean values. Effect of reactivation: F(1, 16)=11.3, p=0.008. (C), As in (B) but for change in spatial information of place cells from early to late trials. Effect of reactivation: F(1, 16)=4.4, p=0.048. (D), The duration from activity during locomotion to the time of first reactivation did not correlate with the later change in spatial information. (E), As in (B) but for correlation between place maps for early and late trials. Effect of reactivation: F(1, 16)=0.29, p=0.59. Permutation tests for repeated measures ANOVA. n=194 reactivated and n=306 non-reactivated place cells. **p<0.01, *p<0.05.

To compare the strength of neuronal activation, we measured area-under-curve (AUC) of calcium events occurring before and after the reactivation (Figure 8C). Cells with the largest activity peaks during locomotion were the most likely to reactivate at the reward location. Following their first reactivation, they had larger activity peaks than the previously non-reactivated cells (0.54 ± 0.01 vs 0.42 ± 0.01 of the cell’s max AUC, F(1,16) = 10.12, p=0.006, Figure 8D). The effect on activity peaks was specific to reward locations, and reactivation at other locations did not affect activity peaks in the following trials (F(1, 16)=0.56, p=0.47, Figure 8D). The effect of reactivation was independent of the cell event rates: of two cell groups with matching event rates in a given trial, the one whose cells were reactivated at reward for the first time, had larger peaks in the following trial (F(1, 16)=7.54, p=0.02, Figure 8—figure supplement 1A). The effect of reactivation at reward had two other similarities with the synaptic plasticity experiments: a long-lasting eligibility trace and persistence of the change. To test the first, we confirmed that the activity peaks in the trial following the reactivation did not correlate with the time from the activity during locomotion to the reactivation (Figure 8E). To test the persistence of the change, we confirmed that the reactivated cells maintained higher calcium activity peaks than non-reactivated cells throughout the later trials (Figure 8F, Figure 8—figure supplement 1B). Typically, the peaks during locomotion increased in the trial immediately before the first reactivation at reward (Figure 8—figure supplement 1B), suggesting that more excited cells are the ones that undergo the change. The calcium activity peaks we report could be affected by photobleaching of the GCaMP6f sensor. As the reactivated cells were more active than non-reactivated cells (Figure 8—figure supplement 1A), we would expect them to be more affected by photobleaching, but nevertheless, the magnitude of their calcium transients remained higher than in the non-reactivated cells.

60% of the reactivated cells (n=971 from 7 mice) and 42% of the non-reactivated cells (n=284 from 7 mice) showed activity at specific locations and were classified as place cells (see Methods). Both place cells and non-place cells showed higher calcium peaks following reactivation and the increase in place cells was not significantly different from that in non-place cells (F(1,16) = 0.11, p=0.75, Figure 8—figure supplement 1C, D). To investigate any learning-induced changes in place maps we assessed how the location-averaged activity in place cells changed with reactivation from the first to the second half of the trials (change from trials 1–4 to trials 5–8, Figure 8—figure supplement 2A). The place map peaks increased significantly more in the place cells that reactivated in trial 4 or later than in the non-reactivated place cells (Figure 8—figure supplement 2B, place map peak ratio 1.4 ± 0.1 vs 1.1 ± 0.1, n=194 reactivated and n=306 non-reactivated place cells; permutation test for repeated measures ANOVA: F(1, 16)=11.3, p=0.008). The change in spatial information also significantly differed between the two groups (Figure 8—figure supplement 2C, change by 0.011 ± 0.016 to −0.047 ± 0.014 a.u.; permutation test for repeated measures ANOVA: F(1, 16)=4.4, p=0.048), but there was no significant difference in place map stability (Figure 8—figure supplement 2E, correlation of 0.45 ± 0.02 vs 0.47 ± 0.02; permutation test for repeated measures ANOVA: F(1, 16)=0.29, p=0.59). The time from the activity during locomotion to the reactivation did not correlate with a change in spatial information (Figure 8—figure supplement 2D). Reactivated place cells conveyed more spatial information in late trials (trials 5–8) compared to place cells that did not reactivate (0.15 ± 0.01 vs 0.12 ± 0.01 a.u.; permutation test for repeated measures ANOVA: F(1, 16)=4.4, p=0.055). This learning-associated increase in calcium response and spatial information supports a reactivation-dependent LTP-like mechanism in vivo (Cacucci et al., 2007).

Discussion

In summary, we investigated the effects of postsynaptic neuronal reactivation on hippocampal synaptic plasticity, reinforcement learning, and spatial coding. We found that postsynaptic burst reactivation of CA1 pyramidal cells in the presence of the reward signal DA rapidly potentiates synapses that have previously undergone a spike timing-dependent priming protocol, providing direct evidence for reactivation-induced synaptic plasticity. A computational model showed how this learning rule increases specificity in reinforcement learning models. Recordings from freely moving mice showed that neurons that reactivated at reward locations had enhanced CA1 place cell calcium signals and carried more spatial information than cells that did not reactivate.

The results suggest that reactivation-induced plasticity is mediated by two sequential coincidence detectors: postsynaptic NMDARs detecting coincident pre- and postsynaptic activity and AC1/AC8 as coincidence detector of DA and reactivation-induced Ca2+ increase. Although we used a spike pairing protocol, we cannot exclude the possibility that activation of postsynaptic NMDA receptors without postsynaptic action potentials would be sufficient to set the eligibility trace.

AC1/AC8 is synergistically activated when the two signals, Gs-coupled dopamine D1/D5 receptor activation and Ca2+ influx, occur at the same time (Wayman et al., 1994; Watson et al., 2000; Ferguson and Storm, 2004; Neve et al., 2004). The time course of DA signaling depends on brain area, firing mode (tonic or phasic) of dopaminergic cells, DA release and diffusion as well as time course of intracellular signaling pathways (Liu et al., 2021). Recent developments of fluorescent DA sensors (Sun et al., 2018; Sun et al., 2020; Elizarova et al., 2022) would enable monitoring the precise time course of DA in the hippocampus in future studies. Moreover, to investigate the precise timing requirements for DA-dependent reactivation-induced plasticity further, uncaging of caged DA or optogenetically-induced DA release would be suitable approaches for temporal control of the DA transient.

Our experiments were carried out at 2 mM external calcium concentration, which is a standard calcium concentration used in most ex vivo plasticity experiments, but above the reported ionic calcium concentration in rat and human cerebrospinal fluid (Jones and Keep, 1988; Forsberg et al., 2019). Unfortunately, the extracellular calcium concentration at synaptic sites is not known, as discussed elsewhere (Lopes and Cunha, 2019). It was recently reported that burst pairing, but not pairings of single pre- and postsynaptic action potentials, induces synaptic plasticity at 1.3 mM extracellular calcium in rat hippocampal slices (Inglebert et al., 2020). It will be interesting to investigate whether, even if single-spike pairing might not induce plasticity at low calcium concentrations, it would still be sufficient for the initial priming of those synapses. If that were the case, this would set the conditions to enable reactivation-induced plasticity, which relies on bursts, and would hold also in low-calcium conditions. The signaling cascade leading to synaptic potentiation involves postsynaptic PKA and protein synthesis, which are not required for conventional early LTP (Park et al., 2014). Traditionally, LTP has been classified into early- and late-phase LTP (Frey et al., 1988; Frey et al., 1990), and it has been reported that dopaminergic signaling is required for maintenance of late-phase LTP (Frey et al., 1990; Huang and Kandel, 1995; Matthies et al., 1997). The DA-dependent form of plasticity we describe here shares properties with ‘late-phase’ LTP, including a role of postsynaptic action potentials during induction (Dudek and Fields, 2002) and a requirement of protein synthesis for expression (Frey et al., 1988; Huang and Kandel, 1995). However, it is remarkably fast, suggesting a dissociation between different signaling pathways, rather than different temporal phases of LTP. The mechanism we describe here would be compatible with some of the key concepts of the ‘revised’ synaptic tagging hypothesis (Redondo and Morris, 2011). Specifically, our findings strongly support the view that the fate of a memory is not determined at the time of encoding. This is based on the finding that plasticity induction can lead to two events: (1) expression of LTP or LTD, and (2) setting of a synapse-specific eligibility trace (‘tagging’) which allows modulation by protein synthesis-dependent signaling. A major difference is that our findings highlight a role of DA as a key modulator of the eligibility trace. The mechanistic basis for the surprising involvement of protein synthesis in this rapidly induced form of plasticity remains to be investigated.

It has been reported that anisomycin can potentiate JNKs (Iordanov et al., 1997). We cannot exclude the possibility that the drug may have affected intracellular signaling cascades that interfere with the plasticity signaling pathway described here.

Our experimental findings uncover a synaptic learning rule that could support a two-stage model of hippocampal memory formation (Buzsáki, 1989), in which eligibility traces are laid down during hippocampal theta activity with subsequent postsynaptic burst reactivation during sharp wave-ripples inducing LTP at those synapses.

We considered the activity during navigation on the maze as the animal approaches the reward resembling the STDP priming protocol. Substantial evidence supports a role of NMDAR-dependent STDP in the formation of place fields during navigation (Mehta, 2015; Moore et al., 2021). It has been postulated that both LTP and LTD are involved in place field formation. This was based on the observation that place fields shift backward with experience (Mehta et al., 1997), and a computational model predicted that without LTD place field broadening would occur. Thus LTP is required when entering the place field, and LTD when the animal exits the place field (Mehta et al., 2000). This is specific to navigation, as opposed to just walking on a linear track without task, and place field plasticity is predictive of navigational performance (Moore et al., 2021).

Exploring possible computational implications of this synaptic learning rule, we first tested it with a neural network in which the most excitable cells are reactivated. The memory allocation hypothesis suggests that learning triggers a temporary increase in neuronal excitability, enabling the linking of individual memories acquired close in time (Silva et al., 2009; Cai et al., 2016; Sehgal et al., 2018). We found that our learning rule selectively strengthens the reactivated synapses, linking together the memory allocation hypothesis with burst reactivation-induced plasticity. Interestingly, it was recently reported that DA released by locus coeruleus cells projecting to dCA1 has a key permissive role in contextual memory linking (Chowdhury et al., 2021). Moreover, the rule could also accommodate a temporally discontiguous instructive learning signal or a specific supervisory feedback signal. Thus, it is possible that DA serves as a scalar reward signal which combines with a vectorial representation of the reward identity which triggers the reactivation of a specific subset of neurons. Thus, it was suggested that the direct pathway from the entorhinal cortex could provide an instructive signal to generate accumulation of CA1 place cells at the reward location (Grienberger and Magee, 2021). An attractive possibility is that a downstream subset of neurons active during navigation serves as an instructive input onto upstream neurons during reward. This would be consistent with a dual role of DA as reinforcement signal and enhancer of reverse replay (Ambrose et al., 2016), establishing a predictive chain of potentiated synapses toward the rewarded outcome reminiscent of the successor representation (Dayan, 1993; Stachenfeld et al., 2017). It was suggested in a computational study that feedback regulation of synaptic plasticity by bursts in higher hierarchical circuits can coordinate lower-level connections (Payeur et al., 2021). Our results reveal a possible biological substrate to support such a mechanism and re-emphasize the importance of bursting activity for synaptic plasticity (Lisman, 1997; Pike et al., 1999).

The hippocampus receives dopaminergic input from two main sources, the ventral tegmental area (Scatton et al., 1980; Gasbarri et al., 1994), signaling reward, and the locus coeruleus (Smith and Greene, 2012; Kempadoo et al., 2016), thought to signal novelty (Takeuchi et al., 2016; Wagatsuma et al., 2018), but more recently implicated also in spatial reward learning (Kaufman et al., 2020). In a reward location learning task, we found that calcium signals during locomotion were higher in CA1 principal cells that reactivated at reward location. The signal increased in the trial preceding the first reactivation, following which the signal remained higher in all subsequent trials. Moreover, place cells that reactivated showed higher spatial information than non-reactivating place cells in late trials. The overall stability of calcium signals and spatial information is broadly consistent with earlier reports using one-photon imaging in freely moving mice (Ziv et al., 2013). Although it was recently reported in head-fixed mice that reactivation increases long-term stability of place cells that have fields distant from the reward after several days (Grosmark et al., 2021), we did not see a significant increase in place cell stability after a single reactivation of the cell at the reward location. Irrespectively, the difference in both calcium signal and spatial information in reactivating vs non-reactivating cells is suggestive of plasticity related to reactivation events specific to reward location. More work will be required to establish under what conditions the novel burst-induced potentiation mechanism is engaged during hippocampus-dependent learning and memory.

Methods

Key resources table.

Reagent type (species) or resource Designation Source or reference Identifiers Additional information
Strain, strain background (Mus musculus) Wild-type C57BL/6 J, Mus musculus Harlan, Bicester, UK or Central Animal Facility, Physiological Laboratory, Cambridge University Age range used for slice preparation: 12–19 days. Females and males were used.
Strain, strain background (Mus musculus) Adenylate cyclase double knock-out (AC DKO), C57BL/6 J,
Mus musculus
Mouse line generated by Hongbing Wang and imported from Michigan State University, MI, US Age range used for slice preparation: 12–19 days. Females and males were used.
Strain, strain background (Mus musculus) Thy1 – GCaMP6f, C57BL/6 J, Mus musculus Jax
Ref: Dana et al., 2014
024276 Only males were used.
Chemical compound, drug Dopamine hydrochloride Sigma–Aldrich H8502 100 μM
Chemical compound, drug D-AP5 Tocris Bioscience 0106 100 µM
Chemical compound, drug Nimodipine Tocris Bioscience 0600/100 10 µM
Chemical compound, drug PKA inhibitor fragment (6-22) amide Tocris Bioscience 1904/1 1 µM
Chemical compound, drug Anisomycin Tocris Bioscience 1290/10 0.5 mM
Chemical compound, drug MK801 Tocris Bioscience 0924 1 mM
Software, algorithm Igor Pro 6.37 WaveMetrics
Software, algorithm Prism 8.2.0 (435) Graphpad
Software, algorithm Matlab R2021a MathWorks
Software, algorithm CaImAn software (version 1.8.5, Python) Giovannucci et al., 2019

Mice

Experimental procedures and animal use were performed in accordance with UK Home Office regulations of the UK Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act 1986 and Amendment Regulations 2012, following ethical review by the University of Cambridge Animal Welfare and Ethical Review Body (AWERB). All animal procedures were authorized under Personal and Project licences held by the authors.

Mice were housed on a 12 hr light/dark cycle at 19–23 °C and were provided with food and water ad libitum. Experiments were carried out on wildtype C57BL/6 J mice (Harlan, Bicester, UK or Central Animal Facility, Physiological Laboratory, Cambridge University), and adenylate cyclase double knockout (AC DKO) mice, which have the genes for both AC1 and AC8 deleted globally. This mouse line was generated as described previously25 and was imported from Michigan State University, MI, US. For in vivo experiments, seven adult males Thy1 – GCaMP6f transgenic mice were used (Dana et al., 2014) (Jax: 024276). Mice were housed with 2–4 cage-mates in cages with running wheels.

Electrophysiology

Slice preparation

Mice of both sexes at postnatal day (P) 12–19 were used in this study. Mice were anesthetized with isoflurane (4% isoflurane in oxygen) and decapitated. The brain was rapidly removed and immersed in ice-cold artificial cerebrospinal fluid (ACSF) containing (in mM): 126 NaCl, 3 KCl, 26.4 NaH2CO3, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 2 MgSO4, 2 CaCl2, and 10 glucose (pH 7.2, 270–290 mOsm/L). The ACSF solution was continuously bubbled with carbogen gas (95% O2, 5% CO2). Horizontal slices (350 μm thick) were sectioned with a vibrating microtome (Leica VT 1200 S, Leica Biosystems, Wetzlar, Germany). The slices were then incubated for at least 60 min in ACSF at room temperature in a submerged-style storage chamber before recording. Slices were used for 1–7 hr following sectioning.

Whole-cell patch clamp recording

For recordings, individual slices were transferred to an immersion-type recording chamber and perfused with ACSF (2 ml/min) at 24–26 °C. Neurons were visualized and selected using infrared differential interference contrast (DIC) microscopy using a 40 X water-immersion objective. The hippocampal subfields were visually identified and whole-cell patch-clamp recordings were performed on CA1 pyramidal neurons. For stimulation of Schaffer collaterals, monopolar stimulation electrodes were placed in stratum radiatum. Test and control pathway electrodes were placed at the same distance (>100 µm) from and either side of the recorded neuron. Patch pipettes (pipette resistance 4–7 MΩ) were pulled from borosilicate glass capillaries (0.68 mm inner diameter, 1.2 mm outer diameter) using a P-97 Flaming/Brown micropipette puller (Sutter Instruments Co., Novato, California, USA). Pipettes were filled with a solution containing (mM): 110 potassium gluconate, 4 NaCl, 40 HEPES, 2 ATP-Mg, 0.3 GTP (pH 7.2–7.3, 270–285 mOsm/L). The liquid junction potential was not corrected.

All experiments were performed in current-clamp mode. Cells were accepted for the experiment if their resting membrane potential was between −55 and −70 mV. The membrane potential was held at −70 mV throughout the recording by direct current application via the recording electrode. Before the start of each recording, all cells were tested for regular spiking responses to positive current steps—characteristic of pyramidal neurons.

Stimulation protocol

Excitatory postsynaptic potentials (EPSPs) were evoked alternately in two input pathways (test and control) by direct current pulses at 0.2 Hz (stimulus duration 50 μs) through metal stimulation electrodes. Control pathways were used in all experiments to ensure stability control (not always shown). The stimulation intensity was adjusted (100 μA– 500 µA) to evoke an EPSP with peak amplitude between 3 and 8 mV. After a stable EPSP baseline period of at least 10 min, STDP was induced in the test pathway by repeated pairings of single evoked EPSPs and single postsynaptic action potential elicited with the minimum somatic current pulse (1–1.8 nA, 3ms) via the recording electrode. Pairings were repeated 100 times at 0.2 Hz. Spike-timing intervals (Δt in ms) were measured between the onset of the EPSP and the onset of the action potential.

Alternate stimulation of EPSPs was resumed immediately after the pairing protocol and monitored for at least 40 min. For the burst stimulation protocol, stimulation of EPSPs was not resumed for an additional 10 min, and at the end of that period, six bursts, each of five action potentials at 50 Hz, were elicited with an inter-burst interval of 0.1 Hz by somatic current pulses (1.8 nA, 10 ms) via the recording electrode. In a subset of experiments, only five bursts were applied, which led to potentiation of a similar magnitude. Immediately after the bursts, stimulation of EPSPs was resumed and monitored for at least 30 min, however, a small subset of recordings were stopped at 28 min. Presynaptic stimulation frequency to evoke EPSPs remained constant throughout the experiment. The unpaired pathway served to verify input-specificity and as a stability control. The burst stimulation protocol is summarized in Figure 1a (top).

Drugs

Drugs were bath-applied to the whole slice through the perfusion system by dilution of concentrated stock solutions (prepared in water or DMSO) in ACSF, or by adding the drugs to the patch pipette solution when it was applied intracellularly to the postsynaptic cell only. If the drug was not water-soluble, vehicle control experiments were carried out. For each set of recordings, interleaved control and drug conditions were carried out and were pseudorandomly chosen. The following drugs were used in this study: 100 μM dopamine hydrochloride (Sigma–Aldrich, Dorset, United Kingdom), 100 μM D-AP5 (Tocris Bioscience, Bristol, United Kingdom), 10 µM nimodipine (Tocris Bioscience), 1 µM PKA inhibitor fragment (6-22) amide (Tocris Bioscience), 0.5 mM anisomycin (stock solution in EtOH; Tocris Bioscience), and 1 mM MK801 (Tocris Bioscience).

Data acquisition and data analysis of slice recordings

Data were collected using an Axon Multiclamp 700B amplifier (Molecular Devices, Sunnyvale, California, USA) and filtered at 2 kHz. Data were acquired and digitized at 5 kHz using an Instrutech ITC-18 A/D interface board (Instrutech, Port Washington, New York, USA) and custom-made acquisition procedures in Igor Pro (WaveMetrics, Lake Oswego, Oregon, USA).

All experiments were carried out in current clamp (‘bridge’) mode, and only cells with an initial series resistance between 9 and 16 MΩ were included. Series resistance was compensated for by adjusting the bridge balance, and data were discarded if series resistance changed by more than 30%. Offline analyses were done using custom-made procedures in Igor Pro. EPSP slopes were measured on the rising phase of the EPSP as a linear fit between the time points corresponding to 25–30% and 70–75% of the peak amplitude. For statistical analysis, the mean EPSP slope per minute of the recording was calculated from 12 consecutive sweeps and normalized to the baseline (each data point in source data files is the mean of 12 averaged EPSPs). Normalized EPSP slopes from the last 5 min of the baseline (immediately before pairing) and from the last 5 min of the recording were averaged. The magnitude of plasticity, as an indicator of change in synaptic weights, was defined as the average EPSP slope after pairing expressed as a percentage of the average EPSP slope during baseline.

Statistical analysis of slice recordings

Statistical comparisons were performed using one-sample two-tailed, paired two-tailed, or unpaired two-tailed Student’s t-test, with a significance level of α=0.05. Data are presented as mean ± SEM. Significance levels are indicated by *p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001. All datasets passed the test for normality using the Shapiro-Wilk test (α=0.05).

Computational modeling

We simulated a set of n=10 output neurons, which each received input from 10 input neurons. When an instructive input was added, output neurons additionally received input from two out of 10 instructive neurons. Each output neuron projected uniquely onto one readout neuron which again projected back to the output neuron in a one-to-one mapping. Each output neuron was modeled as an Integrate-and-Fire neuron and 100 trials were simulated, where the voltage v is described by

τvdvdt=v+winpoIinp+winstoIinst+wreadoIread+Iintrinsic

where τv=10ms is the membrane time constant, Iinp are the spike trains of the input neurons, Iinst are the spike trains from the instructive neurons, Iread are the spike trains from the read-out neurons, winpo are the weights from the input to the output neurons, winsto=1/N are the weights from the instructive neurons to the output neurons, and wreado=1 are the weights from the readout to the output neurons. In addition, each neuron was receiving an intrinsic current Iintrinsic=αintrinsicη, where η is simply white noise drawn from a uniform distribution between 0 and 1 and αintrinsic is the magnitude of the current (αintrinsic was applied to all neurons in Figure 6B, while in the other configurations no intrinsic current was applied). When the voltage crosses the firing threshold = 0.4, the neuron is reset to the resting potential v=0. Each read-out neuron was also modeled as an Integrate-and-Fire neuron

τvdvreaddt=vread+woreadIo+I

where Io are the spike-trains of the output neurons, woread = 1/N are the weights from the output to the readout neurons, and I are the spike trains from the supervised neurons. Similarly, if the voltage crossed the firing threshold = 0.4, the neurons emit a spike and the voltage is reset to 0. winpo were plastic under the following rule. Every time the input neurons are firing (at tpre), they are leaving a trace xpre, τSTDPdxpredt=-xpre+δt-tpre, where τSTDP=10ms is the trace time constant. Similarly, every time the output neuron spikes (at tpost), it is leaving a trace xpost,τSTDPdxpostdt=-xpost+δt-tpost .

The eligibility trace e is described as

τededt=e+xprexpost

where τe=10 min is the eligibility time constant. This value was based on the experimental protocol, where burst reactivation was applied 10 min after priming. The winpo are potentiated if there is an eligibility trace together with dopamine and a postsynaptic burst:

winpo=αLTPdeb

where αLTP=0.0002 is the learning rate, d=0 if there is no dopamine, and d=1 if there is dopamine.

The postsynaptic burst b is detected as follows. We first computed a trace xburst as

τburstdxburstdt=xburst+δ(ttpost),

where τburst=5ms is the burst time constant. We set b=1 if xburst>burstthreshold , to 0 otherwise, with burstthreshold = 1.1. If there was no dopamine nor bursts during the whole trial, then the updated rule resulted in a depression

winpo=αLTDe

where αLTD=αLTP /500 is the learning rate for the depression. Weights are bound to stay positive. The weights winpo are initialized to 1 /N. We simulated the network for 2 s in Figure 6 and 2.5 s in Figure 7. The input neurons were firing Poisson statistics at 20 Hz for the first 500 ms of the trial (pairing phase). Dopamine was present during the last 500 ms of the trials in Figure 6 and the last 1 s in Figure 7. The network was simulated for 100 trials (error bars are standard deviations).

In Figure 6A, a standard reinforcement learning rule was used: winpo=winpo+αRLde, where αRL=αLTP /50. In Figure 6B, a burst dependent rule was used. Output neurons 3 and 6 had an increased excitability. We modeled that by decreasing the firing threshold to 0.25. All neurons also received an intrinsic current with αintrinsic=0.06 during the last 500 ms of each trial. In Figure 7A, the instructive neuron 3 fired Poisson statistics at 500 Hz from time 1.5–2 s and the instructive neuron 6 fired Poisson statistics at 500 Hz from time 2–2.5 s. In Figure 7B ii/iii, the read-out neuron 3 received additional Poisson inputs at 60 Hz from time 1.5–2 s and the read-out neuron 6 from time 2–2.5 s. For Figure 7B iv, however, the read-out neurons 3 and 6 received their additional Poisson inputs at the same time, from 1.5–2.5 s. In Figure 7B, we added lateral inhibition, where each read-out neuron received an inhibitory filtered version (with a time constant of 50 ms) of the spike trains of other read-out neurons, with weights of –1 /N (without self-connection).

Behavior

Surgery

Mice underwent two surgeries: the first one to implant a GRIN lens directly above the cells of interest, and the other to fix an aluminum baseplate above the GRIN lens for later attachment of the miniature microscope. The procedures followed the protocol described before by Resendez et al., 2016. Surgeries were carried out following minimal standard for aseptic surgery. Analgesic (Meloxicam, 2 mg.kg-1 intraperitoneal) was administered 30 min prior to surgery. Mice were anesthetized with isoflurane (5% induction, 1–2% maintenance, Abbott Ltd, Maidenhead, UK) mixed with oxygen as carrier gas (flow rate 1.0–2.0 L.min-1) and placed in a stereotaxic frame (David Kopf Instruments, Tujunga, CA, USA). The skull was exposed by making skin incision and Bregma and Lambda were aligned horizontally. A 1.5–2 mm-wide craniotomy was drilled above the implantation site. The brain tissue above the implantation site was aspirated. Buffered ACSF was applied throughout the aspiration to prevent desiccation of the tissue. A GRIN lens (1 mm diameter, 4.3 mm length, 0.4 pitch, 0.50 numerical aperture, Grintech) was stereotaxically lowered at coordinates –1.75 AP, 1.75 ML, 1.35–1.40 DV (in mm from Bregma) and fixed to the skull surface with ultraviolet-light curable glue (Loctite 4305) and further fixed with dental adhesive (Metabond, Sun Medical) and dental acrylic cement (Simplex Rapid, Kemdent). A metal head bar was attached to the cranium using dental acrylic cement for head-fixing the animal during the microscope mounting.

If the GCaMP6f expression was visible in the implanted mouse, 4 weeks later the animals were anesthetized for the purpose of attaching a baseplate for the microscope above the top of the GRIN lens. The baseplate was cemented into place and the miniscope was unlocked and detached from the baseplate.

Behavioral learning task

The mice performed a rewarded spatial navigation task on a round-shaped maze (cheeseboard maze; Dupret et al., 2010). The 120 cm diameter cheeseboard had a total of 177 evenly spaced wells. The rewarded wells were baited with ∼100 μL of condensed milk mixed 1:1 with water.

For the first three days, the mice foraged for rewards baited in randomly selected wells. A different, random set of wells was baited in each trial. Next, we performed a spatial learning task. The mice had to learn two locations with baited wells. The baited wells had fixed locations that were at least 40 cm apart, chosen pseudo randomly for each mouse. Mice started the trial in one of the three locations on the maze: south, east or west. The maze was rotated and wiped with a disinfectant (Dettol) in-between the trials to discourage the use of intra-maze cues. Landmarks of black and white cues were installed on the walls surrounding the maze. The trials were terminated once the mice ate both rewards or after 300 s. Each learning day consisted of 8 trials with 2–4-minute-long breaks between the trials. To minimize the effects of the novel environment and task structure, we analyzed the neural activity on the first learning day after one of the previous reward locations was moved. After the 5-day-long learning, the memory retention was tested on the next day in a 4 to 5-minute-long unbaited trial. Following the learning and testing of the memory for the first set of locations, we translocated one of the reward locations. The new location was a pseudo-randomly chosen to be at least 40 cm away from the current and previous reward locations. The learning of the new set of locations was performed over two days and tested in an unbaited trial as described above.

The trials were recorded with an overhead webcam video camera. The video was recorded at 24 Hz frame rate. The mice body location was tracked with DeepLabCut software (Mathis et al., 2018), a custom-written software was written to map the mouse coordinates to relative location on the maze. The extracted tracks were smoothed with a Gaussian kernel. Periods of running were identified when velocity of the mouse smoothed with a moving average 0.5 s window exceeded 4 cm/s. Immobility was defined as periods of not running that exceeded a duration of 0.5 s.

Calcium imaging

CaImAn software (version 1.8.5, Python) was used to motion-correct any movements between the calcium imaging frames, identify the cells and extract their fluorescence signal from the video recordings (Giovannucci et al., 2019). The method for the cell and signal detection is based on constrained non-negative matrix factorization (CNMF-E; Pnevmatikakis et al., 2016)⁠. CaImAn extracted background-subtracted calcium fluorescence values and the deconvolved the signal. The deconvolved signal can be interpreted as a scaled probability of a neuron being active. The calcium imaging videos recorded in the same-day trials were concatenated and motion-corrected to a common template frame. Signal extraction and further processing were performed on the resulting long video, allowing to detect the cells and signal present across the trials. To improve the computational performance, the video frames were cropped to a rectangle containing the regions of interest, and the video width and height were downsampled by a factor of 2.

The identified putative cells were automatically filtered using CaImAn. The results were visually inspected and the filtering parameters adjusted to exclude non-cell like shapes and traces from the filtered components. The criteria used for the filtering included a threshold for signal to noise ratio of the trace, the minimum and maximum size of the component‘s spatial footprint, threshold for consistency of the spatial footprint at different times of the component‘s activation, and a threshold for component‘s resemblance to a neuronal soma as evaluated by a convolutional neural network provided with CaImAn software.

The deconvolved trace was time binned, averaging the values in 200 ms bins. A calcium event was detected whenever the cell‘s deconvolved signal crossed 20% of its day-maximum value. A cell was classified as active during locomotion if it had at least one calcium event. A cell was classified as reactivated if it had at least one calcium event during immobility period and it was active during preceding locomotion. If during the immobility period mice were located within 6 cm of the reward, the reactivation was classified as reactivation at reward, otherwise as reactivation at non-rewarded location. Activity peaks were quantified by their area-under-curve (AUC) which was calculated as a convolution of the preprocessed calcium signal with a 2-s-long flat kernel. The preprocessing of calcium signal subtracted the cell’s median value and truncated the values below 0, so that only the above-median calcium signal is integrated in the AUC calculation. We excluded any samples from cells whose maximum value AUC in a given trial did not exceed 0.

Place cell detection and analysis

To assess how spatial locations modulated activity of a cell, we considered periods of running as described in the 'Behavioral learning task' section and calculated place maps — mean neural activity per spatial bin. The total activity inside 6×6 cm bins was summed from the smoothed deconvolved signal. The mean neural activity in the spatial bin was then calculated as a ratio of the total activity to the total occupancy in the bin after both maps were smoothed across the space using a 2D Gaussian kernel with σ=12 cm. The place map was filtered to include spatial bins with total occupancy that exceeded 1 s (5 time bins, thresholded on unsmoothed total occupancy).

Spatial information of a cell‘s activity was calculated using the place map values. Spatial information (Markus et al., 1994) was defined as:

SpatialInformation=i=1Npiλiλ¯log2(λiλ¯)

where λ¯ represents the mean value of the neural signal, pi represents probability of the occupancy of the i-th bin, and λi represents its mean neural activity. Dividing by λ¯ ensures the metric is independent of the cell‘s average activity. The units of spatial information calculated on calcium fluorescence can be reported as bits per action potential (Climer and Dombeck, 2021). However, because the actual action potentials were not measured, we report them as arbitrary units.

Spatial information was compared to the value expected by chance. The chance level was calculated by circularly shifting the activity with regards to the actual location. For each cell, the activity was circularly shifted within the trial by a time offset chosen randomly (minimum offset of 10 s). If the cell‘s spatial information exceeded 95% values calculated on 1000 random shifts of its activity, it was defined as a place cell.

A limited number of neuronal responses sampled per spatial bin can lead to an upward bias in the estimated spatial information (Treves and Panzeri, 1995). To correct this bias, we subtracted its estimated value from the estimated spatial information. The bias was estimated as the mean spatial information from the time-shifting procedure used for place cell detection. This procedure does not require binning of the neuronal responses from the calcium imaging as required by analytical estimation (Panzeri et al., 2007), and has been used previously to estimate mutual information bias (Akrami et al., 2018).

Statistical testing of in vivo results

To compare the activity peaks between the reactivated and non-reactivated cells throughout the day, we used permutation tests for repeated measures ANOVA (Figure 8E, Figure 8—figure supplement 1A,B, Figure 8—figure supplement 2). The ANOVA modeled fixed effects of trial ordinal and reactivation and random effects of within animal-session factors. Significance level was set to α=0.05. To compare the effect of reactivation in each trial on cells grouped by the trial of their first activation, we used permutation t-tests (Figure 8F, Figure 8—figure supplement 1C). Multiple comparisons were corrected with Benjamini-Hochberg method with the type I error rate set to 0.05. The permutations were restricted to within animal-session permutations. Both ANOVA and t-test statistics were computed based on and 10,000 permutations using ‘permuco‘ R package. Statistical analysis was performed in R version 3.6.3.

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by the Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council, U.K. We are grateful for discussions of this project with other members of the Neuronal Oscillations Group.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Ole Paulsen, Email: op210@cam.ac.uk.

Marco Capogna, University of Aarhus, Denmark.

Laura L Colgin, University of Texas at Austin, United States.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council BB/N019008/1 to Tanja Fuchsberger.

  • Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council BB/P019560/1 to Tanja Fuchsberger.

  • Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council Studentship to Przemyslaw Jarzebowski.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Conceptualization, Investigation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review and editing, Designed the experiments, Conducted the experiments and analyzed the data, Wrote the manuscript.

Software, Investigation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review and editing, Developed the computational model, Wrote the manuscript.

Software, Formal analysis, Investigation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review and editing, Conducted the experiments and analyzed the data, Wrote the manuscript.

Investigation, Writing – review and editing, Conducted the experiments and analyzed the data.

Resources, Provided transgenic AC DKO mice.

Conceptualization, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Writing – original draft, Writing – review and editing, Designed the experiments, Wrote the manuscript.

Ethics

Experimental procedures and animal use were performed in accordance with UK Home Office regulations of the UK Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act 1986 and Amendment Regulations 2012, following ethical review by the University of Cambridge Animal Welfare and Ethical Review Body (AWERB). All animal procedures were authorized under Personal and Project licences held by the authors.

Additional files

MDAR checklist
Source code 1. Code for computational model.

Data availability

Data availability Experimental data and code are available at: Code for computational model and code for in vivo analysis (including a link to in vivo data) are available at: https://github.com/przemyslawj/dCA1-reactivations copy archived at swh:1:rev:22a4e82293f6c36c6fef8c0f06c3f6c68c4045ad. Data of plasticity experiments and of simulation data from computational model are available at: https://data.mendeley.com/datasets/dx7cdgpcz3/1.

The following datasets were generated:

Jarzebowski P. 2022. dCA1-reactivations. Github. github.com/przemyslawj/dCA1-reactivations

Fuchsberger T. 2022. Postsynaptic burst reactivation of hippocampal neurons enables associative plasticity of temporally discontiguous inputs. Mendeley Data.

References

  1. Akrami A, Kopec CD, Diamond ME, Brody CD. Posterior parietal cortex represents sensory history and mediates its effects on behaviour. Nature. 2018;554:368–372. doi: 10.1038/nature25510. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Ambrose RE, Pfeiffer BE, Foster DJ. Reverse replay of hippocampal place cells is uniquely modulated by changing reward. Neuron. 2016;91:1124–1136. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.07.047. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Andrade-Talavera Y, Duque-Feria P, Paulsen O, Rodríguez-Moreno A. Presynaptic spike timing-dependent long-term depression in the mouse hippocampus. Cerebral Cortex. 2016;26:3637–3654. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw172. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bittner KC, Milstein AD, Grienberger C, Romani S, Magee JC. Behavioral time scale synaptic plasticity underlies CA1 place fields. Science. 2017;357:1033–1036. doi: 10.1126/science.aan3846. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Brzosko Z, Schultz W, Paulsen O. Retroactive modulation of spike timing-dependent plasticity by dopamine. eLife. 2015;4:e09685. doi: 10.7554/eLife.09685. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Buzsáki G. Two-stage model of memory trace formation: a role for “noisy” brain states. Neuroscience. 1989;31:551–570. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(89)90423-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Buzsáki G, Horváth Z, Urioste R, Hetke J, Wise K. High-frequency network oscillation in the hippocampus. Science. 1992;256:1025–1027. doi: 10.1126/science.1589772. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Cacucci F, Wills TJ, Lever C, Giese KP, O’Keefe J. Experience-dependent increase in CA1 place cell spatial information, but not spatial reproducibility, is dependent on the autophosphorylation of the alpha-isoform of the calcium/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27:7854–7859. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1704-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Cai DJ, Aharoni D, Shuman T, Shobe J, Biane J, Song W, Wei B, Veshkini M, La-Vu M, Lou J, Flores SE, Kim I, Sano Y, Zhou M, Baumgaertel K, Lavi A, Kamata M, Tuszynski M, Mayford M, Golshani P, Silva AJ. A shared neural ensemble links distinct contextual memories encoded close in time. Nature. 2016;534:115–118. doi: 10.1038/nature17955. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Chowdhury A, Luchetti A, Fernandes G, Almeida Filho D, Kastellakis G, Tzilivaki A, Ramirez EM, Tran MY, Poirazi P, Silva AJ. A locus coeruleus- dorsal CA1 dopaminergic circuit modulates memory linking. Neuron. 2021;110:3374–3388. doi: 10.2139/ssrn.3985190. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Climer JR, Dombeck DA. Information theoretic approaches to deciphering the neural code with functional fluorescence imaging. ENeuro. 2021;8:ENEURO.0266-21.2021. doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0266-21.2021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Csicsvari J, O’Neill J, Allen K, Senior T. Place-selective firing contributes to the reverse-order reactivation of CA1 pyramidal cells during sharp waves in open-field exploration. The European Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;26:704–716. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05684.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Dana H, Chen TW, Hu A, Shields BC, Guo C, Looger LL, Kim DS, Svoboda K. Thy1-GCaMP6 transgenic mice for neuronal population imaging in vivo. PLOS ONE. 2014;9:e108697. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0108697. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Dayan P. Improving generalization for temporal difference learning: the successor representation. Neural Computation. 1993;5:613–624. doi: 10.1162/neco.1993.5.4.613. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  15. Diba K, Buzsáki G. Forward and reverse hippocampal place-cell sequences during ripples. Nature Neuroscience. 2007;10:1241–1242. doi: 10.1038/nn1961. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Dudek SM, Fields RD. Somatic action potentials are sufficient for late-phase LTP-related cell signaling. PNAS. 2002;99:3962–3967. doi: 10.1073/pnas.062510599. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Dupret D, O’Neill J, Pleydell-Bouverie B, Csicsvari J. The reorganization and reactivation of hippocampal maps predict spatial memory performance. Nature Neuroscience. 2010;13:995–1002. doi: 10.1038/nn.2599. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Elizarova S, Chouaib AA, Shaib A, Hill B, Mann F, Brose N, Kruss S, Daniel JA. A fluorescent nanosensor paint detects dopamine release at axonal varicosities with high spatiotemporal resolution. PNAS. 2022;119:e2202842119. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2202842119. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Ferguson GD, Storm DR. Why calcium-stimulated adenylyl cyclases? Physiology. 2004;19:271–276. doi: 10.1152/physiol.00010.2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Forsberg M, Seth H, Björefeldt A, Lyckenvik T, Andersson M, Wasling P, Zetterberg H, Hanse E. Ionized calcium in human cerebrospinal fluid and its influence on intrinsic and synaptic excitability of hippocampal pyramidal neurons in the rat. Journal of Neurochemistry. 2019;149:452–470. doi: 10.1111/jnc.14693. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Foster DJ, Wilson MA. Reverse replay of behavioural sequences in hippocampal place cells during the awake state. Nature. 2006;440:680–683. doi: 10.1038/nature04587. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Frémaux N, Gerstner W. Neuromodulated spike-timing-dependent plasticity, and theory of three-factor learning rules. Frontiers in Neural Circuits. 2016;9:85. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2015.00085. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Frey U, Krug M, Reymann KG, Matthies H. Anisomycin, an inhibitor of protein synthesis, blocks late phases of LTP phenomena in the hippocampal CA1 region in vitro. Brain Research. 1988;452:57–65. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(88)90008-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Frey U, Schroeder H, Matthies H. Dopaminergic antagonists prevent long-term maintenance of posttetanic LTP in the CA1 region of rat hippocampal slices. Brain Research. 1990;522:69–75. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(90)91578-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Frey U, Huang YY, Kandel ER. Effects of cAMP simulate a late stage of LTP in hippocampal CA1 neurons. Science. 1993;260:1661–1664. doi: 10.1126/science.8389057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Gasbarri A, Verney C, Innocenzi R, Campana E, Pacitti C. Mesolimbic dopaminergic neurons innervating the hippocampal formation in the rat: a combined retrograde tracing and immunohistochemical study. Brain Research. 1994;668:71–79. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(94)90512-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Gerstner W, Lehmann M, Liakoni V, Corneil D, Brea J. Eligibility traces and plasticity on behavioral time scales: experimental support of neoHebbian three-factor learning rules. Frontiers in Neural Circuits. 2018;12:53. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2018.00053. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Ghosh KK, Burns LD, Cocker ED, Nimmerjahn A, Ziv Y, Gamal AE, Schnitzer MJ. Miniaturized integration of a fluorescence microscope. Nature Methods. 2011;8:871–878. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.1694. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Giovannucci A, Friedrich J, Gunn P, Kalfon J, Brown BL, Koay SA, Taxidis J, Najafi F, Gauthier JL, Zhou P, Khakh BS, Tank DW, Chklovskii DB, Pnevmatikakis EA. CaImAn an open source tool for scalable calcium imaging data analysis. eLife. 2019;8:e38173. doi: 10.7554/eLife.38173. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Grienberger C, Magee JC. Entorhinal Cortex Directs Learning-Related Changes in CA1 Representations. bioRxiv. 2021 doi: 10.1101/2021.12.10.472158. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  31. Grosmark AD, Sparks FT, Davis MJ, Losonczy A. Reactivation predicts the consolidation of unbiased long-term cognitive maps. Nature Neuroscience. 2021;24:1574–1585. doi: 10.1038/s41593-021-00920-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Grover LM, Teyler TJ. Two components of long-term potentiation induced by different patterns of afferent activation. Nature. 1990;347:477–479. doi: 10.1038/347477a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Huang YY, Kandel ER. D1/D5 receptor agonists induce a protein synthesis-dependent late potentiation in the CA1 region of the hippocampus. PNAS. 1995;92:2446–2450. doi: 10.1073/pnas.92.7.2446. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Igata H, Ikegaya Y, Sasaki T. Prioritized experience replays on a hippocampal predictive map for learning. PNAS. 2021;118:e2011266118. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2011266118. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Inglebert Y, Aljadeff J, Brunel N, Debanne D. Synaptic plasticity rules with physiological calcium levels. PNAS. 2020;117:33639–33648. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2013663117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Iordanov MS, Pribnow D, Magun JL, Dinh TH, Pearson JA, Chen SL, Magun BE. Ribotoxic stress response: activation of the stress-activated protein kinase JNK1 by inhibitors of the peptidyl transferase reaction and by sequence-specific RNA damage to the alpha-sarcin/ricin loop in the 28S rRNA. Molecular and Cellular Biology. 1997;17:3373–3381. doi: 10.1128/MCB.17.6.3373. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Jones HC, Keep RF. Brain fluid calcium concentration and response to acute hypercalcaemia during development in the rat. The Journal of Physiology. 1988;402:579–593. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1988.sp017223. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Kaufman AM, Geiller T, Losonczy A. A role for the locus coeruleus in hippocampal CA1 place cell reorganization during spatial reward learning. Neuron. 2020;105:1018–1026. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2019.12.029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Kempadoo KA, Mosharov EV, Choi SJ, Sulzer D, Kandel ER. Dopamine release from the locus coeruleus to the dorsal hippocampus promotes spatial learning and memory. PNAS. 2016;113:14835–14840. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1616515114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Kudrimoti HS, Barnes CA, McNaughton BL. Reactivation of hippocampal cell assemblies: effects of behavioral state, experience, and EEG dynamics. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1999;19:4090–4101. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-10-04090.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Lee AK, Wilson MA. Memory of sequential experience in the hippocampus during slow wave sleep. Neuron. 2002;36:1183–1194. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)01096-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Lisman JE. Bursts as a unit of neural information: making unreliable synapses reliable. Trends in Neurosciences. 1997;20:38–43. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(96)10070-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Liu C, Goel P, Kaeser PS. Spatial and temporal scales of dopamine transmission. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience. 2021;22:345–358. doi: 10.1038/s41583-021-00455-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Ljungberg T, Apicella P, Schultz W. Responses of monkey dopamine neurons during learning of behavioral reactions. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1992;67:145–163. doi: 10.1152/jn.1992.67.1.145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Lopes JP, Cunha RA. What is the extracellular calcium concentration within brain synapses? Journal of Neurochemistry. 2019;149:435–437. doi: 10.1111/jnc.14696. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Markus EJ, Barnes CA, McNaughton BL, Gladden VL, Skaggs WE. Spatial information content and reliability of hippocampal CA1 neurons: effects of visual input. Hippocampus. 1994;4:410–421. doi: 10.1002/hipo.450040404. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Mathis A, Mamidanna P, Cury KM, Abe T, Murthy VN, Mathis MW, Bethge M. DeepLabCut: markerless pose estimation of user-defined body parts with deep learning. Nature Neuroscience. 2018;21:1281–1289. doi: 10.1038/s41593-018-0209-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Matthies H, Becker A, Schröeder H, Kraus J, Höllt V, Krug M. Dopamine D1-deficient mutant mice do not express the late phase of hippocampal long-term potentiation. Neuroreport. 1997;8:3533–3535. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199711100-00023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. McNamara CG, Tejero-Cantero Á, Trouche S, Campo-Urriza N, Dupret D. Dopaminergic neurons promote hippocampal reactivation and spatial memory persistence. Nature Neuroscience. 2014;17:1658–1660. doi: 10.1038/nn.3843. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Mehta MR, Barnes CA, McNaughton B. Experience-dependent, asymmetric expansion of hippocampal place fields. PNAS. 1997;94:8918–8921. doi: 10.1073/pnas.94.16.8918. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Mehta MR, Quirk MC, Wilson MA. Experience-dependent asymmetric shape of hippocampal receptive fields. Neuron. 2000;25:707–715. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)81072-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Mehta MR. From synaptic plasticity to spatial maps and sequence learning. Hippocampus. 2015;25:756–762. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22472. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Milstein AD, Li Y, Bittner KC, Grienberger C, Soltesz I, Magee JC, Romani S. Bidirectional synaptic plasticity rapidly modifies hippocampal representations. eLife. 2021;10:e73046. doi: 10.7554/eLife.73046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Moore JJ, Cushman JD, Acharya L, Popeney B, Mehta MR. Linking hippocampal multiplexed tuning, Hebbian plasticity and navigation. Nature. 2021;599:442–448. doi: 10.1038/s41586-021-03989-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Nádasdy Z, Hirase H, Czurkó A, Csicsvari J, Buzsáki G. Replay and time compression of recurring spike sequences in the hippocampus. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1999;19:9497–9507. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-21-09497.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Neve KA, Seamans JK, Trantham-Davidson H. Dopamine receptor signaling. Journal of Receptor and Signal Transduction Research. 2004;24:165–205. doi: 10.1081/rrs-200029981. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. O’Neill J, Senior T, Csicsvari J. Place-selective firing of CA1 pyramidal cells during sharp wave/ripple network patterns in exploratory behavior. Neuron. 2006;49:143–155. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.10.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Panzeri S, Senatore R, Montemurro MA, Petersen RS. Correcting for the sampling bias problem in spike train information measures. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2007;98:1064–1072. doi: 10.1152/jn.00559.2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Park P, Volianskis A, Sanderson TM, Bortolotto ZA, Jane DE, Zhuo M, Kaang BK, Collingridge GL. NMDA receptor-dependent long-term potentiation comprises a family of temporally overlapping forms of synaptic plasticity that are induced by different patterns of stimulation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences. 2014;369:20130131. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0131. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Payeur A, Guerguiev J, Zenke F, Richards BA, Naud R. Burst-dependent synaptic plasticity can coordinate learning in hierarchical circuits. Nature Neuroscience. 2021;24:1010–1019. doi: 10.1038/s41593-021-00857-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Pike FG, Meredith RM, Olding AW, Paulsen O. Postsynaptic bursting is essential for “ hebbian” induction of associative long-term potentiation at excitatory synapses in rat hippocampus. The Journal of Physiology. 1999;518 (Pt 2):571–576. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.1999.0571p.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Pnevmatikakis EA, Soudry D, Gao Y, Machado TA, Merel J, Pfau D, Reardon T, Mu Y, Lacefield C, Yang W, Ahrens M, Bruno R, Jessell TM, Peterka DS, Yuste R, Paninski L. Simultaneous denoising, deconvolution, and demixing of calcium imaging data. Neuron. 2016;89:285–299. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.11.037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Redondo RL, Morris RG. Making memories last: the synaptic tagging and capture hypothesis. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2011;12:17–30. doi: 10.1038/nrn2963. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Resendez SL, Jennings JH, Ung RL, Namboodiri VMK, Zhou ZC, Otis JM, Nomura H, McHenry JA, Kosyk O, Stuber GD. Visualization of cortical, subcortical and deep brain neural circuit dynamics during naturalistic mammalian behavior with head-mounted microscopes and chronically implanted lenses. Nature Protocols. 2016;11:566–597. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2016.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Sassone-Corsi P. The cyclic AMP pathway. Cold Spring Harbor Perspectives in Biology. 2012;4:a011148. doi: 10.1101/cshperspect.a011148. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Scatton B, Simon H, Le Moal M, Bischoff S. Origin of dopaminergic innervation of the rat hippocampal formation. Neuroscience Letters. 1980;18:125–131. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(80)90314-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Schultz W, Dayan P, Montague PR. A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science. 1997;275:1593–1599. doi: 10.1126/science.275.5306.1593. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Schultz W. Behavioral dopamine signals. Trends in Neurosciences. 2007;30:203–210. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2007.03.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Sehgal M, Zhou M, Lavi A, Huang S, Zhou Y, Silva AJ. Memory allocation mechanisms underlie memory linking across time. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 2018;153:21–25. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2018.02.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Shipton OA, Paulsen O. Glun2A and GluN2B subunit-containing NMDA receptors in hippocampal plasticity. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences. 2014;369:20130163. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0163. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Silva AJ, Zhou Y, Rogerson T, Shobe J, Balaji J. Molecular and cellular approaches to memory allocation in neural circuits. Science. 2009;326:391–395. doi: 10.1126/science.1174519. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Singer AC, Frank LM. Rewarded outcomes enhance reactivation of experience in the hippocampus. Neuron. 2009;64:910–921. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.11.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Smith CC, Greene RW. CNS dopamine transmission mediated by noradrenergic innervation. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32:6072–6080. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6486-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Stachenfeld KL, Botvinick MM, Gershman SJ. The hippocampus as a predictive map. Nature Neuroscience. 2017;20:1643–1653. doi: 10.1038/nn.4650. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Sun F, Zeng J, Jing M, Zhou J, Feng J, Owen SF, Luo Y, Li F, Wang H, Yamaguchi T, Yong Z, Gao Y, Peng W, Wang L, Zhang S, Du J, Lin D, Xu M, Kreitzer AC, Cui G, Li Y. A genetically encoded fluorescent sensor enables rapid and specific detection of dopamine in flies, fish, and mice. Cell. 2018;174:481–496. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2018.06.042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Sun F, Zhou J, Dai B, Qian T, Zeng J, Li X, Zhuo Y, Zhang Y, Wang Y, Qian C, Tan K, Feng J, Dong H, Lin D, Cui G, Li Y. Next-Generation GRAB sensors for monitoring dopaminergic activity in vivo. Nature Methods. 2020;17:1156–1166. doi: 10.1038/s41592-020-00981-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Sutton RS, Barto AG. Reinforcement Learning: An Introduction. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press; 2018. [Google Scholar]
  78. Takeuchi T, Duszkiewicz AJ, Sonneborn A, Spooner PA, Yamasaki M, Watanabe M, Smith CC, Fernández G, Deisseroth K, Greene RW, Morris RGM. Locus coeruleus and dopaminergic consolidation of everyday memory. Nature. 2016;537:357–362. doi: 10.1038/nature19325. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Treves A, Panzeri S. The upward bias in measures of information derived from limited data samples. Neural Computation. 1995;7:399–407. doi: 10.1162/neco.1995.7.2.399. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  80. Wagatsuma A, Okuyama T, Sun C, Smith LM, Abe K, Tonegawa S. Locus coeruleus input to hippocampal CA3 drives single-trial learning of a novel context. PNAS. 2018;115:E310–E316. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1714082115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Watson EL, Jacobson KL, Singh JC, Idzerda R, Ott SM, DiJulio DH, Wong ST, Storm DR. The type 8 adenylyl cyclase is critical for Ca2+ stimulation of camp accumulation in mouse parotid acini. The Journal of Biological Chemistry. 2000;275:14691–14699. doi: 10.1074/jbc.275.19.14691. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Wayman GA, Impey S, Wu Z, Kindsvogel W, Prichard L, Storm DR. Synergistic activation of the type I adenylyl cyclase by Ca2+ and Gs-coupled receptors in vivo. The Journal of Biological Chemistry. 1994;269:25400–25405. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Wilson MA, McNaughton BL. Reactivation of hippocampal ensemble memories during sleep. Science. 1994;265:676–679. doi: 10.1126/science.8036517. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Wong ST, Athos J, Figueroa XA, Pineda VV, Schaefer ML, Chavkin CC, Muglia LJ, Storm DR. Calcium-stimulated adenylyl cyclase activity is critical for hippocampus-dependent long-term memory and late phase LTP. Neuron. 1999;23:787–798. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(01)80036-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Yiu AP, Mercaldo V, Yan C, Richards B, Rashid AJ, Hsiang H-LL, Pressey J, Mahadevan V, Tran MM, Kushner SA, Woodin MA, Frankland PW, Josselyn SA. Neurons are recruited to a memory trace based on relative neuronal excitability immediately before training. Neuron. 2014;83:722–735. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.07.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Ziv Y, Burns LD, Cocker ED, Hamel EO, Ghosh KK, Kitch LJ, El Gamal A, Schnitzer MJ. Long-term dynamics of CA1 hippocampal place codes. Nature Neuroscience. 2013;16:264–266. doi: 10.1038/nn.3329. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Editor's evaluation

Marco Capogna 1

This article contains fundamental findings that substantially advance understanding of an important research question, mostly using an appropriate and validated methodology in line with the current state-of-the-art, with good and convincing support for the claims. The message of the article will have a profound and lasting influence on neuroscience.

Decision letter

Editor: Marco Capogna1

Our editorial process produces two outputs: (i) public reviews designed to be posted alongside the preprint for the benefit of readers; (ii) feedback on the manuscript for the authors, including requests for revisions, shown below. We also include an acceptance summary that explains what the editors found interesting or important about the work.

Decision letter after peer review:

Thank you for submitting your article "Postsynaptic burst reactivation of hippocampal neurons enables associative plasticity of temporally discontiguous inputs" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by 3 peer reviewers, and the evaluation has been overseen by a Reviewing Editor and Laura Colgin as the Senior Editor. The reviewers have opted to remain anonymous.

The reviewers have discussed their reviews with one another, and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this to help you prepare a revised submission.

Essential revisions:

1) Further discuss the relationships between burst and DA timing.

2) Acknowledge the relatively non-physiological concentration of calcium used and discuss its influence on the interpretation of the data.

3) Consider including a number of editorial changes as detailed by Reviewer 2.

4) Improvements in methods and statistical analysis, as suggested by reviewer 2.

5) Strengthen the link between the in vitro STDP data as commented by reviewer 3.

Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

1. The so-called "Hebbian pairing protocol" (e.g., p 4) is not Hebbian. Donald Hebb famously never mentioned synaptic weakening in his books; he only spoke of synaptic strengthening. Also, his postulate also speaks of a cell A that along with a set of other presynaptic cells elicits the spiking in the postsynaptic cell B and how this leads to the strengthening of the connection between A and B. In other words, A necessarily fires before B. In spike-timing-dependent plasticity, the tLTP window is therefore consistent with what Hebb predicted, whereas the tLTD window is neither in disagreement nor agreement with his postulate. Rather, it is an extension of Hebb's postulate. Therefore, the authors should not call this "Hebbian pairing", because it is not. They could, however, call it "spike pairing", "correlated firing", "acausal spiking", or some such thing.

2. It is unclear what is supposed to drive the postsynaptic activity during reactivation, and also when. During sleep? For example, p8 "During DA, information is allocated to primed synapses without the need of further coincident pre- and postsynaptic activity, but by reactivation of the postsynaptic neuron alone." Please clarify here and elsewhere. Please generally try to elaborate on this.

3. The statistical treatment is at times unclear. For example, in Figure 8, "Permutation t-tests with Benjamini-Hochberg correction", but to what value was the false discovery rate set, and how was this value determined? Figure 8F, Stats for React in 4 done over all bins or just 4-8? If the latter, how was this selected for? Same in Figure 8-S1.

4. Electrophysiology methods are at times unclear. Why were the slice experiments carried out at 24-26 {degree sign}C? Why not a more physiological temperature, which is what most labs do? Maybe the eligibility trace decays faster at more physiological temperatures? This would reduce the biological plausibility of this candidate mechanism. P12-19 is not a very mature age. Why were the experiments not carried out in mature animals? I am concerned that these findings might be particular to juveniles. Page 19, "monopolar stimulation electrodes were placed in stratum radiatum", but how far from the recorded CA1 cells? Stimulation too close to the recorded cell is known to possibly activate neuromodulatory fibres such as DA or ACh, which could affect the outcome. Please clarify. It is not immediately clear how many bursts were used. Figures indicate only one arrow, yet the methods state "5-6 bursts" were used, but then later, it says "by somatic current pulses via the recording electrode (5x 1.8 nA, 10 ms each)", where 5x would indicate five bursts. The authors need to be clear in the figures about the precise number of bursts and how many spikes each burst carried. P20, "digitized at 5 kHz", and so filtered at 2.5kHz as per the Nyquist criterion? Please state. P21, R_s selection is unclear, "Series resistance was monitored (10-15 MΩ)", surely the R_s wasn't always <15MOhm, esp. if it could change by as much as 30%, so I am not sure what this means. What was the pipette resistance measured to?

5. Modelling could be clearer. P 22, "tau_e = 10 min is the eligibility time constant. " Where does this value come from? Please justify. P 21, "(α_intrinsic is taken as 0 unless otherwise stated)", please clarify what this means and why it would sometimes be not zero. P26, "Experimental data and code are available at https://github.com/ …" I cannot evaluate code that is not made available.

6. Why is the priming done with post-pre and not pre-post pairing? Conceptually, this part is entirely unclear to me. (related to Major Point 1) Is this choice of timing explained somewhere and I missed it?

7. I struggle with evaluating Figures6 and 7. I simply do not understand what is going on. For example, F7Aii top, Instructive Neurons, is completely blank. The same thing in Bii, Supervised neurons. Why show empty graphs? Why show things that are not talked about in the figure caption text? Figure 8 is also quite unclear.

8. Control experiments seem to be missing or are perhaps just not consistently shown. Please clarify.

– Control experiments (i.e. control pathway) are clear for Figure 1 but are they missing or just not shown elsewhere?

– In Figure 5, Stability control with just anisomycin application seems to be missing too.

– Anisomycin has been shown to result in "profound suppression of neural activity" in the hippocampus (Sharma, Nargang, Dickson, JN 2012), which can affect STDP pairing. Have the authors compared the effects of anisomycin on AP parameters, possibly with anisomycin wash-in?

– Anisomycin can also potentiate JNKs (Iordanov et al. Mol Cell Biol 1997), which are important in synaptic release (e.g. Natisco et al., Sci Rep 2015, Abrahamsson et al., Neuron 2017). It may therefore be helpful to use an alternative protein synthesis inhibitor to confirm the results.

Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

It would be informative if the authors vary the timing between priming and dopamine application. In their previous work where they used continuous stimulation in the presence of dopamine (Brzosko et al., 2015), a successful potentiation occurs if dopamine is applied immediately after STDP pairing, whereas with a 10 min gap no change is observed. Is the timing between pairing and dopamine application critical or rather the synaptic stimulation (bursting in this case) in the presence of dopamine the point?

Since MPEP blocks presynaptic LTD, it is surprising to me that the amount of potentiation is comparable whether MPEP is present or not (figure 1F vs figure 2A). Any explanation?

Unlike the parts for electrophysiology, the calcium imaging and the navigation-reward task sections are not provided with ample details.

To have a better comparison, the average percentage of reactivated cells at any pair of locations in the maze needs to be calculated. The same analyses shown in Figure 8 need to be done for the locations without rewards.

Page 25: "… The chance level was calculated by circularly shifting the activity with regards to the actual location." Why was circularly shifted activity with a delay used instead of randomly shuffling the activity with regards to the actual location? By shifting, some information still remains in the activity.

Is the increase in the spatial information of a neuron correlated with the temporal gap between its activity during the mice approach and its reactivation at the reward location? On average, does a shorter time gap correlate with a larger activity peak in the following trials?

The reference for Csicsvari et al., has been repeated twice.

eLife. 2022 Oct 13;11:e81071. doi: 10.7554/eLife.81071.sa2

Author response


Essential revisions:

1) Further discuss the relationships between burst and DA timing.

We have now added this to the Discussion section (page 12, lines 304).

2) Acknowledge the relatively non-physiological concentration of calcium used and discuss its influence on the interpretation of the data.

We have added a discussion of this point on page 12, lines 309.

3) Consider including a number of editorial changes as detailed by Reviewer 2.

We have addressed all of the points that Reviewer 2 raised, and incorporated most of their suggestions into our manuscript. Additionally, several changes on figures have been made as described in detail below (Figure1, 4, 6 and 7).

4) Improvements in methods and statistical analysis, as suggested by reviewer 2.

We have addressed all issues that were raised concerning the Methods section, including the addition of control experiments showing that postsynaptically applied anisomycin has no effect on baseline stability and action potential properties (see supplementary Figure 5-S1). All issues concerning statistical analysis were addressed. Specifically, results for the reward based navigation task have been updated compared with the previous version. We corrected the code that excluded a small number of samples from the results. Some small changes in p-values were further caused by rerunning randomization-based permutation tests.

5) Strengthen the link between the in vitro STDP data as commented by reviewer 3.

Additional analysis of in vivo data has been carried out (pages 9-11, and Figure 8, Figure 8-S1 and Figure 8-S2) to strengthen the link between in vitro and in vivo data. Furthermore, we have added a section to the Discussion providing evidence for the requirement of STDP for place cell formation during navigation (page 13, lines 345).

Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

1. The so-called "Hebbian pairing protocol" (e.g., p 4) is not Hebbian. Donald Hebb famously never mentioned synaptic weakening in his books; he only spoke of synaptic strengthening. Also, his postulate also speaks of a cell A that along with a set of other presynaptic cells elicits the spiking in the postsynaptic cell B and how this leads to the strengthening of the connection between A and B. In other words, A necessarily fires before B. In spike-timing-dependent plasticity, the tLTP window is therefore consistent with what Hebb predicted, whereas the tLTD window is neither in disagreement nor agreement with his postulate. Rather, it is an extension of Hebb's postulate. Therefore, the authors should not call this "Hebbian pairing", because it is not. They could, however, call it "spike pairing", "correlated firing", "acausal spiking", or some such thing.

We thank the Reviewer for this correction. We agree and have replaced ‘Hebbian’ with ‘spike pairing’.

2. It is unclear what is supposed to drive the postsynaptic activity during reactivation, and also when. During sleep? For example, p8 "During DA, information is allocated to primed synapses without the need of further coincident pre- and postsynaptic activity, but by reactivation of the postsynaptic neuron alone." Please clarify here and elsewhere. Please generally try to elaborate on this.

Following this statement on p8, "During DA, information is allocated to primed synapses without the need of further coincident pre- and postsynaptic activity, but by reactivation of the postsynaptic neuron alone.", the different possibilities, depending on what drives postsynaptic activity, are discussed (page 8, starting from lines 203 from ‘the broader’). To improve clarity, we have now changed the sentence to

“During DA modulation, information is allocated to primed synapses by reactivation of the postsynaptic neuron, and the broader computational implications of this learning rule depend on the control of postsynaptic neuronal bursting activity.”

Briefly, the two main hypotheses are (1) intrinsic excitability according to the memory allocation hypothesis (Yiu et al., 2014) (2) or presynaptic input from other areas (e.g.entorhinal cortex) that encode additional information. Additionally, in the Introduction we give some background on when reactivation occurs and how it is related to neuronal activity (principal neurons fire action potentials in brief bursts during sharp wave ripples) (please see page 3, line 70-77). This is discussed further in the Discussion section on page 14, line 364.

3. The statistical treatment is at times unclear. For example, in Figure 8, "Permutation t-tests with Benjamini-Hochberg correction", but to what value was the false discovery rate set, and how was this value determined? Figure 8F, Stats for React in 4 done over all bins or just 4-8? If the latter, how was this selected for? Same in Figure 8-S1.

For Benjamini-Hochberg corrections, type I error rate was set to 0.05, and we added this information to Methods section (page 30, line 788).

“Multiple comparisons were corrected with Benjamini-Hochberg method with the type I error rate set to 0.05.”

The statistical differences between cell groups shown in Figure 8F were calculated across all trials (trials 1 to 8). We built two statistical models for the effect of reactivation and trial: one model comparing the calcium activity peaks of the reactivated in trial 1 cells with the nonreactivated cells, and another comparing the reactivated in trial 4 cells with non-reactivated cells. The cells reactivated in trial 4 had significantly higher calcium peaks than the nonreactivated cells in trials 4, 5, 7 and 8. Their calcium peaks were not significantly higher in trials 1 to 3 (trials before reactivation), and trial 6 (trial after reactivation). We now state the following in the Figure 8F legend:

“Cells that reactivated in trial 1 had significantly higher normalized calcium peaks in all trials. Cells reactivated for the first time in trial 4 had significantly higher normalized calcium peaks in trials 4, 5, 7 and 8 but not in trial 6 and trials before the reactivation.”

Because the meaning of asterisks marking the significant differences was confusing the

statistically compared values, we removed the asterisks from Figure 8F and Figure 8–S1C. n.s. mark on Figure 8–S1C refers to non-significant interaction between the reactivation and cell type (place cell vs other cell).

We also corrected the n counts in Figures 8F and 8-S1C. Please note that the counts for these panels differ from the ones in Figure 8D. This is because the samples in Figure 8D are restricted for the reactivated cells to the first trial after the reactivation.

4. Electrophysiology methods are at times unclear. Why were the slice experiments carried out at 24-26 °C? Why not a more physiological temperature, which is what most labs do? Maybe the eligibility trace decays faster at more physiological temperatures? This would reduce the biological plausibility of this candidate mechanism.

In our experience, cells in slice recordings at higher temperatures deteriorate more quickly. This is not a problem for shorter recordings, but for spike-timing dependent plasticity we need stable conditions for at least 1 hour during the whole-cell recordings, and it was not feasible to do these recordings closer to body temperature. Furthermore, one of the goals of this study was to compare the signalling mechanism to the one observed during dopamine dependent plasticity with synaptic stimulation (Brzosko et al., 2015), so it was important to keep the conditions as comparable as possible.

P12-19 is not a very mature age. Why were the experiments not carried out in mature animals? I am concerned that these findings might be particular to juveniles.

We agree that, ideally, the ex vivo and in vivo experiments should be done at the

same age.

There are two main reasons for the choice of a younger age for slice preparation. Firstly,

younger tissue is less affected by the sectioning procedure. Secondly, the plasticity induction protocol relies on negative spike pairing, which is typically not leading to synaptic depression in slices of older animals. Thus, the investigation of the conversion from synaptic depression into potentiation required the use of juvenile tissue. However, our findings in vivo suggest that the priming mechanism is physiologically relevant also in adult animals.

Page 19, "monopolar stimulation electrodes were placed in stratum radiatum", but how far from the recorded CA1 cells? Stimulation too close to the recorded cell is known to possibly activate neuromodulatory fibres such as DA or ACh, which could affect the outcome. Please clarify.

We fully agree with the Reviewer that electrical stimulation can potentially lead to the corelease of neuromodulators. In fact, we previously reported that spike pairing at Δt = -10 ms can lead to synaptic potentiation instead of depression due to co-released dopamine (Brzosko et al., eLife 2015). Thus care was taken to always place test- and control pathway electrodes at equal distances from the recorded neuron and we ensured the distance exceeded 100 μm. This has been added to the Methods section (page 24, line 576).

Although we cannot exclude the possibility that activation of neuromodulatory fibers couldaffect the outcome of the experiments, this appears unlikely for the experiments

investigating the effect of dopamine and reactivation, since we did not stimulate the test

pathway during bath-application of dopamine.

It is not immediately clear how many bursts were used. Figures indicate only one arrow, yet the methods state "5-6 bursts" were used, but then later, it says "by somatic current pulses via the recording electrode (5x 1.8 nA, 10 ms each)", where 5x would indicate five bursts. The authors need to be clear in the figures about the precise number of bursts and how many spikes each burst carried.

We apologise for the lack of clarity on this. The Methods text has now been updated

accordingly (page 24, line 599), and we now also state this in the Figure legend of Figure 1a.

“For the burst stimulation protocol, stimulation of EPSPs was not resumed for an additional 10 mins and at the end of that period, six bursts, each of five action potentials at 50 Hz, were elicited with an inter-burst interval of 0.1 Hz, by somatic current pulses (1.8 nA, 10 ms) via the recording electrode. In a subset of experiments only five bursts were applied which led to potentiation of similar magnitude.”

P20, "digitized at 5 kHz", and so filtered at 2.5kHz as per the Nyquist criterion? Please state.

Yes, as now stated in the Methods section, the signal was filtered at 2 kHz and sampled at 5 kHz. (page 25, line 622)

P21, R_s selection is unclear, "Series resistance was monitored (10-15 MΩ)", surely the R_s wasn't always <15MOhm, esp. if it could change by as much as 30%, so I am not sure what this means.

We thank the Reviewer for pointing this out and apologize for a lack of clarity. This has now been rewritten (page 25, line 625).

All experiments were carried out in current clamp (‘bridge’) mode, and only cells with an initial series resistance between 9 and 16 MΩ were included. Series resistance was

compensated for by adjusting the bridge balance, and data was discarded if series resistance changed by more than 30%.

What was the pipette resistance measured to?

The pipette resistance was 4–7 MΩ. (page 24, line 577)

5. Modelling could be clearer. P 22, "tau_e = 10 min is the eligibility time constant. " Where does this value come from? Please justify.

tau_e = 10 min was based on the experimental protocol, where burst reactivation was

applied 10 minutes after priming. This has been added to the Methods section (page 27, line 667).

P 21, "(α_intrinsic is taken as 0 unless otherwise stated)", please clarify what this means and why it would sometimes be not zero.

α_intrinsic was applied to all neurons in Figure 6B, while in the other configurations no

intrinsic current was applied. We have changed the wording of this sentence to make this clearer (page 26, line 655).

P26, "Experimental data and code are available at https://github.com/ …" I cannot evaluate code that is not made available.

Experimental data and code have now been uploaded to github.com and referenced in the manuscript (page 30, line 793).

Code for computational model and code for in vivo analysis (including a link to in vivo data) are available at: https://github.com/przemyslawj/dCA1-reactivations. Data of plasticity experiments and of simulation data from computational model are available at: https://data.mendeley.com/datasets/dx7cdgpcz3/1.

6. Why is the priming done with post-pre and not pre-post pairing? Conceptually, this part is entirely unclear to me. (related to Major Point 1) Is this choice of timing explained somewhere and I missed it?

We chose the negative pairing paradigm in order to be able to clearly distinguish the effect of reactivation-induced plasticity from pairing-induced plasticity. With positive pairing it would be challenging to dissociate one type of potentiation from another and results would be difficult to interpret. We have shown though (Figure 1B MPEP) that t-LTD per se is not needed for reactivation-induced plasticity, thus, we concluded that only coincident activity is needed for priming. We have now stated, including in the abstract, that negative pairing was used.

Furthermore, in a behavioural setting it has been postulated that both LTP and LTD occur during place field formation when an animal navigates through an environment. This was based on the observation that place fields shift backwards with experience (Mehta and McNaughton, PNAS 1997), and a computational model predicted that without LTD, place field broadening would occur (Mehta et al., Neuron 2000). Thus LTP is required when entering the place field, and LTD when the animal exits the place field (Mehta et al., Neuron 2000, Mehta, Hippocampus 2015). These observations support the use of negative spike pairing as a behaviorally-relevant and appropriate model for priming.

This has been added to the Discussion (page 13, line 344).

7. I struggle with evaluating Figures 6 and 7. I simply do not understand what is going on. For example, F7Aii top, Instructive Neurons, is completely blank. The same thing in Bii, Supervised neurons. Why show empty graphs? Why show things that are not talked about in the figure caption text? Figure 8 is also quite unclear.

We thank the Reviewer for pointing out that this was not clear. We have now adapted the layout to aid correct interpretation of these figures (see updated Figures 6 and 7). In certain circumstances, there was no activity during part of the protocol (e.g. no activity of instructive neurons during priming period, but they are active during reactivation period), and it is important to show the absence of activity during the initial period.

8. Control experiments seem to be missing or are perhaps just not consistently shown. Please clarify.

– Control experiments (i.e. control pathway) are clear for Figure 1 but are they missing or just not shown elsewhere?

We confirm all experiments were carried out with a control pathway for stability control, but not always shown. In Figure 1, additionally to stability control, it was important to show input specificity of this type of plasticity, thus test and control pathway were shown in all panels. In Figures 2-5, while stability control was carried out, we omitted the control pathway traces for visual clarity. We now state this in the Methods section (page 24, line 590). We have added the control pathway in figures when drugs were used that could affect baseline stability (Anisomycin results, see below).

– In Figure 5, Stability control with just anisomycin application seems to be missing too.

We measured EPSP responses over a 60-minute period (the total time of the experiment) in baseline condition (without pairing or dopamine) and observed that the application of the protein synthesis inhibitor did not induce any change to baseline EPSPs. We agree with the Reviewer that this should be shown, and we have now included these results in our manuscript (supplementary Figure 5-S1a), and results are reported in the text (page 7, line 174).

We confirmed that postsynaptically applied anisomycin did not affect synaptic responses in baseline conditions (95% ± 9.7% vs 100%, t(6) = 0.56, p = 0.59, n = 7) (Figure 5, S1a).

– Anisomycin has been shown to result in "profound suppression of neural activity" in the hippocampus (Sharma, Nargang, Dickson, JN 2012), which can affect STDP pairing. Have the authors compared the effects of anisomycin on AP parameters, possibly with anisomycin wash-in?

We thank the Reviewer for this suggestion. We measured the effect of anisomycin on AP

parameters during pairing (supplementary Figure 5 S1 b and c), and report results in the text (page 7, line 176).

We compared action potential properties during pairing in cells with anisomycin to cells loaded with vehicle controls. Spike amplitude (AM 112 mV ± 3 mV, Vehicle 111 mV ± 3 mV) and spike width (AM 3.3 ms ± 0.2 ms, Vehicle 3.3 ms ± 0.2 ms) showed no significant differences (amplitude t(10) = 0.09050, p = 0.92; width t(10) = 0.1134), p = 0.91, (Figure 5-S1B,C).

– Anisomycin can also potentiate JNKs (Iordanov et al. Mol Cell Biol 1997), which are important in synaptic release (e.g. Natisco et al., Sci Rep 2015, Abrahamsson et al., Neuron 2017). It may therefore be helpful to use an alternative protein synthesis inhibitor to confirm the results.

We thank the Reviewer for raising this important point. Since, in all our experiments,

anisomycin was loaded into the postsynaptic cell through the patch pipette, we would not expect it to affect synaptic release. We confirmed that anisomycin did not affect spike properties during pairing and EPSP responses in baseline condition within the 60-minute period of the experiment (see previous point 8). Nevertheless, we have now added to the discussion that a potential effect on JNKs cannot be excluded (page 13, line 336).

Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

It would be informative if the authors vary the timing between priming and dopamine application. In their previous work where they used continuous stimulation in the presence of dopamine (Brzosko et al., 2015), a successful potentiation occurs if dopamine is applied immediately after STDP pairing, whereas with a 10 min gap no change is observed. Is the timing between pairing and dopamine application critical or rather the synaptic stimulation (bursting in this case) in the presence of dopamine the point?

Our results showed that AC1/AC8 is required for DA- and burst-induced potentiation to

occur. AC1/AC8 are synergistically activated when the two signals, Gs-coupled dopamine D1/D5 receptor activation and ca2+ influx, occur at the same time (Wayman et al., 1994; Watson et al., 2000; Ferguson and Storm, 2004; Neve et al., 2004). To investigate the precise timing requirements for dopamine-dependent reactivation-induced plasticity further, uncaging of caged DA or optogenetically-induced DA release would be suitable approaches for temporal control of the DA transient. These experiments are beyond the scope of the present study. We have added this to the Discussion section (page 12, line 304).

Since MPEP blocks presynaptic LTD, it is surprising to me that the amount of potentiation is comparable whether MPEP is present or not (figure 1F vs figure 2A). Any explanation?

We have previously shown (Brzosko et al., 2015) that, at least in the case of synaptic

stimulation, DA-induced conversion of t-LTD into t-LTP is mediated through two pathways (de-depression and potentiation). If we assume that these two components occur in parallel and are independent, the extent of the potentiation should not be affected by the block of t- LTD.

The strengthening of synaptic weights occurs within minutes and we found that reactivation Induced plasticity requires protein synthesis. Thus there may be a maximum amount of synaptic receptors that can be recruited in that time. This may be a limiting factor in the amount of potentiation that can be reached, at least within the tested time window. Alternatively, in addition to blocking t-LTD, MPEP may partially block/interfere with signaling independent of DA-burst potentiation.

Unlike the parts for electrophysiology, the calcium imaging and the navigation-reward task sections are not provided with ample details.

We added some missing information to the Methods section in the Calcium imaging

subsection (page 28-29). In the Results section, we now state the statistical results as in the electrophysiology section (pages 9-11).

To have a better comparison, the average percentage of reactivated cells at any pair of locations in the maze needs to be calculated. The same analyses shown in Figure 8 need to be done for the locations without rewards.

The Reviewer is right to suggest a control analysis for the effect of immobility at

non-rewarded locations. Mice stopped a median of 4 times at non-reward locations per trial. The number of stops at different locations does not allow us to compare all possible non-reward location pairs. Instead, we investigated if the reactivation at non-rewarded locations affected the following calcium activity peaks.

We now show % of cells reactivated during stops at non-reward locations in Figure 8B. We did not find an effect of reactivation at non-rewarded locations on calcium activity peaks in the trial following the reactivation (Figure 8D).

Page 25: "… The chance level was calculated by circularly shifting the activity with regards to the actual location." Why was circularly shifted activity with a delay used instead of randomly shuffling the activity with regards to the actual location? By shifting, some information still remains in the activity.

The circular shifts of the activity were performed with a randomly drawn delay. Shifting the activity by different delays for each trial with regards to the location data results in

inconsistent information about the location between the trials, as a result, removing spatial information from the calculated place map. Circularly shifting the activity has become a commonly used method in recent years, for example, see Wills et al., 2010, Meshulam et al., 2017, Grosmark et al., 2021. The main advantage over random shuffles is that it preserves temporal dynamics of neuronal activity, making the generated calcium traces more realistic.

References:

Grosmark, A.D. et al. 2020. Offline Memory Reactivation Promotes the Consolidation Of Spatially Unbiased Long-Term Cognitive Maps’. bioRxiv [Preprint]. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.08.20.259879.

Meshulam L, Gauthier JL, Brody CD, Tank DW, Bialek W. 2017. Collective Behavior of Place and Non-place Neurons in the Hippocampal Network. Neuron 96:1178-1191.e4. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2017.10.027

Wills TJ, Cacucci F, Burgess N, O’Keefe J. 2010. Development of the Hippocampal Cognitive Map in Preweanling Rats. Science 328:1573–1576. doi:10.1126/science.1188224

Is the increase in the spatial information of a neuron correlated with the temporal gap between its activity during the mice approach and its reactivation at the reward location? On average, does a shorter time gap correlate with a larger activity peak in the following trials?

We performed additional analysis to answer these questions. The time elapsed from the last activation of the neuron before the mouse arrived at the reward to the time of neuron’s first reactivation did not correlate with a larger activity peak change in the trial following (result now in Figure 8-S1A). Similarly, there was no correlation between the elapsed time and spatial information change (Figure 8-S2C). The discovered synaptic rule leaves a long-duration eligibility trace suggestive a lack of dependence on the time from the initial activity to the reactivation, consistent with these two results.

The reference for Csicsvari et al., has been repeated twice.

We thank the Reviewer for spotting this. The duplicated reference has been removed.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Data Citations

    1. Jarzebowski P. 2022. dCA1-reactivations. Github. github.com/przemyslawj/dCA1-reactivations
    2. Fuchsberger T. 2022. Postsynaptic burst reactivation of hippocampal neurons enables associative plasticity of temporally discontiguous inputs. Mendeley Data. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]

    Supplementary Materials

    Figure 1—source data 1. Normalized EPSP slopes of all recorded cells.
    Figure 2—source data 1. Normalized EPSP slopes of all recorded cells.
    Figure 3—source data 1. Normalized EPSP slopes of all recorded cells.
    Figure 4—source data 1. Normalized EPSP slopes of all recorded cells.
    Figure 5—source data 1. Normalized EPSP slopes, spike amplitudes and spike width of all recorded cells.
    Figure 6—source data 1. Raster plot data and synaptic weights.
    Figure 7—source data 1. Raster plot data and synaptic weights.
    MDAR checklist
    Source code 1. Code for computational model.

    Data Availability Statement

    Data availability Experimental data and code are available at: Code for computational model and code for in vivo analysis (including a link to in vivo data) are available at: https://github.com/przemyslawj/dCA1-reactivations copy archived at swh:1:rev:22a4e82293f6c36c6fef8c0f06c3f6c68c4045ad. Data of plasticity experiments and of simulation data from computational model are available at: https://data.mendeley.com/datasets/dx7cdgpcz3/1.

    The following datasets were generated:

    Jarzebowski P. 2022. dCA1-reactivations. Github. github.com/przemyslawj/dCA1-reactivations

    Fuchsberger T. 2022. Postsynaptic burst reactivation of hippocampal neurons enables associative plasticity of temporally discontiguous inputs. Mendeley Data.


    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES